You are on page 1of 9

Article

Social Psychological and


Personality Science
Friends With Moral Credentials: Minority 1-9
ª The Author(s) 2016
Reprints and permission:
Friendships Reduce Attributions of Racism sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1948550615624140
for Majority Group Members Who Make spps.sagepub.com

Conceivably Racist Statements

Michael Thai1, Matthew J. Hornsey1, and Fiona Kate Barlow1,2

Abstract
People commonly reference minority friendships when expressing conceivably prejudiced attitudes. The prevalence of this
strategy suggests a widespread belief that having minority friends makes one look less racist, but to date, there is little research
demonstrating whether or not this is the case. White and Asian participants were presented with a Facebook profile depicting a
White target who posted an anti-Asian statement. Being depicted with Asian friends (Study 1) or even verbally claiming that they
had Asian friends (Study 2) reduced attributions of racism irrespective of whether they were being evaluated by White or Asian
observers. Furthermore, the presence of Asian friends made the conceivably racist comments seem relatively benign, and
observers were less offended and upset by them. The data suggest that minority friendships can partially offset costs associated
with expressing prejudice.

Keywords
moral credentials, racism, attribution, person perception

People often use the disclaimer ‘‘some of my best friends are drawing inferences from what contextual information they are
X . . . ’’ when expressing conceivably prejudiced attitudes, presented with (see Fiske & Taylor, 1991; Inman & Baron,
presumably to deflect any consequential imputations of 1996; Marino, Negy, Hammons, McKinney, & Asberg, 2007).
racism ( Bradley-Geist, King, Skorinko, Hebl, & McKenna, People who fear their behavior will be construed as preju-
2010; Jackman & Crane, 1986). To date, however, there has diced may strategically display certain information, termed
been little empirical investigation into whether actors who moral credentials (Monin & Miller, 2001), to signal an impres-
have minority friendships are seen by observers as less preju- sion of nonprejudice. If effective, moral credentials should pro-
diced generally or whether minority friendships buffer actors tect actors from attributions of prejudice and reframe their
from the negative social consequences associated with potentially incriminating actions in a more benevolent light
expressing prejudice. In this article, we provide the first (Effron & Monin, 2010). Although research demonstrates that
answers to these questions, offering a novel contribution to people use moral credentials to license conceivably prejudiced
the growing literature on moral licensing as well as practical behavior, and are personally convinced of their effectiveness
insights into how visual cues and conversational hedges influ- (Effron, Cameron, & Monin, 2009; Merritt et al., 2012; Monin
ence attributions of racism in everyday discourse. & Miller, 2001), little research has looked into how effective
moral credentials actually are in shifting observers’ interpreta-
tion of the behavior in question.
Moral Credentials as Cues to Nonprejudice
Being considered a ‘‘racist’’ is heavily stigmatizing; people
strive to distance themselves from that label and maintain 1
School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Queensland,
an egalitarian image, even if they may hold prejudiced atti- Australia
2
tudes (Sommers & Norton, 2006). This means that much con- School of Applied Psychology, Griffith University, Mt. Gravatt, Queensland,
temporary racism has taken on a subtle, ambiguous form Australia
(Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986). As a result, recognizing racism
Corresponding Author:
has become increasingly challenging: In the absence of overt Michael Thai, School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St. Lucia,
prejudicial behavior, people are required to make complex Queensland 4072, Australia.
moral judgments about discrimination (or lack thereof) by Email: m.thai@uq.edu.au

