You are on page 1of 206

INTEGRATING PIANO TECHNIQUE, PHYSIOLOGY, AND MOTOR LEARNING:

STRATEGIES FOR PERFORMING THE CHOPIN ETUDES

by

Tracy D. Lipke-Perry

_____________________
Copyright © Tracy D. Lipke-Perry 2008

A Document Submitted to the Faculty of the

SCHOOL OF MUSIC

In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements


For the Degree of

DOCTOR OF MUSICAL ARTS

In the Graduate College

THE UNIVERSITY OF ARIZONA

2008
2

THE UNIVERSITY OF ARIZONA


GRADUATE COLLEGE

As members of the Document Committee, we certify that we have read the document

prepared by Tracy D. Lipke-Perry entitled, “Integrating Piano Technique, Physiology,

and Motor Learning: Strategies for Performing the Chopin Etudes,” and recommend that

it be accepted as fulfilling the document requirement for the Degree of Doctor of Musical

Arts.

_______________________________________________________________________ Date: 6/9/08


Paula Fan

_______________________________________________________________________ Date: 6/9/08


Tannis Gibson

_______________________________________________________________________ Date: 6/9/08


Lisa Zdechlik

Final approval and acceptance of this document is contingent upon the candidate’s
submission of the final copies of the document to the Graduate College.

I hereby certify that I have read this document prepared under my direction and
recommend that it be accepted as fulfilling the document requirement.

________________________________________________ Date: 6/9/08


Document Director: Paula Fan
3

STATEMENT BY AUTHOR

This document has been submitted in partial fulfillment of requirements for an


advanced degree at the University of Arizona and is deposited in the University Library
to be made available to borrowers under rules of the Library.

Brief quotations from this document are allowable without special permission, provided
that accurate acknowledgment of source is made. Requests for permission for extended
quotation from or reproduction of this manuscript in whole or in part may be granted by
the copyright holder.

SIGNED: Tracy D. Lipke-Perry


4

TABLE OF CONTENTS

LIST OF FIGURES.........................................................................................................9

ABSTRACT.................................................................................................................. 11

CHAPTER 1: HISTORICAL CONTEXT OF THE NINETEENTH-CENTURY PIANO

ETUDE: TECHNICAL AND PHILOSOPHICAL DEVELOPMENTS......................... 13

Exercises, Etudes, and Concert Studies...................................................................... 13

The Rise of the Nineteenth-Century Piano Etude ....................................................... 14

The Chopin Etudes .................................................................................................... 15

Piano Technique and Philosophical Changes: The Nineteenth and Twentieth

Centuries ................................................................................................................... 16

CHAPTER 2: CHOPIN’S TEACHING PHILOSOPHY AND METHODOLOGY....... 21

General Principles: Technique Building.................................................................... 21

Scales and arpeggios.............................................................................................. 21

Exercises ............................................................................................................... 22

Chopin and Psychological Processes Involved in Playing the Piano........................... 23

CHAPTER 3: THE NEUROMUSCULAR SYSTEM, HAND PHYSIOLOGY, AND

PIANO PRACTICE AND PERFORMANCE: AN OVERVIEW OF SELECT

ISSUES......................................................................................................................... 25

The Nature-Nurture Debate ....................................................................................... 25

Skill Training and the Central Nervous System.......................................................... 28

Neural Plasticity .................................................................................................... 29

Motor Cortex ..................................................................................................... 31


5

TABLE OF CONTENTS - Continued

Spinal Mechanisms ............................................................................................ 34

Motor Units ....................................................................................................... 35

α-Motoneurons .............................................................................................. 37

Hand Physiology ....................................................................................................... 39

Skeletal Muscles and Motor Units.......................................................................... 39

Structural Differences Within Individual Muscle Fibers..................................... 40

Physiology of Movement Control .............................................................................. 41

Speed..................................................................................................................... 41

Exercise Training and Maximal Velocity (Vmax)................................................. 41

Force ..................................................................................................................... 42

Gradation of Force............................................................................................. 43

Force and Efficiency.......................................................................................... 44

Two-joint and multi-joint muscles.................................................................. 44

Bilateral deficit .............................................................................................. 44

The Fingers and Thumb............................................................................................. 45

Finger Independence.............................................................................................. 45

The Thumb ............................................................................................................ 47

Pianists and Hand Physiology.................................................................................... 49

CHAPTER 4: SKILL ACQUISITON: SENSORIMOTOR LEARNING ..................... 51

Feedback ................................................................................................................... 51

Stages of Learning, Specificity of Practice, and Feedback...................................... 52


6

TABLE OF CONTENTS - Continued

Auditory Feedback ................................................................................................ 53

Visual Feedback .................................................................................................... 55

Multi-Sensory Integration.......................................................................................... 56

Audio-Motor Coupling .......................................................................................... 57

Mirror Neurons...................................................................................................... 61

Music and Mental Rehearsal...................................................................................... 67

Motor Imagery....................................................................................................... 68

Visual Imagery ...................................................................................................... 70

Musical Imagery.................................................................................................... 71

Imagery, Performance, and Brain Activity................................................................. 76

Attention ................................................................................................................... 77

Motor Learning and the Human Hand........................................................................ 82

Handedness and Facility of the Non-Dominant Hand............................................. 82

Bi-manual Coordination......................................................................................... 84

Modes of Interlimb Coordination ....................................................................... 85

Finger Tapping and Polyrhythms ....................................................................... 88

Bi-manual Transfer ............................................................................................ 90

Musical Skills and Acquisition of New Coordination Patterns............................ 94

CHAPTER 5: THE CHOPIN ETUDES AND SKILL ACQUISITION......................... 98

Force Gradation: Chopin Etudes Op. 10, Nos. 3, 6, and 11; Op. 25, No. 3; and

KK II b/3 Nr. 2 .......................................................................................................... 98


7

TABLE OF CONTENTS - Continued

Etude Op. 10, No. 3 ............................................................................................. 102

Finger Independence: Chopin Etudes Op. 10, Nos. 2, 4, 8 and 12 ........................... 104

Etude Op. 10, No. 8 ............................................................................................. 106

Left Hand Facility: Chopin Etude Op. 10, No. 12 .................................................... 108

Etude Op. 10, No. 12, mm. 1-2 ............................................................................ 109

Etude Op. 10, No. 12, m. 9................................................................................... 110

The Thumb: Chopin Etude Op. 10, No. 5................................................................. 112

Speed: Chopin Etudes Op. 10, No. 4 and Op. 25, and 12 ......................................... 115

Etude Op. 10, No. 4 ............................................................................................. 115

Etude Op. 25, No. 12 ........................................................................................... 115

Flexibility and Suppleness via Imagery: Etude Op. 25, No. 1................................... 120

Polyrhythms: Drei Etüden: KK II b/3, No. 1........................................................... 123

CHAPTER 6: CONCLUSIONS ................................................................................. 126

Strategies for Learning and Teaching....................................................................... 126

Practice and Performance..................................................................................... 126

Aural feedback and aural representations ......................................................... 131

Observational learning ..................................................................................... 132

Scales, piano literature, and the brain ............................................................... 132

Discussion ............................................................................................................... 135

APPENDIX A: HENLE URTEXT EDITION PERMISSION...................................... 137


8

TABLE OF CONTENTS - Continued

APPENDIX B: WIENER URTEXT EDITION PERMISSION.................................... 138

REFERENCES............................................................................................................ 139
9

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 1. Chopin Etude, Op. 10, No. 3, mm. 1-4........................................................... 98

Figure 2. Chopin Etude, Op. 10, No. 6, mm. 1-3........................................................... 98

Figure 3. Chopin Etude, Op. 10, No. 11, mm. 1-3......................................................... 99

Figure 4. Chopin Etude, Op. 25, No. 3, mm. 1-3........................................................... 99

Figure 5. Chopin Nouvelles Etudes KK II b/3, No. 2, mm. 1-4...................................... 99

Figure 6. Chopin Etude, Op. 10, No. 3, mm. 5-8......................................................... 102

Figure 7. Chopin Etude, Op. 10, No. 2, mm. 1-2......................................................... 104

Figure 8. Chopin Etude, Op. 10, No. 4, mm. 1-2......................................................... 104

Figure 9. Chopin Etude, Op. 10, No. 8, mm. 1-2......................................................... 105

Figure 10. Chopin Etude, Op. 10, No. 8, mm. 8-13 ..................................................... 106

Figure 11. Chopin Etude, Op. 10, No. 8, mm. 26-29 ................................................... 106

Figure 12. Chopin Etude, Op. 10, No. 12, mm. 1-3 ..................................................... 107

Figure 13. Chopin Etude, Op. 10, No. 12, mm. 10-12 ................................................. 107

Figure 14. Chopin Etude, Op. 10, No. 12, mm. 13-18 ................................................. 108

Figure 15. Chopin Etude, Op. 10, No. 12, mm. 61-66 ................................................. 111

Figure 16. Chopin Etude, Op. 10, No. 12, mm. 19-21 ................................................. 111

Figure 17. Chopin Etude, Op. 10, No. 5, mm. 20-26 ................................................... 112

Figure 18. Chopin Etude, Op. 10, No. 5, mm. 1-7....................................................... 113

Figure 19. Chopin Etude, Op. 10, No. 4, mm. 1-8....................................................... 115

Figure 20. Chopin Etude, Op. 10, No. 4, mm. 24-29 ................................................... 117

Figure 21. Chopin Etude, Op. 25, No. 12, mm. 1-2 ..................................................... 118
10

LIST OF FIGURES - Continued

Figure 22. Chopin Etude, Op. 25, No. 1, mm. 1-2....................................................... 120

Figure 23. Chopin Nouvelles Etudes KK II b/3, No. 1, mm. 1-11................................ 123
11

ABSTRACT

Chopin’s twenty-seven études are both unique and standard within the genre of

advanced piano literature. Having been composed as the instrument itself was

standardized and on the heels of the didactic studies of the classical period, Chopin’s

études are widely heralded as exemplary pedagogical material for their uniform quality

and comprehensiveness. Nevertheless, despite the vast number of resources devoted to

the topic of how one might approach the études and the innumerable endorsements which

tout their incomparable worth, relatively cursory mention is made of their musical value.

From a physiological perspective, what makes Chopin’s études exceptional amongst vast

pedagogical repertory, and how does their musical value impact what pianists learn from

their study?

From a modern perspective, a musical image is both the model and the yardstick

for the measure of technical achievement as one compares performance with his or her

musical image. The Chopin études are therefore unique in two ways. First, a pianist’s

musical image of each of the Chopin études initiates an individual process of motor

learning. The musical images, and therefore the goals and the processes, are inherently

different from the vast majority of purely didactic studies and exercises. Secondly, the

genius of Chopin permeates the overall conception of the études as he intuitively

employed the human ability to develop motor skills in natural ways which continue to be

understood and supported by ongoing research.

This paper explores the Chopin études from a largely physiological and

psychological perspective such that modern studies of mental imagery, skill acquisition,
12

and human motor abilities converge and highlight what is readily available in the music

itself.
13

CHAPTER 1

HISTORICAL CONTEXT OF THE NINETEENTH-CENTURY PIANO ETUDE:

TECHNICAL AND PHILOSOPHICAL DEVELOPMENTS

Exercises, Etudes, and Concert Studies

Exercises, études, and concert studies are intended to strengthen a particular

aspect of piano technique. Each type of piece uniquely reflects the changes occurring

throughout the course of music history with regard to stylistic characteristics and taste,

the development and evolution of instruments themselves, and the period of time during

which the piece is composed. In his discussion of the Chopin études entitled, “The

Twenty-Seven Etudes and Their Antecedents,” Finlow notes the following distinctions

within the genre of didactic keyboard music:

(i) exercises, in which a didactic objective--the isolation and repetition of a specific


technical formula--is assigned primary attention, any musical or characteristic interest
being incidental; (ii) etudes, wherein musical and didactic functions properly stand in a
complementary and indivisible association; and (iii) concert studies, in which the
didactic element is mostly incidental to the primary characteristic substance (though
the music will invariably involve some particular exploitation and demonstration of
virtuoso technique).”1

Because individual études typically focus on one aspect of technique, they often

evolve from a single musical germ. As a result, the degree to which musical qualities are

woven into the fabric of the étude is dependent upon the individuality of the composer.

1
Simon Finlow, “The Twenty-Seven Etudes and Their Antecedents,” in The
Cambridge Companion to Chopin, ed. Jim Samson (Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press, 1992), 53.
14

The Rise of the Nineteenth-Century Piano Etude

The growing popularity of the piano and the rise of the middle class in the

nineteenth century spurred the demand for piano pedagogues and teaching material

related to the capabilities of the instrument and the demands of contemporary repertoire.

Among the virtuoso studies written in the nineteenth century were those by Felix

Mendelssohn (1809-1847), Robert Schumann (1810-1856), Frédéric Chopin (1810-

1849), Franz Liszt (1811-1886), Charles-Valentin Alkan (1813-1888), Adolph von

Henselt (1814-1889), Johannes Brahms (1833-1897), and Camille Saint-Saëns (1835-

1921). Only a select few remain a significant part of modern concert repertory, however.

Schumann, Chopin, and Liszt are the three musicians who by their work saved piano
composition from the shallowness into which it was in danger of falling or had already
fallen. . . . In all of these, this impulse [toward seriousness of thought] was lively and
effective, although in Chopin it had less of a controversial tinge than it did in
Schumann, and less of an exhibitionistic attitude than it did in the virtuoso Liszt.2

Schumann, Chopin, and Liszt were all fine pianists, and each extended the scope of the

piano étude by incorporating musical challenges into their studies which were technically

more demanding as well. In some ways, their études represented the culmination of a

particular type of étude and more closely exemplified the spirit of the original meaning of

the word. As Ganz noted in his survey of the history of the piano étude, “in French the

term étude has been in use since the twelfth century; initially it denoted, as did the

2
Alfred Einstein, Music in the Romantic Era (New York: W. W. Norton & Co.,
Inc., 1947), 202.
15

original Latin studium, application or taste, the latter word referring to its critical and

artistic sense of implication.”3

The Chopin Etudes

Writing in 1900, Donald Tovey declared, “Chopin’s Etudes stand alone.”4

Indeed, by many accounts, Chopin’s twenty-seven études are both unique and standard

within the genre of advanced piano literature. Composed as the instrument itself was

standardized and on the heels of the didactic studies of the classical period, they have

become part of standard teaching literature and performance repertoire. As noted by the

eminent pianist and scholar, Paul Badura-Skoda, “ . . . these are the Etudes on which the

world’s elite pianists have cut their teeth, so to speak, ever since they were composed a

century and a quarter ago.”5

The significance of Chopin’s contribution to the genre was recognized early on by

both Franz Liszt and Robert Schumann. In his article, “Some Piano Studies Arranged

According to Their Technical Aims,” published in the Neue Zeitschrift für Musik in 1836,

Robert Schumann named Chopin as one of five composers ‘who are clearly the most

important’ writers of études, the others being J. S. Bach, Clementi, Cramer, and

Oscar Bloch, Dictionnaire étymologique de la langue française (Paris: Les Presses


3

universitaires de France, 1932), I, 277; quoted in Peter Felix Ganz, “The Development of
the Etude for Pianoforte,” (Ph. D. diss., Northwestern University, 1960), 8-9.

Donald Tovey, “Observations on Chopin’s Etudes,” in Essays in Musical


4

Analysis: Chamber Music (London, 1944), 155-6.

Paul Badura-Skoda, preface to Etudes Op. 10, by Frédéric Chopin (Vienna:


5

Wiener Urtext Edition, 1973), vi.


16

Moscheles.6 In the second half of his article which categorized études according to their

technical features, Schumann indicated with an asterisk those études which, in addition to

their technical worth, were judged to be poetic as well. Schumann highlighted eleven of

the twelve Op. 10 Chopin études with asterisks leaving out only Op. 10, No. 2.7

Despite the vast number of resources devoted to the topic of how one might

approach the Chopin études and the innumerable endorsements which tout their technical

worth, the technical and musical value of the études often seem to be considered

independent of one another. For example, Finlow states that the Chopin études are

“matchless in their capacity to train the fingers, while their musical quality clearly

permits – or rather demands – public performance.”8 What has yet to have been

considered are the ways in which musical and technical qualities are intertwined mentally

and physically and the impact that such an arrangement might have on the development

of piano technique.

Piano Technique and Philosophical Changes:

The Nineteenth and Twentieth Centuries

One of the striking aspects of discussion regarding piano technique in the

nineteenth and twentieth centuries is the emphasis on not simply the fingers, but the

relationship between the mind and physical execution in technical development and piano

6
Finlow, “The Twenty-Seven Etudes,” 51-2.

7
Robert Schumann, “Die Pianoforte-Etuden, ihren Zwecken nach geordnet,” Neue
Zeitschrift für Musik 4 (1836): 45-6.

8
Idem, 50.
17

performance. Tobias Matthay is widely remembered for his philosophy regarding the

“invisible” aspects of playing the piano and for developing the principle of rotation, but

he was also a proponent of the use of imagery. In The Visible and Invisible in Pianoforte

Technique Being a Digest of the Author’s Technical Teachings up to Date, he stated that

“technique means the power of expressing oneself musically. . . . Technique is rather a

matter of the Mind than of the ‘fingers.’ . . . To Acquire Technique therefore implies that

you must induce and enforce a particular mental-muscular association and co-operation

for every possible musical effect.”9

Matthay’s view is corroborated by many. English pianist and music educator,

Sidney Harrison, noted that “we cannot make a sharp division between technique and

interpretation. As the technique gains in command and confidence, it actually seems to

prompt the imagination to bolder and bolder flights.”10 Describing Artur Schnabel’s

approach to technique, one of his students wrote that “it was his constantly expressed

belief that if you knew exactly what you wanted, you would find—invent if necessary—

the means to achieve it.”11 Leon Fleisher concurs suggesting that, “it’s your musical

ideas that form or decide for you what kind of technique you are going to use. In other

9
Tobias Matthay, The Visible and Invisible in Pianoforte Technique Being a
Digest of the Author’s Technical Teachings up to Date (London: Oxford University
Press, 1932), 3.

Sidney Harrison, Piano Technique (London: Sir Isaac Pitman & Sons, Ltd.,
10

1953), 57.

David Goldberger, “Artur Schnabel’s Master Classes,” The Piano Teacher 5(4)
11

(1963): 6.
18

words, if you are trying to get a certain sound, you just experiment around to find the

movement that will get this sound.”12

Another prominent figure and one of the significant pedagogues in the first half of

the twentieth century was Abby Whiteside. Many of her views are described in The

Indispensables of Piano Playing published in 1955 and Mastering the Chopin Etudes and

Other Essays published posthumously in 1969.13 Most significantly for the topic at hand,

the principles and methodology that were the foundation of her teaching can now be

understood and substantiated with the most recent scientific advances of the twenty-first

century.

While advocating a holistic approach to piano technique, Whiteside suggested that

the impetus for pianistic development and performance lies in imagery and the power of

the mind. “It is only when the emotional response to the aural image of the music creates

in the performer’s body a physical response, a basic rhythm, as a counterpart to the

rhythmic flow in a composition, that he is enabled to realize to the fullest extent the

beauty inherent in the music.”14 Likewise, Whiteside encouraged mental rehearsal and

imagery as part of a pianist’s daily routine and wrote the following. “Just never take it

for granted that the fingers have given up. Instead, set up a ‘daily dozen’ of thinking

12
Leon Fleisher, “About Practicing and Making Music,” Clavier 2(4) (1963): 12.

See A. Whiteside, Indispensables of Piano Playing (New York: Coleman-Ross


13

Co., 1955) and Ibid., Mastering the Chopin Etudes, ed. Joseph Prostakoff and Sophia
Rosoff (New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons, 1969).

Joseph Prostakoff and Sophia Rosoff, foreword to Mastering the Chopin Etudes
14

and Other Essays, by Abby Whiteside (New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons, 1969), 4.
19

patterns before the hands are allowed on the keyboard.”15 Whiteside further noted the

importance of imagery and emotional involvement in practice, not just performance.

Two of her students, Joseph Prostakoff and Sophia Rosoff, recalled the following:

She constantly stressed the necessity of being emotionally involved in practicing a


performance. Early in her career she learned that the human body is so constituted that
the physical coordination used when one is emotionally involved in a performance is
different from the one operating when one is not. This, incidentally, is the reason why
the knowledge of which bones are moved by which muscles is, by itself, of
comparatively little value to the pianist or teacher. Because she was searching for the
basis of a beautiful performance, it was clear to her that an automatic, uninvolved
performance—unfortunately, a commonplace state for the musician practicing
exercises--is not merely negative, it is an actively harmful experience. An essential
element of a superlative performance is systematically ignored, avoided. The ultimate
result of automatic, uninspired practicing can only be an automatic, uninspired
performance.16

It would come as no surprise therefore, that Whiteside was not a particular

proponent of scales or Hanon and Czerny exercises. Regarding the latter, Whiteside

wrote the following.

Czerny has been responsible for untold boredom, and that is exactly why his exercises
should be discarded. Creativeness in ideas is fostered by response to beauty, not to
boredom. It is time we learned to use beautiful music for achieving results if we are
interested in producing beautiful playing. Hanon is used for developing independent
fingers with equal hitting power. Obviously this cannot be accomplished. Each finger
may gain more power, but there will still be inequality in the fingers. Fingers need to
be expert only in transmitting the power of the arm. That is a different and far simpler
problem, which does not demand mechanical and uninteresting patterns.17

Whiteside, Mastering the Chopin Etudes, 38.


15

Prostakoff and Rosoff, foreword, Mastering the Chopin Etudes, 20.


16

Whiteside, Mastering the Chopin Etudes, 177-8.


17
20

Not only did Whiteside espouse the virtues of imagery in performance, but imagery

was central to her approach to teaching as well.

Even then she believed that imagery was much more important in transferring the
desired coordination to the student than a prosaic listing of the various factors of this
coordination. Above all, she was always a teacher and was as much concerned with
the means for communicating an analysis to a pupil as with the analysis itself. For
example, in the early Thirties she had read and very much approved of Otto Ortmann’s
book, The Physiological Mechanics of Piano Technique.18 Commenting many years
later, almost at the end of her career, she said, ‘I am still convinced that his analysis of
what happens when one plays well is accurate. My question is, how would he teach
it?’”19

In addition to her insistence on the importance of imagery, another of Whiteside’s

significant tenets as a pianist and pedagogue was her unwavering belief in the value of

the Chopin études.

Each Etude brings into relief a special balance in activity. One can say, ‘here are the
ingredients. For this or that Etude add a bit more of this or that action.’ In these
Etudes there is a completely fascinating handling of the various problems of a skilled
coordination. There is no other set of Etudes which so comprehensively presents
every necessary aspect of virtuosity, always combined with musical beauty.20

Whiteside’s insight into the mental components of pianistic development and

performance and her views regarding the Chopin études combined with recent

developments in physiology and motor learning set the stage for enhancing our

understanding of how pianists approach practice, performance, and pedagogy.

O. Ortmann, The Physiological Mechanics of Piano Technique (New York: E.P.


18

Dutton & Co., 1929).

Prostakoff and Rosoff, foreword, Mastering the Chopin Etudes, 8.


19

Whiteside, Mastering the Chopin Etudes, 43-4.


20
21

CHAPTER 2

CHOPIN’S TEACHING PHILOSOPHY AND METHODOLGY

Chopin was an influential pedagogue despite contributing very few words on the

subject of pedagogy. Of particular interest are the annotated scores of his pupils,

exercises he wrote for his niece, sketches of a method book, his compositions, and the

reminiscences of several of his students.

General Principles: Technique Building

Scales and Arpeggios

For Chopin, “The development of technical skill was not an end in itself, but

merely a means of freeing the hands for musical expression.”21 “He [Chopin]

recommended daily work on scales and arpeggios played with regularity [presumably of

rhythm], and set great store by scales lightly accented in groups of three or four, or even

played three against four and vice versa.”22

Now Chopin, from the very first lesson, insisted upon the hand’s retention of its
position [fingers fall freely and lightly; hand held as though suspended in the air]; . . .
the hand still retaining its horizontal position; the hand was thus prepared for the more
difficult scales, and for arpeggio passages, in which the wide intervals were eventually
reached without effort and without greatly raising the fingers.23

Jeanne Holland, “Chopin the Teacher,” Journal of the American Liszt Society 17
21

(1985): 42.

Maria von Grewingk, Eine Tochter Alt-Rigas, Schülerin Chopins (Riga: Löffler,
22

1928), 20.

Jean Kleczynski, How to Play Chopin. The Works of Frederic Chopin, Their
23

Proper Interpretation, trans. Alfred Whittingham (London: William Reeves, 1913), 29-
32.
22

Due to the ease of placing longer fingers on the black keys, Chopin approached scales

beginning with B, F-sharp, and D-flat whereas C major was taught last.24 “It is useless to

start learning scales on the piano with C major, the easiest to read, and the most difficult

for the hand, as it has no pivot. Begin with one that places the hand at ease, with the

longer fingers on the black keys, like B major for instance.”25

Exercises

Mikuli noted that Chopin’s students studied Clementi’s Préludes et Exercices in a

sequence corresponding to their scale practice.26 As his students progressed, Chopin also

assigned selections from Cramer’s Etudes, Clementi’s Gradus ad Parnassum,

Moscheles’s Stylstudien zur höheren Vollendung, J.S. Bach’s Suites, and fugues from

Das Wohltemperirte Clavier. 27 His most advanced students were also assigned selections

from his Etudes, Opp. 10 and 25.28

Carl Mikuli, foreword to Fr. Chopin’s’ Pianoforte-Werke, 17 vols., ed. Carl


24

Mikuli (Leipzig: Kistner, 1880), 3.

Frédéric Chopin, Projet de Méthode; quoted in Jean-Jacques, Eigeldinger,


25

Chopin: Pianist and Teacher as Seen By His Pupils, trans. Naomi Shohet with Krysia
Osostowicz and Roy Howat, ed. Roy Howat (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press,
1986), 34.
26
Mikuli, Fr. Chopin’s’ Pianoforte-Werke, 3.

27
Ibid., 3-4.

28
Ibid.
23

Chopin and Psychological Processes Involved in Playing the Piano

In the sketches for his method, Chopin differentiated between three categories of

technical study based on intervals: those based on adjacent notes, disjunct notes, and

double notes. Writing “there is nothing more to be invented for study as far as

mechanism of piano playing [Chopin’s italics] is concerned,” Chopin seemed to imply

the existence of other areas of study related to piano playing aside from what he termed,

the “mechanism.”29 Particular mention can be made of Chopin’s thoughts regarding

touch, sentiment, and psychological processes in approaching a piece of music.

With regard to his teaching style, Mikuli and Kleczynski each made reference to

Chopin’s attention to touch and tone. “A maximum of suppleness (‘facilement,

facilement’ [easily, easily] he would repeat tirelessly), and a cultivation of sensitivity of

hearing and touch—these were the purposes of the exercises he prescribed in the first

lessons.”30 Chopin’s philosophy also directed that, “any work selected for study should

be carefully analysed for its formal structure, as well as for the feelings and psychological

processes which it evokes.”31

As a pedagogue, Chopin advocated inspired concise practice. “He feared above

all . . . the abrutissement [stupefaction by overwork] of the pupils. One day he heard me

29
Frédéric Chopin, Projet de Méthode; quoted in Jean-Jacques Eigeldinger,
Chopin: Pianist and Teacher as Seen By His Pupils, ed. Roy Howat, trans. Naomi
Shohet with Krysia Osostowicz and Roy Howat (Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press, 1986), 25.
30
Eigeldinger, Chopin, 17.

Raoul Koczalski, Frédéric Chopin. Betrachtungen, Skizzen, Analysen (Köln:


31

Tischer & Jagenberg, 1936), 13.


24

say that I practiced six hours a day. He became quite angry, and forbade me to practise

more than three hours.”32 Dubois’s experience was corroborated by Gretsch as well. “He

always advised the pupil not to work for too long at a stretch and to intermit between

hours of work by reading a good book, by looking at masterpieces of art, or by taking an

invigorating walk.”33

Finally, as part of his teaching, Chopin’s students had ample opportunity to

observe and imitate their teacher. “Field’s and his own Nocturnes also figured to some

extent as Etudes, for through them the pupil would learn—partly from his explanations,

partly from observing and imitating Chopin, who played them indefatigably to the

pupil—to recognize, love and produce the beautiful ‘bound’ [gebunden] vocal tone and

the legato.”34

Chopin’s methods, revolutionary for their time in comparison to many of his

contemporaries, sound familiar to pianists of this day and age. His approach to both

composition and pedagogy was exceptional in that he intuitively demonstrated attitudes

and ideas which, having since been accepted and practiced ubiquitously amongst pianists,

are now finding support in the sciences. These aspects will be addressed in a subsequent

chapter.

Frederick Niecks, Frederick Chopin as a Man and Musician, Vol. II (London:


32

Novello, 1902), 183-4.

33
Grewingk, Eine Tochter Alt-Rigas, 20.

34
Mikuli, foreword to Fr. Chopin’s’ Pianoforte-Werke, 4.
25

CHAPTER 3

THE NEUROMUSCULAR SYSTEM, HAND PHYSIOLOGY, AND PIANO

PRACTICE AND PERFORMANCE: AN OVERVIEW OF SELECT ISSUES

The Nature-Nurture Debate

One of most enduring discussions in science and medicine concerns the nature-

nurture debate, an argument regarding the extent to which a person is a product of either

his or her genes or his or her environment and experiences.35 The debate has drawn

increasing interest, particularly with regard to human intellectual and physical

performance.

With respect to genetic influences and variability, molecular biologists have

attempted to identify single gene variants that might either lead to a particular disease or

profoundly impact individual performance. For example, efforts have been made to

identify sprinters and endurance runners on the basis of differing forms of a single gene.36

Rarely, however, is one gene solely responsible for a particular trait. Furthermore,

studies on human behavior have demonstrated that genes work in combined networks to

35
See V. J. Vitzthum, “A Number No Greater Than the Sum of Its Parts: The Use
and Abuse of Heritability,” Human Biology 75 (2003): 539-88; E. O. Wilson,
Sociobiology: The New Synthesis (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1975); H.
Hellman, Great Feuds in Science: Ten of the Liveliest Disputes Ever (New York: Wiley,
1998); S. Ceci and W. Williams, The Nature-Nurture Debate: The Essential Readings
(New York: Blackwell Publishing Co., 2000), and R. C. Lewontin, It Ain’t Necessarily
So: The Dream of the Human Genome and Other Illusions (London: Granta Books,
2000).

See A. Coghlan, “Elite Athletes Born to Run,” New Scientist 30 (2003): 4-5 and
36

N. Yang et al., “ACTN3 Genotype is Associated with Human Elite Athletic


Performance,” American Journal of Human Genetics 73 (2003): 627-31.
26

influence biological function.37 “All observable properties of an organism are determined

by the workings of a degenerate network of many genes.”38 “Degeneracy in gene

networks promotes evolutionary fitness of a species by ensuring that genetic diversity

supports functional adaptation to variable environments.”39

On the other hand, significant support for the importance of environment and

experience is drawn from studies involving skills such as cigar rolling40 and reading

inverted text,41 studies which have demonstrated a strong correlation between time spent

practicing and improvements in performance. Ericsson et al. also concluded that the

primary factor distinguishing violinists at different skill levels was the number of hours

spent in “deliberate practice,” practice characterized by focused attention; relevant,

effortful activities; and specific performance goals.42 While it has also been demonstrated

T. D. Johnston and L. Edwards, “Genes, Interactions and the Development of


37

Behaviour,” Psychological Review 109 (2002): 26-34.

G. M. Edelman and J. A. Gally, “Degeneracy and Complexity in Biological


38

Systems,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of


America 98 (2001): 13766.

K. Davids and J. Baker, “Genes, Environment and Sport Performance: Why the
39

Nature-Nurture Dualism is No Longer Relevant,” Sports Medicine 37(11) (2007): 975.

E. R. F. W. Crossman, “A Theory of the Acquisition of Speed-Skill,”


40

Ergonomics 2 (1959): 153-66.

P. A. Kolers, “Memorial Consequences of Automatized Encoding,” Journal of


41

Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 1 (1975): 689-701.

42
K. A. Ericsson, R. T. Krampe, and C. Tesch-Römer, “The Role of Deliberate
Practice in the Acquisition of Expert Performance,” Psychological Review 100 (1993):
363-406.
27

that expert athletes accumulate more training hours than non-experts,43 the notion that

simply acquiring a particular number of training hours to develop expertise has received

significant criticism. As noted by Davids and Baker, however, “there seems to be little

doubt that attention to high-quality training is critical for promoting the development of

expert performance.”44 That leads one to consider the processes involved in training and

the increasing emphasis on what appears to be, a complementary relationship between

nature and nurture since neither environmental nor genetic constraints are able to account

for all the data on performance variability.45

“The ‘complementary nature’ of phenomena in the natural world suggests that it

is highly important for geneticists to identify many single gene variants, although the role

of these genes in regulating behaviour needs to be framed by their overarching tendencies

to network and to cooperate or compete with environmental constraints.”46 One of the

formidable challenges facing behavioral researchers is that performance-related variables

are not completely predictable, and yet it seems reasonable that we might be able to better

43
See J. L. Starkes et al., “Deliberate Practice in Sports: What is it Anyway?” in
The Road to Excellence: The Acquisition of Expert Performance in the Arts, Sciences,
Sports and Games, ed. K. A. Ericsson (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 1996), 80-106; W. F.
Helsen, J. L. Starkes, and N. J. Hodges, “Team Sports and the Theory of Deliberate
Practice,” Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology 20 (1998): 12-34; and T. Hodge and J.
Deakin, “Deliberate Practice and Expertise in the Martial Arts: The Role of Context in
Motor Recall,” Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology 20 (1998): 260-79.

44
Davids and Baker, “Genes, Environment and Sport Performance,” 966.

J. A. S. Kelso and D. A. Engström, The Complementary Nature (Cambridge,


45

MA: MIT Press, 2006).

46
Davids and Baker, “Genes, Environment and Sport Performance,” 962-80.
28

understand a range of influence of particular variables which might affect performance.

The following will focus on integrating principles regarding the neuromuscular system

and hand physiology with piano practice and performance.

Skill Training and the Central Nervous System

The dynamic relationship of biological factors and environmental influences is

exemplified by research regarding the plasticity, or changing structure, of the central

nervous system in response to learning and experience.47 Morphological and functional

characteristics of the neuromuscular system change with chronic levels of physical

activity. These changes, or adaptations, affect performance variables such as muscle

contraction force, power, and the rate of force development (RFD).48 Research has

demonstrated that significant effects associated with motor training include neural

adaptations,49 such as experience-specific patterns of plasticity induced across the motor

K. A. Dodge, “The Nature-Nurture Debate and Public Policy,” Merrill-Palmer


47

Quarterly 50 (2004): 418-27.

See P. Aagaard et al., “A Mechanism for Increased Contractile Strength of


48

Human Pennate Muscle in Response to Strength Training: Changes in Muscle


Architecture,” Journal of Physiology 534 (2001): 613-23.

49
See P. Aagaard et al., “Neural Inhibition During Maximal Eccentric and
Concentric Quadriceps Contractions: Effects of Resistance Training,” Journal of Applied
Physiology 89 (2000): 2249-57; P. Aagaard et al., “Neural Adaptation to Resistance
Training: Changes in Evoked V-Wave and H-Reflex Responses,” Journal of Applied
Physiology 92 (2002): 2309-18; and M. Van Cutsem, J. Duchateau, and K. Hainuat,
“Changes in Single Motor Unit Behavior Contribute to the Increase in Contraction Speed
After Dynamic Training in Humans,” Journal of Physiology 513 (1998): 295-305.
29

cortex and spinal cord50 and changes in motor unit behavior and muscle. Furthermore,

with regard to both neural and physiological adaptations, the adaptations appear to be in

response to differential motor experience and training. The following discussion will

focus on neural plasticity associated with skill training.

Neural Plasticity

Significant evidence supporting the role of neural factors comes from studies

involving limb immobilization, aging, and those demonstrating phenomena particular to

strength training. For example, increases in strength occur in the first few weeks of

training prior to any significant increase in muscle mass.51 Furthermore, observed

50
See M. J. N. McDonagh and C. T. M. Davies, “Adaptive Response of
Mammalian Skeletal Muscle to Exercise with High Loads,” European Journal of Applied
Physiology 52 (1984): 139-55; D. G. Sale, “Neural Adaptation to Resistance Training,”
Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise 20 (1988): S135-45; R. M. Enoka, “Neural
Adaptations with Chronic Physical Activity,” Journal of Biomechanics 30 (1996): 447-
55; DeAnna L. Adkins et al., “Motor Training Induces Experience-Specific Patterns of
Plasticity Across Motor Cortex and Spinal Cord,” Journal of Applied Physiology 101
(2006): 1776-82; and D. Gabriel, G. Kamen, and G. Frost, “Neural Adaptations to
Resistive Exercise: Mechanisms and Recommendations for Training Practices,” Sports
Medicine 36 (2): 133-49.

51
See R. C. Hickson et al., “Successive Time Courses of Strength Development and
Steroid Hormone Responses to Heavy-Resistance Training,” Journal of Applied
Physiology 76 (1994): 663-70; D. A. Jones and O. M. Rutherford, “Human Muscle
Strength Training: The Effects of Three Different Regimes and the Nature of the
Resultant Changes,” Journal of Physiology (London) 391 (1987): 1-11; P. V. Komi,
“Training of Muscle Strength and Power: Interaction of Neuromotoric, Hypertrophic,
and Mechanical Factors,” International Journal of Sports Medicine 7 (1986): 10-5; N.
McCartney et al., “The Effects of Strength Training in Patients with Selected
Neuromuscular Disorder,” Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise 20 (1988): 362-8;
M. V. Narici et al., “Changes in Forces, Cross-Sectional Areas and Neural Activation
During Strength Training and Detraining of the Human Quadriceps,” European Journal of
Applied Physiology 66 (1989): 310-9; and L. L. Ploutz et al., “Effect of Resistance
30

increases in strength are evident not only in the trained limb, but in the contralateral,

untrained limb as well, an effect known as cross-education.52 Finally, increases in

strength do not transfer to all activities in which the trained muscle is involved.53

While this evidence supports the likelihood that neural mechanisms are significant

in neuromuscular adaptations associated with physical activity and training, it has been

much more difficult to identify the particular mechanisms involved. This has led some

scientists to conclude that “the mechanisms mediating neuromuscular adaptations

probably vary across conditions, but, in general, must be related to the maximality,

specificity, or pattern of the neural drive to the muscle.”54 For example, most normally

active individuals find it difficult to maximally activate a particular muscle by voluntary

command which in turn limits the force one might elicit as well.55 It appears, however

that this ability may be influenced by one’s level of chronic physical activity.

Training on Muscle Use During Exercise,” Journal of Applied Physiology 76 (1994):


1675-81.

R. M. Enoka, “Muscle Strength and its Development: New Perspectives,”


52

Sports Medicine 6 (1988): 146-68.

53
O. M. Rutherford and D. A. Jones, “The Role of Learning and Coordination in
Strength Training,” European Journal of Applied Physiology 55 (1986): 100-5.

R. M. Enoka, “Neural Adaptations with Chronic Physical Activity,” Journal of


54

Biomechanics 30(5) (1997): 448.

See Allen et al., “Reliability of Measurements of Muscle Strength and Voluntary


55

Activation Using Twitch Interpolation,” Muscle & Nerve 18 (1995): 583-600; Adams et
al., “Mapping of Electrical Muscle Stimulation Using MRI,” Journal of Applied
Physiology 74 (1993): 532-7; R. M. Enoka and A. J. Fuglevand, “Neuromuscular Basis of
the Maximum Voluntary Force Capacity of Muscle,” in Current Issues in Biomechanics,
31

Motor Cortex

The primary motor cortex is organized into highly interconnected neural

assemblies that control discrete movements across different joints.56 Motor skill

acquisition apparently involves changes in the connectivity between these neural

assemblies,57 changes particular to the nature of one’s motor experience. With regard to

skill training, Adkins et al. suggest that these changes include synaptogenesis, synaptic

potentiation, and the reorganization of movement representations within motor cortex.58

ed. M. D. Grabiner (Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 1993), 215-35; F. Bellemare et al.,
“Motor-Unit Discharge Rates During Maximal Voluntary Contractions of Three Human
Muscles,” Journal of Neurophysiology 50 (1983): 1380-92; C. J. De Luca et al.,
“Behavior of Human Motor Units in Different Muscles During Linearly Varying
Contractions,” Journal of Physiology (London) 329 (1982): 113-28; A. Y. Belanger and
A. J. McComas, “Extent of Motor Unit Activation During Effort,” Journal of Applied
Physiology 51 (1981): 381-93; C. G. Kukulka and H. P. Clamann, “Comparison of the
Recruitment and Discharge Properties of Motor Units in Human Brachial Biceps and
Adductor Pollicis During Isometric Contractions,” Brain Research 219 (1981): 45-55; A.
W. Monster and H. Chan, “Isometric Force Production by Motor Units of Extensor
Digitorum Communis Muscle in Man,” Journal of Neurophysiology 40 (1977): 1432-43;
H. J. Freund, H. J. Büdingen, and V. Dietz, “Activity of Single Motor Units from Human
Forearm Muscles During Voluntary Isometric Contractions,” Journal of Neurophysiology
38 (1975): 933-46; and J. Tanji and M. Kato, “Firing Rate of Individual Motor Units in
Voluntary Contraction of Abductor Digit Minimi Muscle in Man,” Experimental
Neurology 40 (1973): 771-83.

56
See J. P. Donoghue and S. P. Wise, “The Motor Cortex of the Rat:
Cytoarchitecture and Microstimulation Mapping,” The Journal of Comparative
Neurology 212 (1982): 76-88; A. Keller, “Intrinsic Synaptic Organization of the Motor
Cortex,” Cerebral Cortex 3 (1993): 430-41; and Schieber, “Constraints on Somatotopic
Organization,” 2125-43.

M. Graziano, “The Organization of Behavioral Repertoire in Motor Cortex,”


57

Annual Review of Neuroscience 29 (2006): 105-34.

Adkins et al., “Motor Training Induces Experience-Specific Patterns of


58
32

Transcranial magnetic stimulation and neuroimaging studies have demonstrated

changes in the human motor cortex59 similar to results obtained in studies involving rats.60

Plasticity,” 1776-82.

59
See J. Classen et al., “Multimodal Output Mapping of Human Central Motor
Representation on Different Spatial Scales,” Journal of Physiology 512 (1998): 163-79;
M. Hund-Georgiadis and D. Y. von Cramon, “Motor-Learning-Related Changes in Piano
Players and Non-Musicians Revealed by Functional Magnetic-Resonance Signals,”
Experimental Brain Research 125 (1999): 417-25; A. Karni et al., “Functional MRI
Evidence for Adult Motor Cortex Plasticity During Motor Skill Learning,” Nature 377
(1995): 155-8; and A. Pascual-Leone et al., “The Role of Reading Activity on the
Modulation of Motor Cortical Outputs to the Reading Hand in Braille Readers,” Annals
of Neurology 34 (1993): 33-7.

60
See R. P. Allred and T. A. Jones, “Unilateral Ischemic Sensorimotor Cortical
Damage in Female Rats: Forelimb Behavioral Effects and Dendritic Structural Plasticity
in the Contralateral Homotopic Cortex,” Experimental Neurology 190 (2004): 433-45; S.
D. Bury and T. A. Jones, “Unilateral Sensorimotor Cortex Lesions in Adult Rats
Facilitate Motor Skill Learning with the “Unaffected” Forelimb and Training-Induced
Dendritic Structural Plasticity in the Motor Cortex,” Journal of Neuroscience 22 (2002):
8597-8606; W. T. Greenough, J. R. Larson, and G. S. Withers, “Effects of Unilateral and
Bilateral Training in a Reaching Task on Dendritic Branching of Neurons in the Rat
Motor-Sensory Forelimb Cortex,” Behavioral and Neural Biology 44 (1985): 301-14; T.
A. Jones, “Multiple Synapse Formation in the Motor Cortex Opposite Unilateral
Sensorimotor Cortex Lesions in Adult Rats,” Journal of Comparative Neurology 414
(1999) 57-66; T. A. Jones, C. J. Chu, L. A. Grande, and A. D. Gregory, “Motor Skills
Training Enhances Lesion-Induced Structural Plasticity in the Motor Cortex of Adult
Rats,” Journal of Neuroscience 19 (1999): 10153-63; J. A. Kleim et al., “Motor
Learning-Dependent Synaptogenesis is Localized to Functionally Reorganized Motor
Cortex,” Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 77 (2002): 63-77; J. A. Kleim, S.
Barbay, and R. J. Nudo, “Functional Reorganization of the Rat Motor Cortex Following
Motor Skill Learning,” Journal of Neurophysiology 80 (1998): 3321-25; J. A. Kleim et
al., “Cortical Synaptogenesis and Motor Map Reorganization Occur During Late, but Not
Early, Phase of Motor Skill Learning,” Journal of Neuroscience 24 (2004): 628-33; J. A.
Kleim et al., “Synaptogenesis and Fos Expression in the Motor Cortex of the Adult Rat
After Motor Skill Learning,” Journal of Neuroscience 16 (1996): 4529-35; and Y. Wang
et al., “Motor Learning Changes GABAergic Terminals on Spinal Motoneurons in
Normal Rats,” European Journal of Neuroscience 23 (2006): 141-50.
33

Individuals trained to produce skilled digit movements on a piano display an increase in

the area of digit representation corresponding to the trained hand and a decrease in motor-

evoked potential (MEP) threshold.61 Similar cortical changes have been observed in

blind braille readers62 and subjects trained in ankle63 and tongue tasks.64 Furthermore,

highly skilled volleyball players have larger map areas and more overlapping

representations of medial deltoid and carpi radialis muscles than runners, a finding

consistent with volleyball players’ acquisition of coordinated shoulder movement

sequences.65

A. Pascual-Leone et al., “Modulation of Muscle Responses Evoked by


61

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation During the Acquisition of New Fine Motor Skills,”
Journal of Neurophysiology 74 (1995): 1037-45.

