You are on page 1of 12

AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR

Volume 32, pages 528–539 (2006)

Social Cognition and Moral Cognition in Bullying:


What’s Wrong?
Gianluca Gini

Department of Developmental and Socialisation Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy

: : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : :
Two different models have been proposed that describe the bully alternatively as a child lacking in social skills [Crick and Dodge,
1994], or as a cold manipulative individual, who leads gangs to achieve personal goals [Sutton et al., 1999a]. The present study
examined the performance of 204 8–11-year-olds in a set of stories that assessed understanding of cognitions and emotions, in
relation to their Participant Role in bullying. Moreover, children’s understanding of moral emotions and proneness to moral
disengagement was assessed. Victims showed some difficulties in the social cognition task, whereas bullies did not. Aggressive
children, instead, were found to be more ready to show moral disengagement mechanisms, whereas defenders showed higher levels
of moral sensibility. Results are discussed in relation to the two models, and the need for further research into empathy and moral
cognition of children involved in bullying episodes is highlighted. Aggr. Behav. 32:528–539, 2006. r 2006 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
: : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : :

Keywords: school bullying; social cognition; moral cognition; theory of mind; participant roles

INTRODUCTION than their non-aggressive peers (hostile attributional


bias) [Dodge and Frame, 1982; Dodge and Som-
Over the last 20 years, the literature about the
berg, 1987]. Another difference between aggressive
development of aggressive behavior showed an
and non-aggressive children refers to the selection of
increasing interest in the study of the relations
the behavioral response in a social situation: the
between cognition, social information processing
former, in fact, seem to have a more limited range of
and social interaction in children and adolescents
non-aggressive answers than the latter and, for this
[e.g., Crick and Dodge, 1994; Harvey et al., 2001]. In
reason, they are more inclined to choose and
particular, several authors studied how the social
perform aggressive behaviors, especially in the case
information processing strategies used by children
of interpersonal conflicts [Dodge, 1980; Dodge and
and the way in which they interpret situational cues
Newman, 1981; Dodge et al., 1990]. According to
and use their previous experiences can influence
the same approach, victims, who are shy and
their aggressive conduct [Dodge and Price, 1994;
withdrawn and lack social skills of assertiveness,
Huesmann and Guerra, 1997; Randall, 1997]. One
show high levels of anxiety and loneliness, and have
of the most important results of this field of research
less experience in social interaction and play fighting
has been the possibility to identify the presence of
(which may enhance social skills), as well as control
specific deficits and systematic biases in some
and interpretation of emotional expression [Hodges
components of the social cognition of aggressive
et al., 1997; Smith et al., 1993].
children. These results led Dodge and colleagues to
Some authors tried to apply this model to a
formulate the ‘‘Social Skills Deficit’’ model [Crick
particular kind of aggressive phenomenon: school
and Dodge, 1994; Dodge, 1980]. According to the
first formulation of this model, in which reactive and
proactive categories of aggression were not clearly Correspondence to: G. Gini, Department of Developmental and
distinguished, aggressive children show some diffi- Socialisation Psychology, University of Padova, via Venezia 8, 35131
culties in one or more phases of their social Padova, Italy. E-mail: gianluca.gini@unipd.it
information processing. For example, these authors Received 14 December 2004; Accepted 14 April 2005
found that aggressive children tend to interpret Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.
ambiguous situations in an aggressive way more com). DOI: 10.1002/ab.20153.

r 2006 Wiley-Liss, Inc.


Social Cognition in Bullying 529

bullying [Boulton and Smith, 1994; Slee, 1993; Smith [1999b] did not explicitly consider a particular kind
et al., 1993]. However, despite the popular view of of emotion found to be important to account for
the bully as an ‘‘oaf’’ boy, that is, an intellectually aggressive and antisocial behaviors: moral emotions
simple or backward boy [Besag, 1989], very few (in particular, guilt and shame). If bullies are cold,
studies confirmed that Dodge’s model can be manipulative experts in social contexts, their beha-
applied to bullies in exactly the same way as to vior may be related to emotions surrounding moral
aggressive children [e.g., Hazler, 1996; Randall, transgression. These emotions are important be-
1997]. On the contrary, some other authors stressed cause, as Hoffman [1976, 2000] pointed out, they are
the role of adaptive motivation to explain bullying closely connected with empathy and they can be
behavior, arguing that bullying may be seen as an considered mediators of conscience. Other authors
inappropriate way to reach a socially effective goal, suggested that moral development plays a major role
such as leadership within a group. As a consequence, in behavioral regulation [Arsenio, 1988; Nucci,
these authors stated that the bully should not 2001; Smetana, 1995; Turiel, 1998] and that guilt is
necessarily be seen as an individual with specific an interpersonal phenomenon aimed at avoiding the
difficulties. On the contrary, he or she may some- disruption of relationships and bonds [Baumeister
times be a skilled individual who takes advantage et al., 1994; Tagney, 1992]. Moreover, individuals
of his/her high social cognitive competence to reach who are more prone to guilt are less aggressive and
personal benefits, such as interpersonal domi- less likely to act out behaviors [Harpur et al., 1988;
nance, through a ‘‘Machiavellian’’ conduct [Sutton Quiles and Bybee, 1997]. In contrast, if children had
et al., 1999a]. lower feelings of guilt, this could ‘‘enable’’ them to
Some data presented by Sutton et al. [1999b] act aggressively without feeling guilty. However, the
empirically support this view; in fact, they found reverse causal link could be true, in that children
that 7–10-year-old bullies performed better than who repeatedly commit acts of aggression may
other classmates in a social cognition task that become inured to this type of behavior, exhibiting
required the understanding of mental states and dulled emotional responses.
emotions of the main character of a story (theory of
mind task). The relevance of these results, above all,
Moral Disengagement
lies in the fact that, in a bullying situation, ‘‘having a
and Aggressive Behavior
grasp of the mental states of those involved, along
with an ability to manipulate these thoughts and A useful contribution to the analysis of some
beliefs, may be crucial for the bully in developing social cognitive components of aggressive behavior
and maintaining such inter-role relations’’ [Sutton is made by the Social Cognitive Theory of the Moral
et al., 1999b, p. 437]. In previous studies, therefore, Self [Bandura, 1986, 1990, 1991]. According to this
bullies’ social cognitive skills, such as social per- theory, moral reasoning is linked to moral action
spective taking and decoding of emotional informa- through affective self-regulatory mechanisms by
tion, may have been underestimated [Sutton et al., which moral agency is exercised. In their develop-
1999a,b,c]. However, some bullies may possess a ment, children need to develop standards of right
different conception of the costs and benefits of and wrong and to adopt these standards as guides
aggression, and they may lack in empathic under- and deterrents for their conduct. This self-regulatory
standing of the victim’s suffering. Therefore, it process implies that individuals usually act in ways
appears more adequate to regard social skills as a that give them satisfaction and a sense of self-worth,
neutral tool, which may be used for both whereas, on the other side, they tend to avoid
prosocial and antisocial purposes [Björkqvist et al., behaviors that violate their moral standards in order
2000; Hawley, 2003]. From this perspective, the to avoid self-condemnation.
crucial point to understand bullying behavior refers Moreover, Bandura [2002] claimed that the con-
to the kind of values that guide bullies’ conduct, struct of moral disengagement could be considered
rather than their higher or lower ability in social as a mediator between the individual’s moral
information processing [Arsenio and Lemerise, 2001]. principles and his/her real conduct, which may not
Following this line of research, the present paper necessarily be coherent with those principles. Inter-
focuses on cognitive and emotional understanding nal controls, in fact, are not fixed, and several
of social situations in bullying, using a theory of psychological and social processes can contribute
mind task with children who assume different to their selective deactivation. In particular, Ban-
participant roles during a bullying episode. In the dura described eight different mechanisms through
above-mentioned study, however, Sutton et al. which moral control can be disengaged. One set of

