You are on page 1of 15

Journal of Geography

ISSN: 0022-1341 (Print) 1752-6868 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rjog20

Spatial Thinking Assists Geographic Thinking:


Evidence from a Study Exploring the Effects of
Geospatial Technology

Sandra Metoyer & Robert Bednarz

To cite this article: Sandra Metoyer & Robert Bednarz (2017) Spatial Thinking Assists Geographic
Thinking: Evidence from a Study Exploring the Effects of Geospatial Technology, Journal of
Geography, 116:1, 20-33, DOI: 10.1080/00221341.2016.1175495

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/00221341.2016.1175495

Published online: 19 May 2016.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 1409

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 18 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rjog20
Spatial Thinking Assists Geographic Thinking: Evidence from a Study
Exploring the Effects of Geospatial Technology
Sandra Metoyer and Robert Bednarz

ABSTRACT INTRODUCTION
This article provides a description and Research in geography education supports the belief that spatial thinking is
discussion of an exploratory research crucial to academic and career success in geography and other spatially depen-
study that examined the effects of using dent sciences (Pallrand and Seeber 1984; Kali and Orion 1996; Hsi, Linn, and
geospatial technology (GST) on high Bell 1997) and that spatial thinking is malleable; it can be improved through
school students’ spatial skills and spa- education and training (Terlecki 2004; Shin 2007; Wright et al. 2008; Lee and Bed-
tial-relations content knowledge. It
narz 2009). Research also suggests that tools such as geospatial technologies
presents results that support the use of
GST to teach spatially dependent con- (GST), for example virtual globes and geographic information systems (GIS),
tent. It also provides indication of an support and facilitate the acquisition of spatial thinking (Committee on Support
important link between spatial thinking for Thinking Spatially, National Research Council 2006). Going a step further,
and geographic thinking. The article the current study suggests a correlation between thinking spatially and
concludes with a discussion of how thinking geographically. Spatial thinking is a combination of cognitive skills
these results inform instructional strate- comprised of knowing concepts of space, using tools of representation, and
gies for the teaching of geography. applying processes of reasoning. Geographic thinking extrapolates beyond
spatial thinking to encompass the recognition and elaboration of the rela-
Key Words: geography education, spatial tions among spatial concepts, the advanced associations derived from these
thinking, geographic thinking concepts, and the formal linking of the associations into theories and
geospatial technology generalizations.
The purpose of this exploratory study is to examine the influence of GST as an
instructional tool on the development of spatial skills and the understanding of
a spatially dependent geography topic, central place theory, within an authentic
classroom context. A quasi-experimental design was used to compare three
groups: an intervention group using GST, a comparison group using traditional
paper-and-pencil maps, and a control group. Groups’ spatial skills, spatial atti-
tudes, and content knowledge were assessed.
This article discusses the impact of instruction with GST on spatial skills
and spatial-relations content knowledge. In addition, the influence of having
a high starting level of spatial skill on gains in spatial-relations content
knowledge is compared to low or average skill groups. Findings suggest a
correlation between thinking spatially and thinking geographically. Does
instruction with geospatial technologies promote the acquisition of spatially
dependent content? Can instruction utilizing GST facilitate the development
of spatial and geographic thinking? These questions are explored and the
article concludes with a discussion of implications and recommendations for
geography education.

Sandra Metoyer is grants writer in the


Office of Development at Galveston College, BACKGROUND
Galveston, Texas, USA. Her recent work has
focused on developing sponsored program Spatial Thinking
and scholarship opportunities for historically Although the characterization of spatial thinking is still debated, researchers
underrepresented students in higher educa- agree that spatial thinking is a distinct cognitive ability separate from general
tion through public and private intelligence (Thorndike 1921; Eliot 1987; Brosnan 1998; Kyllonen and Gluck
partnerships.
2003). The body of knowledge regarding when and how spatial thinking devel-
ops is growing, but no model explaining its development is widely accepted.
Robert Bednarz is professor of geography at
Texas A&M University, College Station, For example, researchers have repeatedly studied the impact of nature versus
Texas, USA. His recent research has focused nurture in an effort to understand the roles innate ability and acquired skills
on spatial thinking, the impact of using geo- play in the development of spatial thinking without reaching consensus.
spatial technologies on spatial-thinking, and Spatial thinking, as defined by the National Research Council’s Committee on
the assessment of spatial-thinking skills. Support for Thinking Spatially, is a constructive combination of cognitive skills