62
See A. Pascual-Leone et al., “Modulation of Motor Cortical Outputs to the
Reading Hand of Braille Readers,” Annals of Neurology 34 (1993): 33-7; A. Pascual-
Leone, J. Grafman, and M. Hallett, “Procedural Learning and Prefrontal Cortex,” Annals
of the New York Academy of Sciences 769 (1995): 61-70; and Pascual-Leone et al., “The
Role of Reading Activity,” 910-15.

M. A. Perez et al., “Motor Skill Training Induces Changes in the Excitability of


63

the Leg Cortical Area in Healthy Humans,” Experimental Brain Research 159 (2004):
197-205.

P. Svensson et al., “Plasticity in Corticomotor Control of the Human Tongue


64

Musculature Induced by Tongue-Task Training,” Experimental Brain Research 152


(2003): 42-51.

F. Tyc, A. Boyadjian, and H. Devanne, “Motor Cortex Plasticity Induced by


65

Extensive Training Revealed by Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation in Human,” The


European Journal of Neuroscience 21 (2005): 259-66.
34

Spinal Mechanisms

The spinal cord is the final common pathway involved in motor behavior, but the

ways in which it is specifically involved in motor skill are largely unknown. Research

involving operant conditioning of spinal reflexes has demonstrated how learning may

induce spinal cord plasticity. Specifically, studies involving operant conditioning of the

spinal stretch reflex (SSR) and its electrical analog, the H reflex, have shown that

humans,66 monkeys,67 and rats68 can gradually increase or decrease the SSR or H reflex.

A conditioned decrease in the H reflex is due to an increase in motoneuron firing

threshold whereas an increase in the reflex is the result of reduced inhibition of the

motoneuron.69 Decreasing the H reflex causes an increase of inhibitory synapses onto the

spinal cord70 and reduced motoneuron axon conductance.71

R. L. Segal et al., “Uncoupling of Human Short and Long Latency Stretch Reflex
66

Responses with Operant Conditioning,” Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience 17


(2000): 17-22.

J. S. Carp and J. R. Wolpaw, “Motoneuron Properties After Operantly


67

Conditioned Increase in Primate H-Reflex,” Journal of Neurophysiology 73 (1995): 1365-


73.

68
X. Y. Chen, L. Chen, and J. R. Wolpaw, “Time Course of H-Reflex Conditioning
in the Rat,” Neuroscience Letters 302 (2001): 85-8.

See J. S. Carp and J. R. Wolpaw, “Motoneuron Plasticity Underlying Operantly


69

Conditioned Decrease in Primate H-Reflex,” Journal of Neurophysiology 72 (1994): 431-


42 and Carp and Wolpaw, “Motoneuron Properties,” 1365-73.

See K. C. Feng-Chen and J. R. Wolpaw, “Operant Conditioning of H-Reflex


70

Changes Synaptic Terminals on Primate Motoneurons,” Proceedings of the National


Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 93 (1996): 9206-11 and Wang et al.,
“Motor Learning Changes GABAergic Terminals,” 141-50.
35

Several studies have demonstrated that changes in spinal reflexes also occur

during long-term training of more complex limb movements.72 Collectively, the data

indicate that operant conditioned motor skills induce alterations in the spinal cord.

Further studies are necessary, however, to better understand the exact nature of these

adaptations.

Motor Units

Several observations suggest the likelihood of motoneuron adaptation to training.

For example, in a study of motor units of the first dorsal interosseus, Adam, De Luca, and

Erim demonstrated that average firing rates, initial firing rates, and firing rate discharge

variability were all significantly lower for the dominant as opposed to the nondominant

hand.73 Furthermore, while several studies have demonstrated that humans are unable to

fully activate muscles voluntarily,74 some evidence suggests that adaptations in motor unit

J. S. Carp et al., “Operant Conditioning of Rat H-Reflex Affects Motoneuron


71

Axonal Conduction Velocity,” Experimental Brain Research 136 (2001): 269-73.

72
See J. Meyer-Lohmann, C. N. Christakos, and H. Wolf, “Dominance of the
Short-Latency Component in Perturbation Induced Electromyographic Responses of
Long-Trained Monkeys,” Experimental Brain Research 64 (1986): 393-9 and R. V. Ung
et al., “On the Potential Role of the Corticospinal Tract in the Control and Progressive
Adaptation of the Soleus H-Reflex During Backward Walking,” Journal of
Neurophysiology 94 (2005): 1133-42.

A. Adam, C. J. De Luca, and Z. Erim, “Hand Dominance and Motor Unit Firing
73

Behavior,” Journal of Neurophysiology 80 (1998): 1373-82.

74
See Enoka and Fuglevand, “Neuromuscular Basis,” 215-35; J. J. Dowling et al.,
“Are Humans Able to Voluntarily Elicit Maximum Muscle Force?” Neuroscience Letters
1994 (179): 25-8; S. Gandevia, “Spinal and Supraspinal Factors in Human Muscle
Fatigue,” Physiological Reviews 2001 (81): 1725-89; and C. A. Knight and G. Kamen,
36

activity may contribute to improvements in motor performance. On one hand, for

example, Kamen and Knight demonstrated that increases in the maximal rate of torque

development following six weeks of strength training were accompanied by increases in

the maximal discharge rate of motor units.75 This is significant since the force a muscle

exerts is dependent upon motor unit activity,76 specifically, the number of motor units

recruited and the rate at which they discharge action potentials. On the other hand, force

modulation training results in decreased recruitment thresholds in young adults and

reduced firing frequencies of motor units at percentages of maximal voluntary

contraction.77 This in turn allows more precise and accurate control with regard to

various increments of muscle force.78

“Adaptations in Muscular Activation of the Knee Extensor Muscles with Strength


Training in Young and Older Adults,” Journal of Electromyography and Kinesiology 11
(2001): 405-12.

75
G. Kamen and C. A. Knight, “Training-Related Adaptations in Motor Unit
Discharge Rate in Young and Older Adults,” The Journals of Gerontology. Series A,
Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 59 (2004): 1334-8.

E. Adrian and D. Bronk, “The Discharge of Impulses in Motor Nerve Fibres. II.
76

The Frequency of Discharges in Reflex and Voluntary Contractions,” Journal of


Physiology 204 (1929): 231-57.

C. Patten and G. Kamen, “Adaptations in Motor Unit Discharge Activity with


77

Force Control Training in Young and Older Human Adults,” European Journal of Applied
Physiology and Occupational Physiology 83 (2000): 128-43.

M. Bernardi et al., “Motor Unit Recruitment Strategy Changes with Skill


78

Acquisition,” European Journal of Applied Physiology 74 (1996): 52-9.


37

It is important to note that the force a muscle exerts during a voluntary contraction

is not constant. Instead, it is influenced by factors such as the intensity of the contraction,

the type of muscle contraction, the muscle group involved in the task, the level of

physiological arousal, and fatigue and therefore fluctuates around an average intended

value.79 This variability is exacerbated at low levels of force because recruited motor

units are contributing an unfused tetanus to the net force.80

α-Motoneurons

Properties of α-motoneurons change under a variety of conditions, during

development81 and following spinal cord injury,82 for example. Gardiner, Dai, and

79
See E. A. Christou, M. Grossman, and L. G. Carlton, “Modeling Variability of
Force During Isometric Quadriceps Femoris Muscle Contractions,” Journal of Motor
Behavior 34 (2002): 67-81; E. A. Christou, M Zelent, and L. G. Carlton, “Force Control
is Greater in Upper Compared with the Lower Extremity,” Journal of Motor Behavior 35
(2003): 322-4; M. E. Galganski, A. J. Fuglevand, and R. M. Enoka, “Reduced Control of
Motor Output in a Human Hand Muscle of Elderly Subjects During Submaximal
Contractions,” Journal of Neurophysiology 69 (1993): 2108-15; C. J. Mottram et al.,
“Frequency Modulation of Motor Unit Discharge Has Task-Dependent Effects on
Fluctuations in Motor Output,” Journal of Neurophysiology 94 (2005): 2878-87; and T.
Rudroff et al., “Net Excitation of the Motor Unit Pool Varied with Load Type During
Fatiguing Contractions,” Muscle & Nerve 29 (2005): 78-87.

80
See Z. Erim et al. “Rank-Ordered Regulation of Motor Units,” Muscle & Nerve
19 (1996): 563-73; V. G. Macefield, A. J. Fuglevand, and B. Bigland-Ritchie,
“Contractile Properties of Single Motor Units in Human Toe Extensors Assessed by
Intraneural Motor Axon Stimulation,” Journal of Neurophysiology 75 (1996): 2509-19;
Monster and Chan, “Isometric Force Production,” 1432-43; and C. T. Moritz et al.,
“Discharge Rate Variability Influences the Variation in Force Fluctuations Across the
Working Range of a Hand Muscle,” Journal of Neurophysiology 93 (2005): 2449-59.

N. Alessandri-Haber et al., “Molecular Determinants of Emerging Excitability in


81

Rat Embryonic Motoneurons,” Journal of Physiology 541 (2002): 25-39.


38

Heckman note that “these changes include changes in properties that would be expected

to alter the ways in which neurons, and neuronal circuits, translate and transfer excitation

patterns into trains of action potentials at the end organ.”83 They also cite the following

as known adaptations to α-motoneurons in response to exercise training: increased

dendritic arbor; increased protein synthesis and altered gene expression; altered ion

conductances in soma and the initial segment; increased axon transport of maintenance

proteins, neurotrophins and receptors; metabolic signals from muscles, and enhanced

neuromuscular transmission efficacy.84

While the functional implications of some of these adaptations are less obvious

and require further study, several of these adaptations likely have functional

consequences during endurance exercise. One significant observation is increased

synaptic efficacy resulting from enhanced neurotransmitter release occurring with

increased activity.85 The effect is decreased likelihood that neuromuscular propagation

might be impaired during sustained activation of the neuromuscular junction.

82
See E. Beaumont et al., “Passive Exercise and Fetal Spinal Cord Transplant Both
Help to Restore Motoneuronal Properties After Spinal Cord Transection in Rats,”
Muscle & Nerve 29 (2004): 234-42 and S. Hochman and D. McCrea, “Effects of Chronic
Spinalization on Ankle Extensor Motoneurons. II. Motoneuron Electrical Properties,”
Journal of Neurophysiology 71(1994): 1468-79.

P. Gardiner, Y. Dai, and C. J. Heckman, “Effects of Exercise Training on α-


83

Motoneurons,” Journal of Applied Physiology 101 (2006): 1228.

84
Ibid., 1229.

See A. Argaw, P. Desaulniers, and P. F. Gardiner, “Enhanced Neuromuscular


85

Transmission Efficacy in Overloaded Rat Plantaris Muscle,” Muscle & Nerve 29 (2004):
97-103; P. Desaulniers, P. A. Lavoie, and P. F. Gardiner, “Habitual Exercise Enhances
39

Further research is necessary to study motoneurons in conditions closer to those in

vivo during exercise.

Hand Physiology

Philosophies of piano practice with regard to motor learning and skill acquisition

continue to evolve in order to enhance efficiency and maintain physical health. Training

of skeletal muscle is a complicated issue with many interrelated variables, the

relationships of which may not be fully understood. Nevertheless, the aspiring pianist

may benefit from the most recent research regarding physiology and training techniques.

Discussion of basic properties of muscles, how muscles function, and issues particular to

the human hand will lay the foundation for considering physiological aspects of several

Chopin études.

Skeletal Muscles and Motor Units

Histochemical characteristics, those related to the composition of cells and tissues

in the body, and structural and behavioral characteristics underlie functional properties of

muscle such as the magnitude of force a muscle is capable of generating, the speed with

which force is developed, and the length of time force may be maintained.86 There are

two general categories of muscle fibers, fast twitch and slow twitch. The primary

Neuromuscular Transmission Efficacy of Rat Soleus Muscle in Situ,” Journal of Applied


Physiology 90 (2001): 1041-8; and M. Dorlöchter et al., “Effects of Enhanced Activity
on Synaptic Transmission in Mouse Extensor Digitorum Longus Muscle,” Journal of
Physiology 436 (1991): 283-92.

Walter F. Boron and Emile L. Boulpaep, Medical Physiology: A Cellular and


86

Molecular Approach (China: Elsevier Science, 2003), 147.


40

functional difference between them is the time required for each to reach peak tension.

Fast twitch, or type II fibers, require approximately one-seventh of the time required by

slow twitch, or type I fibers, to reach peak tension.87 Another significant difference

between muscle types is the length of time they are able to maintain force, in other words

the extent to which the muscles resist fatigue. Whereas slow-twitch fibers have the

ability to sustain moderate contractions for tens of minutes or even hours, fast-twitch

fibers, those fibers associated with maximal contractions of a muscle group, fatigue

within tens of seconds or minutes.88 The soleus in the lower leg is an example of a

muscle comprised of mainly slow-twitch fibers while muscles of the eye are mainly fast-

twitch. Interestingly, muscles of the human hand have a significantly greater proportion

of fast-twitch fibers than slow-twitch fibers in the dominant hand and vice versa.89

Structural Differences Within Individual Muscle Fibers

Muscle fibers comprise several types of proteins—myosin, actin, and six

accessory proteins. Although the contraction of muscle occurs as a result of a chain of

events associated with the interaction of actin and myosin, the characteristics of muscle

contractions are thought to be significantly related to the structure of myosin. Each

molecule of myosin is comprised of two heavy chains (HC) and four light chains (LC),

and different muscles express different forms of myosin heavy chains (MHC) and myosin

87
Ibid., 152.

88
Ibid., 1244.

L. Jósza et al., “Specific Fibre Composition of Human Hand and Arm Muscles,”
89

Handchirurgie 10 (3) (1978): 153-7.


41

light chains (MLC). Differences in the proportions in which these chains are expressed

are implicated in contraction velocity and result in several types of type II fibers.

Physiology of Movement Control

Speed

It is apparent that training-induced adaptations in contractile function and fiber-

type characteristics depend upon duration, intensity, frequency, and the type of training

program used. Results of current research indicate that resistance training enhances cross-

sectional area of muscle and force generated by muscle fibers while endurance training

increases maximal unloaded shortening velocity of muscles.

Exercise Training and Maximal Shortening Velocity (Vmax)

It is a consistent finding that dynamic, but not isometric training, increases

maximal shortening velocity (Vmax) of muscles. Isometric training is a type of strength

training in which joint angle and muscle length are maintained either by working against

an immovable force or in opposition to fixed resistance. On the other hand, dynamic

training keeps the joints and muscles moving. Examples of dynamic training include

biking, running, swimming, and weight training. Duchateau and Hainaut found that three

months of isometric training at peak voluntary force failed to alter Vmax, but a similar

period of dynamic training performed at a resistance equivalent to thirty to forty percent

of peak force increased Vmax by twenty-one percent.90 It is thought that this increase may

be the result of an increase in the Vmax of individual fibers expressing a particular MHC

90
J. Duchateau and K. Hainaut, “Isometric or Dynamic Training: Differential
Effects on Mechanical Properties of a Human Muscle,” Journal of Applied Physiology
56(2) (1984): 296-301.
42

isoform.91 Strength training increases the number of Type IIa MHC at the expense of

fibers expressing Types IIx or IIb MHC. Because human skeletal muscle fibers

expressing either IIb or IIx MHC have greater maximal shortening velocity than fibers

expressing Type IIa MHC (R. H. Fitts and J. J. Widrick, unpublished data 1995),92 a

change in MHC expression would be expected to affect the Vmax of the whole muscle.

Force

Two basic types of training influence both the force a muscle is capable of

generating and its ability to resist fatigue. Endurance training promotes increased

resistance to fatigue and therefore greater ability to maintain submaximal power output

while strength training facilitates the ability to generate greater force.93

Strength is defined as the capacity to exert force under a particular set of

biomechanical conditions; however, most movements arise from the cooperation of a

number of muscles acting together. The amount of force that can be generated in a

particular movement is therefore determined not only by intramuscular factors but by the

relative coordination of muscles as well. While it is likely that neural adaptations

contribute to increases in strength by facilitating coordination amongst muscles, it is

widely documented that resistance training can lead, not only to neural adaptations, but

R. H. Fitts and J. J. Widrick, “Muscle Mechanics: Adaptations with Exercise-


91

Training,” Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews 24 (1996): 427-73.

See L. Larsson and R. L. Moss, “Maximum Velocity of Shortening in Relation


92

to Myosin Isoform Composition in Single Fibres From Human Skeletal Muscles,” The
Journal of Physiology 472(1) (1993): 595-614 and R. H. Fitts and J. J. Widrick,
unpublished data, 1995.

Boron and Boulpaep, Medical Physiology, 1244.


93
43

changes in structural and histochemical properties of muscle resulting in increased

muscular strength.

Gradation of Force

Properties of muscle are dependent upon a balance of characteristics which

facilitate the variety of movements of which human beings are capable. Of primary

importance to the pianist is the ability to control the speed at which the piano key

descends which is in turn dependent upon the force with which it is depressed. The

ability to depress piano keys with an infinite variety of movements relies on the ability to

exert force in such a way that movements are both smooth and graded. Muscle fibers

belonging to a single motor unit are all of the same type but may interdigitate with those

from another. The interdigitation, together with the orderly recruitment of motor units,

permits a graded response that is evenly distributed through the cross section of the

muscle.

Not only are musicians more accurate than non-musicians in reaching a desired

force, but musicians also exhibit better control over the fingers at all levels of force.94 It

has also been demonstrated that accuracy of force production increases as a function of

practice, and this accuracy is largely attributed to neural adaptation and/or motor

learning.95 Deficient coordination, or the inability of muscles to contract at the most

S. M. Slobounov et al., “Modulated Cortical Control of Individual Fingers in


94

Experienced Musicians: An EEG Study,” Clinical Neurophysiology 113 (2002): 2013-


24.

H. Chiang, S. M. Slobounov, and W. Ray, “Practice-Related Modulations of


95

Force Enslaving and Cortical Activity as Revealed by EEG,” Clinical Neurophysiology


44

optimal force levels for a particular motor task, is largely related to high brain

mechanisms including the neocortex, basal ganglia, cerebellum, and brainstem.

Force and Efficiency

Two-joint and multi-joint muscles

Many muscles in the human body, including those crossing the wrist and all

finger joints, cross two or more joints. Because the amount of tension present in any

muscle is essentially constant throughout its length as well as at the sites of tendinous

attachment to bone, these muscles affect motion at both or all of the joints crossed. One

disadvantage associated with two-joint and multi-joint muscles is known as active

insufficiency. Active insufficiency refers to the fact that two-joint and multi-joint

muscles are incapable of shortening to the extent required to produce a full range of

motion at all joints simultaneously. For example, finger flexors are unable to produce as

tight a fist when the wrist is in flexion as when it is in a neutral position.

Bilateral deficit

In normally active individuals, the maximum force a muscle can exert decreases

when the homologous muscle in the contralateral limb is activated concurrently.96 In

115 (2004): 1033-43.

See T. Ohtsuki, “Decrease in Human Voluntary Isometric Arm Strength Induced


96

by Simultaneous Bilateral Exertions,” Behavioural Brain Research 7 (1983): 165-78; P.


Schantz et al., “Maximal Voluntary Force of Bilateral and Unilateral Leg Extension,”
Acta Physiologica Scandinavica 136 (1989): 185-92; N. J. Secher, S. Rorsgarrd, and O.
Secher, “Contralateral Influence on Recruitment of Curarized Muscle Fibres During
Maximal Voluntary Extension of the Legs,” Acta Physiologica Scandinavica 103 (1978):
456-62; and N. H. Secher, N. Rube, and J. Ellers, “Strength of Two- and One- Leg
Extension in Man,” Acta Physiologica Scandinavica 134 (1988): 333-39.
45

most people, the bilateral deficit during slow contractions is less than twenty percent,97

but for rapid contractions it has been reported to be as high as twenty-five to forty-five

percent.98 Interestingly enough, bilateral deficit can be obviated with training.99 For

example, individuals who participate in training that requires concurrent activation of

homologous muscles with intense contractions but not other types of training, rowers and

weightlifters, for example, have been observed to exhibit a bilateral facilitation rather

than a bilateral deficit.100 In these subjects, the maximum force a muscle can exert occurs

during a bilateral rather than a unilateral contraction.

The Fingers and Thumb

Finger Independence

While humans seem to demonstrate significant independence amongst the digits

in daily activities, they apparently lack the ability to move the fingers completely

97
See J. D. Howard and R. M. Enoka, “Maximum Bilateral Contractions are
Modified by Neurally Mediated Interlimb Effects,” Journal of Applied Physiology 70
(1991): 306-16 and T. J. Koh, M. D. Grabiner, and C. A. Clough, “Bilateral Deficit is
Larger for Step Than for Ramp Isometric Contractions,” Journal of Applied Physiology
74(3) (1993): 1200-5.

98
See Koh, Grabiner, and Clough, “Bilateral Deficit,” 1200-5 and A. A.
Vandervoort, D. G. Sale, and J. Moro, “Comparison of Motor Unit Activation During
Unilateral and Bilateral Leg Extension,” Journal of Applied Physiology 56 (1984): 46-
51.

N. Rube and N. H. Secher, “Effect of Training on Central Factors in Fatigue


99

Following Two- and One-Leg Static Exercise in Man,” Acta Physiologica Scandinavica
141 (1990): 87-95.

See Howard and Enoka, “Maximum Bilateral Contractions,” 306-16 and N. H.


100

Secher, “Isometric Rowing Strength of Experienced and Inexperienced Oarsmen,”


Medicine and Science in Sports 7 (1975): 280-3.
46

independently. For example, studies by Schieber, Häger-Ross and Schieber, and Lang

and Schieber have shown that primates and humans are unable to voluntarily flex one

finger without incurring movement of the other fingers as well.101 This phenomenon has

been referred to as “enslavement.” It appears that the degree to which one finger is

enslaved to another is influenced by whether the finger is the index, middle, ring, or little

finger and the speed with which a particular movement is performed. Häger-Ross and

Schieber have shown that the index finger is more independent than the other fingers,

followed by the little, middle, and ring fingers, respectively, and that movements

performed with greater speed tend to be less individuated than those performed more

slowly.102 Perhaps surprisingly, it appears that hand dominance is not a significant factor

affecting finger independence as research by Reilly has shown no difference between the

individuation of the digits on the preferred and non-preferred hands during force

production with single digits in isolation.103 The question then arises as to how neural and

motor organization may impact the human ability to move the fingers and whether or not

See Marc H. Schieber, “Individuated Finger Movements of Rhesus Monkeys:


101

A Means of Quantifying the Independence of the Digits,” Journal of Neurophysiology 65


(1991): 1381-91 and C. Häger-Ross and M. H. Schieber, “Quantifying the Independence
of Human Finger Movements: Comparisons of Digits, Hands, and Movement
Frequencies,” Journal of Neuroscience 20(22) (2000): 8542-50; and Catherine Lang and
Marc H. Schieber, “Differential Impairment of Individuated Finger Movements in
Humans After Damage to the Motor Cortex or the Corticospinal Tract,” Journal of
Neurophysiology 90 (2003): 1160-70.

Häger-Ross and Schieber, “Quantifying the Independence,” 8542-50.


102

K. T. Reilly, M. A. Nordstrom, and M. H. Schieber, “Short-Term


103

Synchronization Between Motor Units in Different Functional Subdivisions of the


Human Flexor Digitorum Profundus Muscle,” Journal of Neurophysiology 92 (2004):
734-42.
47

the ability might be amenable to adaptation or learning. Chiang, Slobounov, and Ray

have suggested that finger control is not hard-wired but instead plastic and influenced by

deliberate practice,104 a position supported by that fact that musicians demonstrate greater

independence of the digits than non-musicians.105 While significant debate exists as to

the mechanisms underlying finger independence, it is thought that, through practice,

central control of movement of the fingers may be differentially developed in musicians

in comparison with non-musicians who perhaps have less opportunity to practice

individualized movements.106

The Thumb

While at least one study suggests that the thumb is treated, not as a special digit,

but as another finger by the nervous system,107 there are several anatomical and

physiological reasons why the thumb has often been considered separate from the fingers.

Most obviously, the thumb is capable of rotary and three-dimensional movement while

movements of the other four digits involve primarily flexion and extension.

Anatomically, the thumb’s expanded metacarpal head and three additional muscles add

Chiang et al., “Practice-Related Modulations,” 1033-43.


104

Slobounov et al., “Modulated Cortical Control,” 2013-24.


105

Ibid.
106

107
Halla Olafsdottir, Vladimir M. Zatsiorsky, and Mark L. Latash, “Is the Thumb
a Fifth Finger? A Study of Digit Interaction During Force Production Tasks,”
Experimental Brain Research 160 (2005): 203-13.
48

strength and refined motor control to its movements108 while lacking the multi-digit,

multi-tendon muscles that are involved in finger action.109

The structure of the thumb has evolved from non-human primates to humans,110

and its development has apparent ramifications in the brain as well. One study in

particular has demonstrated that the representations of the thumb in the brain are very

large and disparate from those of the fingers111 although recent work by Schieber

challenges these results. 112 Patterns of brain activation involving the thumb also appear to

be different than those involving the fingers. Two separate studies have shown that

sensorimotor processing in the primary motor cortex and somatosensory cortices is

different for the thumb and middle finger, an observation which is thought to indicate that

specific somatosensory processing for the thumb may provide information for the motor

R. L. Susman, “Fossil Evidence for Early Hominid Tool Use,” Science 265
108

(1994): 1570-3.

K. L. Moore and A. F. Dalley, “The Upper Limb,” in Clinically Oriented


109

Anatomy, 5th ed. (Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2006), 726-885.

See J. Napier, “The Evolution of the Hand,” Scientific American 207 (1962): 56-
110

62; Susman, “Fossil Evidence,” 1570-3; and Idem, “Hand Function and Tool Behavior in
Early Hominids,” Journal of Human Evolution 35 (1998): 23-46.

W. Penfield and E. Boldrey, “Somatic Motor and Sensory Representation in the


111

Cerebral Cortex of Man as Studied by Electrical Stimulation,” Brain 60 (1937): 389-443.

M. H. Schieber, “Constraints on Somatotopic Organization in the Primary


112

Motor Cortex,” Journal of Neurophysiology 86 (2001): 2125-43.


49

cortex for fine motor control.113 Furthermore, Hamada has shown differences in patterns

of cortical activation during movements of the thumb and index finger suggesting that

sensory information from the index finger is processed more bilaterally than from the

thumb.114

Pianists and Hand Physiology

Knowledge of hand physiology is pertinent to pianists for several reasons. First, a

basic understanding of muscles in the wrist and fingers encourages conscious choices in

hand and wrist position which in turn facilitate ease and variety of movement.

Furthermore, it is possible to train muscles and to therefore alter some of the properties

they exhibit including behavioral characteristics. This is significant since, due to the

demands of piano playing, pianists require muscles that are capable of contracting

quickly but that are also resistant to fatigue.

Quite interestingly, there appear to be some similarities and yet some differences

in the way the thumb and fingers are treated by the nervous system. Not only do the

thumb and fingers differ in their structure, but it is also apparent that patterns of brain

activation involving them are also somewhat different. Because of these differences,

particular attention should be given to the development of the thumb as well as the

fingers.

See Masato Tanosaki et al., “Specific Somatosensory Processing in


113

Somatosensory Area 3b for Human Thumb: A Neuromagnetic Study,” Clinical


Neurophysiology 112 (2001): 1516-22 and J. Jarvelainen and M. Schurmann, “The
Motor Cortex Approximately 20 Hz Rhythm Reacts Differently to Thumb and Middle
Finger Stimulation: An MEG Study,” NeuroReport 13 (2002): 1243-46.

Y. Hamada, et al., “Different Laterality Between the Thumb and Index Finger in
114

Human SII Activities,” NeuroReport 11 (2000): 3603-06.


50

It is clear that musicians demonstrate neural as well as muscular adaptations in

their abilities to grade force and move the fingers independently. Evidence

demonstrating training-induced adaptations in muscle has shown that results are

dependent upon the duration, intensity, frequency, and the type of training program used.

The implications for pianists suggest specificity in how one practices, what one practices,

and the results one desires.


51

CHAPTER 4

SKILL ACQUISITION: SENSORIMOTOR LEARNING

Human beings interact with their environment via their motor repertoire, but

changes in a person’s environment, body, or task require motor learning to facilitate

corresponding changes in motor performance. Some of the changes experienced by

humans are a result of physical maturation or disease, but others are associated with the

desire to learn new skills. In order to optimize strategies of learning, theorists and

researchers continue to study the acquisition and development of skills, topics that have

attracted their interest since the beginnings of modern psychology.

With regard to motor learning, musicians are a particularly unique population

since their training is largely devoted, not to the development of strength or endurance,

but to skill acquisition resulting in technical facility, coordination, and artistic control.

Based upon current research and information regarding processes involved in motor

learning, numerous parameters of pianists’ training have the potential to influence their

learning and performance.

Feedback

“Feedback” refers to information used for motor control and learning.115 Despite

differences amongst several theories of learning, there is general agreement that motor

learning appears to be strongly facilitated by the availability of feedback during

R. A. Schmidt and T. D. Lee, Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral


115

Emphasis, 3rd ed. (Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 1999).


52

learning.116 Salmoni, Schmidt, and Walter, for example, suggest that “information

provided by the sense organs—usually termed feedback is almost without dispute

considered critical for learning new motor actions.”117

Stages of Learning, Specificity of Practice, and Feedback

Debate continues as to the degree to which feedback is used during skill

acquisition.118 Two theories have emerged, however. One view highlights the

importance of feedback during early stages of motor learning and suggests that, with

practice, gradual online, feedback-based control diminishes.119 Specifically, proponents

of this view suggest that motor skill progresses from an initial stage, in which slow

performance is under close sensory guidance, through an intermediate stage to an

advanced stage in which movements may be rapid and automated.

The other theory concerns the specificity of practice and suggests that

performance is specific to the sources of afferent information available during practice.120

116
See J. A. Adams, “A Closed-Loop Theory of Motor Learning,” Journal of
Motor Behavior 9 (1971): 111-150; I. M. Bilodeau, “Information Feedback,” in
Acquisition of Skill, ed. E. A. Bilodeau (New York: Academic Press, 1966), 255-96; and
R. A. Schmidt, “A Schema Theory of Discrete Motor Skill Learning,” Psychological
Review 82 (1975): 225-60.
117
Alan W. Salmoni et al., “Knowledge of Results and Motor Learning: A Review
and Critical Reappraisal,” Psychological Bulletin 95(3) (1984): 356.

Olave Krigolson and Geraldine Van Gyn, “Is There ‘Feedback’ During Visual
118

Imagery? Evidence From a Specificity of Practice Paradigm,” Canadian Journal of


Experimental Psychology 60(1) (2006): 24-32.

R. W. Pew, “Acquisition of Hierarchical Control Over the Temporal


119

Organization of a Skill,” Journal of Experimental Psychology 71(5) (1966): 764-71.


120
See E. J. Gibson, Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development (New
53

In other words, a person’s skill in a transfer condition will be affected by the degree to

which feedback in the acquisition and transfer conditions is similar.121 For example,

regarding musicians who intend to perform from memory, the specificity of practice

hypothesis suggests that they will be most successful if also practicing from memory.

Furthermore, if one has practiced from memory but performs with music, performance

will likely be disrupted.

Auditory Feedback

Feedback in music learning and performance is of several types: tactile;

proprioceptive; visual regarding hand position, etc.; visual as it may relate to reading

notation; and auditory. What impact does auditory feedback have on motor

performance? Early attempts to address the question suggest that altered auditory

feedback disrupts feedback control of sequence production.122 However, it is apparent

York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1969); L. Proteau et al., “On the Type of Information


Used to Control and Learn an Aiming Movement after Moderate and Extensive Practice,”
Human Movement Science 6 (1987): 181-9; L. Proteau and J. Cournoyer, “Vision of the
Stylus in a Manual Aiming Task: The Effects of Practice,” Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology 42A (1990): 811-28; and L. Proteau et al., “A Sensorimotor
Basis for Motor Learning: Evidence Indicating Specificity of Practice,” The Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 44(3) (1992): 557-75.

121
See J. A. Adams, D. Gopher, and G. Lintern, “Effects of Visual and
Proprioceptive Feedback on Motor Learning,” Journal of Motor Behavior 9 (1977): 11-
22; D. Elliot, and M. Jaeger, “Practice and the Visual Control of Manual Aiming
Movements,” Journal of Human Movement Studies, 14, (1988): 279-91; Proteau and
Cournoyer, “Vision of the Stylus in a Manual Aiming Task,” 811-28; and Proteau,
Marteniuk, and Lévesque, “A Sensorimotor Basis for Motor Learning,” 557-75.

122
See J. W. Black, “The Effect of Delayed Side-Tone Upon Vocal Rate and
Intensity,” Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders 16 (1951): 56-60; R. A. Chase, “An
Information-Flow Model of the Organization of Motor Activity. I: Transduction,
Transmission and Central Control of Sensory Information,” Journal of Nervous & Mental
54

from studies involving speech that many, but not all, alterations of auditory feedback

disrupt performance.123 Finney124 and Pfordresher125 provided evidence that some

alterations of auditory feedback fail to disrupt music performance while several studies

have demonstrated that performance often proceeds unhindered despite the absence of

feedback. While discussion continues, Finney and Palmer suggest that, “even if sound is

not necessary for an adequate level of performance in music, it might be important for

learning or recall.”126 Repp also explicitly makes the claim that auditory feedback is

important for music learning.127

Disease 140 (1965): 239-51; F. Fairbanks, “Systematic Research in Experimental


Phonetics: 1. A Theory of the Speech Mechanism as a Servosystem,” Journal of Speech
and Hearing Disorders 19 (1954): 133-39; G. Fairbanks and N. Guttman, “Effects of
Delayed Auditory Feedback Upon Articulation,” Journal of Speech & Hearing Research
1 (1958): 12-22; and B. S. Lee, “Some Effects of Side-Tone Delay,” Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America 22 (1950): 639-40.

G. J. Borden, “An Interpretation of Research on Feedback Interruption in


123

Speech,” Brain & Language 7 (1979): 307-19.

S. A. Finney, “Auditory Feedback and Musical Keyboard Performance,” Music


124

Perception 15 (1997): 153-74.

P. Q. Pfordresher, “Auditory Feedback in Music Performance: The Role of


125

Melodic Structure and Musical Skill,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human


Perception & Performance 29 (2005): 1331-45.

Steven A. Finney, and Caroline Palmer, “Auditory Feedback and Memory for
126

Music Performance: Sound Evidence for an Encoding Effect,” Memory & Cognition
31(1) (2003): 51-64.

Bruno Repp, “Effects of Auditory Feedback Deprivation on Expressive Piano


127

Performance,” Music Perception 16(4) (1999): 409-38.


55

Visual Feedback

Previous research has suggested that proprioceptive and visual feedback

indicating the initial location of limbs is critical to facilitating accurate planning of

movements.128 To determine how visual and proprioceptive information is combined in

perception, scientists often employ distortion techniques to dissociate the usually

corresponding relationship between vision and proprioception. Under these conditions

which involve either optical prisms or virtual reality environments, subjects perceive their

hand to be in a location in between those specified by either vision or proprioception.129

See M. Favilla, W. Hening, and C. Ghez, “Trajectory Control in Targeted Force


128

Impulses. VI. Independent Specification of Response Amplitude and Direction,”


Experimental Brain Research 75 (1989): 280-94; C. Ghez, W. Hening, and J. Gordon,
“Organization of Voluntary Movement,” Current Opinion in Neurobiology 1 (1991):
664-71; C. Ghez et al., “Discrete and Continuous Planning of Hand Movements and
Isometric Force Trajectories,” Experimental Brain Research 115 (1997): 217-33; J.
Gordon and C. Ghez, “Trajectory Control in Targeted Force Impulses. II. Pulse Height
Control,” Experimental Brain Research 67 (1987a): 241-52; Idem, “Trajectory Control
in Targeted Force Impulses. III. Compensatory Adjustments for Initial Errors,”
Experimental Brain Research 67 (1987b): 253-69; J. Gordon et al., “Accuracy of Planar
Reaching Movements. II. Systematic Extent Errors Resulting from Inertial Anisotropy,”
Experimental Brain Research 99 (1994a): 112-30; D. A. Rosenbaum, “Human
Movement Initiation: Specification of Arm, Direction, and Extent,” Journal of
Experimental Psychology. General 109 (1980): 444-74; and Y. Rossetti, M. Desmerget,
and C. Problanc, “Vectorial Coding of Movement: Vision, Proprioception, or Both?”
Journal of Neurophysiology 74 (1995): 457-63.

See C. S. Harris, “Perceptual Adaptation to Inverted, Reversed, and Displaced


129

Vision,” Psychological Review 72 (1965): 419-44; J. C. Hay, H. L. Pick, and K. Ikeda,


“Visual Capture Produced by Prism Spectacles,” Psychonomic Science 2 (1965): 215-6;
H. L. Pick, D. H. Warren, and J. C. Hay, “Sensory Conflict in Judgments of Spatial
Direction,” Perception and Psychophysics 6 (1969): 203-5; R. J. Van Beers et al., “How
Humans Combine Simultaneous Proprioceptive and Visual Position Information,”
Experimental Brain Research 111 (1996): 253-61; Idem, “Integration of Proprioceptive
and Visual Position-Information: An Experimentally Supported Model,” Journal of
Neurophysiology 81 (1999): 1355-64; D. H. Warren, “Response Factors in Intermodality
Localization Under Conflict Conditions,” Perception and Psychophysics 27 (1980): 28-
56

Most of these studies have indicated that subjects perceive a hand position that is biased

toward the position indicated by vision. This evidence suggests that, at least in some

circumstances, visual feedback is a very important if not somewhat dominant source of

afferent information.

Multi-Sensory Integration

It has been suggested that different types of representations built from reading

music are based on complex relevant visual patterns, explicit knowledge of musical

structure,130 rules governing music perception in the auditory modality, or the likely co-

existence and interaction of such representations.131 While musicians are intimately

familiar with auditory imagery, prior to recent use of functional imaging techniques, it

has been difficult for the scientific community to substantiate the presence of this internal

phenomenon. Recent research by Schön and Besson substantiates the likelihood that

musicians have an auditory-like representation of the written music before they play it

and suggests that, while visuomotor coding may be sufficient to play written notes, an

anticipated auditory representation of written music is important for an expressive

performance that assigns different significance to each note based upon its structural

32; and D. H. Warren and W. T. Cleaves, “Visual-Proprioceptive Interaction Under


Large Amounts of Conflict,” Journal of Experimental Psychology 90 (1971): 206-14.

Daniele Schön and Mireille Besson, “Visually Induced Auditory Expectancy in


130

Music Reading: A Behavioral and Electrophysiological Study,” Journal of Cognitive


Neuroscience 17(4) (April 2005): 694-705.

See Idem, “Processing Pitch and Duration in Music Reading: A RT-ERP


131

Study,” Neuropsychologia 40 (2002a): 868-78 and D. Schön, C. Semenza, and G. Denes,


“Naming of Musical Notes: A Selective Deficit in One Musical Clef,” Cortex 37 (2001):
407-21.
57

function.132 This hypothesis, that written music may induce both musical auditory

expectancy and motor expectancy, may in fact influence the way music is perceived by

performers and audiences.

Audio-Motor Coupling

Playing a musical instrument places great demands upon the human motor and

auditory systems due to the speed and accuracy required in the coordination of

movements and the necessary ability to perceive fine nuances aurally.133 Musicians

demonstrate high-level function of both the motor and auditory systems and plasticity of

the auditory and motor cortices as well.134 Not only have researchers been interested in

the function and plasticity of these systems individually but in the likely interaction of the

two systems as well.

As utilized by Baumann et al. and other scientists, the term “transmodal” refers

both to activity in the motor cortex elicited by auditory stimulation and to activity in the

auditory cortex elicited by movements.135 It is significant that Whiteside made reference

to just such a relationship. “The player is told often enough that listening to oneself is the

Schön and Besson, “Visually Induced Auditory Expectancy,” 694-705.


132

133
Simon Baumann et al., “A Network for Audio-Motor Coordination in Skilled
Pianists and Non-Musicians,” Brain Research 1161 (2007): 65-78.

See T. F. Münte, E. Altenmüller, and L. Jäncke, “The Musician’s Brain as a


134

Model of Neuroplasticity,” Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 3 (2002): 473-8 and G.


Schlaug, “The Brain of Musicians: A Model for Functional and Structural Adaptation,”
Biological Foundations of Music 930 (2001): 281-99.

Baumann et al., “Audio-Motor Coordination,” 67.


135
58

important thing in practice and performance. But he should be told more often that the

physical action of the performer conditions his listening.”136 Furthermore, she highlighted

the importance of improvisation because of such a relationship between the auditory and

motor areas. “Improvisation was another of her prime tools. It was used with two goals

in mind: involving the ear and creating a basic rhythm in the student. . . . The improviser

does not strike one note and then stop to decide what note to play next. From the

standpoint of the ear, improvisation establishes the most immediate relationship between

what the ear images and the playing mechanism performs.”137

Evidence for transmodal activity was first based on the language processing

studies of Aboitiz and Garcia138 and the research of Watkins and Paus139 and Watkins et

al. 140 involving speech perception. Studies in speech production by Doupe and Kuhl141

and Hickok and Poeppel were also significant.142 These studies involving language

Whiteside, Mastering the Chopin Etudes, 157.


136

Ibid., 23.
137

F. Aboitiz and R. Garcia, “The Anatomy of Language Revisited,” Biological


138

Research 30(4) (1997): 171-83.

K. Watkins and T. Paus, “Modulation of Motor Excitability During Speech


139

Perception: The Role of Broca’s Area,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 16(6) (2004):
978-87.

K. E. Watkins, A. P. Strafella, and T. Paus, “Seeing and Hearing Speech Excites


140

the Motor System Involved in Speech Production,” Neuropsychologia 41(8) (2003): 989-
94.
141
A. J. Doupe and P. K. Kuhl, “Birdsong and Human Speech: Common Themes
and Mechanisms,” Annual Review of Neuroscience 22 (1999): 567-31.
59

provided the groundwork for research investigating brain activity in motor performance

in music.143 Studies by Bangert, Haeusler, and Altenmüller,144 Bangert and Altenmüller,145

Bangert et al.,146 and Baumann et al.147 have since focused upon the interaction of the two

modalities.

With regard to piano practice, Bangert et al. concluded that “after practice,

cortical auditory and sensorimotor hand areas are jointly activated for purely auditory as

well as for mute motor tasks.” Furthermore “the degree of similarity [in activation]

correlates to the accumulated practice time.”148 Classen et al. found that corepresentation

of the areas is established during the first few minutes of training and is consolidated

G. Hickok and D. Poeppel, “Dorsal and Ventral Streams: A Framework for


142

Understanding Aspects of the Functional Anatomy of Language,” Cognition 92 (2004):


131-8.

143
See M. Lotze et al., “The Musician’s Brain: Functional Imaging of Amateurs
and Professionals During Performance and Imagery,” NeuroImage 20 (2003): 1817-29
and I. G. Meister et al., “Playing Piano in the Mind—An fMRI Study on Music Imagery
and Performance in Pianists,” Cognitive Brain Research 19 (2004): 219-28.

M. Bangert, U. Haeusler, and E. O. Altenmüller, “On Practice: How the Brain


144

Connects Piano Keys and Piano Sounds,” Biological Foundations of Music 930 (2001):
425-8.

