You are on page 1of 7

0743-4618/86/0204/165$2.

00/0
AAC Augmentative and Alternative Communication
Copyrtght © 1986 by Williams & Wilkins

FORUM
Toward an Augmentative and Alternative
Communication Symbol Taxonomy: A Proposed
Superordinate Classification
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by RMIT University on 08/08/13

Lyle L. Lloyd and Donald R. Fuller


Special Education (L. L. L.) and Audiology and Speech Sciences (L. L. L., D. R. F.), Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana 47907

As the first step in developing a symbol taxonomy, the major augmentative and alternative
communication symbol classifications reported in the literature over the past decade are reviewed.
The terms used to classify symbols are critically evaluated according to their internal logic, their
sociolinguistic implications, and their compatibility with common definitions and usage. The clinical
and educational relevance of the classification approach in question, and the reduction of dual-
classification ambiguity that a given dichotomy may provide are also evaluated. The aided/unaided
classification is based on production of the symbol by the user, while the static/dynamic classifi-
cation is based upon the nature of the symbol’s signal. Although both of these dichotomies have
clinical/educational and theoretical/research implications, the aided/unaided dichotomy is pro-
For personal use only.

posed as the less ambiguous and the more practical classification for the superordinate level of a
taxonomy. A major purpose of this paper is to stimulate discussion of symbol classification that
will lead to the development of a more complete augmentative and alternative communication
symbol taxonomy.

In the last three decades, and especially within the to different definitions and taxonomic approaches
last decade, there has been an increase in the use of (Lloyd, 1984/1985; Waksvik, 1985). In an effort to
augmentative and alternative communication systems develop consistent terminology for the profession, the
as a means of functional communication for individuals International Society for Augmentative and Alternative
with severe speech impairments. As the clinical/edu- Communication (ISAAC) has established a terminology
cational practices and the theoretical and empirical committee whose charge is to collect information and
research base for augmentative and alternative com- ultimately to propose a standard set of descriptors.
munication are developed on an international and trans- Such a task will undoubtedly take several years to
disciplinary level, there has been considerable confu- complete and will hopefully result in a position paper
sion and miscommunication among professionals due concerning terminology that eventually will be adopted

Editorial Note: The forum section is designed to develop the field of augmentative and alternative communication
through the discussion and debate of clinical/educational, technical/manufacturing, and theoretical/empericaI issues.
The forum is designed for letters to the editor, position papers, and in some cases papers which may be considered
provocative by some members of the profession. Readers are encouraged to submit letters to the editor about
manuscripts published in AAC. Such letters may amplify basic points or issues raised in the original paper or may
take exception to procedures, data analysis, interpretation, and discussion in the original papers. Readers are also
encouraged to submit original position papers or “think pieces.” Papers in this section undergo the same peer
review process as other manuscripts with the exception that letters to the editor are given an expedited review.
The expedited Review process typically involves only three members of the editorial board (including the associate
editor of original manuscripts when the letter to the editor is making reference to previously published papers).
This procedure for letters is designed to increase the possibility of letters being published in the one or two issues
immediately following the original manuscript. Readers are encouraged to submit papers to the journal which are
specifically identified for this section of the journal, being designated as either a letter to the editor or a position
paper. Readers are strongly encouraged to submit potentially publishable comments on any manuscript in the
journal. However, we are particularly interested in receiving comments on items published in this section of the
journal.

165
166 Lloyd and Fuller

by ISAAC. In the interim, it is our professional respon- TABLE 1: Published Dichotomies for Symbol Classifications
sibility as clinicians, educators, and researchers to de- Author (in alphabetical order) Classification
bate issues concerning terminology and to strive to- Blau (1983) Graphic and Dynamic
ward consistency in the use of terms, as well as to Fundamental
develop an overall taxonomy for the field of augment-
ative and alternative communication. The process of Carlson (1981) Visual Motor
developing a complete taxonomy for the components
of augmentative and alternative communication must Fristoe & Lloyd (1979)a Static or Aided Dynamic or
Unaided
include: (a) the symbols, (b) the transmission tech-
niques, and (c) the communicative process. The pur- Jones & Cregan (1 986)b Symbols Signs
pose of this paper is to briefly outline the evaluation
metrics for a symbol taxonomy, review previous symbol Kiernan (1977) Representational Sign Language
taxonomies, and propose the adoption of a specific Systems and Sign
Systems
terminology for the superordinate level of a symbol
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by RMIT University on 08/08/13

