You are on page 1of 13

AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR

Volume 00, pages 1–13 (2012)

Main and Moderated Effects of Moral Cognition and


Status on Bullying and Defending
Simona C. S. Caravita1 ∗ , Gianluca Gini2 , and Tiziana Pozzoli2
1
C.R.I.d.e.e, Department of Psychology, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Milano, Italy
2
Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy

: : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : :
This study analyzed the relations of two dimensions of moral cognition (i.e., acceptance of moral transgression and moral dis-
engagement) and two forms of status in the peer group (i.e., social preference and perceived popularity) with bullying and de-
fending among 235 primary-school children and 305 middle-school early adolescents. Social status was tested as a moderator of
the associations between moral cognition and bullying and defending. Participants completed self-reports assessing the two di-
mensions of moral cognition and peer nominations for status, bullying, and defending. Both acceptance of moral transgression
and moral disengagement were associated to bullying among early adolescents only, whereas in childhood moral disengagement
was linked to defending among girls. Social status moderated the associations between morality dimensions and bullying and
defending. The moderating effects of status were discussed considering status as a magnifying lens for the relations between in-
dividual characteristics and social behavior. The results were also discussed with reference to age and gender differences in the
associations. Aggr. Behav. 00:1–13, 2012. 
C 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

: : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : :

Keywords: bullying; defending; morality; social preference; perceived popularity

INTRODUCTION planation of bullying and defending behavior in late


childhood and early adolescence. Most importantly,
Recent approaches to bullying have described it as
in keeping with the view of bullying as a group pro-
strategic, goal-oriented, even skillful behavior, aimed
cess [Salmivalli et al., 1996] and the broader child-
at changing the structure of the peer group, manipu-
by-environment perspective [e.g., Ladd, 2003], recent
lating peer relationships, and gaining social power by
research has provided some evidence that the individ-
intentionally inflicting harm [e.g., Peeters et al., 2010;
ual position within the peer-group, in terms of social
Sijtsema et al., 2009]. As such, bullying represents an
status, moderates the associations between defending
immoral act because it is in contrast with children’s
and bullying, on the one hand, and children’s charac-
fundamental rights of not being humiliated and op-
teristics, such as affective and cognitive empathy, and
pressed, and of living in a safe place [Hymel et al.,
theory of mind, on the other hand [Caravita et al.,
2010]. On the other end, some children hold negative
2009, 2010]. Based on this background, we worked
attitudes toward bullying behavior, feel personal re-
with two different age groups to test the hypothesis
sponsibility for intervention and, at least under some
that social status in the peer group could moderate the
contextual circumstances (such as when peers support
defending behavior), actively intervene to defend the
Contract grant sponsor: Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore; Con-
bullied classmate [Pozzoli et al., 2012; Pozzoli et al., in tract grant number: D1–2006; Contract grant sponsor: University of
press; Salmivalli and Voeten, 2004]. In comparison to Padova; Contract grant number: Grant CPDA085704.
bullying behavior as an immoral conduct, defending Part of this study was presented at the 2010 Biennial Meeting of the
the victim is a moral behavior, aimed at increasing the Society for Research on Adolescence, Philadelphia, PA, March 2010.
other child’s safety and well being. ∗ Correspondence to: Simona C. S. Caravita, C.R.I.d.e.e., Diparti-
Based on this conceptualization of bullying [Hymel mento di Psicologia, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, L.go
et al., 2010], in the present study we concurrently Gemelli, 1, 20123 Milano, Italy. E-mail: simona.caravita@unicatt.it
tested whether two components of morality, namely Received 21 June 2011; Accepted 11 June 2012
acceptance of moral transgression and moral disen- Published online in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com).
gagement, offered a different contribution in the ex- DOI: 10.1002/ab.21447


C 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
2 Caravita et al.

associations between morality and these behaviors. With respect to bullying behavior, a recent study
More specifically, we aimed to test two hypotheses on [Caravita et al., 2009] showed that, in comparison to
the role of status that have been recently formulated peers, child and early adolescent bullies considered
[see Caravita and Cillessen, 2012], namely the “sta- the breaking of moral rules (e.g., a rule forbidding
tus as a motivator” and, above all, the “status as a pushing and hitting other schoolmates) as acceptable
magnifying lens” hypotheses. To our knowledge, no when it was allowed by authorities of that context
previous studies have tested these hypotheses in rela- (i.e., teachers and head teachers for school). More-
tion to morality and different individual behaviors in over, Gasser and Keller [2009] found that, when pro-
bullying. vided with hypothetical scenarios about moral trans-
gressions (e.g., verbal bullying), 7–8-year-old bullies
scored significantly lower than prosocial peers (i.e.,
BULLYING, DEFENDING, AND MORALITY
peers sharing with others, comforting, and helping
Bullying is an instrumental form of aggression, in others in distress) on the understanding of moral rule
which attacking another person is mainly used as a transgression as wrong. Based on these findings, we
tool to gain or keep a prominent position among peers may speculate that, compared to (above all proso-
and obtain higher levels of resource control [Sijtsema cial) peers, bullies may be more prone to adopt social-
et al., 2009]. Children who bully usually know that conventional criteria to judge moral rules, because
they are causing pain to the victim, thus breaking they can accept the transgression of these rules, at
moral rules that forbid harming others [Turiel, 1983]. least when “allowed” by authorities [Caravita et al.,
Therefore, bullies might either conceive moral rules 2009].
as more breakable than their nonaggressive peers do, As far as moral self-justification is concerned, moral
or be more prone to self-justifying their offensive con- disengagement processes can also enable the trans-
duct in order to avoid feelings of guilt. In other words, gressor to self-justify the immoral action [Bandura,
two moral processes may be relevant in this context: (i) 1991]. Indeed, a bulk of studies has evidenced that bul-
judging moral rules as breakable because of a wrong lies tend to use moral disengagement mechanisms, es-
conception of these rules as not universally valid and pecially from early adolescence onwards [e.g., Barchia
worthy by themselves, and consequently more easily and Bussey, 2011; Gini, 2006; Gini et al., 2011; Hymel
accepting moral transgressions, and (ii) using mech- et al., 2005; Menesini et al., 2003; Obermann, 2011].
anisms of self-justification that allow the individual This can allow bullies to avoid sense of guilt.
to act in an aggressive way without feeling guilty, However, conceptualizing bullying as an immoral
namely mechanisms of moral disengagement [Ban- act may be not enough. Some authors [e.g., Arsenio
dura, 1991]. However, till now these two moral pro- and Lemerise, 2001; Sutton and Keogh, 2000] have
cesses have not been analyzed together in a single argued that bullying behavior does not derive only
study. from individual deficits (e.g., in terms of social cog-
As far as the first component is concerned, there is nition or moral understanding), but is sometimes an
some evidence that from the early age of three years, adaptive strategy within the peer group, because it al-
children perceive moral rules (which are aimed at pre- lows to gain social status and access resources. For
serving others’ well being) as universally valid, inde- example, bullies are sometimes described as assertive,
pendent of the statements of authorities, and their good leaders, popular, and well integrated into the
transgression as not acceptable. In contrast, the va- peer group [Estell et al., 2007; Pellegrini et al., 1999].
lidity of other rules, namely the social-conventional In order to better understand the extent to which chil-
rules (which are aimed at preserving the social order), dren who bully differ from well adjusted and socially
is conceived to be dependent on the dictates of au- skillful, but nonaggressive peers, researchers usually
thority, and to be more easily breakable than moral compare bullying with defending behavior [e.g., Car-
rules are. However, people can sometimes attribute the avita et al., 2009; Gini et al., 2007]. Defending in bul-
characteristics of social-conventional rules, such as lying situations refers to actively intervening to stop
their dependency on authority states, to moral rules, the bullying, helping, or consoling the bullied school-
thus believing that their transgression is, to some ex- mate, or asking for adults’ intervention, and it is asso-
tent, acceptable. Acceptance of moral transgression, ciated with good social skills, high social self-efficacy,
in general or when “allowed” by authorities, is con- and efficacious coping strategies [e.g., Caravita et al.,
sidered a signal of a social-conventional perception of 2009; Gini et al., 2008; Pöyhönen et al., 2010; Poz-
moral rules. In the end, this process can favor the ac- zoli and Gini, 2010; Pozzoli et al., 2012]. Moreover,
tual transgression of moral rules [Leenders and Brug- defenders have a high sense of moral responsibility
man, 2005]. and feelings of guilt or shame in bullying situations