Aggr. Behav. DOI 10.1002/ab


530 Gini

disengagement practices operates on the reconstruc- and Spain, conduced by Menesini et al. [2003b], who
tion of the behavior itself. For example, an immoral assessed moral reasoning of bullies, victims and
conduct is made personally and socially acceptable children who assumed the role of the defender, using
by portraying it in the service of moral purposes the Scan Bullying test [Almeida et al., 2001]. These
(moral justification) or self-deplored acts can be authors confirmed the tendency of bullies to show
made to appear righteous by contrasting them with higher levels of moral disengagement, and the
worse conducts (advantageous comparison). More- presence of a profile of egocentric reasoning in
over, in some circumstances, activities can take on these pupils.
different appearances depending on what they are Quite recently, the ‘‘traditional’’ classification
called (euphemistic labeling). The second set of of school children into bullies, victims and controls
disengagement practices operates by obscuring or has been criticized and several authors stressed the
minimizing the agentive role in the harm one causes importance of considering bullying a group
(displacement of responsibility and diffusion of process [Sutton and Smith, 1999]. The empirical
responsibility). Other ways of weakening moral confirmation of the importance of peer ecology
control operate by minimizing, disregarding or applied to bullying phenomenon derives from
distorting the effects of one’s action; if minimization several observational studies carried out during
does not work, the evidence of harm can be periods of free interaction among children in
discredited (minimizing or misconstruing conse- unstructured contexts, such as during recess and
quences). Finally, disengagement practices may outdoor play. These studies, in fact, consistently
operate on the recipients of detrimental acts by found that peers are involved in 85% of bullying
stripping them of human qualities (dehumanization) episodes [Atlas and Pepler, 1998; Craig and Pepler,
or considering aggression as provoked by the victim 1997; Craig et al., 2000; Pepler and Craig, 1995].
(attribution of blame). These mechanisms can lead Moreover, Salmivalli et al. [1996] identified six
to aggressive behaviors through a process of moral different participant roles taken by individual
disengagement, that is, a partial gap between the children in the bullying process (i.e., victim, bully,
‘‘abstract’’ personal idea of moral behavior and reinforcer of the bully, assistant to the bully,
the individual’s behavior in real life. In this way, the defender of the victim and outsider). Another study
individual protects him/herself from negative feel- by Salmivalli et al. [1997] has also revealed a strong
ings, such as guilt or shame, that usually follow an influence of how the members of a group behave in
immoral conduct [Bandura, 1991]. bullying situations on an individual child’s behavior
Different studies showed a positive relation in such situations.
between aggressive behavior and the activation of In line with this approach that considers bullying
one or more of these mechanisms in both adults and as a group phenomenon, all distinct roles within the
children [Bandura et al., 1996; Caprara et al., 1995, class were considered in the present study. Not only
1996; Yadava et al., 2001]. In particular, Caprara bullies and victims participated, but also those
et al. [1995] confirmed the strong link between moral pupils who, in different ways, act important roles
disengagement and physical and verbal aggression, in reinforcing bullying behavior (the so-called
both self- and peer-evaluated, especially in male ‘‘Followers’’: the Assistants of the bully and the
children. More recently, Bandura et al. [2001] have Reinforcers) or, in contrast, intervene to defend the
replicated these results. They found a positive victim (the Defenders). Finally, Outsiders, that is,
relation between moral disengagement and trans- children not directly involved in the phenomenon,
gressive behavior, and a negative relation between were considered.
moral disengagement and self-regulatory efficacy
and prosocial behavior.
Aims of the Study and Hypotheses
The above-mentioned studies actually referred to
aggressive individuals, but a similar pattern of data The first aim of the present study was to assess the
has been found in school bullying research, as well. performance of these pupils in a social cognition
Bacchini et al. [1998] and Menesini et al. [1997], task similar to the one used by Sutton et al. [1999b].
for example, found that male bullies utilized the In this task, the same two typologies of stories used
moral disengagement mechanisms more than by Sutton and colleagues were used: cognitive and
did other peers and, most of all, they seemed to emotion stories. The cognitive stories, in particular,
use the mechanisms of Dehumanization and Moral assessed the child’s ability to understand mental
Justification. A confirmation of these results states, beliefs and intentions of the character of the
emerged in a cross-national study, involving Italy story. The emotion stories, instead, assessed the

