You are on page 1of 5

Jack C.

Richards

Thombury, S. (1998). Comments on direct approaches in L2 instruction. TESOL Quarter/y, 9M'i!! ri SMM!
32(1),109-116.
Van Patten, W. (1993). Grammar-teaching for the acquisition-rich c1assroom. Foreign Lan-
guage Annals, 26(4),435-450.
Willis, J., & Willis, D. (Eds.), (1996). Challenge and change in language teaching, Oxford:
Heinemann.
CHAPTER 15
Grammar Teaching .; Practice or
Consciousness- Raising?
Rod Ellis

INTRODUCTION
Two major questions need to be considered with regard to grammar teaching in second
language (L2) pedagogy:
1. Should we teach grammar at ali?
2. lf we should teach grammar, how should we teach it?

The first question has been answered in the negative by some applied linguists. Krashen
(1982), for instance, has argued that formal instruction in grammar will nol contribute 10 lhe
development of 'acquired' knowledge - lhe knowledge needed 10 participare in authentic
communication. Prabhu (1987) has tried to show, with some success, that c1assroom learners
can acquire an L2 grammar naturalistically by participating in meaning-focused tasks. Oth-
ers, however, inc1uding myself, have argued that grammar teaching does aid L2 acquisition,
although not necessarily in the way teachers often t1únk it does. My principal contention is
that formal grammar teaching has a delayed rather than instant effect.
The focus of this artic1e is the second questiono I am going to assume that we should
teach grammar (see Ellis, 1990, for the reasons why) and tum my attention to how we
should set about doing so. Specifically, I want 10 consider two approaches, which I shall
refer to as 'practice' and 'consciousness-raising'. I shall begin by defining these. I will
then briefly consider the case for practice and argue that the available evidence sug-
gests that it may not be as effective as is generally believed. I will then present a num-
ber of arguments in support of consciousness-raising and conc1ude with an exarnple of a
'CR-Iask'.

