You are on page 1of 15

NICK WINKELMAN

Coaching Science—Theory into Practice

I’m Nick Winkelman, the Director of Training If we basically take the example of cloning an
Systems and Education for Athletes Performance. athlete by 10, creating the exact same program
I also oversee all of the speed development for our each of those cloned athletes are going to go
NFL combine development program. through, but ask 10 different coaches to deliver that
Today, we’re looking at the concept of coaching programming, arguably, even though the program
science—theory into practice. We can also label this and the athlete is unified, you’re still going to see 10
a ‘skills acquisition presentation.’ We’re going to be different results.
looking at a framework for coaching that allows us We understand that while ‘Xs’ and ‘Os’ are
to optimize the environment and the delivery of extremely important, the biomechanics, physiology
information, thus allowing our athletes to retain and information we learn over and over again in
and learn at a higher rate that ideally transfers to the college and throughout our careers are extremely
field of play. important. The limiting factor oftentimes is the
Even if you work with the general population, delivery of that information.
many of the principles here, in terms of how to Today, we really want to explore why certain
instruct people to move and give them feedback on things we do work unbelievably well, both in
their movement, are going to be very important to practice and the transfer to play, but also why some
their success even if it’s just in the weight room. things we do probably don’t work as well. This is
When we look at the whole framework for especially if they practice well, but for some reason
coaching, we have to take a look at motor control and they don’t seem to transfer that to the field. All of
learning. Motor control and learning really create that falls into coaching science.
the foundation for practice and the development of Again, we go back and look at those motor
an environment that optimizes learning. control and learning books, because they give us the
If we understand how someone controls foundation for the applied information we’re going
movement, we have a better understanding of how to discuss today.
someone learns to move. If we understand how If we look at the framework for coaching, at the
someone learns to move, we can optimize those bottom, or base of it, we want to discuss practice
pieces within our practice design, our instruction design. At a very universal level, independent of
and our feedback. what you say to your athlete or client, you can design
These are our three major objectives for today’s a specific environment that optimizes learning.
presentation. Frankly, if we understand that information,
• Understanding how to design an we can immediately start having an impact, not
environment for optimal learning only on how an athlete practices, but also how that
information is retained and, more importantly,
• How to prime the system with proper transferred to the field. While practice is important,
instruction it’s all about that transfer.
• How to refine the motor system with From there, we want to start looking at
proper feedback instruction. How do we actually teach an athlete
If we can do these three things extremely well, to move? What are those initial words we give to a
we’re going to place our athletes and our clients in novice, an intermediate or a highly expert athlete?
the best position to learn. How can we optimize it just from that first exposure?

~1~
This is the transcript of Nick Winkelman’s Coaching Science—Theory into Practice audio lecture.
If you’ve received this material, do not own the audio lecture and would like to learn about it, visit www.movementlectures.com.
For more from Nick, visit him at facebook.com/nwinkelman.
Finally, once an athlete or client moves, how can contexts or a diversity of different movements
we give appropriate feedback to refine that motor within the same session. Within that, we have
system? Are there certain bits of information that this concept of contextual interference. The goal
are going to help them, and also certain bits of is to give the athlete or client context, meaning
information that can hinder them? they’re empowered; they understand how to do
We want to know the information that’s going to the movement, how to correct it, understand when
optimize learning and immediately start retracting they’re doing it right or understand it when they’re
any statements that could be negative to that doing it wrong. Contextual interference essentially
learning. is how I can design a session to interfere with that.
The definition is the memory and performance
We’ll start by looking at practice design and disruption that results from performing multiple
optimizing the learning environment. When we skills or variations within the context of a practice.
look at practice design, the first thing we want to
attain is our goal. Our goal is to optimize learning You’re probably thinking., ‘Why would we want
and retention in an effort to reach maximum to interfere with context? Why would we want to
transfer to the sporting environment. interfere with our athletes’ understanding of what
they’re doing right or what they’re doing wrong?’
Of course, we want our athlete to practice well.
It’s all about how that practice transfers to the Essentially, if I have you do the same movement
environment. One thing we have to understand over and over again within the same session, it almost
immediately is that just because someone is practicing becomes automatic. You know the predictability of
well does not mean the information they’re learning the pattern because you’re doing it over and over
is going to transfer to the environment of sport or again. Your focus—what we call your intrinsic
play. focus—on that movement isn’t as high. When your
intrinsic focus on the movement isn’t as high, you
Today we’re going to look at why that happens tend not to learn as well.
and how we actually optimize practice to get
transfer. Within that, we have two specific terms If I interfere with that by giving you more
in which we’re very interested—practice variability movements, more variations and even randomizing
and the concept of contextual interference. the order of different movements, your cognitive
system undergoes greater overload. Just like the
When we look at practice variability, we body, when we overload a certain area of the system,
want to define it as the variety of movement and that system will adapt by learning and being able to
context characteristics a person experiences while deal with these higher interference factors.
practicing a skill. In looking at this, our goal initially
is to create context. Think of sport. Sport is constantly exposing
the athlete to a variety of movement or variety of
We want the athlete or client to understand why stimuli. This is essentially the world. Our practice
they’re doing what they’re doing to empower them then needs to do the same thing in a very systematic
essentially to make those changes by themselves. order. Contextual interference tries to mimic the
This way, when they make an error, they can look demands of the sport environment successfully in
back at it and tell you what they did wrong. On the practice.
next repetition, we don’t even need to give them
information. They understand how to make the fix. If you think this does work, there’s a concept
This is what we call context. The higher the level of of the contextual interference effect. This was
context our athletes have, the better it tells me how essentially found in 1979. It was defined as the
expert they are. learning benefit from performing multiple skills in
a high contextual interference practice schedule or
When we introduce practice variability, what they call random, rather than performing skills
essentially we’re giving them a diversity of different

~2~
This is the transcript of Nick Winkelman’s Coaching Science—Theory into Practice audio lecture.
If you’ve received this material, do not own the audio lecture and would like to learn about it, visit www.movementlectures.com.
For more from Nick, visit him at facebook.com/nwinkelman.
in a low contextual interference practice schedule— with multiple stimuli, it’s more demanding. While
what they call blocked. practice is more demanding and they may have
In one case, blocked means you do the same more errors in which to accommodate, there is
movement over and over again. It’s very predictable also greater cognitive engagement. When there is
and your body basically can go into auto-pilot, greater cognitive engagement with the practice, we
whereas random means you’re doing a variety of see better retention.
movements against a variety of stimuli that starts to If we look at retention and learning, or essentially
max the demands of the sport even more. how they’re actually going to do on Sunday when
You can’t get complacent. You can’t predict or they play the football game, high contextual
anticipate what’s going to happen next. Your body interference conditions can result in significantly
constantly has to be prepared to react. It ends up higher retention and learning following a series of
being a higher load. practice sessions.