Downloaded from spp.sagepub.com at Flinders University on February 17, 2016


2 Social Psychological and Personality Science

One vignette study indicated that having moral credentials minority friendships and nonprejudice are ubiquitously
(e.g., voicing support for gay marriage or having gay friends) linked, perhaps even in the minds of observers. It is therefore
reduced perceptions of discrimination for straight observers but possible that observers, especially majority group observers,
not gay observers (Krumm & Corning, 2008). These findings may pay special attention to minority friendship information
were inconclusive, however, as participants read different vign- when making moral judgments about actors displaying con-
ettes describing different types of credentials and discrimina- ceivably prejudiced behavior.
tory behaviors in the licensed and unlicensed conditions. Although preliminary work suggests that minority group
Furthermore, the authors collapsed over multiple vignettes and observers may be less convinced by moral credentials in gen-
were not able to ascertain the differential effectiveness of the eral (Krumm & Corning, 2008), research into intergroup
various types of moral credentials. In the present article, we dynamics suggests they too may be persuaded by minority
focus on one source of moral credentials that we propose friendship cues. Being aligned with minority group members
should be relatively effective in inoculating majority group via friendship may give majority group actors the benefit of
actors against charges of prejudice from both majority and being perceived as embedded within the minority group. This
minority group observers: minority friendships. embeddedness may confer advantages with respect to convin-
cing both majority and minority observers of the actor’s non-
prejudice. The extended contact literature, for example,
Minority Friendships as Moral Credentials? shows that minority group members feel more positively
One of the most common disclaimers used to establish a moral about majority group members if they know of minority/
safety net to protect against attributions of prejudice is refer- majority friendships (Gómez, Tropp, & Fernández, 2011).
ence to minority friendships (Bradley-Geist et al., 2010; Jack- Furthermore, people who criticize a particular group are eval-
man & Crane, 1986). The prevalence of the ‘‘minority friends’’ uated more positively by members of that group, if they them-
disclaimer indicates a widespread belief, at least among major- selves are also part of that group (Hornsey & Imani, 2004;
ity group members, that minority friendships signal nonpreju- Hornsey, Oppes, & Svensson, 2002). It is possible, then, that
dice. To date, however, there is little evidence that actors majority group actors who have minority friends, and are
who have minority friendships are perceived by observers as therefore seen to be embedded within the group, are more
less prejudiced, let alone whether minority friendships effec- likely to be exonerated by minority group observers after an
tively license conceivably prejudiced behavior. apparent display of prejudice than those who do not have
It is possible that beliefs held by majority group actors minority friends.
about the moral credentialing value of minority friendships The extant literature thus provides two competing possibili-
are unfounded; minority friendships may simply not translate ties regarding the effectiveness of minority friendships as a
to nonprejudice in the eyes of observers, especially minority source of moral credentials. On one hand, minority friendships
group observers. Indeed, actors who are motivated to moral may not convey nonprejudice nor license prejudiced expres-
credential often overestimate how credentialed observers will sions, despite what majority group actors may believe. On the
perceive them as being (Effron, 2014). Research also shows other hand, minority friendships may effectively license con-
that transgressing in the same domain as that in which moral ceivably prejudiced behavior, given their ubiquitous use as
credentials have been established may provoke backlash from moral credentials and their cues to embeddedness within the
observers due to perceptions of hypocrisy (Effron & Monin, minority group. We test these competing possibilities in two
2010). Disparaging a particular minority group from which experiments.
one possesses friends may similarly be deemed hypocritical
and consequently incite more negative attributions. Thus,
minority friendships may not effectively convince observers
Study 1
of an actor’s nonprejudice in the presence of incriminating In Study 1, White and Asian observers studied a Facebook pro-
behavior. file belonging to a White target who was visually depicted as
A second possibility is that minority friendships do serve being surrounded by no, few, or many Asian friends. The pro-
as effective moral credentials. Here too, past research pro- file contained an anti-Asian (or baseline control) statement
vides some support. For example, actors who have moral cre- posted by the target. We hypothesized that, if minority friend-
dentials are let off the hook by observers for their ships cue nonprejudice, targets would generally be rated more
transgressions, irrespective of domain, if those transgressions positively the more minority friends they were surrounded by.
are ambiguous enough to allow for reconstrual (Effron & Furthermore, if minority friendships are effective moral cre-
Monin, 2010). This ambiguity caveat may be especially rele- dentials, this effect of minority friendships should hold even
vant in the context of racism. Given contemporary antiracist when targets have posted a conceivably prejudiced anti-
norms, prejudice in the real world is often masked by ambigu- Asian statement. Specifically, observers should evaluate a tar-
ity. Thus, observers’ interpretation of conceivably racist get making an anti-Asian statement while surrounded by
events may be quite malleable and may shift, given the pres- Asian friends as less racist in comparison to a target making
ence of appropriate moral credentials. The prevalence of the an anti-Asian statement while surrounded by only White
minority friends disclaimer suggests that the concepts of friends. Observers should also assume the anti-Asian