145
M. Bangert and E. O. Altenmüller, “Mapping Perception to Action in Piano
Practice: A Longitudinal DC-EEG Study,” BMC Neuroscience 4 (2003),
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2202/4/26.

Bangert et al., “Shared Networks for Auditory and Motor Processing in


146

Professional Pianists,” 917-26.

Baumann et al., “A Network for Audio-Motor Coordination,” 65-78.


147

Bangert, Haeusler, and Altenmüller, “On Practice,” 427.


148
60

within weeks. Furthermore, the researchers hypothesized that this process may provide

the basis for further skill achievement.149

The aforementioned sensorimotor corepresentation appears to be largely

automatized and, perhaps surprisingly, can be activated preattentively.150 Haueisen and

Knösche demonstrated that pianists listening to well-studied piano music exhibit

unconscious contralateral primary motor cortical activity which demonstrates that

information transfer from the auditory to motor system has, at the very least, an

involuntary component.151

Transmodal activity is observed in non-musicians as well as musicians. Lahav et

al.152 have demonstrated that non-musicians are capable of forming audio-motor

interactions with training, and it is thought that the “previous existence of an audio-motor

network is probably crucial for fast formation of interactions.”153 Not surprising

therefore, is what appears to be a strong overlap in audio-motor networks associated with

J. Classen et al., “Rapid Plasticity of Human Cortical Movement


149

Representation Induced by Practice,” Journal of Neurophysiology 79 (1998): 1117-23.

S. Koelsch, E. Schroger, and M. Tervaniemi, “Superior Pre-Attentive Auditory


150

Processing in Musicians,” Neuroreport 10 (1999): 1309-13.

J. Haueisen and T. R. Knösche, “Involuntary Motor Activity in Pianists


151

Evoked by Music Perception,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13(6) (2001): 786-92.

A. Lahav et al., “The Power of Listening: Auditory-Motor Interactions in


152

Musical Training,” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1060 (2005): 189-94.

Baumann et al., “A Network for Audio-Motor Coordination,” 73.


153
61

language tasks and musical tasks. 154 Furthermore, “Speech, song and instrumental music

performance have in common that feedback processes and an anticipation of the

produced sound play an important role in all of them.”155

Although non-musicians demonstrate transmodal activity with training, it is

significant that pianists seem to demonstrate significantly stronger transmodal activity in

the motor part of the network associated with music listening than non-musicians. For

example, Baumann et al. recorded greater activity in the lateral dorsal premotor cortex

and the pre-supplementary motor cortex which suggested an increased motor

representation for piano sound in pianists. These results further suggest that an

intermodal transformation network of auditory and motor areas is subject to plasticity as

a result of intensive training.156

Mirror Neurons

The notion that actions might be intrinsically linked to perception was first

proposed in 1890 by William James who suggested that “every mental representation of a

movement awakens to some degree the actual movement which is its object.”157 The

implication is that observing, imagining, preparing, or in any way representing an action

Ibid.
154

Ibid.
155

Ibid., 65-78.
156

W. James, Principles of Physiology (New York: Holt, 1980), 293.


157
62

excites the motor program used to execute the same action.158 Developmental

psychologists have long accepted the idea of observational learning, or modeling, on the

belief that it is largely responsible for acquisition of language and social skills in

children. Modeling and motor skill acquisition has also been an important topic in

physical education.

According to Bandura, an “observer” obtains a cognitive representation of an

action from a model’s performance.159 Although the cognitive representation may be

incomplete because important dimensions of the movement such as pressure and

muscular tension are internal and not available to be observed,160 Adams has

demonstrated that the timing of the segments of a movement sequence can be learned by

observation.161 Observational learning, which incorporates viewing the model’s response

to the activity and the observer’s understanding or knowledge of results regarding the

activity, facilitates formation of a cognitive representation. Furthermore, Prinz has

demonstrated that when an observer sees a motor event that shares features with a similar

motor event in his or her own motor repertoire, he or she is primed to repeat it. The

See M. Jeannerod, “The Representing Brain. Neural Correlates of Motor


158

Intention and Imagery,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1994): 187-245 and W. Prinz,
“Perception and Action Planning,” European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 9 (1997):
129-54.

159
A. Bandura, ed., Psychological Modeling (New York: Lieber-Atherton, 1971).

J. A. Adams, “Learning of Movement Sequences,” Psychological Bulletin 96


160

(1984): 3-28.

Idem, “Use of the Model’s Knowledge of Results to Increase the Observer’s


161

Performance,” Journal of Human Movement Studies 12 (1986): 89-98.


63

greater the similarity between the observed event and the motor event, the stronger the

priming is.162 Growing interest in the area has led to the discovery of mirror neurons and

a mirror neuron system.

Mirror neurons are a particular class of visuomotor neurons originally discovered

in area F5 of the monkey premotor cortex. These neurons discharge both when the

monkey performs a particular action and when it observes another individual performing

a similar action.163 It has been suggested that the functional role of mirror neurons is two-

fold. First, mirror neuron activity mediates imitation.164 Second, mirror neurons appear

to provide the basis for understanding an action.165 Kohler et al. have provided evidence

for the latter in demonstrating that F5 mirror neurons are able to recognize actions from

their sound.166 The researchers recorded F5 mirror neuron activity both while a monkey

was observing a noisy action, such as ripping a piece of paper, and while presenting the

same noise without observing the associated action. Results indicated that approximately

Prinz, “Perception and Action Planning,” 129-54.


162

See G. di Pellegrino et al., “Understanding Motor Events: A Neurophysiological


163

Study,” Experimental Brain Research 91 (1992): 176-80; V. L. Gallese et al., “Action


Recognition in the Premotor Cortex,” Brain 119 (1996): 593-609; and G. Rizzolatti et al.,
“Premotor Cortex and the Recognition of Motor Actions,” Cognitive Brain Research 3
(1996a): 131-41.

Jeannerod, “The Representing Brain,” 187-245.


164

G. Rizzolatti, L. Fogassi, and V. Gallese, “Neurophysiological Mechanisms


165

Underlying the Understanding and Imitation of Action,” Nature Reviews. Neuroscience


2(9) (2001): 661-70.

Evelyne Kohler et al., “Hearing Sounds, Understanding Actions: Action


166

Representation in Mirror Neurons,” Science 297(5582) (2002): 846-8.


64

fifteen percent of mirror neurons responsive to presentation of the action and sound also

responded to presentation of the sound by itself. The neurons that responded specifically

to the sounds of actions were called “audio-visual” mirror neurons.

Evidence for a mirror neuron system has recently been extended to humans with

studies indicating that mirror regions are activated when human subjects imitate or

imagine an action. Filimon et al. have used fMRI to compare human cortical activations

for executed, observed, and imagined reaching with the goal of identifying a mirror

neuron system that represents reaching and reaching-related behaviors.167 Significantly,

results suggest that such a mirror neuron system does exist and that it is both different

from and similar to the mirror neuron system underlying grasping movements and object

manipulation. Therefore, it appears that mirror neuron systems are specific to the type of

hand action performed. Results from the same research also demonstrated an overlap

between executed, observed, and imagined reaching activations in the human cortex,

specifically the dorsal premotor cortex as well as the superior parietal lobe and the

intraparietal sulcus. Furthermore, properties of the human mirror neuron system appear

to be neither wholly innate nor final as Catmur, Walsh, and Heyes have recently

demonstrated that these properties may be developed through sensorimotor learning.168

Flavia Filimon et al., “Human Cortical Representations for Reaching: Mirror


167

Neurons for Execution, Observation, and Imagery,” NeuroImage 37(4) (2007): 1315-28.

C. Catmur, V. Walsh, and C. Heyes, “Sensorimotor Learning Configures the


168

Human Mirror System,” Current Biology 17(17) (2007): 1527-31.


65

Discovery of audio-visual mirror neurons in monkeys gave rise to the hypothesis

that premotor areas are inherently involved, not only when observing actions, but also

when listening to action-related sound.169 Recent evidence indicates that humans possess

an echo-neuron system, a system that motorically “resonates” when an individual listens

to verbal material. For example, experiments by Fadiga et al.170 and Watkins et al.171

show activation of speech-related motor centers when an individual listens to verbal

stimuli. Studies by Aziz-Zadeh et al. suggest that, as a person hears and recognizes a

sound, the brain may also simulate the action.172 This might explain how the mirror

neuron system responds to actions and sounds which do not have verbal meaning and

which are well-controlled and newly acquired.

Several researchers have found audiomotor activation patterns within the core

regions of the frontoparietal mirror neuron circuit previously found in humans in a

variety of action-observations tasks.173 Some of these observations involved watching

Amir Lahav, Elliot Saltzman, and Gottfried Schlaug, “Action Representation of


169

Sound: Audiomotor Recognition Network While Listening to Newly Acquired Actions,”


The Journal of Neuroscience 27(2) (2007): 308-14.

L. Fadiga et al., “Speech Listening Specifically Modulates the Excitability of


170

Tongue Muscles: A TMS Study,” European Journal of Neuroscience 15 (2002): 399-


402.

Watkins, Strafella, and Paus, “Seeing and Hearing Speech,” 989-94.


171

L. Aziz-Zadeh et al., “Left Hemisphere Motor Facilitation in Response to


172

Manual Action Sounds,” European Journal of Neuroscience 19 (2004): 2609-12.

173
See J. Grèzes, et al., “Activations Related to ‘Mirror’ and ‘Canonical’ Neurones
in the Human Brain: An fMRI Study,” NeuroImage 18 (2003): 928-37; M. Iacoboni et
al., “Grasping the Intentions of Others with One’s Own Mirror Neuron System,” PLoS
66

chord progressions on the guitar174 or finger-playing movements on the piano.175 In

studies involving music performance, it is particularly interesting that the posterior

inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), including Broca’s area, was active only when subjects

listened to music they knew how to play. When subjects listened to music they had never

played, auditory activation was dominant since subjects were unable to match the music

with existing action representations. However, Lahav et al. have shown premotor activity

when subjects listened to untrained music comprised of the same notes as learned

music.176 The researchers hypothesized that premotor activity might reflect the ability of

subjects to link some of the notes they heard with corresponding fingers and piano keys.

Other evidence also suggests that mental simulations and operations of the mirror neuron

system might either be functionally related to one another or a form of one another;

further studies are needed to support this hypothesis, however. Furthermore, it has been

suggested that a cross-modal neural system responsible for formally orchestrating mirror

Biol 3(3) (2005): 529-35; and M. Lotze, “Differential Cerebral Activation During
Observation of Expressive Gestures and Motor Acts,” Neuropsychologia 44 (2006):
1787-95.

G. Buccino, F. Finkofski, and L. Riggio, “The Mirror Neuron System and


174

Action Recognition,” Brain and Language 89(2) (2004a): 362-9.

B. P. Haslinger et al., “Transmodal Sensorimotor Networks During Action


175

Observation in Professional Pianists,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17(2) (2005):


282-93.

176
Lahav, Saltzman, and Schlaug, “Action Representation of Sound,” 308-14.
67

neurons might exist in humans.177 A questions then arises as to whether or not mirror

neuron activity is triggered by mental representation of an action.178

Music and Mental Rehearsal

Mental rehearsal incorporates both committing a score to memory and the ability to

create an internalized representation of the sound. With the exception of the primary

motor and sensory areas, many of the other cortical regions that are normally active while

playing are also active during mental rehearsal.179 The effectiveness of mental practice in

improving aspects of the dynamics of movement, such as the accuracy of a movement

trajectory, has also been verified experimentally.180 While the gains achieved by those

undertaking mental rehearsal have not been as significant as those undertaking physical

practice, mental rehearsal is apparently better than no practice.181 Additionally, mental

rehearsal may help to mediate injuries related to overuse.

See Kohler et al., “Hearing Sounds, Understanding Actions,” 846-8 and C.


177

Keysers et al., “Audiovisual Mirror Neurons and Action Recognition,” Experimental


Brain Research 153(4) (2003): 628-36.

M. A. Umiltà et al., “I Know What You Are Doing. A Neurophysiological


178

Study,” Neuron 31(1) (2001): 155-65.

See F. J. Langheim et al., “Cortical Systems Associated with Covert Music


179

Rehearsal,” NeuroImage 16(4) (2002): 901-8; Lotze et al., “The Musician’s Brain,”
1817-29; and Meister et al., “Playing Piano in the Mind,” 219-28.

L. Yágüez et al., “A Mental Route to Motor Learning: Improving Trajectorial


180

Kinematics Through Imagery Training,” Behavioural Brain Research 90(1) (1998): 95-
106.

181
A. Gabrielsson, “The Performance of Music,” in The Psychology of Music, 2nd
ed., ed. D. Deutsch (San Diego: Academic Press, 1999), 501-602.
68

Motor Imagery

Motor imagery has been defined as the covert process of imagining movement of

one’s own body (-part) without actually moving that body (-part).182 Motor imagery is

significant since it leads to activation in motor areas of the brain.183 In addition to similar

brain activation, Pascual-Leone et al. demonstrated that motor imagery of finger

movements resulted in the same organizational changes as actual physical practice.184

The scientists studied the effects of mental and physical practice on the learning of a one-

handed, five-finger piano exercise and the modulation of the cortical motor areas

targeting muscles involved in the task. Over the course of five days, subjects rehearsed

the sequence either mentally or physically, and both groups showed progressive

improvement. Results showed that the size of the contralateral output map for the long

finger flexor and extensor muscles increased progressively each day as the subjects

practiced the task. The increases in size of the representations were equivalent in both

physical and mental training conditions, but the level of performance in the mental-

practice condition after 5 days was equivalent to that of the physical-practice condition

182
Sjoerd de Vries and Theo Mulder, “Motor Imagery and Stroke Rehabilitation: A
Critical Discussion,” Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine 39 (2007): 5-13.

See Kristeva et al., “Activation of Cortical Areas in Music Execution and


183

Imagining: A High-resolution EEG Study,” NeuroImage 20(3) (2003): 1872-83; Lotze,


“The Musician’s Brain,” 1817-29; and Meister et al., “Playing Piano in the Mind,” 219-
28.

A. Pascual-Leone et al., “Modulation of Muscle Responses Evoked by


184

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation During the Acquisition of New Fine Motor Skills,”
Journal of Neurophysiology 74 (1995): 1037-45.
69

after 3 days. However, after adding 1 physical training session at the end of a period of 5

days of mental practice, subjects reached the same level of performance as those who

were in the physical training group. The scientists interpreted the results as suggesting

that mental practice has a preparatory effect on the task which in turn increases the

efficiency of subsequent physical training.

Studies using positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic

resonance imaging (fMRI) also show that the cortical regions involved in the execution

of a movement are active during motor imagery as well. Using fMRI, Lotze et al. studied

brain activation during executed and imagined movements of the right and left hand

using fMRI and found that the supplementary motor area (SMA), the premotor cortex

(PMC), and the primary motor area (M1) are significantly activated during both actual

and imagined movement.185 The SMA and PMC play a prominent role in the planning,

generation, and execution of more complex motor tasks.186

In addition, although the underlying mechanisms are not yet clear, there is

significant evidence supporting the hypothesis that overt movement, motor imagery, and

observation are essentially based on the same processes.187 Some of the evidence

supporting this is drawn from studies showing that the time to complete an imagined

M. Lotze et al., “Activation of Cortical and Cerebellar Motor Areas,” 491-501.


185

G. Abbruzzese, C. Trompetto, and M. Schieppati, “The Excitability of the


186

Human Motor Cortex Increases During Execution and Mental Imagination of Sequential
but not Repetitive Finger Movements,” Experimental Brain Research 111 (1996): 465-
72.

de Vries and Mulder, “Motor Imagery and Stroke Rehabilitation,” 5-13.


187
70

movement is similar to the time needed for actual execution of that movement, an

observation that extends to the field of music as well. Two studies displayed a great deal

of correlation in the times that it took string players to perform pieces and to imagine

performances of the pieces.188

Visual Imagery

Practice through use of visual imagery has been shown to benefit acquisition and

performance of motor skills.189 This phenomenon is supported by behavioral,

neuropsychological, and neuroimaging studies demonstrating that visual images and

perceived stimuli recruit the same temporal and occipital brain areas necessary for visual

perception.190 When utilizing visual imagery in practice, subjects experienced imagined

visual feedback similar to the visual feedback available during physical practice.191

Langheim et al., “Covert Music Rehearsal,” 901-8.


188

189
See M. U. Lejeune, C. Decker, and X. Sanchez, “Mental Rehearsal in Table
Tennis Performance,” Perceptual and Motor Skills 79(1) (1994): 627-41; W. G. Meacci
and D. L. Pastore, “The Effects of Occluded Vision and Imagery on Putting Golf Balls,”
Perceptual and Motor Skills 80(1): 179-86; D. W. Mendoza and H. Wichman, “‘Inner’
Dart: Effects of Mental Practice on Performance of Dart Throwing,” Perceptual and
Motor Skills 47 (1978): 1195-99; R. Noel, “The Effect of Visuo-Motor Behavioural
Rehearsal of Tennis Performance,” Journal of Sport Psychology 2 (1980): 221-6; Pascual-
Leone, “Modulation of Muscle Responses,” 1037-45; and G. Yue and K. J. Cole,
“Strength Increases From the Motor Program: Comparison of Training with Maximum
Voluntary and Imagined Muscle Contractions,” Journal of Neurophysiology 67 (1992):
1114-23.
190
See M. J. Farah, “The Neural Bases of Mental Imagery,” in The New Cognitive
Neurosciences, ed. M. S. Gazzaniga (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2000), 965-74 and S.
M. Kosslyn and W. L. Thompson, “Shared Mechanisms in Visual Imagery and Visual
Perception: Insights from Cognitive Neuroscience,” in The New Cognitive
Neurosciences, ed. M. S. Gazzaniga (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2000), 975-85.
191
Krigolson and Van Gyn, “‘Feedback; During Visual Imagery,” 24-32.
71

Although the benefits of visual imagery have been most significantly associated

with first-person imaging, substantial gain has also been reported as a result of third-

person observation. Considerable evidence suggests that observation of others’ actions

and the preparation of one’s own actions may lead to a common motor representation.192

Application of practice through use of imagery would therefore extend from lessons,

masterclasses, and concerts in which one observes another to the concert stage where

one’s preparation might incorporate imagery.

Musical Imagery

Aleman et al. define musical mental imagery as the “ability to ‘hear’ melodic

sound-sequences with the ‘mind’s ear’ in the absence of external stimulation.”193 While it

is a common phenomenon amongst musicians—conductors, composers, and performers

for example, it has only been relatively recently with the aid of imaging tools that musical

imagery could be substantiated scientifically. In large part due to the work of Schürman

et al. with magneto-encephalography,194 Halpern and Zatorre195 and Zatorre et al.196 with

M. Jeannerod, “The 25th Bartlett Lecture. To Act or Not to Act: Perspectives


192

on the Representation of Actions,” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 52


(1999): 1-29.

A. Aleman et al., “Music Training and Mental Imagery Ability,”


193

Neuropsychologia 38 (2000): 1664-8.

See M. Schurmann et al., “Mind’s Ear in a Musician: Where and When in the
194

Brain,” NeuroImage 16 (2002): 434-40.

A. R. Halpern and R. J. Zatorre, “When that Tune Runs Through Your Head: A
195

PET Investigation of Auditory Imagery for Familiar Melodies,” Cerebral Cortex 9


(1999): 697-704.
72

positron emission tomography, and Halpern et al.,197 Kraemer et al.,198 and Yoo et al.199

with functional MRI, converging evidence has demonstrated that neural activity in the

auditory cortex can occur in the absence of sound and that the activity is likely mediated

by the experience of imagining music. An fMRI study conducted by Nakada, Fujii,

Suzuki, & Kwee200 and a magnetoencephalography (MEG) study by Schürmann, Raij,

Fujiki, and Hari201 also clearly show audiocortical activity while musical notation is

read.202 Furthermore, despite the fact that musicians are able to remember visually-

presented sequences of musical notes via musical imagery, nonmusicians are not able to

transform visual notation into musical images.203

R. J. Zatorre et al., “Hearing in the Mind’s Ear: A PET Investigation of Musical


196

Imagery and Perception,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 8 (1996): 29-46.

Halpern et al., “Behavioral and Neural Correlates of Perceived and Imagined


197

Musical Timbre,” Neuropsychologia 42(9) (2004): 1281-92.

D. J. M. Kraemer et al., “Musical Imagery: Sound of Silence Activates


198

Auditory Cortex,” Nature 434 (2005): 158.

S. S. Yoo, C. U. Lee, and B. G. Choi, “Human Brain Mapping of Auditory


199

Imagery: Event-Related Functional MRI Study,” Neuroreport 12(14) (2001): 3045-9.

T. Nakada et al., “‘Musical Brain’ Revealed by High-field (3Tesla) Functional


200

MRI,” NeuroReport 9 (1998): 3853-56.

201
Schurmann et al., “Mind’s Ear in a Musician,” 434-40.

J. A. Sloboda, review of Language, Music, and Mind, by Diana Raffman, Mind


202

& Language 9 (1994): 377-85.

203
See Virpi Kalakoski, “Effect of Level on Recall of Visually Presented Patterns of
Musical Notes,” Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 48 (2007): 87-96; Warren Brodsky
et al., “Auditory Imagery from Musical Notation in Expert Musicians,” Perception &
Psychophysics 65(4) (2003): 6012-12; and M. Schürmann et al., “Mind’s Ear in a
73

Recently, interest has grown in areas related to mental practice, and several

individuals have shown the overall efficacy of mental practice in music performance.204

Recent studies have demonstrated that pianists’ mental practice with an auditory model

resulted in advantages over mental practice alone205 while it has also been demonstrated

that analytical score study often aided memorization of unfamiliar music.206 It has since

been suggested that analytical study might lead to auditory imagery or motor imagery.207

Several studies have indicated that listening to a performance is effective in learning

unfamiliar music.208 Highben and Palmer demonstrated that pianists with strong aural

Musician: Where and When in the Brain,” NeuroImage 16 (2002): 434-40.

D. D. Coffman, “Effects of Mental Practice, Physical Practice, and Knowledge


204

of Results in Piano Performance,” Journal of Research in Music Education 38 (1990):


187-96 and S. L. Ross, “The Effectiveness of Mental Practice in Improving the
Performance of College Trombonists,” Journal of Research in Music Education 33 (1985):
221-30.

See S. Lim and L. G. Lippman, “Mental Practice and Memorization of Piano


205

Music,” Journal of General Psychology 118 (1991): 21-30 and A. M. Theiler and L. G.
Lippman, “Effects of Mental Practice and Modeling on Guitar and Vocal Performance,”
Journal of General Psychology 122 (1995): 329-43.

G. Rubin-Rabson, “The Influence of Analytical Pre-Study in Memorizing Piano


206

Music,” Archives of Psychology 20 (1937): 3-53.

S. Lim and L. G. Lippman, “Mental Practice and Memorization of Piano


207

Music,” Journal of General Psychology 118 (1991): 21-30.

See R. K. Rosenthal, “The Relative Effects of Guided Model, Model Only,


208

Guided Only, and Practice Only on the Accuracy of Advanced Instrumentalists’ Musical
Performance, Accuracy,” Journal of Research in Music Education 32 (1984): 265-73 and
R. K. Rosenthal et al., “Effects of Different Practice Conditions on Advanced
Instrumentalists’ Performance Accuracy,” Journal of Research in Music Education 36
(1988): 250-7.
74

skills were least affected by the absence of auditory feedback during learning and suggest

that “good performance in the absence of feedback could be due to little or no reliance on

that type of feedback, or alternatively, to good imagery skills that allow performers to use

mental practice to ‘fill in’ for the missing feedback.”209 In suggesting the likelihood of

the latter, the scientists note a significant correlation between pianists’ aural skills

measures and their performance from memory following the absence of auditory

feedback during practice. Furthermore, this conclusion is consistent with earlier studies

demonstrating increased efficacy of mental practice with an auditory model.210

How does a musical image compare to the music itself? Although literature

concerning auditory imagery has not been as prevalent as literature concerning visual

imagery, similar conclusions have been reached in several cases. Because musical

images contain melodic and harmonic information and encode fairly precise information

about tempo and pitch, they apparently possess a sensory quality similar to the experience

of perceiving actual music.211 Furthermore, studies in which subjects demonstrate

recruitment of the secondary auditory cortex as a result of imagining tones in familiar and

Z. Highben and C. Palmer, “Effects of Auditory and Motor Mental Practice in


209

Memorized Piano Performance,” Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education
159 (2004): 64.

See Lim and Lippman, “Mental Practice and Memorization of Piano Music,”
210

21-30; R. K. Rosenthal et al., “Effects of Different Practice Conditions,” 250-7; and


Theiler and Lippman, “Effects of Mental Practice and Modeling,” 329-43.

See T. L. Hubbard and K. Stoeckig, “Musical Imagery: Generation of Tones


211

and Chords,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 14


(1988): 656-67 and Idem, “The Representation of Pitch in Musical Imagery,” in Auditory
Imagery, ed. D. Reisberg (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1992): 199-236.
75

unfamiliar melodies suggest that shared substrates exist for auditory perception and

imagery.212

Auditory images also seem to demonstrate perceptual properties in that they

influence perceptual processing of auditory stimuli.213 Farah and Smith used a signal

detection test to show that the intensity threshold for detecting a pure tone in noise was

lower when subjects formed an image of the target tone either before or during the

observation than when subjects formed an image of a different tone.214 The researchers

concluded that forming the mental image drew attention to the appropriate frequency and

therefore facilitated perception. Similar results have been obtained regarding perception

and the timing of events. Wright and Fitzgerald determined that forming an expectancy

of a target tone based on a cue facilitates the tone’s detection compared to unexpected

occurrences of the target tone.215 Furthermore, it is apparent that musical training affects

perception of auditory stimuli. Janata and Paroo observed significant correlations

212
See Zatorre et al., “Hearing in the Mind’s Ear,” 29-46; Halpern and Zatorre,
“When that Tune Runs Through Your Head,” 697-704; Kraemer et al., “Sound of
Silence,” 158; P. Janata, “Brain Electrical Activity Evoked by Mental Formation of
Auditory Expectations and Images,” Brain Topography 13 (2001a): 169-93; and Idem,
“Neurophysiological Measurements Underlying Auditory Image Formation in Music,” in
Musical Imagery, ed. R. I. Godoy and H. Jorgensen (Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets &
Zeitlinger), 27-41.

Petr Janata and Kaivon Paroo, “Acuity of Auditory Images in Pitch and Time,”
213

Perception & Psychophysics 68(5) (2006): 829-44.

M. J. Farah and A. F. Smith, “Perceptual Interference and Facilitation with


214

Auditory Imagery,” Perception & Psychophysics 33 (1983): 475-8.

B. Z. Wright and M. B. Fitzgerald, “The Time Course of Attention in a Simple


215

Auditory Detection Task,” Perception & Psychophysics 66 (2004): 508-16.


76

between the amount of musical training and image thresholds with regard to both

intonation and context membership tasks such that greater musical training resulted in

more finely tuned images.216

Imagery, Performance, and Brain Activity

With the exception of the primary motor cortex, it has been demonstrated that

essentially the same cortical regions are activated in imagining and performance

activities.217 It is particularly interesting therefore that Parsons et al. have demonstrated

that performing scales and J.S. Bach’s Italian Concerto in F Major (BWV 971) from

memory elicit different patterns of brain activation.218 Performance of scales and the

concerto activated many of the same areas of the brain, but distinct areas of the brain

were also active in each condition and can be linked to specific functions regarding

performance of the particular task. Furthermore, the scientists observed a significant

amount of deactivation throughout the brain, particularly during performance of the

Bach. While differences in attention may account for some of the differences in brain

activation, it may not be the only factor. Furthermore, the results of this research seem to

216
Janata and Paroo, “Acuity of Auditory Images,” 829-44.

See E. Gerardin et al., “Partially Overlapping Neural Networks for Real and
217

Imagined Hand Movements,” Cerebral Cortex 10 (2000): 1093-1104; M. Lotze et al.,


“Activation of Cortical and Cerebellar Motor Areas During Executed and Imagined Hand
Movements: An fMRI Study,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 11 (1999): 491-501;
and C. A. Porro et al., “Primary Motor and Sensory Cortex Activation During Motor
Performance and Motor Imagery: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study,”
Journal of Neuroscience 16 (1996) 7688-98.

Lawrence M. Parsons, et al., “The Brain Basis of Piano Performance,”


218

Neuropsychologia 43 (2005): 199-215.


77

indicate that the human body is differentially involved in performing distinct musical

activities.

Attention

Many factors interact and contribute to the performance of a well-learned skill:

knowledge structures,219 memory capacities,220 problem-solving abilities,221 individual

differences,222 and attentional mechanisms supporting skill execution.223 Attentional

mechanisms incorporate “the manner in which experienced performers allocate attention

to skill processes and procedures as actual skill execution unfolds, as well as differences

M. T. Chi, P. J. Feltovich, and R. Glaser, “Categorization and Representation of


219

Physics Problems by Experts and Novices,” Cognitive Science 5 (1981): 121-52.

See W. G. Chase and H. A. Simon, “Perception in Chess,” Cognitive Psychology


220

4 (1973): 55-81; A. de Groot, Thought and Choice in Chess (The Hague, the
Netherlands: Mouton, 1978); and J. L. Starkes, and J. Deakin, “Perception in Sport: A
Cognitive Approach to Skilled Performance,” in Cognitive Sport Psychology, eds. W. F.
Straub & J. M. Williams (Lansing, MI: Sport Science Associates, 1984), 115-28.

See Priest and Lindsay, “New Light on Novice-Expert Differences,” 389-405


221

and Tenenbaum and Bar-Eli, “Decision-Making in Sport,” 171-92.

See P. L. Ackerman, “Individual Differences in Skill Learning: An Integration of


222

Psychometric and Information Processing Perspectives,” Psychological Bulletin 102


(1987): 3-27; P. L. Ackerman and A. T. Cianciolo, “Cognitive, Perceptual-Speed, and
Psychomotor Determinants of Individual Differences During Skill Acquisition,” Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Applied 6 (2000): 259-90; and R. Kanfer and P. L.
Ackerman, “Motivation and Cognitive Abilities: An Integrative/Aptitude—Treatment
Interaction Approach to Skill Acquisition,” Journal of Applied Psychology 74 (1989):
657-90.

Beilock et al., “When Paying Attention Becomes Counterproductive,” 6-16.


223
78

in the attentional requirements of low- and high-level performances.”224 Knowledge of

attentional mechanisms in skill acquisition has the potential to aid the development of the

most optimal techniques for skill acquisition225 and to help explicate suboptimal

performances of well-learned skills.226

The role of attention in skill acquisition and execution seems to be dependent

upon the three general stages of learning characterized by both qualitative differences in

cognitive structures supporting performance and differences in performance. Research

has suggested that early in learning, skill execution is supported by a set of non-

integrated control structures held in working memory and attended to in a step-by-step

fashion.227 As a result of this close attentional monitoring to skill execution, attention

Ibid., 6.
224

225
See R. N. Singer, R. Lidor, and J. H. Cauraugh, “To Be Aware or Not Aware?
What to Think About While Learning and Performing a Motor Skill,” The Sport
Psychologist 7 (1993): 19-30; G. Wulf, M. Hob, and W. Prinz, “Instructions for Motor
Learning: Differential Effects of Internal Versus External Focus of Attention,” Journal of
Motor Behavior 30 (1998): 169-79; and G. Wulf et al., “Attentional Focus in Complex
Skill Learning,” Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 71 (2000): 229-39.

226
See R. F. Baumeister, “Choking Under Pressure: Self-Consciousness and
Paradoxical Effects of Incentives on Skillful Performance,” Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology 46 (1984): 610-20; S. L. Beilock and T. H. Carr, “On the Fragility of
Skilled Performance: What Governs Choking Under Pressure?” Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General 130 (2001): 701-25; and B. Lewis and D. Linder, “Thinking About
Choking? Attentional Processes and Paradoxical Performance,” Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin 23 (1997): 937-44.

See J. R. Anderson, The Architecture of Cognition (Cambridge, MA: Harvard


227

University Press, 1983); Idem, Rules of Mind (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1993); Fitts and
Posner, Human Performance; and R. W. Proctor and A. Dutta, Skill Acquisition and
Human Performance (Thousand oaks, CA: Sage, 1995).
79

devoted to controlling task performance is largely unavailable for either interpretation or

processing of non-task related stimuli. Therefore, in the early stages of the learning

process, self-regulation may actually disrupt execution of a particular skill by recruiting

attentional resources required for executing the task.228 With practice, procedural

knowledge specific to the execution of the task develops and improves real-time skill

execution leading to automated performance.229 This procedural knowledge operates

largely outside of working memory and does not require constant control.230 As a result,

attentional mechanisms may become available for processing stimuli that are not task-

related. However, evidence suggests that performance of a second ancillary task may

prevent learning of the principal task231 and diminish performance in the principal task.232

F. H. Kanfer and M. K. Stevenson, “The Effects of Self-Regulation on


228

Concurrent Cognitive Processing,” Cognitive Therapy and Research 6 (1985): 667-84.

A. D. Fisk and W. Schneider, “Memory as a Function of Attention, Level of


229

Processing, and Automatization,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,


Memory, and Cognition 10 (1984): 181-97.

See Anderson, The Architecture of Cognition; Fitts and Posner, Human


230

Performance; S. W. Keele and J. J. Summers, “The Structure of Motor Programs,” in


Motor Control: Issues and Trends, ed. G.E. Stelmach (New York: Academic Press,
1976); G. A. Kimble and L. C. Perlmuter, “The Problem of Volition,” Psychological
Review 77 (1970): 361-84; and E. Langer, and G. Imber, “When Practice Makes
Imperfect: Debilitating Effects of Overlearning,” Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 37 (1979): 2014-24.

M. J. Nissen and P. Bullemer, “Attentional Requirements of Learning: Evidence


231

from Performance Measures,” Cognitive Psychology 19 (1987): 1-32.

R. Dell’Aqua and P. Jolicoeur, “Visual Encoding of Patterns is Subject to Dual-


232

Task Interference,” Memory & Cognition 28(2) (2000): 184-91.


80

Other studies involving musicians,233 hockey players,234 soccer players,235 and golfers236

have shown that experienced performers are better able to deal with conditions involving

dual-task environments than novices which suggests that constant online attentional

control may not be necessary once a high level of performance has been achieved. In

fact, once skills have been well-learned, attention devoted to the step-by-step components

of skill may be detrimental to the execution of the skill.237

Beilock et al., “Attention Becomes Counterproductive,” 6-16.


233

J. Leavitt, “Cognitive Demands of Skating and Stick Handling in Ice Hockey,”


234

Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Sciences 4 (1979): 46-55.

M. D. Smith and C. J. Chamberlin, “Effect of Adding Cognitively Demanding


235

Tasks on Soccer Skill Performance,” Perceptual and Motor Skills 75 (1992): 955-61.

See S. L. Beilock, S. A. Wierenga, and T. H. Carr, “Expertise, Attention, and


236

Memory in Sensorimotor Skill Execution: Impact of Novel Task Constraints on Dual-


Task Performance and Episodic Memory,” The Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology 55(4) (2002): 1211-40 and Idem, “Memory and Expertise: What Do
Experienced Athletes Remember?” in Expert Performance in Sports: Advances in
Research on Sport Expertise, ed. Janet L. Starkes and K. Anders Ericsson (Champaign, IL:
Human Kinetics, 2003), 295-320.

237
See Baumeister, “Choking Under Pressure,” 610-20; S. L. Beilock and T. H.
Carr, “Fragility of Skilled Performance,” 701-25; Kimble and Perlmuter, “The Problem of
Volition,” 361-84; Langer and Imber, “When Practice Makes Imperfect,” 2014-24; and
Lewis and Linder, “Thinking About Choking?” 937-44.
81

Masters and colleagues238 proposed that attention to high-level skills results in their
‘breakdown,’ in which the compiled real-time control structure of a skill is broken
down into a sequence of smaller, separate, independent units—similar to how
performance may have been organized early in learning. Once broken down, each unit
must be activated and run separately, which slows performance and, at each transition
between units, creates an opportunity for error that was not present in the ‘chunked’
control structure.239

Researchers have suggested that attention to step-by-step components of a well-

learned skill may be prompted by pressure to perform at a high level which in turn may

contribute to suboptimal performance of well-learned skills.240 It is apparent that novel

and well-learned tasks require different levels of attentional resources for successful

execution.

If [skill-focused attention is] applied in other circumstances, such as practice


situations, in which performers are consciously attempting to dismantle their skill and
modify certain parts in accord with data collected by self-regulatory activities such as
those mentioned above, skill-focused attention may actually be helpful. That is, when
the goal is not to maximize real-time performance but instead to explicitly alter or
change performance processes to achieve a different outcome, skill-focused attention
may be beneficial. In this manner, skill-focused attention may become embedded in
the metacognitive activities of self-regulation.241

238
See R. S. W. Masters, “Knowledge, Knerves, and Know-How: The Role of
Explicit Versus Implicit Knowledge in the Breakdown of a Complex Motor Skill Under
Pressure,” British Journal of Psychology 83 (1992): 343-58; R. S. W. Masters et al.,
“‘Reinvestment:’ A Dimension of Personality Implicated in Skill Breakdown Under
Pressure,” Personality & Individual Differences 14 (1993): 655-66.

Beilock et al., “Attention Becomes Counterproductive,” 8.


239

240
See Baumeister, “Choking Under Pressure,” 610-20; Beilock and Carr, “Fragility
of Skilled Performance,” 701-25 and Lewis and Linder, “Thinking About Choking?” 937-
44.

Beilock et al., “Attention Becomes Counterproductive,” 15.


241
82

The importance of attentional mechanisms and their training or practice implications are

obvious with compulsory attention to step-by-step components in early stages shifting to

self-regulation in later stages of skill acquisition.

Motor Learning and the Human Hand

Handedness and Facility of the Non-Dominant Hand

Over the course of history, the number of left-handers in the general population

has been estimated to be between approximately one and twenty-nine percent depending

upon the testing method.242 On the basis of performance studies, Hardyck and

Petrinovich have estimated the number to be between eight and ten percent.243 Several

studies have been conducted to determine whether or not various groups might differ in

measures of handedness from the general population. A study conducted by Oldfield244

found no differences in handedness between musicians and nonmusicians while other

studies have found left and mixed-handers to be overrepresented in musician

populations.245 A particularly interesting study by Byrne examined handedness of music

C. Hardyck and L. F. Petrinovich, “Left-Handedness,” Psychological Bulletin 84


242

(1977): 391.

Ibid.
243

R. C. Oldfield, “Handedness in Musicians,” British Journal of Psychology 60


244

(1969): 91-9.

See B. Byrne, “Handedness and Musical Ability,” British Journal of Psychology


245

65 (1974): 279-81; K. O. Göttestam, “Left-Handedness Among Students of Architecture


and Music,” Perceptual and Motor Skills 70 (1990): 1323-27; and M. Hassler and N.
Birbaumer, “Handedness, Musical Abilities, and Dihaptic and Dichotic Performance in
Adolescents: A Longitudinal Study,” Developmental Neuropsychology 4 (1988): 129-45.
83

students and found that the proportion of mixed-handedness was greater among

instrumentalists but not singers.246 Results of research by Christman also demonstrated

that populations of musicians such as string players who, according to his definition, play

instruments requiring greater bimanual coordination than others, display greater

proportion of left- and/or mixed-handedness.247 However, recent studies have shown that

the prevalence of left- or mixed-handedness in musicians is only four to six percent

higher than in nonmusicians.248 What remains more difficult to assess is whether the

observed differences amongst populations occur as musicians self-select with regard to

their chosen profession or whether developing skills necessary to play particular

instruments results in weakening the degree of one’s handedness.

Attempts to address this issue often involve studying the long-term effects of

practice on hand skill asymmetry. Research by Jäncke, Schlaug, and Steinmetz found

that right-handed musicians demonstrated a reduced degree of hand skill asymmetry in

comparison to right-handed nonmusicians.249 The reduced asymmetry demonstrated by

Byrne, “Handedness and Musical Ability,” 279-81.


246

Stephen Christman, “Handedness in Musicians: Bimanual Constraints on


247

Performance,” Brain and Cognition 22 (1993): 266-72.

248
See J. P. Aggleton, R. W. Kentridge, and J. M. M. Good, “Handedness and
Musical Ability: A Study of Professional Orchestral Players, Composers, and Choir
Members,” Psychology of Music 22 (1994): 148-56; A. N. Gilbert and C. J. Wysocki,
“Hand Preference and Age in the United States,” Neuropsychologia 30 (1992): 601-8;
and I. B. Perelle and L. Ehrman, “An International Study of Human Handedness: The
Data,” Behavior Genetics 24 (1994): 217-28.

Lutz Jäncke, Gottfried Schlaug, and Helmuth Steinmetz, “Hand Skill


249

Asymmetry in Professional Musicians,” Brain and Cognition 34 (1997): 424-32.


84

musicians was thought to be due to gains in skill of the non-dominant hand. Within

musician populations, keyboardists performed better than string players in a finger

tapping task, but better performance was not linked to performance asymmetry as the

level of asymmetry was similar in both groups. Furthermore, results indicated that hand

skill asymmetry was related to the age at musicians began their training with less

asymmetry associated with earlier training.

Bi-manual Coordination

Many daily activities, such as tying one’s shoes or pouring a glass of milk, for

example, require coordinated bimanual movements. The motion of the limbs is

coordinated with regard to each task and displays a great amount of temporal and spatial

diversity between tasks. Numerous activities, such as walking, riding bike, or swimming,

are cyclical and involve synchronized or alternated movement of muscle pairs, but other

tasks, such as tying shoelaces, opening a bottle, or playing a musical instrument, require

alternated movement of limb pairs. Interestingly enough, principles of interlimb

coordination cannot be deduced from those regarding unimanual movements.

Research indicates that biological systems often demonstrate synchronization. 250

Indeed, there is a basic tendency of limbs to operate in synchrony which results in

See S. L. Bressler and J. A. S. Kelso, “Cortical Coordination Dynamics and


250

Cognition,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5 (2001): 26-36; J. A. S. Kelso, Dynamic


Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior (Cambridge, Massachusetts:
MIT Press, 1995); and F. Varela et al., “The Brainweb: Phase Synchronization and
Large-Scale Integration,” Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 2 (2001): 229-39.
85

coordination constraints due to temporal and spatial parameters of movement.251 Even

amputees with phantom limbs maintain a tendency toward bimanual coupling.252 Many

activities, such as playing the piano, require that this synchronization be overcome.

Modes of Interlimb Coordination

Despite the fact that other modes exist, it is generally agreed that quadrupedal

animals and humans display a basic tendency toward either in-phase or anti-phase

coordination of the limbs with a prevalent one-to-one relationship in the frequency of

movements.253 In-phase coordination of the index fingers, for example, occurs when

similar muscles in the two fingers are active and the fingers mirror one another. Anti-

phase coordination occurs when nonhomologous muscles are active, when the index

fingers point in the same direction, for example. Research regarding cyclical bimanual

251
See E. A. Franz, “Spatial Coupling in the Coordination of Complex Actions,”
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A50 (1997): 684–704; J. A. S. Kelso, D.
L. Southard, and D. Goodman, “On the Coordination of Two-Handed Movements,”
Journal of Experimental. Psychology 5 (1979): 229–38; Idem, “On the Nature of Human
Interlimb Coordination,” Science 203 (1979): 1029–31; R. G. Marteniuk, C. L.
MacKenzie, and D. M. Baba, “Bimanual Movement Control: Information Processing
and Interaction Effects,” The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A36(2)
(1984): 335-65; D. E. Sherwood, “Distance and Location Assimilation Effects in Rapid
Bimanual Movement,” Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 62 (1991): 302-8;
Idem, “Hand Preference, Practice Order, and Spatial Assimilations in Rapid Bimanual
Movement,” Journal of Motor Behavior 26 (1994): 123-34; S. P. Swinnen et al.,
“Control of Bilateral Asymmetrical Movements,” Experimental Brain Research 85
(1991): 163-73; and C. B. Walter et al., “Systematic Error in the Hierarchical
Organization of Physical Action,” Cognitive Science 25 (2001): 393-422.

E. A. Franz and V. S. Ramachandran, “Bimanual Coupling in Amputees with


252

Phantom Limbs,” Nature Neuroscience 1(6) (1998): 443-4.

T. Wannier et al., “Arm to Leg Coordination in Humans During Walking,


253

Creeping and Swimming,” Experimental Brain Research 14 (2001): 375-9.