taxonomy. Kiernan, Reid & Jones Symbols Signs


By critically comparing the different classifications, it (1979, 1 982)b
is possible that an inclusive system for the superordi-
nate classification of symbols will emerge and be refined LAMH (1984) Symbol Gestural
to meet the current needs of the entire profession. The
Lloyd (1980b), Lloyd & Aided Unaided
classification that is ultimately chosen should be a Karlan (1984)
referent system that will best serve to organize subor-
dinate levels of the taxonomy for symbols. This referent Musselwhite & St. Louis Symbolic Gestural
system should be functional and unambiguous. As eval- (1982)
uation metrics for determining the most suitable tax-
onomy, one should consider the reduction of ambiguity Owens & House (1984) Visual Manual
For personal use only.

and the clinical and educational implications such a Romski, Sevcik, & Joy- Aided Unaided
system would provide. The terminology used should ner (1984)
also be evaluated in terms of its sociolinguistic aspects,
internal logic, and compatibility with common definitions Silverman (1980) Gestural-As- Gestural
and usage. sisted and
Neuro-As-
Available Symbol Classifications sisted

There are several extant symbol classifications avail- Vanderheiden & Lloyd Static Dynamic
able. Table 1 lists the published approaches to symbol (1986)
classification currently employed in the field of aug- a
First presented to ASHA in a November 2, 1977 short course by
mentative and alternative communication. For three of Fristoe, Lloyd, and Wilbur as “Aided Systems and their Graphic
the listings in Table 1, a trichotomous classification has Symbols” and “Manual or Unaided Symbols and Systems.”
been modified to fit into a dichotomy (Blau, 1983; b
In common use in the United Kingdom.
Kiernan, 1977; Silverman, 1980). Similarly, Fristoe and c
Presented to ASHA in a November 20, 1981 short course by
Lloyd (1979) used two terms for each part of their Goossens’ and Lloyd.
dichotomy, that is, “static or aided” and “dynamic or
unaided.” In some cases, specific definitions were pre- of augmentative and alternative communication, they
sented (Fristoe & Lloyd, 1979; Kiernan, 1977; Kiernan, are not compatible with common dictionary definitions
Reid & Jones, 1979, 1982; Lloyd, 1980b; Lloyd & and usage. For example, the classification of “symbols”
Karlan, 1984; Vanderheiden & Lloyd, 1986). In other and “signs” is quite common in the United Kingdom and
cases, the authors did not provide exact definitions of some Commonwealth countries. However, these are
their terms for symbol classification but simply classified idiosyncratic uses of “sign” and “symbol” as is clear
symbols according to the terms they chose. from the following definition:
The terms ‘sign’ and ‘symbol’ have acquired par-
Evaluating the Symbol Classifications ticular meaning in the field of non-vocal communi-
The classification systems listed in Table 1 must be cation. ‘Sign’ is normally used in this field to refer
critically evaluated in order to determine if any of them to the use of manual communication through the
can serve as the superordinate level of an augmentative use of sign languages or sign systems. ‘Symbol’
and alternative communication symbol taxonomy. Once is used to refer to a static visual representation of
the superordinate level has been established, subordi- an element in communication. (Kiernan et al., 1982,
nate levels can be considered. p. 4)
This “particular meaning” of symbol adopted by some
Common Definitions and Usage practitioners and scientists during the past decade is
While some terms employed to classify symbols may not consistent with the common dictionary definitions
have frequent usage by some individuals within the field of symbol such as the following:
AAC Symbol Taxonomy 167