Aggr. Behav. DOI 10.1002/ab


Social Cognition in Bullying 531

ability to understand emotional states of the METHOD


character and the possibility that an emotion could
Participants
be dissimulated to influence and modify others’
knowledge. Contrary to the idea of the bully as a The initial sample comprised 581 Italian primary-
social ‘‘oaf’’, it is hypothesized that the Bully group school children (295 boys and 286 girls) aged
will perform better than, or at least at the same level between 8 and 11 years (mean age 5 9.7 years,
as, other pupils in a theory of mind task that s.d. 5 11 months). Although socio-economic status
requires understanding the thoughts, beliefs, inten- was not directly measured, our sample included
tions that guide others’ behavior. students from a wide range of social backgrounds
Moreover, in addition to these two kinds of (low and working classes through upper middle
stories, a third category (moral stories), not used class). In terms of racial/ethnic background, the
by Sutton et al. [1999b], was designed in order to sample was predominantly Caucasian (96%), with a
explore the role of moral emotions in the context of small proportion of Asian (2%) and North African
aggressive conduct. The second aim of the present (2%) origin. All children received school and
study, therefore, was to analyze the ability to parental permission to participate prior to the
understand moral emotions (guilt and shame) within collection of the data.
hypothetical social scenarios by children who On the basis of peer nominations, children were
assume different roles. Considering the literature first assigned to one of the following Role groups:
about moral cognition in aggressive behavior [e.g., Bully, Assistant, Reinforcer, Defender, Outsider and
Hoffman, 2000; Quiles and Bybee, 1997; Tagney, Victim, based on the procedure described below.
1992], it was expected that Bullies would perform Seventy-three children did not receive a specific role
worse than non-aggressive mates in moral stories, because they did not satisfy the classification criteria
that is, be less likely to make the correct attribution proposed by Salmivalli et al. [1996] and were not
of moral emotions to the character of the story included in the final sample. The distribution of
owing to his/her negative behavior. In contrast, Participant Roles for the whole sample is reported
Defenders should be particularly sensitive to this in Table I.
kind of emotion as an expression of what is right As can be seen in Table I, cell sizes are rather
and what is wrong and, therefore, we expected this unequal for both Participant Roles and gender. In
group of children to perform better than any other order to handle problems connected with strongly
in this type of stories. unbalanced factorial designs [Cramer, 1998], espe-
Finally, the third aim of the study was to consider cially with small cell numbers, we decided to balance
the relation between moral disengagement and our Role groups for number and gender. Therefore,
bullying behavior [e.g., Caprara et al., 1995; a total of 204 children, divided into six Role groups
Menesini et al., 1999]. The comparison among (n 5 34; 17 males and 17 females for each one), were
pupils who assume different roles may lead to a randomly selected and participated in the second
deeper understanding of the importance of these phase of the study. The analyses presented in the
mechanisms in bullying episodes. Specifically, we result section have been conducted on this sample
expected that the tendency to activate moral with equalized groups.
disengagement mechanisms would be higher in
aggressive children (the Bullies and their Followers).
Material
In contrast, we hypothesized that Victims and those
children who frequently intervene to defend their Participant Role Questionnaire (PRQ). The
victimized classmates (Defenders) would show a Italian version [Menesini and Gini, 2000] of the
higher moral sensibility and, consequently, a lower Participant Role Questionnaire–PRQ [Salmivalli
level of moral disengagement. et al., 1996; Sutton and Smith, 1999] was used to

TABLE I. Distribution of Participant Roles (%) Within the Whole Sample

Participant roles

Bullies Assistants Reinforcers Defenders Outsiders Victims No role Total

Males 15.9 16.3 11.9 11.5 12.2 17.6 14.6 100


Females 5.9 6.3 7.7 26.2 25.5 17.8 10.6 100

Aggr. Behav. DOI 10.1002/ab


532 Gini

collect peer nominations. Children were asked to TABLE II. Role Score Correlations for the Whole Sample
nominate up to five boys and/or girls in their class
Bully Assistant Reinforcer Defender Outsider
who fitted each of the 21 behavioral descriptions of
bullying situations that compose the questionnaire. Assistant .68
In the present study, self-nominations were not Reinforcer .66 .60
considered. The items are divided into six scales, Defender .16 .11 .10
Outsider .22 .24 .07 .30
which refer to six different Participant Roles: Bully Victim .07 .01 .12  .01 .13
(4 items, e.g.: ‘‘Starts bullying’’), Assistant (2 items,
e.g.: ‘‘Helps the bully, maybe by catching or holding Note: N 5 578.
Po.001 (Bonferroni corrected).
the victim’’), Reinforcer (5 items, e.g.: ‘‘Laughs
at people getting bullied’’), Defender (5 items, e.g.:
‘‘Stick up for the victim’’), Outsider (4 items, e.g.:
‘‘Isn’t usually there, stays away’’) and Victim (a) Cognitive stories: these stories require the under-
(1 item, ‘‘Gets bullied’’). Menesini and Gini [2000] standing of mental states, thoughts, beliefs,
reported a good internal reliability of these scales intentions of characters, which determine their
for the Italian population (a 5 .78–.94). In Table II, behavior. Example of cognitive story [Happé,
correlations between different role scores are 1994]:
reported.
During the war, the Red army captures a
Roles1 were assigned according to the original
member of the Blue army. They want him to
procedure, proposed by Salmivalli et al. [1996]:
tell them where his army’s tanks are; they know
they are either by the sea or in the mountains.
* for each child a Role Score is calculated in each of They know that the prisoner will not want to
the six scales of the questionnaire. A nomination tell them, he will want to save his army, and so
for ‘‘sometimes’’ showing a behavior is scored as he will certainly lie to them. The prisoner is very
1, and ‘‘often’’ is scored as 2. Then, the brave and very clever, he will not let them find
nominations are summed, divided by the number his tanks. The tanks are really in the mountains.
of nominators and multiplied by 100; Now when the other side ask him where his
* a Participant Role is assigned on the basis of the tanks are, he says, ‘‘They are in the mountains’’;
child’s highest standardized role score. No role is
assigned if the child’s score is below the mean
(b) Emotion stories: in these stories the understand-
(Zo0) in all the six scales, or if the difference
ing of what kind of emotion is appropriate
between the two highest scores is less than .1.
within a specific situation is crucial. Moreover,
the child needs to understand that emotions can
be clearly shown or, on the contrary, dissimu-
Social cognition task: the stories. A set of 15 lated, to influence and modify others’ knowl-
short stories was designed to test children’s under- edge. In order to facilitate participants’ answers,
standing of mental states and emotions. Some of children were presented with drawings of faces
these stories were translated from Happé [1994] and representing different emotions (happiness, sad-
Sutton et al. [1999b], and additional stories were ness, anger, guilt and a neutral face). An
created reflecting the same structure of the pre- example of an emotion story taken from Sutton
existent stories. Even though some stories (e.g., the et al. [1999b] is:
‘‘army/war’’ story) are less relevant for children than
Mike wants to go out with his friends, but he has
others, they all seemed to be understandable and
a really bad tummy ache. He knows that if his
valid for primary school pupils.
Mum notices he is ill, she won’t let him go out to
The 15 stories were divided into three categories,
play. Mike goes downstairs and asks his Mum,
five stories for each category:
‘‘Can I go out to play please?’’;

