You are on page 1of 12

Scientific Information System

Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and
Portugal

Sabry M. Abd-El-Fattah
Garrison's Model of Self-Directed Learning: Preliminary Validation and Relationship to
Academic Achievement The Spanish Journal of Psychology, vol. 13, núm. 2, 2010, pp.
586-596,
Universidad Complutense de Madrid
España

Available in: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=17217376006

The Spanish Journal of Psychology,


ISSN (Printed Version): 1138-7416
psyjour@sis.ucm.es
Universidad Complutense de Madrid
España

How to cite Complete issue More information about this article Journal's homepage

www.redalyc.org
Non-Profit Academic Project, developed under the Open Acces Initiative
The Spanish Journal of Psychology Copyright 2010 by The Spanish Journal of Psychology
2010, Vol. 13 No. 2, 586-596 ISSN 1138-7416

Garrison’s Model of Self-Directed Learning:


Preliminary Validation and
Relationship to Academic Achievement

Sabry M. Abd-El-Fattah
Minia University (Egypt)

In this project, 119 undergraduates responded to a questionnaire tapping three psychological constructs
implicated in Garrison’s model of self-directed learning: self-management, self-monitoring, and
motivation. Mediation analyses showed that these psychological constructs are interrelated and that
motivation mediates the relationship between self-management and self-monitoring. Path modeling
analyses revealed that self-management and self-monitoring significantly predicted academic
achievement over two semesters with self-management being the strongest predictor. Motivation
significantly predicted academic achievement over the second semester only. Implications of these
findings for self-directed learning and academic achievement in a traditional classroom setting are
discussed.
Keywords: self-directed learning, self-management, self-monitoring, motivation, path modeling, mediation
analysis.

En este trabajo, 119 estudiantes posgraduados fueron evaluados de acuerdo con un cuestionario que
mide tres constructos psicológicos implicados en el modelo de Garrison sobre aprendizaje autodirigido:
autogestión, autoseguimiento, y motivación. Los análisis de mediación mostraron que estos constructos
psicológicos están interrelacionados y que la motivación media en la relación entre autogestión y
autoseguimiento. Los análisis de Path modeling indicaron que la autogestión y el autoseguimiento
predecían significativamente el logro académico a lo largo de dos semestres en los que la autogestión
era el mejor indicador. La motivación predecía significativamente el éxito académico sólo a lo largo
del segundo semestre. Se discuten las implicaciones que estos resultados pueden tener sobre el
aprendizaje autodirigido y el éxito académico en el contexto de un aula tradicional.
Palabras clave: aprendizaje autodirigido, autogestión, autoseguimiento, motivación, Path modeling,
análisis de mediación.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Sabry M. Abd-El-Fattah. Department of Educational Psychology.
Faculty of Education. Minia University. Minia. (Egypt). E-mail: sabryrahma@hotmail.com