Downloaded from spp.sagepub.com at Flinders University on February 17, 2016


Thai et al. 3

statement was made with more benign intentions, find it less profile depicted the target as being surrounded by no (0 Asian,
offensive, and be less upset by it. Finally, in line with past 5 White), few (1 Asian, 4 White), or many Asian friends
research, we expected these moral credentialing effects (if (4 Asian, 1 White); see Figure 1.
present) might be more pronounced for majority than minority
group observers. Measures
Perceived target racism was measured with 1 item: ‘‘To what
Method extent do you believe that Jake Miller is racist?’’(1 ¼ not at all
to 7 ¼ very much so; M ¼ 2.63, SD ¼ 1.96).
Participants
Perceived negative intent was measured by asking partici-
A power analysis (GPower: Faul, Erfelder, Lang, & Buchner, pants whether they believed the statement was (a) ‘‘probably
2007) determined that the approximate number of participants a joke’’ (reverse scored) and (b) ‘‘malicious’’ (1 ¼ not at all
required for an 80% chance of detecting a significant small- to 7 ¼ very much so; r ¼ .49, p < .001, M ¼ 5.06, SD ¼ 1.67).
moderate effect of size f ¼ .15 (Cohen, 1992) was 432. We Perceived offensiveness was measured by asking partici-
decided to collect roughly this number of participants. The final pants whether they believed the statement was ‘‘offensive,’’
sample comprised 203 White Americans and 254 Asian Amer- ‘‘acceptable’’ (reverse scored), ‘‘harmless’’ (reverse scored),
icans (56.7% male and Mage ¼ 29.41) recruited through Ama- ‘‘fine’’ (reverse scored), ‘‘nothing to get caught up over’’
zon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk; see Buhrmester, Kwang, & (reverse scored), and ‘‘went too far’’ (1 ¼ not at all to 7 ¼ very
Gosling, 2011). The data were collected in two MTurk much so; a ¼ .95, M ¼ 3.38, SD ¼ 1.94).
batches—one general batch was used to collect White Ameri- Negative affect toward statement was measured by asking
cans and one targeted batch was advertised specifically for participants whether the statement made them feel ‘‘angry,’’
Asian Americans. Race was also checked within the survey. ‘‘sad,’’ ‘‘happy’’ (reverse scored), ‘‘anxious,’’ ‘‘calm’’ (reverse
We only included data from the relevant majority and minority scored), and ‘‘uneasy’’ (1 ¼ not at all to 7 ¼ very much so;
group, and so 31 non-White and non-Asian participants were a ¼ .83, M ¼ 3.31, SD ¼ 1.20).
excluded from the analyses (19 Black Americans and 17
‘‘Other’’).
Results
Design and Procedure Means and standard deviations for all variables can be seen
The study used a 2 (statement: anti-Asian vs. control)  3 in Table 1. Correlations between all variables can be seen
(minority friends: none vs. few vs. many)  2 (observer race: in Table 2. A 2 (statement)  3 (minority friends)  2 (obser-
White vs. Asian) between-groups design. Participants were ver race) between-groups, multivariate analysis of variance
informed that the present study required them to determine how (MANOVA) revealed significant multivariate effects of
much they could tell about somebody based on their Facebook statement, F(4, 442) ¼ 185.79, p < .001, Zp 2 ¼ .63, and
profile. Participants were then presented with the Facebook minority friends, F(8, 884) ¼ 4.77, p < .001, Zp2 ¼ .04, that
profile of a White, male target named Jake Miller. This profile were qualified by a significant multivariate interaction
contained the independent manipulations (described earlier). between statement and minority friends, F(8, 884) ¼ 3.68,
After reading the Facebook profile, participants evaluated the p < .001, Zp2 ¼ .03. No multivariate three-way interaction
target on a series of dimensions.1 with observer race emerged, indicating that the previous
Statement  Minority friend interaction was consistent
Manipulations across White and Asian participants, F(8, 884) ¼ 0.76, p ¼
.640, Zp2 ¼ .01.
Statement. The Facebook profile contained a public statement
ostensibly posted by the target. Depending on the condition
to which participants were randomly assigned, the statement
Perceived Target Racism
was manipulated such that it was conceivably racist and Examination of the univariate effects confirmed, consistent
directed toward Asian people (e.g., ‘‘way too many Asians with the manipulation, that the target was seen as more racist
around’’) or toward squirrels, as a control (e.g., ‘‘way too many if they had made an anti-Asian statement (M ¼ 3.97) than if
squirrels around’’). For generalizability reasons, participants they had not (M ¼ 1.30), F(1, 445) ¼ 446.16, p < .001, Zp2
viewed one of four anti-Asian statements (or their control ¼ .50. A significant main effect of minority friends,
equivalents) that we collapsed together for analyses (i.e., ‘‘so F(2, 445) ¼ 15.60, p < .001, Zp ¼ .07, was qualified by an inter-
sick of Asians right now,’’ ‘‘Asians are annoying,’’ ‘‘can’t action between statement and minority friends, F(2, 445) ¼
stand Asians’’ and, ‘‘way too many Asians around’’). 13.53, p < .001, Zp2 ¼ .06. Minority friends did not impact upon
perceived target racism when the target made a neutral state-
Minority friendships. The Facebook profile contained visual ment, F(2, 445) ¼ 0.04, p ¼ .965, Zp2 < .01. When the target
information pertaining to the close friendships the target pos- made an anti-Asian statement, however, there was a significant
sessed. Depending on the condition, the cover photo on the effect of minority friends on perceived target racism, F(2, 445)

Downloaded from spp.sagepub.com at Flinders University on February 17, 2016


4 Social Psychological and Personality Science

Figure 1. Experimental manipulation of minority friendships: Study 1.

¼ 30.19, p < .001, Zp2 ¼ .12. A White target without Asian significantly more racist than the target with many Asian friends,
friends was rated as significantly more racist than the target with p ¼ .048, d ¼ .24. No significant three-way interaction with
one Asian friend, p < .001, d ¼ .67, who was in turn rated as observer race emerged, F(2, 445) ¼ 0.21, p ¼ .813, Zp2 < .01.

Downloaded from spp.sagepub.com at Flinders University on February 17, 2016


Thai et al. 5

Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations: Study 1.

Control Statement Anti-Asian Statement

Measure No Asian friends Few Asian friends Many Asian friends No Asian friends Few Asian friends Many Asian friends

Target racism 1.34a (0.76) 1.29a (0.77) 1.27a (0.70) 4.88d (1.51) 3.72c (1.93) 3.28b (1.81)
Negative intent 2.07a (1.05) 1.88a (0.87) 1.99a (0.88) 4.57c (1.52) 3.63b (1.67) 3.47b (1.70)
Statement offensiveness 1.86a (0.89) 1.94a (0.88) 1.86a (0.87) 5.33c (1.33) 4.82b (1.50) 4.46b (1.64)
Negative affect 2.59a (0.63) 2.49a (0.67) 2.64a (0.63) 4.39c (1.04) 4.01b (1.19) 3.76b (1.17)

Note. Means within the same row that do not share the same subscripts are significantly different at p < .05. Standard deviations are shown in parentheses.

