You are on page 1of 6

Journal of Geromologv

1983, Vol. 38. No. I, 111-116

The Life Satisfaction Index: A


Methodological and Theoretical Critique1
Danny R. Hoyt, PhD2 and James C. Creech, MA:

The Life Satisfaction Index A (LSIA) was examined by factor analytic techniques. The data do not
support the original conceptual framework of Neugarten and her colleagues. Although the results indicate a

Downloaded from http://geronj.oxfordjournals.org/ at North Carolina State University on May 7, 2015


strong need for caution, there is evidence of an alternative interpretation of the LSIA that has a degree of
consistency across racial and gender categories. In addition, these findings raise implicit questions about
prior research using the LSIA and demonstrate the importance of making comprehensive, critical evalua-
tions of scales with complex theoretical origins.
Key Words: Psychological well-being, Factor analysis, Validity, Measurement

RDINARILY the transition to old age is a Havighurst, and Tobin (1961) on life satisfaction.
O period of status and role losses. The ability to
adjust or cope with these losses has been, conse-
Neugarten and her associates were concerned with
developing a multidimensional measure that would
quently, central to the theoretical and empirical represent the complexity of psychological well-
development of social gerontology since the begin- being. In this vein, five components of life satisfac-
ning of the modern era (cf. Burgess, 1950; Cavan et tion were identified: (a) zest versus apathy — the
al., 1949; Folsom& Morgan, 1937;Pollak, 1948). degree of involvement in activities, with other per-
The operational definition of adjustment usually has sons, or with ideas; (b) resolution and fortitude —
been in terms of morale, happiness, psychological the extent that persons take responsibility for their
well-being, and life satisfaction; and to this end, own lives; (c) congruence — the extent to which
several measures have been developed (see Sauer & life goals were achieved; (d) self-concept — the
Warland, 1982). Although these indicators were person's concept of self, physically, psychological-
constructed initially to test general theories of ly, and socially; and (e) mood tone — whether the
adjustment, such as disengagement (Cumming & person holds optimistic attitudes and happy feelings
Henry, 1961) or activity (Havighurst & Albrecht, (Neugarten et al., 1961). Using in-depth inter-
1953), in recent years they have become a major views, a Life Satisfaction Rating Scale (LSR) en-
subject of inquiry in their own right (Adams, 1971; compassing each of these five dimensions was de-
Edwards & Klemmack, 1973; Larson, 1978; veloped. The structure and validity of the LSR was
Lohmann, 1977). Much of this research, however, confirmed through examination of the intercorrela-
has been characterized by an underlying, almost tions of the dimensions and the independent assess-
unquestioned acceptance of the particular measure ments of clinical psychologists.
of well-being chosen. Given the extensive interviewing required to
Others have indicated that, typically, measures construct the LSR, Neugarten et al. (1961) de-
of psychological well-being are not grounded with veloped the Life Satisfaction Index A (LSIA) for
clear conceptualization (Lawton, 1977; Rosow, use in shorter survey instruments. In its final form
1963, 1977; Sauer & Warland, 1982). One of the the LSIA consisted of 20 "agree" or "disagree"
few exceptions is the work by Neugarten, attitude items (see appendix for items). Although
the LSIA was conceptualized as having the same
multidimensional structure as the LSR, the pro-
'We arc grateful to Nicholas Babchuk, Harold Orbach. Edward Powers, and
Hugh Whitl for comments made on earlier drafts. The data were made available
posed coding scheme was additive and unidi-
through the National Archive of Computerized Data on Aging. The authors have been mensional. The validity of the LSIA was checked
supported, in part, by National Institute on Aging predoctoral traineeships with the
Midwest Council for Social Research in Aging. Neither the original collector of the through its correlation with the original LSR and the
data nor any of the agencies named above bear any responsibility for the analyses or
interpretations presented here.
assessments of clinical psychologists. The reported
department of Sociology and Anthropology, Iowa State University, Ames, 1A correlations were only .55 and .39 respectively
50011.
•'Department of Sociology, University of Nebraska, Lincoln, NE 68588.
(Neugarten et al., 1961). These correlations, as