Journal of Geography 116: 20–33


Ó 2017 National Council for Geographic Education 20
Spatial Thinking Assists Geographic Thinking

comprised of knowing concepts of space, using tools of correlated to success in geography and other sciences
representation, and applying processes of reasoning (Pallrand and Seeber 1984; Kali and Orion 1996; Hsi, Linn,
(Committee on Support for Thinking Spatially, National and Bell 1997). Spatial thinking is an essential set of skills
Research Council 2006, 12). Spatial thinking allows people necessary for the development and application of GIS
to use space to model the world (real and theoretical) to (Sanchez and Wiley 2010). Spatial thinking can be
structure problems, find answers, and express and com- improved with training and/or direct instruction (Baen-
municate solutions. The inclusion of concepts of space ninger and Newcombe 1989). Various human cognitive
makes spatial thinking unique from other types of cogni- factors and life events contribute to the development of
tion (Committee on Support for Thinking Spatially, spatial thinking (e.g., spatial vocabulary, play experiences,
National Research Council 2006). memory capacity) (Levine et al. 2005; Dasen and Mishra
Concepts of space are declarative forms of knowledge 2010). And, sex differences (often minimized with training
that are the building blocks for spatial thinking such as or instruction) exist for some types of spatial thinking and
location, dimensionality, continuity, pattern, spatial asso- under certain conditions (Ward, Newcombe, and Overton
ciation, network, and proximity (Lloyd, Patton and Cam- 1986; Halpern 2000).
mack 1996; Golledge 2002; Gersmehl and Gersmehl 2007; Little consensus exists regarding the effect of spatial-
Janelle and Goodchild 2009). thinking training on the ability to learn, understand, and
Using tools of representation is necessary for compe- apply discipline-specific spatial concepts nor the effect of
tency in spatial thinking. Representations, such as maps, using GST, as an instructional tool, on spatial thinking or
graphs, sketches, diagrams, flow charts, images, and mod- on discipline-specific content learning. Some strategies
els, are used in a variety of modes (mental images, visual and methods for teaching and learning spatial thinking
media, tactile, auditory, and kinesthetic) to identify, have been developed, but few studies have examined the
describe, explain, and communicate information about impact of the instructional strategies on spatial thinking
objects and their associated spatial relationships (Commit- and discipline-specific content knowledge (for an excep-
tee on Support for Thinking Spatially, National Research tion, again see Lee and Bednarz 2009).
Council 2006).
Spatial thinking often necessitates complex reasoning Geographic Thinking
(Jo and Bednarz 2009). Reasoning is the capacity of indi- Geographic knowledge is the declarative knowledge of
viduals to think, to make sense of the world, and to space and the intellectual knowledge about space. It is a
understand. Processes of reasoning are crucial for learn- product of geographic thinking and processes of reason-
ing as individuals obtain, change, or justify practices, ing (Golledge 2002). Geographic thinking requires intellec-
institutions, and beliefs (Kompridis 2000). Processes of tual knowledge about space. Knowledge about space
reasoning include low levels of thinking, such as recog- consists of the recognition and elaboration of the rela-
nizing, defining, and listing, as well as higher levels of tions among spatial concepts, the advanced associations
thinking, such as evaluating, synthesizing, and generaliz- derived from these concepts, and the formal linking of
ing (Jo and Bednarz 2009). To reiterate, spatial thinking is the associations into theories and generalizations. Geo-
defined as a combination of these three components: spa- graphic thinking, and therefore intellectual knowledge
tial concepts, tools of representation, and processes of about space, “extrapolates far beyond simple sensory
reasoning. or observational information” (Golledge 2002, 1). In
Spatial thinking is important for academic success in geography this extrapolation is captured in various
geography and other sciences, yet effective methods for tools of representation such as maps and GIS. These
explicitly teaching spatial thinking do not exist. Evidence representations can then be analyzed and interpreted
of transfer from training in spatial skills to changes in the in order to understand spatial patterns and spatial
ability to understand and solve discipline-specific spatial relations.
problems is scarce (for an exception see Talley 1973). It would seem logical that geographic thinking requires
Many have proposed using GIS to improve spatial skills spatial thinking. Spatial thinking, on the other hand, does
and spatial thinking (Committee on Support for Thinking not necessitate geographic thinking. Geographic thinking
Spatially, National Research Council 2006). More specifi- is the application of spatial thinking to address complex
cally, researchers have argued that GIS helps students geographic concepts or problems. As an example, deter-
develop spatial abilities, solve spatial problems, reason mining an ideal and profitable location for a new store
spatially, and improve map-reading skills (Albert and requires geographic thinking. Competent geographic
Golledge 1999; Forer and Unwin 1999; Hall-Wallace and thinking in this example is dependent upon spatial-think-
McAuliffe 2002; Kerski 2003) although little evidence ing processes such as recognizing patterns and making
exists to support these arguments (for an exception see spatial associations. Recognizing patterns such as density
Lee and Bednarz 2009). or hexagons (market areas) among existing stores requires
Although the understanding of some aspects of spatial spatial thinking. It does not require geographic thinking.
thinking is incomplete and not confirmed, research sup- Even though evidence exists to support the correlation
ports the following. High spatial thinking is positively between high spatial-thinking ability and success in