Perceived Negative Intent, Offensiveness, and Table 2. Intercorrelations: Study 1.


Negative Affect
Measure 1 2 3 4
The same pattern of univariate effects emerged across per-
ceived negative intent, offensiveness, and negative affect. 1. Target racism —
There was a significant main effect of statement on all three 2. Negative intent .79*** —
variables, Fs(1, 445)  235.71, ps  001, Zp2s  .35. Partici- 3. Statement offensiveness .84*** .80*** —
4. Negative affect .75*** .69*** .81*** —
pants perceived more negative intent behind anti-Asian state-
ments (M ¼ 3.90) than control statements (M ¼ 1.98). Anti- *p  .05. **p  .01. ***p  .001.
Asian statements (M ¼ 4.87) were also rated as more offensive
than the control statement (M ¼ 1.89). Participants felt greater racism ratings (or any other variable) emerged for those in the
negative affect toward the anti-Asian statement (M ¼ 4.05) control statement condition. In fact, participants seemed
than the control statement (M ¼ 2.57). unwilling to pass judgment on the target’s racial attitudes in the
Across all variables, consistent and significant main effects absence of evidence of prejudice, leading to floor compression.
of minority friends, Fs(2, 445)  3.96, ps  .020, Zp2s  .02, These baseline data also demonstrated that minority friend-
were qualified by significant interactions between statement ships did not completely license an anti-Asian statement;
and minority friends, Fs(2, 445)  4.92, ps  .008, Zp2s  although observers judged targets making an anti-Asian state-
.02. Minority friends did not impact on any of the variables ment less negatively if they were depicted with minority friend-
when the target made a neutral statement, Fs(2, 445)  .72, ships, these targets were still perceived more negatively than
ps  .486, Zp2s < .01. When the target made an anti-Asian those who had not made an anti-Asian statement.
statement, however, there was a significant effect of minority This study extends previous research by demonstrating that
friends on all variables, Fs(2, 445)  8.49, ps < .001, Zp2s  minority friendships are somewhat effective in convincing
.04. The pattern of effects for perceived negative intent, offen- observers that one is less prejudiced when a conceivably pre-
siveness, and negative affect differed slightly from those for judiced expression has been made. Importantly, and contrary
perceived target racism, however. The statement of a target with- to past literature, a consistent pattern of effects emerged
out Asian friends was perceived as more likely driven by nega- across majority and minority group observers. This indicates
tive intent, rated more offensive, and responded with more that minority friendships may be a comparatively effective
negative affect, than that of a target with either few, ps  source of moral credentials (see Krumm & Corning, 2008).
.017, ds  .34, or many Asian friends, ps  .001, ds  .57. Study 2 was designed to test boundary conditions of this
No significant differences in perceived negative intent, offen- effect.
siveness, or negative affect emerged between targets who had
few or many Asian friends, ps  .077, ds  .23. No significant
three-way interaction with observer race emerged on any vari-
able, Fs (2, 445)  1.18, ps  .308, Zp2s < .01. Study 2
In Study 2, we examined whether the moral credentialing
effects observed in Study 1 would hold with a verbal, rather
Discussion than visual, reference to minority friends. In the real world,
Supporting the moral credentialing hypothesis of minority majority group members may not always have minority friends
friendships, and contrary to the null possibility, White targets on hand to offset fallout from conceivably prejudiced expres-
who expressed an anti-Asian sentiment were seen as less racist sions. As such, they may verbally reference these friends. Ver-
if surrounded by Asian friends. Observers also assumed more bal allusions, however, may lack the persuasive power of hard
benign intentions behind, were less offended by, and experi- visual evidence and thus engender disbelief in observers.
enced less negative affect toward the statement of targets with Furthermore, research demonstrates that verbal disclaimers
Asian friends. Although we predicted that minority friendships sometimes backfire (El-Alayli, Myers, Petersen, & Lystad,
would make a target seem less racist in general, no difference in 2008). It is therefore possible that mere reference to minority