Aggr. Behav.
Morality and Bullying: Moderations by Status 3

[Menesini and Camodeca, 2008; Pozzoli and Gini, by peers, and be motivated to defend, because this
2010]. In other words, children who defend victims behavior is rewarded with peer-acceptance.
may experience what Menesini and Camodeca [2008] Beyond literature on the positive relation between
called “the moral conflict of the innocent bystander,” being high in status and enacting social behaviors
which may increase the likelihood that they actively rewarded by status [e.g., Hymel et al., 2010], a re-
intervene in front of a peer in pain, danger, or distress. cent study [Caravita and Cillessen, 2012] has provided
Nevertheless, to date morality characteristics likely to some further evidence for the status as a motiva-
be associated with defending have been explored in tor hypothesis by showing that, among early ado-
few studies, and need to be better examined. lescents (11–14 years), perceived popularity medi-
This study adds to the current literature by ana- ated the positive association between motivation to
lyzing the role of both dimensions of morality dis- be prominent among peers (i.e., agentic goals) and
cussed here (i.e., acceptance of moral transgression bullying, whereas social preference mediated the neg-
and moral disengagement) together in two age groups ative associations between motivation to be well liked
and in relation to both bullying and defending. (i.e., communal goals) and bullying. Because status
can motivate status-enhancing behavior, it may even
magnify the associations between status-enhancing
behaviors and their psychological correlates (status
BULLYING, DEFENDING, AND STATUS
as a magnifying lens hypothesis; Caravita and Cil-
Recent literature on peer-group dynamics and net- lessen, 2012]. For instance, being perceived popular
works recognizes the existence of two distinct forms may promote the use of one’s own social cognitive
of social status reflecting the child’s social position skills to bully others in order to maintain one’s sta-
among peers, which is social preference and perceived tus, strengthening the associations between skills and
popularity. Social preference reflects the extent to the behavior. Consistently, in early adolescence good
which a child is accepted or rejected by peers, whereas understanding of others’ feelings is positively associ-
perceived popularity reflects the level of individual ated to bullying for perceived popular girls [Caravita
visibility, power, and prominence among peers [Cil- et al., 2009]. Albeit still limited, these findings can
lessen, 2009], and it is associated to higher levels of suggest that the status acts as a magnifying lens for
dominance and resource control in the peer-group the relation between other personal characteristics—
[e.g., Caravita et al., 2011]. Even though an overlap including moral processes—and bullying especially
exists between social preference and perceived pop- during early adolescence, the age in which the sta-
ularity, they are usually oppositely related to peer tus in the peer groups becomes a priority [LaFontana
aggression, including bullying. Indeed, bullying is as- and Cillessen, 2010].
sociated negatively with social preference and posi- With reference to social preference, being socially
tively with perceived popularity, whereas defending preferred is likely to strengthen the associations be-
is positively associated to both social preference and tween defending behavior and its correlates (again,
perceived popularity [Caravita et al., 2009; de Bruyn the status as a magnifying lens hypothesis) because, on
et al., 2010; Pöyhönen et al., 2010]. the one hand, well-liked peers wish to maintain peer
Based on these data and studies that provided evi- acceptance by behaving prosocially and, on the other
dence of concurrent and prospective bidirectionality hand, they may feel they have their peers’ support
of the associations between forms of status and so- when they stand up for the victimized peer against bul-
cial behavior [e.g., Cillessen and Mayeux, 2004], re- lies [Juvonen and Galvan, 2008]. Accordingly, a high
cent studies suggested that having high status among social preference status has been found to strengthen
peers, either as socially preferred or perceived popu- the positive associations between empathy and de-
lar, promotes the kinds of behavior likely to keep the fending among children and early adolescents [Car-
influential position within the peer-group and the as- avita et al., 2009; Pöyhönen et al., 2010], and between
sociated rewards: the so-called status as a motivator theory of mind and defending in late childhood [Car-
hypothesis [Caravita and Cillessen, 2012]. In particu- avita et al., 2010]. In other words, only when young-
lar, high-perceived popularity, often gained by means sters are well liked by peers, empathic feelings, and
of bullying, is likely to motivate to continue to bully. good theory of mind skills are positively associated to
In contrast, a high status as socially preferred may defending.
inhibit bullying and promote defending. Indeed, chil- To our knowledge no studies, so far, have investi-
dren who are well accepted by peers are likely to wish gated whether the individual status within the peer-
to continue to be well liked, thus they should refrain group moderates the associations between the indi-
from bullying others, because this behavior is disliked vidual morality and social behaviors, in particular

Aggr. Behav.
4 Caravita et al.