. . . 2: something that stands for or suggests some- linguistic characteristics, such as syntax or phonology.
thing else by reason of relationship, association, The terms representational, symbolic, symbols, funda-
convention, or accidental but not intentional resem- mental, manual, motor, visual, and gestural should not
blance. . . 3: an arbitrary or conventional sign (as a be used for classification at the superordinate level of
character, a diagram, a letter, or an abbreviation) a symbol taxonomy due to sociolinguistic or logical
used in writing or printing relating to a particular problems. As a result, two published dichotomies are
field (as mathematics, physics, chemistry, music, still eligible for consideration. These are static/dynamic
or phonetics) to represent operations, quantities, and aided/unaided.
spatial position, valence, direction, elements, rela- The static/dynamic classification is based primarily
tions, qualities, sounds, or other ideas or quali- on the transmission characteristics of the symbol’s
ties. . . 6: an act, sound, or material object having signal. This dichotomy is defined by a single intelligibility
cultural significance and the capacity to excite or feature of the symbol, that is, whether movement (or
objectify a response. . . (Gove, 1976, p. 2316) change) within the signal is a critical factor in symbol
Other practitioners and scientists in the field of aug- intelligibility. This intelligibility factor may be considered
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by RMIT University on 08/08/13

mentative and alternative communication use symbol in isolation, without contextual cues, and becomes
with its more conventional meaning. For example, Fris- apparent in the following definitions (modified from Van-
toe and Lloyd state the following: derheiden & Lloyd, 1986):
Symbols, used to represent objects, actions, rela- • Static symbols (and static symbol systems) in-
tionships, etc., can be spoken, graphic, or manual. clude both graphic symbols and objects which
While spoken symbols are conveyed through the are permanent and enduring. They do not re-
auditory-vocal modality, graphic and manual sym- quire any change or movement to express
bols are conveyed through the visual modality. meaning. Because of this, they lend themselves
(Fristoe & Lloyd, 1979, p. 402) very well to display on communication boards,
This definition of symbol has been used in other publi- electronic devices, and other aids so that most,
cations (Lloyd, 1984/1985; Romski, Lloyd, & Sevcik, in but not all, static symbols could be thought of
press). This conventional use of symbol should facilitate as aided. Most of the letters in most manual
For personal use only.

communication between professionals within the field alphabets are examples of static, but unaided
(e.g., educators, engineers, occupational therapists, symbols.
physical therapists, physicians, psychologists, and • Dynamic symbols (and dynamic symbol sys-
speech pathologists), parents, consumers, and the gen- tems) have their meanings conveyed by change,
eral public. transition, and/or movement, and therefore can-
not be considered as permanent and enduring.
lnternal Logic and Sociolinguistic implications Most gestures and manual signs, and speech
Considering the internal logic of the terms chosen, are dynamic in nature, which generally corre-
several dichotomies can be dismissed. For example, sponds with unaided symbols. However, syn-
classification systems that use terms such as represen- thetic productions of “speech,” “gestures,” and
tational, symbolic, or symbols to describe some symbol “manual signs” are examples of dynamic sym-
sets and systems but not others has an internal logic bols which are aided.
problem. All symbol systems and sets could be consid- Speech, fingerspelling, and printed letters (traditional
ered representational or symbolic, whether they are orthography, or T.O.) are examples of how the intelli-
graphic (e.g., Blissymbolics) or manual (e.g., American gibility factor plays a part in classifying symbols as
Sign Language). Use of the term fundamental, meaning either static or dynamic. Speech is dynamic in nature
basic, to describe some symbol systems and sets, but due to the fact that each phoneme is a result of move-
not others, could also lead to confusion because many ments of the articulators used to produce it and the
symbol sets and systems include both basic symbols transitions from phoneme to phoneme. In connected
and higher level or specialized symbols. speech, the transition from phoneme to phoneme is a
The visual/motor classification also lacks internal critical determinant of intelligibility. In American finger-
logic since many of the motor symbols (e.g., gestures, spelling, the C, A, and T that make up the word CAT
manual signs) are also visual in nature. The same are each static in nature because no movement is
problem exists for the visual/manual classification. required in order to decipher the meaning of each letter
Gestural also presents a problem of internal logic. in isolation. In context, however, movement is required
Since some specific symbols are referred to as gestures to convey the meaning of the word CAT (specifically,
or gestural (e.g., Amerind Gestural Code and Generally the transition from C, to A, to T), but the rate and/or
Understood Gestures), it seems inappropriate to use quality of this movement are not critical intelligibility
the term gestural as a superordinate classification. Fur- factors. The C, A, and T of the word CAT are all
ther, the use of the term gestural has sociolinguistic decipherable regardless of whether or not the receiver
implications and can be confusing when used to classify sees the transition from letter to letter. In other words,
manual signs typically used by the deaf. This term may most of the letters in manual alphabets are static. The
be considered by some persons to have a demeaning letters on this page are also static because no move-
connotation in that it implies a system that is devoid of ment or change in the letters is necessary in order to
168 Lloyd and Fuller