586
GARRISON’S MODEL OF SELF-DIRECTED LEARNING 587

Self-directed learning (SDL) is a central concept in learner takes responsibility for the construction of personal
the study and practice of adult education (Brockett & meaning through integrating new ideas and concepts
Hiemstra, 1991). As important as the construct is to adult with previous knowledge. It facilitates a metacognitive
education, little attention has been directed to the cognitive perspective on learning and a generalized ability to
and motivational factors implicated in SDL (Brookfield, learn reflectively. Reflective learning can help “develop
1986; Knowles, 1975). Long (1989, p. 3) stressed the learners who are capable of monitoring themselves in a
importance of the psychological perspective in SDL and variety of situations” (Candy, Harri-Augstein, &Thomas,
argued that “The critical dimension in self-directed learning 1985, p. 115). The teacher can provide effective feedback
is not the sociological variable, nor is it the pedagogical to help the learner self-monitoring the quality (meaning
factor. The main distinction is the psychological variable.” and validity) of the learning outcome because internal
Furthermore, the overriding theme of SDL has mainly feedback alone may lack accuracy and explicitness. The
been the external management of the learning process. degree of self-monitoring will depend upon the learner’s
According to this perspective, the learner exercises a great proficiency (abilities and strategies) in conjunction with
deal of independence in deciding what is worthwhile to contextual and epistemological demands (Butler & Winne,
learn and how to approach the learning tasks, regardless 1995; Garrison, 1991).
of entering competencies and contextual contingencies Lastly, motivation helps initiate and maintain effort
(Brookfield, 1986). As such, SDL has largely been defined towards learning and the achievement of cognitive
in terms of external control and facilitation, rather than goals. Motivation includes entering motivation and task
internal cognitive processing and learning. motivation. Entering motivation establishes commitment
To address these concerns, Garrison (1997) proposed to a particular goal and the intent to act. It can be perceived
a SDL model which integrated external management as “commitment--the coming together of attitudes, feelings,
(contextual control), internal monitoring (cognitive and goals” (Thompson, 1992, p. 103). Task motivation is
responsibility), and motivational (entering and task) factors the tendency to focus on and persist in learning activities
associated with learning in an educational context. Garrison and goals. As Corno (1989) suggested, “Motivational
defined SDL as an approach where learners are motivated factors . . . shape intentions and fuel task involvement”
to assume personal responsibility and collaborative control (pp. 114-115). It is hypothesized that entering motivation
of the cognitive (self-monitoring) and contextual (self- is determined by valence and expectancy. Valence reflects
management) processes in constructing and confirming the attraction to particular learning goals. The factors
meaningful and worthwhile learning outcomes. Garrison’s that determine valence are personal needs (values) and
collaborative perspective of SDL has the learner taking affective states (preferences). In a learning context,
responsibility for constructing meaning (cognitive expectancy refers to the belief that a desired outcome can
perspective) while including the participation of others in be achieved (Pintrich & DeGroot, 1990). Task motivation
confirming worthwhile knowledge (social perspective). is integrally connected to task control, self-management,
According to Garrison (1997), self-management and the issue of volition. In a learning context, “volition
concerns task control issues including the enactment of refers to bringing discordant affective and executional
learning goals and the management of learning resources preferences in line with one’s task goals” (Kanfer, 1989,
and support. The essence of the term can be found in the p. 381). Volition is concerned with sustaining intentional
self-regulated motivational literature (Pintrich & DeGroot, effort or diligence that can influence persistence and task
1990). Task control is determined by balancing the factors performance (Pintrich & DeGroot, 1990).
of proficiency, resources, and interdependence (Garrison, The literature on SDL asserts that self-directed learners
1993). Proficiency represents the abilities and skills of the demonstrate great awareness of their responsibility in
facilitator and the learner. Resources encompass a range of making learning meaningful and monitoring themselves
support and assistance available in an educational setting. (Garrison, 1997). They were found to be curious and willing
Interdependence reflects institutional or subject norms and to try new things, view problems as challenges, desire
standards as well as a learner’s integrity and choice. Self- change, and enjoy learning (Temple & Rodero, 1995).
management of learning tasks represents a collaborative Guthrie, McGough, Bennett, and Rice (1996) reported
experience between the teacher and the learner. The that in a Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction (CORI)
teacher maintains an appropriate dynamic balance of program, self-directed learners demonstrated the ability to
external control necessary for successful educational search for information in multiple texts, employ different
outcomes (Prawat, 1992; Resnick, 1991). strategies to achieve goals, and to represent ideas in different
Another component of Garrison’s SDL model is self- forms (drawing and writing). Morrow and Young (1997)
monitoring. It addresses the cognitive and metacognitive observed that with proper planning and implementation,
processes which include monitoring the repertoire of self-directed learning can encourage students to develop
learning strategies as well as awareness and an ability their own rules and leadership patterns. Thus, self-
to think about our thinking. It is the process whereby the directed learners take responsibility for the construction
588 ABD-EL-FATTAH

of personal meaning, initiation and maintenance of effort the learning process.” Anticipated control is an important
toward learning tasks, and achievement of cognitive goals, perception when assessing expectancy of success and
and therefore they are more likely to be promoted as high making decisions regarding goal-directed behavior. It is
achievers (Garrison, 1997). believed that control expectations “influence the direction
of much of our behavior; they help to determine where we
Linking self-directed learning to academic invest our achievement energies” (Weisz, 1983, p. 234). If
achievement students are to have an expectation of control, they must
have some choice over their educational goals. Providing
For example, several studies have reported a significant opportunities for control and choice from the beginning
positive relationship between SDL and academic can significantly strengthen the entering motivational
achievement in a traditional classroom setting (Darmayanti, state, which subsequently influences whether students will
1994), a non-web based distance learning setting (Hsu & take responsibility to be self-directed and persist in their
Shiue, 2005), a web-based learning setting (Haron, 2003), learning tasks.
and a distant education setting (Harriman, 1990). Other Although the linkages among the psychological traits
studies have reported a significant positive relationship implicated in Garrison’s SDL model may be theoretically
within a specific content areas including nursing (Savoie, meaningful and conceptually sound, such relationships
1979), social and political sciences (Anderson, 1993), have not been empirically validated using real world
business (Morris, 1995), business, communication, public data. In addition, most of the studies on SDL have been
administration, and hospitality management (Ogazon, 1995), conducted using Western samples (Garrison, 1997). It is,
and biology (Haggerty, 2000). In a recent study, Stewart therefore, imperative to examine the practical implications
(2007), for example, found a positive relationship between of Garrison’s SDL model in Non-western contexts to
SDL readiness and the overall learning outcome ratings in determine the generalizability of the findings reported
project-based learning in engineering with self-management using Western samples. Actually, it is possible that careful
being the strongest predictor. Self-control and desire for examination and validation of Garrison’ SDL model in
learning significantly predicted the overall learning outcome Non-western contexts bring about profitable use of the
ratings but their R2 values were relatively low. model and its applications. On the other hand, uncritical
use of Garrison’s SDL model in Non-western contexts
Rational of the study to make students more self-directed learners may not
necessarily result in better academic outcomes, since
Self-monitoring is intimately linked to the external countries may differ in values and beliefs about education
management of reaming tasks and activities. An interesting and opportunities offered to students.
and important issue arises with regard to responsibility Thus, one goal of the present study is to examine the
(self-monitoring) and control (self-management) within relationships among self-management, self-monitoring,
a learning context is whether responsibility must precede and motivational factors, associated with learning in an
control or vice versa. Although theoretically they go hand in educational context. Of particular interest is to investigate
hand, it is very difficult for learners to assume responsibility whether motivational factors may mediate the relationship
for their own learning without feeling they have some between self-management and self-monitoring. A second
control over the educational transaction. Without choice goal of the present study is to examine the relationships of
and collaboration, it may well be unrealistic to expect these psychological constructs to academic achievement
students to assume responsibility for their learning. measured over two semesters in a traditional classroom
Meanwhile, absolute learners’ control may adversely setting. Of particular interest is to relate academic
affect or reduce the efficiency of achieving quality learning achievement measured over two semesters to the earlier
outcomes. There is research evidence that collaborative collected psychological indices.
control results in more effective self- monitoring and,
therefore improved performance. In addition, sharing control Methods
of learning activities and tasks provides opportunities for
instructional support while encouraging students to assume Participants
cognitive responsibility (Butler & Winne, 1995).
The inseparability of monitoring and managing of the Subjects of the present study included 119 first year
learning process is further complicated by motivational undergraduates enrolled in an education program in a
concerns. According to Garrison (1997, p. 9), “Motivation public university in Minia, Egypt. There were 65 males and
reflects perceived value and anticipated success of learning 54 females. Fifty-six students majored in science and 63
goals at the time learning is initiated and mediates between majored in arts. The mean age of the participants was 18.7
context (control) and cognition (responsibility) during years (SD = 1.7) with a range from 18 to 21 years.
GARRISON’S MODEL OF SELF-DIRECTED LEARNING 589