bullying and defending. In fact, studies on moral- much as half [Caravita and Cillessen, 2012]. The pos-
ity related to bullying, for instance research by Gini itive association between social preference and de-
[2006] and Gasser and Keller [2009], have mainly ex- fending found in childhood remains through early
plored the associations between either moral disen- adolescence, while defending the victims is linked to
gagement or the acceptance of moral transgression a high-perceived popular status among children but
and bullying, without examining possible influences not adolescents [Caravita et al., 2009].
by peer-group processes and, in particular, by sta-
tus among peers. However, a high status as perceived
THE PRESENT STUDY
popular is likely to strengthen the associations be-
tween bullying and acceptance of moral transgres- In sum, the main aim of this study was to test main
sion and moral disengagement, whereas social pref- effects of status on behaviors in bullying and moder-
erence might particularly strengthen the associations ation effects of status on the associations between the
between defending and lower levels of both kinds of morality dimensions and bullying and defending. As
distortion in morality. In other terms, we expect to discussed above, studies have consistently showed that
confirm the validity of the status as a magnifying lens bullying is related to higher levels of perceived popu-
hypothesis also in the context of the morality-bullying larity and lower levels of social preference, whereas de-
connection. fending is related positively to social preference and,
at least in childhood, to perceived popularity [Car-
avita et al., 2009]. Moreover, literature has provided
AGE AND GENDER DIFFERENCES
some evidence that, compared to other peers, particu-
Literature on forms of participation in bullying has larly defenders, bullies may perceive moral transgres-
consistently found higher levels of bullying among sions as more acceptable (at least at younger ages) and
boys and older students [e.g., Salmivalli et al., 1996], show higher levels of moral disengagement. Based
compared to girls and younger students who, con- on these findings and the status as a motivator and
versely, tend to show higher levels of defending the the status as a magnifying lens hypotheses described
victim [e.g., Pozzoli and Gini, 2010, 2012]. Research above [Caravita and Cillessen, 2012], perceived pop-
findings on moral disengagement, on the other hand, ularity was expected to be positively associated with
are less clear-cut. For example, Paciello et al. [Paciello bullying, and to strengthen the positive associations
et al., 2008] have found that, in adolescence, boys tend of bullying with accepting moral transgression and
to endorse higher levels of moral disengagement than morally disengaging, especially in early adolescence,
girls do, and that overall moral disengagement de- when having a high perceived popularity status be-
creases with increasing age, but remains highly sta- comes a priority [LaFontana and Cillessen, 2010].
ble in aggressive youths. With regards to perception In contrast, a high social preference status was ex-
of rules, children perceive both moral, and social- pected (1) to be negatively associated with bullying
conventional rules as more highly authority depen- and to reduce the positive associations between bul-
dent and breakable than do early adolescents [Car- lying and accepting moral transgression and morally
avita et al., 2009]. disengaging, and (2) to be positively associated with
From late childhood to early adolescence, social defending and to strengthen the negative associations
preference, and perceived popularity become more between morality dimensions and defending. Indeed,
distinct. Among boys the association between these well-liked kids are likely to wish to maintain their well-
forms of status declines with age but remains positive, liked status, by withdrawing from bullying, which is
whereas among girls it shifts from positive to non- usually disliked by peers, and by defending the vic-
significant or even negative in adolescence [Hymel tims, which is rewarded with higher peer-acceptance.
et al., 2010]. After the transition between primary As a secondary aim of this study, we also investi-
school and middle school, having high-perceived pop- gated the associations of acceptance of moral trans-
ularity becomes a priority over other valued domains, gression and moral disengagement with bullying and
such as friendship and academic achievement [La- defending by including these dimensions together in
Fontana and Cillessen, 2010]. Such priority attached same models. In fact, no studies so far have analyzed
to popularity further predicts aggression in early ado- these two moral dimensions by taking into accounts
lescence [Cillessen et al., 2009]. Accordingly, from their reciprocal effects on forms of participation in
late childhood to early adolescence the positive as- bullying. Based on previous findings, both accept-
sociation between perceived popularity and bullying ing moral transgression and morally disengaging were
almost doubles, whereas the negative association be- expected to be associated to higher levels of bullying
tween social preference and bullying decreases by as and lower levels of defending.

Aggr. Behav.
Morality and Bullying: Moderations by Status 5

We tested for possible gender differences in the ex- ropean Countries, .9% from Africa, .6% from South
pected associations between morality, status, and be- America and .2% from Asia. School principals and
havior, by including the interactions of gender with teachers’ committees authorized the participation of
the two types of moral cognition and the two forms the classes in the study, and before the data collection
of social status in the analyses. In keeping with some a letter explaining the purposes of the research project
studies that reported a stronger relation between so- was sent to participants’ parents to ask for active con-
cial status and bullying behavior among boys than sent. Parents of all the pupils in the school grades were
among girls [e.g., Caravita and Cillessen, 2012], we contacted for consent and 85 pupils (13.7% of the
anticipated that boys would show greater associa- whole sample) were not allowed to participate in this
tions between status (especially perceived popularity), study. Nonauthorized pupils did not answer question-
moral dimensions and bullying. Conversely, we hy- naires but could be nominated in peer-nomination
pothesized a stronger effect of social preference on measures. Pupils without permission did not signif-
the relation between morality and defending among icantly differ from the participants on bullying, but
girls. showed significantly lower levels of defending behav-
Finally, possible age-related differences were inves- ior (t(145.51) = 6.81, P < .001), perceived popularity
tigated by testing the associations of the study vari- (t(107.66) = 3.92, P < .001), and social preference
ables separately in two age groups: a late childhood (t(110.37) = 5.64, P < .001). Before data collection,
group and an early adolescence group. To this respect, individual consent for participation was also obtained
in keeping with literature on status [Hymel et al., from the participants, who were informed that partic-
2010], we hypothesized that, compared to late child- ipation in the study was voluntary and were allowed
hood, during early adolescence the perceived popular both to refuse to participate and to withdraw from the
status was more relevant than social preference in study at any time. None of the participants refused to
favoring bullying behavior and that its role as a mag- participate.
nifying lens was also more evident at this age level
[Caravita and Cillessen, 2012]. As far as morality is
Measures
concerned, we expected that accepting moral trans-
gression was related to bullying especially among Peer nominations of bullying and defending
younger participants, whereas moral disengagement behavior. Forms of participation in bullying were
might be associated to bullying more strongly among measured by administering the Italian validated ver-
adolescents, who may be more able to use sophis- sion of the Participant Roles Questionnaire [PRQ;
ticated processes of self-justification of moral trans- Menesini and Gini, 2000; Salmivalli et al., 1996] based
gression. on the shortened version for primary school children
by Sutton and Smith [1999]. The whole measure con-
METHOD sists of 21 items, i.e. behavioral descriptions of bully-
ing situations, of which four items measure bullying
Participants
(e.g., “starts bullying”; Cronbach’s α = .94; original
Participants were 235 children, aged 9–11 years (av- α = .94) and five items assess defending (e.g., “stick
erage age = 9.8 years, SD = .66, 51.9% boys), attend- up for the victim”; α = .82; original α = .81). First, we
ing grades four and five, i.e. the last two grades of provided a definition of bullying, which makes clear
the Italian primary school, and 305 early adolescents, to the child the specificity of bullying in compari-
aged 12–15 years (average age: 12.48 years, SD = 1.00; son to other forms of aggression: “We say a child or
54.4% boys), attending grades six to eight, i.e. the young person is being bullied, or picked on when an-
three grades of middle school in the Italian school other child or young person, or a group of children or
system. Participants were recruited from two primary young people, say nasty and unpleasant things to him
schools and one middle school in the Province of Mi- or her. It is also bullying when a child or young per-
lan, in Northern Italy. Socio-economic status (SES) son is hit, kicked, threatened, locked inside a room,
of participants’ families was assessed by asking stu- sent nasty notes, when no one ever talks to them and
dents to report their parents’ qualifications and jobs. things like that. These things can happen frequently
Seventy-two youngsters (13.3%) were not able to pro- and it is difficult for the child or young person being
vide this information and their SES data were missing. bullied to defend himself or herself. It is also bully-
Of the remaining 468 participants’ families, 18.3% ing when a child or young person is teased repeatedly
had low-middle SES, 47.4% middle SES, and 20.9% in a nasty way. But it is not bullying when two chil-
middle-high SES. The majority of participants were dren or young people of about the same strength have
Italian (95.2%), 3.1% were immigrants from other Eu- the odd fight or quarrel.” Then, for each item the