convey meaning. Each orthographic character can be TABLE 2: Static and Dynamic Communication Symbols”
deciphered without movement or transition. Static Dynamic
Table 2 provides a listing of all the major communi-
Objects Gestures, e.g., pointing, yes/
cation symbols (and most of the infrequently used
no head shakes, mime,
symbols) organized according to the static/dynamic Pictures, i.e., photographs & Amer-lnd, Generally Under-
dichotomy and incorporates several dimensions. Em- drawings including simple (or stood Gestures, esoteric
pirical evidence for classification along these dimen- basic) rebus signs/gestures
sions is limited. For the most part, they have only been
Sigsymbols Natural sign language, e.g.,
hypothesized on the basis of clinical/educational ex- ASL, BSL, CSL, FSL, JSL,
perience and logic (e.g., Goossens’ & Lloyd, 1981; Pictogram Ideogram Communi- KSL, SSL, & TSL
Lloyd & Karlan, 1984). The dimensions are: (a) from cation
those demanding the least cognitive ability to those Gestuno
demanding the highest cognitive ability for symbol/ Picsyms Manually Coded English, e.g.,
referent association and use; (b) for the unaided sym- Signed English, PGSS,
bols-those requiring the least motor control to those Blissymbols SEE-I, & SEE-II (or manual
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by RMIT University on 08/08/13

requiring the greatest motor control; (c) from nonverbal coding for other spoken lan-
Graphic representations of man- guages; e.g., manually
(or nonlinguistic) to verbal (or linguistic), that is, those ual signs and/or gestures, e.g. coded Swedish)
best approximating English or other native languages HANDS, pictures of signs,
of the general community; (d) from the more concrete Sign Writer, Sigsymbols & Synthetic or animated “manual
to the more abstract (primarily based upon the referent Worldsign signs and/or gestures”
which can be represented by the symbols); and (e) from Complex (or expanded) Rebus Fingerspelling or manual al-
the more iconic (i.e., transparent and translucent) to the phabets
more opaque (or from those which have some visual Other logographs with referent
representation in some or all of the symbols to those in relationship Synthetic “fingerspelling”
which all of the symbols are arbitrary). For descriptions Eye blink, gestural, and/or vo-
of these various symbol sets and systems, the reader Modified orthography with other
symbols cal alphabet codes, e.g.,
For personal use only.