111
112 HOYT AND CREECH

noted by the authors, were not as high as might be analyses typically have been small, homogeneous,
expected, inasmuch as the LSIA was derived from and limited to primarily rural states. The present
the LSR. Accordingly, Neugarten et al. observed inquiry will reconsider some of the measurement
that the effort to develop an adequate short report properties of the LSIA by using a large national
measure of life satisfaction "was only moderately sample and statistical techniques that have only
successful" and that the index should be "used become available during the past decade.
with caution" (1961, p. 143).
Since publication, the LSIA has become widely
OBJECTIVES
adopted. Due to its frequent use, it has also been the
subject of methodological analyses. For example, This study will examine measurement issues that
Adams (1969) examined the reliability and multi- concern the LSIA. Two questions were central to
this investigation. When tested on a larger, more

Downloaded from http://geronj.oxfordjournals.org/ at North Carolina State University on May 7, 2015


dimensionality of the LSIA. Reliability was evalu-
ated by using a discrimination value and the biserial heterogeneous sample, does the LSIA demonstrate
correlation between the mean of the affirmative- an underlying structure similar to the one concep-
response group for each item and the LSIA mean tualized by Neugarten et al. (1961)? If not, then is
score for the whole sample. The results indicated there an alternative interpretation that describes the
that all but two items (the last two listed in the empirical results in a theoretically meaningful way?
appendix) were reliable. Adams then factor ana- The rationale for these assessments can be sought
lyzed the LSIA in search of the five theoretical on both methodological and theoretical grounds.
components specified in Neugarten et al. (1961). In this assessment there were two methodologi-
The first three factors, according to Adams, corres- cal criteria. First, do the items display a simple
pond with the mood tone, zest versus apathy, and factor structure (do they load primarily on a single
congruence dimensions. The fourth factor was said factor and not much on any other factor)? Such a
to be more ambiguous because it tapped both the structure is a minimal condition for asserting the
resolution and fortitude and the congruence dimen- presence of one or more underlying attitude dimen-
sions. The component items of these four factors sions. Second, is the observed factor structure con-
differed in some instances from those implied by sistent across the various subgroups in the sample?
Neugarten et al. (1961), and as a result Adams was This condition is necessary to imply that a scale
not able to identify any factor corresponding to the measures the same thing for different segments of
self-concept dimension. the population. This question is of particular impor-
tance because life satisfaction scores are often com-
Wood et al. (1969) also examined the LSIA and
pared across groups (e.g., men vs. women, blacks
suggested a shortened, alternative form, the LSIZ.
vs. whites).
Using coefficient alpha as a measure of reliability,
seven of the original 20 items were not included in Although these methodological criteria will
serve to identify an optimal item structure, they will
the LSIZ. They also proposed an alternative coding
not indicate whether the scale actually measures
scheme, where "don't know" responses were
what it is purported to measure. Evaluation of valid-
coded as a neutral, separate category rather than
ity ultimately must rest upon theoretical considera-
combined with those who disagreed with the item.
tions. However, validity may be asserted by the
In two recent papers Lohmann (1977, 1980) has
empirical demonstration of a relationship between a
investigated the construct validity of several stan-
set of indicators and its statistical true score (see
dard measures of psychological well-being, includ- Lord & Novick, 1968). In brief, the issue is whether
ing the LSIA. The first study correlated the various the item structure indicated by the methodological
scales with one another; the LSIA correlated in the results is consistent with that predicted by Neugar-
range of .6 to .8 with almost all of the other scales. ten et al. (1961) conceptual discussion on life
Lohmann's (1980) second study consisted of a fac- satisfaction.
tor analysis of all the items from seven measures of
psychological well-being. Her results indicate that
there are two dimensions to life satisfaction and that DATA AND METHODS
the LSIA contributes to both dimensions of her new The data used in this analysis are from the 1974
composite scale. Myth and Reality of Aging Survey (Harris &
A point that emerges from these methodological Associates, 1975). These data were considered par-
inquiries is a lack of consensus regarding the under- ticularly suitable because they are based on a
lying structure of the LSIA. This may be, in part, national sample that oversampled both blacks and
due to the fact that the data bases used in the prior adults aged 65 and older. Because the LSIA was
EVALUATING THE LIFE SATISFACTION INDEX 113