Aggr. Behav.
6 Caravita et al.

participants were asked to nominate up to five class- popular kids [Cillessen and Mayeux, 2004]. Accord-
mates who fitted each behavioral description and, for ing to procedure proposed in sociometric literature
each nominated classmate, to indicate whether s/he [Cillessen, 2009], after summing the peer-nominations
“sometimes” (scored as 1) or “often” (scored as 2) for each item and standardizing them by class, two
showed that behavior. The weighted sum of the re- combined indices were computed: nominations as the
ceived peer-nominations for each item formed the least liked were subtracted from nominations as the
score, which was standardized by class, and these most liked to assess social preference; nominations as
scores were then averaged to form bullying and de- the least popular were subtracted from nominations
fending scores. as the most popular to assess perceived popularity.
Moral disengagement. The tendency to The final scores for social preference and perceived
morally disengage was assessed by means of the popularity were the two subtraction indices after stan-
14-item version of the Moral Disengagement scale, dardization by class.
a self-report measure specifically designed and
Procedure
validated for school children [Caprara et al., 1995].
For each item, respondents were asked to rate the The research project was approved by the Ethical
strength of their endorsement or rejection of moral Committee of the Department of Psychology of the
exoneration of detrimental conduct on a 5-point Catholic University of the Sacred Heart. Measures
scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly were group-administered in one session per class by
agree). Higher scores indicate a higher tendency a research assistant during regular school hours. The
to engage in one or more of these mechanisms administration session took around 1 hr per class,
(example items: “Some kids deserve to be treated like and the order of the questionnaires was counterbal-
animals”; “Kids cannot be blamed for misbehaving anced across classes to control for the order effect,
if their friends pressured them to do it”). In line with so that each questionnaire was at least once in any
previous studies [e.g., Caprara et al., 1995; Gini, rank position in the administering order. For each
2006] items were averaged to form a global score measure the research assistant explained how to fill
(α = .73). in the questionnaire. For the peer-nomination mea-
Acceptance of moral transgression. Chil- sures, the class roster was written on the classroom
dren’s acceptance of transgression of moral rules was blackboard with names associated to anonymous nu-
investigated by administering a self-report question- meric codes and the participants were instructed to
naire based on the interview schedule by Buchanan- use these codes in place of classmates’ names to an-
Barrow and Barrett [1998; Caravita et al., 2009]. swer questions. At the end of the session, children were
The measure included two hypothetical scenarios, in thanked for their participation and debriefed about
which moral rules, i.e. do not hurt other kids and do the purpose of the study, and their questions were
not steal others’ properties, were transgressed by a answered.
child. The rules were presented as explicit rules of the
school, so that the character breaking the rule was RESULTS
aware of his/her rule-transgression. A sample sce-
Missing Data
nario is the following: “In Davide’s school, there is a
rule that you must never push anyone off the climbing Missing data were handled by first verifying that
frame. One day at school, while Davide is playing on they were random in nature, and that there was not
the climbing frame, he pushes Stefano so hard that he a systematic pattern of nonresponse [Meyers et al.,
falls to the ground.” 2006]. Then, we considered the questionnaire to be
For each scenario respondents judged how much reliable if the participants answered at least 80% of
the rule breaking was right on a 5-point scale (from the questions and if they had less than 20% miss-
0 “not right at all,” to 4 “totally right”). The two ing data in one or more of the scales used. Follow-
item scores (r = .35) were averaged (range: 0–4) in ing these criteria, 32 students were excluded from
order to obtain the acceptance of moral transgression the sample and were not included in subsequent
score. The higher the score, the more acceptable the analyses.
transgression of the moral rule.
Correlations and Group Differences
Social preference and perceived popularity.
Four peer-nomination items assessed social status. Correlations were computed in the two age groups,
Participants nominated up to five same- and cross- separately (Table I). In both age-groups bullying was
gender classmates they liked the most, they liked the higher for boys than girls, whereas defending was
least, and they thought were the most and the least higher for girls than boys. In adolescence boys scored

Aggr. Behav.
Morality and Bullying: Moderations by Status 7

TABLE I. Descriptive Statistics and Correlations Among the Study Variables


Measure 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 M SD

1. Gender (0 = boys, 1 = girls) – − .35*** .37*** − .09 − .26*** .09 − .09 - -


2. Bullying − .32*** - − .26*** .18** .30*** − .24*** .43*** .44 .78
3. Defending .45*** − .22** – − .14* − .16** .29*** .06 .73 .65
4. Acceptance of moral transgression .00 .01 .06 – .23** .01 .04 .89 .75
5. Moral disengagement − .12 .09 − .21** .14* − − .08 .14* 2.28 .57
6. Social preference .05 − .30*** .38*** .11 − .16* − .39*** .002 .97
7. Perceived popularity − .20** .23*** .19** .11 − .10 .56*** - − .02 .95
M – .50 1.04 .57 2.33 .04 .01
SD – .73 .80 .59 .55 .95 .98
Note. Correlations for primary school children (n = 222) are presented below the diagonal, and correlations for middle school early adolescents
(n = 286) are presented above the diagonal.
* P < .05, ** P < .01, *** P < .001.