21
Sandra Metoyer and Robert Bednarz

geography and other spatially dependent sciences, little automatically transferred to a regular geography class
evidence exists to support the reverse relationship. Both of for the second semester. This study occurred late in the
these relationships were explored in this study by assess- second semester. Students still enrolled in an honor
ing students’ spatial-thinking skills prior to the introduc- class at that time were assumed to be successful in the
tion of a novel spatially dependent concept and then, course, thus indicating both motivation and academic
following instruction, comparing changes in their spatial- ability. Student participation within these classes was
relations content knowledge. solicited and acknowledged with a five-dollar gift certif-
icate to a local vendor. Three of the five honor classes
METHOD were taught by one teacher, and two classes by a second
A two-day instructional unit focused on central place teacher. Both teachers had several years of experience
theory was developed as an intervention in order to teaching honor-level geography and were recognized as
understand the impact GST has on students’ spatial excellent teachers by the school district.
thinking and on their understanding of a geographic Each class started with approximately twenty-three stu-
concept. The primary purpose of the intervention was dents with relatively equal numbers of males and females.
to provide students with (1) instruction on central place One class (n D 20 after attrition) was reserved as a control
theory explicitly focusing on spatial-thinking strategies class in which no instruction on spatial-thinking strategies
and (2) intentional practice with spatial thinking or content related to central place theory was presented.
through GST. Three primary research questions defined Figure 1 illustrates the sampling design in which the
the design for the study. First, what is the effect of remaining students (n D 82 after attrition) were divided
using GST as an instructional tool on students’ spatial- into the four treatment classes by sex then randomly
thinking skills? Second, what is the effect of using GST assigned to either the intervention group (using digital
as an instructional tool on students’ content knowledge maps and GST) or the comparison group (nondigital,
of a spatially dependent concept? And third, what is paper-and-pencil maps). Stratified random sampling
the relationship between spatial thinking and geo- ensured equal gender representation by class period in
graphic thinking? the two groups. As a result of attrition caused by absences
Three assessments were used to examine students’ spa- during portions of the activity or during the posttesting,
tial thinking and spatial relations content knowledge: a 102 high school students participated in the full study:
spatial-visualization skills test, a spatial-orientation skills 48 females and 54 males.
test, and a spatial-relations content knowledge test. Class-
room observations by two third-party observers and by Instruments
the researcher informed a qualitative description of the Pre- and postassessments were obtained from partici-
instructional unit and intervention. Statistical methods pating students for spatial-visualization skills, spatial-
were used to compare scores among different participant orientation skills, and spatial-relations content knowl-
groups and to explore potential relationships. edge. Practice items were administered prior to the
timed portion of each spatial-skill test (visualization
Setting and Participants and orientation) to prevent any misunderstanding or
The study was set in a Texas urban public high school. confusion about the instructions or the task. Nineteen
Permissions to conduct the research were secured from an days following the instructional activity and twenty-
institutional review board and from the school district five days after the pretests, posttests were administered
following a research application process and committee for spatial-visualization skills, spatial-orientation skills,
review. The classes recruited for the study were five and spatial-relations content knowledge (Table 1). The