designed explicitly for use in older populations, FINDINGS


only those respondents aged 65 and older were The results from the initial test, which posited a
analyzed. This yielded the following subsample five-factor model underlying the LSIA, do not dis-
sizes: white men, 846; white women, 1341; black play an acceptable fit for either of the white groups.
men, 177; and black women, 287. The chi-square fits, with 73 degrees of freedom, are
Although only 18 of the 20 items from the LSI A as follows: white men, 107.97 (p = .005); white
were utilized in this survey, they should, if the women, 99.25 {p = .022); black men, 72.31 (p =
implied structure is correct, still sufficiently repre- .501); and black women, 75.99 (p = .382).
sent the five dimensions identified by Neugarten et Similar tests were also conducted (results not
al. (1961). Accordingly, it was assumed that the shown) on an 18-item, four-factor model and a
results would not be altered substantially by the 13-item, four-factor model representing Adams'

Downloaded from http://geronj.oxfordjournals.org/ at North Carolina State University on May 7, 2015


exclusion of the two items. (1969) and Wood et al. (1969) revisions, respec-
The data were analyzed using the confirmatory tively. Neither model adequately fit the data for the
and exploratory factor analysis techniques de- white groups, but the Wood et al. model did fit for
veloped by Joreskog, (1969; Joreskog & Sorbom, both black men and women.
1978). These techniques permit tests for the number Whereas the initial model was not acceptable, the
of factors, alternative loadings of the items onto number of factors underlying the LSIA was then
factors, and equality of factor structures across sub- identified. In brief, an exploratory analysis was
groups. Furthermore, by using a maximum likeli- used to determine the best fitting model as defined
hood solution, these programs also provide a chi- by two criteria. First, the fit of the model, based on
square statistic for assessing and comparing the fit the chi-square statistic, must be acceptable.
of various models. The purpose of this chi-square is Second, the next higher model, allowing for an
to assess goodness of fit. In other words, the model additional factor, should not provide significant im-
should fit the data, so a low chi-square value and a provement in the fit (based on the difference of the
high probability level are desired. two chi-square statistics). The results from this
As previously noted, one concern was whether analysis show large cross-racial differences. The
the scale is consistent across various subgroups in best fitting models for the black respondents require
the population. Traditional views on attitude six factors, and, by contrast, eight factors were
measurement indicate that the rules of correspon- needed for the white participants. Clearly, both the
dence associated with a scale should transcend the discrepancy in the number of factors across racial
observed group (Thurstone, 1928; Upshaw, 1968). categories and the large number of factors required
The factor structure of a scale, then, should be for an acceptable fit indicate the need to reevaluate
consistent across population groupings. If not, it the component items of the scale.
would denote that some caution would need to be Using exploratory factor analysis, an attempt
exercised in cross-group comparisons. In the pres- was made to find some underlying factor structure
ent analysis race and gender were selected as partic- common to each group, while retaining as many of
ularly relevant groupings for testing these prop- the original items as possible. The preliminary re-
erties. The basic concern was whether any observed sult is the following four-factor, 11-item configura-
differences across these categories reflect differing tion: Factor 1 —Satis, Have-Exp, Past-OK; Factor
factor structures, or just differing scores on the 2 — Happy, Best-Yrs, Happier; Factor 3 — Future,
scales. That is, if group scores do vary, are they Plans; and Factor 4 — Dreary, Boring, Get-Dep
discriminating levels of well-being or are they arti- (see appendix for items). The chi-square fits for the
factual, owing to differing properties of measure- respective subgroups, with 17 degrees of freedom,
ment? Following the procedure outlined by Jores- are 18.91 (p = .334) for white men, 18.78 (p =
kog (1971), the models must be assessed initially .341) for white women, 22.85 (p = .154) for black
for each group independently. If some model exists men, and 16.76 (p = .471) for black women.
that cannot be rejected for any subgroup, then a test Although this structure could not be rejected for any
for equality of factor structures across groups can of the groups, the results remain problematic. There
be made. are, for example, marked variations in the factor
The first step in this analysis was a simple test of loadings across groups. The interpretation was
the five factors implied in the original formulation clearer for both white men and women as neither
of the LSIA. If this model displays an acceptable fit black subgroup displayed a simple factor structure.
for each subgroup, the loading of the items across From a methodological viewpoint this new factor
groups will be explored. structure is, at best, only minimally adequate. In an
114 HOYT AND CREECH