Measurements Results

The Self-Directed Learning Aptitude Scale. The Exploratory factor analysis


instrument most widely used in educational research to
measure SDL readiness is Guglielmino’s (1977) Self- An exploratory factor analysis with principal components
was conducted to identify a viable factor structure of the 28-
Directed Learning Readiness Scale (SDLRS). Based on
item pool of the SDLAS. The resulting factors were rotated
problems with validity testing of the SDLRS, Field (1989)
to a simple structure using oblimin rotation. The number of
suggested discontinuing this tool. Furthermore, several
factors retained was determined using the following criteria:
studies have raised questions about the reliability of the (1) Kaiser’s rule of retaining factors with eigenvalues greater
SDLRS when used in different racial and class populations than 1, (2) Cattell’s Scree test of the plot of eigenvalues, and
(Straka 1995). Even though scales such as the SDLRS (3) each factor had to have at least three items. Inclusion
have been developed, they do not translate easily and are criteria for items on the retained factor were that they had
not readily available and incur a cost for their use. Thus, loadings of at least .30 on that factor. Items with high cross-
the development of a new scale should allow for these loadings, wherein an item had a loading of .30 or greater
problems to be addressed. on more than one factor, were assigned to a factor on the
The Self-Directed Learning Aptitude Scale (SDLAS) basis of logical fit. A corrected item-total correlation of .30
was developed for the purpose of the present study. The or above was required to confirm the assignment decision.
initial item pool comprised 40 items designed to measure The factors that were identified were named on the basis of
students’ aptitude to SDL based on a review of the research their content (Nunnally, 1978).
of Knowles (1975), Brookfield (1986), and Garrison (1991, The analysis retained three factors: self-management (8
1993). Using a Delphi technique (Sharicey & Sharpies, items), motivation (9 items), and self-monitoring (9 items).
2001), the members of an expert panel independently These three factors explained 22, 18, and 15 per cent of
assessed each of the 40 items using a 5-point scale. A score of the total variance extracted respectively. The loadings of
1 denoted Absolutely Inappropriate and a score of 5 denoted two items, “I enjoy studying” and “I would like to make
Absolutely Appropriate. Panel members were given space decisions for myself” fall substantially below the traditional
accepted criteria of .30 for retained item loadings (Velicer
to modify each item if they chose. Following the guidelines
& Jackson, 1990) and therefore were dropped from the
suggested by Kline (1986) and Crocker and Algina (1986),
scale. The 26 items of the SDLAS and their loadings on
an item was retained when the panel consensus for that
corresponding factors are shown in Table 1. There were no
item was of at least 80% of appropriateness. Items where mean differences in self-management, motivation, or self-
consensus was not achieved, but where less than 20% of the monitoring, reported in Table 2, due to the effect of gender
panel denoted Inappropriate (i.e. 80% either Appropriate, or academic major (p > .05).
Absolutely Appropriate, or were Unsure) were retained for
the subsequent round. A total of 28 items were retained after Path modeling
two rounds of the Delphi technique.
The relationships among self-management, motivation,
Procedures self-monitoring, and academic achievement over two
semesters were analyzed through path modeling, using the
partial least squares (PLS) method. This analytical procedure
Students were recruited to participate voluntary during
was chosen because it does not hinge upon large samples, it
their normal classes at their university. All participants were
can be used with both formative and reflective constructs,
informed that they could deny their participation in data
and it does not make assumptions about the underlying data
collection without any explanation, penalty, or cost to them.
distribution when estimating the model parameters (Chin &
The initial 28 items of the SDLAS were administered to the Newsted, 1999; Ringle, Wende, & Will, 2005). The software
sample of the study during the third week of the 2008/2009 used was SmartPLS 2.0 (Ringle et al., 2005).
academic year. Students were asked to describe themselves In practice, a PLS model is developed in two stages.
by indicating on a 4-point Likert-type scale the extent to In the first stage, the measurement model is tested by
which each item was descriptive of their own characteristics performing reliability and discriminative validity analyses
with a score of 1 denoted Strongly Disagree and a sore of on each of the measures to ensure that reliable and valid
4 denoted Strongly Agree. In addition, students’ academic measures of the constructs are being employed. In the
achievement scores over two semesters were obtained, second stage, the inner or structural model is tested by
with permission, from the university records. These scores estimating the paths between the constructs, determining
were the courses aggregated total score (i.e., the sum of on- their significance as well as the predictive ability of the
courses assignments and examinations score). model. The PLS procedure calculates an estimate for each
590 ABD-EL-FATTAH

