You are on page 1of 10

Cell, Vol. 83, 1233-1242.

December 29, 1995, Copyright 0 1995 by Cell Press

Impaired Motor Coordination Correlates with


Persistent Multiple Climbing Fiber Innervation
in PKCy Mutant Mice
Chong Chen, l Masanobu Kano,t Asa Abeliovich, l forming compound movementssmoothly(motor coordina-
Lu Chen,* Shaowen Baa,* Jeansok J. Kim,* tion).
Kouichi Hashimoto,t Richard F. Thompson,+ Evidence has been accumulated suggesting that the
and Susumu Tonegawa’ memory traces for discrete motor learning, such as adap-
‘Howard Hughes Medical Institute tation of vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) and classical condi-
Center for Learning and Memory tioning of eyeblink response, are stored in the cerebellum
Center for Cancer Research (for VOR, see Ito, 1984; for eyeblink, see McCormick and
and Department of Biology Thompson, 1984; Yeo et al., 1984; Lavond et al., 1985;
Massachusetts Institute of Technology Thompson, 1986; Krupa et al., 1993; Daum et al., 1993;
Cambridge, Massachusetts 02139 Molchan et al., 1994). The synaptic plasticity, long-term
fDepartment of Physiology depression (LTD) at parallel fiber (PF)-Purkinje cell (PC)
Jichi Medical School synapses, has been suggested to be a cellular substrate
Minamikawachi-machi for these forms of discrete motor learning (for VOR, see
Tochigi-ken 329-04 Itoet al., 1982; Ito, 1989,1994; foreyeblink, seeThompson
Japan et al., 1993; Chen and Thompson, 1995). Furthermore,
fNeuroscience Program LTD has been proposed to underlie motor coordination
University of Southern California (lto, 1984). However, there has been little direct evidence
Los Angeles, California 90089-2520 linking LTD to discrete motor learning or motor coordi-
nation.
Gene knockout mice could potentially be useful in as-
sessing the relationship between phenomena observed
Summary at different levels of complexity (for reviews, see Grant
and Silva, 1994; Mayford et al., 1995; Chen and Tone-
It is generally believed that a smooth execution of a gawa, 1995). For instance, mice lacking the functional
compound movement, or motor coordination, re- gene for the metabotropic glutamate receptor type 1
quires learning of component movements as well as (mGluR1) are deficient in the cerebellar LTD and are im-
experience-based refinement of the motor program as paired in eyeblink conditioning (Aiba et al., 1994; Conquet
a whole. PKCy mutant mice display impaired motor et al., 1994). These data strengthen the notion that the
coordination but intact eyeblink conditioning, a form LTD is a cellular substrate for discrete motor learning.
of component movement learning. Cerebellar long- In the present study, we have analyzed the mutant mice
term depression, a putative cellular mechanism for deficient in the y isoform of protein kinase C (PKCr). PKCr
component motor learning, is also unimpaired. Thus, is brain-specific and is highly expressed in cerebellar PCs
PKCy mutant mice are defective in refinement of the (Huang et al., 1988; Kose et al., 1988; Saito et al., 1988;
motor program. In the accompanying paper, we dem- Tanaka and Saito, 1992; for review, see Tanaka and Nishi-
onstrate that innervation of multiple climbing fibers zuka, 1994). The expression of PKQ reaches a transient
onto Purkinje cells persists in adulthood in these mu- peak in the third postnatal week (Hashimoto et al., 1988;
tant mice. We propose that this defective elimination Herms et al., 1993; Moriya and Tanaka, 1994) which cor-
of surplus climbing fibers underlies motor discoordi- responds temporally to a transition from multiple CF in-
nation. nervation to a single CF innervation to each PC (Crepel,
1982). Furthermore, PKC has been implicated in LTD
(Crepe1 and Krupa, 1988; Crepe1 and Jaillard, 1991; Lin-
Introduction den and Connor, 1991; for reviews, see Linden, 1994;
Hemart et al., 1995).
The classic neurological concept that the cerebellum is a In the accompanying paper in this issue of Cell (Kano
site of motor control originates from a number of observa- et al., 1995), we report that about 40% of PCs in mature
tions that patients with cerebellar damages often exhibit PKCr mutant mice remain multiply innervated by CFs. In
motor-related clinical symptoms such as gait ataxia, dys- this paper, we report that these mutant mice are defective
metria, hypotonia, and tremor (Dow and Moruzzi, 1958). in motor coordination but are fully capable of discrete mo-
Thach et al. (1992) suggest that the adaptive role of the tor learning, and LTD is fully inducible in the cerebellar
cerebellar cortex is to combine simpler elements of move- slices. Comparing these findings on PKCT mutant mice
ment into more complex coordinated acts. They also sug- with the phenotypes of several other gene knockout mice
gest that the cerebellum is the apparatus that allows sim- described in recently published work (Aiba et al., 1994;
ple, stereotyped reflexes to respond appropriately under Kashiwabuchi et al., 1995; K. Shibuki et al., submitted),
different task conditions. These notions imply two distinct we propose that the primary cause of the observed motor
functionsof the cerebellum: learning associated with com- discoordination in PKCy mutant mice is the persistent mul-
ponent movements (discrete motor learning), and per- tiple innervation of PCs by CFs.
Cell
1234

0123456

B
0 Wild-type (n=8)

2 3 4 5 6

Wild-type PKCv mutant


0 1 2 3 4 5 6

TrlalS Figure 2. Hind Footprint Pattern during Walking


The PKCy mutant mouse walks with a shorter pace and slight wob-
Figure 1. PKCv Mutant Mice Are Impaired in Motor Coordination bling. Wobbling becomes morevisible when the mutant mouse speeds
(A) The stationary rotorod. Wild-type mice (n = 6) were able to stay up (data not shown).
on the roller for an extended period of time (mean + SEM, 60 sallowed
maximally); although they improved considerably after three trials, mu-
tant mice (n = 6) spent significantly less time on the stationary rotorod
than did the wild type (F[1,95] = 244.9, P < 0.0001). Eyeblink Conditioning Is Facilitated
(B) Running rotorod (6 PRM). Initially, both wild-type and mutant mice
We investigated simple associative learning of discrete
fell off the running roller immediately. Wild-type mice showed a steady
improvement over trials, while mutant mice did not improve at all motor responses in PKCy mutant mice. We found that
(F[1,95] = 134.5, P < 0.0001). PKCy mutant miceshowed eyeblinkconditioning following
(C) Horizontal thin rod. Wild-type mice eventually managed to remain pairings of an initially neutral tone with a 100 ms periocular
on the rod for an extended period of time, but mutant mice failed to shock (Figure 3). The general features of the conditioned
do so (F[1,95] = 36.3, P < 0.0001).
eyeblink response resembled those previously reported
for rabbits (Gormezano et al., 1962), rats (Skelton, 1988)
Results and mice (Aiba et al., 1994). The increase of electromyo-
graph (EMG) activity following a presentation of a tone
Motor Coordination Is Impaired represents a conditioned response (CR), and the activity
PKCr mutant mice are impaired in several motor coordina- following the shock is an unconditioned response (UR)
tion tasks (Figure 1). For instance, the mutant mice had
difficulty in staying on the stationary rotorod, although
practice improved the performance (Figure 1A). Neverthe-
Wild-type PKCy Mutant
less, even after six trials, the staying duration by the mu- F”
tant mice was significantly shorter than that of the wild-type 100rnS
CS US CS US
mice. When the rotorod was running, wild-type mice
showed a steady improvement over trials, but mutant mice
Day 1 . . ..L c da
did not improve at all (Figure 16). When placed on a thin
rod, wild-type mice eventually managed to remain on the Day2 M ,..,
rod for the period of time allowed (60 s), but mutant mice
failed to do so (Figure 1C). Thus, although they retain my7 - ,..,..,,,.,..