higher than girls in moral disengagement. Among pared with early adolescents (M = .73; t(506) = 4.92,
children, boys were more perceived popular than girls. P < .001, d = .43, 95% CI [.25, .61]).
At both age levels, defending was significantly asso-
ciated to lower levels of moral disengagement and, in Analyses Predicting Bullying
early adolescence, to lower acceptance of moral trans- and Defending Behavior
gression. Bullying was associated with higher scores Even if intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC)
in both moral dimensions, but only in early adoles- for bullying (late-childhood: .006; early adolescence:
cence. Moreover, bullying positively correlated with .003) and defending (late-childhood: .019; early ado-
perceived popularity and this correlation was higher lescence: .081) were low, to control for the possi-
among early adolescents than among children. The ble clustering effects of classrooms (students nested
correlation between bullying and social preference within classes) we utilized the Complex method pro-
was negative in both age groups, but higher among vided by the Mplus software (Muthèn and Muthèn,
children. Defending was positively associated to both 1998–2007).
status dimensions in children, but only to social pref- Hypothesized associations among the study vari-
erence in early adolescents. In adolescence perceived ables were investigated through regression analyses
popularity was associated to higher levels of moral with bullying and defending behavior as the out-
disengagement, and in both age groups of social pref- comes. Separate analyses were conducted for the two
erence too, even if this association was higher among age groups. For each analysis, two models were tested.
children than early adolescents. In model 1 (main effect model), gender (dummy
Age and gender differences were tested using t- coded: 0 = boys, 1 = girls) was entered together
tests. Effect sizes are expressed as Cohen’s d, based with acceptance of moral transgression, moral dis-
on the pooled standard deviation of the two groups. engagement, social preference, and perceived popu-
Results showed that in the overall sample boys larity. Moreover, the defending score was included as
scored higher than girls in bullying (Mgirls = .20, a predictor of bullying and the bullying score in the
SDgirls = .33; Mboys = .71, SD boys = .93; t(506) = 7.94, prediction of defending, in order to control for their
P < .001, d = .72, 95% CI [.54, .90]), moral disen- reciprocal influence. Then, to test for moderation ef-
gagement (Mgirls = 2.19, SDgirls = .54; Mboys = 2.41, fects, in model 2 (interaction model) we entered the
SD boys = .55; t(506) = 4.49, P < .001, d = .40, 95% four 2-way interaction terms of perceived popular-
CI [.23, .58]) and perceived popularity (Mgirls = −.15, ity and social preference with moral disengagement
SDgirls = .93; Mboys = .12, SD boys = .97; t(506) = 3.15, and acceptance of moral transgression, as well as the
P = .002, d = .28, 95% CI [.11, .46]). In contrast, girls 2-way interactions of moral and status dimensions
reported greater defending behavior than boys did with gender and the four 3-way interactions of moral
(Mgirls = 1.18, SDgirls = .84; Mboys = .58, SD boys = .49; constructs with status dimensions and gender. Inter-
t(506) = 7.94, P < .001, d = .88, 95% CI [.70, 1.07]). action terms [Aiken and West, 1991] were created by
As far as age differences are concerned, older students mean centering predictors. The improvement of fit be-
(M = .89) showed greater acceptance of moral trans- tween the first and the second model was evaluated
gression than younger pupils (M = .57; t(506) = 5.29, through the log-likelihood ratio test (based on the χ2
P < .001, d = .47, 95% CI [.30, .65]), while children distribution), by comparing the log-likelihood value
(M = 1.04) scored higher in defending behavior com- of the interaction model with the log-likelihood value

Aggr. Behav.
8 Caravita et al.

TABLE II. Hierarchical Regression Analyses on Bullying and Defending Behavior


Bullying behavior Defending behavior

Late childhood Early adolescence Late-childhood Early adolescence

Predictor R2 β R2 β R2 β R2 β

Step 1 .363*** .476*** .348*** .233***


Gender (0 = boys, 1 = girls) − .18*** − .23*** .47*** .32***
Defending/bullying − .01 − .05 − .01 − .08
Acceptance of moral transgression .01 .02 .05 − .10*
Moral disengagement (MD) .03 .12*** .02 − .04
Social preference (SP) − .58*** − .53*** .26*** .22***
Perceived popularity (PP) .53*** .71*** 05 . .04
Step 2 .077 .149*** .054* 041 .
SP × MD − .04 − .08 − .09 − .11
PP × MD .08 .24** − .04 − .02
SP × acceptance of moral transgression .06 − .21** − .09 .09
PP × acceptance of moral transgression .06 .17 .10 .12*
Gender × MD − .05 − .02 − .17* .05
Gender × acceptance of moral transgression − .09 .00 − .09 − .05
Gender × SP .46*** .25*** − .04 .19*
Gender × PP − .30* − .32*** .17 .02
Gender × MD × SP .08 − .01 .00 .16
Gender × MD × PP − .08 − .06 − .02 .01
Gender × acceptance of moral transgression × SP − .01 .16*** .09 − .12
Gender × acceptance of moral transgression × PP .01 − .14* − .10 − .05
Total R2 .440*** .625*** .402*** .274***
N 222 286 222 286
*P < .05, ** P < .01, *** P < .001.

of the main effect model, including predictions from dicting changes in the variance of bullying. With re-
the interaction terms fixed to 0. gards to the acceptance of the moral transgression,
Bullying behavior. Results are reported in both its 2-way interaction with social preference and
Table II. In the late-childhood sample the interaction its 3-way interactions with status dimensions and gen-
terms entered in model 2 did not fit the data better der were significantly associated to bullying.
than model 1 (log-likelihood ratio test(12) = 20.17, P In order to interpret the nature of these significant
> .05). So, model 1 was retained as the final model. interactions, simple slopes of acceptance of moral
Predictors entered in model 1 explained 36.3% of the transgression and moral disengagement on bullying
variance in children’s bullying behavior. Bullying was were derived for high (+1 SD) and low levels (−1 SD)
significantly higher among boys than girls, negatively of social preference and perceived popularity as the
associated to social preference and positively to per- moderating variables [Aiken and West, 1991]. For in-
ceived popularity. Neither of the moral components teractions including gender, models were run again,
had significant main associations with bullying in late separately in the two gender groups and excluding
childhood. gender and its interactions from predictors. Figure 1
Among early adolescents, model 1 explained 47.6% depicts the interaction between perceived popularity
of variation in bullying, but model 2 explained a sig- and moral disengagement. The computation of the
nificantly higher variation (62.5%) of this behavior simple slopes showed that higher levels of moral dis-
(log-likelihood ratio test(12) = 67.23, P < .001). There- engagement were significantly and positively related
fore model 2 was retained as the final model. Similarly to bullying behavior at high levels of perceived popu-
to what emerged among children, bullying was higher larity (β = .30, P < .001), but it was not significantly
among boys. Higher levels of perceived popularity associated to bullying when the perceived popularity
and lower levels of social preference were associated was low (β = −.08, P = .33).
with higher levels of bullying behavior. The interac- Because the 2-way interactions of the acceptance of
tion terms of the status dimensions with gender were moral transgression with social status in predicting
both significant. Moreover, in this age-sample, moral changes in the variance of bullying were included in
disengagement was significantly associated to bully- the significant 3-way interactions of acceptance with
ing, and interacted with perceived popularity in pre- status and gender, we separately tested in the two

Aggr. Behav.
Morality and Bullying: Moderations by Status 9

Fig. 1. The perceived popularity x moral disengagement effect on change


in the variance of bullying behavior among early adolescents.

gender-groups the regression model that included all


the 2-ways interactions after removing gender and the
interaction terms including gender from predictors.
The acceptance of moral transgression significantly
interacted with social preference (β = −.16, P < .01;
Fig. 2) and perceived popularity (β = .15, P < .05;

Fig. 3. The gender x perceived popularity x acceptance of moral trans-


gression effect on change in the variance of bullying behavior among
early adolescents for boys (A) and girls (B).