To bring this to life, think about a time when Imagine a baseball practice when you take 100
you’ve driven home. You’ve driven home thousands fastballs, 100 change-ups and 100 curves. There’s
of times on the same road. I’m sure there have been another person who gets those same 300 pitches,
times where you didn’t even realize how you got but they’re now completely randomized. When we
home. You’ve covered that 20 or 30 minutes, but in wait three weeks and test them again in a normal
your mind, you’ve been going through what you did sport or environment, which is inherently random,
that day, what you’re going to do tonight and what the person who did the random practice is going to
you’re going to do tomorrow because your body has be more prepared to handle the diversity of pitches
the auto-pilot, this autonomy. they see, whereas the person who sees the fastball
is inherently going to expect a fastball next. When
Essentially, this same thing can happen in they see the curve ball, they’re not prepared for it.
practice when it’s a blocked practice. Your body just They haven’t dealt with that random environment.
knows what’s going to happen. Thus, you do it over Cognitively, they have not retained the ability to
and over again. The learning effect isn’t as high. This truly hit a fastball, a curve ball and a change-up as
is why if we interfere with contextual interference they see it because that’s not the environment in
through a random-type practice, it has been shown which they learned the skill.
to drive up learning effects in transfer skills and
actually transferring to the sport. While practice may be affected, learning and
retention are optimized. This is essentially what
We look at this contextual interference in we’re trying to do. Can what we’re teaching in
practice. What we know is that high contextual practice actually transfer to the field?
interference conditions match every negative effect
on current performance within a practice setting If we look at this applied, you’ll see three
compared with low contextual interference. different terms in literature use—block practice,
serial practice or random practice. These are the
Now, you’re thinking, ‘ Nick, why am I going to three different ways you can increase contextual
want to have my athletes practice in an environment interference within a practice.
where they’re not going to have as much success?’
Because it’s not predictable and because they Blocks can be defined as multiple movements
can’t anticipate the next movement, everything trained within a week and single movements
is randomized. They’re going to undergo greater trained in a blocked order within a day. This means
cognitive load. on day one, you may only work on one movement.
The next day you work on a new movement. The
Anytime you’re learning a new skill, one that’s next day you work on another new movement, but
more demanding. Anytime we affect that skill in each day, you only focus on one thing. It’s like

~3~
This is the transcript of Nick Winkelman’s Coaching Science—Theory into Practice audio lecture.
If you’ve received this material, do not own the audio lecture and would like to learn about it, visit www.movementlectures.com.
For more from Nick, visit him at facebook.com/nwinkelman.
doing those 100 curve balls or those 100 fastballs all random practice to progress an individual at the
in one blocked session. fastest level to allow the person to retain information
Let’s look at the definition of serial. It’s going to or movement at the highest level.
be multiple movements training a pre-determined If someone has a low skill level, meaning
series within a session. We’re not randomized, the person is a novice and the movement is low
because we’re doing movements in a very specific complexity, the skill level of the athlete is the limiting
order, but we’re going to complete all of those factor. Therefore, you’re going to want to start with a
movements before moving to the next. low contextual interference environment and move
Now instead of doing our fastballs, curve to more of a high contextual interference.
balls and change-ups on different days, we’ll do You’ll start on the block-type practice moving
25 fastballs, 25 curve balls and 25 change-ups all into serial and random. How fast you move from
within the same day. There is greater variation, but block to random is essentially the speed at which
still highly predictable. you see the athlete learning. I’m not going to give
When we look at a random-type practice, you strict guidelines in terms of how long you spend
it’s going to be multiple movements trained or in each one of these.
sequenced in a randomized order within a session. In block practice, you give them context. Now
In this session are the same 75 pitches, but they’re they have the innate capacity to interact with stimuli
now going to be in a randomized order. of the environment and the movement and decide if
Let’s look a step deeper at an actual movement they’re doing it right or wrong. They can now make
skill or skills acquisition example. If I’m doing block, the changes intrinsically on their own.
that means day one I may only do acceleration. On Once they have high context, we can interfere
day two, I only do deceleration. On day three, I work with it. We can then give them more serial and
on something like the drop step. random-type practices because they’re going to have
Let’s look at serial now within one day. On day the intrinsic ability to fix or correct themselves—
one, the first part of the session we may work on what we call discovery learning—in addition to the
acceleration. The second part will be deceleration. instruction and feedback you’re giving them.
The third part will be the drop step. We’re working Let’s look at a second example. Someone has
on the same three movement patterns, but within high skill abilities or maybe has more autonomy,
one single day. but is also now learning a high complex movement.
The random is the same one-day practice. The We’re going to want to still do the same thing. In
first drill might involve acceleration to deceleration. this case because the movement is novel, it’s highly
The second drill set might be deceleration to a drop complex.
step. The third might be acceleration to deceleration Even though the person has high abilities, we
to a drop step, all the while varying the different still want to start with a block practice with a new
stimuli that are initiating those patterns. As we move skill. Once the individual has learned that new skill,
from block to serial to random, we’re increasing we can quickly move into more random. Thus, we
the amount of contextual interference within the have low to high contextual interference.
system. If the athlete has a low skill level and high
Now we look at this in terms of skill complexity complexity movement, we have to start with block
and the skill of the athlete or client. Not everyone is practice. More than likely because the complexity
going to be equal. Some people need to start with of the movement is so high and the skill level of
block. Some people need to start with random. the athlete is so low, you might have to spend a bit
It’s going to be how we combine block, serial and more time in that block practice. However, you still