certain limited ability to manage simple coordinated acts cs only I .


such as staying on the stationary rotorod, PKCy mutant
mice could not adapt to more challenging tasks such as
Figure 3. Examples of Discriminated EMG Activity during Associative
staying on the running rotorod or the thin rod. Eyeblink Conditioning in Wild-Type and Mutant Mice
PKCy mutant mice showed a mild ataxic gait but not Examples of eyeblink response to paired tone-shock presentations
any sign of tremor. A slight wobbling could be detected in the initial (day l), intermediate (day 2), and well-trained stages (day
by close observations, which became more visible during 7) in wild-type (left) and mutant (right) mice. The bottom traces are
running. Mutant mice typically walk faster than wild-type conditioned response to tone-alone trials (CS only). Onsets of tone
(CS) and shock (US) are indicated in the top of the figure. Discriminated
mice, and they tend to be more active. By examining the EMG activity elicited by the tone is a CR. Owing to saturation of the
footprint pattern during walking, we noticed that mutant amplifier during the US period, the increase in EMG activity upon
mice walked with a shorter pace (Figure 2). termination of the shock is recorded as a UR.
Motor Learning in PKCy Mutant
1235

70 -

60 4

8 40
2 50 :
$ 30 :

1234567 0-t
Days 0

Ftgure 6. Mutant Mice Show More Memory Saving of Conditioned


Responses during Extinction Test
After being switched to CS-alone trials, both groups demonstrate a
gradual extinction of percent CR over 4 days. But percent CR is not
yet extinguished to preconditioning baselines (see day 1 in Figure 4A).
The percent CR in the mutant group is consistently higher than in the
0 5 IO 15 20 25 30 wild type (F[1,23] = 9.30, p < 0.01).
IO-trial blocks

Figure 4. PKCy Mutant Mice Show Factlitated Eyeblink Conditroning


learning of CRs, asshown by higher percent CRs exhibited
(A) Percent CR by session. Wild-type mice (n = 12) showed gradual in mutant mice (Figure 4). This facilitation of eyeblink con-
increase in percent CR (mean f SEM) over first 4 days of training.
ditioning was most visible on the second day of training.
In contrast, mutant mice (n = 12) exhibited asymptotic CRs by day
2 of training. Percent CR in mutant mice is significantly higher than On subsequent days, mutant and wild-type mice reached
wild-type mice on the second day of training, as marked by an asterisk a similar level of conditioning. The facilitation of CRs in
(F[1,23] = 6.29, p < 0.01) while no statistically reliable differences mutant mice was also seen as larger CR amplitudesduring
were observed in other days.
the second day of training (Figure 5). As with percent CR,
(6) Percent CR by block was calculated for each lo-trial block for the
first 3 days of training. The facilitation of eyeblink conditroning in mu- the CR amplitudes of mutant and wild-type mice became
tant mice is most apparent from block 20 to block 30 (corresponding similar as the training continued. In contrast, the ampli-
to the second session in [A]). tudes of URs did not change over the course of training
and did not differ between wild-type and PKCy mutant
(Figure 3). We used two indicators for assessing learning, groups (Figure 5). Thus, the facilitation in PKQ mutant
one being percent CR and the other being CR amplitude. mice seemed to occur specifically for the CR and not for
CR was scored by statistical comparison between EMG the UR.
activity triggered by a tone and the pre-tone baseline (see The notion that the associative learning by PKCy mu-
Experimental Procedures for more details). tant mice is facilitated was also supported by the extinction
To our surprise, PKCr mutant mice showed a facilitated test (Figure 6). When switching to conditioned stimulus
(CS)-alone trials, the percent CR in the mutant group was
consistently higher than that in the wild-type group during
the course of extinction, but the rate of extinction did not
A
appear to be different between the two groups. One possi-
CO6
1 -
Pas, bility is that the facilitated learning by PKCv mutant mice
I- leads to more memory savings. Although the memory sav-
1 ‘-+:J
c;o3 ings are not shown in either percent CR or CR amplitudes
I
zo2-
during the late phase of training, possibly owing to a ceiling
$04 F Ol- -
effect of behavioral expression, savings can be seen more
00; 1 / I II, 1

01234567 clearly during extinction (Figure 6).

B LTD Is Present in PF-PC Synapses


~25 To examine LTD of PF-PC transmission, we measured
%20 excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs) with a whole-cell
recording technique of PCs in cerebellar slices from 16-
to 25-day-old mice. PFs were stimulated in the middle mo-
lecular layer, about 200 pm away from the recorded PCs.
To induce LTD, we used a conjunction protocol that is
01234567
composed of 240 PF stimuli in conjunction with a depolar-
izing pulse (200 ms, -6O-+0 mV) repeated at 1 Hz.
Figure 5. Amplitudes of CR and UR durrng Eyeblmk Condttronrng In In the wild-type PCs, the conjunction protocol induced a
Wild-Type and Mutant Mice significant decrease in the amplitude of PF-EPSCs (Figure
(A) CR amplitude. Mutant mice exhibit srgnificantly higher CR ample- 7A), which persisted for the full 35 min during which re-
tudes than wrld-type mice on the second day, as marked by an asterisk
sponses were monitored. The magnitudes of LTD mea-
(F(1.231 = 4.35, p < 0 05), but not on subsequent days.
(B) UR amplitude. On US-alonetrials, UR amplitudes, measured imme- sured during a 25-35 min period after conditioning range
diately followmg the shock, do not doffer between the two 9roups. from 59% to 95% (76.5% +- 4.3%, n = 12 from nine
Cell
1236