86

movements in humans has demonstrated that, not only is the in-phase mode more

accurate and stable,254 but it also requires less attention than the anti-phase mode.255 In

order to understand basic principles of interlimb coordination, scientists have generally

attempted to identify the limitations of the central nervous system by studying different

movements performed simultaneously. Swinnen indicated that “archaic response

tendencies or coordination modes that the system settles into when stressed might also

reflect what most easily potentiated pathways of neural wiring are.”256

Observed limitations involve asymmetry between the hands and spatial and

temporal features of movement. For example Ibbotson and Morton demonstrated that

when right-handed individuals tap in synchrony with a metronome while simultaneously

tapping as quickly as possible with the other hand, performance is better when the right

254
See Kelso, Dynamic Patterns; R. G. Carson, “The Dynamics of Isometric
Bimanual Coordination,” Experimental Brain Research 105 (1995): 465–76; J. A. S.
Kelso, “Phase Transitions and Critical Behavior in Human Bimanual Coordination,”
American Journal of Physiology 246 (1984): R1000–R1004; A. Semjen, J. J. Summers,
and D. Cattaert, “Hand Coordination in Bimanual Circle Drawing,” Journal of
Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance 21 (1995): 1139–57; S.
P. Swinnen, and K. Jardin, “Egocentric and Allocentric Constraints in the Expression of
Patterns of Interlimb Coordination,” Journal of Cognitive. Neuroscience 9 (1997): 348–
77; S. P. Swinnen et al., “Exploring Interlimb Constraints During Bimanual Graphic
Performance: Effects of Muscle Grouping and Direction,” Behavioural Brain Research
90 (1998): 79–87; and J. Yamanishi, M. Kawato, and R. Suzuki, “Two Coupled
Oscillators as a Model for the Coordinated Finger Tapping by Both Hands,” Biological
Cybernetics 37 (1980): 219–25.
255
J. J. Temprado et al., “Attentional Load Associated with Performing and
Stabilizing Preferred Bimanual Patterns,” Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human
Perception and Performance 25 (1999): 1579–94.

S. P. Swinnen, “Intermanual Coordination: From Behavioural Principles to


256

Neural-Network Interactions,” Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 3 (2002): 353.


87

hand performs the faster-paced tapping.257 Spatial constraints are observed in drawing

lines of different amplitudes as there is a tendency for the amplitudes of the lines to

become similar.258 Subjects in whom the corpus callosum has been severed have less

difficulty in producing simultaneous movements with unique directional requirements

than normal subjects.259 Perhaps somewhat surprisingly, in finger tapping, these subjects

demonstrate temporal coupling during movements such as finger tapping;260 however,

when tasks become more complicated and require coordination beyond basic in-phase

coupling, they do not perform as well.261

257
N. R. Ibbotson, and J. Morton, “Rhythm and Dominance,” Cognition 9 (1981):
125-38.

See Marteniuk, MacKenzie, and Baba, “Bimanual Movement Control,” 335-65;


258

Sherwood, “Distance and Location,” 302-8; Idem, “Rapid Bimanual Movement,” 123-34;
H. Heuer et al., “The Time-Course of Cross-Talk During the Simultaneous Specification
of Bimanual Movement Amplitudes,” Experimental Brain Research 118 (1998): 381-92;
and W. Spijkers and H. Heuer, “Structural Constraints on the Performance of
Symmetrical Bimanual Movements with Different Amplitudes,” Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology A48 (1995): 716-40.

259
See J. C. Eliassen, K. Baynes, and M. S. Gazzaniga, “Direction Information
Coordinated Via the Posterior Third of the Corpus Callosum During Bimanual
Movements,” Experimental Brain Research 128 (1999): 573-77; Idem, “Anterior and
Posterior Callosal Contributions to Simultaneous Bimanual Movements of the Hands and
Fingers,” Brain 123 (2000): 2501-11; and E. A. Franz et al., “Dissociation of Spatial and
Temporal Coupling in the Bimanual Movements of Callosotomy Patients,” Psychological
Science 7 (1996): 306-10.

B. Tuller and J. A. S. Kelso, “Environmentally-Specified Patterns of Movement


260

Coordination in Normal and Split-Brain Subjects,” Experimental Brain Research 75


(1989): 306-16.

See B. Preilowski, “Possible Contribution of the Anterior Forebrain


261

Sommissures to Bimanual Motor Coordination,” Neuropsychologia 10 (1972): 267-77


and Idem, “Bilateral Motor Interaction: Perceptual-Motor Performance of Partial and
88

Finger Tapping and Polyrhythms

Studies involving finger tapping have often been used to study bimanual

coordination. In these studies, a distinction is often made between simple rhythms, those

in which one frequency is an integer multiple of another, and polyrhythms, such as 3:2 or

5:4. In general, research has shown that polyrhythms are more difficult to produce and

are produced with greater variability than simple rhythms.262 Early studies involving

polyrhythms required participants to play two to six notes per second and showed that, at

those speeds, participants were able to keep up with the beat263 or even anticipate the

beat.264 However, it appears that higher-order ratios comprised of large numerators and

denominators are less stable than lower-order ratios. “One reason why musicians must

Complete ‘Split-Brain’ Patients,” in Cerebral Localization, ed. K. J. Zulch, O.


Creutzfeld, and G. C. Galbraith (New York: Springer, 1975), 115-32.

262
See Deutsch, “Sequences in Parallel,” 331-37; R. J. Jagacinski et al., “Test of
Parallel Versus Integrated Structure in Polyrhythmic Tapping,” Journal of Motor
Behavior 20 (1988): 416-42; S. Klapp et al., “On Marching to Two Different Drummers:
Perceptual Aspects of the Difficulties,” Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human
Perception and Performance 11 (1985): 814-27; C. E. Peper, P. J. Beek, and P. C. W.
van Wieringen, “Multifrequency Coordination in Bimanual Tapping: Asymmetrical
Coupling and Signs of Supercriticality,” Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human
Perception and Performance 19 (1993): 416-28; and P. J. Treffner and M. T. Turvey,
“Resonance Constraints on Rhythmic Movements,” Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Human Perception and Performance 19(6) (1993): 1221-37.

See J. Pressing, J. Summers, and J. Magill, “Cognitive Multiplicity in


263

Polyrhythmic Pattern Performance,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human


Perception and Performance 22(5) (1996): 1127-48 and J. Summers et al., “Productions
of Polyrhythms,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Performance 19(2) (1993): 416-28.

R. J. Jagacinski et al., “Tests of Parallel Versus Integrated Structure in


264

Polyrhythmic Tapping,” Journal of Motor Behavior 20(4) (1988): 416-42.


89

practice so-called difficult rhythms stems from this differential stability.”265 As the

frequency of tapping is increased, a transition from higher-order ratios to lower-order

ratios is observed.266 Although two studies have obtained results contrary to the

following,267 Peters and Schwartz268 and Klapp et al.269 have demonstrated that the hands

are not independent of one another even at intermediate speeds. Furthermore,

performance of multi-frequency tasks is often associated with an asymmetrical coupling

effect in which the fast hand has a larger influence on the slow hand than vice versa.270

Nevertheless, musically trained subjects are more accurate in performance of

P. G. Zanone and J. A. S. Kelso, “Evolution of Behavioral Attractors With


265

Learning: Nonequilibrium Phase Transitions,” Journal of Experimental Psychology:


Human Perception and Performance 18(2) (1992): 418.

See Ibid. and C. E. Peper, P. J. Beek, and P. C. W. van Wieringen, “Bifurcation


266

in Polyrhythmic Tapping: In Search of Farey Principles,” in Tutorials in Motor


Neuroscience, ed. G.E. Stelmach (New York: Kluwer, 1991): 413-31.

See R. T. Krampe, et al., “The Fast and Slow of Skilled Bimanual Rhythm
267

Production: Parallel Versus Integrated Timing,” Journal of Experimental Psychology:


Human Perception and Performance 26(1) (2000): 206-33 and L. H. Shaffer,
“Performances of Chopin, Bach and Bartok: Studies in Motor Programming,” Cognitive
Psychology 13 (1981): 326-76.

268
Peters and Schwartz, “Coordination of the Two Hands,” 215-24.

S. T. Klapp, J. M. Nelson, and R. J. Jagacinski, “Can People Tap Concurrent


269

Bimanual Rhythms Independently?” Journal of Motor Behavior 30(4) (1998): 301-22.


270
See Ibid.; Summers et al., “Productions of Polyrhythms,” 416-28; W. D.
Byblow et al., “Performance Asymmetries and Coupling Dynamics in the Acquisition of
Multifrequency Bimanual Coordination,” Psychological Research 61 (1998): 56-70; M.
Peters, “Constraints in the Coordination of Bimanual Movements and Their Expression in
Skilled and Unskilled Subjects,” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A37
(1985): 171-96; and Peters and Schwartz, “Coordination of the Two Hands,” 215-224.
90

polyrhythms than non-musicians271 which suggests the skill is amenable to practice.

Researchers have demonstrated that the ability to play the correct rhythm apparently

varies with one’s skill level,272 focus of attention,273 and the complexity of the rhythm.

Subsequent research has focused on the stability of the performance of polyrhythms and

has attempted to resolve such as issues as what happens when motor control breaks

down.274

Bi-manual Transfer

Different kinds of transfer of learning are known to occur after the acquisition of a

motor task, ranging from transfer of relational to specific parameters of control.275 One

aspect that has received significant attention since the early twentieth century is bilateral

transfer. Early studies of bilateral transfer often used tasks like handwriting, drawing,

and maze learning.276 Criticism of this early body of work centered around the fact that,

271
Summers et al. “Productions of Polyrhythms,” 416-28.

272
Idem.

273
Pressing, Summers, and Magill, “Cognitive Multiplicity,” 1127-48.

274
Bogacz, “Performance of Polyrhythms,” 21-34.

D. E. Young and R. A. Schmidt, “Transfer of Movement Control in Motor Skill


275

Learning,” in Transfer of Learning: Contemporary Research and Applications, ed. S. M.


Cornier and J. D. Hagman (San Diego, Academic Press, 1987), 47-79.

276
See T. W. Cook, “Studies in Cross Education: III. Kinesthetic Learning of an
Irregular Pattern,” Journal of Experimental Psychology 17 (1934): 749-62; E. Weig,
“Bilateral Transfer in the Motor Learning of Young Children and Adults,” Child
Development 3 (1932): 247-67; and R. B. Ammons, C. H. Ammons, and R. L. Morgan,
“Transfer of Skill and Decremental Factors Along the Speed Dimensions in Rotary
Pursuit,” Perceptual and Motor Skills 6 (1956): 43.
91

in general, the motor component of the tasks was not particularly demanding. Critics

therefore suggested that any observed transfer of learning could largely be attributed to

subjects’ acquisition of a cognitive representation of the structure of the task that could

then be transferred to the other hand in order to reproduce the skill.

More recent contributions to the literature provide additional support277 and some

limitations for bilateral effects.278 More complicated tasks and those requiring greater

motor skill have since provided some insight. For example, in a study by Shapiro,

subjects learned a complex wrist-rotation task in which they were required to move to

seven ordered target positions in a particular amount of time.279 Over the course of five

days, subjects practiced the task with their right hand and received feedback from a

computer screen after each trial. Results over the five days changed dramatically as a

result of practice. On the fifth day, the subjects were unexpectedly asked to perform the

same task but with the left hand. Results indicated that the left- and right-hand

movements were nearly identical therefore demonstrating a high degree of transfer from

the right to the left hand. These results were particularly interesting since the same

See P. Dunham, “Effect of Bilateral Transfer on Coincidence/Anticipation


277

Performance,” Research Quarterly 48 (1977): 51-5; S. L. Puretz, “Bilateral Transfer:


The Effects of Practice on the Transfer of Complex Dance Movement Patterns,”
Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 54 (1983): 48-54; and K. Tsuji and Y. Ide,
“Development of Bilateral Transfer Skills in the Mirror Tracing,” Japanese
Psychological Research 16 (1974): 171-8.

R. E. Hicks, J. M. Frank, and M. Kinsbourne, “The Locus of Bimanual Skill


278

Transfer,” The Journal of General Psychology 107 (1982): 277-81.

D. C. Shapiro, “Bilateral Transfer of a Motor Program.” Paper presented at the


279

AAHPER annual meeting, March, 1977.


92

direction of movement was required of each limb therefore employing non-homologous

muscle groupings. Young and Schmidt interpret Shapiro’s results as suggesting “a

relatively abstract program structure that can employ various limb systems in producing a

response.”280 Previous studies by Bray,281 Raibert,282 and Merton283 support this view.

Bray demonstrated transfer effects of mirror-tracing performance from the hand to the

foot while Raibert’s research showed similarities in movement patterns when writing

with the dominant hand, the dominant arm, the nondominant hand, with the pen between

his teeth, and with the pen taped to his foot. Likewise, Merton demonstrated the

similarities between signatures of different sizes despite the difference in muscles

involved.

Completely specific acquisition of motor skills or perfect transfer of learning,

however, is rarely observed.284 Several studies have demonstrated that the subdominant

hand benefits more from dominant hand training than does the dominant hand from

280
Young and Schmidt, “Transfer of Movement Control,” 62.

C. W. Bray, “Transfer of Learning,” Journal of Experimental Psychology 11


281

(1928): 443-67.

M. H. Raibert, “Motor Control and Learning by the State-Space Model,” (Ph.D.


282

diss., Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 1977).

P. A. Merton, “How We Control the Contraction of Our Muscles,” Scientific


283

American 226 (1972): 30-7.

L. A. Teixeira, “Timing and Force Components in Bilateral Transfer of


284

Learning,” Brain and Cognition 44 (2000): 455.


93

subdominant hand training.285 To assess the effects of practice on performance with

regard to unimanual and bimanual movements, Schulze, et al.286 used a variant of the

pegboard task, a task widely used in neuropsychology for the examination of complex

uni- and bimanual motor functions.287 Results showed strong practice effects for each

hand and for each training condition. Of particular interest were the effects for the

untrained hands.

After training, movement times were considerably reduced for all hands and for all
training conditions. 2. Practice effects were found for the hand trained and also for the
untrained hand. 3. There was not a great difference in the size of the practice effects
for the right hand after left hand training or for the left hand after right hand training.
4. Task difficulty had no clear influence on the practice effect. 5. Bimanual
movements not only profit from bimanual training but also from unimanual training
and conversely unimanual movements benefit from bimanual training. However,
unimanual training resulted in strong practice effects for the bimanual tasks. However,
the practice effect of unimanual training on bimanual movements was smaller than the
practice effect of bimanual training on bimanual movements.288

285
See U. Halsband, “Left Hemisphere Preponderance in Trajectorial Learning,”
NeuroReport 3 (1992): 397-400; J. L. Laszlo, R. A. Baguley, and P. J. Bairstow,
“Bilateral Transfer in Tapping Skill in the Absence of Peripheral Information,” Journal
of Motor Behavior 2 (1970): 261-71; R. Milisen and C. V. Riper., “Differential Transfer
of Training in a Rotary Activity,” Journal of Experimental Psychology 24 (1939): 640-6;
and S. E. Parlow and M. Kinsbourne, “Asymmetrical Transfer of Training Between
Hands: Implications for Interhemispheric Communication in Normal Brain,” Brain and
Cognition 11 (1989): 98-113.

K. Schulze, E. Lüders, and L. Jäncke, “Intermanual Transfer in a Simple Motor


286

Task,” Cortex 38 (2002): 805-15.

See M. Annett, Left, Right, Hand and Brain: The Right Shift Theory (London:
287

Erlbaum, 1985) and A. L. Doyen and M. Carlier, “Measuring Handedness: A Validation


Study of Bishop’s Reading Card Test,” Laterality 7 (2002): 115-30.

Schulze, Lüders, and Jäncke, “Intermanual Transfer,” 805.


288
94

Although mechanisms underlying transfer of learning have yet to have been

identified, the body of evidence supporting bilateral transfer is significant. Most

importantly, the mounting body of research enhances our ability to optimize training

regiments, teaching methodologies, and ultimately performance.

Musical Skills and Acquisition of New Coordination Patterns

Numerous factors including hand dominance289, physiological functionality,290 and

training and attention291 have been shown to influence coordination patterns. The effect

of attention was studied by Peters and Schwartz who required subjects to count aloud one

of the streams in a 3:2 polyrhythm.292 Results indicated that both musically skilled and

unskilled subjects performed better when attending to the faster of the two streams.

However, a study by Pressing et al. demonstrated that expert percussionists were able to

perform a 4:3 polyrhythm with a high degree of accuracy regardless of which hand

P. J. Treffner and M. T. Turvey, “Handedness and the Asymmetric Dynamics


289

of Bimanual Rhythmic Coordination,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human


Perception and Performance 23(1) (1997): 1-15.

See P. J. Beek, C. E. Peper, and A. Daffertshofer, “Modeling Rhythmic


290

Interlimb Coordination: Beyond the Kahen-Kelso-Bunz Model,” Brain and Cognition 48


(2002): 149-65 and C. L. Peper, et al., “Explanatory Limitations of the HKB Model:
Incentives for a Two-Tiered Model of Rhythmic Interlimb Coordination,” Human
Movement Science 23 (2004): 673-97.

See Zanone and Kelso, “Evolution of Behavioral Attractors with Learning,” 403-
291

21; Klapp, Nelson, and Jagacinski, “Can People Tap,” 301-22; and J. Temprado et al.,
“Attentional Demands Reflect Learning-Induced Alterations of Bimanual Coordination
Dynamics,” European Journal of Neuroscience 16 (2002): 1390-4.

292
Peters and Schwartz, “Coordination of the Two Hands,” 215-24.
95

performed which stream or which stream was the focus of attention.293 Summers

suggested that the percussionists’ performance was perhaps a result of their training

which requires the learning of a given polyrhythm in three ways: one stream as the

fundamental pulse, the other stream as the fundamental pulse, and as a linear composite

of the two.294

Summers et al. also examined the potential for transfer effects regarding practice

of polyrhythms.295 Subjects who practiced a 5:3 polyrhythm were then asked to perform

the polyrhythm under a variety of conditions: reverse-hand arrangement, as fast as

possible, and with a concurrent memory task. Additionally, subjects were asked to

perform previously unrehearsed 3:2, 5:2, 4:3, and 5:4 polyrhythms. Results indicated that

the polyrhythms were performed with a high degree of accuracy in within-transfer

conditions, but transfer to tasks involving the new polyrhythms was poor.296

Because coordinated patterns have been shown to be amenable to training, the

mechanisms underlying learning and performance are of great interest to scientists and

those in associated health-related fields. Acquisition of new coordination patterns is

often considered in relationship to modes of coordination that have been previously

Pressing, Summers, and Magill, “Cognitive Multiplicity,” 1127-48.


293

J. Summers, “Practice and Training in Bimanual Coordination Tasks:


294

Strategies and Constraints,” Brain and Cognition 48 (2002): 173.

Summers et al., “Production of Polyrhythms,” 416-28.


295

J. J. Summers, “Learning and Transfer of Multifrequency Patterns.” In Rhythm


296

Perception and Production, ed. P. Desain and L. Windsor (Lisse: Swets and Zeitlinger,
2000): 69-80.
96

discussed.297 Swinnen suggests that the nature of pre-existing preferred patterns and the

tendency toward phase and frequency synchronization can often account for difficulties

in learning new coordination patterns.298 This implies that learning coordination skills

may involve two components: overcoming basic action patterns299 and re-integrating

action patterns. Efficiency in formation of the neural connections associated with the

new action patterns is dependent upon both extrinsic and intrinsic conditions. For

example, differences between individuals may be the result of genetic predisposition for

neural plasticity.300 On the other hand, it has been suggested that instructional techniques

have the ability to enhance the learning process. This is particularly true when the
297
See Kelso, Dynamic Patterns; Zanone and Kelso, “The Evolution of Behavioral
Attractors with Learning,” 403-21; S. Swinnen et al., “Preferred and Induced
Coordination Modes During the Acquisition of Bimanual Movements with a 2:1
Frequency Ratio,” Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and
Performance 23 (1997): 1087-1110; S. P. Swinnen, “Age-Related Deficits in Motor
Learning and Differences in Feedback Processing During the Production of a Bimanual
Coordination Pattern,” Cognitive Neuropsychology 15 (1998): 439-66; and P. Zanone
and J. Kelso, “Coordination Dynamics of Learning and Transfer: Collective and
Component Levels,” Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and
Performance 23 (1997): 1454-80.

Swinnen, “Intermanual Coordination,” 355.


298

299
See T. D. Lee, S. P. Swinnen, and S. Verschueren, “Relative Phase Alterations
During Bimanual Skill Acquisition,” Journal of Motor Behavior 27 (1995): 263-74; G.
Schöner et al., “Learning as Change of Coordination Dynamics: Theory and Experiment,”
Journal of Motor Behavior 24 (1992): 29-48; S. P. Swinnen and C. B. Walter,
“Constraints in Coordinating Limb Movements,” in Cognition and Action in Skilled
Behaviour, ed. A. M. Colley and J. R. Beech (Amsterdam: North-Holland, 1988), 127-
43; S. P. Swinnen et al., “Acquiring Bimanual Skills: Contrasting Forms of Information
Feedback for Interlimb Decoupling,” Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning,
Memory, and Cognition 19 (1993): 1328-44; and Zanone, and Kelso, “The Evolution of
Behavioral Attractors with Learning,” 403-21.

S. P. Swinnen, “Intermanual Coordination,” 355.


300
97

desired action is conceptualized as a familiar symbol or entity301 or when feedback

techniques integrate information from the respective limbs in a meaningful way.302

E. A. Franz et al., “Spatial Conceptual Influences on the Coordination of


301

Bimanual Actions: When a Dual Task Becomes a Single Task,” Journal of Motor
Behavior 33 (2001): 103-12.

See S. P. Swinnen et al., “Preferred and Induced Coordination Modes,” 1087-


302

1110; Swinnen, “Age-Related Deficits,” 439-66; Zanone, and Kelso, “The Evolution of
Behavioral Attractors with Learning,” 403-21; Lee, Swinnen, and Verschueren, “Relative
Phase Alterations,” 263-74, and F. Mechsner et al., “Perceptual Basis of Bimanual
Coordination,” Nature 414 (2001): 69-73.
98

CHAPTER 5

THE CHOPIN ETUDES AND SKILL ACQUISITION

Whiteside observed that, “each Etude simply highlights one aspect of the

technical equipment; each Etude uses all of the equipment all the time.”303 If one studies

the Chopin études for the purpose of developing particular aspects of technique, how

does one learn to utilize the “technical equipment” to perform them successfully and to

realize the artistic and physical ideals consistent with Chopin’s philosophy? Depending

upon the motor skills one has developed, studying the études could involve learning to

perform novel movements and/or enhancing the coordination of existing patterns of

movement. To that end, when combined with musical intuition and basic physiological

knowledge, Chopin’s interpretive markings may be of particular interest and help to the

pianist. Upon close examination of the dynamic and articulation markings in particular, I

believe that Chopin’s markings are not only interpretive but also suggest particular

movements and gestures that facilitate playing the études. The following is a discussion

of particular technical aspects and possible physical interpretation of Chopin’s stylistic

markings in representative études.

Force Gradation:

Chopin Etudes Op. 10, Nos. 3, 6, and 11; Op. 25, No. 3; and KK II b/3 Nr. 2

Gradation of force is a vital component of playing the piano in terms of dynamics,

color, and musical line, and it appears to be a particularly integral component of Chopin

Whiteside, Mastering the Chopin Etudes, 109-10.


303
99

Etudes Op. 10, Nos. 3, 6, and 11 (see Figures 1, 2, and 3); Op. 25, No. 3 (see Figure 4);

and KK II b/3 Nr. 2 (see Figure 5).

Figure 1. Etude Op. 10, No. 3, Measures 1-4. Matching tone and shaping the melodic
line in the uppermost voice while integrating inner voices of the accompaniment in the
right hand. (Quoted from Frédéric Chopin, Sämtliche Etüden, hg. Von Paul Badura-
Skoda, Wiener Urtext Edition, Wien 1973/2005 (UT 50205). Printed by permission).

Figure 2. Etude Op. 10, No. 6, Measures 1-3. Relatively slow tempo, long melodic
notes, and intricate figuration marked “sempre legatissimo” in the left hand. (Copyright
1983 G. Henle Verlag, Munich. Printed by permission.)
100

Figure 3. Etude Op. 10, No. 11, Measures 1-3. “Voicing” the uppermost voice to project
the melodic line amidst the harmonic support of the other voices. (Copyright 1983 G.
Henle Verlag, Munich. Printed by permission.)

Figure 4. Etude Op. 25, No. 3, Measures 1-3. Parallel and contrary motion between the
hands. Small melodic germs requiring one to think of the longer phrase as well as the
slur markings. (Copyright 1983 G. Henle Verlag, Munich. Printed by permission.)

Figure 5. Nouvelles Etudes KK II b/3 Nr. 2, Measures 1-4. Voicing the melody amongst
chords in close proximity requiring subtle and seamless changes of fingering. (Copyright
1983 G. Henle Verlag, Munich. Printed by permission.)
101

The tempo indication for Op. 10, No. 6 is Andante. The étude requires legato

touch from note to note and mentally “thinking through” the tone in longer notes to

facilitate matching intensity and the perception of a seamless melodic line (see Figure 2).

Legato and staccato need redefining as they are related to piano playing. For singer
and string player, connecting tones produce an entirely different effect from the
connection of piano tones. The actual holding of the tone with the breath or the bow is
an expression of the emotional reaction involved with the music, because the dynamics
are controlled throughout the holding process. This is not so with the piano. . . . Any
projection of emotion, through the use of dynamics, must deal with the intensity of
tones at the moment of production. It is this relation of the intensity of tones at the
moment of their inception that almost entirely conveys the feeling of legato. Thus,
tonal relationships are more meaningful at the piano not through key connection but
through gradation in the energy used at the moment of tone production.304

The left hand is marked “sempre legatissimo” in accompanying the long notes of

the right, and the phrase markings highlight the importance of approaching the figures

with the largest mechanism possible, not just the fingers. A particularly interesting facet

is the parallel motion between the hands in executing the sixteenth notes and contrary

motion in the subsequent eighth notes.

Op. 10, No. 11 requires voicing the arpeggiations in each hand such that the

highest voice “speaks” melodically above the harmonic support of the inner voices (see

Figure 3). The staccato articulation marks over the first eighth notes of the measure

encourage a release of tension and initiate preparation for the shift of position that

follows. While the dots may also suggest a particular color of tone, they do not affect the

duration of the notes since the damper pedal is depressed. This supports the view that the

dots have specific implications with regard to physical execution. Voicing subsequent

Ibid., 183-4.
304
102

chords is facilitated if one truly heeds the phrase marking over each gesture indicating the

balance of the hand be shifted toward the little finger.

The middle, ring, and little fingers execute the melodic line in Op. 25, No. 3 (see

Figure 4). The suggested fingerings place the middle finger on the first note of each

three-note musical germ. The slur markings over each of these groups suggest a

continuous pattern of wrist movement beginning with a drop of the wrist initiating the

phrase with the middle finger followed by progressive release of the wrist.

Voicing the melody in KK II b/3 Nr. 2 is a particular challenge amid the close

proximity of the chords (see Figure 5). Furthermore, the phrase markings indicate

musical ideas at least four measures in length. Attention to the phrasing involves caring

for the continuity of the melodic line which incorporates repeated notes that might be

played with the same finger.

Etude Op. 10, No. 3

Op. 10, No. 3 requires matching tone from note to note and shaping the melodic

line in the uppermost voice while also integrating inner voices of the accompaniment in

the right hand (see Figure 1). Employing fingerings suggested by Chopin involves using

the same finger on consecutives notes and, at times, crossing the little finger under the

ring finger (see Figure 6).305 Although Chopin’s approach to fingering shocked many of

his contemporaries, he felt that certain fingerings made it possible to achieve particular

colors of sound. Associating particular qualities with each finger, he encouraged

Frédéric Chopin, Etüden, (Munich: G. Henle Verlag, 1983), 22-5.


305
103

cultivation of these differences.306 “There are, then, many different qualities of sound,

just as there are several fingers. The point is to utilize the differences; and this, in other

words, is the art of fingering.”307

Figure 6. Etude Op. 10, No. 3, Measures 5-8. Fingerings suggested by the composer use
the same finger on consecutives notes and, at times, crossing the little finger under the
ring finger. Note the use of crescendo and decrescendo in m. 5 coinciding with the ring
and little fingers. (Quoted from Frédéric Chopin, Sämtliche Etüden, hg. Von Paul
Badura-Skoda, Wiener Urtext Edition, Wien 1973/2005 (UT 50205). Printed by
permission).

Finger substitutions and crossing fingers make it possible to physically connect

the melodic notes, but a sensitive ear and the ability to implement what the mind hears is

necessary to achieve the illusion of legato. The dynamic markings indicating subtle

swells and shapes encourage the ring and little fingers, those fingers which are the least

individuated and typically the weakest functionally, to play with more energy thereby

Marianne Ullyot, “Chopin and Liszt: A Legacy of Teaching,” Journal of the


306

American Liszt Society 10 (1981): 40.

James Huneker, Chopin; The Man and His Music (New York: C. Scribner’s
307

Sons, 1900), 53.


104

utilizing and accounting for some of the inherent physical differences amongst the fingers

(see Figure 6.)

Phrase markings and accents also provide clues to execution. The accent over the

quarter note in the right hand of m. 7, for example, implies a drop of the wrist and some

amount of energy initiating the ensuing grace note and sixteenths in the same measure.

Finger Independence: Chopin Etudes Op. 10, Nos. 2, 4, 8 and 12

With those by Chopin, pianists encounter études devoted to both the development

of finger independence and the ability to grade force. A particularly interesting

phenomenon is the apparent tradeoff between accuracy in force production and finger

independence.308 In other words, finger independence seems to diminish as accuracy in

force production increases.

Chopin Etudes Op. 10, Nos. 2, 4, 8, and 12 focus explicitly on the overall

development of finger independence. Op. 10, No. 2 is noteworthy for aiming to develop

independence of the little, middle, and ring fingers, those fingers that have been

demonstrated to be the least independent (see Figure 7).309 The fingerings Chopin

indicated entail crossing the middle finger over both the ring and little fingers and

crossing the ring finger over the little finger in ascending passages while descending

passages involved alternating either the ring and little finger or the ring and middle

Chiang et al., “Practice-Related Modulations,” 1033-43.


308

Häger-Ross and Schieber, “Quantifying the Independence,” 8542-50.


309
105

finger.310 Op. 10, No. 4 features scale-like passagework and intricate four-note finger

patterns facilitated by wrist and forearm rotation (see Figure 8).

Figure 7. Etude Op. 10, No. 2, Measures 1-2. Fingerings Chopin indicated entail
crossing the middle finger over both the ring and little fingers and crossing the ring finger
over the little finger in ascending passages. Descending passages involve alternating
either the ring and little finger or the ring and middle finger. (Copyright 1983 G. Henle
Verlag, Munich. Printed by permission.)

Figure 8. Etude Op. 10, No. 4, Measures 1-2. Scale-like passagework and intricate four-
note finger patterns facilitated by wrist and forearm rotation. (Copyright 1983 G. Henle
Verlag, Munich. Printed by permission.)

Op. 10 No. 12, emphasizing the use of the left hand in melodic passagework, will

be discussed in greater detail in subsequent sections of this chapter.

Chopin, Etüden, 18-21.


310
106

Etude Op. 10, No. 8

Four-note arpeggio figures incorporating step-wise links dominate Op. 10, No. 8

(see Figure 9). The accents occurring on each beat suggest a drop of the wrist on the

middle or ring fingers or the thumb depending on the direction of the melodic line and the

particular pattern (see Figure 10). In addition to the phrase marks over each ascending

and descending figure, the implied use of the wrist suggests use of larger mechanisms in

addition to the fingers, coordination that will facilitate speed and help prevent fatigue.

Additionally, the phrase markings direct the ear and upper arm to care for the longer line.

In this case, one must coordinate finger independence with upper arm motion in addition

to wrist and forearm rotation. Some rotation occurs as consecutive fingers are played. A

pattern of rotation is also initiated in places where the figuration is more tightly

constructed (see Figure 11, mm. 27-28). In these instances, the rotation occurs more

quickly and frequently than in mm. 1-2, for example.

Figure 9. Etude Op. 10, No. 8, Measures 1-2. Four-note arpeggio figures incorporating
step-wise links. Note the accents on each beat and the phrase markings over each
descending and ascending gesture. (Copyright 1983 G. Henle Verlag, Munich. Printed
by permission.)
107

Figure 10. Etude Op. 10, No. 8, Measures 8-13. Accents corresponding to drops of the
wrist on the middle finger and thumb. (Copyright 1983 G. Henle Verlag, Munich.
Printed by permission.)

Figure 11. Etude Op. 10, No. 8, Measures 26-29. Figuration requiring more rapid and
frequent wrist and forearm rotation. (Copyright 1983 G. Henle Verlag, Munich. Printed
by permission.)
108

Left Hand Facility: Chopin Etude Op. 10, No. 12

The technical challenges of Op. 10, No. 12 are largely constructed by way of

elaboration and alteration of three primary figures based on scales and arpeggios. The

first pattern, spanning nearly three octaves in two measures, is a decorated arpeggiation

of the opening G dominant seventh-chord in the right hand (see Figure 12). The second

pattern, arpeggiation of the tonic triad and added ninth (see Figure 13), establishes the

tonic key while the final pattern, an intricate chromatic scale (see Figure 14), ushers a

return to the tonic key and principal theme.

Figure 12. Etude Op. 10, No. 12, Measures 1-3. Decorated arpeggiation of the opening
G dominant seventh-chord in the right hand. (Copyright 1983 G. Henle Verlag, Munich.
Printed by permission.)

Figure 13. Etude Op. 10, No. 12, Measures 10-12. Arpeggiation of the tonic triad and
added ninth establishing the tonic key. (Copyright 1983 G. Henle Verlag, Munich.
Printed by permission.)
109

Figure 14. Etude Op. 10, No. 12, Measures 13-18. Intricate chromatic scale ushering a
return to the tonic key and principal theme. (Copyright 1983 G. Henle Verlag, Munich.
Printed by permission.)

Etude Op. 10, No. 12, mm. 1-2

Two-joint and multi-joint muscles are incapable of shortening to the extent

required to produce a full range of motion at all joints crossed simultaneously.311 For

example, when the wrist is flexed, the fingers are able to generate only a fraction of their

maximal force. Altering the position of the wrist also initiates subtle changes in the use

and coordination of the physical mechanisms which helps prevent fatigue.

The accents over the sixteenth notes which lie on each beat in mm. 1-2 (see

Figure 12) are significant because they suggest a drop in the wrist which is important. As

Whiteside noted in her discussion of the études, the wrist has the ability to facilitate such

Susan, J. Hall, Basic Biomechanics, 4th ed. (New York: McGraw-Hill, 2003),
311

160-1.
110

elaborated passagework.312 Because the muscles responsible for flexing the fingers cross

several joints, finger function is affected by the position of the joints. Here, led perhaps

by the upper arm, the wrist can carry the hand and allow the fingers to articulate while

not being solely responsible for depressing each key. To that end, as mentioned

previously, while a particular part of the physical mechanism is highlighted in each étude,

the ingredients remain the same. It is therefore often the balance in integrating the

mechanisms that facilitates technical execution.

Etude Op. 10, No. 12, m. 9

The accompaniment pattern initiated in m. 9 requires crossing both the ring and

middle fingers over the thumb. “To facilitate passing the thumb under the other fingers

and passing the fingers over, the hand was to be given a corresponding turn inward.”313

The figure is then varied to suit the harmonic progression in mm. 14-16 (see Figure 14).

Phrase markings here suggest the association of a single fluid physical gesture

incorporating rotation from the little finger to the thumb and back with each arpeggio

figure. The gesture is then varied as the patterns last either for two or four beats.

Thematic material

While the challenges of the left hand are formidable, pianists must not allow the

technical difficulties to eclipse the thematic substance of the right hand. “One of the

commonest experiences in the concert hall, during a performance of an Étude, is hearing

one hand played in a scintillating manner while the other one is insensitive and

312
Whiteside, Mastering the Chopin Etudes, 102.

Mikuli, Vorwort to Fr. Chopin’s Pianoforte-Werke, 3.


313
111

pedestrian. The technical problems have been solved—the music has been lost.”314 To

that end, Chopin was exact in his performance and insistence of dynamic shading and

nuance in both hands. “In performance you should develop an ample, full and rounded

tone; shade the scale of nuances with infinite gradations between pianissimo and

fortissimo, though in pianissimo avoid any indistinct muttering, just as in fortissimo avoid

the sort of pounding that would hurt a sensitive ear.”315 Executing the dynamics indicated

in mm. 62-64 requires not only careful monitoring of the sound from note to note or

chord to chord (see Figure 15), but managing separate scenarios for the hands as well.

For example, the left hand patterns are based on continuous cycles of crescendo and

decrescendo occurring either once or twice per measure whereas the right hand often

observes dynamics opposite those of the left hand. This coordination would likely be

developed in the initial phases of learning the étude in which slow performance is heavily

reliant upon sensory guidance. Finally, it is worth noting that practice at different

dynamic levels, which would require various levels of force, is written into the étude to

some extent. For example, the principal theme first appears forte in mm. 10-11 while it is

designated piano in mm. 20-21 (see Figures 13 and 16). Presumably, these differences

would be reflected in the corresponding accompaniment figures as well. Properties of

muscle are dependent upon a balance of characteristics to facilitate such variety.

Prostakoff and Rosoff, eds., Mastering the Chopin Etudes, 105.


314

Koczalski, Frédéric Chopin. Betrachtungen, Skizzen, Analysen, 12.


315
112

Figure 15. Etude Op. 10, No. 12, Measures 61-66. Dynamics requiring careful
monitoring of the sound from note to note or chord to chord and managing separate
scenarios for the hands. (Copyright 1983 G. Henle Verlag, Munich. Printed by
permission.)

Figure 16. Etude Op. 10, No. 12, Measures 19-21. Thematic material marked piano in
comparison to m. 10 where it is marked forte. (Copyright 1983 G. Henle Verlag,
Munich. Printed by permission.)

The Thumb: Chopin Etude Op. 10, No. 5

Eigeldinger suggests that melodic use of the thumb on the black keys in Op. 10,

No. 5 is one of the significant technical innovations of the Chopin études. Op. 10, No. 5

requires integrating forearm and wrist rotation with articulation of the thumb and fingers

to achieve facile execution and brilliant tone quality. In particular, in mm. 23-26, for
113

example, it is clear from the phrase markings that Chopin suggested use of wrist and

forearm rotation to play either adjacent notes or those separated by a seventh, the seventh

between the A-flat and high G-flat in this case (see Figure 17). The articulation markings

over each G-flat imply a particular quality of sound but also a release of muscle tension

in the hand and forearm preparing the playing mechanism for the next gesture. Surely

one must balance use of the fingers as articulators as well, but use of the wrist and

forearm will facilitate speed and ease. Because this étude is in the key of G-flat major,

the pianist must become comfortable executing movements primarily on the black keys

with the hands farther from the body and the wrists necessarily higher than on the white

keys.

Figure 17. Etude Op. 10, No. 15, Measures 20-26. Note the phrase markings in mm. 23-
26 suggesting wrist and forearm rotation. (Copyright 1983 G. Henle Verlag, Munich.
Printed by permission.)
114

Measures 1-4 present the basic patterns that are fundamental to the étude (see

Figure 18). A descending G-flat major arpeggiation spanning approximately two octaves

consisting of thirds, fourths, and sixths in mm. 1-2 is followed by an ascending dominant

arpeggio consisting of fourths, fifths, and octaves. Wrist and forearm rotation may aid

execution of the fragments involving smaller intervals, as in beat two of m. 1, for

example. Rotation also facilitates the coverage of keyboard distance and change of

direction occurring on the first beat of m. 3 as the D-flat is a common harmonic link

between the first two measures and mm. 3-4. In the ascending passage in m. 3, a drop of

the wrist coincides with the thumb and initiates an arc of movement that is larger than the

mechanisms of rotation and which carries the hand to m. 5 in this instance.

Figure 18. Etude Op. 10, No. 5, Measures 1-7. Note the fundamental pattern of the
étude. A descending G-flat major arpeggiation spanning approximately two octaves is
followed by an ascending dominant arpeggio consisting of fourths, fifths, and octaves.
(Copyright 1983 G. Henle Verlag, Munich. Printed by permission.)
115

The thumb and little finger are particularly important since they contact the piano

key and place the hand in its new position in the descending and ascending patterns,

respectively. In descending patterns for example, it is the upper arm, forearm, hand, and

thumb that bring the fourth and fifth fingers into position to continue the pattern.

Furthermore, as Whiteside notes, “If these details are accurately timed with the

progression of the first lever [upper arm], and this lever keeps its control of distances and

level, there will be no consciousness of any break in the legato when the hand is passed to

the new position on the keyboard.”316

Speed: Chopin Etudes Op. 10, No. 4 and Op. 25, No. 12

Etude Op. 10, No. 4

One of the primary challenges of Op. 10, No. 4 appears to be rapid execution of

scalar and arpeggiated figures requiring different amounts of rotation (see Figure 19).

This étude requires development of this coordination in each hand which ultimately

requires the training of non-homologous muscles since the direction of the figuration is

the same regardless of which hand has the pattern. In his fingerings and articulation

markings, Chopin seems to have offered clues as to how to approach the étude physically.

Whiteside, Mastering the Chopin Etudes, 103.


316
116

Figure 19. Etude Op. 10, No. 4, Measures 1-8. Scalar and arpeggiated figures requiring
varying degrees of rotation. (Quoted from Frédéric Chopin, Sämtliche Etüden, hg. Von
Paul Badura-Skoda, Wiener Urtext Edition, Wien 1973/2005 (UT 50205). Printed by
permission).

The accent mark on the first beat of m. 1 encourages a bounce of the wrist

facilitating a release of tension functioning as a preparatory gesture to the phrase

beginning on the second sixteenth note of the same measure. In many ways, Chopin

incorporated the conductor’s preparatory gesture with this accent.

The fingerings Chopin indicated in the first measure, with the thumb of the right

hand playing the third sixteenth note of every beat, initiate a pattern of rotation occurring

every four sixteenth notes through the first half of beat one in the second measure. The
117

first G-sharp in m. 2 functions both as the second half of the scalar four-note pattern

which began two sixteenths earlier and as the beginning of a new pattern of rotation

toward the little finger and back to either the index finger or thumb on beats two, three,

and four.

Accents and phrase markings in m. 3 facilitate a drop of the wrist on each beat.

They imply cyclical rotation from the thumb toward the little finger and the highest note

of the gesture and returning to the thumb and the beginning of another cycle on the next

beat.

Phrase markings corresponding to the melody in m. 4 suggest rotation from the

thumb to the little finger and further indicate and allow a non-legato approach between

the top note of one octave and the first note of the pattern that follows.

The processes occurring in the right hand in mm. 1-4 are similar in the left hand

but sometimes employ subtle differences in expected fingerings. For example, whereas

the thumb initiates the right-hand pattern in m. 1, and one might therefore expect the little

finger of the left hand to function similarly, it is instead the fourth finger of the left hand

that balances the hand in rotation in m. 6 (see Figure 19). Likewise, whereas the right

hand rotates to the little finger in m. 2, in order to maintain the tonal relationships in m. 6,

the left hand rotates toward the thumb.

The thematic material of the entire étude is generated from the first four measures.

As discussed above, the theme appears in both hands, and it appears in various keys.

This in turn changes the spatial relationship between the hands and the body. For

example, in m. 25, the theme appears a diminished fourth higher than in m. 1 (see Figure
118

20), and the suggested fingerings place the index finger on the lowest note of each of the

four-note patterns. The challenge in this case and in similar places is maintaining the

ease of executing a basic pattern of rotation despite changes in the patterns of black and

white keys which ultimately affect the balance of the hand.

Figure 20. Etude Op. 10, No. 4, Measures 24-29. Theme appears a diminished fourth
higher than in m. 1. (Quoted from Frédéric Chopin, Sämtliche Etüden, hg. Von Paul
Badura-Skoda, Wiener Urtext Edition, Wien 1973/2005 (UT 50205). Printed by
permission).

Etude Op. 25, No. 12

Propelling the hands laterally up and down the keyboard is one of the chief skills

to be mastered in Op. 25, No. 12 (see Figure 21). For the most part, accent marks appear

on the first and third beats of the measure and coincide with changes in direction of the

figuration. As such, the accents not only suggest harmonic significance but

corresponding drops of the wrists as well. In general, the phrase markings incorporate a
119

sweep up and back down the keyboard and lead the ear to the harmonic progressions

highlighted by the accented notes.

Figure 21. Etude Op. 25, No. 12, Measures 1-2. Gestures requiring smooth lateral
movements. (Copyright 1983 G. Henle Verlag, Munich. Printed by permission.)