tenth grade world geography honor classes. Honor time gap between pretests and posttests was inten-
classes were intentionally utilized in an effort to con- tionally long in an attempt to diminish test-retest
trol, to some extent, for differences in motivation and effects.
academic success. Motivation is a crucial factor in aca-
demic success. Educators typically encounter two Spatial Visualization
types; motivation arising from a conditioned expecta- Mental rotation from an allocentric viewpoint is an
tion of academic success and intrinsic motivation aris- example of spatial visualization (Golledge and Stimson
ing from a personal interest in the subject or activity 1997; Hegarty and Waller 2004; Zacks and Tversky
(Mills 1991). Equivalent motivation of the first type and 2005). It requires the mental manipulation of an object
effective study habits were assumed among the partici- or array of objects with no imagined change to the
pants because all were honor students with good aca- individual’s viewpoint or orientation. Mental rotation
demic standing. of both two-dimensional and three-dimensional objects
Honor classes are open the first semester to all stu- is a standard test for spatial visualization that has been
dents regardless of grade point average or test scores. used in many studies measuring spatial skills (Voyer,
However, if a student is unsuccessful in the honor Voyer, and Bryden 1995; Burton 2003; Quaiser-Pohl
class during the first semester, that individual is 2003).

22
Spatial Thinking Assists Geographic Thinking

along one axis but not flipped, it


would be marked S for same. If
the object was flipped creating a
mirror image of the example
item, it would be marked D for
different (Fig. 2). Twenty-four
items of this type appeared on the
pre- and posttests for two-dimen-
sional visualization.
Three-dimensional test items
selected to assess spatial-visuali-
zation skills, cubes marked with
patterns of either round dots in
a fashion similar to dice or with
six unique geometric shapes,
were modified from Ekstrom,
French, and Harmon (1976). The
participant was required to
compare an example cube to
three other cubes that were the
exact same shape and size but
had been rotated sequentially
one face at a time. The task was
to number the three comparison
items in the correct order that
the cube was rotated. Three of
six faces were visible on each
cube. The cube could turn or
rotate only one face at a time,
but it could be rotated in any
direction or along any axis
(Fig. 3). Both the pretest and
posttest were comprised of
fourteen items of this type;
eight items with dots on the
cube faces and six items with
unique geometric shapes.
Participants were given four
minutes to complete the 2-D and
3-D visualization tests, two
minutes for each. Before each
test, the researcher explained the
solution for one item to ensure
Figure 1. Sampling design. Four world regional geography honor-level classes that participants knew what was
were split into an intervention group (n D 41) and a comparison group (n D 41). expected and to answer any
One world regional geography honor-level class was reserved as a control group questions about the test prior to
(n D 20). the start. Participants were
instructed to complete as many
items as possible (maximum of
Two-dimensional items selected for this study, simple twenty-four items for 2-D and fourteen items for 3-D)
geometric shapes, were modified from Ekstrom, French, with the greatest degree of accuracy within the two-min-
and Harmon (1976). The exercise required the participants ute time limit.
to compare an example item to four other items that were The 2-D and 3-D spatial-visualization tests were
the same shape and size but had been rotated (trans- given as a pre- and posttest. The pre- and posttests
formed on one axis) or rotated and flipped (transformed contained the same number and type of items. The
on two axes). The task required the students to distinguish sequence of items and sequence of answer choices for
similar from different shapes. If the object was rotated posttest differed from the pretest. Accuracy, the