Normal Intracellular Solution PKC (19-36) 1OtlM Figure 7. LTD of PF-PC Synapses Appears
Normal, but the Efficacy of a Specific PKC In-
A wild type C wild type
hibitor to Block LTD Is Altered in PKCy Mutant
before alter
Mice
(A) LTD was induced by a conjunction protocol
in wild-type PCs (mean f SEM, n = 12 from
B 100
nine mice, P16-P25, five cells studied blind).
,z 90 This protocol comprises 240 PF stimuli in con-
B
5 80 junction with a depolarizing pulse (200 ms,
0 ‘0 -6O-+0 mV) repeated at 1 Hz.
2 (6) LTD was induced by the same conjunction
w 6o -10 0 10 20 30 protocol as in (A) in mutant PCs (n = 9 from
Time (min) Time (min) six mice of P17-P26, six cells studied blind).
B mutant (C) LTD was blocked in wild-type PCs (n = 12
from seven mice of P16P23, three cells stud-
ied blind), when the pipette solution contained
a specific peptide inhibitor of PKC, PKC(19-
36).
(D) LTDwas not totally abolished in mutant PCs
(n = 11 from six mice of P16P23, five cells
studied blind) with the pipette solution con-
taining PKC(19-36).
Each point represents the amplitude of the av-
eraged PF-EPSC of sixty consecutive stimuli
Time (min) Time (min) expressed as percentage of the baseline be-
fore conditioning. Sample PF-EPSC traces are
taken before and 30 min after LTD induction.
Each sample trace is the average of ten con-
secutive EPSCs.

mice, five cells studied blind). In the mutant mice, the con- during 25-35 min after the conjunctive stimulation ranged
junction protocol also induced persistent depression of from 82% to 107% (94.2% r 2.7010, n = 11 from six mice,
PF-EPSCs (Figure 7B). The magnitudes of LTD in mutant five cells studied blind), which was significantly smaller
slices, ranging from 59% to 104% (78.7% f 4.90/o, n = than that of the mutant mice with control intracellular solu-
9 from six mice, six cells studied blind) are comparable tion (compare Figure 78 with Figure 70; p < 0.05, Mann-
with those of the wild-type mice (p > 0.05, Mann-Whitney Whitney U test). Furthermore, in the presence of PKC(lS-
U test). Thus, LTD is apparently normal in the mutant mice. 36), the amplitudes of PF-EPSCs after the conjunctive
stimulation differed significantly between the wild-type and
mutant mice (compare Figure 7C with Figure 7D; p < 0.05,
Efficacy of PKC Inhibitor to Block LTD Is Modified Mann-Whitney U test). These results suggest that LTD in
The presence of LTD in the mutant mice was unexpected, the wild-type mice requires activation of PKC, while LTD
since previous studies indicated that LTD is abolished by in the mutant mice is mediated additionally by protein ki-
PKC inhibitors (Linden and Connor, 1991; Hemart et al., nases that are more resistant to PKC(19-36).
1995) and because PKCy is the major PKC isoform in The induction of LTD requires transient elevation of in-
PCs (Huang et al., 1988; Kose et al., 1988; Saito et al., tracellular Ca*+ concentration mediated by the activation
1988; Tanaka and Saito, 1992). To examine whether the of voltage-gated Ca2+ channels (Sakurai, 1990; Konnerth
LTD in the wild-type and mutant mice is PKC-dependent, et al., 1992). Hence, we tested whether Ca*+ currents in
we applied a specific peptide inhibitor of PKC, PKC(lS- PCs were normal in the mutant mice. Whole-cell mem-
36) (10 mM), intracellularly through the recording patch brane currents in response to voltage steps were recorded
pipette. In the presence of the inhibitor, the conjunction from wild-type and mutant PCs in cerebellar slices from
protocol was ineffective in inducing depression of PF- young (5- to 6-day-old) mice. Young mice were used be-
EPSCs in the wild-type mice (Figure 7C). In fact, PF- cause PCs at this age are almost devoid of dendrites;
EPSCs were slightly potentiated after conditioning. Ampli- therefore, a good voltage control of the cell membrane
tudes of PF-EPSCs measured during 25-35 min after can be achieved. We found that voltage-gated Ca*+ cur-
conditioning ranged from 77% to 147% (107.8% + 6.00/o, rents were similar in their kinetics and amplitudes in wild-
n = 12 from seven mice, three cells studied blind), which type and mutant PCs (Figures 8A and 8B), indicating that
was significantly different from the values obtained in the these currents are normal in the mutant PCs. Moreover,
wild-type mice with control intracellular solution (compare intracellularly applied PKC(19-36) had no effect on the
Figure 7A with Figure 7C, p < 0.001, Mann-Whitney U Ca2+ currents in either group of PCs (Figures 8C and 8D).
test). Thus, blockade of LTD by PKC(19-36) in the wild-type
In contrast with wild-type slices, the application of mice (Figure 8C) was not due to suppression of Ca*+ influx
PKC(19-36) did not completely abolish LTD of PF-EPSCs through voltage-gated Ca2+ channels, but due to interfer-
in mutant slices (Figure 70). The amplitudes of PF-EPSCs ing with subsequent cellular processes.
Motor Learning in PKCy Mutant
1237