Fig. 3) in the prediction of bullying among boys,


whereas these associations were nonsignificant among
girls (βs .10 and −.10, ps .34 and .37, respectively).
In the boys group, higher levels of acceptance of
moral transgression were significantly associated to
bullying for low levels of social preference (β = .16,
P < .05), whereas the association was not significant
for high levels of social preference (β = −.07, P < .33).
When considering perceived popularity as the mod-
erator, the acceptance of moral transgression signifi-
cantly predicted variance in bullying for high levels of
perceived popularity (β = .17, P = .05), but not for
low levels of this form of status (β = −.05, P = .57).
Lastly, gender moderated the associations of bully-
ing with status dimensions: perceived popularity was
more strongly associated to bullying among boys (.63,
P < .001) than girls (.57, P < .001), whereas the
negative association of social preference and bully-
ing was stronger among girls (−.51, P < .001) than
Fig. 2. The gender x social preference x acceptance of moral transgres- boys (−.43, P < .001).
sion effect on change in the variance of bullying behavior among early Defending behavior. The results of the analy-
adolescents for boys (A) and girls (B). ses on defending behavior are displayed in Table II.

Aggr. Behav.
10 Caravita et al.

gagement processes, were associated positively to bul-


lying and negatively to defending. Furthermore, the
main aim of the study was to test two hypotheses
on the role of status that have been recently formu-
lated [Caravita and Cillessen, 2012]: the “status as a
motivator” and the “status as a magnifying lens” hy-
potheses. To this end, we investigated whether forms
of status among peers, namely social preference and
perceived popularity, both were directly associated
to outcome behaviors, above and beyond moral di-
mensions, and moderated the associations between
moral cognition and bullying and defending. Finally,
the potential moderating role of gender was analyzed
by entering the interaction terms, whereas age-related
Fig. 4. The gender x moral disengagement effect on change in the vari- differences in the investigated associations were ex-
ance of defending behavior among children. amined by performing separate analyses for the two
age groups.
In childhood, model 1 predicted 34.8% of variation Morality, Status, and Behavior in Bullying
in defending, and model 2 40.2%. The difference in
the amount of variation explained by the two models Among the study variables high status dimensions
was significant (log-likelihood ratio test(12) = 21.64, had the strongest associations with the behavior, espe-
P < .05), thus model 2 was retained as the final model. cially with bullying. Consistently with previous stud-
Gender was associated with different amounts of de- ies [e.g., Caravita et al., 2009; de Bruyn et al., 2010], in
fending behavior, such that girls were more prone to both age-groups lower levels of social preference and
defend the victims than boys were. Social preference higher levels of perceived popularity (i.e., high pres-
was positively related with this behavior. Neither the tige status) in the peer group were associated with
acceptance of moral transgression nor the interac- bullying others, whereas defending the victims was
tions terms including acceptance were significantly linked to high social preference. These findings fit
associated to defending. Moral disengagement signif- with the status as a motivator hypothesis [Caravita
icantly and negatively interacted with gender in pre- and Cillessen, 2012], that states that a high status as
dicting variation in defending. When the model was perceived popular is likely to be associated with in-
separately tested in gender-groups, moral disengage- creases in the variance of bullying, and a high status
ment was negatively associated to defending among as socially well liked with higher levels of prosocial be-
girls (β = −.22, P < .05), but the association was low havior. Interesting enough, this hypothesis was con-
and not significant among boys (β = −.04, P = .69) firmed in both age groups, thus suggesting that social
(Fig. 4). status may be an important determinant of differ-
Among early adolescents, model 1 explained 23.3% ent behaviors in bullying already in late childhood.
of variability of defending. Model 2 explained 27.4% In this perspective, popular children may be more
of variation, and it did not significantly differ from likely to bully others because bullying is rewarded
model 1 (log-likelihood ratio test(12) = 16.69, P > .05). with keeping high-perceived popular status [Cillessen
Therefore model 1 was retained as the final model. and Mayeux, 2004; Hymel et al., 2010]. Differently
Defending was significantly associated to being a from perceived popular kids, well-accepted students
girl, low levels of acceptance of moral transgression (i.e., students high in social preference) may be moti-
(β = −.10, P < .05) and higher social preference vated to avoid acting aggressively and, conversely, to
(β = .22, P < .001). behave prosocially because prosocial behavior is usu-
ally rewarded by higher levels of peer acceptance (the
status as motivator hypothesis). This is also true for
DISCUSSION
defending behavior in bullying, which is usually well
This study was the first to analyze the associations approved by peers [Salmivalli and Voeten, 2004].
of morality and status in the peer group with bully- However, when considering the specificity of de-
ing and defending in late childhood and early ado- fending, our data are also consistent with the hy-
lescence. Specifically, we tested whether two differ- pothesis that children that feel they are supported
ent forms of dysfunctional moral cognition, namely by their peers may be encouraged to actively defend
acceptance of moral transgression and moral disen- victimized peers [Juvonen and Galvan, 2008]. Indeed,

Aggr. Behav.
Morality and Bullying: Moderations by Status 11

when compared to other types of prosocial behav- bullying among adolescent boys. Also these findings
ior, active intervention in favor of the victim in the are in accordance with the magnifying lens hypothe-
context of bullying represents a risky behavior, be- sis, stating that higher levels of social preference can
cause the helper must confront a powerful bully and, be a protective factor for antisocial behavior, even
often, a supporting group. Being well liked by peers buffering the effects of potential risk factors such as
may, therefore, increase the defenders’ perception that perceiving moral rules as breakable. Well-liked chil-
peers will help them in contrasting bullies. In further dren may avoid engaging in disruptive behavior in
accordance with this hypothesis, defending behavior order to maintain their likeability status among peers.
has been shown to be promoted by class norms in fa-
Effects of Gender and Age
vor of this behavior, i.e. classmates’ positive attitudes
toward defending [Pozzoli et al., in press]. Thus, de- While most findings were consistent across gender
fending behavior is likely to be motivated by both per- and age groups, a few relations were moderated by
sonal characteristics and group conditions, including gender and age. First, gender moderated the associa-
a high status as socially accepted [Caravita et al., 2009; tions of status with bullying, in that perceived popu-
Pozzoli and Gini, 2010; Pozzoli et al., in press]. larity was more strongly associated to bullying among
As far as morality is concerned, consistent with boys than girls, whereas the negative association of so-
our hypotheses based on the status as a magnifying cial preference and bullying was stronger among girls
lens hypothesis, in early adolescence the tendencies to than boys. Second, after taking into account the ef-
morally disengage and to accept the moral transgres- fects of social status and in keeping with previous find-
sion interacted with status dimensions in explaining ings [e.g., Hymel et al., 2005; Menesini et al., 2003],
bullying behavior. Specifically, perceived popularity higher levels of moral disengagement were associated
exacerbated the link of bullying with moral disen- to and interacted with perceived popularity in pre-
gagement in both gender groups, so that bullying be- dicting higher levels of bullying only among early
havior was more likely in students who were more adolescents, thus suggesting that these mechanisms
prone to morally disengage and high in perceived of self-justification may become particularly impor-
popularity. The moderating role of perceived popu- tant in explaining aggressive behavior at this age [see
larity was also confirmed for the association between also Paciello et al., 2008]. Third, again only in adoles-
acceptance of moral transgression and bullying, but cence acceptance of moral transgression was related
only among boys. Altogether, these findings expand to boys’ bullying and to defending. These results may
the results of recent studies that provided evidence indicate that moral cognition becomes important in
that high perceived popular status not only promotes explaining social behavior mainly from the early ado-
bullying directly but also strengthens the association lescence years. Fourth, consistent with previous stud-
between personal characteristics, such as social cog- ies reporting stronger effects of status in the male
nition, and bullying among early adolescents [Car- group [Caravita and Cillessen, 2012], the moderating
avita et al., 2009], the age group in which perceived role of perceived popularity and social preference on
popularity becomes a priority over other valued do- the association between acceptance of moral trans-
mains [LaFontana and Cillessen, 2010]. Furthermore, gression and bullying emerged only among adolescent
this finding is consistent with the hypothesis that a boys. These results are consistent with the hypothesis
high status may act as a magnifying lens for status- that high perceived popular status not only promotes
enhancing behaviors, such as bullying [Caravita and bullying directly, but also strengthens the relations be-
Cillessen, 2012]. Finally, when the effects of moral tween individual characteristics and bullying among
disengagement and status were taken into account, early adolescents, the age group in which perceived
acceptance of moral transgression was not directly popularity becomes a priority [Hymel et al., 2010;
related to bullying. These findings may also indicate LaFontana and Cillessen, 2010].
that even once bullies understand that there are moral With regards to defending behavior, lower levels
issues involved, this still does not guarantee that they of acceptance of moral transgression were associated
will care about these moral issues. to higher defending among early adolescents but not
As far as social preference is concerned, its mod- children. Moreover, only among girls a lower ten-
eration effects indicated that, among early adolescent dency to morally disengage was linked to higher lev-
boys (but not girls), accepting moral transgression els of defending behavior in late childhood. This is
was related to bullying behavior only for boys and consistent with the picture of girls as both less morally
who were not well liked by peers. In other terms, disengaged [Paciello et al., 2008] and more prone
having a high social preference status weakens the to defending than boys [Pozzoli and Gini, 2010].
associations of possible distortions in morality with Furthermore, these findings indicate the value of