is referred to other general chapters and texts (e.g., Morse Code


Fristoe & Lloyd, 1979; Jones & Cregan, 1986; Karlan Abstract logographs, e.g., Lexi-
& Lloyd, 1986; Kiernan et al., 1982; Lloyd, 1976; Mus- grams; and shapes, e.g., Pre- Eye blink, gestural, and/or vo-
cal word and/or message
selwhite & St. Louis, 1982; Silverman, 1980; Tebbs, mack-type symbols
codes
1978; Vanderheiden & Grilley, 1976; Vanderheiden &
Traditional orthography (TO)
Lloyd, 1986). Tadoma and other vibrotactile
The aided/unaided classification was first described Fingerspelling or manual alpha-
codes
in 1977 and further developed in summary table form bets
by Lloyd and his colleagues (e.g., Bloomberg & Lloyd, Electronic vibrotacticle codes
1986; Fristoe & Lloyd, 1979; Fristoe, Lloyd, & Wilbur, Synthetic “fingerspelling”
Hand-cued speech, e.g., Cued
1977; Karlan & Lloyd, 1986; Lloyd, 1980a, 1980b, Speech & Danish Mouth
Graphic representation of finger-
1984/1985; Lloyd & Karlan, 1983, 1984; Romski et al., spelling Hand
in press). This dichotomy is based upon production
demands of the symbol, as is illustrated in the following Braille and other static tactile Electronic-cued speech
definitions (modified from Lloyd, 1980a, pp. 44-45): codes
Natural speech
• Aided symbols (and aided symbol systems) refer
to those symbols that require some type of Synthetic speech
external assistance, or an aid or device (e.g., a
From Vanderheiden and Lloyd (1986). These are “formal” or
paper, pencil, pictures, charts, communication
conventionalized symbols and systems; informal nonverbal behaviors
boards, and in some cases electronic devices) or ritualized behaviors have not been included.
for production. Most, but not all aided symbols
may be referred to as graphic. Aided symbols
are frequently enduring and are relatively fixed
or permanent (i.e., remain available in the same quently involve movement or change. In many
form), and therefore most of the aided symbols instances, the movement or change (including
may be thought of as static. durational, temporal, and transitional factors)
• Unaided symbols (and unaided symbol systems) carries much of the meaning, and therefore most
refer to those symbols that do not require any of the unaided symbols may be thought of as
aids or devices for production. Unaided symbols dynamic.
require only the sender’s face, head, hands, Table 3 provides a listing of all the symbols in Table
arms, and other parts of the body, and are 2 according to an aided/unaided classification. Because
frequently referred to as manual, although of the overlap in terminology across the two classifica-
speech is included in this category. Unaided tion systems, Tables 2 and 3 are almost identical,
symbols are typically nonenduring and fre- making the five organizational dimensions mentioned
AAC Symbol Taxonomy 169

TABLE 3: Aided and Unaided Communication Symbolsa namic dichotomy. This point brings us to the evaluation
Aided Unaided metric of ambiguity.
Objects Gestures, e.g., pointing, yes/ Dual-classification Ambiguity
no head shakes, mime,
Pictures, i.e., photographs & Amer-lnd, Generally Under- One factor that should be considered in choosing a
drawings including simple (or stood Gestures, esoteric taxonomy for augmentative and alternative communi-
basic) rebus signs/gestures
cation symbols is the ambiguity of the classification
Sigsymbols Natural sign language, e.g., system that is used to describe them. The classification
ASL, BSL, CSL, FSL, JSL, of fingerspelling is a case in point. Two of the 26 English
Pictogram Ideogram Communi- KSL, SSL, & TSL letters (J and Z) that are represented by American
cation fingerspelling require movement in order to be intelligi-
Gestuno ble. Those two symbols would have to be classified as
Picsyms dynamic under the static/dynamic dichotomy. The other
Manually Coded English, e.g.,
Blissymbols Signed English, PGSS, 24 symbols require no movement to convey meaning,
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by RMIT University on 08/08/13

SEE-I, & SEE-II (or manual and therefore are classified as static. American Sign
Graphic representations of man- coding for other spoken lan- Language (ASL) provides another example of dual clas-
ual signs and/or gestures, e.g. guages; e.g., manually sification of symbols within the same system. Most of
HANDS, pictures of signs, coded Swedish)
the signs in ASL are dynamic; movement is required in
Sign Writer, Sigsymbols &
Worldsign Fingerspelling or manual al- order to convey meaning. However, for some signs
phabets (e.g., MORNING, TELEPHONE, TIME) neither move-
Synthetic or animated “manual ment nor transition are necessary to convey meaning.
signs and/or gestures” Eye blink, gestural, and/or vo- Therefore, some signs could be classified as static.
cal alphabet codes, e.g., Obviously, this dual classification for symbols within
Complex (or expanded) Rebus Morse Code
the same set or system is ambiguous and may create
Other logographs with referent Eye blink, gestural, and/or vo- confusion. On the other hand, one can see by viewing
relationship cal word and/or message Table 3 that there is a clean distinction between sym-
For personal use only.

codes bols classified as unaided and those classified as aided.