1
An important point raised by a reviewer refers to the possibility to (c) Moral stories: in these stories the appropriate
consider the bully–victim group. However, in our sample, only eight emotion is a moral emotion because some
children scored above the cutting point for both bullies and victims
(equally distributed in the bully and the victim group), which was not wrongdoing has happened. In these stories
enough for separate analyses. Therefore, we decided not to include children were also helped with the drawings.
these subjects in our analyses. An example of moral story (new) is as follows:

Aggr. Behav. DOI 10.1002/ab


Social Cognition in Bullying 533

Claudia has just moved to her new school. She has Therefore, higher scores in this scale indicate a
brown hair, green eyes and she is a little bit shorter higher tendency to engage in one or more of these
than her classmates. During the maths class, mechanisms. Even though the items of the scale
without being noticed by her teacher, Susan, describe different mechanisms, previous studies
who sits behind Claudia, sends her a written note using this scale with Italian samples [Caprara
like ‘‘Pigmy, dwarf’’. Claudia reads this note and et al., 1995, 1996; Menesini et al., 1997] have
starts crying in front of all her mates. demonstrated a unidimensional structure of the
scale, especially for younger children. This was also
Each story was followed by a control question confirmed in our sample. Therefore, following the
based on the content of the story to verify that the suggestions of the authors of the scale, analyses were
child understood it; then, an experimental question conducted on the total score and not on separate
assessing the understanding of mental states or subscales in the present study. The reliability of the
emotions was asked. For the cognitive stories, an scale was a 5 .69.
answer was considered correct if the child referred to
the mental state, thoughts or beliefs of the char- Procedure
acters (e.g., critical question: ‘‘Where will the Red
Stage 1: The PRQ and the Moral Disengagement
army look for the Blue army and why?’’; answer:
Scale were completed during a single classroom
‘‘They will look by the sea because they think that
session and the order of presentation of the two
the prisoner is lying to them’’). For the emotion
measures was counterbalanced. This session began
stories, a correct answer included both the identifi-
by introducing the general purpose of the study.
cation of the appropriate emotion (positive or
Then, the following definition of bullying [Whitney
negative) and the reference to the intention/wish of
and Smith, 1993] was presented and discussed with
the main character to hide his/her own real emotion
the class:
to the other characters, for example, when a
character is ‘‘sad’’ but he or she appears ‘‘happy’’
We say a child or young person is being bullied, or
(e.g., ‘‘Mike has a bad tummy ache, but he appears
picked on when another child or young person, or
happy, otherwise his Mum notices that he is feeling
a group of children or young people, say nasty and
bad and won’t let him go out’’). Finally, for the
unpleasant things to him or her. It is also bullying
moral stories, an answer was considered correct if
when a child or young person is hit, kicked,
the child recognized the fact that, in the situation
threatened, locked inside a room, sent nasty notes,
described, the character of the story should have
when no one ever talks to them and things like that.
felt a moral emotion (guilt or shame) for his/her
These things can happen frequently and it is difficult
own behavior (e.g., ‘‘She (Susan) feels guilty because
for the child or young person being bullied to defend
she shouldn’t have said bad things to her
himself or herself. It is also bullying when a child or
mate Claudia’’).
young person is teased repeatedly in a nasty way.
Moral Disengagement Scale. This scale mea-
But it is not bullying when two children or young
sures the individual’s tendency to use cognitive
people of about the same strength have the odd fight
mechanisms that can disengage self-sanctions and
or quarrel.
justify the use of violent and aggressive behaviors.
Stage 2: When the data from Stage 1 had been
This version of the scale [Caprara et al., 1995],
analyzed, children selected through the PRQ were
designed for primary-school children, consists of 14
tested individually in an area of the school that
items describing individual’s readiness to construe
afforded privacy. During this session, the stories
injurious conduct as serving righteous purposes,
were presented in a randomized order and the
masquerading censurable activities by palliative
children’s answers were audio-recorded to facilitate
language or rendering them benign by advantageous
the successive transcription and coding procedure.
comparison, minimizing the harmful effects of one’s
In order to avoid any possible influence upon their
detrimental conduct and devaluing those who are
responses, the experimenter who interviewed chil-
maltreated (example items: ‘‘Some kids deserve to be
dren did not know their Participant role.
treated like animals’’; ‘‘Kids cannot be blamed for
misbehaving if their friends pressured them to do
it’’). Children were asked to rate the strength of their
RESULTS
endorsement or rejection of moral exoneration of
detrimental conduct on a 5-point Likert-type scale Results are presented using the categorical mea-
(from 1 5 strongly disagree to 5 5 strongly agree). sure of ‘‘Participant Roles’’ to compare group

Aggr. Behav. DOI 10.1002/ab


534 Gini

means in social cognition and moral disengagement cantly differ from each other (Bonferroni compar-
with independent 6 (Roles)  2 (Gender) ANOVAs. isons). The main effect of Gender and the
The continuous measure ‘‘Role Scores’’ is used to Role  Gender interaction were not statistically
test for correlations between the extent of different significant.
types of behavior in the whole sample and the social These data do not support the prediction of
cognition and moral disengagement measures. Hypothesis 1 that bullies should perform better
than other people in the theory of mind tasks, but
Preliminary Analysis they also fail to show social deficits among bullies.
In contrast to Hypothesis 2, bullies did not do worse
A preliminary one-way ANOVA was performed
than the other groups in the moral stories
to investigate the possible effect of Participant Roles
(Table III).
on the answers to the control questions. No
statistically significant differences emerged among
Role Scores and Social Cognition
the different roles [F(5, 192) 5 .962, P 5 n.s.], thus
confirming that the level of comprehension of A partial correlation analysis between social
the stories presented was similar for all Participant cognition and the actual extent of different types
Roles. of behavior (the role scores) was performed.
Moreover, in order to control for inter-rater Considering the fact that the scales are correlated
reliability, two independent judges coded the 30% with each other, to examine the relation of each role
of all children’s answers. Their level of agreement with the cognitive, emotion and moral information
reached 96%. Cohen’s kappa was also calculated, processing skills, in the correlation analysis the
resulting in K 5 .91. Disagreements were due to remaining PRQ scales and age were partialled out.
those answers in which the reference to mental states Moreover, considering the high number of compar-
was not completely clear. All discrepancies were isons, in order to avoid the increase of type 1 error,
resolved by consultation between the coders. Bonferroni’s correction was used (adjusted level of
a: .05/24 5 .002). Correlation coefficients are shown
Participant Roles and Social Cognition in Table IV. In line with Hypothesis 1, Bully role is
positively correlated with the total social cognition
As the three categories into which our stories were score and with the cognitive score (the ability
divided (cognitive, emotion and moral stories) are to understand thoughts, intentions, beliefs, etc.)
different from each other, three separate ANOVAs and the emotion score. Moreover, in accordance
were conducted on the mean performance in the with Hypothesis 2, the Defender score is positively
three sets of stories, with Role and Gender as correlated with all social cognition scores.
between factors. From the analysis, a statistically
significant main effect of Role emerged only for the Participant Roles and Moral Disengagement
cognitive stories [F(5, 192) 5 2.376, P 5 .04,
Z2p ¼ :058]. Post hoc analyses revealed that Victims’ As concerns the third aim, a two-way ANOVA on
performance (M 5 2.88, s.d. 5 1.61) was signifi- the mean score in the Moral Disengagement Scale
cantly lower than Defenders (M 5 4.03, was performed with Role and Gender as between
s.d. 5 1.38), whereas other groups did not signifi- factors. A statistically significant main effect of Role