Normal Intracellular Solution onto PCs persists into the adulthood in PKCy mutant mice
(Kano et al., 1995). Thus, our data suggest that an animal
has a problem in conducing compound movement
L
smoothly unless monoinnervation by CFs onto PCs is es-
tablished, even if it is capable of learning component
movements.
?
x
* Role of PKCy in LTD Induction
Previous results indicated that LTD induction requires acti-
-2 vation of PKC: LTD is blocked by inhibitors of PKC (Linden
C wild type
and Connor, 1991; Hemart et al., 1995) and activation of
. mUrant
ii PKC by phorbol esters induced depression of synaptic
IOms Ol (riA)
transmission similar to LTD (Crepe1 and Krupa, 1988;
PKC (19-36) 1Od Crepe1 and Jaillard, 1991; Linden and Connor, 1991). How-
ever, the present results show that the LTD induction is
C wild type D VWi
-40 ~20 0 20 40 intact in PKCr mutant mice. This was unexpected, since
'+ -
PKCy is the major isoform in PCs (Huang et al., 1988;
0
Kose et al., 1988; Saito et al., 1988; Tanaka and Saito,
1992). Our study with a PKC inhibitor peptide, PKC(lS-
36) revealed that the intracellular mechanisms for LTD
are modified in PKCr mutant mice. PKC(19-361 totally
abolished LTD in the wild-type mice, as reported pre-
viously (Linden and Connor, 1991; Hemart et al., 1995).
wild type
I:
In contrast, the same inhibitor decreased the magnitude
’ !
3 . mutant
of, but did not abolish, LTD in the mutant mice. This sug-
I (ilAj
gests that LTD in PKCy mutant mice is mediated by protein
Figure 8. Voltage-Gated Ca2’ Currents Are Normal in the Mutant PCs, kinases that are less sensitive to PKC(19-36). The S iso-
and They Are Not Affected by the Inhibitor PKC(19-36) form of PKC (PKC6) has been reported to be present in
(A) Whole-cell membrane currents elicited with 20 ms voltage steps the cell bodies, dendrites, and axons of PCs in the ventral
from a holding potential of -60 mV to various test potentials of -40
foliaof the cerebellum (Chen and Hillman, 1993; Merchen-
mV, 0 mV, +lO mV, +20 mV, and +30 mV. Slices were obtained from
a wild-type (P5) and a mutant (P6) mouse. thaler et al., 1993). It is, therefore, possible that the lack
(B) Current-voltage relations for average peak Ca2’ currents (mean of PKCy is compensated by some other kinases in the
r SEM) in wild-type (open circles, n = 10, four mice) and mutant mutant mice, although this isoform is used for LTD induc-
(closed circles, n = 14. three mice) PCs. tion in the wild-type mice.
(C and D) Voltage-gated Ca2+ currents were not affected by PKC(lS-
36). The pipette solution contained PKC(19-36) (10 mM). Sample re-
cords in (C) were obtained from a wild-type (P5) and a mutant (P4)
mouse. Number of PCs in (D): wild-type, n = 8, three mice; mutant, Impaired Motor Coordination Correlates with
n = 6. two mice. Persistent Multiple CF Innervation
Our behavioral study indicates that PKCr mutant mice can
perform classical eyeblink conditioning, but are impaired
Discussion in motor coordination. This suggests that PKCr mutant
mice can acquire the memory for elements of movement,
Our behavioral study indicated that PKCr mutant mice are but cannot properly combine the elements spatially and
impaired in motor coordination, but are capable of discrete temporally to achieve complex coordinated acts. Thus,
motor learning. Our electrophysiological study demon- PKCy mutant mice seem to provide a unique system in
strated that in the cerebella of these mutant mice, voltage- which the capabilities for discrete motor learning and mo-
gated Ca” channels function normally and that LTD is tor coordination can be dissociated.
induced by the standard procedure. The accompanying To understand the underlying mechanisms, we com-
paper demonstrates that the innervation of multiple CFs pared the results from PKCy mutant mice with those from

Table 1, Summary of Motor Symptoms and Cerebellar Synaptic Abnormalities

Mutant Mice Motor Coordination Eyeblink Conditioning CF Innervation LTD Reference

PKCy Impaired Facilitated Multiple Normal This paper


Kano et al., 1995
mGluR1 lmparred impaired Multiple Impaired Aiba et al., 1994
Conquet et al., 1994
Unpublished data
GIuRS2 Impaired Not tested Multiple Impaired Kashiwabuchi et al., 1995
GFAP Normal Impaired Normal Impaired Shibuki et al., submitted
Cell
1238

three other gene knockout mice that have recently been ticity at PF-PC synapses plays a role in discrete motor
studied and found to exhibit an impairment in either or learning has also been incorporated into the so-called re-
both forms of motor behaviors (Table 1). These are mice current network model of the VOR by Lisberger and Sej-
deficient in mGluR1 (Aiba et al., 1994) the glutamate re- nowski (1992) although in this model contribution of syn-
ceptor 6 subunit (GluRS2) (Kashiwabuchi et al., 1995) and aptic plasticity in the brainstem was also considered.
the glial fibrillary acidic protein (GFAP), an intermediate Thompson and his colleagues found a class of PCs dis-
filament protein highly expressed in cerebellar Bergmann playing a decrease in the rate of simple spike discharge
glia (K. Shibuki et al., submitted). in the animal undergoing eyeblink conditioning (see
In the PKCv mutant mice, motor coordination is im- Thompson and Krupa, 1994). They suggested that this
paired, and multiple CF innervation persists abnormally decrease in discharge results from LTD induced at the
into adulthood. On the other hand, LTD is inducible by the PF-PC synapses and that this LTD is a cellular substrate
standard protocol, and the animals are capable of condi- for the conditioning (Thompson et al., 1993; Chen and
tioned eyeblink responses. Interestingly, mutant mice Thompson, 1995). The correlation observed among three
lacking GFAP exhibit an opposite phenotype. They show gene knockout mice (mGluR1, GFAP, and PKCy; see Ta-
no sign of impairment either in motor coordination or in ble 1) between an absence or presence of LTD and an
the elimination of surplus CFs. In contrast, LTD is deficient impairment or intactness of eyeblinkconditioning supports
and eyeblinkconditioning is impaired in these mutant mice the notion thatLTD is a cellular substrate for discrete motor
(K. Shibuki et al., submitted). learning.
Mice deficient in mGluR1 exhibit impairments in both Why is eyeblink conditioning facilitated in PKCy mutant
motor‘coordination and eyeblink conditioning, as well as mice that exhibit apparently normal LTD? One possibility
a deficiency in cerebellar LTD (Aiba et al., 1994; Conquet is that the multiple CF innervation results in a higher than
et al., 1994). In addition, our recent study revealed that normal number of impulses for PCs. Under current-clamp
adult mGluR1 mutant mice, like PKCy mutant mice, retain recording conditions, we observed that each CF input of
multiple CF innervation (25% of 51 recorded PCs show multiply innervated PCs was able to generate multiple
multiple CF innervation; our unpublished data). Recently, spikes that resemble complex spikes (data not shown).
some characteristics of another strain of mice deficient During the acquisition of CRs, a higher number of complex
for GluRS2 have been reported. These mice also exhibit spikes may be elicited in response to an unconditioned
motor discoordination, lack developmental elimination of stimulus (US) in PKCy mutant PCs than in wild-type PCs,
multiple CF innervation (46% of 85 recorded PCs show and consequently, LTD induction may be facilitated in
multiple CF innervation in 4- to g-week-old mice), and have PKCr mutant mice compared with that in the wild-type
impaired LTD (Kashiwabuchi et al., 1995). However, their mice (Figure 9A). This may lead to a faster acquisition of
capability in learning a discrete motor response (such as CRs. The validity of this hypothesis could be tested by
eyeblink conditioning) has not yet been studied. monitoring complex spikes in PCs that occur during eye-
Thus, physiological deficits and behavioral conse- blink conditioning.
quences observed in mutant mice with deficiency in
GFAP, mGluR1, or GluRS2 are all consistent with the no- How Does Multiple CF Innervation Lead
tion that monoinnervation of PCs by CFs is required for to Motor Discoordination?
motor coordination. However, it is possible that motor dis- The cerebellar cortex is composed of numerous distinct
coordination exhibited in these mutant mice is caused by compartments, known as microzones (Oscarsson, 1979;
an impairment in discrete motor learning and its putative Ito, 1984). PCs in each microzone send efferents to a small
underlying mechanism, e.g., LTD. To argue forcefully for group of subcortical nuclear neurons, which in turn project
the critical role of mono-CF innervation in motor coordina- to a certain subset of motor neurons. PCs in each micro-
tion, it is important to show that the same mutant strain zone receive common excitatory inputs from CFs that em-
that exhibits motor discoordination and persistent multiple anate, in normal adult animals, from a distinct subgroup
CF innervation is capable of discrete motor learning. of olivary neurons in the brainstem. The cerebellar micro-
Among the four genetically engineered mice listed in Table zones and associated structures are thought to be the
1, the PKCr strain is the only one that shows the appro- functional units that control component movements.
priate phenotype. A compound movement requires a program that will co-
ordinate many component movements in an orderly fash-
LTD Is Probably a Cellular Substrate for Discrete ion (Ito, 1984; Thach et al., 1992). It is generally believed
Motor Learning that the basic program for a compound movement, such
In the past, several groups studied the relevance of LTD as walking, resides in the cerebral cortex (Allen and Tsuka-
in discrete motor learning. For instance, Ito and others hara, 1974; Fetz, 1993). It is also generally believed that
found that during the adaptation of the VOR, simple spike such a basic cerebral program has to be modified or re-
responsiveness of floccular PCs to head rotation is pro- fined if the animal is to perform the movement smoothly
gressively reduced (e.g., Dufosse et al., 1978; Watanabe, or skillfully (ho, 1984; Thach et al., 1992). It is likely that the
1984; Nagao, 1991). They proposed that this reduction is a cerebellum is involved in the refinement process, because
consequence of LTD at the PF-PC synapses, and hence, animals with cerebellar lesions can generally perform
LTD is the key underlying cellular mechanism for this dis- compound movements but only imprecisely and unsurely
crete motor learning (Ito, 1989,1994). The notion that plas- (Dow and Moruzzi, 1958). However, neuroscientists differ
Motor Learning in PKCy Mutant
1239