~4~
This is the transcript of Nick Winkelman’s Coaching Science—Theory into Practice audio lecture.
If you’ve received this material, do not own the audio lecture and would like to learn about it, visit www.movementlectures.com.
For more from Nick, visit him at facebook.com/nwinkelman.
eventually want to move on to higher abilities, but from one to the next. The name of the study was
initially you might spend more time in block. Moderately Skilled Learners Benefit by Practicing
If it’s a high skill level athlete and a low complexity with Systematic Increases in Contextual Interference.
movement, you can probably go right into random- It had 45 participants practice three different
type practice. You don’t even need to do block-type basketball passing strategies under blocked, random
work because the person already has high ability or progressive practice schedules. Passing accuracy
and the demand of the skill is very, very low. was based on a target and how close they got to the
target based on the three different passing skills.
These examples display what we call the
‘challenge point hypothesis.’ This means you can’t What they found was very simple. A progressive
just live in one camp or the other, only block or only increase in contextual interference from a block to
random. It’s basically a diversity of the progression. a random schedule improved retention of passing
skills better than if they just did the random or
If we think about blocked to serial to random the block alone. A random practice is better than
increasing contextual interference, children need to blocked. If we look at a progressive schedule from
start with block practice whereas adults can better block to more random, we create the context with
handle random-type practice. As a progression, we block practice, teach them the skill and teach them
move from block to random. how to correct themselves. Then interfering with
If it’s a low skill level individual versus a high that with more random-type practice is better than
skill level individual, typically we want to start starting with one or the other by itself.
with block, move toward more random and then This goes hand-in-hand with the challenge-point
finally high skilled complexity versus low skilled hypothesis. The speed at which we progress from
complexity. With high skilled complexity, start with block to random is highly dependent on the age,
block moving toward more random. the ability and the complexity of the skill, innately
All of these are specific research studies that allowing you to progress as their skills improve,
fall under the challenge point hypothesis. Progress getting eventually to random practice because that’s
from block to random, but as a rule of thumb, going to be the key to truly lock in this information
higher levels of random practice will result in better from a retention and transfer standpoint.
retention and learning. This is the reason we want to The major take-homes here are to move from
move people toward a more random environment block to random and get people to random practice
successfully after they can handle blocked. successfully and safely. This is what’s going to
So far we’ve really just been discussing black and transfer to the field of play.
white—blocked or random, blocked or random. Some further considerations when looking
However, some of the modern research on this is at practice that we have to factor in is what we
saying that a progression might be better. call ‘regulatory’ and ‘non-regulatory’ conditions.
If you look in the literature specifically for Independent of blocked, serial or random practice,
the terms ‘contextual interference,’ these are the we want to then look at the environment in which
two terms people are going to use when they talk the person is practicing.
about practice. There are well over 1,000 published Regulatory conditions are factors that can affect
research studies. Most of those just look at block motor skill characteristics—things like what surface
practice versus random. The vast majority of them you play on, how many opponents there are and the
will tell you that random practice is always going to sport rules. All of these affect the type of movements.
result in better transfer and learning.
Now, we manipulate these all the time. If I’m
However in 2010, Jared Porter looked at block playing soccer, maybe I make the field a bit smaller.
versus random versus someone who progresses

~5~
This is the transcript of Nick Winkelman’s Coaching Science—Theory into Practice audio lecture.
If you’ve received this material, do not own the audio lecture and would like to learn about it, visit www.movementlectures.com.
For more from Nick, visit him at facebook.com/nwinkelman.
Maybe I adjust the rules now and we only get one 100 meters. They’re always going to do it in the same
touch on the ball versus two. fashion on relatively similar tracks and in relatively
All of these are what they call ‘manipulation of similar environments. It’s a closed predictable skill.
regulatory conditions’ that inherently changes how We want to vary the regulatory and non-regulatory
athletes move to bias a specific skill they’re trying conditions in practice based on the actual variation
to learn. Whether it’s block or random, you can still seen during the sport. If there’s not a lot of variation,
optimize or change these regulatory conditions to you don’t need to vary in practice.
bias out a specific movement. Open skills, on the other hand, are your
From there, we can look at ‘non-regulatory reaction, random, non-predictable environments.
conditions.’ These are factors that indirectly affect These are typically team or field sports. The nature
motor skills. These can be things like the crowd, of open skills calls for variation in both regulatory
the game score and the game ports. If someone is and non-regulatory conditions used systematically
shooting a free throw in basketball, the athlete may throughout practice. Therefore, you want to vary
in practice be able to do this with 95% accuracy. the things that affect movement and things that
However, the second atheltes get into the game, indirectly affect movement, especially as a game’s
especially if it’s playoffs, maybe that accuracy goes importance increases.
down to 75%. Even though that has no direct There are different environmental things we want
correlation to how they move, it’s still from a to think about—block to more random, regulatory
cognitive perspective that limits them. and non-regulatory conditions and creating an
How can we affect these non-regulatory environment for learning. Again if you’re interested
conditions? Is it having loud music or having loud in this, you want to look up the terms ‘contextual
noises that sound like a crowd? You see this all the interference’ or ‘practice design’ in the literature.
time, especially in football environments where That falls under motor learning and motor control
they’ll blast very loud crowd-type noise. categories in terms of science.