attempt to identify a structure that is more consis- second model provided a significantly better fit over
tent across groups, configurations involving a vari- the more restrictive first model (Model 1: x2 (52) =
ety of fewer items and fewer factors were em- 86.45, p< .001; Model 2 : / ( 2 8 ) = 21.36, p >
ployed. For example, it was found that by dropping .80; difference between models: ^ ( 2 4 ) = 65.09,/?
the Get-Dep item from the fourth factor, the fit is < .001). Therefore, even with this reduced model,
improved for the black women and both white it is not safe to assume that the same items can be
groups. However, the fit was rejected (p = .034) interpreted in the same manner for each of the
for black men on this four-factor, 10-item model. groups. Furthermore, although an examination of
The best reduced model evidenced by these data the residuals suggests that the lack of fit is due
is one that eliminates the fourth factor. The promax largely to the third factor with black women, neither
rotated factor loadings, factor correlations, and the the freeing of those specific factor loadings nor
dropping the third factor from all groups signi-

Downloaded from http://geronj.oxfordjournals.org/ at North Carolina State University on May 7, 2015


unique variances for this three-factor, eight-item
model are presented in Table 1. The subgroup chi- ficantly improved the fit.
square fits with seven degrees of freedom are white Setting the methodological issues to one side for
men, 1.55 (p = .980); white women, 8.28 (p = the moment, another important issue that needs
.309); black men, 3.19 (p = .876); and black consideration was the interpretation of the factors.
women, 8.34 (p — .304). Although this model Whereas the five-factor model does not have an
provided a substantial improvement in fit over the adequate fit, it is apparent that these items do not
four-factor model for both subgroups of men, the fit have a structure consistent with the one delineated
is slightly poorer for both racial categories of by Neugarten et al. (1961). The next logical ques-
women. It appears, however, to have a greater tion was whether any of the identified dimensions
cross-group consistency in factor loadings. parallel portions of the hypothesized factor struc-
Because the factor analytic model can be viewed ture. The first factor includes items reported by
as an extension of the classical true score model, Adams (1969) as the congruence dimension. Reex-
where the factor loadings are empirical estimates of amining these items, this factor appears to be
the rules of correspondence, the ability of a scale to measuring something akin to satisfaction with the
transcend any given group can be assessed. To past. The three items making up the second factor is
examine this issue with the LSIA an additional test part of Adams' mood tone dimension. Although a
was performed. This test compared two models: potential interpretation is positive mood tone,
The first constrained all subgroups to have the same another plausible one seems to be satisfaction with
factor loadings; in the second the factor loadings the present. This interpretation, however, is not
were allowed to vary. The results indicate that the without problems, because each item also implies a
hypothesis of equal factor loadings (transcendence baseline comparison with past levels of adjustment.
of attitudes across groups) must be rejected as the The third factor appears to be related to a future

Table 1. Factor Loadings, Factor Correlations, and Unique Variances for the Three-Factor, Eight-Item Model by
Race and Sex
Whites Blacks
Men Women Men Women
Items 1 2 3 1-h2 1 2 3 1-h2 1 2 3 1-h2 1 2 3 1-h2

Satis .687 -.048 -.019 .571 .593 -.020 .106 .604 .818 -.136 .366 .295 .846 -.085 .028 .340
Have-Exp .644 -.007 .020 .578 .769 .013 - .041 .421 .597 .012 .224 .576 .560 -.013 .073 .655
Past-OK .485 .086 -.019 .726 .479 .063 - .028 .746 .541 .095 -.087 .633 .565 .172 - .164 .614
Happy .106 .556 .137 .509 .056 .667 .070 .469 .191 .546 .163 .507 .046 .545 .208 .526
Best-Yrs -.106 .702 .027 .553 - .073 .641 .024 .620 -.178 .874 .244 .317 - .014 .829 .051 .289
Happier .080 .557 .050 .674 .067 .547 - .071 .689 .175 .454 -.179 .644 - .049 .625 - .166 .693
Future -.051 .053 .678 .534 - .006 .013 .676 .539 .079 .131 .570 .631 - .004 .012 .997 .000
Plans .119 .000 .450 .731 - .003 .044 .469 .762 .199 .082 .395 .771 .141 .173 .226 .812
Factor
correlations
2 .496 .507 .594 .415
3 .520 .493 .370 .405 .033 .037 .418 .570
EVALUATING THE LIFE SATISFACTION INDEX 115