(Figure 9B). Thus, the coherence of neural activity in each


a W!ld-type animal b PKCy mutant cerebellar functional unit is disrupted, and the motor coor-
dination is impaired.
Marr, Albus, and Ito argue that CFs and PCs participate
more actively in the refinement of the basic motor program
for a compound movement (Marr, 1969; Albus, 1971; Ito,
1984). CFs appear to transmit error signals to PCs that
are detected by inferior olivary neurons as the difference
between the intended and executed movements (Gellman
et al., 1985; Thach et al., 1992). The error signal transmit-
ted by a CF to a PC will induce LTD in the PF-PC synapses
(lto, 1989). This results in a suppression of the neural activ-
a ity in the neural circuit responsible for the erroneous move-
a Wild-type animal b PKCy mutant ment. In an adult wild-type animal, the error correction can
be accomplished with an exquisite specificity by use of the
individual cerebellar functional units that control distinct
component movements. For this, the single innervation is
an essential aspect of the error correction mechanism. In
Cllmblng fibers animals with multiple CF innervation, the organization of
this cerebellar functional unit is disrupted. PCs of a micro-
zone receive not only the proper error signal but also other
signals via the surplus CFs (Figure 9B). This means that
the strength of PF-PC synapses in certain microzones
Figure 9. A Model for the Role of CFs in Discrete Motor Learning and would be inappropriately modified by wrong error signals.
Motor Coordination
As aconsequence, thecerebellum cannot properly control
(A) Discrete motor learning. (a), CFs determine which set of PF-PC compound movements.
synapses should be modified and how much the synaptic strength
If LTD is a part of the cellular mechanism that controls
should be. The resulting LTD at PF-PC synapses (closed triangles
symbolizing LTD) presumably releases neurons in the deep cerebellar a component movement, why do GFAP mutant mice that
nuclei from inhibitory outputs of PCs. (b). multiple CF innervation in lack LTD show apparently normal motor coordination (Ta-
PKCy mutant mice would send a larger number of CF inputs to PCs, ble 1; K. Shibuki et al., submitted)? One possibility is that
making LTD more inducible than in the wild-type mice (larger closed
the employed behavioral tests for motor coordination are
triangles). Thus, eyeblink conditioning in the mutant mice is facilitated.
(6) Motor coordination. (a), olivary neurons are organized into various not sensitive enough to detect subtle impairments. An-
subgroups (closed versus open circles). In the wild-type cerebellum, other possibility is that the LTD at the PF-PC synapses
each PC is innervated by only one CF and thus can only be driven is not the only synaptic plasticity utilized for the refinement
by one subgroup of olivary neurons at a given time in a given move- of the basic motor program. There may be another cellular
ment. Each functional unit of PCs (closed versus open circles) presum-
ably corresponds to each component movement in a coordinated act.
substrate that may be sufficient for the smooth operation
(b), in PKCy mutant mice, certain PCs are innervated by multiple CFs. of the tested behavioral tasks.
These PCs would be driven by mixed subgroups of olivary neurons Thus, our data are compatible with either one of the
(striped circles). Thus, multiple CF innervation could disrupt either the currently available hypotheses on the neural mechanism
delivary of the refined motor program or the error correction mecha-
for motor coordination. What is new about the present
nism through LTD induction, eventually leading to impaired motor coor-
dlnatlon in the mutant mice. study, particularly when combined with the recent studies
on the three other mutant mice (i.e., mGluR1, GIuRS2,
and GFAP; see Table l), is that it provides an attractive
in their views as to how the cerebellum and its associated hypothesis for the physiological role of the developmental
structures accomplish the program refinement. Llinas and elimination of the surplus CFs: monoinnervation of PCs
his colleagues argue that the refinement instruction for by CFs is needed for motor coordination.
coordinated movements is generated in the inferior olive
bydynamicsynchronization of neuronal subgroups(Llinas Experimental Procedures

and Yarom, 1981, 1986; Llinas and Welsh, 1993; Welsh Production of PKCy Mutant Mice
et al., 1995). According to this idea, the role of CFs is to PKCy mutant mice were produced as described previously (Abellovich
transmit signals for a refined program to PCs. In adult et al., 1993). Wild-type as well as mutant mice utilized were of 1291
wild-type mice with monoinnervation of PCs by CFs, each Sv x C57BU6 genetic background and were kept in the same room
at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology animal facility with a 12
PC presumably follows the direction of one subgroup of hr light-dark cycle. Mice had ad libitum access to food and water
synchronous olivary neurons at a given time in a given except durmg the experiment that was conducted during the light
movement. By contrast, in either adult PKCy, mGluR1, or phase of the cycle All experiments were conducted blindly with re-
GluR62 mutant mice or wild-type mice younger than 3 spect to genotype of animals
weeks, certain PCs with multiple CF innervation would
Tests for Motor Coordination
receive inputs from olivary neurons of two or more different The rotorod consists of a smooth plastic roller (6 cm diameter. 14 cm
subgroups. This makes it difficult for such PCs to respond long) flanked by two large round plates (30 cm diameter) to prevent
in synchrony with a particular subpopulation of olivary ceils animals from escaping. A mouse was placed on the roller, and the
Cell
1240