Can you have two individuals race against Here’s a big-ticket item. Increased contextual
each other or track down a ball to then create an interference is associated with a short-term
environment of sprinting? That essentially shouldn’t performance decrement in practice that results in
affect how they move, but because it increases the significant improvements in learning retention.
importance, as winning is on the line, it creates a When we talk about decreased performance
non-regulatory demand on the body. in practice, we’re not talking about increasing
When we look at this, we can finalize this as a movement dysfunction. Rather the speed at which
considerable level of variation within the regulatory they move might be limited since they’re having
or non-regulatory conditions. Simply try to match to process more information because it’s more of a
those environments. Especially as you get closer random environment. This means the more they’re
to very important demanding games, you want exposed to that, the better they can handle it and the
to make sure the practice of regulatory and non- faster those movements will become.
regulatory conditions, in addition to blocked versus The performance decrement is still going to allow
random practice, is going to match what we need higher retention when it comes to actual sporting
them to do. environments. That’s where this can be misleading
Some final examples would be as follows. and why it’s important for us to understand practice
Closed skills are those that do not involve random design and transfer.
or reactionary-type conditions. Sprinting would be Once we understand how to create an
an example. They’re always going to run the same environment for learning—and I think that last

~6~
This is the transcript of Nick Winkelman’s Coaching Science—Theory into Practice audio lecture.
If you’ve received this material, do not own the audio lecture and would like to learn about it, visit www.movementlectures.com.
For more from Nick, visit him at facebook.com/nwinkelman.
section is very important for any kind of sport, Let’s start by looking at verbal instruction.
coaches and performance coaches who do field You want to provide one to two focus cues to
work—we now want to look at the second level, build awareness. Within this, you want to limit
what we call ‘task instruction,’ or priming the motor unnecessary information to avoid over-coaching.
system. The more information we give them, the more of
For the personal trainers, those who maybe their attentional capacity we take. The more of their
just work in the gym and are teaching movement, attentional capacity we take, the less attention they
this starts to become increasingly important for the have to actually focus on the movement they’re
general population client, priming the motor system doing.
with proper task instruction. A perfect example is this. Have you ever had
If we look at an instruction model, we basically someone do a movement and then while they’re
can instruct in one of two ways—either using verbal doing the movement, you say, ‘Great job. Keep that
information or visual information. Under verbal up.’ All of a sudden, they mess up.
information, we can either give it an internal focus The reason is because all of a sudden they’ve
or an external focus. diverted their attention from what they were doing
We’ll go into this deeper, but an internal focus is to you now exposing their movement as having a
essentially talking about body parts—talking about dysfunction.
muscles, talking about the process of the movement. In all reality, their movement was fine. It’s your
Everything between the head and the heel within interference with what they were doing or their
the body is where you draw their attention or their attentional focus that drove the movement down to
focus. This is why we call it internal. go haywire. Thus, we want to avoid over-coaching
External focus is less of a process of the both in the instruction as well as the feedback we’ll
movement, but more about the outcome. Now, all of discuss here in a bit.
these cues are going to have to do with everything We want to always start and finish instruction
environmentally around the body. with what we want versus what we don’t want. A
Internal focus during acceleration may be good friend of mine, Victor Hall, calls this, ‘the sippy
something like extending your knee, firing a quad cup principle.’ This is a great conversation he and
or squeezing your glute. External focus may be I had. Now having my own daughter, I completely
pushing the ground away or driving your body understand this. Victor gave the example of his son.
off of the line. One is inside of the body and one is Every time his son was finished with his sippy cup,
outside of the body; hence, internal versus external he would throw it. He said, ‘Don’t throw the sippy
focus. As we’ll discuss, the difference between these cup.’ Instead of throwing it, his son would dump it
is unbelievably massive. out. He said, ‘Don’t dump your sippy cup.’ When his
son was done, he would start banging it on the table.
With visual, on the other hand, we can watch
one of two models. We can watch a novice model, Every time Victor told his son what he didn’t
someone learning the movement for the first time, want, his son chose something else to do. He finally
or we can watch an expert model. realized this and said, ‘When you’re done drinking,
I want you to put the sippy cup on the counter.’ He
Inherently, we all try to teach in terms of an didn’t give him 1,000 different options of what he
expert model. We want to show them the perfect could do. He gave his son one option of what he
way of doing things. However, there may actually wanted him to do correctly.
be some benefit for showing them from a novice
perspective, having them to be able to observe a This is no different than with our athletes. If we
novice and learn from that. We’re going to look at tell them not to do something or we identify an error,
both the visual and verbal model. there are 1,000 other things they can do. Essentially,