Downloaded from spp.sagepub.com at Flinders University on February 17, 2016


6 Social Psychological and Personality Science

friendships, as commonly observed in reality, may be compara- Results


tively ineffective in reducing racism attributions.
Means and standard deviations for all variables can be seen in
In Study 2, we tested the same two competing possibilities
Table 3. Correlations between all variables can be seen in Table
as in Study 1. To determine whether observers would be per-
4. A 4 (disclaimer)  2 (observer race) between-groups MAN-
suaded by a verbal disclaimer alluding to minority friends,
OVA revealed a significant multivariate effect of disclaimer,
we measured the extent to which they perceived the actor was
F(15, 450) ¼ 2.99, p < .001, Zp2 ¼ .09. No multivariate inter-
integrated with that minority group. We hypothesized that, if
action with observer race emerged such that the effect of dis-
observers were convinced by the target’s minority friend dis-
claimer was consistent across White and Asian participants,
claimer, they would perceive the target to be more integrated
F(15, 450) ¼ 0.64, p ¼ .842, Zp2 ¼ .02.
with the minority group and would confer the targets the same
moral benefits previously observed.
Perceived Target Integration
Method Examination of the univariate effects revealed a main effect of
disclaimer, F(3, 152) ¼ 4.97, p ¼ .003, Zp2 ¼ .09. Compared to
Participants the target who did not precede their statement with a disclai-
Because we were again recruiting from the MTurk pool, we mer, targets who referenced ‘‘one’’ (marginal), p ¼ .059, d ¼
specified that only new participants (those who did not com- .51, ‘‘some,’’ p ¼ .006, d ¼ .62, or ‘‘many’’ Asian friends,
plete Study 1) could participate. A power analysis revealed p < .001, d ¼ .81, were perceived to be significantly more
that we needed a sample size of 85 in Study 2 to detect the integrated with Asian people. There was no significant differ-
same-sized moral credentialing effect on target racism seen ence in perceived target integration for those who referenced
in Study 1. Given the possibility of a smaller effect with a ver- one, some, or many Asian friends, ps > .092, ds < .40. No Dis-
bal reference to minority friends, we decided instead to collect claimer  Observer race interaction emerged, F(3, 152) ¼
approximately double this number. The final sample com- 0.65, p ¼ .585, Zp2 ¼ .01.
prised 85 White Americans and 76 Asian Americans (61.5%
male; Mage ¼ 31.12). Again, observations from non-White Perceived Target Racism
and non-Asian participants were excluded from the analyses
A main effect of disclaimer emerged on racism ratings, F(3,
(14 Black Americans, 8 ‘‘Other’’).
152) ¼ 6.67, p < .001, Zp2 ¼ .12. Compared to the target who
did not precede their statement with a disclaimer, targets who
Design and Procedure referenced one, p ¼ .002, d ¼ .75, some, p ¼ .024, d ¼ .50,
The study used a 4 (friendship disclaimer: see below)  2 or many Asian friends, p < .001, d ¼ .87, were perceived to
(observerrace: White vs. Asian) between-groups design. As be significantly less racist. There were no significant differ-
in Study 1, participants evaluated a White, male target whose ences in perceived target racism between the target who refer-
Facebook profile contained a conceivably racist statement enced only one Asian friend and targets who referenced some
directed toward Asians. Depending on the condition to which or many Asian friends, ps > .299, ds < .23. A target who refer-
participants were randomly assigned, the statement was posted enced ‘‘many Asian friends,’’ however, was perceived to be
without a disclaimer or was preceded by one of three disclai- significantly less racist than a target who referenced ‘‘some
mers: (1) ‘‘one of my best friends is Asian, but . . . ,’’ (2) Asian friends,’’ p ¼ .039, d ¼ .41. No Disclaimer  Observer
‘‘some of my best friends are Asian, but . . . ’’ , and (3) ‘‘most race interaction emerged, F(3, 152) ¼ 0.98, p ¼ .402, Zp2 ¼
of my best friends are Asian, but . . . ’’.2 .02. White participants (M ¼ 4.12), however, were marginally
less likely than Asian participants (M ¼ 4.57) to evaluate the
target as racist overall, F(1, 152) ¼ 3.51, p ¼ .063, Zp2 ¼ .02.
Measures
Perceived target racism (M ¼ 4.33, SD ¼ 1.82), perceived neg-
Perceived Negative Intent, Offensiveness, and Negative
ative intent (r ¼ .52, p < .001, M ¼ 3.97, SD ¼ 1.58), perceived
offensiveness (a ¼ .90, M ¼ 4.75, SD ¼ 1.38), and negative
Affect Toward Statement
affect toward statement (a ¼ .76, M ¼ 4.01, SD ¼ 1.12) were The same pattern of effects emerged across perceived negative
measured as in Study 1. intent, offensiveness, and negative affect. A main effect of dis-
claimer emerged for all variables, Fs(3, 152)  5.59, ps  .001,
Perceived target integration. An adaptation of the Inclusion of Zp2s  .10. Compared to the target who did not precede their
Other in the Self-Scale (Aron, Aron, & Smollan, 1992) was statement with a disclaimer, targets who referenced Asian
used to measure the extent to which participants believed the friends had their statements perceived to be less driven by neg-
target was embedded within the minority group (‘‘Please indi- ative intent, less offensive, and evoked less negative affect
cate the extent to which you feel that Jake Miller is integrated (one: ps  .001, ds  .72, some: ps  .002, ds  .63, and many:
with Asian people’’; 1 ¼ 0% overlapping circles to 7 ¼ 100% ps  .001, ds  .69). There were no significant differences in
overlapping circles; M ¼ 1.92, SD ¼ 1.08). perceived negative intent, offensiveness, or negative affect for

Downloaded from spp.sagepub.com at Flinders University on February 17, 2016


Thai et al. 7

Table 3. Means and Standard Deviations: Study 2.