Within these gestures, one must change hand positions on the repeated notes in

order to travel the lateral distance. The lack of accent marks and the longer phrase

markings indicate that the changes of position must occur smoothly to maintain the

musical line. Some amount of forearm rotation is surely necessarily to execute each set

of three notes if the fingers are not to be entirely responsible for depressing the keys and

facilitating the changes of position without particular articulation of the occurrence.

“Passing is always easy if the power travels smoothly and maintains the control of

placement and level, and if the hand is propelled into position by the two large levers. . . .

The hand [and not the fingers] should do whatever it can to take lateral distance.”317 The

fingerings Chopin indicated in Op. 10, No. 10 suggest a regular pattern of arm and wrist

Whiteside, Mastering the Chopin Etudes, 103.


317
120

rotation with the thumb playing all of the single notes in the right hand and the highest

notes being played presumably by either the ring or little fingers depending on hand size.

Flexibility and Suppleness via Imagery: Etude Op. 25, No. 1

A study in cantabile playing, the “Harp Etude” presents several challenges

involving flexibility, speed, and conceptualizing tone in highlighting the melody over

continuous sextuplet figures in the accompaniment (see Figure 22). Managing the melody

and accompaniment requires flexibility and suppleness of the wrists and arms to facilitate

playing each sextuplet figure as a single gesture comprising the phrase as opposed to

articulating each note individually with the fingers. “To alert the upper arm to take the

place of fingers in responding to the aural image is the task which comes first in

achieving the desired coordination in the playing mechanism for virtuosity in playing the

Etudes.”318 “Abby Whiteside’s work on the control of playing by the upper arm helped

her to discover that the physical continuity which the upper arm exerts through its pull is

not only the basis of speed without strain, it is also the essential physical counterpart of

musical continuity—the continuity within the phrase, and in the phrase-to-phrase

progression.”319

318
Ibid., 31.

Prostakoff and Rosoff, Mastering the Chopin Etudes, 4.


319
121

Figure 22. Etude Op. 25, No. 1, Measures 1-2. Importance of flexibility and suppleness
in balancing the melody amongst continuous sextuplet figures. (Copyright 1983 G.
Henle Verlag, Munich. Printed by permission.)

Continuity in the melody over the course of phrases requires care in matching the

tone of successive melody notes within a framework of listening for the direction and

completion of the phrase.

The fact is, the student who is accustomed to notewise listening will not always hear
the subtleties of dynamics and timing which are involved, and when, on rare occasions,
he does hear them he will not always prefer the superior, more continuous
performance. The notes seem to go by too fast, in too bland a fashion. He wants to
stop and listen to each note in turn. . . . Training the ear to listen phrasewise or, as
Abby Whiteside would say, “with a rhythm,” calls for establishing an involvement
with the upper arm—specifically with the pull exerted by the upper arm—because the
circular joint by which it is connected with the torso enables the arm to control
horizontal progression—the progression between the notes of the phrase.320

The ear and the upper arm are therefore significant components in successfully

playing this étude, but how does one shift importance from the fingers to the upper arm in

practice? Whiteside’s image of the lariat is particularly appropriate in discussing this

coordination.

The upper arm is the fulcrum for the forearm. The fulcrum-force in the upper arm is
the pull which controls the level at which tone is produced, as well as the slight turning

Whiteside, Mastering the Chopin Etudes, 22-3.


320
122

of the humerus which controls distance. The illustration of this fulcrum-force in the
upper arm, which seems most pertinent to me, is the control of a lariat: All of the
patterns which the rope is made to go through are controlled by the hand as an
extension of the primary action of the upper arm, as it makes miniature turnings, plus a
consistent and continuous slight pull at the shoulder joint. Tiny actions produce
fantastic shapes and patterns with the loop of the rope. Tiny actions in the upper arm
produce great beauty in performance. Let the arm stop this slight turning and pulling,
and the rope falls to the ground.321

It is worth noting that the rotary movements of the upper arm Whiteside describes

are “miniature.” That is, while its importance cannot be overstated, the movement itself

must not be exaggerated. The movement must be natural or one will have created

another difficulty. What provides the impetus for the upper arm in playing this étude?

According to both Whiteside and Chopin, it is a sharp aural image that lays the

foundation. “It is said that Chopin explained to one of his pupils the manner in which

this study should be executed. ‘Imagine,’ he said, ‘a little shepherd who takes refuge in a

peaceful grotto from an approaching storm. In the distance rushes the wind and the rain,

while the shepherd gently plays a melody in his flute.’”322 Whiteside goes even further

and links the aural image with the physical response to the basic rhythm of the piece.

A sensitive, phrase-wise performance will not take place—no matter how much
scholarship, emotional involvement, and natural musical endowment is present—
unless the performer uses this basic rhythm continuously whether he is aware of it or
not. It is only when the emotional response to the aural image of the music creates in
the performer’s body a physical response, a basic rhythm, as a counterpart to the

Ibid., 38.
321

322
Jean Ckeczynski [Jan Kleczynski], Chopin’s Greater Works, trans. with
additions by Natalie [sic] Janotha (London, William Reeves, 188?), 19; quoted in Jean-
Jacques Eigeldinger, Chopin: Pianist and Teacher as Seen by His Pupils, trans. by
Naomi Shohet with Hrysia Osostowica and Roy Howatt, ed. by Roy Howatt
(Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1986), 69.
123

rhythmic flow in a composition, that he is enabled to realize to the fullest extent the
beauty inherent in the music.323

Polyrhythms: Drei Etüden: KK II b/3, No. 1

It has been demonstrated indirectly by Peters and Schwartz and directly by Klapp

et al. that the hands are not independent in performance of polyrhythms even at

intermediate speeds.324 Additionally, a recent study of pianists and guitarists revealed

decreased inhibition between the hemispheres of the brain which might actually facilitate

bimanual coordination by increasing signal transfer between the hemispheres.325 There is

a continuum of cognitive and perceptual monitoring from slow speeds up to about six

notes per second that is detailed and fine-grained. However as speed increases beyond

approximately six notes per second, control becomes global and more coarse. The

incorporation of polyrhythms into technical study is therefore noteworthy and significant

as pianists further develop, not independence, but bi-manual coordination.

It is obvious regarding his philosophy of advocating scale practice in patterns of

four notes against three that Chopin viewed polyrhythms as a unique technical challenge.

Furthermore, polyrhythms are an integral component of his études. Coordinating the

323
Whiteside, Mastering the Chopin Etudes, 4-5.

See M. Peters and S. Schwartz, “Coordination of the Two Hands and Effects of
324

Attentional Manipulation in the Production of a Bimanual 2:3 Polyrhythm,” Australian


Journal of Psychology 41(2) (1989): 215-24 and S. T. Klapp, J. M. Nelson, and R. J.
Jagacinski, “Can People Tap Concurrent Bimanual Rhythms Independently?” Journal of
Motor Behavior 30(4) (1998): 301-22.

325
M. C. Ridding, B. Brouwer, and M. A. Nordstrom, “Reduced Interhemispheric
Inhibition in Musicians,” Experimental Brain Research 133(2) (July 2000): 249-53.
124

hands in continuous 4:3 polyrhythms is one of the obvious challenges of the first of three

études composed for the method of Moscheles and Fétis (see Figure 23), and yet the

individual parts require subtle skill.

Figure 23. Etude KK II b/3, No. 1, Measures 1-11. Though primarily focused on the 4:3
polyrhythm, note the close proximity of the hands and the crossing of fingers over the
thumb in the left hand in m. 5. (Copyright 1983 G. Henle Verlag, Munich. Printed by
permission.)

The quarter note triplet figures of the right hand and the duple figures of the left

hand are established separately in the first eight measures. These figures present several

challenges. First, the melodic figure in m. 1 begins after the first beat of the measure, and

it is fingered with the thumb. As the phrase mark indicates, the pianist must listen

carefully to execute the beginning of the melody as a preparatory gesture, to refrain from

accenting the thumb, and to maintain the integrity of the intended line. Continuity is

achieved in shaping the melody over the barline of each measure, a pattern that continues

in the étude with long phrases throughout. The eighth notes in the left hand require
125

crossing fingers over the thumb in harmonic arpeggiations. The lowest note of these

arpeggiations is played with the little finger, and movement of the forearm and hand

facilitates rotation to and over the thumb. A drop of the wrist coinciding with the little

finger may be used at the beginning of each cycle of rotation, however; it must be

unobtrusive and within the scope of the line. Changes of the damper pedal occur each

measure with the changes of harmony, but care is needed to monitor clarity of the right

hand due to the chromaticism of the melody and the sound of the modern piano.

Furthermore, the proximity of the hands also necessitates care in balance and voicing the

melody (see Figure 23, m. 9).


126

CHAPTER 6

CONCLUSIONS

Strategies for Learning and Teaching

Practice and Performance

Since it has been demonstrated that performance gains are positively correlated to

practice, what parameters of practice might be altered to facilitate and enhance

performance? The following discussion considers types of practice that might be

beneficial, piano literature and exercises that might be helpful, and instructional strategies

that might be particularly effective.

Parameters of mental practice pertaining to effectiveness

It has been suggested that benefits in physical performance are particularly

significant when training involves a combination of physical and mental practice.326

Wohldmann, Healy, and Bourne Jr. have recently suggested that “these benefits might

derive from the fact that the same memory representations are used in both types of

practice,” and that “mental practice may work by activating and strengthening

representations formed previously by physical practice.”327

C. B. Corbin, “Mental Practice,” in Ergogenic Aids and Muscular Performance,


326

ed. W. P. Morgan (New York: Academic Press, 1972), 93-118.

E. L. Wohldmann, A. F. Healy, and L. E. Bourne Jr., “Pushing the Limits of


327

Imagination: Mental Practice for Learning Sequences,” Journal of Experimental


Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 33(1) (2007): 254.
127

Although there is evidence suggesting that mental practice can be an effective

ergogenic aid,328 there are apparently parameters related to the effectiveness of its use in

practice. In combining the results of numerous studies concerning mental practice, a

meta-analysis of the literature pertaining to mental practice, Driskell, Copper, and Moran

determined that the effectiveness of mental practice is moderated by the type of task, the

interval between practice and performance, and the duration of mental practice.329

With regard to the type of task, Driskell, Copper, and Moran concluded that,

while mental practice was effective in both cognitive and physical tasks, it appeared to be

particularly effective the more a particular task required cognitive activities. For

musicians, this may highlight the importance of basic musicianship skills in addition to

intellectual and interpretive analysis of a particular piece of music.

328
See R. S. Sackett, “The Influence of Symbolic Rehearsal Upon the Retention of
a Maze Habit,” Journal of General Psychology 10 (1934): 376-95; Ibid., “The
Relationship Between Amount of Symbolic Rehearsal and Retention of a Maze Habit,”
Journal of General Psychology 13, (1935): 113-28; H. M. Perry, “The Relative
Efficiency of Actual and Imaginary Practice in 5 Selected Tasks,” Archives of Psychology
4 (1939): 5-75; R. A. Vandell, R. A. Davis, and N. A. Clugston, “Function of Mental
Practice in the Acquisition of Motor Skills,” Journal of General Psychology 29 (1943):
243-50; E. D. Ryan and J. Simons, “Efficacy of Mental Imagery in Enhancing Mental
Rehearsal Motor Skills,” Journal of Sport Psychology 4 (1982): 41-51; S. L. Ross, “The
Effectiveness of Mental Practice in Improving the Performance of College Trombonists,”
Journal of Research in Music Education 33 (1985): 221-30; C. Lee, “Psyching up for a
Muscular Endurance Task: Effects of Image Content on Performance and Mood State,”
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 12 (1990): 66-73; and E. L. Wohldmann, A. F.
Healy, and L. E. Bourne Jr., “Pushing the Limits of Imagination: Mental Practice for
Learning Sequences,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition 33(1) (2007): 254-61.

J. Driskell, C. Copper, and A. Moran, “Does Mental Practice Enhance


329

Performance?” Journal of Applied Psychology, 79(4) (1994): 481-92.


128

Prior to much of the scientific research in this area, Whiteside seemed to have

been a proponent of this notion in insisting on the importance of understanding the

structure of a piece and responding physically and emotionally to one’s impression of a

piece.

A primary tool for achieving a basic rhythm [“the continuous activity in the
arms and torso which control the total physical apparatus as the performer plays a
composition.”]330 is outlining a composition. This means playing the highlights and
omitting details (some of the notes of the music). In leaving out details remember that
the sole purpose is to emphasize the structural outline of the music. If this outlining is
done in a cut-and-dried fashion, without the emotional response to the music, it is of no
value. It is only when the outlining intensifies the grace of going forward with a lilting
step, as it were, that it illuminates, quickens, and frees the emotional rhythm in the
torso.”331

Quite interestingly, Driskell, Copper, and Moran concluded that experienced

subjects benefit equally from mental practice in both cognitive and physical activities and

suggested that differences between experienced subjects and novices may indicate that

novices have not formed an approximation of the skill. Therefore, symbolic rehearsal

may not be sufficient to guide novice performance.

The extent to which novices benefit from mental training remains unclear,

however. Several studies have demonstrated that novices may enhance physical

performance through use of mental imagery alone. Kohl and Roenker demonstrated

transfer of learning from use of imagery to physical execution even when mental practice

and performance involved different hands.332 Likewise, a 2007 study by Wohldmann,

Prostakoff and Rosoff, glossary, Mastering the Chopin Etudes, 198.


330

Whiteside, Mastering the Chopin Etudes, 53


331
129

Healy, and Bourne Jr. demonstrated that mental practice was sufficient to produce or

maintain a level of performance comparable to that obtained by physical practice.333 A

recent study by Shanks and Cameron, however, resulted in no effect of mental practice in

learning a motor sequence task.334 Differences in the results of these studies are thought

to arise from different emphases on the perceptual, mental, and motor components of the

tasks.335

Driskell, Copper, and Moran also concluded that the effects of mental rehearsal

are most robust when performance immediately follows mental practice. They concluded

that the initial effects of mental practice were reduced to approximately one-half of their

initial magnitude if the period between mental practice and performance was extended to

14 days.

Regarding the duration of the intervention, while it was determined that the

cumulative effect of mental practice was positive, the beneficial effect diminished with

duration. In other words, the longer an individual practices mentally, the less beneficial it

becomes. Based on these results, Driskell, Copper, and Moran suggested a period of

R. M. Kohl and D. L. Roenker, “Mechanism Involvement During Skill


332

Imagery,” Journal of Motor Behavior 15 (1983): 179-90.

333
Wohldmann, Healy, and Bourne Jr., “Pushing the Limits of Imagination,” 254-
61.

D. R. Shanks and A. Cameron, “The Effect of Mental Practice on Performance


334

in a Sequential Reaction Time Task,” Journal of Motor Behavior 32 (2000): 305-13.

335
Wohldmann, Healy, and Bourne Jr., “Pushing the Limits of Imagination,” 254-
61.
130

mental practice of approximately twenty minutes and a regiment of refreshing mental

practice every week or two.

Several other studies have demonstrated that performance gains related to mental

practice occur only when subjects have previous experience with the task.336 Mulder et

al. demonstrated that subjects were unable to learn a novel toe movement with mental

practice alone.337 Only subjects with some experience in the task benefited from mental

practice. The scientists therefore concluded that humans are able to mentally train only

those movements performed before.

Finally, Mulder et al. have recently observed a correlation between age and

imagery capacity noting that elderly subjects displayed slightly worse motor imagery

capacity than younger subjects particularly with regard to first-person imagery.338 Their

results indicated not a general decline in motor imagery capacity, but instead a significant

shift from first-person motor imagery to third-person motor imagery. Mulder et al.

hypothesized that their results might be indicative of a relationship between one’s level of

physical activity and motor imagery capacity. Support for a correlation between physical

336
See C. B. Corbin, “The Effects of Covert Rehearsal on the Development of a
Complex Motor Skill,” Journal of General Psychology 76 (1967): 143-50; R. A. Finke,
Principles of Mental Imagery, (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1989); and N. Ram et al.,
“A Comparison of Modelling and Imagery in the Acquisition and Retention of Motor
Skills,” Journal of Sports Sciences 25(5) (2007): 587-97.

T. Mulder et al., “The Role of Motor Imagery in Learning a Totally Novel


337

Movement,” Experimental Brain Research 154 (2004): 211-17.

T. Mulder, et al., “Motor Imagery: The Relation Between Age and Imagery
338

Capacity,” Human Movement Science 26 (2007): 203-11.


131

activity and motor image capacity does have support in the literature although the results

should be viewed with caution since some data is based on subjects’ self-reports.

Nevertheless, Colvin and Myers questioned 3,000 children regarding their imagery

abilities and reported that motor imagery was vivid only in children who were constantly

involved in movement experiences.339 Isaac and Marks found that females over 50 years

of age reported significantly less vivid movement imagery than other adults between the

ages of 20 and 50.340 Isaac and Marks suggested that the difference might stem from that

fact that the particular female population is typically not as active as either males of the

same age or younger age groups.

Aural feedback and aural representations

Several studies have highlighted the importance of aural feedback and aural

representations associated with music performance, facets of motor skill acquisition and

motor performance likely unique to musicians. As discussed previously, research by

Schön and Besson substantiates the likelihood that musicians have an auditory-like

representation of written music before they actually play it.341 Furthermore, their research

suggests that, while visuomotor coding may be sufficient to play written notes, an

S. S. Colvin and E. J. Myers, “The Development of Imagination in School


339

Children and the Relation Between Ideational Types and the Retentivity of Material
Appealing to Various Sense Departments,” Review Monographs 11 (1909): 85-125.

A. R. Isaac and D. F. Marks, “Individual Differences in Imagery Experience:


340

Developmental Changes and Specialization,” British Journal of Psychology 85 (1994):


479-500.

Schön and Besson, “Visually Induced Auditory Expectancy,” 694-705.


341
132

anticipated auditory representation of written music is important for an expressive

performance that assigns different significance to each note based upon its structural

function. Schön and Besson’s research suggests that written music may induce both

musical auditory expectancy and motor expectancy. This in turn may point to the likely

subconscious use of auditory imagery and potential for its conscious use as well.

Observational learning

Recent research concerning the discovery of a mirror neuron system in humans

suggests the possible efficacy of observational learning. It is obvious with regard to

piano performance that only the external components of the playing mechanism are

observable. However, it seems clear from research concerning mirror neurons that the

human body automatically responds to the activities of a model and that the human mind

is capable of perceiving and inferring subtle relationships that are not readily observable.

The power of observational learning underscores some of the significant principles in

prominent learning theories suggesting the effectiveness of experiential learning prior to

cognitive conceptualization. For pianists, this suggests benefits associated with concert

attendance and modeling both in the classroom and the private studio.

Scales, piano literature, and the brain

As mentioned earlier, a 2005 study by Parsons et al. compared activated brain

networks of pianists performing both scales and the third movement of J.S. Bach’s Italian

Concerto in F Major (BWV 971).342 Performance of the Bach occurred while

Parsons et al., “The Brain Basis of Piano Performance,” 199-215.


342
133

blindfolded. While there was significant correlation between brain areas activated in

performing scales and the Bach, there were also numerous activations unique to each.

In comparison to performance of scales, performance of the Bach was associated

with activation of subcortical areas including bilateral, predominantly right thalamus and

bilateral basal ganglia. Activation in somatomotor regions included bilateral dorsolateral

premotor cortex (BA6), bilateral primary motor cortex (BA 4), right insula, right SMA,

bilateral lingual gyrus (BA 19 and 18), bilateral posterior cingulate (BA 31 and 23), and

anterior and posterior cerebellum.

On the other hand, performance of scales resulted in strong activations in bilateral

anterior cingulate gyrus (BA 31 and 32), bilateral middle temporal cortex (BA 21), and

right superior temporal cortex (BA 41). In addition, activation was observed in right

frontal regions (BA 9 and 10), right tempoparietal regions (BA 39 and 40), right fusiform

gyrus (BA 37), left inferior frontal gyrus (BA 47), right precuneus (BA 7), and right

primary motor cortex (BA 4). Furthermore, increases in activation were observed in

particular areas of the anterior and posterior cerebellum.

Overall, Parsons et al. observed “unexpected intensity and extent of deactivation

during the performance of Bach. In a conjoint measure of deactivation intensity and

extent (relative to rest), there was 43% greater deactivation throughout the brain during

the Bach performance than scales.”343 The deactivated areas included frontal areas (BA 8,

9, and 44), posterior cingulate areas (BA 31), bilateral middle temporal,

Ibid.
343
134

parahippocampus, precuneus, and posterior cerebellum.344 Although some of the

aforementioned areas were deactivated during the performance of scales, they were

deactivated to a lesser extent. The observed differences in brain activity suggest

functions associated with the cortical and subcortical structures that may underlie playing

the piano and auditory perception.345

The preceding comparisons of activated networks are delimited by the interplay of


various differences in playing scales and the concerto. First, the performance of the
Bach piece requires recalling more complicated information than does scales. Effects
of this variable are probably apparent in the greater activation in auditory association
areas (BA 22 and 21), posterior cingulate, and cuneus and lingual areas. Second, the
concerto performance requires more intricate, controlled fingering and tone
production. This factor is likely reflected in increases in thalamus, basal ganglia, SMA,
insula, dorsolateral premotor cortex, and anterior cerebellum. Third, the concerto
performance requires vividly conveying a comprehension of the musical structure
(e.g., tonality, rhythm, dynamics, interpretative features, etc.). Effects of this factor
may be observed in auditory areas (BA 38, 22, and 21), posterior cerebellum, posterior
cingulate, cuneus, and lingual areas. Fourth, musical performance is associated with
emotional responses, which are likely much more limited when playing scales.
However, areas known to activate during emotional responses to music were not
appreciably active here. Nonetheless, it is possible that activations related to emotional
responses were in posterior cingulate, insula, and basal ganglia. Fifth, the concerto
performance demands more attention than scales. This factor may be reflected in
different ways here. There were no increases for the concerto in core attentional areas,
but the effects may be more diffuse because there was more detected activation overall
than for scales: nearly twice as many distinct foci and 72% greater overall extent of
activation. Moreover, there was nearly 50% greater deactivation overall for the
concerto performance than scales, an effect that may be related to a deepened focus of
attention, as discussed earlier. It is conceivable then that both the greater activations
and greater deactivations are related to differences in attention in the two
performances.346

Ibid.
344

Ibid.
345

Ibid., 212.
346
135

Discussion

There is significant indirect evidence to suggest that studying and performing

different types of exercises and piano literature results in distinct physiological

adaptations and brain activity. The implications could affect the ways in which pianists

practice and build technique. To that end, the aforementioned research by Parsons et al.

demonstrate significant differences in brain activity during performance of Bach’s Italian

Concerto as opposed to scales.347 Results of research by Filimon et al. demonstrate an

overlap between executed, observed, and imagined reaching activations in the human

cortex.348 Furthermore, a growing number of studies have shown that many

neuropsychological and physiological similarities exist between physically executed and

imagined movements.349 These studies suggest that imagined and executed movements

are interrelated and dependent upon the characteristics of the musical task.

Ibid., 199-215.
347

Filimon et al., “Human Cortical Representations for Reaching,” 1315-28.


348

349
See J. Decety et al., “Vegetative Response During Imagined Movement is
Proportional to Imagined Effort,” Behavioural Brain Research 42 (1991): 1-5; Ibid.,
“Central Activation of Autonomic Effectors During Mental Simulation of Motor Actions
in Man,” Journal of Physiology (London) 461 (1993): 549-63; Jeannerod, “The
Representing Brain,” 187-245; M. Jeannerod and J. Decety, “Mental Motor Imagery: A
Window Into the Representational Stages of Action,” Current Opinion in Neurobiology 5
(1995): 727-32; C. Hall, L. Bernoties, and D. Schmidt, “Interference Effects of Mental
Imagery on a Motor Task,” British Journal of Psychology 86 (1995): 181-90; A. Pascual-
Leone et al., “Modulation of Muscle Responses,” 1037-45; J. Decety, “The
Neurophysiological Basis of Motor Imagery,” Behavioral Brain Research 77 (1996): 45-
52; J. Decety and J. Grèzes, “Neural Mechanisms Subserving the Perception of Human
Actions,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3 (1999): 172-8; M. Lotze et al., “Activation of
Cortical and Cerebellar Motor Areas,” 491-501; P. L. Jackson et al., “Potential Role of
136

Pascual-Leone demonstrated that, in comparison to subjects who practiced a

particular five-finger exercise over the course of 5 days, subjects who practiced for the

same amount of time but were not taught the 5-finger sequence displayed changes in

cortical motor output that were similar to but less significant than those who had learned

the sequence.350 This suggests effects associated with specificity of practice.

Collectively, this research may suggest a correlation between imagined

movements, executed movements, physiological adaptation, and cortical structure.

Applied to piano performance and practice, this may highlight the specificity of piano

literature and exercises and suggest the use of methods for developing aspects of

technique particular to them. Further direct research is needed, however. Areas of

interest might compare brain activity during the preparation, practice, and performance of

a broad variety of ètudes and piano repertoire by pianists of different ages and levels of

experience.

Mental Practice Using Motor Imagery in Neurologic Rehabilitation,” Archives of Physical


Medicine and Rehabilitation 82 (2001): 1133-41; and T. Hanakawa et al., “Functional
Properties of Brain Areas Associated with Motor Execution and Imagery,” Journal of
Neurophysiology 89 (2003): 989-1002.

Pascual-Leone et al., “Modulation of Muscle Responses,” 1037-45.


350
137

APPENDIX A: HENLE URTEXT EDITION PERMISSION

With kind permission, Figures 2-5, 7-18, and 21-23 are printed from the Henle Urtext

edition of Chopin’s Etudes, Copyright 1983 G. Henle Verlag, Munich .


138

APPENDIX B: WIENER URTEXT EDITION PERMISSION

With kind permission, Figures 1, 6, 19, and 20 are quoted from Frédéric Chopin,

Sämtliche Etüden, hg. Von Paul Badura-Skoda, Wiener Urtext Edition, Wien 1973/2005

(UT 50205).
139

REFERENCES

Aagaard, P., E. B. Simonsen, J. L. Andersen, S. P. Magnusson, J. Halkjaer-Kristensen,


and P. Dyhre-Poulsen. “Neural Inhibition During Maximal Eccentric and Concentric
Quadriceps Contractions: Effects of Resistance Training.” Journal of Applied
Physiology 89 (2000): 2249-57.

Aagaard, P., E. B. Simonsen, J. L. Andersen, S. P. Magnusson, J. Halkjaer-Kristensen, P.


Dyhre-Poulsen, and E. B. Simonsen. “A Mechanism for Increased Contractile
Strength of Human Pennate Muscle in Response to Strength Training: Changes in
Muscle Architecture.” Journal of Physiology 534 (2001): 613-23.

Aagaard, P., E. B. Simonsen, J. L. Andersen, S. P. Magnusson, and P. Dyhre-Poulsen.


“Neural Adaptation to Resistance Training: Changes in Evoked V-Wave and H-
Reflex Responses.” Journal of Applied Physiology 92 (2002): 2309-18.

Abbs, J. H., V. L. Gracco, and K. J. Cole. “Control of Multimovement Coordination:


Sensorimotor Mechanisms in Speech Motor Programming.” Journal of Motor
Behavior 16(2) (1984): 195-231.

Aboitiz, F. and R. Garcia. “The Anatomy of Language Revisited.” Biological Research


30(4) (1997): 171-83.

Ackerman, Phillip L. “Individual Differences in Skill Learning: An Integration of


Psychometric and Information Processing Perspectives.” Psychological Bulletin 102
(1987): 3-27.

________. “Determinants of Individual Differences During Skill Acquisition:


Cognitive Abilities and Information Processing.” Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General 117(3) (1988): 288-318.

Ackerman, P. L. and A. T. Cianciolo. “Cognitive, Perceptual-Speed, and Psychomotor


Determinants of Individual Differences During Skill Acquisition.” Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Applied 6 (2000): 259-90.

Adam, A., C. J. De Luca, and Z. Erim. “Hand Dominance and Motor Unit Firing
Behavior.” Journal of Neurophysiology 80 (1998): 1373-82.

Adams, J. A. “A Closed-loop Theory of Motor Learning.” Journal of Motor Behavior 9


(1971): 111-50.

________. “Learning of Movement Sequences.” Psychological Bulletin 96 (1984): 3-


28.
140

REFERENCES - Continued

________. “Use of the Model’s Knowledge of Results to Increase the Observer’s


Performance.” Journal of Human Movement Studies 12 (1986): 89-98.

________. “Historical Review and Appraisal of Research on the Learning, Retention,


and Transfer of Human Motor Skills.” Psychological Bulletin 101(1) (1987): 41-74.

Adams, J. A., D. Gopher, and G. Lintern. “Effects of Visual and Proprioceptive


Feedback on Motor Learning.” Journal of Motor Behavior 9 (1977): 11-22.

Adams, J. A., E. Goetz, and P. Marshall. “Response Feedback and Motor Learning.”
Journal of Experimental Psychology 92 (1972): 391-7.

Adams, J. A. and N. W. Bray. “A Closed-Loop Theory of Paired-Associate Verbal


Learning.” Psychological Review 77 (1970): 385-405.

Adams, G. R., R. T. Harris, D. Woodard, and G. Dudley. “Mapping of Electrical Muscle


Stimulation Using MRI.” Journal of Applied Physiology 74 (1993): 532-7.

Adkins, De Anna L., Jeffery Boychuk, Michael S. Rempe, and Jeffrey A. Kleim. “Motor
Training Induces Experience-Specific Patterns of Plasticity Across Motor Cortex and
Spinal Cord.” Journal of Applied Physiology 101: (2006): 1776-82.

Adrian, E. and D. Bronk. “The Discharge of Impulses in Motor Nerve Fibres. II. The
Frequency of Discharges in Reflex and Voluntary Contractions.” Journal of
Physiology 204 (1929): 231-57.

Aggleton, J. P., R. W. Kentridge, and J. M. M. Good. “Handedness and Musical Ability:


A Study of Professional Orchestral Players, Composers, and Choir Members.”
Psychology of Music 22 (1994): 148-56.

Akshoomoff, N. A., E. Courchesne, and J. Townsend. “Attention Coordination and


Anticipatory Control.” In The Cerebellum and Cognition, ed. J. D. Schmahmann,
575-98. New York: Academic Press, 1997.

Albus, J. S. “A Theory of Cerebellar Function.” Math Bioscience 10 (1971): 25-61.

Aleman, A., M. R. Nieuwenstein, K. B. E. Bocker, and H. F. de Hann. “Music Training


and Mental Imagery Ability.” Neuropsychologia 38 (2000): 1664-8.

Alessandri-Haber, N., G. Alcaraz, C. Deleuze, F. Jullien, C. Manrique, F. Couraud, M.


Crest, and P. Giraud. “Molecular Determinants of Emerging Excitability in Rat
Embryonic Motoneurons.” Journal of Physiology 541 (2002): 25-39.
141

REFERENCES - Continued

Allen, G. M., S. C. Gandevia, and D. K. McKenzie. “Reliability of Measurements of


Muscle Strength and Voluntary Activation Using Twitch Interpolation.” Muscle &
Nerve 18 (1995): 593-600.

Allison, T., A. Puce, and G. McCarthy. “Social Perception from Visual Cues: Role of
the STS Region.” Trends in Cognitive Science 4 (2000): 267-78.

Allred, R. P. and T. A. Jones. “Unilateral Ischemic Sensorimotor Cortical Damage in


Female Rats: Forelimb Behavioral Effects and Dendritic Structural Plasticity in the
Contralateral Homotopic Cortex.” Experimental Neurology 190 (2004): 433-45.

Ammons, R. B., C. H. Ammons, and R. L. Morgan. “Transfer of Skill and Decremental


Factors Along the Speed Dimensions in Rotary Pursuit.” Perceptual and Motor Skills
6 (1956): 43.

Amunts, K., G. Schlaug, L. Jäncke, H. Steinmetz, A. Schleicher, A. Dabringhaus, and K.


Zilles. “Motor Cortex and Hand Motor Skills: Structural Compliance in the Human
Brain.” Human Brain Mapping 5 (1997): 206-15.

Anderson, J. R. The Architecture of Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University


Press, 1983.

________. Rules of Mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1993.

Annett, M. Left, Right, Hand and Brain: The Right Shift Theory. London: Erlbaum,
1985.

Aoki T., P. R. Francis, H. Kinoshita. “Differences in the Abilities of Individual Fingers


During the Performance of Fast, Repetitive Tapping Movements.” Experimental
Brain Research 152 (2003): 270-80.

Aoki T, S. Furuya, H. Kinoshita. “Finger-Tapping Ability in Male and Female Pianists


and Nonmusician Controls.” Motor Control 9(1) (2005): 23-39.

Argaw, A., P. Desaulniers, and P. F. Gardiner. “Enhanced Neuromuscular Transmission


Efficacy in Overloaded Rat Plantaris Muscle.” Muscle & Nerve 29 (2004): 97-103.

Aroniadou, V. A. and A. Keller. “Mechanisms of LTP Induction in Rat Motor Cortex In


Vitro.” Cerebral Cortex 5(4) (1995): 353-62.

Asanuma, H. and C. Pavlides. “Neurobiological Basis of Motor Learning in Mammals.”


NeuroReport 8(4) (1997): i-vi.
142

REFERENCES - Continued

Atkeson, C. G. “Learning Arm Kinematics and Dynamics.” Annual Review of


Neuroscience 12 (1989): 157-83.

Au, Angelina Ngan-chu. “The Piano Etude in the Nineteenth Century: From the
Acquisition of Facility to Demonstration of Virtuosity.” D.M.A. diss., University of
Cincinnati, 1999.

Aziz-Zadeh, L., M. Iacoboni, E. Zaidel, S. Wilson, and J. Mazziotta. “Left Hemisphere


Motor Facilitation in Response to Manual Action Sounds.” European Journal of
Neuroscience 19 (2004): 2609-12.

Baddeley, A. and D. Longman. “The Influence of Length and Frequency of Training


Session on the Rate of Learning to Type.” Ergonomics 21 (1978): 627-35.

Baddeley, A. and R. Logie. “Auditory Imagery and Working Memory.” In Auditory


Imagery, ed. Daniel Reisberg, 201-35. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,
1992.

Badura-Skoda, Paul. Preface to Etudes Op. 10, by Frédéric Chopin. Vienna: Wiener
Urtext Edition, 1973.

Bagesteiro, L. B., F. R. Sarlegna, and R. L. Sainburg. “Differential Influence of Vision


and Proprioception on Control of Movement Distance.” Experimental Brain Research
171(3) (May 2006): 358-70.

Baldissera, F., P. Cavallari, G. Marini, and G. Tassone. “Differential Control of In-Phase


and Anti-Phase Coupling of Rhythmic Movements of Ipsilateral Hand and Foot.”
Experimental Brain Research 83 (1991): 373-80.

Baltes, P. B. and R. Kliegl. “Further Testing of Limits of Cognitive Plasticity: Negative


Age Differences in a Mnemonic Skill are Robust.” Developmental Psychology 28
(1992): 121-5.

Bandura, A., ed. Psychological Modeling. New York: Lieber-Atherton, 1971.

Bangert, M. and E. O. Altenmüller. “Mapping Perception to Action in Piano Practice: A


Longitudinal DC-EEG Study.” BMC Neuroscience 4(26) (2003),
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2202/4/26.
143

REFERENCES - Continued

Bangert, M., T. Peschel, G. Schlaug, M. Rotte, D. Drescher, H. Hinrichs, H. J. Heinze,


and E. Altenmüller. “Shared Networks for Auditory and Motor Processing in
Professional Pianists: Evidence from fMRI Conjunction.” NeuroImage 30 (2006):
917-26.

Bangert, M., U. Haeusler, and E. Altenmüller. “On Practice: How the Brain Connects
Piano Keys and Piano Sounds.” Biological Foundations of Music 930 (2001): 425-8.

Banton, L. “The Role of Visual and Auditory Feedback During the Sight-Reading of
Music.” Psychology of Music 23 (1995): 3-16.

Bashaw, Donna R. “The Evolution of Philosophies and Techniques of Piano Pedagogy


from 1750 to 1900 Traced Through the Teachings of C. P. E. Bach, Clementi, Czerny,
Chopin, and Leschetizky.” M.A. Thesis, California State University, Fullerton, 1980.

Baumann, S., S. Koeneke, C. F. Schmidt, M. Meyer, K. Lutz, and L. Jäncke. “A


Network for Sensory-Motor Integration: What Happens in the Auditory Cortex
During Piano Playing Without Acoustic Feedback?” Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences 1060 (2005): 186-88.

________. “A Network for Audio-Motor Coordination in Skilled Pianists and Non-


Musicians.” Brain Research 1161 (August 2007): 65-78.

Baumeister, R. F. “Choking Under Pressure: Self-Consciousness and Paradoxical


Effects of Incentives on Skillful Performance.” Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 46 (1984): 610-20.

Beaumont, E., J. D. Houlé, C. A. Peterson, and P. F. Gardiner. “Passive Exercise and


Fetal Spinal Cord Transplant Both Help to Restore Motoneuronal Properties After
Spinal Cord Transection in Rats.” Muscle & Nerve 29 (2004): 234-42.

Beek, P. J., C. E. Peper, and A. Daffertshofer. “Modeling Rhythmic Interlimb


Coordination: Beyond the Kahen-Kelso-Bunz Model.” Brain and Cognition 48
(2002): 149-65.

Beilock, S. L., S. A. Wierenga, and T. H. Carr. “Expertise, Attention, and Memory in


Sensorimotor Skill Execution: Impact of Novel Task Constraints on Dual-Task
Performance and Episodic Memory.” The Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology 55(4) (2002): 1211-40.
144

REFERENCES - Continued

________. “When Paying Attention Becomes Counterproductive: Impact of Divided


Versus Skill-Focused Attention on Novice and Experienced Performance of
Sensorimotor Skills.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 8(1) (2002): 6-
16.

________. “Memory and Expertise: What Do Experienced Athletes Remember?” In


Expert Performance in Sports: Advances in Research on Sport Expertise, ed. Janet L.
Starkes and K. Anders Ericsson, 295-320. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 2003.

Beilock, S. L. and T. H. Carr. “On the Fragility of Skilled Performance: What Governs
Choking Under Pressure?” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130
(2001): 701-25.

Beisteiner, R., E. Altenmüller, W. Lang, G. Lindinger, and L. Decke. “Musicians


Processing Music: Measurement of Brain Potentials with EEG.” European Journal
of Psychology 6 (1994): 311-27.

Belanger, A. Y. and A. J. McComas. “Extent of Motor Unit Activation During Effort.”


Journal of Applied Physiology 51 (1981): 381-93.

Bellemare, F., J. J. Woods, R. S. Johansson, and B. Bigland-Ritchie. “Motor-Unit


Discharge Rates During Maximal Voluntary Contractions of Three Human Muscles.”
Journal of Neurophysiology 50 (1983): 1380-92.

Bentovoglio, M., K. Kultas-Llinsky, and I. Llinsky. “Limbic Thalamus: Structure,


Intrinsic Organization, and Connections.” In Neurobiology of Cingulate Cortex and
Limbic Thalamus, ed. B. A. Vogt and M. Gabriel, 71-122. Boston: Birkhauser, 1993.

Bernardi, M., M. Solomonow, G. Nguyen, A. Smith, and R. Baratta. “Motor Unit


Recruitment Strategy Changes with Skill Acquisition.” European Journal of Applied
Physiology 74 (1996): 52-9.

Bernstein, N. The Coordination and Regulation of Movements. London: Pergamon,


1967.

Besson, M. and D. Schön. “Comparison Between Language and Music.” Annals of the
New York Academy of Sciences 930 (2001): 232-58.

Bihan, D. L., R. Turner, T. A. Zeffiro, C. A. Cuenod, P. Jezzard, and V. Bonnerat.


“Activation of Human Primary Visual Cortex During Visual Recall: A Magnetic
Resonance Imaging Study.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 90
(1993): 11802-5.
145

REFERENCES - Continued

Bilodeau, I. M. “Information Feedback.” In Acquisition of Skill, ed. E. A. Bilodeau, 255-


96. New York: Academic Press: 1966.

Binkofski, F. and G. Buccino. “Motor Functions of the Broca’s Region.” Brain and
Language 89 (2004): 362-9.

________. “The Role of Ventral Premotor Cortex in Action


Execution and Action Understanding.” Journal of Physiology (Paris) 99 (2006): 396-
405.

Black, J. W. “The Effect of Delayed Side-Tone Upon Vocal Rate and Intensity.”
Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders 16 (1951): 56-60.

Bogacz, Sally. “Understanding How Speed Affects Performance of Polyrhythms:


Transferring Control as Speed Increases.” Journal of Motor Behavior 37(1) (2005):
21-34.

Bonaiuto, James, Edina Rosta, and Michael Arbib. “Extending the Mirror Neuron
System Model, I: Audible Actions and Invisible Grasps.” Biological Cybernetics 96
(2007): 9-38.

Borden, G. J. “An Interpretation of Research on Feedback Interruption in Speech.”


Brain & Language 7 (1979): 307-19.

Boron, Walter F. and Emile L. Boulpaep. Medical Physiology: A Cellular and


Molecular Approach. China: Elsevier Science, 2003.

Bouisset, S. and M. Zattara. “A Sequence of Postural Movements Precedes Voluntary


Movement.” Neuroscience Letters 22 (1981): 263-70.

Bower, G. H. and E. R. Hilgard. Theories of Learning, 5th ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice Hall, 1981.

Bower, J. M. “Control of Sensory Data Acquisition.” In The Cerebellum and Cognition,


ed. J. D. Schmahmann, 490-513. New York: Academic Press, 1997.

Bower, J. M. and L. M. Parsons. “Rethinking the Lesser Brain.” Scientific American 289
(2003): 50-7.

Brass, M., H. Bekkering, A. Wohlschlager, and W. Prinz. “Compatibility Between


Observed and Execute Finger Movements: Comparing Symbolic, Spatial, and
Imitative Cues.” Brain Cognition 44 (2000): 124-43.
146

REFERENCES - Continued

Bray, C. W. “Transfer of Learning.” Journal of Experimental Psychology 11 (1928):


443-67.

Bressler, S. L. and J. A. S. Kelso. “Cortical Coordination Dynamics and Cognition.”


Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5 (2001): 26-36.

Brodsky, Warren, Avishai Henik, Bat-Sheva Rubinstein, and Moshe Zorman. “Inner
Hearing Among Symphony Orchestra Musicians: Intersectional Differences of
String-Players Versus Wind-Players.” In Music, Mind, and Science, ed. Suk Won Yi,
374-96. Seoul: Seoul National University, Western Music Research Institute, 1999.

________. “Auditory Imagery from Musical Notation in Expert Musicians.” Perception


& Psychophysics 65(4) (2003): 602-12.

Brown, S., M. J. Martinez, D. A. Hodges, P. T. Fox, and L. M. Parsons. “The Song


System of the Human Brain.” Cognitive Brain Research 20(3) (2004): 363-7.

Bryan, William Lowe and Noble Harter. “Studies in the Physiology and Psychology of
the Telegraphic Language.” Psychological Review 4 (1897): 27-53.

________. “Studies on the Telegraphic Language: The Acquisition of a Hierarchy of


Habits.” Psychological Review 6(4) (1899): 345-75.

Bryson, A. E. and Y. C. Ho. Applied Optimal Control. New York: Halsted Press, 1975.

Buccino, G., F. Binkofski, G. R. Fink, L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi, V. Gallese, R. J. Seitz, K.


Zilles, G. Rizzolatti, and H. J. Freund. “Action Observation Activates Premotor and
Parietal Areas in a Somatotopic Manner: An fMRI Study.” European Journal of
Neuroscience 13(2) (2001): 400-4.

Buccino, G., F. Binkofski, and L. Riggio. “The Mirror Neuron System and Action
Recognition.” Brain and Language 89(2) (2004a): 362-9.

Buccino, G., S. Vogt, A. Ritzl, G. R. Fink, K. Zilles, H. J. Freund, and G. Rizzolatti.


“Neural Circuits Underlying Imitation Learning of Hand Actions: An Event-Related
fMRI Study.” Neuron 42 (2004b): 323-34.

Burns, E. M. and W. D. Ward. “Categorical Perception – Phenomenon or


Epiphenomenon: Evidence From Experiments in the Perception of Melodic Musical
Intervals.” Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 53 (1978): 456-68.
147

REFERENCES - Continued

Bury, D. and T. A. Jones. “Unilateral Sensorimotor Cortex Lesions in Adult Rats


Facilitate Motor Skill Learning with the ‘Unaffected’ Forelimb and Training-Induced
Dendritic Structural Plasticity in the Motor Cortex.” Journal of Neuroscience 22
(2002): 8597-8606.

Byblow, W. D., D. Bysouth-Young, J. J. Summers, and R. G. Carson. “Performance


Asymmetries and Coupling Dynamics in the Acquisition of Multifrequency Bimanual
Coordination.” Psychological Research 61 (1998): 56-70.