~7~
This is the transcript of Nick Winkelman’s Coaching Science—Theory into Practice audio lecture.
If you’ve received this material, do not own the audio lecture and would like to learn about it, visit www.movementlectures.com.
For more from Nick, visit him at facebook.com/nwinkelman.
we’re not focusing their attention. We want to tell the top of your head. That cueing is keeping the
them what we want them to do correctly. Give them focus within the body. An external cue would be to
one option—one focus point. It decreases their drive the bottom of your shoe at the fence behind
attentional demand, allowing the focus and move to you.
be successful. Now for all of you who use analogies and
Thus, we have the ‘sippy cup principle.’ Tell them metaphors when you’re teaching people to move,
what you want. Try to diminish the focus on what that is spot on. Analogies and metaphors fall into
you don’t want. the external cueing focus because they don’t involve
Finally, you want to focus their attention internal focus. Analogies and metaphors—or direct
externally on the outcomes as opposed to internally external cues—fall into this category.
on the process of the movements. Going back to If we’re teaching someone to do some type
verbal, we can either give them an internal cue or of plyometric, an internal cue might be to drive
an external cue. your hips through your head. It essentially keeps
Dr. Gabriel Wolf ’s work has shown that all of the energy inside their body, not allowing
focusing someone externally absolutely improves the coordination to release or finish the whole
performance and efficiency as opposed to focusing movement versus trying to touch the ceiling above
someone internally, which decreases performance you. If you’re using a Vertex, try to touch the highest
and decreases efficiency. rung. External cueing in this case is going to improve
efficiency.
When we look at both, external focus improves
transfer and retention whereas internal focus One of the studies Gabriel Wolf did was with
diminishes it. This is probably the most important jumping. In one case, she told them to try to get
concept for both instruction and feedback. When their fingers as high as possible. In another case,
we cue, we want to cue with an external focus and she simply told them to try to reach for the highest
diminish any kind of internal cueing that drives rung. Just by saying the word ‘fingers’ versus ‘reach
up an internal focus. Internal cueing is inside the for the highest rung,’ think about the difference
system and external cueing is outside the system. there. It’s miniscule. One is outside of the body and
one is inside the body.
Dr. Wolf has a book entitled Attention and Motor
Learning. Looking at attention and motor learning What they found was staggering. Statistically,
is essentially what we’re doing here. Every time we the people who just simply tried to touch the highest
give information, we’re affecting the attention of rung jumped significantly higher than those who
the athlete. If we focus the attention externally, it focused on the height of their fingers or getting the
takes up less demand. If it takes up less attentional fingers as high as they could.
demand, it allows more attentional resources toward They also found that people who focused on
the movement. This is what we want. We want the trying to touch the highest rung not only jumped
cognitive load intrinsically from the athlete to focus higher, but they did it with lower EMG. In the people
on what they’re doing—not to focus on what we’re who focused on their fingers, the EMG of the lower
saying. body went up. This means their muscle activation
What we find is that internal cues draw too went up, but their jump height went down.
much of the attentional demand away from the To increase muscle activation, should this
actual movement, thereby decreasing the optimal actually increase jump height? Yes and no. If
ability of the athlete or client to move. that activation results in more co-contraction,
Let’s take, for example, a RDL. An internal cue or decreased timing and efficiency, it could be a
would essentially be to drive your heel away from constraint on the system.

~8~
This is the transcript of Nick Winkelman’s Coaching Science—Theory into Practice audio lecture.
If you’ve received this material, do not own the audio lecture and would like to learn about it, visit www.movementlectures.com.
For more from Nick, visit him at facebook.com/nwinkelman.
This is actually what happened. The decreased attention to each other, meaning you do a waterfall
EMG showed improved efficiency and less co- start. Every athlete chooses acceleration and does a
contraction, allowing the body to complete the 10-yard sprint. Now, you’ve seen how to do it right.
coordination better and more effectively, thereby Let’s say you’ve watched your nine other fellow
increasing efficiency and less energy and also athletes who are learning just like you, but who
increasing performance. When they looked at a are not doing it perfectly. That allows you to fill in
couple of retention tests, the people who focused the gaps of what the expert did right and what the
externally still jumped higher. novice did wrong, already driving your attention to
When you look at this information, it’s what you should be focusing. This is all happening
staggering. What you say affects performance. Jump subconsciously.
height affects efficiency, timing and synchronization, Thus, it improves problem solving and discovery
as well as affects transfer to the actual sporting learning. In fact, a combination of expert and novice
environment. could be the best way to use visual instruction. Don’t
External cueing to instruction is what high just assume it always has to be the person doing it
random or contextual interference is to practice right.
design. This optimizes the environment within Within this, it doesn’t mean you have to call
practice, but also to transfer. This is unbelievably attention to what the novice is doing wrong. Just by a
important. Dr. Gabriel Wolf ’s book, Attention and novice watching another novice do a movement after
Motor Skill Learning, is the resource you’re going to seeing what an expert did right, they automatically
want to look at to find out more. can pick up what is done wrong and focus their
If verbal is half the equation, visual is the attention on how to fix those things in themselves.
other half. Then, we’re going to look at how they Therefore, combining both creates context to
go together. If we watch an expert performer, we know what the novice is doing wrong and drives
understand there’s this concept of mirrored neurons. learning. This is a great model. Have the athletes
In our brain, we have the ability to mirror things we watch you or an expert do it perfectly. Have them
see outward. do three or four practice repetitions. Then maybe
Think of a baby boy or girl. I have a young on every third or fourth repetition, do a waterfall
daughter who is now six months old. When she was start. Draw their attention to each other so they can
two to three months old, she started to smile, but learn. It’s no different than them doing visualization.
she only smiled when I smiled at her. Eventually, she They can learn from watching others move, thereby
learned that smiling meant happiness. Now when eventually increasing their learning and retention.
Mommy or Daddy see her, she smiles on her own. These are the big-ticket items. Combining visual
Her mirrored neurons gave her time to create an and verbal instruction may be more beneficial than
understanding and context of what smiling meant. either independently, especially when teaching a
Now, she’s able to own the movement. novice or learning a novel task.
Our athletes are no different. Our mirrored Visual creates an image and verbal, utilizing an
neurons still have the capacity to do this. When you external focus, can drive the outcome of that image.
watch an expert performer, you can mirror what Think about this. Visual creates the image in the
they’re doing. However, we also want to look at the mind and verbal drives the outcome of what that
benefit of watching a novice performer because it image represents.
improves problem solving and discovery.
Giving them outcome externally oriented
Imagine now that you started by watching an feedback takes up less attentional demand allowing
expert performer, seeing exactly what they need them to intrinsically focus on the movement at
to do right. Now, you draw all of your athletes’ hand and allowing the cue to drive the outcome of
the movement.
~9~
This is the transcript of Nick Winkelman’s Coaching Science—Theory into Practice audio lecture.
If you’ve received this material, do not own the audio lecture and would like to learn about it, visit www.movementlectures.com.
For more from Nick, visit him at facebook.com/nwinkelman.
We see theoretically that this is the reason Knowledge of performance is information
why external cueing and the use of visual together about the movement characteristics that led to the
improves not only their performance in practic,e outcome. These are more qualitative. This is where
but also the retention and transfer of the movement. we’re actually cueing them on movement qualities.
Finally, if instruction is priming the motor In looking at this, we typically give a lot more
system, we now want to look at feedback refining information on knowledge of performance versus
the motor system. Within this refinement, we’re still knowledge of results. Frankly, as athlete improve
utilizing verbal information. Therefore, the internal ability and hve the context on how to correct
versus external cue is going to fall hand-in-hand themselves, we tend to give them more knowledge
with this feedback model we want to discuss. A of results—how fast they ran, if their cadence was
model of feedback is going to follow like this. Again, okay and if they met the quantitative goal of what
there are two different categories—task-intrinsic they were doing.
feedback and augmented feedback. In the example of running a 40, knowledge of
We look at task-intrinsic feedback as being very results would be that you ran the 40 in 4.56 seconds.
simple. It’s essentially the natural feedback that Knowledge of performance would be driving the
comes from doing a movement. It can be visual, knees during the first five yards. If you want to be
auditory, tactile or proprioceptively connected. highly external, draw a black dot on the end of the
Think about people in archery. If they pull knee and say, ‘Drive that black dot more during the
back and shoot, they can use visual feedback to first five yards’ to drive in even more external focus.
understand exactly where they hit on the target. You The big-ticket items here are very simple. The
don’t need to tell them they missed the bull’s eye. higher the task-intrinsic feedback, the less need for
Look at auditory versus tactile versus augmented feedback. The lower the task-intrinsic
proprioceptive. If we’re doing any type of balance feedback, the greater the need for augmented
work, the proprioceptive system is giving spatial feedback. Therefore, we do not want to talk just to
awareness. I don’t need to tell the person they fell talk. If a movement inherently gives the information
over. Just by allowing someone to do more balance to the athletes on what needs to be fixed, we don’t
tasks improves balance because it has high task- need to tell them. If we continue to give error, error
intrinsic feedback. and error to the athletes, they’re going to take that
as a negative.
Certain things from a sprinting perspective—
how much extension they have or how much knee To drive up self-efficacy, we essentially want
drive—might not inherently increase awareness. to hand over the keys to the car. As much of the
Therefore, we need to tell them those things. Maybe environment they can self-regulate, we want to give
the time they ran the 10-yard or 20-yard sprint and them that ability. At the end of the day, the coach
don’t inherently know that, they’re going to need that is on the sideline. The coach is in the box when the
information. That’s essentially where augmented athletes are playing on the field.
feedback comes in. We want to give them the environment to
Augmented feedback falls into one of two transfer. We don’t want to have a high coaching
results—knowledge of results or knowledge of environment and a minimal-to-no-coaching
performance. If we look at augmented feedback, environment. Allow task intrinsic feedback to do
knowledge of results is essentially defined as what it does for the athlete. However, if it has low
information about the outcome of a skill or the task-intrinsic feedback as this section focuses, that’s
goals achieved. It’s quantitative in nature. It’s the where augmented feedback is very important.
numbers—how fast you run. Next the question becomes, ‘How much
feedback?’ We tend to see this especially in new