time remained on the stationary or rotating roller was measured. A uously perfused with the oxygenated standard saline. Recognition of
maximal 60 s was allowed for each animal for all motor skill tests. The layers within the cerebellar cortex and identification of PCs were easily
thin rod consists of a smooth plastic rod (1.5 cm diameter, 50 cm long) achieved on slices when they were viewed with a 40 x water immersion
held horizontally on both ends. A mouse was placed in the midpoint objectiveattached toaZeissupright microscope(Axioscope)(Edwards
of the rod, and the time remained on the rod was measured. et al., 1989; Llano et al., 1991). All experiments were performed with
Footprints were made with waterproof red ink and watercolor paper. whole-cell configuration of the patch-clamp technique with borosiii-
Ink was applied to the hind paws of individual mice. Mice were induced cate pipettes (resistance of 3-8 MD when filled with an intracellular
to walk forward in a narrow alley (IO cm wide, 15 cm high). Three solution; see below). Ionic currents were recorded with an EPC-9
wild-type and three mutant mice were used in footprinting. patch-clamp amplifier (HEKA) and stored on a digital audiotape (DAT)
data recorder (Sony, PC204, Japan) for later analysis. The pipette
Eyeblink Conditioning and Analysis access resistance was compensated as described by Llano et al.
We utilized theeyeblinkconditioning techniquesdeveloped by Skelton (1991). Data acquisition and analysis were performed by use of the
(1988) for rats (also Stanton et al., 1992) and adapted by Aiba et al. PULSE program on a Macintosh computer (version 7.5, HEKA). The
(1994) for mice, with minor modifications. Under ketamine (80 mglkg signals were filtered at 3 kHz and digitized at 20 kHz. For stimulation
intraperitoneal) and xylazine (20 mglkg, intraperitoneal) anesthesia, of CFs and PFs. a glass pipette with 5-10 frrn tip diameter filled with
animals were implanted with four Teflon-coated stainless steel wires standard saline was used. Square pulses (duration, 0.1 ms; strength,
(bare, 0.003 in; coated, 0.0045 in; A-M Systems, Incorporated, Everett, O-100 V for CF stimulation, l-10 V for PF stimulation) were applied
Washington) subcutaneously to their left upper eyeblink. Two of the for focal stimulation.
wires were used to record differential EMG from the obicularis oculi, To induce LTD, we used a conjunction protocol that is composed
and the other two wires were used to deliver periorbital shock. A four- of 240 PF stimuli in conjunction with a depolarizing pulse (200 ms,
pin strip connector to which the wires were soldered was cemented -6O-+0 mV) repeated at 1 Hz. Conjunctive stimulation was applied
to the skull of the animal with dental acrylic. after stable recording of PF-EPSCs for 10 min. Passive membrane
Training took place in a small cylindrical Plexiglas container (diame- properties of PCs were monitored by applying 5 mV hyperpolarizing
ter, 3.75 in) that was placed inside a sound- and light-attenuating cham- pulses. Series resistance was monitored intermittently every 3 min
ber (Ralph Gerbrands Company, Arlington, MA). The strip connector and was kept constant around 12-20 MQ.
was attached to a mating plug of a commutator that has channels to The composition of standard saline was as follows: 125 mM NaCI,
relay EMG signal and to deliver shock. After 1 day of habituation, 2.5 mM KCI, 2 mM CaCI,, 1 mM MgSOI, 1.25 mM NaH2P04, 26 mM
animals underwent 7 consecutive days of paired training. The daily NaHC03, and 20 mM glucose, which was bubbled continuously with
paired training consisted of 100 trials grouped in ten blocks. Each a mixture of 95% O2 and 5% CO?. Bicuculline (10 pM) was always
block included one CS-alone (the first trial), one US-alone (the sixth present in the saline to block spontaneous inhibitory postsynaptic cur-
trial), and eight CS-US paired trials. The CS and US were a 352 ms rents (Konnerth et al., 1992; Kano and Konnerth, 1992). For measuring
tone (1000 Hz, 80 dB. 5 ms rise/fall time) and a coterminating 100 ms voltage-gated Ca*+ currents, tetrodotoxin (TTX, 0.5 mM) and tetraethyl-
shock (1.5 mA, 100 Hz biphasic square pulses), respectively, with a ammonium (TEA, 75 mM) were included to block Na+ and K+ currents,
252 ms interstimulus interval and pseudo-randomized inter-trial interval and the concentration of NaCl was reduced to 50 mM to keep the
between 20 and 40 s (mean, 30 s). Following paired trials, animals osmoiarity constant. The standard pipette solution contained 30 mM
received 4 consecutive days of CS-alone extinction trials. KCl, 90 mM K D-glUCOnate, 4 mM MgCiI, 4 mM Na-ATP, 0.4 mM Na-
For CR analysis, the EMG signal was amplified in the band of 300- GTP, and 50 mM HEPES (pH 7.3, adjusted with KOH). in experiments
5000 Hz and sent to a window discriminator, where the number of for measuring voltage-gated Ca2+ currents and CF EPSCs, a Cs-based
pulses above the noise envelope was sampled and stored by a com- pipette solution was used: 60 mM CsCI, 30 mM Cs D-glUCOnate, 20
puter into 189 consecutive 4 ms bins. The discriminated EMG activity mM TEA-Cl, 20 mM BAPTA, 4 mM MgCI>, 4 mM ATP, and 30 mM
was analyzed trial by trial according to the following criteria. First, the HEPES (pH 7.3, adjusted with CsOH). All experiments were carried
trial was considered bad and excluded from analysis if, during the 252 out at a bath temperature of 32OC, except those for voltage-gated Ca2+
ms pre-CS period, the average unit count per bin was higher than 2 currents, which were performed at room temperature.
(reflecting high EMG activity prior to the CS onset), or the standard
deviation (SD) of unit counts per bin was larger than the average unit Acknowledgments
count per bin (reflecting unstable EMG activity prior to the CS onset),
or the average unit count within 28 ms (7 bins) after the CS onset was We thank K. Poss for reading the manuscript. The cover image was
bigger than the average plus the SD of the pre-CS period (reflecting prepared by C. C. and Dong Feng Chen. This work was supported
short latency startle response to the CS). Second, the CR was defined by the Howard Hughes Medical Institute (S. T. and C. C.), National
as average unit count of any consecutive 7 bins during the period from Institutes of Health grant ROl NS32925 (S. T.), the Japanese Ministry
84 ms after tone onset to the onset of the shock (total of 168 ms) that of Education, Science and Culture (05454677, 06253216, 06260237)
was higher than average plus SD plus 1 unit per bin of pre-CS period. (M. K.), the Uehara Foundation (M. K.), the Ciba-Geigy Foundation
A constant value of 1 unit per bin was included as a CR threshold. for the Promotion of Science (M. K.), the Kato Memorial Bioscience
This value is comparable to the CR threshold value (0.5 mm deflection Foundation (M. K.), National Institutes of Health grant NRSA
in the eyeblinklnictitating membrane response) used in the rabbit eye- lF32MN10521-01 BNR (J. J. K.), National Science Foundation grant
blink conditioning paradigm. On tone-alone trials, the period for scor- IBN9215069 (R. F. T.), National Institutesof Health grant NIAAF05142
ing a CR was extended to the termination of the tone (total of 268 ms). (R. F. T.), and Sankyo (R. F. T.).
The UR amplitudes were determined from US-alone trials. Because
the US (shock) saturates the EMG activity and because the shock Received October 12, 1995; revised November 19. 1995.
offset is not instantaneous (owing to capacitance), we analyzed the unit
activity during the 100 ms (25 bins) period 50 ms after the termination of References
the US. The UR amplitude was defined as the difference in averaged
unit activity during the UR period and averaged unit activity 100 ms Abeliovich, A., Chen, C., Goda, Y., Silva, A.J., Stevens, C.F., and
before the US onset. Tonegawa, S. (1993). Modified hippocampal long-term potentiation in
PKCy mutant mice. Cell 75, 1253-1262.
Aiba, A., Kano, M., Chen, C., Stanton, M.E., Fox, G.D., Herrup, K.,
Electrophysiology
Zwingman, T.A., and Tonegawa, S. (1994). Deficient cerebellar long-
Cerebellar slices were prepared from the wild-type and mutant mice
term depression and impaired motor learning in mGluR1 mutant mice.
asdescribed previously(Edwardset al., 1989; Llano et al., 1991; Kano
Cell 79, 377-368.
and Konnerth, 1992; Aiba et al., 1994). Either sagittal or transverse
cerebellar slices of 200-300 urn thickness were cut with a vibroslicer Albus, J.S. (1971). A theory of cerebellar function. Math. Biosci. 70,
and kept at 32°C for at least 1 hr in a chamber containing standard 25-61.
saline (see below) that was bubbled with 95% O2 and 5% CO,. One Allen, G.I., and Tsukahara, N. (1974). Cerebrocerebellar communica-
slice was then transferred to a recording chamber, where it was contin- tion systems. Physiol. Rev. 54, 957-1006.
?2c$r Learnrng in PKCy Mutant