Modified orthography and other It allows for a true dichotomous classification of symbol
symbols Tadoma and other vibrotactile
codes systems and sets. In other words, there is no dual
classification for symbols within the same sets or sys-
Abstract logographs, e.g. Lexi-
grams; and shapes, e.g., Pre- Hand-cued speech, e.g., Cued tems, and this clear dichotomy minimizes ambiguity. In
mack-type symbols Speech & Danish Mouth terms of ambiguity, the static/dynamic classification is
Hand at a disadvantage to the aided/unaided classification
Traditional orthography (TO) system. In order for the static/dynamic feature to be
Natural Speech
used for classification, three classes are needed to fit
Graphic representation of finger-
spelling
each of the symbol sets and systems into only one
class, that is, static, dynamic, and mixed (or both static
Synthetic “fingerspelling” and dynamic). It would seem to be more appropriate to
choose a classification system that does not require
Braille and other static tactile any modification in order to eliminate dual-classification
codes ambiguity. Because the static/dynamic dichotomy must
Electronic vibrotactile codes be modified into a trichotomy in order to eliminate
ambiguity, it would appear to be less desirable than the
Electronic-cued speech aided/unaided classification, which requires no modifi-
cation. It seems more parsimonious to use a simple
Synthetic speech dichotomy rather than a more complex trichotomous
a
The overview of communication symbol sets and symbol systems classification at the superordinate level.
as presented on this table was developed by Lloyd and his colleagues
Clinical/Educational lmplications
(e.g., Bloomberg & Lloyd, 1986; Goossens’ & Lloyd, 1981; Karlan &
Lloyd, 1986; Lloyd, 1980a, 1980b, 1984/1985; Lloyd & Karlan, 1983, Another important metric for evaluating the suffi-
1984; Romski, Lloyd, & Sevcik, in press). These are “formal” or ciency of any classification system is the clinical and
conventionalized symbols and systems; informal nonverbal behaviors educational implications that are delineated through use
or ritualized behaviors have not been included. of that classification. Both the static/dynamic and the
aided/unaided dichotomies have implications for clinical
and educational applications; however, there is a major
for Table 2 also applicable to Table 3. However, there difference in the focus of the two approaches. As
is one significant difference in the two classification mentioned earlier, the static/dynamic dichotomy fo-
schemes. Whereas fingerspelling is considered to be cuses on the transmission characteristics of the sym-
exclusively unaided in the aided/unaided scheme, it is bols while the aided/unaided dichotomy highlights the
considered both static and dynamic in the static/dy- production of the symbol by the user (or sender). Under
170 Lloyd and Fuller

the static/dynamic classification, a symbol is classified Lloyd, 1981; Lloyd & Karlan, 1984; Nietupski & Hamre-
according to what is required to make that symbol Nietupski, 1979; Romski, Sevcik & Joyner, 1984; Van-
intelligible, whereas under an aided/unaided dichotomy, derheiden & Harris-Vanderheiden, 1976). In a subse-
the orientation is toward its use with an individual even quent paper, these potential subordinate dimensions
though it is the symbol that is being classified. Regard- will be discussed as an additional step towards devel-
less of an individual’s physical abilities, aided symbols oping a multilevel symbol taxonomy (Fuller & Lloyd,
may or may not be readily available to that individual 1986).
depending on the availability of an external device, aid, In conclusion, we propose the development of a
and/or symbol display. Unaided symbols are always symbol taxonomy using the aided/unaided dichotomy
available to an individual within the constraint of that as the superordinate level of classification. Such a
person’s own physical limitations. proposal is an initial step in developing a multilevel
symbol taxonomy. Additional research and clinical/ed-
Conclusions and Recommendations ucational insights are needed before a taxonomy can
In evaluating the current classification systems ac- be fully developed, but the use of a common terminol-
cording to internal logic, reduction of dual classification ogy at the first taxonomic level should facilitate research
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by RMIT University on 08/08/13

ambiguity, and clinical/educational implications, the and clinical/educational practice and communication.
aided/unaided and static/dynamic dichotomies appear Comments from other clinicians, educators, theorists,
to be the most practical. The real difficulty lies in trying and researchers are encouraged.
to determine which of these two classification ap-
proaches better serves as a superordinate level dichot- Acknowledgments
omy. The two dichotomies are in general agreement The authors would like to thank the members of the
regarding terminology used to classify symbols. How- Purdue Augmentative and Alternative Communication
ever, the aided/unaided scheme is less ambiguous and Research Group for their suggestions during the de-
is oriented more to the physical abilities of the user than velopment course of this paper. The preparation of this
the static/dynamic one. Therefore, we suggest that the paper was partially supported by grants from the Office
aided/unaided dichotomy be used to classify augment- of Special Education and Rehabilitation Services,
For personal use only.