~10~
This is the transcript of Nick Winkelman’s Coaching Science—Theory into Practice audio lecture.
If you’ve received this material, do not own the audio lecture and would like to learn about it, visit www.movementlectures.com.
For more from Nick, visit him at facebook.com/nwinkelman.
coaches, thinking more is always better. They coach of sport skills through an external focus feedback.
every single repetition. They’re talking the entire You know why that’s important.
time because they value themselves as coaches in We know that external is going to be better than
terms of what they’re saying. internal, so here’s what the study looked like. Fifty-
I’m going to challenge this thought process. two participants took part in a passing accuracy
We should not value ourselves in terms of what task. This was a kicking-type passing accuracy task.
we’re saying and how it sounds, but rather how the Feedback frequency and internal versus external
environment we create transfers to the sporting focus was examined. They had four different groups.
environment. At the end of the day, that is what They had one group that received 100% feedback
our athletes are paying us to do. They want to see and there was an internal cue. The second group
transfer. They want to be better at their sports. received 100% feedback and there was an external
The concept of ‘a guidance hypothesis,’ which cue. The third group received 33% feedback with an
was really first hallmarked by Salmoni in 1984, tells internal cue. Finally, the fourth group received 33%
us that feedback guides the athlete and client toward with an external cue.
the correct movement or toward the correct context. As a whole, both in practice as well as a couple
That’s very important—we need feedback and of days later in retention, an external focus was
instruction. However, when given too frequently, it superior to internal focus. Performance, accuracy
can have detrimental effects on the movement skill and all of the variables improved not only in
and create feedback dependence. practice, but in retention as well. That’s what’s so
Imagine this. If athletes are having problems, important about this.
rather than depending on themselves, rather than The athletes are going to feel better because
depending on their own feedback or their own they’re practicing better, whether it’s blocked or
long-term memory, they focus on you as the coach. random. Even more importantly, they’re going to
Their dependence on you is so high that the second retain that information. There’s actual learning
you remove that constant form of feedback, they are going on.
essentially paralyzed. They can’t do the movement. The interesting fact is that 33% feedback was
These are oftentimes the athletes who practice superior to 100% feedback for all internal focus
unbelievably well and are used to having high conditions. This makes sense. If internal focus
coaching dependence. However, the second the messes up athletes, it’s believed that giving them
coach is removed and play starts in an actual sport less of that feedback is better. That’s what this study
environment, they can’t transfer. It makes sense. shows.
They are depending on you as the resource. You This is right in line with the guidance hypothesis
are almost an external hard drive for them. The that 33% feedback is better than 100% when
second you unplug an external hard drive from the giving internal focus. This means you give them
computer or the athlete, they no longer can access information every third repetition.
that information.
In studies that have looked at internal and
Now, we want to look at how much feedback is external, sometimes they actually found controlled
appropriate. Let’s actually look at a study from Dr. groups did better than internal focusers. This means
Gabriel Wolf. This study is going to reinforce the if you didn’t give the athletes any information at all,
difference between internal and external focus, as they’re going to do better than if you drive them to
well as how much information should be given. internally focus on their own bodies. In looking at
This study was done in 2002. It is one of the this 33% feedback, less is more if it’s non-externally
most powerful studies ever done on instruction and based.
feedback. The concept was enhancing the learning