23
Sandra Metoyer and Robert Bednarz

percentage of attempted items cor-


rectly answered, determined partic- Table 1. List of instruments.
ipants’ performance on the spatial-
visualization skills tests.
Instrument Name Placement Source Type of Data
Spatial Orientation Spatial Visualization Pre and Post Adapted from Ekstrom, Quantitative
Spatial orientation is “the ability to Skills: Mental French, and Harmon (1976) Timed (2 min.)
imagine how configurations of ele- Rotation Same/Different
ments would appear from different Spatial Orientation Pre and Post Adapted from Hegarty Quantitative
perspectives” (Golledge and Stimson Skills: Object- and Waller (2004) Timed (2 min.)
1997, 158). It requires spatial reorien- Perspective Test Angular measure
tation with respect to one’s own Spatial Relations Pre and Post Developed for Quantitative
body, that is, mental movement of Content Test this study Not timed
one’s self. Multiple choice
Assessing spatial-orientation skill
using paper-and-pencil items is dif-
ficult because the representation of a large three-dimen- correct measured angle and the angle drawn by the partici-
sional area is illustrated in a small two-dimensional pant. These differences were converted to a normalized
space. Spatial orientation is often measured using an mean-percent-accuracy score.
authentic performance such as pointing or walking to
indicate a bearing or direction. Hegarty and Waller Spatial-Relations Content Knowledge
(2004) developed a paper-and-pencil test, the Object Spatial relations is a spatial ability requiring higher-
Perspective Test, to assess orientation as an egocentric level processes of reasoning involving spatial concepts
transformation skill measuring an individual’s reorien- and the possibility of multiple-solution strategies. Spatial-
tation ability. Analysis demonstrates this task is inde- relations thinking requires spatial visualization, spatial
pendent from mental rotation tasks, indicating it is a orientation, or both as foundational spatial-thinking skills.
separate and measurable spatial skill (Hegarty and Spatial-relations thinking necessitates higher-level pro-
Waller 2004). The Object Perspective Test was utilized cesses of reasoning to understand relationships and/or pat-
in this study to measure orientation skills of the terns among different spatially arranged objects. Change in
participants. students’ spatial-relations content knowledge was used as
Both the pre- and post- spatial-orientation tests con- a proxy for geographic thinking.
sisted of twenty items. Pre- and posttests were comprised Spatial relations include a broad and diverse set of
of the same items, but they were presented in a different higher-level, spatial-thinking skills. The test items devel-
order. Participants were required to imagine standing at oped for the spatial-relations content test were based on
one feature on the map and imagine facing a second fea- (1) spatial operations described in Learning to Think Spa-
ture. They then pointed to a third feature by drawing a line tially: GIS as a Support System in the K–12 Curriculum
to indicate the angle of difference
between the feature they were fac-
ing and the feature they were point-
ing to (Fig. 4).
Participants were given two
minutes to complete the spatial-
orientation test. Before the test,
the researcher worked through
an example to ensure the partici-
pants knew what was expected
and to answer any questions.
Participants were instructed to
complete as many items as possi-
ble with the greatest degree of
accuracy within the two-minute
time limit.
Performance on the spatial-orien-
tation tests was measured using an
accuracy scale. Accuracy was calcu-
lated by first determining the Figure 2. Two-dimensional visualization test item example.
degrees of difference between the

24
Spatial Thinking Assists Geographic Thinking

representation, and higher-level pro-


cesses of reasoning. The content-
dependent category, however, is
unlikely to be answered correctly
without some understanding of the
generalizations and assumptions of
central place theory. Change in
students’ spatial-relations content
knowledge was used as a proxy for
geographic thinking. Figure 5 pro-
vides an example of a content-inde-
pendent and a content-dependent
question.