167
168 Rod Ellis Grammar Teaching - Practice or Consciousness-Raising? 169

It should be c1ear from this list that the main purpose of consciousness-raising is to
DEFINING PRACTICE ANO CONSCIOUSNESS-RAISING
develop explicit knowledge of grammar. I want to emphasise, however, that this is not the -
For most teachers, the maín idea of grammar teaching is to help leamers intemalise the same as metalingual knowledge. Itis perfect1y possible to develop an explicit understanding
structures taught in such a way that they can be used in everyday communication. To of how a grammatical structure works without learning much in the way of grarnrnatical
this end, the learners are provided with opportunities to practise the structures, first under terrninology. Grammar can be explained, and, therefore, understood in everyday language.
controlled conditions, and then under more normal communicative conditions. Ur (1988, It may be, however, that access to some metalanguage will facilitate the development of
p. 7) describes th~ practice stage of a grammar lesson in these terms: "The practice stage explicit knowledge.
consists of a series of exercises ... whose aim is to cause the leamers to absorb the structure A comparison of the characteristics of consciousness-raising with those Iisted for prac-
thoroughly; or to put it another way, to transferwhat they know from short-term to long-term tice shows that lhe main difference is that consciousness-raising does not involve the leamer
memory'. in repeated production. This is because the aim of this kind of grarnrnar teaching is not to
It is common to distinguish a number of different types of practice activíties>- enable the learner to perform a structure correctly but simply to help her to 'know about it'.
. mechanical practice, contextualised practice, and communicative practice. Mechanical prac- Here is how Rutherford and Sharwood-Smith (1985) put it: 'CR is considered as a potential
tice consists of various types of rigidly controlled activities, such as substitution exercises. facilitator for the acquisition of linguistic competence and has nothing directly to do with
Contextualised practice is still controlled, but involves an attempt to encourage leamers to the use of that competence for the achievernent of specific communicative objectives, or
relate form to meaning by showing how structures are used in real-life situations. Com- with the achievement of fiuency'.
municative practice entails various kinds of 'gap' activities which require the leamers to Whereas practice is primarily behavioural, consciousness-raising is essentially concept-
engage in authentic communication while at the same time 'keeping an eye, as it were, on forrning in orientation.
the structures that are being manipulated in the process' (Ur, 1988, p. 9) .. The two types of grammar work are not mutually exclusive, however. Thus, grammar
Irrespective of whether the practice is controlled, contextualised, or communicative, it teaching can involve a cornbination of practice and consciousness-raising and, indeed,
will have the following characteristics: traditionally does so. Thus, many methodologists recommend that practice work be preceded
by a presentation stage, to ensure that the leamers have a clear idea about what.the targeted
I. There is some attempt to isolate a specific grammatical feature for focused attention.
structure consists of. This presentation stage may invol ve an inductive or deductive treatment
2. The learners are required to produce sentences containing 'the targeted feature. of the structure. AIso, practice work can be rounded off with a formal explanation of lhe
3. The learners will be provided with opportunities for repetition of the targeted feature. structure. Even strict audiolingualists such as Brooks (1960) recognised the value of formal
4. There is an expectancy that the leamers will perform the grarnrnatical feature explanations of pattems as 'summaries' once the practice activities had been completed.
correctly. In general, therefore, practice activities are 'success oriented' (Ur, 1988, Indeed, it is arguable that no grammar teaching can take place without some consciousness-
p.13). raising occurring. Even if the practice work is directed at the implicitleaming of the structure
and no formal explanation is provided, leamers (particularly, adults) are likely to try to
5. The learners receivefeedback on whether their performance of the grammatical
construct some kind of explicit representation of the role. .
structure is correct or not. This feedback may be immediate or delayed.
Nevertheless, the distinction is a real and important one. Whereas practice work cannot
take place without some degree of consciousness-raising (even if this is incidental), the
These five characteristics provide a definition of what most methodologists mean by
obverse is not the case; consciousness-raising can occur without practice. Thus, it is per-
practice. It should be noticed that each characteristic constitutes an assumption about how
fectly possible to teach grammar in the sense of helping leamers to understand and explain
grammar is learnt. B y and large, though, these assumptions go unchallenged and have
grarnrnatical phenomena without having them engage in activities that require repeated
become pari of the mythology of language teaching.
production of the structures concemed. One way this occurs is by presenting leamers with
Consciousness-raising, as I use the term, involves an attempt to equip the leamer with
rules for memorisation - teaching about grarnrnar. This is what occurred in the grammar-
an understanding of a specific grammatical feature - to develop declarative rather than
translation method. Such an approach lias been discredited on a number of grounds, and
procedural knowledge of it. The main characteristics of consciousness-raising activities are
it is not my intention to advocate its reintroduction. There are other ways of raising con-
the following:
sciousness that are compatible with contemporary educational principies, however. Before
1. There is an attempt to isolate a specific linguistic feature for focused attention. considering thern, I want to consider the extent to which the faith methodologists have in
practice is justified.
2. The learners are provided with data which illustrate the targeted feature and they may
also be supplied with an explicit role describing or explaining the feature.
3. The learners are expected to utilise intellectual effort to understand the targeted
feature. DOES PRACTICE WORK?
4. Misunderstanding or incornplete understanding of the grammatical structure by the A number of ernpirical studies have investigated whether practice contributes to L2 acqui-
leamers leads to clarification in the form of further data and description or sition (cf. Ellis, 1988, for a review). These studies are oftwo kinds: those that seek to relate
explanation. the amount of practice achieved by individualleamers with general increases in proficiency
5. Learners may be required (although this is not obligatory) to~te lhe role (e.g., Seliger, 1977; Day, 1984) and those that haye examined whether practising a specific
describing the grammatical structure. linguistic structure results in its acquisition (e.g., Ellis, 1984).
-,
170 • Rod EUis Grammar Teaching - Practice or Consciousness-Raising? 171