~11~
This is the transcript of Nick Winkelman’s Coaching Science—Theory into Practice audio lecture.
If you’ve received this material, do not own the audio lecture and would like to learn about it, visit www.movementlectures.com.
For more from Nick, visit him at facebook.com/nwinkelman.
Now, here’s the interesting fact. In terms of learning is not expressed on the field. Think about
the external focus, 33% and 100% feedback were this.
equally as effective. If it’s you’re working with novice Forever we’ve done a ton of block practice and
athletes or they’re learning a novel task, you might we’ve given them a ton of information. This allows
want to communicate with them a bit more. That’s them to inherently practice unbelievably well. They
fine as long as the feedback is externally oriented. become very skilled. However, the second you
As a whole, I think we can agree that less remove that feedback and you now take them from
feedback is better than more. That 33% to 50% a blocked environment to a random environment,
feedback amount is what you see in the literature. which is sport, we wonder why they can’t play
And external is better than internal. well. It’s because we’re creating a false, artificial
At the end of the day, we’re trying to hand the environment. We want to create an environment
keys to the car over to the athlete. We do not want that most matches the demand, especially when
to interfere with the learning. We don’t want to plug we’re talking about field work and skills acquisition.
in and unplug. Then all of a sudden, there’s a power Finally, the big one is over-coaching—paralysis
outage and they can’t do the movement effectively. by analysis. Sometimes a lot of feedback works.
This is why. Oftentimes, the heightened practice However, the risk is coach-dependence. Also, a lot
capabilities with lower play transfer capabilities of times too much feedback doesn’t work and we get
could be coming from coaches not creating the right immediate feedback from the athlete—paralysis by
environment, instruction and feedback models to analysis. You give a great, long speech and all of a
allow for learning. sudden the athlete looks at you and says, ‘What do
you want me to do, coach?’ We all have felt this.
There are huge dangers in giving too much
feedback. It’s what I like to call the coach dependence, I don’t meant to just tell you what you need to do,
the DVD player analogy. We chuck the DVD but to reinforce probably what you’re already doing
in. That’s practice—high coach volume. They’re well. I want to give you a bit of the science behind
watching that DVD. The second we pull the DVD what you’re doing, why it’s working and maybe why
out and now they’re playing, that system no longer some of the things you used to do or some of the
ceases to be able to play the information. They have things you’re still doing today aren’t working so well.
high coach dependence. In the end, there’s this concept of terminal
Less dependence on intrinsic processing means feedback methods. There are actually certain types
there’s less cognitive overload. If I’m constantly of feedback that help to decrease the amount of
listening to a coach, the coach is giving me the feedback you give and allow you to optimize the
answer versus me being able to implicitly or quality of that feedback.
intrinsically figure out that answer or discover the The first one is called ‘bandwidth feedback.’
answer by myself. It’s called terminal, because it’s given after the
If I turn the lights off in a room and tell you movement, or series of movements is completed.
where to go, it’s easy. However if I turn the lights Bandwidth feedback is given whenever it reaches a
off in a room and you have to figure out where the limit.
light switch is, you will forever know where the light Think about it. You write the session. You’re
switch is located. going to do these movements in this order and you
This same analogy is with coach-athlete structure the whole thing. However, how often do
relationships. To the best of their abilities, we want you structure and plan your coaching?
to empower them to find the answer for themselves. This is where you are now going to be able to tap
Discovery learning is less coach dependant. Practice into not only designing a session, but also how you
well, but when feedback is removed, attentional

~12~
This is the transcript of Nick Winkelman’s Coaching Science—Theory into Practice audio lecture.
If you’ve received this material, do not own the audio lecture and would like to learn about it, visit www.movementlectures.com.
For more from Nick, visit him at facebook.com/nwinkelman.
are going to interact with that session. Bandwidth or an average understanding of how they move,
feedback is a way to do that. We now know for and then give more focused feedback on an error
every movement you’re going to be going through, that’s actually reoccurring. It’s a better focus of
especially the high-level skill application-type information, but less volume of information given.
thesis, you’re going to give yourself very specific ‘Fading feedback’ goes with our block-to-
focus points. This could mean in terms of if they do random contextual interference flow, meaning that
one error wrong, you’re going to give them a cue. feedback is given more frequently at the beginning
Maybe there are four different things they of a session and is progressively decreased over the
typically do wrong. However, you know that one course of that session—again, external feedback
of those is the major thing they need to work on. being the focus.
So your bandwidth feedback might be on that Finally, the one I really want you to think about
one specific area. You’re only going to give them trying, even with non-athletes, is ‘self-selective
information when they’re doing that piece wrong. feedback.’ Feedback is given to the athletes at their
This allows you to set objectives for the session. requests. Try this with both new athletes, as well as
It also allows you to interact with those objectives so highly experienced athletes.
you’re coaching and cueing to meet the objectives This is going to give you an understanding of
of what you’re trying to do on a given day. It also how they like to communicate or how they like to
naturally decreases the volume, because when communicate at the stage of learning they’re in.
they’re doing it right, you don’t need to talk to them.
You allow them to intrinsically drive what they’re Most of the research done in this avenue was
doing. done on non-athletes—the general population.
Think quickly: When do you think they ask for
Then, there’s what they call ‘summary or average feedback? Was it when they did it right, when they
feedback.’ Feedback is given after a number of trials weren’t sure or when they did it wrong?
have been observed and average errors have been
identified. Think about novice athletes or the inexperienced
general population. You have the answer in your
It is very important that we understand this mind now. In the majority of the research, they
inherent idea of movement variability. With every found that when you’re learning a new skill or
single repetition, there are going to be differences. you’re a novice or a general population person, you
You will never run two repetitions exactly alike. actually ask for feedback when you did it right.
What we need to figure out is ‘What is general This matches with everything they’re saying. The
movement variability?’ Is it an error that just reason they asked for information when they did
happened to be exposed on that repetition versus a it right is because they’re trying to create context.
consistent movement dysfunction? If they did it right, they could capture and retain
Too often, we try to coach every repetition, that, therefore better regulate how they move going
which means we never truly get to see what an forward.
actual consistent error looks like. We just see general Now let’s consider an advanced athlete—and
movement variability. I’ve pooled the athletes I work with—when would
As people are learning, they’re going to be doing you want feedback? Think about what you would
all sorts of things wrong. It doesn’t mean they have say for advanced individuals. They don’t want
movement dysfunction. It means they are learning. feedback when they’re doing it right. They don’t want
Again, we allow them to go through three to feedback when they’re doing it wrong. Think of a
five repetitions. We give feedback maybe 33% or great athlete. They know when they’ve done it right.
50% of the time. We actually can get a summary They have enough positive ego and confidence to