Byrne, B. “Handedness and Musical Ability.” British Journal of Psychology 65 (1974):


279-81.

Campbell, M. J., A. J. McComas, and F. Petito. “Physiological Changes in Ageing


Muscle.” Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 36 (1973): 174–82.

Campos, G. E., T. J. Luecke, H. K. Wendeln, K. Toma, F. C. Hagerman, T. F. Murray, K.


E. Ragg, N. A. Ratamess, W. J. Kraemer, and R. S. Staron. “Muscular Adaptations in
Response to Three Different Resistance-Training Regimens: Specificity of Repetition
Maximum Training Zones.” European Journal of Applied Physiology 88 (2002): 50-
60.

Carmichael, S. T. and J. L. Price. “Limbic Connections of the Orbital and Medial


Prefrontal Cortex in Macaque Monkeys.” Journal of Comparative Neurology 363
(1995): 615-41.

Carp, J. S. and J. R. Wolpaw. “Motoneuron Plasticity Underlying Operantly Conditioned


Decrease in Primate H-Reflex.” Journal of Neurophysiology 72 (1994): 431-42.

________. “Motoneuron Properties After Operantly Conditioned


Increase in Primate H-Reflex.” Journal of Neurophysiology 73 (1995): 1365-73.

Carp, J. S., X. Y. Chen, H. Sheikh, and J. R. Wolpaw. “Operant Conditioning of Rat H-


Reflex Affects Motoneuron Axonal Conduction Velocity.” Experimental Brain
Research 136 (2001): 269-73.

Carpenter, A. F., A. P. Georgopoulos, and G. Pellizzer. “Motor Control Encoding


of Serial Order in a Context-Recall Task.” Science 283 (1999): 1752-7.

Carroll, Timothy J., Stephan Riek, and Richard G. Carson. “Neural Adaptations to
Resistance Training.” Sports Medicine 31(12) (2001): 829-40.
148

REFERENCES - Continued

Carson, R. G. “The Dynamics of Isometric Bimanual Coordination.” Experimental


Brain Research 105 (1995): 465–76.

Catmur, C., V. Walsh, and C. Heyes. “Sensorimotor Learning Configures the Human
Mirror System.” Current Biology 17(17) (2007): 1527-31.

Cattaert, D., A. Semjen, and J. J. Summers. “Simulating a Neural Cross-Talk Model for
Between-Hand Interference During Bimanual Circle Drawing.” Biological
Cybernetics 81 (1999): 343-58.

Ceci, S. and W. Williams. The Nature-Nurture Debate: The Essential Readings. New
York: Blackwell Publishing Co., 2000.

Chan, T. “An Investigation of Finger and Manual Dexterity.” Perceptual and Motor
Skills 90 (2000): 537-42.

Chase, R. A. “An Information-Flow Model of the Organization of Motor Activity. I:


Transduction, Transmission and Central Control of Sensory Information.” Journal of
Nervous & Mental Disease 140 (1965): 239-51.

Chase, W. G. and H. A. Simon. “Perception in Chess.” Cognitive Psychology 4 (1973):


55-81.

Chen, X. Y., L. Chen, and J. R. Wolpaw. “Time Course of H-Reflex Conditioning in the
Rat.” Neuroscience Letters 302 (2001): 85-8.

Chi, M. T. H., R. Glaser, and M. J. Farr, ed. The Nature of Expertise. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum, 1988.

Chiang, H., S. M. Slobounov, and W. Ray. “Practice-Related Modulations of Force


Enslaving and Cortical Activity as Revealed by EEG.” Clinical Neurophysiology 115
(2004): 1033-43.

Chopin, Frédéric. Études. Edited by James Huneker and Carl Mikuli. Fingered with
Introductory Material by Carl Mikuli. New York: G Schirmer, 1943.

________. Projet de Méthode. Quoted in Jean-Jacques Eigeldinger, Chopin: Pianist


and Teacher as Seen By His Pupils, ed. Roy Howat. Translated by Naomi Shohet
with Krysia Osostowicz and Roy Howat. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press,
1986.
149

REFERENCES - Continued

________. 12 Studies, Op. 10. Student’s Edition by Alfred Cortot, Translated by


M. Parkinson. Paris: Salabert, 2003.

________. 12 Studies, Op. 25. Student’s Edition by Alfred Cortot, Translated by M.


Parkinson. Paris: Salabert, 2003.

Christman, Stephen. “Handedness in Musicians: Bimanual Constraints on


Performance.” Brain and Cognition 22 (1993): 266-72.

Christou, E. A., M. Grossman, and L. G. Carlton. “Modeling Variability of Force During


Isometric Quadriceps Femoris Muscle Contractions.” Journal of Motor Behavior 34
(2002): 67-81.

Christou, E. A., M. Zelent, and L. G. Carlton. “Force Control is Greater in Upper


Compared with the Lower Extremity.” Journal of Motor Behavior 35 (2003): 322-4.

Cisek, P. and J. F. Kalaska. “Neural Correlates of Mental Rehearsal in Dorsal Premotor


Cortex.” Nature 431 (2004): 993-6.

Classen, J., J. Liepert, S. P. Wise, M. Hallett, and L. G. Cohen. “Rapid Plasticity of


Human Cortical Movement Representation Induced by Practice.” Journal of
Neurophysiology 79(2) (1998): 1117-23.

Classen, J., U. Knorr, K. J. Werhahn, G. Schlaug, E. Kunesch, L. G. Cohen, R. J. Seitz,


and R. Benecke. “Multimodal Output Mapping of Human Central Motor
Representation on Different Spatial Scales.” Journal of Physiology 512 (1998): 163-
79.

Clinchard, Pauline Marie. “A Study and Analysis of Selected Etudes of Frederic Chopin
with Implications for Teaching and Performance.” Edcat D., diss., Columbia
University, 1972.

Coghlan, A. “Elite Athletes Born to Run.” New Scientist 30 (2003): 4-5.

Cohen, D. A., A. Pascual-Leone, D. Z. Press, and E. M. Robertson. “Off-Line Learning


of Motor Skill Memory: A Double Dissociation of Goal and Movement.”
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
102(50) (2005): 18237-41.

Cohen, D. A. and E. M. Robertson. “Motor Sequence Consolidation: Constrained by


Critical Time Windows or Competing Components.” Experimental Research 177
(2007): 440-46.
150

REFERENCES - Continued

Cohen, L. “Synchronous Bimanual Movements Performed by Homologous and Non-


Homologous Muscles.” Perceptual and Motor Skills 32 (1971): 639-44.

Colvin, S. S. and E. J. Myers. “The Development of Imagination in School Children and


the Relation Between Ideational Types and the Retentivity of Material Appealing to
Various Sense Departments.” Review Monographs 11 (1909): 85-125.

Conditt, M. A., F. Gandolfo, and F. A. Mussa-Ivaldi. “The Motor System Does Not
Learn the Dynamics of the Arm by Rote Memorization of Past Experience.” Journal
of Neurophysiology 78 (1997): 544-60.

Connors, B. W. C. E. Landisman, and R. C. Reid. “Thalamus: Organization and


Function.” Trends in Neurosciences 21 (1998): 539-40.

Cook, T. W. “Studies in Cross Education: III. Kinesthetic Learning of an Irregular


Pattern.” Journal of Experimental Psychology 17 (1934): 749-62.

Corbin, C. B. “Effects of Mental Practice on Skill Development after Controlled


Practice.” Research Quarterly 38 (1967): 534-8.

________. “Mental Practice.” In Ergogenic Aids and Muscular Performance, ed. W.


P. Morgan, 93-118. New York: Academic Press, 1972.

Cordo, P. J. and M. Flanders. “Sensory Control of Target Acquisition.” Trends in


Neurosciences 12 (1989): 110-7.

Cortot, Alfred. In Search of Chopin. Trans. Cyril and Rena Clarke. Westport, CT:
Greenwood Press, 1975.

Craighero, L., A. Bello, L. Fadiga, and G. Rizzolatti. “Hand Action Preparation


Influences the Responses to Hand Pictures.” Neuropsychologia 40 (2002): 492-502.

Craighero, L., G. Metta, G. Sandini, and L. Fadiga. “The Mirror-Neurons System: Data
and Models.” Progress in Brain Research 163 (2007): 39-59.

Crammond, D. J. and J. F. Kalaska. “Differential Relation of Discharge in Primary


Motor Cortex and Premotor Cortex to Movement Versus Actively Maintained
Postures During a Reaching Task.” Experimental Brain Research 108 (1996): 45-61.

Crossman, E. R. F. W. “A Theory of the Acquisition of Speed-Skill.” Ergonomics 2


(1959): 153-66.
151

REFERENCES - Continued

Darian-Smith, I., M. P. Galea, and C. Darian-Smith. “Manual Dexterity: How Does the
Cerebral Cortex Contribute?” Clinical and Experimental Pharmacology and
Physiology 23 (1996): 948-56.

D’Ausiolio, A., E. Altenmüller, M. Olivetti Belardinelli, and M. Lotze. “Cross-Modal


Plasticity of the Motor Cortex While Listening to a Rehearsed Musical Piece.”
European Journal of Neuroscience 24 (2006): 955-58.

Davids, K. and J. Baker. “Genes, Environment and Sport Performance: Why the Nature-
Nurture Dualism is No Longer Relevant.” Sports Medicine 37(11) (2007): 961-80.

Davidson, Paul R. and Daniel M. Wolpert. “Widespread Access to Predictive Models in


the Motor System: A Short Review.” Journal of Neural Engineering 2 (2005):
S313-S319.

Debaere, F., N. Wenderoth, S. Sunaert, P. Van Hecke, and S. P. Swinnen. “Internal vs


External Generation of Movements: Differential Neural Pathways Involved in
Bimanual Coordination Performed in the Presence or Absence of Augmented Visual
Feedback.” NeuroImage 19 (2003): 764-76.

Decety, J. “The Neurophysiological Basis of Motor Imagery.” Behavioral Brain


Research 77 (1996): 45-52.

Decety, J. and J. Grèzes. “Neural Mechanisms Subserving the Perception of Human


Actions.” Trends in Cognitive Science 3(5) (1999): 172-8.

Decety, J., M. Jeannerod, M. Germain, and J. Pastene. “Vegetative Response During


Imagined Movement is Proportional to Imagined Effort.” Behavioural Brain
Research 42 (1991): 1-5.

________. “Central Activation of Autonomic Effectors During Mental Simulation of


Motor Actions in Man.” Journal of Physiology (London) 461 (1993): 549-63.

de Groot, A. Thought and Choice in Chess. The Hague, the Netherlands: Mouton, 1978.

Dell’Aqua, R. and P. Jolicoeur. “Visual Encoding of Patterns is Subject to Dual-Task


Interference.” Memory & Cognition 28(2) (2000): 184-91.

De Luca, C. J., R. S., LeFever, M. P., McCue, and A. P. Xenakis. “Behavior of Human
Motor Units in Different Muscles During Linearly Varying Contractions.” Journal of
Physiology (London) 329 (1982): 113-28.
152

REFERENCES - Continued

de Oliveira, Simone Cardoso. “The Neuronal Basis of Bimanual Coordination: Recent


Neurophysiological Evidence and Functional Models.” Acta Psychologica 110
(2002): 139-59.

Desaulniers, P., P. A. Lavoie, and P. F. Gardiner. “Habitual Exercise Enhances


Neuromuscular Transmission Efficacy of Rat Soleus Muscle in Situ.” Journal of
Applied Physiology 90 (2001): 1041-8.

Desmurget, M. and S. Grafton. “Forward Modeling Allows Feedback Control for Fast
Reaching Movements.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4 (2000): 423-31.

Deutsch, D. “The Generation of Two Isochronous Sequences in Parallel.” Perception &


Psychophysics 34(4) (1983): 331-37.

Deutsch, D. and J. R. Pierce. “The Climate of Auditory Imagery and Music.” In


Auditory Imagery, ed. Daniel Reisberg, 237-60. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, 1992.

de Vries, Sjoerd and Theo Mulder. “Motor Imagery and Stroke Rehabilitation: A
Critical Discussion.” Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine 39 (2007): 5-13.

di Pellegrino, G., L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi, V. Gallese, and G. Rizzolatti. “Understanding


Motor Events: A Neurophysiological Study.” Experimental Brain Research 91
(1992): 176-80.

Dodge, K. A. “The Nature-Nurture Debate and Public Policy.” Merrill-Palmer


Quarterly 50 (2004): 418-27.

Donoghue, J. P. and S. P. Wise. “The Motor Cortex of the Rat: Cytoarchitecture and
Microstimulation Mapping.” The Journal of Comparative Neurology 212 (1982): 76-
88.

Dorlöchter, M., A. Irintchev, M. Brinkers, and A. Wernig. “Effects of Enhanced Activity


on Synaptic Transmission in Mouse Extensor Digitorum Longus Muscle.” Journal of
Physiology 436 (1991): 283-92.

Doupe, A. J. and P. K. Kuhl. “Birdsong and Human Speech: Common Themes and
Mechanisms.” Annual Review of Neuroscience 22 (1999): 567-31.

Dowling, J. J., E. Konert, P. Ljucovic, and D. M. Andrews. “Are Humans Able to


Voluntarily Elicit Maximum Muscle Force?” Neuroscience Letters 1994 (179): 25-8.
153

REFERENCES - Continued

Doya, K. “What are the Computations of Cerebellum, Basal Ganglia, and Cerebral
Cortex?” Neural Networks 12 (1999): 961-74.

________. “Complementary Roles of Basal Ganglia and Cerebellum in Learning and


Motor Control.” Current Opinion in Neurobiology 10 (2000): 732-9.

Doyen, A. L. and M. Carlier. “Measuring Handedness: A Validation Study of Bishop’s


Reading Card Test.” Laterality 7 (2002): 115-30.

Drake, Carolyn and Caroline Palmer. “Skill Acquisition in Music Performance:


Relations Between Planning and Temporal Control.” Cognition 74 (2000): 1-32.

Driskell, J., C. Copper, and A. Moran. “Does Mental Practice Enhance Performance?”
Journal of Applied Psychology, 79(4) (1994): 481-92.

Duchateau, J., J. G. Semmler, and R. M. Enoka. “Training Adaptations in the Behavior


of Human Motor Units.” Journal of Applied Physiology 101 (2006): 1766-75.

Duchateau, J. and K. Hainaut. “Isometric or Dynamic Training: Differential Effects on


Mechanical Properties of a Human Muscle.” Journal of Applied Physiology 56(2)
(1984): 296-301.

Duhamel, J. R., C. L. Colby, and M. E. Goldberg. “Ventral Intraparietal Area of the


Macaque: Congruent Visual and Somatic Response Properties.” Journal of
Neurophysiology 79 (1998): 126-36.

Dunham, P. “Effect of Bilateral Transfer on Coincidence/Anticipation Performance.”


Research Quarterly 48 (1977): 51-5.

Dvorak, A., N. L. Merrick, W. L. Dealey, and G. C. Ford. Typewriting Behavior. New


York: American Book, 1936.

Edelman, G. M. and J. A. Gally. “Degeneracy and Complexity in Biological Systems.”


Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 98
(2001): 13763-8.

Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. “The Expressive Behavior of the Deaf-and-Blind-Born.” In Social


Communication and Movement: Studies of Interaction and Expression in Man and
Chimpanzee, ed. M.V. Cranach and I. Vine, 163-94. London: Academic Press, 1973.
154

REFERENCES - Continued

Eigeldinger, Jean-Jacques. Chopin: Pianist and Teacher as Seen by His Pupils.


Translated by Naomi Shohet with Hrysia Osostowica and Roy Howatt. Edited by Roy
Howatt. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1986.

Einstein, Alfred. Music in the Romantic Era. New York: W. W. Norton & Co., Inc.,
1947.

Elbert, T., C. Pantev, C. Wienbruch, B. Rockstroh, and E. Taub. “Increased Cortical


Representation of the Fingers of the Left Hand in String Players.” Science 270
(1995): 305-7.

Eliassen, J. C., K. Baynes, and M. S. Gazzaniga. “Direction Information Coordinated


Via the Posterior Third of the Corpus Callosum During Bimanual Movements.”
Experimental Brain Research 128 (1999): 573-77.

________. “Anterior and Posterior Callosal Contributions to Simultaneous Bimanual


Movements of the Hands and Fingers.” Brain 123 (2000): 2501-11.

Elliot, D., and M. Jaeger. “Practice and the Visual Control of Manual Aiming
Movements.” Journal of Human Movement Studies, 14 (1988): 279-91.

Engel, K. C., M. Flanders, and J. F. Soechting. “Anticipatory and Sequential Motor


Control in Piano Playing.” Experimental Brain Research 113 (1997): 189-99.

Enoka, R. M. “Muscle Strength and its Development: New Perspectives.” Sports


Medicine 6 (1988): 146-68.

________. “Neural Adaptations with Chronic Physical Activity.” Journal of


Biomechanics 30(5) (1997): 448.

Enoka, R. M. and A. J. Fuglevand. “Neuromuscular Basis of the Maximum Voluntary


Force Capacity of Muscle.” In Current Issues in Biomechanics, ed. M. D. Grabiner,
215-35. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 1993.

Ericsson, K. A. “Memory Skill.” Canadian Journal of Psychology 39 (1985): 188-231.

________. “Analysis of Memory Performance in Terms of Memory Skill.” In


Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence, 4th ed., ed. R. J. Sternberg, 137-
79. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1988.
155

REFERENCES - Continued

________. “Development of Elite Performance and Deliberate Practice: An Update


From the Perspective of the Expert Performance Approach.” In Expert Performance
in Sports: Advances in Research on Sport Expertise, ed. Janet L. Starkes and K.
Anders Ericsson, 49-83. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 2003.

Ericsson, K. A. and J. Smith. “Prospects and Limits of the Empirical Study of Expertise:
An Introduction.” In Toward a General Theory of Expertise: Prospects and Limits,
ed. K. A. Ericsson and J. Smith, 1-39. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
Press, 1991b.

Ericsson, K. A. and J. Smith, ed. Toward a General Theory of Expertise: Prospects and
Limits. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, 1991a.

Ericsson, K. A., Ralph Th. Krampe, and Clements Tesch-Römer. “The Role of
Deliberate Practice in the Acquisition of Expert Performance.” Psychological Review
100(3) (1993): 363-406.

Erim, Z., C. J. De Luca, K. Mineo, and T. Aoki. “Rank-Ordered Regulation of Motor


Units.” Muscle & Nerve 19 (1996): 563-73.

Fadiga, L., L. Craighero, G. Buccino, and G. Rizzolatti. “Speech Listening Specifically


Modulates the Excitability of Tongue Muscles: A TMS Study.” European Journal of
Neuroscience 15 (2002): 399-402.

Fahrer, M. “Interdependent and Independent Actions of the Fingers.” In The Hand , ed.
R. Tubania, pp. 399-403. Philadelphia: Saunders, 1981.

Faillenot, I., I. Toni, J. Decety, M. C. Gregoire, and M. Jeannerod. “Visual Pathways for
Object-Oriented Action and Object Recognition, Functional Anatomy with PET.”
Cerebral Cortex 7 (1997): 77-85.

Fairbanks, G. “Systematic Research in Experimental Phonetics: 1. A Theory of the


Speech Mechanism as a Servosystem.” Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 19
(1954): 133-39.

Fairbanks, G. and N. Guttman. “Effects of Delayed Auditory Feedback Upon


Articulation.” Journal of Speech & Hearing Research 1 (1958): 12-22.

Farah, M. J. “The Neural Bases of Mental Imagery.” In The New Cognitive


Neurosciences, ed. M.S. Gazzaniga, 965-74. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2000.
156

REFERENCES - Continued

Farah, M. J. and A. F. Smith. “Perceptual Interference and Facilitation with Auditory


Imagery.” Perception & Psychophysics 33 (1983): 475-8.

Faulkner, J. A. and S. V. Brooks. “Muscle Fatigue in Old Animals. Unique Aspects of


Fatigue in Elderly Humans.” Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology 384
(1995): 471-80.

Favilla M., W. Hening, and C. Ghez. “Trajectory Control in Targeted Force Impulses.
VI. Independent Specification of Response Amplitude and Direction.” Experimental
Brain Research 75 (1989): 280-94.

Feltz, D. L. and D. M. Landers. “The Effects of Mental Practice on Motor Skill Learning
and Performance: A Meta-analysis.” Journal of Sports Psychology 5 (1983): 25-7.

Feng-Chen, K. C. and J. R. Wolpaw. “Operant Conditioning of H-Reflex Changes


Synaptic Terminals on Primate Motoneurons.” Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences of the United States of America 93 (1996): 9206-11.

Fentress, J. C. “Development of Grooming in Mice with Amputated Forelimbs.”


Science 179(74) (1973): 704-5.

Fiez, J. A., S. E. Petersen, M. K. Cheney, and M. E. Raichie. “Impaired Nonmotor


Learning and Error-Detection Associated with Cerebellar Damage—A Single Case
Study.” Brain 115 (1992): 155-78.

Filimon, Flavia, Jonathan D. Nelson, Donald J. Hagler, and Martin I. Sereno. “Human
Cortical Representations for Reaching: Mirror Neurons for Execution, Observation,
and Imagery.” NeuroImage 37(4) (2007): 1315-28.

Finke, R. A. Principles of Mental Imagery. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1989.

Finlow, Simon. “The Twenty-Seven Etudes and Their Antecedents.” In The Cambridge
Companion to Chopin, ed. Jim Samson. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press,
1992.

Finney, Steven A. “Auditory Feedback and Musical Keyboard Performance.” Music


Perception 15 (1997): 153-74.

Finney, Steven A. and Caroline Palmer. “Auditory Feedback and Memory for Music
Performance: Sound Evidence for an Encoding Effect.” Memory & Cognition 31(1)
(2003): 51-64.
157

REFERENCES - Continued

Fish J., J. F. Soechting. “Synergistic Finger Movements in a Skilled Motor Task.”


Experimental Brain Research 91 (1992): 327-34.

Fisk, A. D. and W. Schneider. “Memory as a Function of Attention, Level of Processing,


and Automatization.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition 10 (1984): 181-97.

Fitts, P. “Perceptual-Motor Skill Learning.” In Categories of Human Learning, ed. A.


W. Melton, 243-85. New York: Academic Press, 1964.

Fitts, P. and M. I. Posner. Human Performance. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole, 1967.

Fitts, R. H. and J. J. Widrick. “Muscle Mechanics: Adaptations with Exercise-Training.”


Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews 24 (1996): 427-73.

Flanagan, J. R. and A. M. Wing. “The Role of Internal Models in Motion Planning and
Control: Evidence from Grip Force Adjustments During Movements of Hand-Held
Loads.” Journal of Neuroscience 17(4) (1997): 1519-28.

Flanagan J. R., P. Vetter, R. S. Johansson, and D. M. Wolpert. “Prediction Precedes


Control in Motor Learning.” Current Biology 13 (2003): 146-150.

Franz, E. A. “Spatial Coupling in the Coordination of Complex Actions.” Quarterly


Journal of Experimental Psychology A50 (1997): 684–704.

Franz, E. A., H. N. Zelaznik, S. P. Swinnen, and C. B. Walter. “Spatial Conceptual


Influences on the Coordination of Bimanual Actions: When a Dual Task Becomes a
Single Task.” Journal of Motor Behavior 33 (2001): 103-12.

Franz, E. A., J. C. Eliassen, R. B. Ivry, and M. S. Gazzaniga. “Dissociation of Spatial


and Temporal Coupling in the Bimanual Movements of Callosotomy Patients.”
Psychological Science 7 (1996): 306-10.

Franz, E. A. and V. S. Ramachandran. “Bimanual Coupling in Amputees with Phantom


Limbs.” Nature Neuroscience 1(6) (1998): 443-4.

Freund, H. J., H. J. Büdingen, and V. Dietz. “Activity of Single Motor Units from
Human Forearm Muscles During Voluntary Isometric Contractions.” Journal of
Neurophysiology 38 (1975): 933-46.
158

REFERENCES - Continued

Fuchs, A., V. K. Jirsa, H. Haken, and J. A. Kelso. “Extending the HKB Model of
Coordinated Movement to Oscillators with Different Eigenfrequencies.” Biological
Cybernetics 74 (1996): 21-30.

Gaab, N., C. Gaser, T. Zaehle, L. Jäncke, and G. Schlaug. “Functional Anatomy of Pitch
Memory—An fMRI Study with Sparse Temporal Sampling.” NeuroImage 19 (2003):
1417-26.

Gabriel, D., G. Kamen, and G. Frost. “Neural Adaptations to Resistive Exercise:


Mechanisms and Recommendations for Training Practices.” Sports Medicine 36 (2):
133-49.

Gabrielsson, A. “The Performance of Music.” In The Psychology of Music, 2nd ed., ed.
D. Deutsch, 501-602. San Diego: Academic Press, 1999.

Gagné, R. M. The Conditions of Learning, 2nd ed. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston,
1970.

Galganski, M. E., A. J. Fuglevand, and R. M. Enoka. “Reduced Control of Motor Output


in a Human Hand Muscle of Elderly Subjects During Submaximal Contractions.”
Journal of Neurophysiology 69 (1993): 2108–15.

Gallese, V., L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi, and G. Rizzolatti. “Action Recognition in the


Premotor Cortex.” Brain 119 (1996): 593-609.

Galton, F. Hereditary Genius: An Inquiry Into its Laws and Consequences. New York:
Horizon Press, 1952.

Gandevia, S. “Spinal and Supraspinal Factors in Human Muscle Fatigue.” Physiological


Reviews 2001 (81): 1725-89.

Ganz, Peter Felix. “The Development of the Etude for Pianoforte.” Ph. D. diss.,
Northwestern University, 1960.

Gardiner, P., Y. Dai, and C. J. Heckman. “Effects of Exercise Training on α-


Motoneurons.” Journal of Applied Physiology 101 (2006): 1228-36.

Gates, A. and J. Bradshaw. “Effects of Auditory Feedback on a Musical Performance


Task.” Perception & Psychophysics, 16 (1974): 105-9.

Gauthier, G. M. and J. M. Hofferer. “Eye Tracking of Self-Moved Targets in Absence of


Vision.” Experimental Brain Research 26 (1976): 121-39.
159

REFERENCES - Continued

Georgopoulos, A. P. “Population Activity in the Control of Movement.” International


Review of Neurobiology 37 (1994): 103-19.

________. “Current Issues in Directional Motor Control.” Trends in Neuroscience 18


(1995): 506-10.

________. “Arm Movements in Monkeys: Behavior and Neurophysiology.”


Journal of Comparative Physiology [A] 179 (1996): 603-12.

________. “Neural Aspects of Cognitive Motor Control.” Current Opinions in


Neurobiology 10 (2000): 238-41.

Gerardin, E., A. Sirigu, S. Lehéricy, J. B. Poline, B. Gaymard, C. Marsault, Y. Agid, and


D. Le Bihan. “Partially Overlapping Neural Networks for Real and Imagined Hand
Movements.” Cerebral Cortex 10(11) (2000): 1093-1104.

Gerloff C., B. Corwell, R. Chen, M. Hallett, and L. G. Cohen. “The Role of the Human
Motor Cortex in the Control of Complex and Simple Finger Movement Sequences.”
Brain 121 (1998): 1695-1709.

Ghez, C., M. Favilla, M. F. Ghilardi, J. Gordon, R. Bermejo, and S. Pullman. “Discrete


and Continuous Planning of Hand Movements and Isometric Force Trajectories.”
Experimental Brain Research 115 (1997): 217-33.

Ghez, C., W. Hening, and J. Gordon. “Organization of Voluntary Movement.” Current


Opinion in Neurobiology 1 (1991): 664-71.

Gibson, E. J. Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development. Englewood Cliffs,


NJ: Prentice Hall, 1969.

Gilbert, A. N. and C. J. Wysocki. “Hand Preference and Age in the United States.”
Neuropsychologia 30 (1992): 601-8.

Glasauer, S. “Cerebellar Contribution to Saccades and Gaze Holding: A Modeling


Approach.” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1004 (2003): 206-19.

Gordon, A. M., G. Westling, K. J. Cole, and R. S. Johansson. “Memory Representations


Underlying Motor Commands Used During Manipulation of Common and Novel
Objects.” Journal of Neurophysiology 69 (1993): 1789-96.
160

REFERENCES - Continued

Gordon, A. M., H. Forssberg, and N. Iwasaki. “Formation and Lateralization of Internal


Representations Underlying Motor Commands During Precision Grip.”
Neuropsychologia 32 (1994): 555-68.

Gordon, A. M., H. Forssberg, R. S. Johansson, and G. Westling. “Visual Size Cues in the
Programming of Manipulative Forces During Precision Grip.” Experimental Brain
Research 83 (1991): 477-82.

Gordon, E. E. Learning Sequences in Music: Skill, Content, and Pattern. A Music


Learning Theory, 4th ed. Chicago: GIA, 1993.

________. Learning Theory, Patterns, and Music. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets &
Zeitlinger, 2001.

Gordon, J. and C. Ghez. “Trajectory Control in Targeted Force Impulses. II. Pulse
Height Control.” Experimental Brain Research 67 (1987a): 241-52.

________. “Trajectory Control in Targeted Force Impulses. III. Compensatory


Adjustments for Initial Errors.” Experimental Brain Research 67 (1987b): 253-69.

Gordon, J., M. F. Ghilardi, and C. Ghez. “Accuracy of Planar Reaching Movements. I.


Independence of Direction and Extent Variability.” Experimental Brain Research 99
(1994b): 97-111.

Gordon, J., M. F. Ghilardi, S. E. Cooper, and C. Ghez. “Accuracy of Planar Reaching


Movements. II. Systematic Extent Errors Resulting from Inertial Anisotropy.”
Experimental Brain Research 99 (1994a): 112-30.

Göttestam, K. O. “Left-Handedness Among Students of Architecture and Music.”


Perceptual and Motor Skills 70 (1990): 1323-27.

Grasby, P. M., C. D. Frith, K. J. Friston, C. Bench, R. S. J. Frackowiak and R. J. Dolan.


“Functional Mapping of Brain Areas Implicated in Auditory-Verbal Memory
Function.” Brain 1 (1993): 1-20.

Graton, S. T., M. A. Arbib, L. Fadiga, and G. Rizzolatti. “Localization of Grasp


Representation in Humans by Positron Emission Tomography. 2: Observation
Compared with Imagination.” Experimental Research 112 (1996a): 103-11.

Graziano, M. “The Organization of Behavioral Repertoire in Motor Cortex.” Annual


Review of Neuroscience 29 (2006): 105-34.
161

REFERENCES - Continued

Graziano, M. S. A., C. S. R. Taylor, T. Moore, and D. F. Cooke. “The Cortical Control


of Movement Revisited.” Neuron 36 (2002): 1-20.

Greenough, W. T., J. R. Larson, and G. S. Withers. “Effects of Unilateral and Bilateral


Training in a Reaching Task on Dendritic Branching of Neurons in the Rat Motor-
Sensory Forelimb Cortex.” Behavioral and Neural Biology 44 (1985): 301-14.

Grewingk, Maria von. Eine Tochter Alt-Rigas, Schülerin Chopins. Riga: Löfflerm
1928.

Grèzes, J., J. L. Armony, J. Rowe, and R. E. Passingham. “Activations Related to


‘Mirror’ and ‘Canonical’ Neurones in the Human Brain: An fMRI Study.”
NeuroImage 18 (2003): 928-37.

Grèzes, J. and J. Decety. “Functional Anatomy of Execution, Mental Simulation,


Observation, and Verb Generation of Actions: A Meta-Analysis.” Human Brain
Mapping 12 (2001): 1-19.

Griffiths, T. D., C. Büchel, R. S. J. Frackowiak and R. D. Patterson. “Analysis of


Temporal Structure in Sound by the Human Brain.” Nature Neuroscience 1 (1998):
422-7.

Griffiths, T. D., I. Johnsrude, J. L. Dean, and G. G. R. Green. “A Common Neural


Substrate for the Analysis of Pitch and Duration Pattern in Segmented Sound?”
NeuroReport 10 (1999): 3825-30.

Grush, R. “The Emulation Theory of Representation: Motor Control, Imagery, and


Perception.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27 (2004): 377-442.

Guillery, R. W. “Branching Thalamic Afferents Link Action and Perception.” Journal


of Neurophysiology 90 (2003): 539-48.

Gustin, W. C. “The Development of Exceptional Research Mathematicians.” In


Developing Talent in Young People, ed. B. S. Bloom, 270-331. New York:
Ballantine Books, 1985.

Häger-Ross, C. and M. H. Schieber. “Quantifying the Independence of Human Finger


Movements: Comparisons of Digits, Hands, and Movement Frequencies.” Journal of
Neuroscience 20(22) (2000): 8542-50.

Haken, H., J. Kelso, and H. Bunz. “A Theoretical-Model of Phase-Transitions in Human


Hand Movements.” Biological Cybernetics 51 (1985): 347-56.
162

REFERENCES - Continued

Häkkinen, K., R. U. Newton, S. E. Gordon, M. McCormick, J. S. Volek, B. C. Nindi, L.


A. Gotshalk, W. W. Campbell, W. J. Evans, A. Häkkinen, B. J. Humphries, and W. J.
Kraemer. “Changes in Muscle Morphology, Electromyographic Activity, and Force
Production Characteristics During Progressive Strength Training in Young and Older
Men.” Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences Medical Sciences 53
(1998): B415-23.

Hall, C., L. Bernoties, and D. Schmidt. “Interference Effects of Mental Imagery on a


Motor Task.” British Journal of Psychology 86 (1995): 181-90.

Halland, K. Y. and D. L. Harrington. “Hemispheric Asymmetry of Movement.” Current


Opinion in Neurobiology 6 (1996): 796-800.

Hallett, M. and J. Grafman. “Executive Function and Motor Skill Learning.” In The
Cerebellum and Cognition, ed. J. D. Schmahmann, 297-323. New York: Academic
Press, 1997.

Halpern, A. R. “Mental Scanning in Auditory Images for Songs.” Journal of


Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 14 (1988): 434-43.

________. “Memory for the Absolute Pitch in Familiar Songs.” Memory & Cognition
17 (1989): 572-81.

________. “Musical Aspects of Auditory Imagery.” In Auditory Imagery, ed. D.


Reisberg, 1-28. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1992.

________. “Cerebral Substrates of Musical Imagery.” In The Biological


Foundations of Music (Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences), ed. R.J.
Zatorre and I. Peretz, Vol. 930, 179-92. New York: New York Academy of Sciences,
2001.

Halpern, A. R. and R. J. Zatorre. “When that Tune Runs Through Your Head: A PET
Investigation of Auditory Imagery for Familiar Melodies.” Cerebral Cortex 9 (1999):
697-704.

Halpern, A. R., R. J. Zatorre, M. Bouffard, and J. A. Johnson. “Behavioral and Neural


Correlates of Perceived and Imagined Musical Timbre.” Neuropsychologia 42(9)
(2004): 1281-92.

Halsband, U. “Left Hemisphere Preponderance in Trajectorial Learning.” NeuroReport


3 (1992): 397-400.
163

REFERENCES - Continued

Halsband, U. and R. K. Lange. “Motor Learning in Man: A Review of Functional and


Clinical Studies.” Journal of Physiology, Paris 99 (2006): 414-24.

Hamada, Y., T. Nozawa, H. Kado, and R. Suzuki. “Different Laterality Between the
Thumb and Index Finger in Human SII Activities.” NeuroReport 11 (2000): 3603-06.

Hamze, F., M. Rijntjes, C. Dettmers, V. Glauche, C. Weiller, and C. Buchel. “The


Human Action Recognition System and its Relationship to Broca’s Area: An fMRI
Study.” NeuroImage 19 (2003): 637– 44.

Hanakawa, T., I. Immisch, K. Toma, M. A. Dimyan, P. Van Gelderen, and M. Hallett.


“Functional Properties of Brain Areas Associated with Motor Execution and
Imagery.” Journal of Neurophysiology 89 (2003): 989-1002.

Handel, S. and G. R. Lawson. “The Contextual Nature of Rhythmic Interpretation.”


Perception & Psychophysics 34(2) (1983): 103-20.

Handel, S. and J. S. Oshinsky. “The Meter of Syncopated Auditory Polyrhythms.”


Perception & Psychophysics 30(1) (1981): 1-9.

Hardyck, C. and L. F. Petrinovich. “Left-Handedness.” Psychological Bulletin 84


(1977): 385-404.

Harris, C. M. and Wolpert, D. M. “Signal-Dependent Noise Determines Motor


Planning.” Nature 394 (1998): 780-4.

Harris, C. S. “Perceptual Adaptation to Inverted, Reversed, and Displaced Vision.”


Psychological Review 72 (1965): 419-44.

Hasegawa, T., K. Matsuki, T. Ueno, Y. Maeda, Y. Matsue, Y. Konishi, and N. Sadato.


“Learned Audio-Visual Cross-Modal Associations in Observed Piano Playing
Activate the Left Planum Temporale. An fMRI Study.” Cognitive Brain Research 20
(2004): 510-18.

Haslinger, B., P. Erhard, E. Altenmüller, A. Hennenlotter, M.


Schwaiger, H. G. von Einsiedel, E. Rummeny, B. Conrad, and A. O. Ceballos-
Baumann. “Reduced Recruitment of Motor Association Areas During Bimanual
Coordination in Concert Pianists.” Human Brain Mapping 22 (2004): 206-15.

Haslinger, B., P. Erhard, E. Altenmüller, U. Schroeder, H. Boecker, and A. O. Ceballos-


Baumann. “Transmodal Sensorimotor Networks During Action Observation in
Professional Pianists.” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17(2) (2005): 282-93.
164

REFERENCES - Continued

Hassler, M. and N. Birbaumer. “Handedness, Musical Abilities, and Dihaptic and


Dichotic Performance in Adolescents: A Longitudinal Study.” Developmental
Neuropsychology 4 (1988): 129-45.

Hather, B. M., P. A. Tesch, P. Buchanan, and G. A. Dudley. “Influence of Eccentric


Actions on Skeletal Muscle Adaptations to Resistance Training.” Acta Physiologica
Scandinavica 143 (1991): 177-85.

Haueisen, J. and T. R. Knösche. “Involuntary Motor Activity in Pianists Evoked by


Music Perception.” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13(6) (2001): 786-92.

Hay, J. C., H. L. Pick, and K. Ikeda. “Visual Capture Produced by Prism Spectacles.”
Psychonomic Science 2 (1965): 215-6.

Heiser, M., M. Iacoboni, F. Maeda, J. Marcus, and J. C. Mazziotta. “The Essential Role
of Broca’s Area in Imitation.” European Journal of Neuroscience 17 (2003): 1123-8.

Hellman, H. Great Feuds in Science: Ten of the Liveliest Disputes Ever. New York:
Wiley, 1998.

Helsen, W. F., J. L. Starkes, and N. J. Hodges. “Team Sports and the Theory of
Deliberate Practice.” Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology 20 (1998): 12-34.

Henshaw, E. M. and P. G. Homan. “A Note on Over-Training.” British Journal of


Psychology 20 (1930): 333-5.

Henson, R. N. A., M. D. Rugg, T. Shallice, O. Josephs, and R. J. Dolan. “Recollection


and Familiarity in Recognition Memory: An Event Related Functional Magnetic
Resonance Imaging Study.” Journal of Neuroscience 19 (1999): 3962-72.

Heuer, H., W. Spijkers, T. Kleinsorge, H. van der Loo, and C. Steglich, “The Time-
Course of Cross-Talk During the Simultaneous Specification of Bimanual Movement
Amplitudes.” Experimental Brain Research 118 (1998): 381-92.

Hickok, G. and D. Poeppel. “Dorsal and Ventral Streams: A Framework for


Understanding Aspects of the Functional Anatomy of Language.” Cognition 92
(2004): 131-8.

Hicks, R. E. “Asymmetry of Bilateral Transfer.” American Journal of Psychology 87


(1974): 667-74.
165

REFERENCES - Continued

Hicks, R. E., J. M. Frank, and M. Kinsbourne. “The Locus of Bimanual Skill Transfer.”
The Journal of General Psychology 107 (1982): 277-81.

Hickson, R. C., K. Hidaka, C. Foster, M. T. Falduto, and R. T. Chatterton. “Successive


Time Courses of Strength Development and Steroid Hormone Responses to Heavy-
Resistance Training.” Journal of Applied Physiology 76 (1994): 663-70.

Higgins, Joseph R. and Ree K. Spaeth. “Relationship Between Consistency of


Movement and Environmental Condition.” Quest 17(1) (1972): 619.

Highben, Z. and C. Palmer. “Effects of Auditory and Motor Mental Practice in


Memorized Piano Performance.” Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music
Education 159 (2004): 58-65.

Hikosaka, O., H. Nakahara, M. K. Rand, K. Sakai, X. Lu, K. Nakamura, S. Miyachi,


and K. Doya. “Parallel Neural Networks for Learning Sequential Procedures.”
Trends in Neurosciences 22 (1999): 464-71.

Hochman, S. and D. McCrea. “Effects of Chronic Spinalization on Ankle Extensor


Motoneurons. II. Motoneuron Electrical Properties.” Journal of Neurophysiology
71(1994): 1468-79.

Hodge, T. and J. Deakin. “Deliberate Practice and Expertise in the Martial Arts: The
Role of Context in Motor Recall.” Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology 20 (1998):
260-79.

Holcomb, H. H., D. R. Medoff, P. J. Caudill, Z. Zaho, A. C. Lahti, R. F. Dannals, and C.


A. Tamminga. “Cerebral Blood Flow Relationships Associated with a Difficult Tone
Recognition Task in Trained Normal Volunteers.” Cerebral Cortex 8 (1998): 534-42.

Holland, Jeanne. “Chopin’s Piano Method.” Piano Quarterly 33(129) (1985): 32-43.

________. “Chopin the Teacher.” Journal of the American Liszt Society 17 (1985): 39-
48.

Houk, J. C., J. L. Davis, and D. G. Beiser, ed. Models of Information Processing in the
Basal Ganglia. Cambridge: MIT Press, 1995.

Howard, J. D. and R. M. Enoka. “Maximum Bilateral Contractions are Modified by


Neurally Mediated Interlimb Effects.” Journal of Applied Physiology 70 (1991): 306-
16.
166

REFERENCES - Continued

Huang, He, Steven L. Wolf, and Jiping He. “Recent Developments in Biofeedback for
Neuromotor Rehabilitation.” Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation 3(11)
(2006), http://www.jneuroengrehab.com/content/3/1/11.

Hubbard, T. L. and K. Stoeckig. “Musical Imagery: Generation of Tones and Chords.”


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 14 (1988):
656-67.

________. “The Representation of Pitch in Musical Imagery.” In


Auditory Imagery, ed. D. Reisberg, 199-236. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1992.

Hughes, H. C., T. M. Darcey, H. I. Barkan, P. D. Williamson, D. W. Roberts, and C. H.


Aslin. “Responses of Human Auditory Association Cortex to the Omission of an
Expected Acoustic Event.” NeuroImage 13 (2001): 1073-89.

Hund-Georgiadis, M., and D. Y. von Cramon. “Motor-Learning-Related Changes in


Piano Players and Non-Musicians Revealed by Functional Magnetic-Resonance
Signals.” Experimental Brain Research 125(4) (April 1999): 417-24.

Hutchinson, S., L. H. Lee, N. Gaab, and G. Schlaug. “Cerebellar Volume of Musicians.”


Cerebral Cortex 13(9) (2003): 943-9.

Iacoboni, M., I. Molnar-Szakacs, V. Gallese, G. Buccino, J. C. Mazziotta, and G.


Rizzolatti. “Grasping the Intentions of Others with One’s Own Mirror Neuron
System.” PLoS Biol 3(3) (2005): 529-35.

Iacoboni, M., R. P. Woods, M. Brass, H. Bekkering, J. C. Mazziotta, and G. Rizzolatti.


“Cortical Mechanisms of Human Imitation.” Science 286 (1999): 2526-28.

Ibbotson, N. R. and J. Morton. “Rhythm and Dominance.” Cognition 9 (1981): 125-38.

Imamizu, H., S. Miyauchi, T. Tamada, Y. Sasaki, R. Takino, B. Pütz, T. Yoshioka, and


M. Kawato. “Human Cerebellar Activity Reflecting an Acquired Internal Model of a
New Tool.” Nature 403 (2000): 192-5.

Imamizu, H., T. Kuroda, S. Miyauchi, T. Yoshioka, and M. Kawato. “Modular


Organization of Internal Models of Tools in the Human Cerebellum.” Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100 (2003): 5461-
66.
167

REFERENCES - Continued

Indovina, I. and J. N. Sanes. “Combined Visual Attention and Finger Movement Effects
on Human Brain Representations.” Experimental Brain Research 140 (2001): 265-
79.