Table 1
Oblimin factor loadings, PLS loadings, T values, average variance extracted (AVE), and composite and Cronbach’s alpha
reliability coefficients of the Self-Directed Learning Aptitude Scale
Factor PLS T Composite Cronbach’s
Factor/Statistic AVE
loadings loadings values* reliability alpha

Self-management .73 .75 .82

1. I am well-organized in my learning. .83 .81 8.3


8. I set up strict timeframes to learn something new. .80 .77 6.7
14. I have good management skills. .75 .68 10.3
7. I set up planned solutions to solve my problems. .74 .73 9.4
3. I can decide about the priority of my work. .73 .72 11.3
2. I can manage pursuing my own learning. .72 .67 5.5
5. I prefer to plan my own learning. .70 .65 6.4
18. I am efficient in managing my time. .68 .60 8.3

Motivation .70 .80 .84

2. I take the challenge to learn. .80 .76 7.6


15. I am a ‘why’ person. .78 .80 10.8
11. I critically evaluate new ideas and knowledge. .75 .73 5.6
22. I would like to evaluate the level of my learning progress. .71 .65 4.9
26. I would like to learn from my mistakes. .67 .77 7.8
9. I believe in effort to improve my performance. .66 .63 5.6
1. I enjoy learning new things. .65 .62 4.8
17. I trust my abilities to learn new things. .64 .73 8.3
12. I have positive expectations about what I am learning. .62 .75 10.7

Self-monitoring .68 .81 .86

6. I am a ware of my own weaknesses. .81 .76 12.7


4. I can link pieces of information when I am learning. .80 .77 8.5
13. I pay attention to all details before taking a decision. .77 .73 4.7
19. I would like to set up my goals. .75 .72 6.3
23. I correct myself when I make mistakes. .71 .80 8.9
25. I am a responsible person. .69 .74 10.4
21. I judge my abilities fairly. .66 .73 7.5
24. I think deeply when solving a problem. .65 .68 6.6
16. I prefer to set up my criteria to evaluate my performance. .63 .70 10.3
Note. N = 119. *T-values are significant for all instances, p < .01.

construct or latent variable derived from corresponding Since the approach is variance-based (as distinct from
observed variables and thus partitioning the hypothesized covariance-based), the PLS procedure has been described
inner model into its component constructs. To evaluate the as soft modeling, and is claimed to be more useful in
model against observed data, an iterative procedure fits investigating descriptive and predictive relationships
observed measures to corresponding latent variables, then than confirmatory analysis (Sellin & Keeves, 1997). A
estimates the relationships between the latent variables. A readable review of the procedure has been published
least squares fit between observed and modeled parameters by Haenlein and Kaplan (2004). Two recent examples
is computed. A best-fit solution is regarded as found when of researchers using the PLS method within behavioral
the least squares function stabilizes between iterations research are Abd-El-Fattah (2005) and Boman, Smith,
(Ringle et al., 2005). and Curtis (2003).
GARRISON’S MODEL OF SELF-DIRECTED LEARNING 591

Table 2
Gender and academic major mean differences in self-management, motivation, self-monitoring
Variables /Statistic n M SD F (1,117)

Gender

Self-management 1.3
Males 65 23.7 3.4
Females 54 22.9 3.2
Motivation .97
Males 65 24.9 3.6
Females 54 24.3 3.0
Self-monitoring .83
Males 65 26.3 4.2
Females 54 25.7 3.5
Major

Self-management 1.4
Science 56 21.6 2.9
Arts 63 20.9 2.5
Motivation .61
Science 56 25.7 3.8
Arts 63 25.3 3.4
Self-monitoring .67
Science 56 23.7 4.3
Arts 63 23.2 3.8
Note. N = 119. df = 1, 117. * F-values are nonsignificant for all instances, p > .05.