Aggr. Behav.
12 Caravita et al.

investigating the relation between moral disengage- REFERENCES


ment and different forms of participation in the phe-
Aiken LS, West SG. 1991. Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting
nomenon, not only bullying, and to expand the anal- interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
ysis to younger children. Arsenio WF, Lemerise EA. 2001. Varieties of childhood bullying:
Values, emotion processes, and social competence. Soc Dev 10:59–
Limitations and Implications 73.
Bandura A. 1991. Social cognitive theory of moral thought and ac-
Limitations of this study included the cross- tion. In: Kurtines WM, Gewirtz GL, editors. Handbook of moral
sectional nature of the data, which did not allow us to behavior and development: theory, research and applications. Vol.
clarify the causal direction of associations, and to ex- 1. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. p 71–129.
clude alternative explanations for our findings. In par- Barchia K, Bussey K. 2011. Individual and collective social cogni-
tive influences on peer aggression: Exploring the contribution of
ticular, we cannot exclude that levels of social status aggression efficacy, moral disengagement, and collective efficacy.
may be the outcome and not the precursors of social Aggr Behav 37:107–120.
behavior, even though this latter hypothesis is consis- Buchanan-Barrow E, Barrett M. 1998. Individual differences
tent with longitudinal studies on this topic [Cillessen, in children’s understanding of the school. Soc Dev 7:250–
2009]. Furthermore, we assessed differences in moral 268.
Caprara GV, Pastorelli C, Bandura A. 1995. La misura del disim-
rule comprehension only through two scenarios and pegno morale in età evolutiva [Measuring age differences in moral
by considering just the acceptance of moral transgres- disengagement]. Età Evolutiva 51:18–29.
sion, while other dimensions, such as the perception Caravita SCS, Cillessen AHN. 2012. Agentic or communal? Develop-
of the rule as dependent on the authority, may also as- mental differences in the associations among interpersonal goals,
sess the level of and maturity in perceiving moral rules. popularity, and bullying. Soc Dev 2:376–395.
Caravita SCS, Di Blasio P, Salmivalli C. 2009. Unique and interactive
A replication of these findings with a more thorough effects of empathy and social status on involvement in bullying.
assessment of these dimensions of children’s morality Soc Dev 18:140–163.
is required. Caravita SCS, Di Blasio P, Salmivalli C. 2010. Early adolescents’
Notwithstanding these limitations, the current find- participation in bullying: Is ToM involved? J Early Adolesc 30:138–
ings have potential implications for interventions. For 170.
Caravita SCS, Miragoli S, Di Blasio P. 2009. Why should I behave
example, they indicate that dysfunctions in moral cog- in this way? Rule discrimination within the school context related
nition are important issues for bullying prevention in to bullying. In: Elling LR, editor. Social development. New York:
adolescence, and that educational programs must fo- Nova Science Publishers. p 269–290.
cus on mitigating these distortions in kids to decrease Caravita SCS, Pöyhönen V, Rajala I, Salmivalli C. 2011. The archi-
the likelihood of bullying behavior. Moreover, they tecture of high status among Finnish youth. Br J Dev Psychol
29:668–679.
evidence the importance of taking into consideration Caravita SCS, Pöyhönen V, Salmivalli C. 2009. Prosocial involvement
the role of the peer group in bullying and indicate that in antisocial situations: Defending the victims of bullying. In: Kin-
anti-bullying intervention should address not only the ney TA, Porhola M, editors. Anti- and pro-social communication:
individuals involved, but also the dynamics within Theories, methods, and applications. New York: Peter Lang Pub-
their peer group. Given the strong association between lishing Group. p 173–182.
Chang L. 2004. The role of classroom norms in contextualizing the
motivation to keep a prominent status within the peer relations of children’s social behaviors to peer acceptance. Dev
group and bullying behavior [Caravita and Cillessen, Psychol 40:691–702.
2012; Sijtsema et al., 2009], interventions should work Cillessen AHN. 2009. Sociometric methods. In: Rubin KH, Bukowski
to decreasing the social rewards of bullying others, es- WM, Laursen B, editors. Handbook of peer interactions, relation-
pecially in terms of a high popularity status. On the ships and groups. New York: Guilford. p 82–99.
Cillessen AHN, de Bruyn EH, LaFontana KM. 2009. Behav-
other hand, defenders are highly accepted by peers ioral effects of prioritizing popularity. Paper presented
and, in other studies, have been found to be both well at the Biennal Meeting of the Society for Research in
liked and perceived popular [Caravita et al., 2009]. As Child Development, Denver, CO. Available online at:
a consequence defenders may become positive mod- http://www.srcd.org/meeting/schedule/2009/dayview.cfm?code=3.
els for their classmates, and interventions focusing on Cillessen AHN, Mayeux L. 2004. From censure to reinforcement: De-
velopmental changes in the association between aggression and
proposing defending as a model behavior may be par- social status. Child Dev 75:147–163.
ticularly effective [e.g., Kärnä et al., 2010]. de Bruyn EH, Cillessen AHN, Wissink I. 2010. Associations of
peer acceptance and perceived popularity with bullying and
victimization in early adolescence. J Early Adolesc 30:543–
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
566.
The authors are grateful to the students, teachers, Estell DB, Farmer TM, Cairns BD. 2007. Bullies and victims in ru-
ral African American youth: Behavioral characteristics and social
and school administrators participating in this study,
network placement. Aggr Behav 33:145–159.
and to Mrs. Silvia Capraro for her help in collecting Fontaine NM, McCrory EJ, Boivin M, Moffitt TE, Viding
the data. E. 2011. Predictors and outcomes of joint trajectories of