Isaac, A. R. and D. F. Marks. “Individual Differences in Imagery Experience:


Developmental Changes and Specialization.” British Journal of Psychology 85
(1994): 479-500.

Ito, M. “Mechanisms of Motor Learning in the Cerebellum.” Brain Research 886(1-2)


(2000): 237-45.

Ivry, R. “Cerebellar Timing Systems.” In The Cerebellum and Cognition, ed. J. D.


Schmahmann, 556-73. New York: Academic Press, 1997.

Ivry, R. B. and E. Hazeltine. “Subcortical Locus of Temporal Coupling in the Bimanual


Movements of a Callosotomy Patient.” Human Movement Science 18 (1999): 345-
75.

Ivry, R. B. and J. A. Fiez. “Cerebellar Contributions to Cognition and Imagery.” In The


New Cognitive Neurosciences, 2nd ed., ed. M. S. Gazzaniga, 999-1011. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press, 2000.

Jackendoff, R. “Musical Parsing and Musical Affect.” Music Perception 9 (1991): 199-
230.

Jackson, P. L., M. F. Lafleur, F. Malouin, C. Richards, and J. Doyon. “Potential Role of


Mental Practice Using Motor Imagery in Neurologic Rehabilitation.” Archives of
Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 82 (2001): 1133-41.

Jacobs, K. M. and J. P. Donoghue. “Reshaping the Cortical Motor Map by Unmasking


Latent Intracortical Connections.” Science 251(4996) (1991): 944-7.

Jagacinski, R. J., E. Marshburn, S. T. Klapp, and M. R. Jones. “Test of Parallel Versus


Integrated Structure in Polyrhythmic Tapping.” Journal of Motor Behavior 20 (1988):
416-42.

James, W. Principles of Physiology. New York: Holt, 1890.

Janata, P. “Brain Electrical Activity Evoked by Mental Formation of Auditory


Expectations and Images.” Brain Topography 13 (2001a): 169-93.
168

REFERENCES - Continued

________. “Neurophysiological Measurements Underlying Auditory Image Formation


in Music.” In Musical Imagery, ed. R. I. Godoy and H. Jorgensen, 27-41. Lisse, The
Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger, 2001b.

Janata, P., J. L. Birk, J. D. Van Horn, M. Leman, B. Tillmann, and J. Bharucha. “The
Cortical Topography of Tonal Structures Underlying Western Music.” Science 298
(2002): 2167-70.

Janata, P. and K. Paroo. “Acuity of Auditory Images in Pitch and Time.” Perception &
Psychophysics 68(5) (2006): 829-44.

Janata, P. and S. T. Grafton. “Swinging in the Brain: Shared Neural Substrates for
Behaviors Related to Sequencing and Music.” Nature Neuroscience 6 (2003): 682-7.

Jäncke, L., G. Schlaug, and H. Steinmetz. “Hand Skill Asymmetry in Professional


Musicians.” Brain and Cognition 34 (1997): 424-32.

Jäncke, L., N. J. Shah, and M. Peters. “Cortical Activations in Primary and Secondary
Motor Areas for Complex Bimanual Movements in Professional Pianists.” Cognitive
Brain Research 10(1-2) (2000): 177-83.

Jarvelainen, J. and M. Schurmann. “The Motor Cortex Approximately 20 Hz Rhythm


Reacts Differently to Thumb and Middle Finger Stimulation: An MEG Study.”
NeuroReport 13 (2002): 1243-46.

Jeannerod, M. “The Representing Brain. Neural Correlates of Motor Intention and


Imagery.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1994): 187-245.

________. “Mental Imagery in the Motor Context.” Neuropsychology 33 (1995): 1419-


32.

________. “Visual and Action Cues Contribute to the Self-Other Distinction.” Nature
Neuroscience 5 (2004): 422-3.

Jeannerod, M. and J. Decety. “Mental Motor Imagery: A Window Into the


Representational Stages of Action.” Current Opinion in Neurobiology 5 (1995): 727-
32.

Jeannerod, M. and V. Frak. “Mental Imaging of Motor Activity in Humans.” Current


Opinion in Neurobiology 9(6) (1999): 735-9.
169

REFERENCES - Continued

Jog, M. S., Y. Kubota, C. I. Connolly, V. Hillegaart, and A. M. Graybiel. “Building


Neural Representations of Habits.” Science 286 (1999): 1745-49.

Johansson, R. S. “Sensory Input and Control of Grip.” Novartis Foundation Symposium


218 (1998): 45-59.

Johansson, R. S. and K. Cole. “Sensory-Motor Coordination During Grasping and


Manipulative Actions.” Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2 (1992): 815-23.

Johnston, T. D. and L. Edwards. “Genes, Interactions and the Development of


Behaviour.” Psychological Review 109 (2002): 26-34.

Jones, D. A. and O. M. Rutherford. “Human Muscle Strength Training: The Effects of


Three Different Regimes and the Nature of the Resultant Changes.” Journal of
Physiology (London) 391 (1987): 1-11.

Jones, T. A. “Multiple Synapse Formation in the Motor Cortex Opposite Unilateral


Sensorimotor Cortex Lesions in Adult Rats.” Journal of Comparative Neurology 414
(1999): 57-66.

Jones, T. A., C. J. Chu, L. A. Grande, and A. D. Gregory. “Motor Skills Training


Enhances Lesion-Induced Structural Plasticity in the Motor Cortex of Adult Rats.”
Journal of Neuroscience 19 (1999): 10153-63.

Jordan, M. I. “Computational Aspects of Motor Control and Motor Learning.” In


Handbook of Perception and Action: Motor Skills, ed. H. Heuer and S. Keele, 71-120.
New York: Academic Press, 1996.

Jordan, M. I. and D. E. Rumelhart. “Forward Models: Supervised Learning with a Distal


Teacher.” Cognitive Science 16 (1992): 307-54.

Józsa, L., Z. Demel, E. Vándor, A. Réffy, and I. Szilágyi. “Specific Fibre Composition
of Human Hand and Arm Muscles.” Handchirurgie 10(3) (1978): 153-7.

Kadhiresan, V. A., C. A. Hassett, and J. A. Faulkner. “Properties of Single Motor Units


in Medial Gastrocnemius Muscles of Adult and Old Rats.” Journal of Physiology 493
(1996): 543–52.

Kakei, S., D. S. Hoffman, and P. L. Strick. “Muscle and Movement Representations in


the Primary Motor Cortex.” Science 285 (1999): 2136-39.
170

REFERENCES - Continued

Kalakoski, V. “Effect of Level on Recall of Visually Presented Patterns of Musical


Notes.” Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 48 (2007): 87-96.

Kalaska, J. F. “The Representation of Arm Movements in Postcentral and Parietal


Cortex.” Canadian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology 66 (1988): 455-63.

Kalaska, J. F., R. Caminiti, and A. P. Georgopoulos. “Cortical Mechanisms Related to


the Direction of Two-Dimensional Arm Movements: Relations in Parietal Area 5 and
Comparison with Motor Cortex.” Experimental Brain Research 51 (1983): 247-60.

Kalinkowski, A. G. “The Development of Olympic Swimmers.” In Developing Talent


in Young People, ed. B.S. Bloom, 139-92. New York: Ballantine Books, 1985.

Kamen, G. and C. A. Knight. “Training-Related Adaptations in Motor Unit Discharge


Rate in Young and Older Adults.” The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological
Sciences and Medical Sciences 59 (2004): 1334-38.

Kanda, K. and K. Hashizume. “Changes in Properties of the Medial Gastrocnemius


Motor Units in Aging Rats.” Journal of Neurophysiology. 61 (1989): 737–46.

Kandel, E. R., J. H. Schwartz, and T. M. Jessell. Principles of Neural Science, 4th ed.
New York: McGraw-Hill/Appleton & Lange, 2000.

Kanfer, F. H. and M. K. Stevenson. “The Effects of Self-Regulation on Concurrent


Cognitive Processing.” Cognitive Therapy and Research 6 (1985): 667-84.

Kanfer, R. and P. L. Ackerman. “Motivation and Cognitive Abilities: An


Integrative/Aptitude—Treatment Interaction Approach to Skill Acquisition.” Journal
of Applied Psychology 74 (1989): 657-90.

Karni, A., G. Meyer, C. Rey-Hipolito, P. Jezzard, M. M. Adams, R. Turner, and L. G.


Ungerleider. “The Acquisition of Skilled Motor Performance: Fast and Slow
Experience-Driven Changes in Primary Motor Cortex.” Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 95(3) (1998): 861-8.

Karni, A., G. Meyer, P. Jezzard, M. M. Adams, R. Turner, and L. G. Ungerleider.


“Functional MRI Evidence for Adult Motor Cortex Plasticity During Motor Skill
Learning.” Nature 377(6545) (1995): 155-8.

Kawato, M. “Feedback-Error-Learning Neural Network for Supervised Learning.” In


Advanced Neural Computers, ed. R. Eckmiller, 365-72. Amsterdam: North-Holland,
1990.
171

REFERENCES - Continued

________. “Internal Models for Motor Control and Trajectory Planning.” Current
Opinion in Neurobiology 9 (1999): 718-27.

Kawato, M. and H. Gomi. “The Cerebellum and VOR/OKR Learning Models.” Trends
in Neurosciences 15 (1992): 445-53.

Kawato, M., T. Kuroda, H. Imamizu, E. Nakano, S. Miyauchi, and T. Yoshioka.


“Internal Forward Models in the Cerebellum: fMRI Study on Grip Force and Load
Force Coupling.” Progress in Brain Research 142 (2003): 171-88.

Keele, S. W. “Movement Control in Skilled Motor Performance.” Psychological


Bulletin 70 (1968): 387-401.

________. “Behavioral Analysis of Movement.” In Handbook of Physiology: Section I:


The Nervous System. Volume II. Motor Control, Part 2., ed. V.B. Brooks, 1391-
1414. Bethesda, MA: American Physiological Society, Physiological Society, 1981.

Keele, S. W. and J. J. Summers. “The Structure of Motor Programs.” In Motor Control:


Issues and Trends, ed. G. S. Stelmach, 109-42. New York: Academic Press, 1976.

Keller, A. “Intrinsic Synaptic Organization of the Motor Cortex.” Cerebral Cortex 3


(1993): 430-41.

Kelso, J. A. S. “Phase Transitions and Critical Behavior in Human Bimanual


Coordination.” American Journal of Physiology 246 (1984): R1000–R1004.

________. Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior.


Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press, 1995.

Kelso, J. A. S. and D. A. Engström. The Complementary Nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT


Press, 2006.

Kelso, J. A. S., D. L. Southard, and D. Goodman. “On the Coordination of Two-Handed


Movements.” Journal of Experimental Psychology 5 (1979): 229–38.

________. “On the Nature of Human Interlimb Coordination.” Science 203 (1979):
1029–31.

Kelso, J. A. S., J. J. Buchanan, and S. A. Wallace. “Order Parameters for the Neural
Organization of Single, Multijoint Limb Movement Patterns.” Experimental Brain
Research 85 (1991): 432-44.
172

REFERENCES - Continued

Kelso, J. A. S. and J. J. Jeka. “Symmetry Breaking Dynamics of Human Multilimb


Coordination.” Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and
Performance 18 (1992): 645-68.

Kennerley, S. W., J. Diedrichsen, E. Hazeltine, A. Semjen, and R. B. Ivry. “Callosotomy


Patients Exhibit Temporal Uncoupling During Continuous Bimanual Movements.”
Nature Neuroscience 5(4) (2002): 376-381.

Kermadi, I. and J. P. Joseph. “Activity in Caudate Nucleus of Monkey During Spatial


Sequencing.” Journal of Neurophysiology 74 (1995): 911-33.

Keysers, C., E. Kohler, M. A. Umiltà, L. Nanetti, L. Fogassi, and V. Gallese,


“Audiovisual Mirror Neurons and Action Recognition.” Experimental Brain
Research 153(4) (2003): 628-36.

Kilbreath, S. L., and S. C. Gandevia. “Limited Independent Flexion of the Thumb and
Fingers in Human Subjects.” Journal of Physiology, London 479 (1994): 487-97.

Kilner, J. M., C. Vargas, S. Duval, S. J. Blakemore, and A. Sirigu. “Motor Activation


Prior to Observation of a Predicted Movement.” Nature Neuroscience 7 (2004):
1299-1301.

Kilner, J. M., K. J. Friston, and C. D. Frith. “The Mirror-Neuron System: A Bayesian


Perspective.” NeuroReport 18(6) (2007): 619-23.

Kilner, James M. and Sarah-Jayne Blakemore. “How Does the Mirror Neuron System
Change During Development?” Developmental Science 10(5) (2007): 524-6.

Kimble, G. A. and L. C. Perlmuter. “The Problem of Volition.” Psychological Review 77


(1970): 361-84.

Kitazawa, S., T. Kimura, and P. B. Yin. “Cerebellar Complex Spikes Encode Both
Destinations and Errors in Arm Movements.” Nature 392 (1998): 494-7.

Klapp, S. T., J. M. Nelson, and R. J. Jagacinski. “Can People Tap Concurrent Bimanual
Rhythms Independently?” Journal of Motor Behavior 30(4) (1998): 301-22.

Klapp, S. T., M. D. Hill, J. G. Tyler, Z. E. Martin, R. J. Jagacinski, and M. R. Jones. “On


Marching to Two Different Drummers: Perceptual Aspects of the Difficulties.”
Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance 11
(1985): 814-27.
173

REFERENCES - Continued

Kleczynski, Jean. How to Play Chopin. The Works of Frederic Chopin, Their Proper
Interpretation. Translated by Alfred Whittingham. London: William Reeves, 1913.

Kleim, J. A., E. Lussnig, E. R. Schwarz, T. A. Comery, and W. T. Greenough.


“Synaptogenesis and Fos Expression in the Motor Cortex of the Adult Rat After
Motor Skill Learning.” Journal of Neuroscience 16(14) (1996): 4529-35.

Kleim, J. A., S. Barbay, N. R. Cooper, T. M. Hogg, C. N. Reidel, M. S. Remple, and R. J.


Nudo. “Motor Learning-Dependent Synaptogenesis is Localized to Functionally
Reorganized Motor Cortex.” Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 77 (2002): 63-
77.

Kleim, J. A., S. Barbay, and R. J. Nudo. “Functional Reorganization of the Rat Motor
Cortex Following Motor Skill Learning.” Journal of Neurophysiology 80 (1998):
3321-25.

Kleim, J. A., T. M. Hogg, P. M. VandenBerg, N. R. Cooper, R. Bruneau, and M. Remple.


“Cortical Synaptogenesis and Motor Map Reorganization Occur During Late, but Not
Early, Phase of Motor Skill Learning.” Journal of Neuroscience 24 (2004): 628-33.

Klein, I., A. L. Paradis, J. B. Poline, S. M. Kosslyn, and D. LeBihan. “Transient Activity


in the Human Calcarine Cortex During Visual-Mental Imagery: An Event-Related
fMRI Study.” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12 (2000): 15-23.

Kliegl, R., J. Smith, and P. B. Baltes. “Testing-the-Limits and the Study of Adult Age
Differences in Cognitive Plasticity of a Mnemonic Skill.” Developmental Psychology
25 (1989): 247-56.

________. “On the Locus and Process of Magnification of Age Differences During
Mnemonic Training.” Developmental Psychology 26 (1990): 894-904.

Knight, C. A. and G. Kamen. “Adaptations in Muscular Activation of the Knee Extensor


Muscles with Strength Training in Young and Older Adults.” Journal of
Electromyography and Kinesiology 2001 (11): 405-12.

Koczalski, Raoul. Frédéric Chopin. Betrachtungen, Skizzen, Analysen. Köln: Tischer


& Jagenberg, 1936.

Koelsch S, E. Schröger, and M. Tervaniemi. “Superior Pre-Attentive Auditory


Processing in Musicians.” NeuroReport 10(6) (1999): 1309-13.
174

REFERENCES - Continued

Koh, T. J., M. D. Grabiner, and C. A. Clough. “Bilateral Deficit is Larger for Step Than
for Ramp Isometric Contractions.” Journal of Applied Physiology 74(3) (1993):
1200-5.

Kohl, R. M. and D. L. Roenker. “Mechanism Involvement During Skill Imagery.”


Journal of Motor Behavior 15 (1983): 179-90.

Kohler, Evelyne, Christian Keysers, M. Alessandra Umiltà, Leonardo Fogassi, Vittorio


Gallese, and Giacomo Rizzolatti. “Hearing Sounds, Understanding Actions: Action
Representation in Mirror Neurons.” Science 297(5582) (2002): 846-8.

Kolers, P. A. “Memorial Consequences of Automatized Encoding.” Journal of


Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 1 (1975): 689-701.

Kölsch, S., T. C. Gunter, D. Y. v. Cramon, S. Zysset, G. Lohmann, and A. D. Friederici.


“Bach Speaks: A Cortical ‘Language-network’ Serves the Processing of Music.”
NeuroImage 17(2) (2002): 956-66.

Komi, P. V. “Training of Muscle Strength and Power: Interaction of Neuromotoric,


Hypertrophic, and Mechanical Factors.” International Journal of Sports Medicine 7
(1986): 10-15.

Kornatz, K. W., E. A. Christou, and R. M. Enoka. “Practice Reduces Motor Unit


Discharge Variability in a Hand Muscle and Improves Manual Dexterity in Old
Adults.” Journal of Applied Physiology 98 (2005): 2072-80.

Kosslyn, S. M. and W. L. Thompson. “Shared Mechanisms in Visual Imagery and


Visual Perception: Insights from Cognitive Neuroscience.” In The New Cognitive
Neurosciences, ed. M. S. Gazzaniga, 975-85. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2000.

Kots, Y. M., V. I. Krinskiy, V. L. Naydin, and M. L. Shik. “The Control of Movements


of the Joints and Kinesthetic Afferentation.” In Models of Structural-Functional
Organization of Certain Biological Systems, ed. I. M. Gelfand, V. S. Gurfinkel, S. V.
Fomin, and M. L. Tsetlin, 373-81. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1971.

Kraemer, D. J. M., C. N. Macrae, A. E. Green, and W. M. Kelley. “Musical Imagery:


Sound of Silence Activates Auditory Cortex.” Nature 434 (2005): 158.

Krampe, R. T., and K. Anders Ericsson. “Maintaining Excellence: Deliberate Practice


and Elite Performance in Young and Older Pianists.” Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General 125(4) (1996): 331-59.
175

REFERENCES - Continued

Krampe, R. T., R. Kliegl, U. Mayr, R. Engbert, and D. Vorberg. “The Fast and Slow of
Skilled Bimanual Rhythm Production: Parallel Versus Integrated Timing.” Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 26(1) (2000):
206-33.

Krams, M., M. F. Rushworth, M. P. Deiber, R. S. Frackowiak, and R. E. Passingham.


“The Preparation, Execution and Suppression of Copied Movements in the Human
Brain.” Experimental Brain Research 120 (1998): 386-98.

Krigolson, Olave, and Geraldine Van Gyn. “Is There ‘Feedback’ During Visual
Imagery? Evidence From a Specificity of Practice Paradigm.” Canadian Journal of
Experimental Psychology 60(1) (2006): 24-32.

Krings, T., R. Töpper, H. Foltys, S. Erberich, R. Sparing, K. Willmes, and A. Thron.


“Cortical Activation Patterns During Complex Motor Tasks in Piano Players and
Control Subjects. A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study.” Neuroscience
Letters 278(3) (2000): 189-93.

Kristeva, R., V. Chakarov, J. Schulte-Mönting, and J. Spreer. “Activation of Cortical


Areas in Music Execution and Imagining: A High-resolution EEG Study.”
NeuroImage 20(3) (2003): 1872-83.

Kukulka, C. G. and H. P. Clamann. “Comparison of the Recruitment and Discharge


Properties of Motor Units in Human Brachial Biceps and Adductor Pollicis During
Isometric Contractions.” Brain Research 219 (1981): 45-55.

Lahav, A., A. Boulanger, G. Schlaug, and E. Saltzman. “The Power of Listening:


Auditory-Motor Interactions in Musical Training,” Annals of the New York Academy
of Sciences 1060 (2005): 189-94.

Lahav, A., E. Saltzman, and G. Schlaug. “Action Representation of Sound: Audiomotor


Recognition Network While Listening to Newly Acquired Actions.” The Journal of
Neuroscience 27(2) (2007): 308-14.

Lang, Catherine and Marc H. Schieber. “Differential Impairment of Individuated Finger


Movements in Humans After Damage to the Motor Cortex or the Corticospinal
Tract.” Journal of Neurophysiology 90 (2003): 1160-70.

Langer, E. and G. Imber. “When Practice Makes Imperfect: Debilitating Effects of


Overlearning.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37 (1979): 2014-24.
176

REFERENCES - Continued

Langheim, F. J., J. H. Calicott, V.S. Mattay, J. H. Duyn, and D. R. Weinberger. “Cortical


Systems Associated with Covert Music Rehearsal.” NeuroImage 16(4) (2002): 901-
8.

Larsson, L. “Histochemical Characteristics of Human Skeletal Muscle During Aging.”


Acta Physiologica Scandinavica 117(3) (1983): 469-71.

Larsson, L. and R. L. Moss. “Maximum Velocity of Shortening in Relation to Myosin


Isoform Composition in Single Fibres From Human Skeletal Muscles.” The Journal
of Physiology 472(1) (1993): 595-614.

Lashley, K. S. “The Accuracy of Movement in the Absence of Excitation from the


Moving Organ.” American Journal of Physiology 43 (1917): 169-94.

________. “The Problem of Serial Order in Behavior.” In Cerebral Mechanisms of


Behavior, ed. I. A. Jeffress., 112-31. New York: Wiley, 1951.

Laszlo, J. L., R. A. Baguley, and P. J. Bairstow. “Bilateral Transfer in Tapping Skill in


the Absence of Peripheral Information.” Journal of Motor Behavior 2 (1970): 261-
71.

Lateiner, Jordan E., and Robert L. Sainburg. “Differential Contributions of Vision and
Proprioception to Movement Accuracy.” Experimental Brain Research 151 (2003):
446-54.

Leavitt, J. “Cognitive Demands of Skating and Stick Handling in Ice Hockey.”


Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Sciences 4 (1979): 46-55.

Lee, B. S. “Some Effects of Side-Tone Delay.” Journal of the Acoustical Society of


America 22 (1950): 639-40.

Lee, C. “Psyching Up for a Muscular Endurance Task: Effects of Image Content on


Performance and Mood State.” Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 12 (1990):
66-73.

Lee, D. J., Y. Chen, and G. Schlaug. “Corpus Callosum: Musician and Gender Effects.”
NeuroReport 14(2) (2003): 205-9.

Lee, T. D., S. P. Swinnen, and S. Verschueren. “Relative Phase Alterations During


Bimanual Skill Acquisition.” Journal of Motor Behavior 27 (1995): 263-74.
177

REFERENCES - Continued

Lehéricy, S., H. Benali, P. F. Van de Moortele, M. Pélégrini-Issac, T. Waechter, K.


Ugurbil, and J. Doyon. “Distinct Basal Ganglia Territories are Engaged in Early and
Advanced Motor Sequence Learning.” Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences of the United States of America 102(35) (2005): 12566-71.

Lejeune, M. U., C. Decker, and X. Sanchez. “Mental Rehearsal in Table Tennis


Performance.” Perceptual and Motor Skills 79(1) (1994): 627-41.

Lemon, R. N. “The G. L. Brown Prize Lecture. Cortical Control of the Primate Hand.”
Experimental Physiology 78 (1993): 263-301.

Lenz, William von. “Chopin.” In The Great Piano Virtuosos of Our Time, Trans.
Madeleine R. Baker, 27-74. New York: Schirmer, 1899.

Lesgold, A. M. “Acquiring Expertise.” In Tutorials in Learning and Memory: Essays in


Honor of Gordon Bower, ed. J. R. Anderson and S. M. Kosslyn, 31-60. New York:
Freeman, 1984.

Lewis, B. and D. Linder. “Thinking About Choking? Attentional Processes and


Paradoxical Performance.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 23 (1997):
937-44.

Lewontin, R. C. It Ain’t Necessarily So: The Dream of the Human Genome and Other
Illusions. London: Granta Books, 2000.

Liegeois-Chauvel, C., A. Musolino, J. M. Badier, P. Marquis, and P. Chauvel. “Evoked


Potentials Recorded from the Auditory Cortex in Man: Evaluation and Topography of
the Middle Latency Components.” Electroencephalography and Clinical
Neurophysiology 92 (1994): 204-14.

Liu, Y., Y. Pu, J. H. Gao, L. M. Parsons, J. Xiong, M. Liotti, J. M. Bower, and P. T. Fo.
“The Human Red Nucleus and Lateral Cerebellum in Supporting Roles for Sensory
Information Processing.” Human Brain Mapping 10(4) (2000): 147-59.

Locke, S. and L. Kellar. “Categorical Perception in a Non-Linguistic Mode.” Cortex 9


(1973): 355-69.

Lotze, M., G. Scheler, H. R. Tan, C. Braun, and N. Birbaumer. “The Musician’s Brain:
Functional Imaging of Amateurs and Professionals During Performance and
Imagery.” NeuroImage 20 (2003): 1817-29.
178

REFERENCES - Continued

Lotze, M., P. Montoya, M. Erb, E. Hulsmann, H. Flor, U. Klose, N. Birbaumer, and W.


Grodd. “Activation of Cortical and Cerebellar Motor Areas During Executed and
Imagined Hand Movements: An fMRI Study.” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 11
(1999): 491-501.

Lotze, M., U. Heymans, N. Birbaumer, R. Veit, M. Erb, H. Flor, and U. Halsband.


“Differential Cerebral Activation During Observation of Expressive Gestures and
Motor Acts.” Neuropsychologia 44 (2006): 1787-95.

Macefield, V. G., A. J. Fuglevand, and B. Bigland-Ritchie. “Contractile Properties of


Single Motor Units in Human Toe Extensors Assessed by Intraneural Motor Axon
Stimulation.” Journal of Neurophysiology 75 (1996): 2509-19.

Maddock, R. J., A. S. Garrett, and M. H. Buonocore. “Remembering Familiar People:


The Posterior Cingulate Cortex and Autobiographical Memory Retrieval.”
Neuroscience 104 (2001): 667-76.

Maess, B., S. Koelsch, T. C. Gunter, and A. D. Friederici. “Musical Syntax is Processed


in Broca’s Area: An MEG Study.” Nature Neuroscience 4 (2001): 540-45.

Marr, D. “A Theory of Cerebellar Cortex.” Journal of Physiology 202 (1969): 437-70.

Marsen, C. D., P. A. Merton, and H. B. Morton. “Anticipatory Postural Responses in


the Human Subject.” Journal of Physiology 275(Suppl) (1978): 47-48P.

Marteniuk, R. G., C. L. MacKenzie, and D. M. Baba. “Bimanual Movement Control:


Information Processing and Interaction Effects.” The Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology A36(2) (1984): 335-65.

Marzke, M. W. “Precision Grips, Hand Morphology, and Tools.” American Journal of


Physical Anthropology 102 (1997): 91-110.

Masters, R. S. W. “Knowledge, Knerves, and Know-How: The Role of Explicit Versus


Implicit Knowledge in the Breakdown of a Complex Motor Skill Under Pressure.”
British Journal of Psychology 83 (1992): 343-58.

Masters, R. S. W., R. C. J. Polman, and N. V. Hammond. “‘Reinvestment:’ A Dimension


of Personality Implicated in Skill Breakdown Under Pressure.” Personality &
Individual Differences 14 (1993): 655-66.
179

REFERENCES - Continued

McCartney, N., D. Moroz, S. H. Garner, and A. J. McComas. “The Effects of Strength


Training in Patients with Selected Neuromuscular Disorder.” Medicine and Science in
Sports and Exercise 20 (1988): 362-8.

McDonagh, M. J. N. and C. T. M. Davies. “Adaptive Response of Mammalian Skeletal


Muscle to Exercise with High Loads.” European Journal of Applied Physiology 52
(1984): 139-55.

McGurk, H. and J. McDonald. “Hearing Lips and Seeing Voices.” Nature 264 (1976):
746-8.

Meacci, W. G. and D. L. Pastore. “The Effects of Occluded Vision and Imagery on


Putting Golf Balls.” Perceptual and Motor Skills 80(1) (1995): 179-186.

Mechsner, F., D. Kerzel, G. Knoblich, and W. Prinz. “Perceptual Basis of Bimanual


Coordination.” Nature 414 (2001): 69-73.

Medina, J. F., K. S. Garcia, W. L. Nores, and N. M. Taylor. “Timing Mechanisms in the


Cerebellum: Testing Predictions of a Large-Scale Computer Simulation.” Journal of
Neuroscience 20 (2000): 5516-25.

Mehta, B. and S. Schaal. “Forward Models in Visuomotor Control.” Journal of


Neurophysiology 88 (2002): 942-53.

Meister, I. G., T. Krings, H. Foltys, B. Boroojerdi, M. Müller, R. Töpper, and A. Thron.


“Playing Piano in the Mind—An fMRI Study on Music Imagery and Performance in
Pianists.” Cognitive Brain Research 19 (2004): 219-28.

Mendoza, D. W. and H. Wichman. “‘Inner’ Dart: Effects of Mental Practice on


Performance of Dart Throwing.” Perceptual and Motor Skills 47 (1978): 1195-99.

Merton, P. A. “How We Control the Contraction of Our Muscles.” Scientific American


226 (1972): 30-7.

Meyer, D. E., J. E. K. Smith, and C. E. Wright. “Models for the Speed and Accuracy of
Aimed Movements.” Psychological Review 89 (1982): 449-82.

Meyer-Lohmann, J., C. N. Christakos, and H. Wolf. “Dominance of the Short-Latency


Component in Perturbation Induced Electromyographic Responses of Long-Trained
Monkeys.” Experimental Brain Research 64 (1986): 393-9.
180

REFERENCES - Continued

Miall, R. C. and D. M. Wolpert. “Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control.”


Neural Networks 9 (1996): 1265-79.

Mikuli, Carl. Foreword to Fr. Chopin’s’ Pianoforte-Werke, 17 vols., ed. Carl Mikuli.
Leipzig: Kistner, 1880.

Milisen, R. and C. V. Riper. “Differential Transfer of Training in a Rotary Activity.”


Journal of Experimental Psychology 24 (1939): 640-6.

Milner, A. D. and M. A. Goodale. The Visual Brain in Action. Oxford: Oxford


University Press, 1995.

Minetti, A. E., L. P. Ardigò, and T. McKee. “Keystroke Dynamics and Timing:


Accuracy, Precision and Difference Between Hands in Pianist’s Performance.”
Journal of Biomechanics 40(16) (2007): 3738-43.

Molnar-Szakacs, Istvan and Katie Overy. “Music and Mirror Neurons: From Motion to
‘e’motion.” Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience (2006): 235-41.

Monsaas, J. A. “Learning to be a World-Class Tennis Player.” In Developing Talent in


Young People, ed. B.S. Bloom, 211-69. New York: Ballantine Books, 1985.

Monster, A. W. and H. Chan. “Isometric Force Production by Motor Units of Extensor


Digitorum Communis Muscle in Man.” Journal of Neurophysiology 40 (1977):
1432-43.

Moore, K. L. and A. F. Dalley. “The Upper Limb.” In Clinically Oriented Anatomy, 5th
ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2006.

Moritz, C. T., B. K. Barry, M. A. Pascoe, and R. M. Enoka. “Discharge Rate Variability


Influences the Variation in Force Fluctuations Across the Working Range of a Hand
Muscle.” Journal of Neurophysiology 93 (2005): 2449-59.

Morris, R., D. N. Pandya, and M. Petrides. “Fiber System Linking the Mid-Dorsolateral
Frontal Cortex with the Retrosplenial/Presubicular Region in the Rhesus Monkey.”
Journal of Comparative Neurology 407 (1999): 183-92.

Mottram, C. J., E. A. Christou, F. G. Meyer, and R. M. Enoka. “Frequency Modulation


of Motor Unit Discharge Has Task-Dependent Effects on Fluctuations in Motor
Output.” Journal of Neurophysiology 94 (2005): 2878-87.
181

REFERENCES - Continued

Mulder, T., J. B. H. Hochstenbach, M. J. G. van Heuvelen, and A. R. den Otter. “Motor


Imagery: The Relation Between Age and Imagery Capacity.” Human Movement
Science 26 (2007): 203-11.

Mulder, T., S. Zijlstra, W. Zijlstra, and J. Hochstenbach. “The Role of Motor Imagery in
Learning a Totally Novel Movement.” Experimental Brain Research 154 (2004):
211-17.

Mulder, T. and W. Hulstyn. “Sensory Feedback Therapy and Theoretical Knowledge of


Motor Control and Learning.” American Journal of Physical Medicine 63(5) (1984):
226-44.

Münte, T. F., E. Altenmüller, and L. Jäncke. “The Musician’s Brain as a Model of


Neuroplasticity.” Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 3 (2002): 473-8.

Münte, T. F., W. Nager, T. Beiss, C. Schroeder, and E. Altenmüller. “Specialization of


the Specialized: Electrophysiological Investigations in Professional Musicians.”
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 999 (November 2003): 131-39.

Mussa-Ivaldi, F. A. “Modular Features of Motor Control and Learning.” Current


Opinion in Neurobiology 9 (1999): 713-7.

Nakada, T., Y. Fujii, K. Suzuki, and I. L. Kwee. “‘Musical Brain’ Revealed by High-
Field (3Tesla) Functional MRI.” NeuroReport 9 (1998): 3853-56.

Napier, J. “The Evolution of the Hand.” Scientific American 207 (1962): 56-62.

Narici, M. V., G. S. Roi, L. Landoni, A. E. Minetti, and P. Cerretelli. “Changes in


Forces, Cross-Sectional Areas and Neural Activation During Strength Training and
Detraining of the Human Quadriceps.” European Journal of Applied Physiology 66
(1989): 310-9.

Nelissen, K., G. Luppino, W. Vanduffel, G. Rizzolatti, and G. A. Orban. “Observing


Others: Multiple Action Representation in the Frontal Lobe.” Science 310 (2005):
332-36.

Niecks, Frederick. Frederick Chopin as a Man and Musician. Vol. II. London:
Novello, 1902.

Nishitani, N. and R. Hari. “Temporal Dynamics of Cortical Representation for Action.”


Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 97
(2000): 913-18.
182

REFERENCES - Continued

Nissen, M. J. and P. Bullemer. “Attentional Requirements of Learning: Evidence from


Performance Measures.” Cognitive Psychology 19 (1987): 1-32.

________. “Viewing Lip Forms: Cortical Dynamics.” Neuron 36 (2002): 1211-20.

Noel, R. “The Effect of Visuo-Motor Behavioural Rehearsal of Tennis Performance.”


Journal of Sport Psychology 2 (1980): 221-6.

Nordstrom, M. A., and S. L. Butler. “Reduced Intracortical Inhibition and Facilitation of


Corticospinal Neurons in Musicians.” Experimental Brain Research 144(3) (2002):
336-42.

Nottebohm, Fernando. “Ontogeny of Bird Song.” Science 167(3920) (1970): 950-6.

Nyberg, Lars, Johan Eriksson, Anne Larsson, and Petter Marklund. “Learning by Doing
Versus Learning by Thinking: An fMRI Study of Motor and Mental Training.”
Neuropsychologia 44 (2006): 711-17.

Ohnishi, T., H. Matsuda, T. Asada, M. Aruga, M. Hirakata, M. Nishikawa, A. Katoh, and


E. Imabayashi. “Functional Anatomy of Musical Perception in Musicians.” Cerebral
Cortex 11(8) (2001): 754-60.

Ohtsuki, T. “Decrease in Human Voluntary Isometric Arm Strength Induced by


Simultaneous Bilateral Exertions.” Behavioural Brain Research 7 (1983): 165-78.

Olafsdottir, Halla, Vladimir M. Zatsiorsky, and Mark L. Latash. “Is the Thumb a Fifth
Finger? A Study of Digit Interaction During Force Production Tasks.” Experimental
Brain Research 160 (2005): 203-13.

Oldfield, R. C. “Handedness in Musicians.” British Journal of Psychology 60 (1969):


91-9.

Ortmann, Otto. The Physical Basis of Piano Touch and Tone: An Experimental
Investigation of the Effect of the Player’s Touch Upon the Tone of the Piano. New
York: E.P. Dutton & Co., 1925.

________. The Physiological Mechanics of Piano Technique. New York: E.P. Dutton
& Co., 1929.

Ostry, D .J. and A. G. Feldman. “A Critical Evaluation of the Force Control Hypothesis
in Motor Control.” Experimental Brain Research 153 (2003): 275-88.
183

REFERENCES - Continued

Pantev, C., O. Bertrand, C. Eulitz, C. Verkindt, S. Hampson, G. Schuierer, and T. Elbert.


“Specific Tonotopic Organizations of Different Areas of the Human Auditory Cortex
Revealed by Simultaneous Magnetic and Electric Recordings.”
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 94 (1995): 26-40.

Pantev, C. R. Oostenveld, A. Engelien, B. Ross, L. E. Roberts, and M. Hoke. “Increased


Auditory Cortical Representation in Musicians.” Nature 392 (1998): 811-14.

Parlow, S. E. and M. Kinsbourne. “Asymmetrical Transfer of Training Between Hands:


Implications for Interhemispheric Communication in Normal Brain.” Brain and
Cognition 11 (1989): 98-113.

Parsons, L. M. “Exploring the Functional Neuroanatomy of Music Performance,


Perception and Comprehension.” In The Cognitive Neuroscience of Music, ed. R. J.
Zatorre and I. Peretz, 247-68. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 2003.

Parsons, L. M., J. Sergent, D. A. Hodges, and P. T. Fox. “The Brain Basis of Piano
Performance.” Neuropsychologia 43 (2005): 199-215.

Parsons, L. M. and P. T. Fox. “Sensory and Cognitive Tasks.” In The Cerebellum and
Cognition, International Review of Neurobiology, Vol. 41, ed. J. D. Schmahmann,
255-72. San Diego: Academic Press, 1997.

Pascual-Leone, A., A. Cammarota, E. M. Wassermann, J. P. Brasil-Neto, L. G. Cohen,


and M. Hallett. “Modulation of Motor Cortical Outputs to the Reading Hand of
Braille Readers.” Annals of Neurology 34 (1993): 33-7.

Pascual-Leone, A., D. Nguyet, L. G. Cohen, J. P. Brasil-Neto, A. Cammarota, and M.


Hallett. “Modulation of Muscle Responses Evoked by Transcranial Magnetic
Stimulation During the Acquisition of New Fine Motor Skills.” Journal of
Neurophysiology 74 (1995): 1037-45.

Pascual-Leone, A., E. M. Wassermann, N. Sadato, and M. Hallett. “The Role of Reading


Activity on the Modulation of Motor Cortical Outputs to the Reading Hand in Braille
Readers.” Annals of Neurology 34 (1993): 33-7.

Pascual-Leone, A., J. Grafman, and M. Hallett. “Procedural Learning and Prefrontal


Cortex.” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 769 (1995): 61-70.

Passingham, R. E. Frontal Lobes and Voluntary Action. New York: Oxford University
Press, 1993.
184

REFERENCES - Continued

Patel, V. L. and G. J. Groen. “The General and Specific Nature of Medical Expertise: A
Critical Look.” In Toward a General Theory of Expertise, ed. K.A. Ericsson and J.
Smith, 93-125. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, 1991.

Patten, C. and G. Kamen. “Adaptations in Motor Unit Discharge Activity with Force
Control Training in Young and Older Human Adults.” European Journal of Applied
Physiology and Occupational Physiology 83 (2000): 128-43.

Patuzzo, S., A. Fiaschi, and P. Manganotti. “Modulation of Motor Cortex Excitability in


the Left Hemisphere During Action Observations: A Single- and Paired-Pulse
Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study of Self- and Non-Self-Action Observation.”
Neuropsychologia 41 (2003): 1272-78.

Penfield, W. and E. Boldrey. “Somatic Motor and Sensory Representation in the


Cerebral Cortex of Man as Studied by Electrical Stimulation.” Brain 60 (1937): 389-
443.

Peper, C. E., P. J. Beek, and P. C. W. van Wieringen. “Bifurcation in Polyrhythmic


Tapping: In Search of Farey Principles.” In Tutorials in Motor Neuroscience, ed.
G.E. Stelmach, 413-31. New York: Kluwer, 1991.

________. “Multifrequency Coordination in Bimanual Tapping: Asymmetrical


Coupling and Signs of Supercriticality.” Journal of Experimental Psychology.
Human Perception and Performance 19 (1993): 416-28.

________. “Frequency-Induced Transitions in Bimanual Tapping.” Biological


Cybernetics 73 (1995): 301-9.

Peper, C. L., A. Ridderikhoff, A. Daffertshofer, and P. J. Beek, “Explanatory Limitations


of the HKB Model: Incentives for a Two-Tiered Model of Rhythmic Interlimb
Coordination.” Human Movement Science 23 (2004): 673-97.

Perelle, I. B. and L. Ehrman. “An International Study of Human Handedness: The


Data.” Behavior Genetics 24 (1994): 217-28.

Perez, M. A., B. K. Lungholt, K. Nyborg, and J. B. Nielsen. “Motor Skill Training


Induces Changes in the Excitability of the Leg Cortical Area in Healthy Humans.”
Experimental Brain Research 159 (2004): 197-205.

Perry, H. M. “The Relative Efficiency of Actual and Imaginary Practice in 5 Selected


Tasks.” Archives of Psychology 4 (1939): 5-75.
185

REFERENCES - Continued

Peters, M. “Constraints in the Coordination of Bimanual Movements and Their


Expression in Skilled and Unskilled Subjects.” Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology A37 (1985): 171-96.

Peters, M. and S. Schwartz. “Coordination of the Two Hands and Effects of Attentional
Manipulation in the Production of a Bimanual 2:3 Polyrhythm.” Australian Journal of
Psychology 41(2) (1989): 215-24.

Pew, R. W. “Acquisition of Hierarchical Control Over the Temporal Organization of a


Skill.” Journal of Experimental Psychology 71(5) (1966): 764-71.

Pfordresher, P. Q. “Auditory Feedback in Music Performance: The Role of Melodic


Structure and Musical Skill.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception & Performance 29 (2005): 1331-45.

Pfordresher, P. Q. and C. Palmer. “Effects of Hearing the Past, Present, or Future During
Music Performance.” Perception & Psychophysics 68(3) (2006): 362-76.

Phelps, R. M. and J. Shanteau. “Livestock Judges: How Much Information Can an


Expert Use?” Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 21 (1978): 209-19.

Pick, H. L., D. H. Warren, and J. C. Hay. “Sensory Conflict in Judgments of Spatial


Direction.” Perception and Psychophysics 6 (1969): 203-5.

Platel, H., C. Price, J. C. Baron, R. Wise, J. Lambert, R. Frackowiak, B. Lechevalier,


and F. Eustache. “The Structural Components of Music Perception. A Functional
Anatomical Study.” Brain 120 (1997): 229-43.

Plautz, E. J., G. W. Milliken, and R. J. Nudo. “Effects of Repetitive Motor Training on


Movement Representations in Adult Squirrel Monkeys: Role of Use Versus
Learning.” Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 74 (2000): 27-55.

Ploutz, L. L., P. A. Tesch, R. L. Biro, and G. A. Dudley. “Effect of Resistance Training


on Muscle Use During Exercise.” Journal of Applied Physiology 76 (1994): 1675-81.

Porro, C. A., M. P. Francescato, V. Cettolo, M. E. Diamond, P. Baraldi, C. Zuiani, M.


Bazzocchi, and P. E. di Prampero. “Primary Motor and Sensory Cortex Activation
During Motor Performance and Motor Imagery: A Functional Magnetic Resonance
Imaging Study.” Journal of Neuroscience 16 (1996): 7688-98.

Prablanc, C., M. Desmurget, and H. Gréa. “Neural Control of On-Line Guidance of


Hand Reaching Movements.” Progress in Brain Research 142 (2003): 155-70.
186

REFERENCES - Continued

Prablanc, C. and O. Martin. “Automatic Control During Hand Reaching at Undetected


Two-Dimensional Target Displacements.” Journal of Neurophysiology 67 (1992):
455-69.

Preilowski, B. F. B. “Possible Contribution of the Anterior Forebrain Sommissures to


Bimanual Motor Coordination.” Neuropsychologia 10 (1972): 267-77.

________. “Bilateral Motor Interaction: Perceptual-Motor Performance of Partial and


Complete ‘Split-Brain’ Patients.” In Cerebral Localization, ed. K. S. Zulich, O.
Creutzfeldt, and G. C. Galbraith, 115-32. Berlin: Springer, 1975.

________. “Phases of Motor Skills Acquisition: A Neuropsychological Approach.”


Human Movement Studies 3 (1977): 169-81.