The assessment of the measurement model 1981). Table 1 shows that the AVE for self-management,
motivation, and self-monitoring were > .68.
In the PLS method, a reflective measurement model is For the assessment of discriminant validity of a construct,
assessed by evaluating: (a) the reliability of single items, a rule of thumb recommends accepting a construct if the
(b) the convergent validity of a construct, and (c) the square root of the AVE of that construct is larger than the
discriminant validity of a construct (Hulland 1999). Pearson’s correlation coefficients of that construct to all
For the assessment of individual item reliability, a rule other latent constructs in the model (Fornell & Larcker,
of thumb recommends accepting items with loadings of at 1981). Unfortunately, guidelines about how much larger the
least .5, particularly when new items or newly developed AVE should be than these Pearson’s correlation coefficients
scales are employed (Chin, 1998; Hulland 1999). Table 1 are not available (Gefen, Straub, & Boudreau, 2000).
shows that all items loaded on their corresponding factors Table 3 shows that the square roots of the AVEs for self-
at .60 or higher (t > 4.7, p < .01 for all items). management, motivational factors, and self-monitoring are
For the assessment of convergent validity (also referred greater than the Pearson’s correlation coefficients of these
to as internal consistency) of a construct, a rule of thumb constructs with each other. Based on the findings, one can
recommends accepting a construct with Cronbach’s alpha concluded that the measurement model fits the present data
and/or composite reliability coefficient that exceeds the set adequately.
threshold of .6 as proposed by Bagozzi and Yi (1988), or .7
as suggested by Nunnally (1978). Table 1 shows that both
Cronbach’s alpha and the composite reliability coefficients The assessment of the structural model
for self-management, motivation, and self-monitoring
were > .75. Another index that is used to assess a construct To evaluate the specified structural model, displayed in
convergent validity is the average variance extracted Figure 1, one should report the standardized ß coefficients
(AVE). The AVE measures the amount of variance captured and t values along with the R² (Chin 1998; Hulland 1999).
by the construct relative to the amount of variance due to The standardized ß coefficient is a measure of the strength
measurement error. The AVE should exceed a value of .5 to of the relationship between an independent and a dependent
highlight a construct convergent validity (Fornell & Larcker variable while holding constant the effects of the remaining
592 ABD-EL-FATTAH

Table 3
Pearson’s correlation coefficients, the square root of average variance extracted (AVE), means, and standard deviations of
self-management, motivation, and self-monitoring
Variables 1 2 3 M SD

1. Self-management .87 .36** .31* 3.2 .74


2. Motivation .84 .34** 2.8 .64
3. Self-monitoring .82 3.4 .66
Note. N = 119. Values on diagonal represents the square root of the average variance extracted (AVE) statistic as used within PLS.
Means are expressed along a four point type-Likert scale, 1-4, summed across items.*p < .05; **p < .01.

Item 2 Item15 Item 11 Item 22 Item 26 Item 9 Item 10 Item 17 Item 12

.75
.76 .80 .73 .65 .77 .63 .62 .73

Item 6

Item 1 Motivation .76


R2 = .09
Item 4
.81
.77
Item 8
Item 13
.77
.73
Item 14
.68 Item 19
.72
.30 .29
(t = 6.51) (t = 6.74)
Item 7 .73
.80 Item 23
R2 = .19
.32
.72 Self- (t = 7.53) Self-
Item 3 .74
managment monitoring Item 25
.67
.73
Item 20
.65 .68 Item 21

Item 5 .60
.70 Item 24
.28
(t =

(t = 7.53)
.12 )

Item 18
1.32
(t
.3 .42

Item 16
=
5 )

.31
)
.62

8
=3 1

.36
.3

(t = 5.52)
(t = 7.62)
.41
(t

Achievement (t = 9.33) Achievement


R2 = .30 R2 = .40
Semester1 Semester 2

Figure 1. PLS model of the relationship among self-management, motivation, self-monitoring, achievement in Semester 1, and achievement
Semester 2 (Dashed line indicates nonsignificant effect)

independent variables. The R² represents the proportion of predicted self-monitoring and achievement in Semester 1
variance on a dependent variable explained by all variables (p < .01) but not achievement in Semester 2 (p > .05). Self-
jointly. A t statistics is used to examine the significance of monitoring significantly predicted academic achievement
the parameter estimates (Hulland, 1999). In sum, it was over the two semesters (p < .01). Finally, academic
found that self-management significantly predicted all achievement in Semester1 significantly predicted academic
constructs in the model (p < .01). Motivation significantly achievement in Semester 2.
GARRISON’S MODEL OF SELF-DIRECTED LEARNING 593