TABLE III. Participant Roles and Mean Score [s.d.] in Social TABLE IV. Partial Correlations of Social Cognition and
Cognition Task Individual Role Scores

Participant Cognitive Emotion Moral Total social Social cognition scores


rolesa storiesb storiesb storiesb cognition taskc
Role scores Cognitive Emotion Moral Total score
Bullies 3.06 [1.30] 2.79 [2.20] 3.18 [1.94] 9.03 [4.79]
Assistants 3.18 [1.47] 3.20 [2.04] 3.59 [1.86] 9.97 [4.85] Bully .195 .148 .129 .172
Reinforcers 3.15 [1.56] 3.26 [1.76] 3.62 [1.67] 10.03 [4.28] Assistant .021 .026 .014 .012
Defenders 4.03 [1.38] 4.12 [1.45] 4.00 [1.67] 12.15 [4.04] Reinforcer .074 .059 .098 .078
Outsiders 3.38 [1.76] 3.68 [1.66] 4.06 [1.63] 11.12 [4.58] Defender .214 .226 .197 .223
Victims 2.88 [1.61] 3.26 [1.81] 3.44 [1.76] 9.59 [4.73] Outsider .071 .008 .060 .050
Victim .068 .109 .112 .104
a
n 5 34 for each group.
b
Range 5 0–5. Note: N 5 204.
c Po.001 (Bonferroni corrected).
Range 5 0–15.

Aggr. Behav. DOI 10.1002/ab


Social Cognition in Bullying 535

40 DISCUSSION
35
30 Social Cognition in Bullying
25
20
The present study is located in research about the
15 role of social cognition in the development of
10 bullying behavior among schoolmates. In particular,
5 two different models have been proposed in the
0 literature to explain the Bully’s behavior in terms
of social information processing. The first model
,a

,d
s,

s,

ts,

s,
rs

,b
er

tim

er
de

an

es
sid

rc
hypothesizes the presence of deficits in the social
en

ist
ic

lli
fo
ut

V
ef

ss

Bu
in
O
D

Re
information processing of bullies, similar to those
Fig. 1. Participant roles and moral disengagement. Notes: n 5 34 for found for aggressive children [Crick and Dodge,
each group, range 5 14–70, a,b,c,dMeans with different superscript 1994; Dodge, 1980]. This model, therefore, proposes
differ at Po.05. the idea of bullies as ‘‘oaf’’ children. They are
considered deficient in their social intelligence, that
is their ability to interpret and manage information
emerged [F(5, 192) 5 9.269, Po.001, Z2p ¼ :194], deriving from social interactions with peers and to
whereas neither Gender [F(1, 192) 5 .039, P 5 n.s.] produce adequate social responses. The second
nor the Role  Gender interaction [F(5, 192) 5 .844, model, in contrast, states that bullies are quite
P 5 n.s.] were significant. Post-hoc analyses (Bon- skilful in understanding social cues (e.g., others’
ferroni comparisons) showed that all aggressive mental states) and that they exploit this ability
roles share the same tendency to activate moral to their own advantage, acting as ‘‘skilled social
disengagement mechanisms, in fact Bullies manipulators’’ [Sutton et al., 1999a,b]. However,
(M 5 38.03, s.d. 5 7.58), Reinforcers (M 5 35.76, according to these authors, bullies may lack the
s.d. 5 8.15) and Assistants (M 5 33.74, s.d. 5 7.80) empathic reactivity towards their mates’ emotions
performed significantly higher than Defenders and, in particular, towards victim’s suffering.
(M 5 26.53, s.d. 5 7.13). Moreover, Bullies also In the first two hypotheses of the present study, it
showed a higher level of moral disengagement than was anticipated that bullies may have a good theory
both Outsiders (M 5 30.35, s.d. 5 7.63) and Victims of mind, but they may be deficient in moral
(M 5 32.41, s.d. 5 8.12). These findings support cognition. The analysis of variance, indeed, indi-
Hypothesis 3 (Fig. 1). cated that the bully group did not show any
difficulty, compared to non-aggressive peers, in the
understanding of cognitive stories, which required
Role Scores and Moral Disengagement
them to assume the cognitive point of view of
A partial correlation analysis, controlled for age another person, that is to understand intentions,
of participants, between the score in the Moral beliefs and goals that guide a determinate behavior.
Disengagement Scale and the role scores was Moreover, correlations between the scores in each
performed. Results showed a positive correlation scale of the PRQ and the performance in the social
between moral disengagement and the role scores in cognition task indicated a positive relation between
Bully (r(204) 5 .26, Po.001), Assistant (r(204) 5 .24, bullying behavior and the ability to understand
Po.001) and Reinforcer (r(204) 5 .22, Po.001) others’ mental states. As regards the two models,
scales. In contrast, a negative correlation between therefore, our results are closer to what would be
moral disengagement and the role score in the expected by the Skilled Manipulator model [Sutton
Defender scale (r(204) 5 .22, Po.001) and in the et al., 1999a]. The fact that our bullies did not show
Outsider scale (r(204) 5 .12, Po.01) emerged, particular deficits in this specific area of social
whereas the correlation with the score in the Victim cognition does not suggest a direct application of the
scale was not significant (r(204) 5 .05, P 5 n.s.). Social Skills Deficit model [Crick and Dodge, 1994]
Therefore, these data largely support Hypothesis 3. to bullying behavior. Further analyses are needed
They are also in accordance with previous results into the social cognitive competence of children
showing a positive relation between aggressive involved in bullying episodes. This is particularly
behavior and the tendency to disengage self-sanc- important because theory of mind explanations per
tions and justify the use of violent and aggressive se are not likely to be very helpful for understanding
behaviors, and a negative relation between moral the unique nature of bullying, in that having a
disengagement and prosocial behavior. sophisticated theory of mind can lead to highly