Measure No disclaimer ‘‘One of my best friends . . . ’’ ‘‘Some of my best friends . . . ’’ ‘‘Most of my best friends. . . . ’’

Target integration 1.44a (0.91) 1.89b (0.85) 2.09b (1.18) 2.30b (1.18)
Target racism 5.21c (1.67) 4.00ab (1.55) 4.36b (1.71) 3.59a (2.01)
Negative intent 5.10b (1.49) 3.59a (1.33) 3.65a (1.38) 3.39a (1.52)
Statement offensiveness 5.47b (1.36) 4.41a (1.18) 4.58a (1.20) 4.46a (1.53)
Negative affect 4.61b (1.34) 3.81a (0.86) 3.86a (1.02) 3.69a (0.93)

Note. Means within the same row that do not share the same subscripts are significantly different at p < .05. Standard deviations are shown in parentheses.

Table 4. Intercorrelations: Study 2. plausibly racist statement, they were perceived as less racist
and more embedded within the minority group (Study 2) com-
Measure 1 2 3 4
pared to an actor making the same statement without minority
1. Target integration — friends. Additionally, the statement was construed as better
2. Target racism .40*** — intentioned, less offensive, and less upsetting.
3. Negative intent .43*** .70*** — Importantly, minority group observers were just as likely as
4. Statement offensiveness .44*** .70*** .78*** — majority group observers to give moral license to the majority
5. Negative affect .33*** .71*** .74*** .74***
group target expressing racism if he displayed or referenced
*p  .05. **p  .01. ***p  .001. minority friends.3 This gives credence to the notion that minor-
ity friendships may be more effective relative to other forms of
moral credentials that have been explored previously (Krumm
targets who referenced one, some, or many Asian friends, ps >
& Corning, 2008). Minority friendships may contain potent
.388, ds < .43. No significant Disclaimer  Observer race inter-
cues to embeddedness that can cultivate a more convincing air
action emerged on any variable, Fs(3, 152)  0.54, ps  .657,
of nonprejudice.
Zp2s  .01.
This article bridges the gap between the moral licensing and
intergroup contact literatures. Previous work on extended con-
Discussion tact shows that knowledge of positive minority–majority con-
Study 2 demonstrated that mere verbal reference to minority tact facilitates greater intergroup positivity (Gómez et al.,
friendships was sufficient to convince observers that a target 2011). Our findings extend this literature by demonstrating that
was more embedded within the minority group they were dis- contact can also function as a social signal (see also La Mac-
paraging and that the target was less racist. Observers also per- chia, Louis, Hornsey, Thai, & Barlow, 2016). When exposed
ceived less negative intent, took less offense, and experienced to an actor’s ambiguously prejudiced behavior, people use the
less negative affect toward anti-Asian statements preceded minority makeup of the actor’s social network to inform their
with a reference to minority friendships. As in Study 1, the judgments about that actor. Actors who surround themselves
means suggested that reference to minority friends did not with or cite minority friends are seen as embedded within that
absolve the target completely of negative perceptions but rather minority group; their actions and attributes viewed through a
reduced them. Again, the pattern of effects remained consistent (potentially biased) lens of positivity.
across White and Asian participants. Having minority friendships is no panacea against prejudice,
however. Minority friendships are not necessarily indicative of
majority group members’ true prejudicial attitudes (Jackman &
General Discussion Crane, 1986). Prejudices and stereotypes toward broader
Previous research has shown that people regularly bring up minority groups may be maintained through the subtyping of
their relationships with minority group members to appear less minority friends (Aboud, Mendelson, & Purdy, 2003). Further-
prejudiced, often when they have demonstrated feasibly preju- more, only people higher in prejudice subsequently discrimi-
diced behavior (Bradley-Geist et al., 2010; Jackman & Crane, nate after being credentialed (Effron et al., 2009). It appears
1986). This article is the first to examine whether minority observers recognized this; although having minority friends
friendships actually protect majority group actors from observ- somewhat alleviated negative target attributions, actors who
ers’ attributions of prejudice. We demonstrated that they do, had made conceivably prejudiced comments were still per-
albeit not fully. ceived more negatively than those who had not. Still, our find-
Although reluctant to attribute racism to actors who had not ings suggest that minority friendships can potentially be
engaged in questionable behavior, observers paid special atten- exploited to soften the backlash that is typically expected from
tion to minority friendship cues when making moral judgments observers when prejudice is expressed.
about actors displaying conceivably prejudiced behavior. Our studies demonstrated that the presence of minority
When a majority actor’s minority friends were visually evident friendships tempered the negative reactions that overt acts of
(Study 1) or verbally referenced (Study 2) upon making a racism would typically incite: Participants considered the

Downloaded from spp.sagepub.com at Flinders University on February 17, 2016


8 Social Psychological and Personality Science

conceivably prejudiced statement to be less offensive and expe- credentials, and do minority romantic partners provide more
rienced less negative affect. Considering that negative emo- effective moral credentials than minority friendships? Such
tions (e.g., anger) felt in response to perceived discrimination research may help elucidate the magnitude and qualifiers of the
are powerful precursors to collective action and social change moral benefits that can be gained from having minority group
(van Zomeren, Spears, Fischer, & Leach, 2004), these findings members in our social networks and further help us understand
suggest that minority friendships may not only offset social the interplay between social connections and social cognition.
costs associated with expressing prejudice but also suppress the
potential social benefits that can be achieved through perceiv- Acknowledgment
ing greater discrimination. The authors wish to thank Thomas Norton for agreeing to be the main
target stimulus in the present studies.