Mediation analysis outcomes when they have little control of, and input into,
the learning process (Garrison, 1991, 1997).
According to Baron and Kenny (1986, p. 1176), a However, as necessary as a sense of control is, without
variable may be considered a mediator “to the extent that appropriate support and guidance, learners may not persist
it accounts for the relation between the predictor and the or achieve the desired educational outcomes. From an
criterion”. The mediation model tested in the present study educational perspective, quality of learning outcomes is
conceptually denotes self-management as an independent not simply a question of student control and responsibility.
variable (X), motivational as a mediator variable (M), and The teacher also plays an integral role in education as a
self-monitoring as a dependent variable (Y). Gender and transactional process and has his or her own legitimate and
academic major were also included as covariates (COVs) to often necessary control and responsibility concerns. It is the
test for their possible partial effect on self-monitoring (Y). teacher who is charged with the responsibility of clarifying
The SPSS script developed by Christopher Preacher from goals, shaping learning activities, and assessing learning
the University of Kansas and Andrew Hays from the Ohio outcomes. Furthermore, in collaboration with students,
State University (Preacher & Hayes, 2004) was used to run teachers are responsible for establishing the balance of
the mediation analysis and the results are summarized in control by providing support, directions, and standards to
Table 4. ensure worthwhile outcomes and continuing efforts to learn
Baron and Kenny technique showed that self- (Garrison, 1993).
management significantly predicted self-monitoring (b = .78, The second notable finding concerned motivation as
SE = .07, t = 11.14, p < .01), self-management significantly a mediator of the relationship between self-management
predicted motivation (b = .67, SE =.08, t = 8.38, p < .01), and self-monitoring. Learners are intrinsically motivated
and motivation significantly predicted self-monitoring to assume responsibility for constructing meaning and
(b = .74, SE = .07, t = 10.57, p < .01). When motivation understanding when they have some control over the learning
was controlled for, self-management significantly predicted experience. Garrison (1997) theorized that “Motivation
reflects perceived value and anticipated success of learning
self-monitoring (b = .66, SE = .08, t = 8.25, p < .01). Gender
goals at the time learning is initiated and mediates between
(b = .13, SE = .11, t = 1.18) and academic major (b = .17,
context (control) and cognition (responsibility) during the
SE = .10, t = 1.70), as covariates, did not have significant
learning process.” (p. 9). Garrison (1993) also proposed
effects on self-monitoring (p > .05).
that “Control and choice strengthen motivation, which in
The Sobel test showed identical findings to that of Baron
turn builds a sense of responsibility” (p. 30) Consistent
and Kenny technique (effect (z) = .55, SE = .10, z = 5.5, p <
with Peters (1998), consideration should be given to
.05). The ‘effect’ in Sobel test represents the indirect effect
metacognitive and motivational factors if SDL is to be a
of the mediator. This indirect effect is the product of paths
viable and relevant concept to education in 21st century. It
ab and is equivalent to (c - c’) where path c is the total effect
would seem that, for any student, SDL is predicated on the
and c’ is the direct effect (Sobel, 1982).
student having earned how to learn and having acquired the
The bootstrapping technique (Chernick, 1999) further
necessary epistemic and metacognitive awareness. If SDL is
supported the mediation effect of motivation with a value about controlling one’s learning and achieving meaningful
of zero not included within the lower bound (.26) and the and worthwhile outcomes, then epistemic cognition (i.e.,
upper bound (.73) of the 95% bias-corrected and accelerated cognitive development), metacognitive awareness, and
bootstrap confidence intervals. Based on these findings, one motivational factors are preconditions.
can conclude that the motivation mediated the relationship Garrison’s (1997) model of SDL has considerable
between self-management and self-monitoring. potentials to integrate issues related to motivational
disposition (entering task), strategic planning (self-
Discussion management), and metacognitive awareness (self-
monitoring). It is strongly suggested that consideration
The present analyses revealed several notable be given to these higher-order cognitive dispositions and
findings. First, self-management significantly predicted perspectives so that SDL can truly reflect autonomous
self-monitoring. This means that increasing a learner’s learning. For SDL to have real pragmatic value in an
control through self-management brings with it elevated educational context, the focus must shift to cognitive
responsibilities, particularly with regard to the learning responsibility issues and transactional support for critical
process itself and the construction of meaning. The reflection. The ability of the teacher to provide control of
immediate benefit of increased self-management is the external learning tasks through well-designed learning
increased awareness of the need to make learning more packages must be balanced against concerns associated
meaningful, that is, to take greater responsibility in the with modeling and diagnosing the internal cognitive (i.e.,
monitoring of the learning process itself. It is difficult to critically reflective) processes leading to higher-order
get learners to accept responsibility for meaningful learning outcomes (learning to learn).
594 ABD-EL-FATTAH

Table 4
Summary of results of the mediation analyses based on Baron and Kenny technique, Sobel test, and bootstrapping
Technique Statistics

Baron and Kenny

b SE T
Total effect path c
Self-management (IV) > Self-monitoring (DV) .78 .07 11.14**
Path a
Self-management (IV) > Motivation (M) .67 .08 8.38**
Path b
Motivation (M) > Self-monitoring (DV) .74 .07 10.57**
Direct effect path c'
Self-management (IV) > Self-monitoring (DV) .66 .08 8.25**
Partial effect
Gender (COV) > Self-monitoring (DV) .13 .11 1.18
Academic major (COV) > Self-monitoring (DV) .17 .10 1.70
Sobel Test

Path ab
Indirect effects of IV on DV through proposed mediators
Effect SE Z
Total .55 .10 5.5*
Motivation .55 .10 5.5*
Bootstrapping

Path ab
Indirect effects of IV on DV through proposed mediators
Data Boot Bias SE
Total .65 .63 .02 .11
Motivation .65 .63 .02 .11
Bias Corrected and Accelerated Confidence Intervals
Lower Upper
Total .26 .73
Motivation .26 .73
Note. N = 119. ‘Data’ is the indirect effect calculated in the original sample, ‘Boot’ is the mean of the indirect effect estimates calculated
across all bootstrap samples. ‘Bias’ is the difference between “Data” and “Boot”. “SE” is the standard deviation of the bootstrap
estimates of the indirect effect. This standard deviation could be used as a bootstrap-derived estimate of the standard error of the indirect
effect. *p < .05, **p < .01.