Aggr. Behav. DOI 10.1002/ab


536 Gini

prosocial behavior or to bullying behavior. In other ing its negative consequences on others. In contrast,
words, having a ‘‘superior’’ theory of mind says high levels of empathic responsiveness act as an im-
nothing about how that knowledge will be utilized in portant mediator in social conduct, reducing aggres-
social interactions [Arsenio and Lemerise, 2001]. sion and favoring prosocial behavior [Björkqvist
As regards the two other types of stories used in et al., 2000; Kaukiainen et al., 1994, 1996].
the present study, bullies’ performance in emotion With respect to the other groups considered in the
stories and also in moral stories, a category not used present study, victimized children showed some
by Sutton et al. [1999b], was found to be slightly, but difficulties in the social cognition task. If compared
not significantly lower, than the performance to the defender group in particular, they seem to
obtained by other groups. Contrary to our second lack the ability to understand others’ mind, that is
hypothesis about the possible difficulties of bullies in their thoughts, beliefs and intentions that guide
moral cognition, our results do not completely social behavior. Our data are consistent with results
confirm the literature on moral development in reported by Sutton et al. [1999b]. Moreover, several
aggressive children and bullies [e.g., Bandura, 1991; authors share the idea that victims show deficits in
Hart et al., 2003]. In our sample, in fact, the bullies areas like social skills, social problem solving,
and their followers did not show a significant lack of assertiveness and emotional regulation [e.g., Cham-
ability in the attribution of appropriate moral emo- pion et al., 2003; Kochenderfer and Ladd, 1997;
tions to the characters of the stories. These emotions, Mahady-Wilton et al., 2000; Olweus, 1993; Perry
such as guilt and shame, play an important role in the et al., 1988]. Finally, the results obtained in the
recognition of the damage suffered by another person social cognition task by the defender group should
and are closely connected to the sense of responsi- be noted. These children, in fact, showed high levels
bility of the person who shows the harmful behavior. of performance in all the stories, and especially in
Moreover, these emotions imply the need of repara- the cognitive stories. Moreover, the score in the
tion as a consequence of a negative outcome defender scale of the PRQ correlated positively with
produced by one’s behavior [Bybee, 1998; Hoffman, all the scores in the social cognition task, thus
2000]. suggesting that the adoption of this kind of
However, for both emotion and moral stories, it is prosocial and helpful behavior requires a high level
important to note that they actually assess the of social ability and a well-developed understanding
‘‘cognitive’’ ability to process emotional information of both cognitive and emotional states of others.
in social interactions rather than an ‘‘emotional’’ or However, the fact that the bullying score also cor-
‘‘empathic’’ comprehension of others’ feelings. related with the social cognition task, again, con-
Therefore, the positive performance shown by firms that having a good theory of mind, by itself,
aggressive children in these kinds of stories may does not necessarily mean that this ability will be
misrepresent or underestimate the real deficit of used to act prosocially [Arsenio and Lemerise, 2001].
these children in emotional understanding of others.
In other words, what bullies may lack and what may
Bullying and Moral Disengagement
differentiate them from prosocial children is the
ability to appreciate the emotional consequences As regards the third aim of the present study, the
of their behaviors on others’ feelings, and to share analysis of variance on moral disengagement
in, and empathize with, the feelings of others showed higher levels of the tendency to disengage
[Arsenio and Lemerise, 2001; Eisenberg and Fabes, self-sanctions and justify the use of aggressive
1998]. In line with this perspective, for example, Gini behaviors in all the aggressive roles and, in
et al. [2004] recently found a negative relation particular, in bullies. These results confirmed the
between bullying behavior and empathy, measured role of specific cognitive mechanisms in the regula-
through Davis’ empathic reactivity index [1983], in a tion of social conduct, such as the moral disengage-
sample of Italian adolescents. ment mechanisms that allow the assumption of
Sutton et al. [1999a] also argued along these lines, violent behaviors [Bacchini et al., 1998; Caprara
referring to the ‘‘cold cognition’’ hypothesis pro- et al., 1995; Menesini et al., 1999, 2003a,b].
posed by Mealey [1995] as regards sociopathy. They Regarding this aspect, as suggested by a reviewer,
also spoke of a theory of mind formulated purely in it could be interesting to collect information about
instrumental terms, without access to the empathic the relations between bullying, familial socialization
understanding that most people rely on. As a and moral climate at home and at school.
consequence, the individual may use aggressive In contrast, defenders’ high moral sensibility and
behavior in a Machiavellian way, without consider- empathic reactivity, already found in other studies