The results of bolh types of research are not encouraging for supporters of practice. from controlled to automatic, and it ignores the very real constraints lhat exist on the ability
Correlational studies (i.e., lhe first kind just referred to) have produced mixed results. Some of lhe teacher to influence what goes on inside lhe leamer's head. Practice may have limited
studies have found a relationship hetween amount of practice and gains in proficiency, but psycltolinguistic validity.
olhers have failed to do so. Even when a study does show a strong relationship, it does not
warrant claiming that practice causes leaming. In order to say somelhing ahout cause and
effect, we have to interpret a correlational relationship. lt is perfectly possible to argue that
it is lhe learners' proficiency that influences practice, ralher than vice versa. Teachers may THE CASE FOR CONSCIOUSNESS-RAISING
direct more practice opportunities at those learners who they think are able to supply correct We have seen that lhe goal of practice activities is to develop lhe kind of automatic control of
answers - thus, lhe more proficient receive more practice. lndeed, one of lhe requirements grammatical structures that will enable learners to use them productively and spontaneously.
of practice - that it be success-oriented - would lead us to predict that this will happen. The We have also seen that lhere are reasons to believe that this may not be achievable. The
detailed analysis of classroom interactions that result from practice activities supports such problem lies in assurning that we can teach grammar for use in communication. If we
an interpretation. lower our sights and instead aim to develop lhe learner's awareness of what is correct but
Studies which have investigated whether practising a specific structure results in its wilhout any expectancy that we can bring lhe learner to the point where she can use lhis
acquisition provide evidence to suggest that practice does not result in the autonomous knowledge in normal communication, lhen lhe main lheoretical objections raised against
ability to use the structure. In other words, practising a grammatical structure under con- practice disappear. Consciousness-raising is predicated on this lesser goal.
trolled conditions does not seem to enable the learner to use the structure freely. I carried Practice is directed at lhe acquisition of implicit knowledge of a grammatical
out a study (Ellis, 1984) to see whether practising 'when' questions enabled learners to structure - lhe kind of tacit knowledge needed to use lhe structure effortlessly for com-
acquire this structure. lt did not. Ellis and Ralhbone (1987) investigated whether practising munication. Consciousness-raising is directed at lhe formation of explicit knowledge - the
a difficult word-order rule with learners of L2 Gerrnan resulted in its acquisition. Again, it kind ofintellectual knowledge which we are able to galher about any subject, ifwe so choose.
did not. There are also doubts that learners are able to transfer knowledge from controlled Of course, lhe construction of explicit representations of grammatical structures is of lirnited
to communicative practice. Once learners move into a meaning-focused activity, they seem use in itself. It may help lhe leamer to perforrn successfully in certain kinds of discrele-item
to fali back on their own resources and ignore lhe linguistic material they have practised language lests. 11may also help to írnprove her perforrnance in planning her discourse, as
previously in forrn"-focused activity. when we monitor our output in order to improve it for public perusal. BUI, crucially, it
, There are, of course, problems with such studies as these, and it would be unwise to will not be of much use in lhe normal, everyday uses of language. Explicit knowledge is
c1aim lhat they conclusively demonstrate lhat practice does not work. It may be lhat the not much use when it comes to communicating. For lhis, we need implicit knowledge.
practice was of lhe wrong kind, that it was poorly executed, or lhat lhere was not enough of We need to ask, lherefore, whelher lhe more limited goal of consciousness-raíslng -
it. It may be that practice only works with some kinds of learners. Nevertheless, lhe studies to teach explicit knowledge - has any value. Ultirnately, consciousness-raising can only be
cast doubts on lhe clairns methodologists make about practice. justified if it can be shown lhat it contributes to thé leamer's ability to communicate. I want to
There are also strong theoretical grounds for questioning lhe effectiveness of practice. argue that, although consciousness-raising does nOIcontribute directly to lhe acquisition of
Pienemann (1985) has proposed that some structures are developmental in lhe sense lhat they implicit knowledge, it does so indirectly, ln ~lher words, consciousness-raising facilitates
are acquired in a defined sequence.1t is impossible for lhe learner to acquire a developmental tbe acquisition of lhe grammatical knowledge needed for communication.
structure until the psycholinguistic processing operations associated wilh easier structures The acquisition of implicit knowledge involves three processes:
in lhe acquisitional sequence have been acquired.
I. noticing (the learner becomes conscious of lhe presence of a linguistic feature in lhe
According to Pienemann's teachability hypothesis, a structure cannot be successfully
input, whereas previously she bad ignored it)
taught (in the sense that it will be used correctly and spontaneously in communication) unless
lhe learner is developmentally ready to acquire it. ln other words, the teaching syllabus has to 2. comparing (the learner compares lhe linguistic feature noticed in the input wilh her
match the learner's developmental syllabus. For practice to work, then, lhe teacher will have own mental gramnlar, registering 10 what extent lhere is a 'gap' between lhe input and
to find out what stage of development lhe learners have reached. Allhough it is technically her grammar)
possible for lhe teacher to do this, it is impractical in most teaching situations. 3. integrating (lhe learner integrates a representation of lhe new linguistic feature into
Of course, it does not follow from lhese arguments that practice is wilhout any value her mental grammar)
at all. Practice probably doe; help where pronunciation is concerned - it gives learners
opportunities to get their tongues around new words and phrases, A1so, practice may be The first two processes involve conscious attention to language; lhe third process takes
quite effective in helping leamers to remember new lexical material, including forrnulaic place at a very 'deep' levei, of which tbe learner is generally not aware. Noticing and
chunks such as 'How do you do?', 'Can I have a ... 1', and 'I don't understand'. Some comparing can take place at any time; lhey are not developmental1y regulated. But integration
learners - extroverts who enjoy speaking in the classroom, for example - may respond of new linguistic material into the store of implicit knowledge is subject to tbe kinds of
positively to practice activities. For lhese reasons, practice will always have a place in lhe psycholinguistic constraints discussed earlier.
c1assroom. It needs to be recognised, however, that practice will often not lead to immediate How, lhen, does consciousness-raising contribute to lhe acquisition of implicit knowl-
procedural knowledge of grammatical rules, irrespective of its quantity and quality. edge? I would like to suggest that it does so in two major ways:
To sum up, there are strong grounds - empirical and lheoretical- which lead us to doubt
tbe efficacy of practice. 'Practice' is essentially a pedagogical construct. It assumes that 1. It contributes 10 lhe processes of noticing and comparing and, therefore, prepares th;
grounds for the integration of new linguistic material. However, it will not bring 'about
lhe acquisition of grammatical structures involves a gradual automatisation of production,
- ~ -
Rod Ellis Grammar Teaching - Practice or Consciousness-Raising? 171