Finally, the analyses of the present study indicated These findings can also be explained within the framework
a relationship between SDL aptitude and academic of the self-regulated motivational literature which indicates
achievement. Specifically, self-management was marked that successful learners have more effective and efficient
as the strongest predictor of academic achievement over learning strategies for accessing and using their knowledge,
Semester 1 and Semester 2, followed by self-monitoring. are self-motivated, and can monitor and change their
However, motivation significantly predicted academic strategies to improve their learning outcomes (Corno,
achievement over the second semester only. These findings 1989; Pintrich & DeGroot, 1990). When learners recognize
are consistent with what Stewart (2007) has recently their learning needs, formulate learning objectives, select
reported that self-management was the strongest predictor contents, draw up learning strategies, procure teaching
of overall learning outcome ratings in project-based materials and media, identify additional human and physical
learning in engineering and that self-control and desire for resources and make use of them, and they themselves
learning had a positive relationship with overall learning organize, control, inspect, and evaluate their own learning,
outcome ratings but their R2 values were relatively low. they are more likely to perform highly on learning tasks.
GARRISON’S MODEL OF SELF-DIRECTED LEARNING 595

Zimmerman, Bonner, and Kovach (1996) suggested that Chin, W. W. (1998). The partial least squares approach for
one of the major advantages of using the self-management structural equation modeling. In G. A. Macoulides (Ed.),
process is that it can improve not only one’s learning, but Modern methods for business research (pp. 295-336).
it can enhance one’s perception of self-confidence and Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
control over the learning process. When one practices self- Chin, W. W., & Newsted, P. R. (1999). Structural equation
observing of his or her current learning, one can determine modeling analysis with small samples using partial least
about the effective learning strategies to use. This process squares. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Statistical strategies for small
can help one achieve at higher levels and become a more sample research (pp. 307-341). Thousand Oaks: CA: Sage
self-directed and self-regulated learner. Publications.
In conclusion, self-direction is seen as essential if Corno, L. (1989). Self-regulated learning: A volitional analysis.
students are to achieve the ultimate educational goal of In B. J. Zimmerman, & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated
becoming continuous learners. Learning interests and learning and academic achievement: Theory, research, and
opportunities for control can promote self-direction and practice (pp. 111-141). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Crocker, L., & Algina, J. (1986). Introduction to classical
continued learning. Opportunities for self-directed learning,
and modern test theory. Forth Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace
in turn, can enhance metacognitive awareness and facilitate
Jovanovich College Publishers.
the conditions where students can learn how to learn.
Darmayanti, T. (1994). Readiness for self-directed learning
and achievement of the students of Universitas Terbuka
References (Indonesian Open University). (Unpublished master´s thesis).
University of Victoria, British Columbia, Canadá.
Abd-El-Fattah, S. M. (2005). The effect of prior experience with Field, L. (1989). An investigation into the structure, validity, and
computers, statistical self-efficacy, and computer anxiety on reliability of Guglielmino’s Self-Directed Learning Readiness
students’ achievement in an introductory statistics course: A Scale. Adult Education Quarterly, 39, 125-139.
partial least squares path analysis. International Education Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation
Journal, 5, 71-79. models with unobservable variables and measurement error.
Anderson, M. R. (1993). Success in distance education courses Journal of Marketing Research, 18, 39-5.
versus traditional classroom education courses. (Unpublished Garrison, D. R. (1991). Critical thinking and adult education: A
doctoral thesis). Oregon State University. conceptual model for developing critical thinking in adult
Bagozzi, R. P., & Youjae, Y. (1988). On the evaluation of structural learners. International Journal of Lifelong Education, 10,
equation models. Journal of the Academy of Marketing 287-303.
Science, 16, 74-94. Garrison, D. R. (1993). An analysis of the control construct in self-
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator directed learning. In H. B. Long (Ed.), Emerging perspectives
variable distinction in social psychological research: of self-directed learning (pp. 27-44): Oklahoma: Research
Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal Center for Continuing Professional and Higher Education of
of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182. the University of Oklahoma.
Boman, P., Smith, D. C., & Curtis, D. (2003). Effects of pessimism Garrison, D. R. (1997). Self-directed learning: Toward a
and explanatory style on development of anger in children. comprehensive model. Adult Education Quarterly, 48, 18-33.
School Psychology International, 24, 80-94. Gefen, D., Straub, D., & Boudreau, M. (2000). Structural equation
Brockett, R. G., & Hiemstra, R. (1991). Self-direction in adult modeling and regression: Guidelines for research practice.
learning: Perspective on theory, research, and practice. New Communications of the Association for Information Systems,
York: Routledge. 7, 1-78.
Brookfield, S. (1985). Self-directed learning: A critical review of Guglielmino, L. M. (1977). Development of the Self-Directed
research. In S. Brookfiled (Ed.), Self-directed learning from Learning Readiness Scale. (Unpublished doctoral thesis).
theory to practice (pp. 5-16). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. University of Georgia.
Brookfield, S. D. (1986). Understanding and facilitating adult Guthrie, J. T., McGough, K., Bennett, L., & Rice, M. E. (1996).
learning. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Concept-oriented reading instruction: An integrated
Butler, D. L., & Winne, P. H. (1995). Feedback and self-regulated curriculum to develop motivations and strategies for reading.
learning: A theoretical synthesis. Review of Educational In L. Baker, P. Afflerbach, & D. Reinking (Eds.), Developing
Research, 65, 245-281. engaged readers in school and home communities (pp. 165-
Candy, P., Harri-Augstein, S., & Thomas, L. (1985). Reflection and 190). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
the self-organized learner: A model of learning organizations. Haggerty, D. L. (2000). Engaging adult learners in self-directed
In D. Boud, R. Keough, & D. Walker (Eds.), Reflection: learning and its impact on learning styles. (Unpublished
Turning experience into learning (pp. 100-116). London: doctoral thesis). University of New Orleans.
Kogan Page. Haenlein, M., & Kaplan, A. M. (2004). A beginner’s guide to
Chernick, M. R. (1999). Bootstrap methods: A practitioner’s partial least squares analysis. Understanding Statistics, 3,
guide. New York: Wiley series in probability and statistics. 283–297.
596 ABD-EL-FATTAH