orientation and/or optimism. Both of these items Although there appears to be a feasible interpreta-
are components in the zest for life dimension iden- tion for each of the factors, they do not correspond
tified by Adams. The three items in the fourth factor well with the original conceptual work. Indeed,
are the other half of the mood tone dimension. In there is little in these factors to suggest that they tap
contrast to the items in the first three factors, all the dimensions of life satisfaction as delineated by
these questions were worded negatively. As such, Neugarten et al. (1961); thus, it is questionable
they might be perceived as negative mood tone. whether the items measure what they were designed
This positive/negative contrast might also underlie to measure. Therefore, to use the alternative for-
the differences, noted above, between the three- mulation would require, at least implicitly, a differ-
and four-factor models. Nonetheless, although a ent theoretical framework for the concept of life
reasonably coherent interpretation may be con- satisfaction. The results from the present study
could be viewed as an exploratory analysis to be

Downloaded from http://geronj.oxfordjournals.org/ at North Carolina State University on May 7, 2015


structed for these factors, the correspondence be-
tween Neugarten et al. (1961) theoretical dimen- confirmed or disconfirmed with other data.
sions and their statistically derived counterparts is In sum, this inquiry was designed to either sub-
not at all strong. stantiate the conceptual design of Neugarten and
her associates (1961), or, failing that, to identify a
useful alternative structure. The analysis, rather
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS tellingly, did not support Neugarten et al., but the
The analysis and findings raise serious questions search for an alternative structure was somewhat
about the structure and interpretation of the mea- more successful. Although interpretative caution is
sures in the LSI A. When examined in the context of still warranted because total consistency was not
a heterogeneous, national sample, the original con- found, there nonetheless seems to be a multi-
figuration is not supported. There is, however, dimensional life satisfaction measure that maintains
some support for an alternative structure for a sub- a degree of consistency across race and gender.
set of the items. The most consistent factor structure
found is a three-factor, eight-item model. These
factors generally can be interpreted as measuring
satisfaction with the past, satisfaction with the pres- REFERENCES
Adams, D. Analysis of a life satisfaction index. Journal of
ent, and future orientation/optimism. There is also Gerontology, 1969, 24, 470-474.
some evidence, albeit weaker, of a fourth factor Adams, D. Correlates of satisfaction among the elderly. Geron-
tapping negative mood tone. If precedents were to tologist, 1971, / / , 6 4 - 6 8 .
dictate, the next step would be to advocate the Burgess, E. Personal and social adjustment in old age. In M.
Derber (Ed.), The aged and society. Arno Press, New York,
adoption of a new variant of the LSIA. But there are
1950.
both methodological and theoretical considerations Cavan, R., Burgess, E., Havighurst, R., & Goldhamer, H.
that counsel caution to this course of action. Personal adjustment in old age. Science Research Associ-
Although, methodologically, both the three- ates, Chicago, IL, 1949.
factor, eight-item and the four-factor, 11-item con- Cumming, E., & Henry, W. Growing old. Basic Books, New
York, 1961.
figurations identified in this analysis provide a bet- Edwards, J., & Klemmack, D. Correlates of life satisfaction: A
ter fit to the data than previous conceptualizations, reexamination. Journal of Gerontology, 1973,25, 497-502.
it is important to recognize that neither of those Folsom, J., & Morgan, C. The social adjustment of 381 recip-
formulations is totally adequate. Although both ients of old age allowances. American Sociological Review,
models provided an adequate fit for each of the 1937, 2, 223-229.
Harris, L., & Associates. Myth and reality of aging. National
subgroups examined, there are significant differ- Council on Aging, Washington, DC, 1975.
ences in the factor loadings across both race and Havighurst, R., & Albrecht, R. Older people. Longmans,
sex. Thus, observed group differences may reflect Green, New York, 1953.
something more profound than unequal scores. Joreskog, K. A general approach to confirmatory maximum
This makes it unwise to assume that these items likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika, 1969, 34, 183-
202.
measure the same thing in differing segments of the Joreskog, K. Simultaneous factor analysis in several popula-
population. Indeed, as other researchers have noted tions. Psychometrika, 1971, 36, 409-426.
(Larson, 1978), this problem undermines the ability Joreskog, K., & Sorbom, D. EFAP-11: Exploratoryfactor analy-
to make meaningful cross-group comparisons of sis program. International Education Services, Chicago, IL,
psychological well-being scores. 1978.
Larson, R. Thirty years of research on the subjective well-being
The alternative form identified in this analysis of older Americans. Journal of Gerontology, 1978, 33,
may also be seen as being theoretically deficient. 109-125.
116 HOYT AND CREECH