~13~
This is the transcript of Nick Winkelman’s Coaching Science—Theory into Practice audio lecture.
If you’ve received this material, do not own the audio lecture and would like to learn about it, visit www.movementlectures.com.
For more from Nick, visit him at facebook.com/nwinkelman.
say, ‘Hey. I hit that one on the head.’ They definitely dysfunction and what are simply symptoms of that.
know when they did it wrong. No great athlete likes It’s no different than if I have pain at my knee, which
a coach to hammer them on something they already is a symptom, but the cause may actually be coming
know. from my hip or my ankle.
The athlete typically wants to know when it’s These principles go from isolated focus all the
ambiguous—when they’re not quite sure. ‘Coach, way down to high-speed running. Within this
that wasn’t bad, but it didn’t quite feel right.’ That’s is descriptive versus prescriptive information.
when they typically want feedback. Prescriptive is better for the beginner. We want to
Try self-selective feedback. It’s really going to tell them exactly what we want them to do right to
tell you a lot about how the athlete or client wants fix the problem. Putting it in a positive connotation
to gain information. It inherently decreases the brings us back to the sippy cup principle.
volume of information and it increases self-efficacy. Descriptive means I can just tell the athletes what
It empowers the athlete. It says, ‘I trust you. I’m going they did wrong. If they have high levels of autonomy
to hand you some ownership today.’ Especially as and I tell them what they did wrong, high-level
they get more and more automony, try to self-select autonomist athletes can handle that because they
the feedback. All of these methods focus you—less know how to fix it. They don’t need you to tell them
volume and a higher quality of information. how to fix it.
When you look at the final ‘identifying the However, as most of us work with the
feedback’ content, this is where the ‘Xs’ and ‘Os’ novice, beginners or intermediate, we want to be
become very important. You have to understand prescriptive. Tell them exactly what you want them
how the movements biomechanically and to do right on the next run. Focus on that. Even if
physiologically are supposed to be performed so we what they’re doing is wrong, is it that bad? Tell them
can focus our attention. they’re doing it right to reinforce that pattern—
We want to understand the major technical cause over symptom. Positive, positive, positive.
components of the movement and understand that Take a look at Attention and Motor Skill Learning
multiple errors will oftentimes be seen, especially to further understand feedback messaging. Again,
when we’re looking at running, linear and multi- the book is by Dr. Gabriel Wolf. This is a must-have,
directional movement. We then want to prioritize easy-to-read resource.
the multiple errors—on which ones we want to Dr. Gabriel Wolf developed the concept of
focus. internal and external cueing when she was wind
It’s no different than the Functional Movement surfing. She has since done all of the research and
Screen. I’m not going to try to fix all seven stemmed all of the research on the concept. Reading
movements. I’m going to prioritize and fix the top her book is like reading a book on gravity by Isaac
one or two. Newton. There’s a direct correlation there—an
It’s the same thing when it comes to movement unbelievable science developed by an unbelievable
real time on the field. We want to prioritize the woman.
multiple errors we see and give them top priority. Many motor learning and control studies are on
We want to direct our feedback at the true weakest non-athletes. They do not factor in movement quality
link—cause versus symptom. This means if I see or the kinematic standpoint. This means I want to
five things going wrong, four of them are probably see that they learn and retain the information, but I
symptoms, but one of them is actually a cause. The also want to see the kinematic or movement quality
better we understand our movement on the field, the information. Thus, more research needs to be done.
better we can understand the causes of movement That’s why more coaches have to get into motor

~14~
This is the transcript of Nick Winkelman’s Coaching Science—Theory into Practice audio lecture.
If you’ve received this material, do not own the audio lecture and would like to learn about it, visit www.movementlectures.com.
For more from Nick, visit him at facebook.com/nwinkelman.
learning and control. We need to not only look at
how our athletes learn, but how they retain optimal
movement patterns, meaning place it on yourself.
Don’t just be proud of practice, but look at how
practice and training actually transfer to the field.
You have to move faster, but then does that actually
results in more stolen bases? The athletes are more
powerful, but can they actually drive the ball further
and more accurately when they’re on the golf course?
Look for transfer.
Take home a message here: Coaching is the
science of creating an environment for optimal
learning. We want to do this by understanding that
performance and learning are limited by attentional
capacity and its constraint on coaching.
This means the more we coach incorrectly, the
more we take their attention away from what they’re
doing. The more they become dependent on us, the
less their ability to intrinsically learn.
Manage attention by managing your coaching.
Modifying practice instructions and feedbacks can
diminish conscious thought and create a causative
load that optimizes learning.
Again, take our framework. Practice, instruction
and feedback create an optimal environment to
optimize the cognitive load to make sure they’re
guided, but can discover the right answer implicitly.
Finally, understand that while the ‘Xs’ and ‘Os’
are important, the ‘Xs’ and ‘Os’ are limited by the
delivery. Team, this is truly the equal sign. Thank
you very much.

~15~
This is the transcript of Nick Winkelman’s Coaching Science—Theory into Practice audio lecture.
If you’ve received this material, do not own the audio lecture and would like to learn about it, visit www.movementlectures.com.
For more from Nick, visit him at facebook.com/nwinkelman.

You might also like