Haron, S. (2003). The Relationship between readiness and Prawat, R. S. (1992). Teachers’ beliefs about teaching and
facilitation of self-directed learning and academic learning: A constructivist perspective. American Journal of
achievement: A comparative study of web-based distance Education, 100, 354-395.
learning models of two universities. (Unpublished doctoral Resnick, L. B. (1991). Shared cognition: Thinking as social
thesis). University of Putra, Malaysia. practice. In L. B. Resnick, J. M. Levine, & S. D. Teasley
Harriman, J. K. (1990). The relationship between self-directed (Eds.), Perspectives on socially shared cognition (pp. 1-20).
learning readiness, completion, and achievement in a Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
community college telecourse program. (Unpublished Ringle, C. M., Wende, S., & Will, A. (2005). SmartPLS 2.0 (M3)
doctoral thesis). University of Georgia. Beta. Hamburg, Germany: University of Hamburg.
Hsu, Y. C., & Shiue, Y. M. (2005). The effect of self-directed Savoie, M. M. (1979). Continuing education for nurses:
learning readiness on achievement comparing face-to-face Predictors of success in courses requiring a degree of learner
and two-way distance learning instruction. International self-direction. (Unpublished doctoral thesis). University of
Journal of Instructional Media, 32, 143-155. Toronto, Canadá.
Hulland, J. (1999). Use of partial least squares (PLS) in strategic Sellin, N., & Keeves, J. (1997). Path analysis with latent variables.
management research: A review of four recent studies. In J. P. Keeves (Ed.), Educational research, methodology,
Strategic Management Journal, 20, 195-204. and measurement: An international handbook (pp. 633–640).
Oxford, UK: Pergamon.
Kanfer, R. (1989). Cognitive processes, dispositions, and
Sharicey, S. B., & Sharpies, A. Y. (2001). An approach to
behavior: Connecting the dots within and across paradigms.
consensus building using the Delphi technique: Developing
In R. Kanfer, P. L. Ackerman, & R. Cudeck (Eds.), Abilities,
a learning resource in mental health. Nurse Education Today,
motivation, and methodology (pp. 375-388). Hillsdale, NJ:
21, 398-34.
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect
Kline, P. (1986). A Handbook of test construction: Introduction to
effects in structural equation models. In S. Leinhardt (Ed.),
psychometric design. London: Methuen.
Sociological methodology (pp. 290-312). Washington DC:
Knowles, M. (1975). Self-directed learning. New York:
American Sociological Association.
Association Press.
Stewart, R. A. (2007). Investigating the link between self-directed
Long, H. B. (1989). Self-directed learning: Merging theory and
learning readiness and project-based learning outcome: The
practice. In H. B. Long (Ed.), Self-directed learning: Merging case of international masters students in an engineering
theory and practice (pp. 1-12). Oklahoma: Research Center management course. European Journal of Engineering
for Continuing Professional and Higher Education of the Education, 32, 453-465.
University of Oklahoma. Straka, G. A. (1995). Problems of measuring self-directed learning
Morris, L. S. (1995). The relationship between self-directed readiness. Paper presented at the Asia-Pacific Seminar on
learning readiness and academic performance in a Self-directed Learning, Seoul, Korea.
nontraditional higher education program. (Unpublished Temple, C., & Rodero, M. L. (1995). Active learning in a democratic
doctoral thesis). University of Oklahoma. classroom: The “Pedagogical invariants” of Celesting Freinet
Morrow, L. M., & Young, J. (1997). Parent, teacher, and child (Reading around the world), Reading Teacher, 49, 164-167.
participation in a collaborative family literacy program: The Thompson, D. (1992). Beyond motivation: Nurses’ participation
effects of attitude, motivation, and literacy achievement. and persistence in baccalaureate nursing programs. Adult
Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 736-742. Education Quarterly, 42, 94-105.
Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory (2 ed.). New York: Velicer, W. F., & Jackson, D. N. (1990). Component analysis
McGraw-Hill. versus common factor analysis: Some issues in selecting an
Ogazon, A. G. (1995). The contribution of self-directed learning appropriate procedure. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25,
readiness to the achievement of junior students at a branch of 1-28.
the state of Florida university system. (Unpublished doctoral Weisz, J. R. (1983). Can I control it? The pursuit of veridical
thesis). Florida International University. answers across the life-span. In P. B. Baltes, & O. G. Brim
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (pp. 233-300).
for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. New York: Academic Press.
Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, Zimmerman, B. J., Bonner, S., & Kovach, R. (1996). Developing
717-731. self-regulated learners: Beyond achievement to self-efficacy.
Peters, O. (1998). Learning and teaching in distance education: Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Analyses and interpretations from an international perspective.
London: Kogan Page.
Pintrich, P. R., & DoGroot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and Received June 2, 2009
self-regulated learning components of classroom academic Revision received November 11, 2009
performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 33-4. Accepted December 18, 2009

You might also like