integration. This process is controlled by the learner and will talce place only when
the learner is developmentally ready. TABLE 1. AN EXAMPLE OF A CR PROBJ.EM-SOLVING TASK
2. It results in explicit knowledge, Thus, even if the learner is unable to integrate the 1. Here is some information about when three people joined the
new feature as implicit knowledge, she can construct an altemative explicit company they now work for and how long they have ?~en working there,
representation which can be stored separately and subsequently accessed when the
learner is developmentally primed to handle it. Furthermore, explicit knowledge
Name Date Joined Length of Time
serves to help the learner to continue to notice the feature in the input, thereby
facilitating its subsequent acquisition. MsRegan 1945 45 yrs
Mr Bush 1970· 20 yrs
Consciousness-raising, then, is unlikely to result in immediate acquisition. More likely,
Ms Thatcher 1989 9mths
it will have a delayed effect:
There are a1so educational reasons that can be advanced for grammar teaclúng as Mr Balcer 1990 (Feb) 10 days
consciousness-raising. The incJusion of foreign languages in the school curriculum is not
motivated entirely by the desire to foster communication between spealcers of different 2. Study these sentences about these people. When is 'for' used and
languages, although this has become the most prominent aim in recent years. This inclusion when is 'since' used?
has, and always has had, a more general goal- that of fostering intellectual development. a. Ms Regan has been working for her company for most of her life.
'Grammar' embodies a corpus ofknowledge the study ofwlúch can be expected to contribute b. Mr Bush has been working for his company since 1970.
to students' cognitive skills. It constitutes a serious content and, as such, contrasts with the c. Ms Thatcher has been working for her company for 9 months.
trivial content of many modern textbooks. d. -Mr Balcer has been working for his company since February.
It is not my intention, however, to advocale a return to 'teaching about grammar', or, 3. Which of the following sentences are ungrammatical? Why?
at least, not in the form that this was carried out in the past. The arguments that I have a. Ms Regan has been working for her company for 1945.
presented in favour of consciousness-raising do not justify giving lectures on grammar. b. Mr Bush has been working for his company for 20 years.
Such a transmission-oriented approach runs contrary to progressive educational principies. c. Ms Thatcher has been working for her company since 1989.
What I have in mind is a task-based approach that emphasises discovery learning by asking d. Mr Balcer has been working for his company since 10 days.
learners to solve problems about grammar. The following is an example of this approach.
4. Try and malce up a rule to explain when 'for' and 'since' are used.
5. Malce up one sentence about when you started to learn English and one
sentence about how long you have been studying English. Use 'for' and
'since'.