Limitations and Future Directions Declaration of Conflicting Interests


Future research would do well to assess the boundary condi- The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to
tions of our effect. For example, the present studies used nega- the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
tively valenced statements that were conceivably, but not
indubitably, prejudiced. Given that blatant racism is generally Funding
nonnormative, this captured the ambiguous nature of contem- The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the
porary prejudice. One caveat of this approach, however, is the research and/or authorship of this article: The research contained
resultant uncertainty surrounding the effectiveness of minority within this article was in part supported by the third author’s
friendships in licensing more disparaging or blatantly racist Australian Research Council Discovery Early Career Research Award
behavior. Given that past research demonstrates the effective- (DE120102068).
ness of moral credentials is contingent upon the ambiguity of
the incriminating behavior in question (Effron & Monin, Notes
2010), it is possible that the observed moral credentialing 1. We also measured global target perceptions (e.g., attractiveness,
effects of minority friendships may be nullified when more warmth, likeability, and intelligence, etc.), the likelihood that parti-
overt and unambiguous racist statements are made. Having cipants would befriend the target, and other behaviors (e.g., likeli-
minority friendships may even backfire and incite more nega- hood of ‘‘liking’’ the status update and reporting the user) as filler
tive perceptions in observers due to perceived hypocrisy items. Consistent with the dependent variables (DVs) reported, more
(Effron & Monin, 2010). minority friends were associated with higher scores on all excluded
Future research should also investigate whether our findings DVs. In contrast with the DVs reported in Study 1, however, effects
generalize to other disadvantaged groups. While we found that on these excluded DVs were not qualified by statement type. We did
Asian observers were convinced by minority friendships to a not include these measures, as they detracted from the parsimony and
similar extent that White observers were, work in this area pre- logical flow of the present article and were not relevant to the central
viously demonstrated that gay, but not straight, observers research question. We invite readers to contact us for more details.
remained unconvinced by a straight target’s moral credentials 2. We also tested another common disclaimer that did not involve cit-
(Krumm & Corning, 2008). It is unclear whether this disparity ing minority friends (‘‘I’m not racist, but . . . ’’) and proposed that it
was due to differing methodologies or the different minority would be a relatively ineffective licensing disclaimer, compared to
groups examined. It is possible that the minority friendship citing minority friendships. Although results showed that this was
effect may vary as a function of the disadvantaged group in generally the case, we decided to remove this condition, as it
question. For example, friendships may not reduce prejudice detracted from the focus on minority friendships in the present arti-
attributions if the disparaged group experiences more negative cle. Removal of this condition did not alter our other effects. We
intergroup relations with the majority (e.g., Black Americans) invite readers to contact us for more details.
or if having friends with members of the disparaged group is 3. We conducted post hoc power analyses to determine whether our
commonplace (e.g., sexism attributions and female friends). sample sizes across the two studies allowed for enough power to
Exploring various disadvantaged groups may give us a more detect an interaction with observer race. Both studies were suffi-
nuanced understanding of the moral credentialing effect of ciently powered to detect a moderate (f ¼ .25; Study 1: .99, Study
minority friendships. 2: .77) to large (f ¼ .40; Studies 1 and 2: .99) interaction effect.
Future research could also examine the moral credentialing They were insufficiently powered, however, to detect a small inter-
effects of varying types of relationships with minority group action effect (f ¼ .10; Study 1: .46, Study 2: .16). Thus, although
members. Previous research shows that people feel less we can assume there was no moderate interaction with observer
credentialed by minority acquaintances than minority race across either study, we cannot completely rule out the possi-
friends (Bradley-Geist et al., 2010). It would be interesting bility of a small interaction. We note, however, that means did not
to examine whether this shift in actors’ perceptions of their trend in the direction previous research would imply (i.e., a reduced
own credentials corresponds to external observers’ percep- or absent moral credentialing effect for minority observers). Please
tions. For example, do minority acquaintances (e.g., work see online supplementary materials for descriptive statistics sepa-
colleagues and neighbors) provide less effective moral rated by observer race.