Chen, C., and Thompson, R.T. (1995). Temporal specificity of long- and R. Grantyn, eds. (Wiley-Liss), pp. 54-57.
term depression in parallel fiber-Purkinje synapses in rat cerebellar Kano. M., Hashimoto, K., Chen, C., Abeliovich, A., Aiba, A., Kurihara.
slice. Learning Memory 2, 185-198. H., Watanabe, M., moue, Y., and Tonegawa, S. (1995). Impaired syn-
Chen, C., and Tonegawa, C. (1995). Analysis of synaptic plasticity apse elimination during cerebellar development in PKCy mutant mice.
and memory with gene knockout technology. In LTP: A Debate of Cell 83, this issue.
Current Issues, Part Ill, M. Baudry and J. Davis, eds. (Cambridge, Kashiwabuchi, N., Ikeda, K., Araki, K., Hirano, T.. Shibuki, K., Taka-
Massachusetts: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press), in yama. C., Inoue, Y., Kutsuwada, T., Yagi. T., Kang, Y., Aizawa, S..
press. and Mishina, M. (1995). Impairment of motor coordination, Purkinje
Chen, S., and Hillman, D.E. (1993). Compartmentation of the cerebel- cell synapse formation, and cerebellar long-term depression in GluR62
lar cortex by protein kinase C6. Neuroscience 56, 177-188. mutant mice. Cell 87, 245-252.
Conquet, F., Bashir, Z.I., Davies, C.H., Daniel, H., Ferraguti, F., Bordi. Konnerth, A., Dreessen, J., and Augustin. E.G.J. (1992). Briefdendntic
F., Franz-Bacon, K., Reggiani, A., Matarese, V., Conde. F., Colling- calcium signals initiate long-lasting synaptic depression in cerebellar
ridge, G.L., and Crepel, F. (1994). Motor deficit and impairment of Purkinje cells. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 89, 7051-7055.
synaptic plasticity in mice lacking mGluR1. Nature 372, 237-243. Kose, A., Saito, N., Ito, H., Kikkawa, U., Nishizuka, Y., and Tanaka,
Crepel, F. (1982). Regression of functional synapses in the immature C. (1988). Electron microscopic localization of type I protein kinase C
mammalian cerebellum. Trends Neurosci. 5, 266-269. in rat Purkinje cells. J. Neurosci. 8. 4262-4268.
Crepel, F., and Jaillard, D. (1991). Pairing of pre- and postsynapbc Krupa, D.J..Thompson, J.K., and Thompson, R.F. (1993). Localization
activities in cerebellar Purkinje cells induces long-term changes in of a memory trace in the mammalian brain. Science 260, 989-991.
synaptic efficacy: an in vitro study. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 432, 123-141. Lavond, D.G., Hembree, T.L., and Thompson, R.F. (1985). Effects of
Crepel, F., and Krupa, M. (1988). Activation of protein kinase C induces kainic acid lesions of the cerebellar interpositus nucleus on eyeblink
a long-term depression of glutamate sensitivity of cerebellar Purkinje conditioning in the rabbit. Brain Res. 326, 179-182.
cells: an in vitro study. Brain Res. 458, 397-401. Linden, D.J. (1994). Long-term synaptic depression in the mammalian
Daum, I., Schugens. N.M., Ackermann, H., Lutzenberger, W., Dich- brarn. Neuron 72, 457-472.
gans, J.. and Birbaumer, N. (1993). Classical conditioning after cere- Linden, D.J., and Connor. J.A. (1991). Partrcipation of postsynaptic
bellar lesions in human. Behav. Neurosci. 707, 748-756. PKC in cerebellar long-term depression in culture. Science 254,1656-
Dow, R.S., and Moruzzi, G. (1958). The physiology and pathology of 1659.
the cerebellum (Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press). Lisberger. S.G., and Sejnowskt, T.J. (1992). Motor learning in a recur-
Dufosse, M., Ito, M., Jastrebott, P.I., and Miyashita, Y. (1978). A neu- rent network model based on the vestibule-ocular reflex. Nature 360,
ronal correlate in rabbit’s cerebellum to adaptive modification of the 159-161.
vestibule-ocular reflex. Brain Res. 150, 611-616. Llano. I., Marty, A., Armstrong, C.M., and Konnerth, A. (1991). Synap-
Edwards, F.A., Konnerth, A., Sakmann. B., and Takahashi, T. (1989). tic- and agonist-induced excitatory currents of Purkinje cells in rat
A thin slice preparation for patch-clamp recordings from neurons of cerebellar slices. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 434, 183-213.
the mammalian central nervous system. Pfliigers Arch. 414,600-612. Llinas, A., and Welsh, J.P. (1993). On the cerebellum and motor learn-
Gellman. R.S., Gibson, A.R., and Houk. J.C. (1985). Inferior olivary ing. Curr. Biol. 3, 958-965.
neurons in the awake cat: detection of contact and passive body dis- Llinas, R., and Yarom, Y. (1981). Properties and distribution of ionic
placement. J. Neurophysiol. 54, 40-60. conductances generating electroresponsiveness of mammalian infe-
Gormezano, I., Schneiderman, N., Deaux, E.G.. and Fuentes, J. rior olivary neurons in vitro. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 375, 569-584.
(1962). Nictitating membrane classical conditioning and extinction in Llinas. R.. and Yarom. Y. (1986). Oscillatory properties of guinea-pig
the albino rabbit. Science 738, 33-34. inferior olivary neurones and their pharmacological modulation: an in
Grant, S.G.N., and Silva, A.J. (1994). Targeting learning. Trends Neu- vitro study. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 376, 163-182.
rosci. 17, 71-75. Marr, D. (1969). A theory of cerebellar cortex. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 202,
Fetz, E.E. (1993). Cortical mechanisms controlling limb movement. 437-470.
Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 3, 932-939. Mayford. M., Abel, T., and Kandel, E.R. (1995). Transgenic ap-
Hashimoto, T., Ase, K., Sawamura, S., Kikkawa, U., Saito. N.. Tanaka, proaches to cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 5, 141-148.
C., and Nishizuka, Y. (1988). Postnatal development of a brain-specific McCormick, D.A., and Thompson, R.F. (1984). Cerebellum: essential
subspecies of protein kinase C in rat. J. Neurosci. 8, 1878-1683. involvement in the classical conditioned eyelid response. Science 223,
Hemart, N.. Daniel, H., Jaillard. D., and Crepel, F. (1995). Receptors 296-299.
and second messengers involved in long-term depression in rat cere- Merchenthaler. I., Liposits, 2, Reid, J.J.. and Wetsel, WC. (1993).
bellar slices in vitro: a reappraisal. Eur. J. Neurosci. 7, 45-53. Light and electron microscopic immunocytochemical localization of
Herms, J., Zurmohle. U., Schlingensiepen, K.-H., and Brysch, W. PKCG immunoreactivity in the rat central nervous system. J. Comp
(1993). Transient expression of PKCy mRNA in cerebellar granule Neurol. 336. 378-399.
cells during rat brain development. NeuroReport 4, 899-902. Molchan, S.E., Sunderland, T., McIntosh, A.R., Herscovitch. P.. and
Huang. F.L.. Yoshida, Y., Nakabayashi, H.. Young, W.S.. and Huang, Schreurs, B.G. (1994). A functional anatomical study of associative
K.-P. (1988). lmmunocytochemical localization of protein kinase C iso- learning in human. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 91, 8122-8126.
zymes In rat brain. J. Neurosci. 8, 4734-4744. Moriya, M., and Tanaka, S. (1994). Prominent expression of protein
Ito, M. (1984). The Cerebellum and Neural Control (New York: Raven kinase C (u) mRNA in the dendrite-rich neuropil of mice cerebellum
Press). at the critical period for synaptogenesis. NeuroReport 5. 929-932.
Ito, M. (1989). Long-term depression. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 85- Nagao, S. (1989). Behavior of floccular Purkinje cells correlated with
102. adaptation of vestibule-ocular reflex in pigmented rabbits. Exp. Brain
lto. M. (1994). Is long-term depression associated with learning in the Res. 77, 531-540.
cerebellum? In Cellular and Molecular Mechanisms Underlying Higher Oscarsson, 0. (1979). Functional units of the cerebellum-sagittal
Neural Functions, A.I. Selverston and P. Ascher, eds. (New York: John zones and microzones. Trends Neurosci. 2, 143-145.
Wiley and Sons, Limited), pp. 25-40. Saito. N., Kikkawa, U.. Nishizuka, Y., and Tanaka, C. (1988). Distribu-
ho, M., Sakurai, M., andTongroach, P. (1982). Climbingfibre induced tion of protein kinase C-like immunoreactive neurons in rat brain. J
depression of both mossy ftbre responsiveness and glutamate sensitiv- Neurosci. 8, 369-382.
ity of cerebellar Purkinje cells. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 324, 113-134. Sakurai, M. (1990). Calcium in an intracellular mediator of the climbmg
Kano, M., and Konnerth, A. (1992). Cerebellar skces for patch clamp fiber in induction of cerebellar long-term depression. Proc. Natl. Acad.
recording. In Practical Electrophysrological Methods, H. Kettenmann Sci. 87, 8383-8385.
Cell
1242