Aggr. Behav. DOI 10.1002/ab


Social Cognition in Bullying 537

[Gini and Carli, 2003; Menesini et al., 2003a,b], may REFERENCES


be one of the basic motivations for prosocial Almeida A, del Barrio C, Marques M, Gutierrez H, van der Meulen
behavior, frequently shown by these children to- K. 2001. Scan-bullying: A script-cartoon narrative to assess
wards their victimized mates. These motivations are cognitions, emotions and coping strategies in bullying situations.
very important and must be taken into account In: Martinez M (ed.): ‘‘Prevention and Control of Aggression and
the Impact on its Victims,’’ New York: Kluwer Academic/
in the implementation of intervention programs in
Plenum Publishers, pp 161–168.
schools [Cowie, 2000; Cowie et al., 2002]. However, Arsenio WF. 1988. Children’s conceptions of the situational affective
the fact that the ability to help other peers requires consequences of sociomoral events. Child Dev 58:1611–1622.
several interpersonal competencies that young Arsenio WF, Lemerise EA. 2001. Varieties of childhood bullying:
children do not necessarily possess needs to be Values, emotion processes and social competence. Soc Dev 10:
considered. 59–73.
Atlas RS, Pepler DJ. 1998. Observations of bullying in the
In conclusion, the results presented in this paper classroom. J Educ Res 92:86–97.
have several potential implications for future re- Bacchini D, Amodeo AL, Ciardi A, Valerio P, Vitelli R. 1998. La
search. On the one hand, our results draw attention relazione vittima-prepotente: stabilità del fenomeno e ricorso a
to the importance of studying the role of social meccanismi di disimpegno morale [Bully–victim relationship:
Stability of the phenomenon and use of moral disengagement].
cognitive competencies in the complex circle of
Scienze dell’Interazione 5:29–46.
behaviors, attitudes and expectations upon which Bandura A. 1986. ‘‘Social Foundation of Thought and Action.
school bullying phenomenon is based. On the other A Social Cognitive Theory,’’ Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-
hand, the relevance of adopting a more fine-grained Hall.
classification of roles in the study of bullying Bandura A. 1990. Selective activation and disengagement of moral
control. J Soc Issues 46:27–46.
behavior was confirmed.
Bandura A. 1991. Social cognitive theory of moral thought and
A possible limitation in the interpretation of our action. In: Kurtines WM, Gewirtz GL (eds): ‘‘Handbook of
results was the fact that we did not consider the Moral Behavior and Development: Theory, Research and
bully-victim group, that is, those children who Applications,’’ Vol. 1. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp 71–129.
frequently assume both kinds of role, acting as a Bandura A. 2002. Selective moral disengagement in the exercise of
bully and being victimized as well [e.g., Boulton and moral agency. J Moral Educ 312:101–119.
Bandura A, Barbaranelli C, Caprara GV, Pastorelli C. 1996.
Smith, 1994; Olweus, 1993]. Several authors stressed Mechanisms of moral disengagement in the exercise of moral
the relevance of this group of pupils, also called agency. J Pers Soc Psychol 71:364–374.
aggressive victims [Perry et al., 1988; Schwartz, Bandura A, Caprara GV, Barbaranelli C, Pastorelli C, Regalia C.
2000], in the study of aggressive problems. More- 2001. Sociocognitive self-regulatory mechanisms governing
transgressive behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol 801:125–135.
over, these children have been consistently described
Baumeister RF, Stillwell AM, Heatherton TF. 1994. Guilt: An
as lacking social-emotional abilities, having psycho- interpersonal approach. Psychol Bull 115:243–267.
social impairments and a wide range of other Besag V. 1989. ‘‘Bullies and Victims in Schools,’’ Milton Keynes:
problems [e.g., Besag, 1989]. A methodological Open University Press Milton.
difficulty in the study of these pupils is that it is Björkqvist K, Österman K, Kaukiainen A. 2000. Social intelligen-
not always simple to clearly identify bully-victims as ce–empathy 5 aggression? Aggr Violent Behav 52:191–200.
Boulton M, Smith PK. 1994. Bully/victim problems in middle-school
a distinct group. Despite this complication, future children: Stability, self-perceived competence, peer perceptions
research in this field will necessarily need to study and peer acceptance. Br J Dev Psychol 12:315–329.
the social cognitive and moral characteristics of this Bybee J. 1998. ‘‘Guilt and Children,’’ San Diego: Academic Press.
particular group of children, and to compare their Caprara GV, Pastorelli C, Bandura A. 1995. La misura del
level of cognitive and emotional understanding with disimpegno morale in età evolutiva [Measuring age differences
in moral disengagement]. Età Evol 51:18–29.
that of ‘‘pure’’ bullies and victims. Caprara GV, Barbaranelli C, Vicino S, Bandura A. 1996. La misura
del disimpegno morale [Measuring moral disengagement]. Rass
Psicol 113:93–105.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Champion K, Vernberg E, Shipman K. 2003. Nonbullying victims
of bullies: Aggression, social skills, and friendship characteristics.
The author is grateful to all schools, teachers and
J Appl Dev Psychol 245:535–551.
pupils who participated in the study. Moreover, he Cowie H. 2000. Bystanding or standing by: Gender issues in coping
thanks Francesca Happè and Jon Sutton for their with bullying in English schools. Aggr Behav 261:85–97.
stories, translated and utilized in this study, and Cowie H, Naylor P, Talamelli L, Chauhan P, Smith PK. 2002.
Elena Arimondo, Alessia Bortoloso, Giuseppe Carli Knowledge, use and attitudes towards peer support. J Adoles-
and Simona Pischetola for their help in data cence 25:453–467.
Craig WM, Pepler DJ. 1997. Observations of bullying and
collection. Finally, he is grateful to three anonymous victimization in the schoolyard. Can J School Psychol 2:41–60.
reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier Craig WM, Pepler DJ, Atlas R. 2000. Observations of bullying in the
version of the manuscript. playground and in the classroom. School Psychol Int 21:22–36.