AN EXAMPLE OF A CONSCIOUSNESS-RAISING TASK


Consciousness-raising tasks can be inductive or deductive. In the case of the former, the
learner is provided with data and asked to construct an explicit rule to describe the gram- CONCLUSION
matical feature which the data illustrate. In the case of the latter, the learner is supplied with ln this paper I have argued the case for grammar teaching as consciousness-raising. In one
a rule which is then used to carry out some task. We do not know, as yet, which type results respect, this does not constitute a radical departure from what teachers have a1ways done.
in the more efficient learning of explicit knowledge - probably both will prove useful. Many teachers have felt the need to provide formal explanations of grammatical points.
Table I provides a simple example of an inductive task designed to raise learners' But in another respect, it does represent.a real alternative in that it removes from grammar
awareness about lhe grammatical differences between 'for' and 'since'. This problem has teaching the need to provide learners with repeated opportunities.to produce the target
been designed with a number of points in mind. First, the intention is to f'ocus on a known structure. So much effort has gone into devising ingenious ways of eliciting and shaping
source of difficulty; learners frequently fail to distinguish 'for' and 'since'. Second, the data learners' responses, more often to little or no avail as learners do not acquire the structures
provided must be adequate to enable the learners to discover the rule that governs the usage they have practised. Consciousness-raising constitutes an approach to grarnrnar teaching
of these prepositions in time expressions. In lhe case of this task, the data incJude both which is cornpatible with current thinking about how learners acquire L2 grarnrnar. It also
grammatical and ungrammatical sentences. Third, the task requires mínimal production on constitutes an approach that accords with progressive views about education as a process
the part of the learners; instead, emphasis is placed on developing an 'idea' of when the of discovery through problem-solving tasks.
two forms are used. Fourth, there is an opportunity to apply the rule in the construction of There are, of course, limitations to consciousness-raising. It may not be appropriate
personalised statements. This is not intended to 'practise' the rule but to promote its storage for young learners. Some learners (e.g., those who like to learn by 'doing' rather than
as explicit knowledge; production, therefore, is restricted to two sentences and there is 'studying') may dislike it. It can only be used with beginners if the learners' first language
no insistence on autornatic processing. Such tasks as these can be designed with varying is used as the medium for solving the tasks. However, the alternative in such situations
formats. They can make use of situational infónnation, diagram, charts, tables, and so on. is not practice. Rather, it is to provide opportunities for meaning-focused language use,
They can also be used in both lockstep teaching (i.e., when the teacher works through a for communicating in the L2, initially perhaps in lhe form of listening tasks. Alllearners,
problem with the whole class) or small-group work. even those who are suited to a consciousness-raising approach, will need plenty of such
Rod Ellis

opportunities. Consciousness-raising is not an a1ternative to communication activities, but


a supplement.

.References
Brooks, N. (1960). Language and language leaming. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.
Day, R. R. (1984). Student participation in the ESL classroom. Language Leaming, 34,
69-89.
Ellis, R. (1984). The role of instruction in second language acquisition. In D: Singleton &
D. Little (Eds.), Language leaming in formal and informal contexts.lRAAL.
Ellis, R. (1988). The role ofpractice in c1assroom language le~ing. Teanga 8,1-25.
Ellis, R. (1990). lnstructed second language acquisition, Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Ellis, R., & Rathbone, M. (1987). The acquisuion of German in a classroom contexto
London: EaJing College of Higher Education.
Krashen, S. (1982). Principies and practice in second language acquisition. Oxford:
Pergamon.
Pienemann, M. (1985). LearnabiJity and syllabus construction. In K. Hyltenstam &
M. Pienemann (Eds.), Modelling and assessing second language acquisition. Clevedon,
Avon: Multilingual Matters.
Prabhu, N. (1987): Second language pedagogy, Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Rutherford, W., & Sharwood-Smith, M. (1985). Consciousness-raising and universal
grammar. Applied Linguistics, 6,2'14-281.
Seliger, H. (1977). Does practice make perfect? A study of interaction patterns and
L2 competence. Language Leaming, 27,263-275.
Ur, P. (1988). Grammar practice activities. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

You might also like