Sk&on, R.W. (1988). Bilateral cerebellar lesions disrupt conditioned


eyelid responses in unrestrained rats. Behav. Neurosci. 702, 586.
Stanton, M.E., Freeman, J.H., Jr., and Skelton, R.W. (1992). Eyeblink
conditioning in the developing rat. Behav. Neurosci. 706, 657-665.
Tanaka, C., and Nishizuka, Y. (1994). The protein kinase C family for
neuronal signaling. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 77, 551-567.
Tanaka, C., and Saito, N. (1992). Localization of subspecies of protein
kinase C in the mammalian central nervous system. Neurochem. Int.
27,499~512.
Thach. W.T., Goodkin, H.G., and Keating. J.G. (1992). The cerebellum
and the adaptive coordination of movement. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 75,
403-442.
Thompson, R.F. (1986). The neurobiology of learning and memory.
Science 233, 941-947.
Thompson, R.F., and Krupa, D.J. (1994). Organization of memory
traces in the mammalian brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 77, 519-549.
Thompson, R.F., Thompson, J.K., Kim, J.J.. Krupa, D.J., Norholm,
A.F., and Chen, C. (1993). Synaptic plasticity and memory storage.
In Synaptic Plasticity: Molecular, Cellular, and Functional Aspects, M.
Baudry, R.F. Thompson, and J.L. Davis, eds. (Cambridge, Massachu-
setts: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press), pp. 230-259.
Watanabe, E. (1984). Neuronal events correlated with long-term adap-
tation of the horizontal vestibule-ocular reflex in the primate flocculus.
Brain Res. 297, 169-174.
Welsh, J.P., Lang, E.J., Sugihara, I., and Llinas, R. (1995). Dynamic
organization of motor control within the olivocerebellar system. Nature
374, 453-457.
Yeo, C.H., Hardiman. M.J., and Glickstein, M. (1984). Discrete lesions
of the cerebellar cortex abolish the classically conditioned nictitating
membrane response of the rabbit. Behav. Brain Res. 13, 261-266.

You might also like