Aggr. Behav. DOI 10.1002/ab


538 Gini

Cramer D. 1998. ‘‘Fundamental Statistics for Social Research,’’ Kaukiainen A, Björkqvist K, Österman K, Lagerspetz KMJ,
London: Routledge. Niskanen L. 1994. Social intelligence and the use of indirect
Crick NR, Dodge KA. 1994. A review and reformulation of social aggression. Presented at the XIII Biennal Meetings of the
information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjust- International Society for the Study of Behavioural Development,
ment. Psychol Bull 115:74–101. June 28–July 2, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
Davis MH. 1983. Measuring individual differences in empathy: Kaukiainen A, Björkqvist K, Österman K, Lagerspetz KMJ. 1996.
Evidence for a multidimensional approach. J Pers Soc Psychol Social intelligence and empathy as antecedents of different types
441:113–126. of aggression. In: Ferris CF, Grisson T (eds): ‘‘Understanding
Dodge K. 1980. Social cognition and children’s aggressive behavior. Aggressive Behavior in Children,’’ Ann NY Acad Sci 794,
Child Dev 51:162–170. pp 364–366.
Dodge K, Frame CL. 1982. Social cognitive bias and deficits in Kochenderfer BJ, Ladd G. 1997. Victimized children’s responses to
aggressive boys. Child Dev 53:620–635. peers aggression: Behaviors associated with reduced versus
Dodge K, Newman JP. 1981. Biased decision-making processes in continued victimization. Dev Psychopathol 9:59–73.
aggressive boys. J Abnorm Psychol 90:375–379. Mahady-Wilton MM, Craig WM, Pepler DJ. 2000. Emotional
Dodge K, Price JM. 1994. On the relation between social regulation and display in classroom victims of bullying:
information processing and socially competent behavior in early Characteristic expression of affect, coping styles and relevant
school-aged children. Child Dev 65:1385–1397. contextual factors. Soc Dev 92:226–245.
Dodge KA, Somberg D. 1987. Hostile attributional biases are Mealey L. 1995. The sociobiology of sociopathy: An integrated
exacerbated under conditions of threat to the self. Child Dev 58: evolutionary model. Behav Brain Sci 18:523–599.
213–224. Menesini E, Gini G. 2000. Il bullismo come processo di gruppo:
Dodge K, Bates J, Pettit G. 1990. Mechanisms in the cycle of adattamento e validazione del questionario ‘‘Ruoli dei parteci-
violence. Science 250:1678–1683. panti’’ alla popolazione italiana [Bullying as a group process:
Eisenberg N, Fabes R. 1998. Prosocial development. In: Damon W, Adaptation and validation of the Participant Role Questionnaire
Eisenberg N (eds): ‘‘Handbook of Child Psychology,’’ Vol. 3, 5th to the Italian population]. Età Evol 66:18–32.
edition, New York: Wiley, pp 701–778. Menesini E, Fonzi V, Vannucci M. 1997. Bullies and victims in Italy:
Gini G, Carli G. 2003. Il bullismo a scuola: analisi dei meccanismi di Attitudes and moral disengagement. Paper presented at the 7th
disimpegno morale in una prospettiva di gruppo [Bullying at European Conference for Research on Learning and Instruction
school: Analysis of moral disengagement mechanisms in a group EARLI, Athens, 26–30 August 1997.
perspective]. Orientamenti Pedagogici. Riv Int Educ 50:303–314. Menesini E, Codecasa E, Benelli B, Cowie H. 2003a. Enhancing
Gini G, Albiero P, Benelli B. 2004. Relazioni tra bullismo, empatia, children’s responsibility to take action against bullying: Evalua-
immagine di sè ed autoefficacia percepita in un campione di tion of a befriending intervention in Italian middle schools. Aggr
adolescenti [Relations between bullying, empathy, self-image and Behav 291:10–14.
self-efficacy in adolescents]. Poster presented at the 18th National Menesini E, Sanchez V, Fonzi A, Ortega R, Costabile A, Lo
Conference of Developmental Psychology, Sciacca (AG), Italy. Feudo G. 2003b. Moral emotions and bullying: A cross-national
Happè F. 1994. An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding comparison of differences between bullies, victims and outsiders.
of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, Aggr Behav 29:515–530.
mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J Autism Nucci L. 2001. ‘‘Education in the Moral Domain,’’ Cambridge:
Dev Disorders 24:129–154. Cambridge University Press.
Harpur TJ, Hakstian AR, Hare RD. 1988. Factor structure of the Olweus D. 1993. ‘‘Bullying at School. What we Know and What we
psychopathy checklist. J Consult Clin Psychol 56:741–747. can do,’’ Oxford and Cambridge: Blackwell.
Hart D, Burock D, London B, Atkins R. 2003. Prosocial tendencies, Pepler DJ, Craig WM. 1995. A peek behind the fence: Naturalistic
antisocial behavior, and moral development. In: Slater A, observations of aggressive children with remote audiovisual
Bremner G (eds): ‘‘An Introduction to Developmental Psychol- recording. Dev Psychol 31:548–553.
ogy,’’ Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers, pp 334–356. Perry DG, Kusel SJ, Perry LC. 1988. Victims of peer aggression. Dev
Harvey RJ, Fletcher J, French DJ. 2001. Social reasoning: A source Psychol 24:807–814.
of influence on aggression. Clin Psychol Rev 213:447–469. Quiles ZN, Bybee J. 1997. Chronic and predispositional guilt:
Hawley PH. 2003. Prosocial and coercive configurations of resource Relations to mental health, prosocial behavior and religiosity.
control in early adolescence: A case for the well-adapted J Pers Assess 69:104–126.
machiavellian. Merrill Palmer Quart 49:279–309. Randall P. 1997. ‘‘Adult Bullying: Perpetrators and Victims.’’
Hazler RJ. 1996. ‘‘Breaking the Cycle of Violence: Interventions for London: Routledge.
Bullying and Victimization,’’ Washington, DC: Accelerated Salmivalli C, Lagerspetz K, Björkqvist K, Österman K, Kaukiainen
Development. A. 1996. Bullying as a group process: Participant Roles and their
Hodges EVE, Malone MJ, Perry DG. 1997. Individual risk and relations to social status within the group. Aggr Behav 22:1–15.
social risk as interacting determinants of victimization in the peer Salmivalli C, Huttunen A, Lagerspetz KM. 1997. Peer networks and
group. Dev Psychol 33:1032–1039. bullying in schools. Scand J Psychol 38:305–312.
Hoffman ML. 1976. Empathy, role-taking, guilt, and development of Schwartz D. 2000. Subtypes of victims and aggressors in children’s
altruistic motives. In: Likona T (ed.): ‘‘Moral Development and peer groups. J Abnorm Child Psychol 28:181–192.
Behavior: Theory, Research, and Social Issues,’’ New York: Slee PT. 1993. Bullying: A preliminary investigation of the nature
Holt, Rinehart Winston, pp 124–143. and effects on social cognition. Early Child Development and
Hoffman ML. 2000. ‘‘Empathy and Moral Development,’’ New Care 87:47–57.
York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Smetana JG. 1995. Morality in context: Abstractions, ambiguities
Huesmann LR, Guerra NG. 1997. Children’s normative beliefs and applications. In: Vasta R (ed.): ‘‘Annals of Child Develop-
about aggression and aggressive behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol 72: ment: A Research Annual,’’ Vol. 10. London: Jessica Kingsley,
408–419. pp 83–130.

Aggr. Behav. DOI 10.1002/ab


Social Cognition in Bullying 539

Smith PK, Bowers L, Binney V, Cowie H. 1993. Relationships Sutton J, Smith PK, Swettenham J. 1999c. Socially undesirable needs
of children involved in bully/victim problems at school. In: not be incompetent: A response to Crick and Dodge. Soc Dev 8:
Duck S (ed.): ‘‘Understanding Relationship processes. Vol. 2: 132–134.
Learning About Relationships,’’ Newbury Park, Ca: Sage, Tagney JP. 1992. Situational determinants of shame and guilt in
pp 184–212. young adulthood. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 18:199–206.
Sutton J, Smith PK. 1999. Bullying as a group process: An adaptation Turiel E. 1998. Moral development. In: Damon W, Eisenberg N
of the Participant Role approach. Aggr Behav 25:97–111. (eds): ‘‘Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 3. Social, Emo-
Sutton J, Smith PK, Swettenham J. 1999a. Bullying and ‘‘theory of tional, and Personality Development,’’ 5th edition. New York:
mind’’: A critique of the ‘‘social skills deficit’’ view of anti-social Wiley, pp 863–932.
behaviour. Soc Dev 8:117–134. Whitney I, Smith PK. 1993. A survey of the nature and extent of
Sutton J, Smith PK, Swettenham J. 1999b. Social cognition and bullying in junior/middle and secondary schools. Educ Res 35:3–25.
bullying: Social inadequacy or skilled manipulation? Br J Dev Yadava A, Sharma NR, Gandhi A. 2001. Aggression and moral
Psychol 17:435–450. disengagement. J Pers Clin Stud 17:95–99.

Aggr. Behav. DOI 10.1002/ab

You might also like