ative and alternative communication symbols at the United States Education Department (GO08300868 and
superordinate level of the taxonomy. G00863079). However, the contents do not necessarily
In developing an augmentative and alternative com- represent the policy of that agency and endorsement
munication symbol taxonomy, subordinate levels of by the federal government is not to be assumed.
classification also need to be specified. It could be that
the static and dynamic dimension of symbols may be
appropriate for the first subordinate level, but with Address reprint requests to: Lyle L. Lloyd, Ph.D.,
symbols fitting into one of three classes. Although we Special Education, Purdue University, South Campus
have argued for a true dichotomy at the superordinate Courts-E, W. Lafayette, IN 47907.
level, it may be more acceptable to have a trichotomy
at lower levels of the taxonomy.
If we use aided/unaided as the first (and dichoto- REFERENCES
mous) level, and static/dynamic as the second (and
Blau, A. F. (1983). Vocabulary selection in augmentative communi-
trichotomous) level of a taxonomy, symbols could then cation: Where do we begin? In H. Winitz (Ed.), Treating language
be classified on other levels which may even be on disorders: For clinicians by clinicians (pp. 205-233). Baltimore:
continua. Symbols might be further classified along University Park Press.
dimensions such as the five listed above to describe Bloomberg, K., & Lloyd, L. L. (1986). Graphic/aided symbols and
the organization of Tables 2 and 3, and along other systems: A resource list. Communication Outlook, 7 (4), 24-30.
Carlson, F. (1981). Alternative methods of communication. Danville,
dimensions of clinical/educational relevance. The di- IL: Interstate Printers and Publishers, Inc.
mensions of a complete taxonomy might include: (a) Fristoe, M., & Lloyd, L. L. (1979). Nonspeech communication. In N.
acceptability of the symbol sets or systems, (b) com- R. Ellis (Ed.), Handbook of mental deficiency: Psychological theory
municative interaction facilitation, (c) representational and research (2nd ed.) (pp. 401-430). New York: Lawrence Erl-
baum Associates. (An expansion of the invited symposium pres-
range, (d) future expansion (e.g., logic and abstraction), entation at the 1977 Gatlinburg Conference on Research and
(e) permanency as it relates to initial learning, memory, Theory in Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities).
structure, etc., and to future record and vocational/ Fristoe, M., Lloyd, L. L., & Wilbur, R. B. (1977, November). Non-
educational uses, (f) symbol accessability, (g) reproduc- speech communication: Systems and symbols. Asha, 19, 541-
ibility of graphic symbols, (h) physical demands of man- 542. (Abstract) A Short Course presented at the 52nd Annual
Convention of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Associa-
ual symbols, (i) cognitive demands (including memory), tion, Chicago.
(j) teaching advantages (e.g., abstract symbolization, Fuller, D. R., & Lloyd, L. L. (1986). Toward an augmentative and
syntax and structure, and concepts), (k) training de- alternative communication symbol taxonomy: II. Proposed subor-
mands, and (I) costs, including both initial and ongoing. dinate classifications. Manuscript in preparation.
Goodenough-Trepagnier, C. (1981, June). Representation of language
Symbol dimensions such as these have been proposed for nonvocal communication. Paper presented at the AACP&T-
as symbol selection considerations (e.g., Carlson, NEMC Meeting on Advances in Technical Aids for Children with
1981; Goodenough-Trepagnier, 1981; Goossens’ & Physical Disabilities, Tufts University, Medford, MA.
AAC Symbol Taxonomy 171