Pressing, J., J. Summers, and J. Magill. “Cognitive Multiplicity in Polyrhythmic Pattern


Performance.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Performance 22(5) (1996): 1127-48.

Priest, A. G. and R. O. Lindsay. “New Light on Novice-Expert Differences in Physics


Problem Solving.” British Journal of Psychology 83 (1992): 389-405.

Prinz, W. “Perception and Action Planning.” European Journal of Cognitive


Psychology 9 (1997): 129-54.

Proctor, R. W. and A. Dutta. Skill Acquisition and Human Performance. Thousand


Oaks, CA: Sage, 1995.

Proteau, L., and J. Cournoyer. “Vision of the Stylus in a Manual Aiming Task: The
Effects of Practice.” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 42A (1990):
811-28.

Proteau, L., R. G. Marteniuk, and L. Lévesque. “A Sensorimotor Basis for Motor


Learning: Evidence Indicating Specificity of Practice.” The Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology Section A 44(3) (1992): 557-75.

Proteau, L., R. G. Marteniuk, Y. Girouard, and C. Dugas. “On the Type of Information
Used to Control and Learn an Aiming Movement after Moderate and Extensive
Practice.” Human Movement Science 6 (1987): 181-9.

Puretz, S. L. “Bilateral Transfer: The Effects of Practice on the Transfer of Complex


Dance Movement Patterns.” Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 54 (1983):
48-54.
187

REFERENCES - Continued

Purswell, Joan M. “Chopin as Teacher.” Clavier 18(7) (1979): 16-19.

Puttemans, Veerle, Nicole Wenderoth, and Stephan P. Swinnen. “Changes in Brain


Activation During the Acquisition of a Multifrequency Bimanual Coordination Task:
From the Cognitive Stage to Advanced Levels of Automaticity.” The Journal of
Neuroscience 25(17) (2005): 4270-78.

Ragert P., A. Schmidt, E. Altenmüller, H. R. Dinse. “Superior Tactile Performance and


Learning in Professional Pianists: Evidence for Meta-Plasticity in Musicians.”
European Journal of Neuroscience 19 (2004): 473-8.

Raibert, M. H. “Motor Control and Learning by the State-Space Model.” Ph.D. diss.,
Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 1977.

Raichle, M. E., A. M. MacLeod, A. Z. Snyder, W. J. Powers, D. A. Gusnard, and G. L.


Shulman. “A Default Mode of Brain Function.” Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 98 (2001): 676-82.

Ram, N., S. M. Riggs, S. Skaling, D. M. Landers, and P. McCullagh. “A Comparison of


Modelling and Imagery in the Acquisition and Retention of Motor Skills.” Journal of
Sports Sciences 25(5) (2007): 587-97.

Ranganathan, V. K., V. Siemionow, J. Liu, V. Sahgal, and G. H. Yue. “From Mental


Power to Muscle Power—Gaining Strength by Using the Mind.” Neuropsychologia
42 (2004): 944-56.

Redgrave, P., T. J. Prescott, and K. Gurney. “The Basal Ganglia: A Vertebrate Solution
to the Selection Problem?” Neuroscience 89 (1999): 1009-23.

Reilly, K. T., M. A. Nordstrom, and M. H. Schieber. “Short-Term Synchronization


Between Motor Units in Different Functional Subdivisions of the Human Flexor
Digitorum Profundus Muscle.” Journal of Neurophysiology 92 (2004): 734-42.

Repp, Bruno. “Effects of Auditory Feedback Deprivation on Expressive Piano


Performance.” Music Perception 16(4) (1999): 409-38.

Richardson, A. “Mental Practice: A Review and Discussion, Part I.” Research


Quarterly 38 (1967a): 95-107.

Ridding, M. C., B. Brouwer, and M. A. Nordstrom. “Reduced Interhemispheric


Inhibition in Musicians.” Experimental Brain Research 133(2) (2000): 249-53.
188

REFERENCES - Continued

Rioult-Pedotti, M. S., D. Friedman, G. Hess, and J. P. Donoghue. “Strengthening of


Horizontal Cortical Connections Following Skill Learning.” Nature Neuroscience 1
(1998): 230-4.

Rizzolatti, G. and L. Craighero. “The Mirror-Neuron System.” Annual Review of


Neuroscience 27 (2004): 169-92.

Rizzolatti, G., L. Fadiga, V. Gallese, and L. Fogassi. “Premotor Cortex and the
Recognition of Motor Actions.” Cognitive Brain Research 3 (1996a): 131-41.

Rizzolatti, G., L. Fogassi, and V. Gallese. “Neurophysiological Mechanisms Underlying


the Understanding and Imitation of Action.” Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 2(9)
(2001): 661-70.

Rizzolatti, G. and M. A. Arbib. “Language Within Our Grasp.” Trends in Neuroscience


21 (1998): 188-94.

Robb, Margaret D. “Feedback and Skill Learning.” In The Dynamics of Motor-Skill


Acquisition. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1972.

Rosenbaum, D. A. “Human Movement Initiation: Specification of Arm, Direction, and


Extent.” Journal of Experimental Psychology. General 109 (1980): 444-74.

Rosenthal, R. K. “The Relative Effects of Guided Model, Model Only, Guided Only, and
Practice Only on the Accuracy of Advanced Instrumentalists’ Musical Performance
Accuracy.” Journal of Research in Music Education 32 (1984): 265-73.

Rosenthal, R. K., M. Wilson, M. Evans, and L. Greenwalt. “Effects of Different Practice


Conditions on Advanced Instrumentalists’ Performance Accuracy.” Journal of
Research in Music Education 36 (1988): 250-7.

Ross, S. L. “The Effectiveness of Mental Practice in Improving the Performance of


College Trombonists.” Journal of Research in Music Education 33 (1985): 221-30.

Rossetti, Y., M. Desmerget, and C. Problanc. “Vectorial Coding of Movement: Vision,


Proprioception, or Both?” Journal of Neurophysiology 74 (1995): 457-63.

Rube, N. and N. H. Secher. “Effect of Training on Central Factors in Fatigue Following


Two- and One-Leg Static Exercise in Man.” Acta Physiologica Scandinavica 141
(1990): 87-95.
189

REFERENCES - Continued

Rudroff, T., B. Poston, I. S. Shin, J. Bojsen-Møller, and R. M. Enoka. “Net Excitation of


the Motor Unit Pool Varied with Load Type During Fatiguing Contractions.” Muscle
& Nerve 29 (2005): 78-87.

Rutherford, O. M. and D. A. Jones. “The Role of Learning and Coordination in Strength


Training.” European Journal of Applied Physiology 55 (1986): 100-5.

Ryan, E. D. and J. Simons. “Efficacy of Mental Imagery in Enhancing Mental Rehearsal


Motor Skills.” Journal of Sport Psychology 4 (1982): 41-51.

Sackett, R. S. “The Influence of Symbolic Rehearsal Upon the Retention of a Maze


Habit.” Journal of General Psychology 10 (1934): 376-95.

________. “The Relationship Between Amount of Symbolic Rehearsal and Retention of


a Maze Habit.” Journal of General Psychology 13 (1935): 113-28.

Sainburg, R. L. and D. Kalakanis. “Differences in Control of Limb Dynamics During


Dominant and Nondominant Arm Reaching.” Journal of Neurophysiology 83 (2000):
2661-75.

Sainburg, R. L., J. E. Lateiner, M. L. Latash, and L. B. Bagesteiro. “Effects of Altering


Initial Position on Movement Direction and Extent.” Journal of Neurophysiology 89
(2003): 401-15.

Sakai, H., O. Hikosaka, R. Takino, S. Miyauchi, M. Nielsen, and T. Tamada. “What and
When: Parallel and Convergent Processing in Motor Control.” Journal of
Neuroscience 20 (2000): 2691-2700.

Sakai, K., O. Hikosaka, S. Miyauchi, Y. Sasaki, N. Fujimaki, and B. Pütz.


“Presupplementary Motor Area Activation During Sequence Learning Reflects Visuo-
Motor Association.” Journal of Neuroscience 19(10) (1999): RC1.

Sakata, H. “Coding 3-D Features of Objects Used in Manipulation by Parietal Neurons.”


In Vision and Movement: Mechanisms in the Cerebral Cortex, ed. R. Caminiti, K. P.
Hoffmann, F. Lacquaniti, and J. Altman, 55-63. Strasbourg: Human Frontier Science
Program, 1996.

Salmoni, Alan W., Richard A. Schmidt, and Charles B. Walter. “Knowledge of Results
and Motor Learning: A Review and Critical Reappraisal.” Psychological Bulletin
95(3) (1984): 355-86.
190

REFERENCES - Continued

Samson, S. and R. J. Zatorre. “Melodic and Harmonic Discrimination Following


Unilateral Cerebral Excision.” Brain and Cognition 7 (1988): 348-60.

Satoh, M., K. Takeda, K. Nagata, J. Hatazawa, and S. Kuzuhara. “Activated Brain


Regions in Musicians During an Ensemble: A PET Study.” Cognitive Brain
Research 12 (2001): 101-8.

Schantz, P. G., T. Moritani, E. Karlson, E. Johansson, and A. Lundh. “Maximal


Voluntary Force of Bilateral and Unilateral Leg Extension.” Acta Physiologica
Scandinavica 136 (1989): 185-92.

Schieber, Marc H. “Individuated Finger Movements of Rhesus Monkeys: A Means of


Quantifying the Independence of the Digits,” Journal of Neurophysiology 65 (1991):
1381-91.

________. “Constraints on Somatotopic Organization in the Primary Motor Cortex.”


Journal of Neurophysiology 86 (2001): 2125-43.

________. “Training and Synchrony in the Motor System.” The Journal of


Neuroscience 22(13) (2002): 5277-81.

Schieber, Marc H. and Marco Santello. “Hand Function: Peripheral and Central
Constraints on Performance.” Journal of Applied Physiology 96 (2004): 2293-2300.

Schlaug, G. “The Brain of Musicians. A Model for Functional and Structural


Adaptation. Review.” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 930 (2001):
281-99.

Schlaug, G., L. Jäncke, Y. Huang, and H. Steinmetz. “In Vivo Evidence of Structural
Brain Asymmetry in Musicians.” Science 267 (1995): 699-701.

Schlaug, G., L. Jäncke, Y. Huang, J. F. Staiger, and H. Steinmetz. “Increased Corpus


Callosum Size in Musicians.” Neuropsychologia 33 (1995): 1047-55.

Schmahmann, J. D., J. Doyon, D. McDonald, C. Holmes, K. Lavoie, A. S. Hurwitz, N.


Kabani, A. Toga, A. Evans, and M. Petrides. “Three-Dimensional MRI Atlas of the
Human Cerebellum in Proportional Stereotaxic Space.” NeuroImage 10 (1999): 233-
60.

Schmidt, R. A. “A Schema Theory of Discrete Motor Skill Learning.” Psychological


Review 82 (1975): 225-60.
191

REFERENCES - Continued

________. “Control Processes in Motor Skills.” In Exercise and Sport Sciences


Reviews, ed. J. Keogh and R. S. Hutton, 229-61. Santa Barbara, CA: Journal
Publishing Affiliates, 1976a.

________. “The Schema as a Solution to Some Persistent Problems in Motor Learning


Theory.” In Motor Control: Issues and Trends, ed. G. E. Stelmach, 41-65. New
York: Academic Press, 1976b.

________. “Past and Future Issues in Motor Programming.” Research Quarterly for
Exercise and Sport 51 (1980): 122-40.

________. “More on Motor Programs.” In Human Motor Behavior, ed. J. A. S. Kelso,


189-217. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1982a.

________. Motor Control and Learning. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 1982b.

________. “The Schema Concept.” In Human Motor Behavior, ed. J. A. S. Kelso, 219-
35. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1982c.

________. Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis, 2nd ed. Champaign,
IL: Human Kinetics, 1988.

Schmidt, R. A., H. N. Zelaznik, B. Hawkins, J. S. Frank and J. T. Quinn Jr. “Motor


Output Variability: A Theory for the Accuracy of Rapid Motor Acts.” Psychological
Review 86 (1979): 415-51.

Schmidt, R. A. and T. D. Lee. Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis, 3rd
ed. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 1999.

Schneider, P. M. Scherg, H. G. Dosch, H. J. Specht, A. Gutschalk, and A. Rupp.


“Morphology of Heschl’s Gyrus Reflects Enhanced Activation in the Auditory Cortex
of Musicians.” Nature Neuroscience 5 (2002): 688-94.

Schneider, R. J., D. P. Friedman, and M. Mishkin. “A Modality-Specific Somato-


Sensory Area Within the Insula of the Rhesus Monkey.” Brain Research 621 (1993):
116-20.

Schneider, W. and Shiffrin, R. M. “Controlled and Automatic Human Information


Processing: I. Detection, Search, and Attention.” Psychological Review 84 (1977):
1-66.
192

REFERENCES - Continued

Schön, D., C. Magne, and M. Besson. “The Music of Speech: Music Training Facilitates
Pitch Processing in Both Music and Language.” Psychophysiology 41(3) (2004):
341-9.

Schön, D., C. Semenza, and G. Denes. “Naming of Musical Notes: A Selective Deficit
in One Musical Clef.” Cortex 37 (2001): 407-21.

Schön, D., and M. Besson. “Processing Pitch and Duration in Music Reading: A RT-
ERP Study.” Neuropsychologia 40 (2002a): 868-78.

________. “Visually Induced Auditory Expectancy in Music Reading: a


Behavioral and Electrophysiological Study.” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
17(4) (April 2005): 694-705.

Schöner, G. and J. A. S. Kelso. “Dynamic Pattern Generation in Behavioral and Neural


Systems.” Science 239 (1988): 1513-20.

Schöner, G., P. G. Zanone, and J. A. S. Kelso. “Learning as Change of Coordination


Dynamics: Theory and Experiment.” Journal of Motor Behavior 24 (1992): 29-48.

Schubotz, R. I. and D. Y. von Cramon. “Sequences of Abstract Nonbiological Stimuli


Share Ventral Premotor Cortex with Action Observation and Imagery.” The Journal
of Neuroscience 24 (2004): 5467-74.

Schultz, Arnold. The Riddle of the Pianist’s Finger and its Relationship to a Touch-
Scheme. New York: Fischer, 1936.

Schulze, K., E. Lüders, and L. Jäncke. “Intermanual Transfer in a Simple Motor Task.”
Cortex 38 (2002): 805-15.

Schumann, Robert. “Die Pianoforte-Etuden, ihren Zwecken nach geordnet.” Neue


Zeitschrift für Musik 4 (1836): 45-6.

________. Gesammelte Schriften über Musik und Musiker. Volume II. Leipzig:
Philipp Reclam, 1888.

Schurmann, M., T. Raij, N. Fujiki, and R. Hari. “Mind’s Ear in a Musician: Where and
When in the Brain.” NeuroImage 16 (2002): 434-40.

Secher, N. H. “Isometric Rowing Strength of Experienced and Inexperienced Oarsmen.”


Medicine and Science in Sports 7 (1975): 280-3.
193

REFERENCES - Continued

Secher, N. H., N. Rube, and J. Ellers. “Strength of Two- and One- Leg Extension in
Man.” Acta Physiologica Scandinavica 134 (1988): 333-39.

Secher, N. J., S. Rorsgarrd, and O. Secher. “Contralateral Influence on Recruitment of


Curarized Muscle Fibres During Maximal Voluntary Extension of the Legs.” Acta
Physiologica Scandinavica 103 (1978): 456-62.

Segal, R. L., S. L. Wolf, P. A. Catlin, R. L. Gilliand, J. K. Taffs, H. C. Bass, and E. F.


Vickers. “Uncoupling of Human Short and Long Latency Stretch Reflex Responses
with Operant Conditioning.” Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience 17 (2000): 17-
22.

Sekuler, R., A. B. Sekuler, and R. Lau. “Sound Alters Visual Motion Perception.”
Nature 385 (1997): 308.

Semjen, A., J. J. Summers, and D. Cattaert. “Hand Coordination in Bimanual Circle


Drawing.” Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance
21 (1995): 1139-57.

Semmler, J. B. and M. A. Nordstrom. “Motor Unit Discharge and Force Tremor in Skill-
and-Strength-Trained Individuals.” Experimental Brain Research 119(1) (1998): 27-
38.
Sergent, J., E. Zuck, S. Terriah, and B. MacDonald. “Distributed Neural Network
Underlying Musical Sight-Reading and Keyboard Performance.” Science 257 (1992):
106-9.

Serrien, D. J. and S. P. Swinnen. “Coordination Constraints Induced by Effector


Combination Under Isofrequency and Multifrequency Conditions.” Journal of
Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance 23 (1997): 1493-
1510.

________. “Isofrequency and Multifrequency Coordination Patterns as a Function of the


Planes of Motion.” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A50 (1997): 386-
404.

Shadmehr, R. and F. A. Mussa-Ivaldi. “A 1994 Adaptive Representation of Dynamics


During Learning of a Motor Task.” Journal of Neuroscience 14 (1994): 3208-24.

Shadmehr, R. and H. H. Holcomb. “Neural Correlates of Motor Memory Consolidation.”


Science 277 (1997): 821-25.
194

REFERENCES - Continued

Shaffer, L. H. “Performances of Chopin, Bach and Bartok: Studies in Motor


Programming.” Cognitive Psychology 13 (1981): 326-76.

Shanks, D. R. and A. Cameron. “The Effect of Mental Practice on Performance in a


Sequential Reaction Time Task.” Journal of Motor Behavior 32 (2000): 305-13.

Shapiro, D. C. “Bilateral Transfer of a Motor Program.” Paper presented at the


AAHPER Annual Meeting, March, 1977.

Sherwood, D. E. “Distance and Location Assimilation Effects in Rapid Bimanual


Movement.” Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 62 (1991): 302-8.

________. “Hand Preference, Practice Order, and Spatial Assimilations in Rapid


Bimanual Movement.” Journal of Motor Behavior 26 (1994): 123-34.

Siegel, J. A. and W. Siegel. “Absolute Identification of Notes and Intervals by


Musicians.” Perceptual and Psychophysics 21(2) (1977): 143-52.

Simon, H. A. and W. G. Chase. “Skill in Chess.” American Scientist 61 (1973): 394-


403.

Singer, R. N., R. Lidor, and J. H. Cauraugh. “To be Aware or Not Aware? What to
Think About While Learning and Performing a Motor Skill.” The Sport Psychologist
7 (1993): 19-30.

Sloboda, J. A. “Visual Perception of Musical Notation: Registering Pitch Symbols in


Memory.” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 28 (1976): 1-16.

________. “Psychology of Music Reading.” Psychology of Music 6 (1978): 3-20.

________. “Experimental Studies of Musical Reading: A Review.” Music Perception 2


(1984): 222-36.

________. Review of Language, Music, and Mind, by Diana Raffman. Mind &
Language 9 (1994): 377-85.

Sloboda, J. A., J. W. Davidson, M. J. A. Howe, and D. G. Moore. “The Role of Practice


in the Development of Performing Music.” British Journal of Psychology 87 (1996):
287-309.
195

REFERENCES - Continued

Slobounov, S., H. Chiang, J. Johnston, and W. Ray. “Modulated Cortical Control of


Individual Fingers in Experienced Musicians: An EEG Study.” Clinical
Neurophysiology 113 (2002): 2013-24.

Sluming, V., T. Barrick, M. Howard, E. Cezayirli, A. Mayes, and N. Roberts. “Voxel-


Based Morphometry Reveals Increased Gray Matter Density in Broca’s Area in Male
Symphony Orchestra Musicians.” NeuroImage 17 (2002): 1613-22.

Smith, M. D. and C. J. Chamberlin. “Effect of Adding Cognitively Demanding Tasks on


Soccer Skill Performance.” Perceptual and Motor Skills 75 (1992): 955-61.

Sober, Samuel J. and Philip N. Sabes. “Flexible Strategies for Sensory Integration
During Motor Planning.” Nature Neuroscience 8(4) (2005): 490-97.

Sosniak, L. A. “Learning to be a Concert Pianist.” In Developing Talent in Young


People, ed. B.S. Bloom, 19-67. New York: Balantine Books, 1985.

Spijkers, W. and H. Heuer. “Structural Constraints on the Performance of Symmetrical


Bimanual Movements with Different Amplitudes.” Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology A48 (1995): 716-40.

Stålberg, E. and P. R. Fawcett. “Macro EMG in Healthy Subjects of Different Ages.”


45(10) (1982): 870-8.

Starkes, J. L. “Skill in Field Hockey: The Nature of the Cognitive Advantage.” Journal
of Sport Psychology 9 (1987): 146-60.

Starkes, J. L. and J. M. Deakin. “Perception in Sport: A Cognitive Approach to Skilled


Performance.” In Cognitive Sport Psychology, ed. W. F. Straub and J. M. Williams,
115-28. Lansing, NY: Sport Science Associates, 1984.

Starkes, J. L., J. M. Deakin, F. Allard, N. J. Hodges, and A. Hayes. “Deliberate Practice


in Sports: What is it Anyway?” In The Road to Excellence: The Acquisition of
Expert Performance in the Arts, Sciences, Sports and Games, ed. K. A. Ericsson, 80-
106. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 1996.

Staron, R. S., E. S. Malicky, M. J. Leonardi, J. E. Falkel, F. C. Hagerman, and G. A.


Dudley. “Muscle Hypertrophy and Fast Fiber Type Conversions in Heavy Resistance-
Trained Women.” European Journal of Applied Physiology and Occupational
Physiology 60 (1990): 71-9.
196

REFERENCES - Continued

Stromswold, K., D. Caplan, N. Alpert, and S. Rauch. “Localization of Syntactic


Comprehension by Positron Emission Tomography.” Brain and Language 52 (1996):
452-73.

Summers, J. “Motor Programs.” In Human Skills, ed. D.H. Holding, 41-64. New York:
John Wiley & Sons Ltd., 1981.

________. “Learning and Transfer of Multifrequency Patterns.” In Rhythm Perception


and Production, ed. P. Desain and L. Windsor, 69-80. Lisse: Swets and Zeitlinger,
2000.

________. “Practice and Training in Bimanual Coordination Tasks: Strategies and


Constraints.” Brain and Cognition 48 (2002): 166-78.

Summers, J. J., A. Semen, R. A. Carson, and J. Thomas. “Going Around in Circles: The
Dynamics of Bimanual Circle Drawing.” In Motor Control and Sensory Motor
Integration, ed. D. Glencross and J. Piek, 231-53. Amsterdam: North-Holland,
1995).

Summers, J. J., A. Todd, and Y. H. Kim. “The Influence of Perceptual and Motor
Factors on Bimanual Coordination in a Polyrhythmic Tapping Task.” Psychological
Research 55 (1993): 107-15.

Summers, J., D. A. Rosenbaum, B. D. Burns, and S. K. Ford. “Production of


Polyrhythms.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Performance 19(2) (1993b): 416-28.

Summers, J. J. and T. Kennedy. “Strategies in the Production of a 5:3 Polyrhythm.”


Human Movement Science 11 (1992): 101-12.

Sundberg J., and R. T. Verrillo. “Somatosensory Feedback in Musical Performance.”


Music Perception 9(3) (Spring 1992): 275-83.

Susman, L. “Fossil Evidence for Early Hominid Tool Use.” Science 265 (1994): 1570-
3.

________. “Hand Function and Tool Behavior in Early Hominids.” Journal of Human
Evolution 35 (1998): 23-46.

Sutton, R. and A. G. Barto. Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction. Cambridge, MA:


MIT Press, 1998.
197

REFERENCES - Continued

Suzuki, W. A. and D. G. Amaral. “Perirhinal and Parahippocampal Cortices of the


Macaque Monkey: Cortical Afferents.” Journal of Comparative Neurology 350
(1994): 497-533.

Svensson, P., A. Romaniello, L. Arendt-Nielsen, and B. J. Sessle. “Plasticity in


Corticomotor Control of the Human Tongue Musculature Induced by Tongue-Task
Training.” Experimental Brain Research 152 (2003): 42-51.

Swinnen, S. P. “Age-Related Deficits in Motor Learning and Differences in Feedback


Processing During the Production of a Bimanual Coordination Pattern.” Cognitive
Neuropsychology 15 (1998): 439-66.

________. “Intermanual Coordination: From Behavioural Principles to Neural-Network


Interactions.” Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 3 (2002): 350-61.

Swinnen, S. P. and C. B. Walter. “Constraints in Coordinating Limb Movements.” In


Cognition and Action in Skilled Behaviour, ed. A. M. Colley and J. R. Beech, 127-43.
Amsterdam: North-Holland, 1988.

Swinnen, S. P., C. B. Walter, T. D. Lee, and D. J. Serrien. “Acquiring Bimanual Skills:


Contrasting Forms of Information Feedback for Interlimb Decoupling.” Journal of
Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 19 (1993): 1328-44.

Swinnen, S. P., D. E. Young, C. B. Walter, and D. J. Serrien. “Control of Bilateral


Asymmetrical Movements.” Experimental Brain Research 85 (1991): 163-73.

Swinnen, S. P. and K. Jardin. “Egocentric and Allocentric Constraints in the Expression


of Patterns of Interlimb Coordination.” Journal of Cognitive. Neuroscience 9 (1997):
348–77.

Swinnen, S. P., K. Jardin, S. Verschueren, and R. Meulenbroek. “Exploring Interlimb


Constraints During Bimanual Graphic Performance: Effects of Muscle Grouping and
Direction.” Behavioural Brain Research 90 (1998): 79–87.

Swinnen, S. P., N. Dounskaia, C. B. Walter, and D. J. Serrien. “Preferred and Induced


Coordination Modes During the Acquisition of Bimanual Movements with a 2:1
Frequency Ratio.” Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and
Performance 23 (1997): 1087-1110.
198

REFERENCES - Continued

Swinnen, S. P., N. Dounskaia, S. Verschueren, D. J. Serrien, and A. Daelman. “Relative


Phase Destabilization During Interlimb Coordination: The Disruptive Role of
Kinesthetic Afferences Induced by Passive Movement.” Experimental Brain
Research 105 (1995): 439-54.

Tamada, T., S. Miyauchi, H. Imamizu, T. Yoshioka, and M. Kawato. “Cerebro-


Cerebellar Functional Connectivity Revealed by the Laterality Index in Tool-Use
Learning.” NeuroReport 10 (1999): 325-31.

Tanji, J. “New Concepts of the Supplementary Motor Area.” Current Opinion in


Neurobiology 6 (1996): 782-7.

Tanji, J. and K. Shima. “Role for Supplementary Motor Area Cells in Planning Several
Movements Ahead.” Nature 371 (1994): 413-16.

Tanji, J. and M. Kato. “Firing Rate of Individual Motor Units in Voluntary Contraction
of Abductor Digit Minimi Muscle in Man.” Experimental Neurology 40 (1973): 771-
83.

Tanosaki, Masato, Isao Hashimoto, Yoshinobu Iguchi, Tomoaki Kimura, Ryosuke


Takino, Yuji Kurobe, Yasuhiro Haruta, and Yoko Hoshi. “Specific Somatosensory
Processing in Somatosensory Area 3b for Human Thumb: A Neuromagnetic Study.”
Clinical Neurophysiology 112 (2001): 1516-22.

Taylor, H. G. and K. M. Heilman. “Left-Hemisphere Motor Dominance in


Righthanders.” Cortex 16 (1980): 587-603.

Teasdale, N., C. Bard, M. Fleury, J. Paillard, R. Forget, and Y. Lamarre. “Bimanual


Interference in a Deafferented Patient and in Control Subjects.” In Interlimb
Coordination: Neural, Dynamical, and Cognitive Constraints, ed. S. P. Swinnen, H.
Heuer, J. Massion, and P. Casner, 243-58. San Diego: Academic Press, 1994.

Teixeira, L. A. “Timing and Force Components in Bilateral Transfer of Learning.”


Brain and Cognition 44 (2000): 455-69.

Temprado, J., A. Monno, P. Zanone, and J. Kelso. “Attentional Demands Reflect


Learning-Induced Alterations of Bimanual Coordination Dynamics.” European
Journal of Neuroscience 16 (2002): 1390-4.

Temprado, J. J., P. G. Zanone, A. Monno, and M. Laurent. “Attentional Load Associated


with Performing and Stabilizing Preferred Bimanual Patterns.” Journal of
Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance 25 (1999): 1579–94.
199

REFERENCES - Continued

Tenenbaum, G. and M. Bar-Eli. “Decision-Making in Sport: A Cognitive Perspective.”


In Handbook of Research on Sports Psychology, eds. R. N. Singer, M. Murphey, and
L. K. Tennant, 171-92. New York: Macmillan, 1993.

Thach, W. T. “A Role for the Cerebellum in Learning Movement Coordination.”


Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 7 (1998): 177-88.

Theiler, A. M. and L. G. Lippman. “Effects of Mental Practice and Modeling on Guitar


and Vocal Performance.” Journal of General Psychology 122 (1995): 329-43.

Thelen, E., G. Bradshaw, and J. A. Ward. “Spontaneous Kicking in Month-Old Infants:


Manifestation of a Human Central Locomotor Program.” Behavioral and Neural
Biology 32 (1981): 45-53.

Théoret, H. and A. Pascual-Leone. “Language Acquisition: Do as You Hear.” Current


Biology 12 (2002): R736-7.

Toni, I., M. Krams, R. Turner, and R. E. Passingham. “The Time Course of Changes
During Motor Sequence Learning: A Whole-Brain fMRI Study.” NeuroImage 8
(1998): 50-61.

Treffner, P. J. and M. Peter. “Intentional and Attentional Dynamics of Speech-Hand


Coordination.” Human Movement Science 21 (2002): 641-97.

Treffner, P. J. and M. T. Turvey. “Resonance Constraints on Rhythmic Movements.”


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 19(6)
(1993): 1221-37.

________. “Handedness and the Asymmetric Dynamics of Bimanual Rhythmic


Coordination.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Performance 23(1) (1997): 1-15.

Tresch, M. C., P. Saltiel, and E. Bizzi. “The Construction of Movement by the Spinal
Cord.” Nature Neuroscience 2 (1999): 162-7.

Tsuji, K. and Y. Ide. “Development of Bilateral Transfer Skills in the Mirror Tracing.”
Japanese Psychological Research 16 (1974): 171-8.

Tuller, B. and J. A. S. Kelso. “Environmentally-Specified Patterns of Movement


Coordination in Normal and Split-Brain Subjects.” Experimental Brain Research 75
(1989): 306-16.
200

REFERENCES - Continued

Tyc, F., A. Boyadjian, and H. Devanne. “Motor Cortex Plasticity Induced by Extensive
Training Revealed by Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation in Human.” The European
Journal of Neuroscience 21 (2005): 259-66.

Ullen, F., H. Borssberg, and H. H. Ehrsson. “Neural Networks for the Coordination of
the Hands in Time.” Journal of Neurophysiology 89 (2003): 1126-35.

Ullyot, Marianne. “Chopin and Liszt: A Legacy of Teaching.” Journal of the American
Liszt Society 10 (1981): 39-42.

Umiltà, M. A., E. Kohler, V. Gallese, L. Fogassi, L. Fadiga, C. Keysers, and G.


Rizzolatti. “I Know What You Are Doing. A Neurophysiological Study.” Neuron
31(1) (2001): 155-65.

Ung, R. V., M. A. Imbeault, C. Ethier, L. Brizzi, and C. Capaday. “On the Potential Role
of the Corticospinal Tract in the Control and Progressive Adaptation of the Soleus H-
Reflex During Backward Walking.” Journal of Neurophysiology 94 (2005): 1133-42.

Ungerleider, L. G., J. Doyon, and A. Karni. “Imaging Brain Plasticity During Motor
Skill Learning.” Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 78(3) (2002): 553-64.

Van Beers, R. J., A. C. Sittig, and J. J. Denier van der Gon. “How Humans Combine
Simultaneous Proprioceptive and Visual Position Information.” Experimental Brain
Research 111 (1996): 253-61.

________. “Integration of Proprioceptive and Visual Position-Information: An


Experimentally Supported Model.” Journal of Neurophysiology 81 (1999): 1355-64.

Van Cutsem, M., J. Duchateau, and K. Hainuat. “Changes in Single Motor Unit
Behavior Contribute to the Increase in Contraction Speed After Dynamic Training in
Humans.” Journal of Physiology 513 (1998): 295-305.

Vandell, R. A., R. A. Davis, and N. A. Clugston. “Function of Mental Practice in the


Acquisition of Motor Skills.” Journal of General Psychology 29 (1943): 243-50.

Vandervoort, A. A., D. G. Sale, and J. Moroz. “Comparison of Motor Unit Activation


During Unilateral and Bilateral Leg Extension.” Journal of Applied Physiology 56
(1984): 46-51.

VanLehn, K. “Problem Solving and Cognitive Skill Acquisition.” In Foundations of


Cognitive Science, ed. M. Posner, 527-80. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1989.
201

REFERENCES - Continued

Varela, F., J. P. Lachaux, E. Rodriguez, and J. Martinerie. “The Brainweb: Phase


Synchronization and Large-Scale Integration.” Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 2
(2001): 229-39.

Vercher, Jean-Louis, Frédéric Sarès, Jean Blouin, Christophe Bourdin, and Gabriel
Gathier. “Role of Sensory Information in Updating Internal Models of the Effector
During Arm Tracking.” Progress in Brain Research (142) (2003): 203-22.

Verschueren, S. M. P., S. P. Swinnen, P. J. Cordo, and N. V. Dounskaia. “Proprioceptive


Control of Multijoint Movement: Bimanual Circle Drawing.” Experimental Brain
Research 127 (1999): 182-92.

Vitzthum, V. J. “A Number No Greater Than the Sum of Its Parts: The Use and Abuse
of Heritability.” Human Biology 75 (2003): 539-88.

Vogt, B. A. and M. Gabriel, ed. Neurobiology of Cingulate Cortex and Limbic


Thalamus. Boston: Birkhauser, 1993.

Vogt, B. A., D. M. Finch, and C. R. Olson. “Functional Heterogeneity in Cingulate


Cortex: The Anterior Executive and Posterior Evaluative Regions.” Cerebral Cortex
2 (1992): 435-43.

Vokaer, M., J. C. Bier, S. Elincx, T. Claes, P. Paquier, S. Goldman, E. J. Bartholomé, and


M. Pandolfo. “The Cerebellum May be Directly Involved in Cognitive Functions.”
Neurology 58 (2002): 967-70.

von Holst, E. “Relations Between the Central Nervous System and the Peripheral
Organs.” British Journal of Animal Behaviour 2 (1954): 89-94.

Vorberg, D. and R. Hambuch. “On the Temporal Control of Rhythmic Performance.” In


Attention and Performance VII, ed. J. Requin, 535-55. Hillsdale, NJ; Erlbaum, 1978.

________. “Timing of Two-Handed Rhythmic Performance.” In Timing and Time


Perception, ed. L. Allen, 390-406. New York: New York Academy of Sciences,
1984.

Wallingford, R. “Long Distance Running.” In The Scientific Aspects of Sports Training,


ed. A.W. Tayler and F. Landry, 118-30. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas, 1975.
202

REFERENCES - Continued

Walter, C. B. and S. P. Swinnen. “The Formation and Dissolution of “Bad Habits”


During the Acquisition of Coordination Skills.” In Interlimb Coordination: Neural,
Dynamical, and Cognitive Constraints, ed. S. P. Swinnen, H. Heuer, J. Massion, and
P. Casaer, 491-513. San Diego: Academic Press, 1994.

Walter, C. B., S. P. Swinnen, N. Dounskaia, and H. Van Langendonk. “Systematic Error


in the Hierarchical Organization of Physical Action.” Cognitive Science 25 (2001):
393-422.

Wang, Y., S. Pillai, J. R. Wolpaw, and X. Y. Chen. “Motor Learning Changes


GABAergic Terminals on Spinal Motoneurons in Normal Rats.” European Journal of
Neuroscience 23 (2006): 141-50.

Wannier, T., C. Bastiaanse, G. Colombo, and V. Dietz. “Arm to Leg Coordination in


Humans During Walking, Creeping and Swimming.” Experimental Brain Research
14 (2001): 375-9.

Warren, D. H. “Response Factors in Intermodality Localization Under Conflict


Conditions.” Perception and Psychophysics 27 (1980): 28-32.

Warren, D. H. and W. T. Cleaves. “Visual-Proprioceptive Interaction Under Large


Amounts of Conflict.” Journal of Experimental Psychology 90 (1971): 206-14.

Warrier, C. M. and R. J. Zatorre. “Right Temporal Cortex is Critical for Utilization of


Melodic Contextual Cues in a Pitch Constancy Task.” Brain 127 (2004): 1616-25.

Watkins, K. and T. Paus. “Modulation of Motor Excitability During Speech Perception:


The Role of Broca’s Area.” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 16(6) (2004): 978-87.

Watkins, K. E., A. P. Strafella, and T. Paus. “Seeing and Hearing Speech Excites the
Motor System Involved in Speech Production.” Neuropsychologia 41 (2003): 989-
94.

Weig, E. “Bilateral Transfer in the Motor Learning of Young Children and Adults.”
Child Development 3 (1932): 247-67.

Welford, A.T. Fundamentals of Skill. London: Methuen, 1968.

Whiteside, Abby. Indispensables of Piano Playing. New York: Coleman-Ross Co.,


1955.
203

REFERENCES - Continued

________. Mastering the Chopin Etudes and Other Essays. Edited by Joseph Prostakoff
and Sophia Rosoff. New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons, 1969.

Williamson, D. L., P. M. Gallagher, C. C. Carroll, U. Raue, and S. W. Trappe.


“Reduction in Hybrid Single Muscle Fiber Proportions with Resistance Training in
Humans.” Journal of Applied Physiology 91 (2001): 1955-61.

Wilson, D. M. “The Central Nervous Control of Flight in a Locust.” Journal of


Experimental Biology 38 (1961): 471-90.

Wilson, E. O. Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University


Press, 1975.

Wing, A. M. “Voluntary Timing and Brain Function: An Information Processing


Approach.” Brain Cognition 48 (2002): 7-30.

Wing, A. M. and A. B. Kristofferson. “Response Delays and the Timing of Discrete


Motor Responses.” Perception & Psychophysics 14(1) (1973a): 5-12.

________. “The Timing of Inter-Response Intervals.” Perception & Psychophysics


13(3) (1973b): 455-60.

Wohldmann, E. L., A. F. Healy, and L. E. Bourne Jr. “Pushing the Limits of


Imagination: Mental Practice for Learning Sequences.” Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 33(1) (2007): 254-61.

Wohlschlager, A. and H. Bekkering. “Is Human Imitation Based on a Mirror-Neurone


System? Some Behavioural Evidence.” Experimental Brain Research 143 (2002):
335-41.

Wolpert, D. M. “Computational Approaches to Motor Control.” Trends in Cognitive


Sciences 1(6) (1997): 209-16.

Wolpert, D. M. and M. Kawato. “Multiple Paired Forward and Inverse Models for Motor
Control.” Neural Networks 11 (1998): 1317-29.

Wolpert, D. M., R. C. Miall, and M. Kawato. “Internal Models in the Cerebellum.”


Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2(9) (1998): 338-47.

Wolpert, D. M. and Z. Ghahramani. “Computational Principles of Movement


Neuroscience.” Nature Neuroscience 3 (2000): 1212-17.
204

REFERENCES - Continued

Wolpert, D. M., Z. Ghahramani, and J. R. Flanagan. “Perspectives and Problems in


Motor Learning.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15(11) (2001): 487-94.

Wolpert, D. M., Z. Ghahramani, and M. I. Jordan. “An Internal Model for Sensorimotor
Integration.” Science 269 (1995): 1880-2.

Woodworth, R. S. and H. Schlosberg, eds. Experimental Psychology. New York: Holt,


Rinehart & Winston, 1954.

Wright, B. Z. and M. B. Fitzgerald. “The Time Course of Attention in a Simple Auditory


Detection Task.” Perception & Psychophysics 66 (2004): 508-16.

Wu, T., K. Kansaku, and M. Hallett. “How Self-Initiated Memorized Movements


Become Automatic: A Functional MRI Study.” Journal of Neurophysiology 91(4)
(2004): 1690-8.

Wulf, G., M. Hob, and W. Prinz. “Instructions for Motor Learning: Differential Effects
of Internal Versus External Focus of Attention.” Journal of Motor Behavior 30
(1998): 169-79.

Wulf, G, N. H. McNevin, T. Fuchs, F. Ritter, and T. Toole. “Attentional Focus in


Complex Skill Learning.” Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 71 (2000):
229-39.

Yágüez, L., A. G. Canavan, H. W. Lange, and V. Hömberg. “A Mental Route to Motor


Learning: Improving Trajectorial Kinematics Through Imagery Training.”
Behavioural Brain Research 90(1) (1998): 95-106.

Yamanishi, J., M. Kawato, R. Suzuki. “Two Coupled Oscillators as a Model for the
Coordinated Finger Tapping by Both Hands.” Biological Cybernetics 37 (1980):
219–25.

Yang, N., D. G. MacArthur, J. P. Gulbin, A. G. Han, A. H. Beggs, S. Easteal, and K.


Northe. “ACTN3 Genotype is Associated with Human Elite Athletic Performance.”
American Journal of Human Genetics 73 (2003): 627-31.

Yao, W., R. Fuglevand, and R. Enoka. “Motor-Unit Synchronization Increases EMG


Amplitude and Decreases Force Steadiness of Simulated Contractions.” The Journal
of Neurophysiology 83(1) (2000): 441-52.
205

REFERENCES - Continued

Yoo, S. S., C. U. Lee, and B. G. Choi. “Human Brain Mapping of Auditory Imagery:
Event-Related Functional MRI Study.” NeuroReport 12(14) (2001): 3045-9.

Young, D. E. and R. A. Schmidt. “Transfer of Movement Control in Motor Skill


Learning.” In Transfer of Learning: Contemporary Research and Applications, ed. S.
M. Cornier and J. D. Hagman, 47-79. San Diego, Academic Press, 1987.

Yue, G. and K. J. Cole. “Strength Increases From the Motor Program: Comparison of
Training with Maximum Voluntary and Imagined Muscle Contractions.” Journal of
Neurophysiology 67 (1992): 1114-23.

Zanone, P. and J. Kelso. “The Evolution of Behavioral Attractors with Learning—


Nonequilibrium Phase-Transitions.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance 18 (1992): 403-21.

________. “Coordination Dynamics of Learning and Transfer: Collective and


Component Levels.” Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and
Performance 23 (1997): 1454-80.

Zatorre, R. J. “Discrimination and Recognition of Tonal Melodies After Unilateral


Cerebral Excisions.” Neuropsychologia 23 (1985): 31-41.

________. “Neural Specialization for Tonal Processing.” In The Cognitive


Neuroscience of Music, ed. I. Peretz and R. Zatorre, 231-46. Oxford: Oxford
University Press, 2003.

________. “When the Brain Plays Music: Auditory-Motor Interactions in Music


Perception and Production.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8(7) (2007): 547-58.

Zatorre, R. J., A. C. Evans, and E. Meyer.” Neural Mechanisms Underlying Melodic


Perception and Memory for Pitch.” Journal of Neuroscience 14 (1994): 1908-19.

Zatorre, R. J. and A. R. Halpern. “Mental Concerts: Musical Imagery and Auditory


Cortex.” Neuron 47 (2005): 9-12.

Zatorre, R. J., A. R. Halpern, D. W. Perry, E. Meyer, and A. C. Evans. “Hearing in the


Mind’s Ear: A PET Investigation of Musical Imagery and Perception.” Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience 8 (1996): 29-46.

Zatorre, R. J., D. W. Perry, C. A. Beckett, C. F. Westbury, and A. C. Evans. “Functional


Anatomy of Musical Processing in Listeners with Absolute Pitch and Relative Pitch.”
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A. 95 (1998): 3172-77.
206

REFERENCES - Continued

Zatorre, R. J. and P. Belin. “Spectral and Temporal Processing in Human Auditory


Cortex.” Cerebral Cortex 11 (2001): 946-53.

Ziemann, U. W. Muellbacher, M. Hallett, and L. G. Cohen. “Modulation of Practice-


Dependent Plasticity in Human Motor Cortex.” Brain 124(Pt6) (2001): 1171-81.

Zilles, K., G. Schlaug, M. Matelli, G. Luppino, A. Schleider, and M. Qü, A. Dabringhaus,


R. Seitz, and P. E. Roland. “Mapping of Human and Macaque Sensorimotor Areas
by Integrating Architectonic, Transmitter Receptor, MRI and PET Data.” Journal of
Anatomy 187 (1995): 515-37.

Zupan, L. H., D. M. Merfeld, and C. Darlot. “Using Sensory Weighting to Model the
Influence of Canal, Otolith and Visual Cues on Spatial Orientation and Eye
Movements.” Biological Cybernetics 86 (2002): 2009-30.

You might also like