Lawton, M. Morale: What are we measuring. In C. Nydegger Appendix: Life Satisfaction Index A and Mnemonics
(Ed.), Measuring morale: A guide to effective assessment.
Gerontological Society, Washington, DC, 1977. Mnemonic LS1A Items
Lohmann, N. Correlations of life satisfaction, morale and adjust-
Happy I am just as happy as when 1 was younger.
ment measures. Journal of Gerontology, 1977, 32, 73-75.
Best-Yrs These are the best years of my life.
Lohmann, N. A factor analysis of life satisfaction, adjustment
Dreary This is the dreariest time of my life.
and morale measures with elderly adults. International Jour-
Boring Most things I do are boring or monotonous.
nal of Aging and Human Development, 1980, 11, 35-43.
Get-Dep Compared to other people, I get down in the dumps too
Lord, F., &Novick, M. Statistical theories of mental test scores.
often.
Addison-Wesley, Reading, PA, 1968.
Present The things I do are as interesting to me as they ever were.
Neugarten, B., Havighurst, R., & Tobin, S. The measurement
Plans 1 have made plans for things I'll be doing a month ora year
of life satisfaction. Journal of Gerontology, 1961,76, 134—
from now.
143.
Better As I grow older, things seem better than I thought they
Pollak, O. Social adjustment in old age. Social Science Research

Downloaded from http://geronj.oxfordjournals.org/ at North Carolina State University on May 7, 2015


would.
Council, New York, 1948.
Satis As I look back on my life, I am fairly well satisfied.
Rosow, I. Adjustment of the normal aged: Concept and measure-
Have-Exp I've gotten pretty much what I expect out of life.
ment. In R. Williams, C. Tibbitts, & W. Donahue (Eds.),
Unsatis When I think back over my life, 1 didn't get most of the
Processes of aging: Social and psychological perspectives
important things I wanted.
(Vol. 2). Atherton Press, New York, 1963.
Life-Worse In spite of what some people say, the lot of the average man
Rosow, I. Morale: Concept and measurement. In C. Nydegger is getting worse, not better.
(Ed.), Measuring morale: A guide to effective assessment. Breaks I have gotten more of the breaks in life than most of the
Gerontological Society, Washington, DC, 1977. people 1 know.
Sauer, W., & Warland, R. Morale and life satisfaction. In D.
Happier My life could be happier than it is now.
Mangen & W. Peterson (Eds.), Research instruments in
Appear Compared to other people my age, I make a good appear-
social gerontology: Clinical and social psychology (Vol. 1).
ance.
University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis, MN, 1982.
Future I expect some interesting and pleasant things to happen to
Thurstone, L. Attitudes can be measured. American Journal of
me in the future.
Sociology, 1928, 33. 529-554.
Feel-Old I feel old and somewhat tired.
Upshaw, H. Attitude measurement. In H. Blalock & A. Blalock
Past-OK I would not change the past even if I could.
(Eds.), Methodology in social research. McGraw-Hill, New
Foolish Compared to other people my age, I've made a lot of
York, 1968.
foolish decisions in my life.
Wood, V., Wylie, M., & Sheafor, B. An analysis of a short
Feel-Age I feel my age, but it does not bother me.
self-report measure of life satisfaction: Correlation with rater
judgements. Journal of Gerontology, 1969, 24, 465-469.

You might also like