Downloaded from spp.sagepub.com at Flinders University on February 17, 2016


Thai et al. 9

Supplemental Material Hornsey, M. J., Oppes, T., & Svensson, A. (2002). ‘‘It’s OK if we say
The online data supplements are available at http://spps.sagepub.com/ it, but you can’t’’: Responses to intergroup and intragroup crit-
supplemental. icism. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 293–307.
doi:10.1002/ejsp.90
Inman, M. L., & Baron, R. S. (1996). Influence of prototypes on per-
References ceptions of prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Aboud, F., Mendelson, M., & Purdy, K. (2003). Cross-race peer rela- ogy, 70, 727–739. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.4.727
tions and friendship quality. International Journal of Behavioral Jackman, M. R., & Crane, M. (1986). ‘‘Some of my best friends are
Development, 27, 165–173. doi:10.1080/01650250244000164 black . . . ’’: Interracial friendship and Whites’ racial attitudes. Pub-
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of Other in lic Opinion Quarterly, 50, 459–486. doi:10.1086/268998
the Self Scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal Krumm, A. J., & Corning, A. F. (2008). Who believes us when we try
of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 596–612. doi:10.1037/ to conceal our prejudices? The effectiveness of moral credentials
0022-3514.63.4.596 with in-groups versus out-groups. Journal of Social Psychology,
Bradley-Geist, J. C., King, E. B., Skorinko, J., Hebl, M. R., & 148, 689–709. doi:10.3200/SOCP.148.6.689-710
McKenna, C. (2010). Moral credentialing by association: The La Macchia, S. T., Louis, W. R., Hornsey, M. J., Thai, M., & Bar-
importance of choice and relationship closeness. Personality and low, F. K. (2016). The whitewashing effect: Using racial contact to
Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 1564–1575. doi:10.1177/ signal trustworthiness and competence. Personality and Social Psy-
0146167210385920 chology Bulletin, 42, 118–129. doi:10.1177/0146167215616801
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s Marino, T. L., Negy, C., Hammons, M. E., McKinney, C., & Asberg,
Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, K. (2007). Perceptions of ambiguously unpleasant interracial inter-
data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 3–5. doi: 10. actions: A structural equation modeling approach. The Journal of
1177/1745691610393980 Psychology, 141, 637–663. doi:10.3200/JRLP.141.6.637-666
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, Merritt, A. C., Effron, D. A., Fein, S., Savitsky, K. K., Tuller, D. M., &
155–159. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155 Monin, B. (2012). The strategic pursuit of moral credentials. Jour-
Effron, D. A. (2014). Making mountains of morality from molehills of nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 774–777. doi:10.1016/
virtue: Threat causes people to overestimate their moral creden- j.jesp.2011.12.017
tials. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 972–985. Monin, B., & Miller, D. T. (2001). Moral credentials and the expres-
doi:10.1177/0146167214533131 sion of prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Effron, D. A., Cameron, J. S., & Monin, B. (2009). Endorsing Obama 81, 33–43. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.1.33
licenses favoring Whites. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol- Sommers, S. R., & Norton, M. I. (2006). Lay theories about White
ogy, 45, 590–593. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.02.001 racists: What constitutes racism (and what doesn’t). Group Pro-
Effron, D. A., & Monin, B. (2010). Letting people off the hook: When cesses & Intergroup Relations, 9, 117–138. doi:10.1177/
do good deeds excuse transgressions? Personality and Social Psy- 1368430206059881
chology Bulletin, 36, 1618–1634. doi:10.1177/0146167210385922 Van Zomeren, M., Spears, R., Fischer, A. H., & Leach, C. W. (2004).
El-Alayli, A., Myers, C. J., Petersen, T. L., & Lystad, A. L. (2008). ‘‘I Put your money where your mouth is! Explaining collective action
don’t mean to sound arrogant, but . . . ’’: The effects of using dis- tendencies through group-based anger and group efficacy. Journal
claimers on person perception. Personality and Social Psychology of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 649. doi:10.1037/0022-
Bulletin, 34, 130–143. doi:10.1177/0146167207309200 3514.87.5.649
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power
Author Biographies
3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social,
behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, Michael Thai is completing his PhD in social psychology at the Uni-
39, 175–191. doi:10.3758/BF03193146 versity of Queensland. He studies identity and group dynamics, with a
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition (2nd ed.). New specific interest in the intragroup and intergroup attitudes of ethnic
York, NY: McGraw-Hill minority group members.
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1986). The aversive form of racism.
Matthew J. Hornsey is a professor of social psychology at the Uni-
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
versity of Queensland. His research focuses on the dynamic relation-
Gómez, A., Tropp, L. R., & Fernández, S. (2011). When extended
ship between individual and collective selves and people’s responses
contact opens the door to future contact: Testing the effects of
to trust-sensitive messages (i.e., criticisms, recommendations for
extended contact on attitudes and intergroup expectancies in
change, and gestures of remorse).
majority and minority groups. Group Processes and Intergroup
Relations, 14, 161–173. doi:10.1177/1368430210391119 Fiona Kate Barlow is a social psychologist at Griffith University spe-
Hornsey, M. J., & Imani, A. (2004). Criticizing groups from the inside cializing in intergroup relations. Her past work looks at how inter-
and the outside: An identity perspective on the intergroup sensitiv- group anxiety and fears around race-based rejection can sever
ity effect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, intergroup relations. She is particularly interested in minority
365–383. doi:10.1177/0146167203261295 psychology.

Downloaded from spp.sagepub.com at Flinders University on February 17, 2016

You might also like