Aggr. Behav.
Morality and Bullying: Moderations by Status 13

callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. alla popolazione italiana [Bullying as a group process: adaptation
J Abnorm Psychol 120:730–742. and validation of the Participant Role Questionnaire to the Italian
Gasser L, Keller M. 2009. Are the competent the morally good? Per- population]. Età Evolutiva 66:18–32.
spective taking and moral motivation of children involved in bul- Menesini E, Sanchez V, Fonzi A, Ortega R, Costabile A, Lo Feudo G.
lying. Soc Dev 18:798–816. 2003. Moral emotions and bullying: A cross-national comparison
Gibbs JC. 2010. Moral development and reality: Beyond the theories of differences between bullies, victims and outsiders. Aggr Behav
of Kohlberg and Hoffman. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. 29:515–530.
Gini G. 2006. Social cognition and moral cognition in bullying: What’s Meyers LS, Gamst G, Guarino A. 2006. Applied multivariate research:
wrong? Aggr Behav 32:528–539. Design and interpretation. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publishers.
Gini G, Albiero P, Benelli B, Altoè G. 2007. Does empathy predict Obermann ML. 2011. Moral disengagement in self-reported and peer-
adolescents’ bullying and defending behavior? Aggr Behav 33:467– nominated school bullying. Aggr Behav 37:133–144.
476. Paciello M, Fida R, Tramontano C, Lupinetti C, Caprara GV. 2008.
Gini G, Albiero P, Benelli B, Altoè G. 2008. Determinants of adoles- Stability and change of moral disengagement and its impacts on
cents’ active defending and passive bystanding behavior in bullying. aggression and violence in late adolescence. Child Dev 79:1288–
J Adolesc 31:93–105. 1309.
Gini G, Pozzoli T, Hauser M. 2011. Bullies have enhanced moral Peeters M, Cillessen AHN, Scholte RHJ. 2010. Clueless or powerful?
competence to judge relative to victims, but lack moral compassion. Identifying subtypes of bullies in adolescence. J Youth Adolesc
Pers Indiv Differ 50:603–608. 39:1041–1052.
Hymel S, Closson LM, Caravita SCS, Vaillancourt T. 2010. Social Pellegrini AD, Bartini M, Brooks F. 1999. School bullies, vic-
status among peers: From sociometric attraction to peer accep- tims, and aggressive victims: Factors relating to group affiliation
tance to perceived popularity. In: Smith PK, Hart CH, editors. and victimization in early adolescence. J Educ Psychol 91:216–
Handbook of childhood social development. 2nd ed. Malden, MA: 224.
Wiley/Blackwell. p 375–392. Pöyhönen V, Juvonen J, Salmivalli C. 2010. What does it take to stand
Hymel S, Rocke Henderson N, Bonanno RA. 2005. Moral disengage- up for the victim of bullying? The interplay between personal and
ment: A framework for understanding bullying among adolescents. social factors. Merrill-Palmer Quart 56:143–163.
J Soc Sci 8:1–11. Pozzoli T, Gini G. 2010. Active defending and passive bystanding
Hymel S, Schonert-Reichl KA, Bonanno RA, Vaillancourt T, Rocke behavior in bullying: The role of personal characteristics and per-
Henderson N. 2010. Bullying and morality. Understanding how ceived peer pressure. J Abnorm Child Psychol 38:815–827.
good kids can behave badly. In: Jimerson SR, Swearer SM, Es- Pozzoli T, Gini G. 2012. Why do bystanders of bullying help or not? A
pelage DL, editors. Handbook of bullying in schools. An interna- multidimensional model. J Early Adolesc. Available online ahead
tional perspective. New York: Routledge. p 101–118. of print: DOI: 10.1177/0272431612440172
Juvonen J, Galvan A. 2008. Peer contagion in involuntary social Pozzoli T, Ang RP, Gini G. 2012. Bystanders’ reactions to bullying:
groups: Lessons from research on bullying. In: Prinstein M, Dodge A cross-cultural analysis of personal correlates among Italian and
K, editors. Peer influence processes among youth. New York: Guil- Singaporean students. Soc Dev. Available online ahead of print.
ford Press. p 225–244. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00651.x
Kärnä A, Voete M, Little T, Poskiparta E, Kaljonen A, Salmivalli C. Pozzoli T, Gini G, Vieno A. in press. The role of individual correlates
2010. A large-scale evaluation of the KiVa anti-bullying program. and class norms in defending and passive bystanding behavior in
Child Dev 32:311–330. bullying: A multilevel analysis. Child Dev.
Ladd GW. 2003. Probing the adaptive significance of children’s behav- Salmivalli C, Lagerspetz K, Björkqvist K, Österman K, Kaukiainen
ior and relationships in the school context: A child-by-environment A. 1996. Bullying as a group process: Participant roles and their
perspective. In: Kail R, editor. Advances in child behavior and de- relations to social status within the group. Aggr Behav 22: 1–15.
velopment. New York: Wiley. p 43–104. Salmivalli C, Voeten M. 2004. Connections between attitudes, group
Ladd GW. 2005. Children’s peer relations and social competence: A norms, and behaviors associated with bullying in schools. Int J
century of progress. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Behav Dev 28:246–258.
LaFontana KM, Cillessen AHN. 2010. Developmental changes in the Sijtsema J, Veenstra R, Lindenberg SS, Salmivalli C. 2009. Empirical
priority of perceived status in childhood and adolescence. Soc Dev test of bullies’ status goals: Assessing direct goals, aggression, and
19:130–147. prestige. Aggr Behav 34:1–11.
Leenders I, Brugman D. 2005. Moral/non-moral domain shift in Sutton J, Keogh E. 2000. Social competition in school: Relationships
young adolescents in relation to delinquent behavior. Br J Dev with bullying, machiavellianism, and personality. Br J Educ Psy-
Psychol 23:65–79. chol 70:443–456.
Menesini E, Camodeca M. 2008. Shame and guilt as behaviour reg- Sutton J, Smith PK. 1999. Bullying as a group process: An adap-
ulators: Relationships with bullying, victimization and prosocial tation of the participant role approach. Aggr Behav 25:97–
behaviour. Br J Dev Psychol 26:183–196. 111.
Menesini E, Gini G. 2000. Il bullismo come processo di gruppo: adat- Turiel E. 1983. The development of social knowledge: Morality and
tamento e validazione del questionario “Ruoli dei partecipanti” convention. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Aggr. Behav.

You might also like