Goossens’, C. A., & Lloyd, L. L. (1981, November). Clinical experi- of the International Association of Logopedics and Phoniatrics (Vol.
ence and research: Implications for teaching nonspeech commu- Ill). University of Edinburgh, Scotland, August, 1115-1160.
nication. Asha, 23, 697. (Abstract) Short Course presented at the Lloyd, L. L., & Karlan, G. R. (1984). Nonspeech communication
56th Annual Convention of the American Speech-Language-Hear- symbols and systems: Where have we been and where are we
ing Association, Los Angeles. going? Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 28, 3-20. (An invited
Gove, P. O. (Ed.) (1976). Webster’s third international dictionary of plenary presentation at the 6th Congress of the International As-
the English language (unabridged). Springfield, MA: G. & C. Mer- sociation for the Scientific Study of Mental Deficiency, Toronto,
riam, Co. August, 1982).
Jones, K. R., & Cregan, A. (1986). Sign and symbol communication Musselwhite, C. R., & St. Louis, K. W. (1982). Communication pro-
for mentally handicapped people. Beckenham, England: Croom gramming for the severely handicapped: Vocal and non-vocal strat-
Helm. egies. San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.
Karlan, G. R., & Lloyd, L. L. (1986). Communication intervention for Nietupski, J., & Hamre-Nietupski, S. (1979). Teaching auxilliary com-
the moderately and severely handicapped. Manuscript in prepara- munication skills to severely handicapped students. AAESPH Re-
tion. view, 4, 13-23.
Kiernan, C. (1977). Alternatives to speech: A review of research on Owens, R., & House, L. (1984). Decision-making processes in aug-
manual and other forms of communication with the mentally hand- mentative communication. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disor-
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by RMIT University on 08/08/13

icapped and other noncommunicating populations. British Journal ders, 49, 18-25.
of Mental Subnormality, 23, 6-28. Romski, M. A., Lloyd, L. L., & Sevcik, R. A. (in press). Augmentative
Kiernan, C., Reid, B., & Jones, L. (1979). Signs and symbols: Who and alternative communication issues. In R. L. Schiefelbusch & L.
uses what? Special Education: Forward Trends, 6, 32-35. L. Lloyd (Eds.), Language perspectives II. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Kiernan, C., Reid, B., & Jones, L. (1982). Signs and symbols: A review Romski, M. A., Sevcik, R. A., & Joyner, S. E. (1984). Nonspeech
of literature and survey of the use of non-vocal communication. communication systems: Implications for language intervention
London: Heinemann Educational Books. with mentally retarded children. Topics in Language Disorders, 5,
Lamh. (1984). Language alternatives for mentally handicapped (Lamh) 66-81.
manual. Dublin: LAMH (Developed by M. Clark, T. Kearns, F. K. Silverman, F. (1980). Communication for the speechless. Englewood
Meldon, D. Malocca, A. O’Connell, and M. Rafferty). Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Lloyd, L. L. (Ed.) (1976). Communication assessment and intervention Tebbs, T. (Co-ordinator) (1978). Ways and means. Houndmills, Bas-
strategies. Baltimore: University Park Press. ingstoke, Hampshire: Globe Education.
Lloyd, L. L. (1980a). Non-speech communication: Discussant’s com- Vanderheiden, G. C., & Grilley, K. (1976). Non-vocal communication
ments. In B. Urban (Ed.), Proceedings of the 18th Congress of the techniques and aids for the severely physically handicapped. Bal-
International Association of Logopedics and Phoniatrics (Vol. II). timore: University Park Press.
For personal use only.

Washington, DC: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Vanderheiden, G. C., & Harris-Vanderheiden, D. (1976). Communi-
(pp 43-48). (An invited presentation, August). cation techniques and aids for the nonverbal severely handicapped.
Lloyd, L. L. (1980b). Unaided non-speech communication for severely In L. L. Lloyd (Ed.), Communication assessment and intervention
handicapped individuals: An extensive bibliography. Education and strategies (pp. 607-652). Baltimore: University Park Press.
Training of the Mentally Retarded, 15, 15-34. Vanderheiden, G. C., & Lloyd, L. L. (1986). Nonspeech modes and
Lloyd, L. L. (1984). Comments on terminology. Communicating To- systems. In S. Blackstone (Ed.), Augmentative communication (pp.
gether, February, 2, 19-21. (Reprinted in Augmentative and Alter- 49-162), Rockville, MD: American Speech-Language-Hearing As-
native Communication, 1985, 1, 95-97). sociation.
Lloyd, L. L., & Karlan, G. R. (1983). Nonspeech communication Waksvik, K. (1985). Reader’s forum. Augmentative and Alternative
symbol selection considerations. Proceedings of the 19th Congress Communication, 1, 52-54.

You might also like