Intervention
A quasi-experimental interven-
tion design was conducted in a field
setting with the intent of producing
an instructional impact. A sequen-
tial explanatory strategy was uti-
lized for the research design with
priority on quantitative data col-
lected using a repeated measures
format. The effect of the instruc-
tional unit was explored by compar-
ing pre- and posttests for three
Figure 3. Three-dimensional visualization test item examples. groups: the intervention group, the
comparison group, and a control
group.
All three groups completed pre/
posttests of spatial-visualization
(Committee on Support for Thinking Spatially, National skills, spatial-orientation skills, and spatial-relations con-
Research Council 2006, 68); (2) examples of spatial-relation tent knowledge tests. One class acting as the control group
abilities listed by Golledge and Stimson (1997, 158); (3) received normally scheduled instruction as determined by
level of spatial thinking assigned using a taxonomy of spa- their classroom teacher. The remaining four divided clas-
tial thinking (Jo and Bednarz 2009); and (4) content rele- ses participated in a guided exploratory instructional unit
vant to central place theory. The test was created to on central place theory using either virtual globe and digi-
measure changes in students’ understanding of central tal maps or traditional paper-and-pencil maps. Both the
place theory from a spatial-relations-skill perspective. In intervention and comparison groups received explicit
other words, to answer questions correctly required spa- instruction during the activity for using visualization
tial skill and content knowledge. Drawing on examples strategies to enhance their use of the respective tools.
from Lee’s (2005) spatial-relations instrument and Jo and On day 1, the intervention and comparison group
Bednarz’s (2009) taxonomy for evaluating questions for received an introduction to central place theory using a
spatial thinking, questions were developed that integrated formatted structure of guided inquiry and direct
concepts of space, tools of representation, and higher-level instruction. The students explored assumptions of cen-
processes of reasoning to assess students’ understanding tral place theory using comparative examples and con-
of central place theory. sidered conditions that could cause variations to the
Questions were categorized as (1) requiring primarily assumptions of the theory.1 Students were provided
spatial skills to answer, (2) requiring primarily content with a progression of simple to more complex exam-
knowledge specific to central-place theory, or (3) task-based ples of the spatial patterns of market centers (cities).
questions. The first category is referred to in data analysis The following day, day 2 of the instructional unit,
as content-independent, the second as content-dependent, and students were divided using a stratified random sam-
the last as task-based. All three question categories measure pling method. Half of the students (exclusive of the
spatial thinking as all include the essential components of control group) received instruction in the computer
spatial thinking as defined in Learning to Think Spatially lab using GST as a geovisualization tool. The other
(Committee on Support for Thinking Spatially, National half remained in the classroom using paper maps
Research Council 2006), that is, concepts of space, tools of and data tables. Working in pairs, both groups

25
Sandra Metoyer and Robert Bednarz

followed a written guided activ-


ity to support the investigation
of the spatial patterns of market
centers in the Czech Republic.
The first activity on day 2, for
both groups, explored the spatial
distribution of cities. The interven-
tion group used GIS layers devel-
oped in ArcMap, converted to
zipped Keyhole Markup Language
files (KMZ) and displayed in Goo-
gle Earth using proportional sym-
bols to represent population size.
Using the measurement tool in Goo-
gle Earth, students calculated dis-
tances between various market
centers. Using the road layer in
Google Earth, they explored trans-
portation routes among the market
centers. Using the draw tool and
without prompting, many students
added bold lines directly connecting
the market centers, thus enhancing
their ability to visualize the geomet-
ric patterns. And, by tilting the
view, students in the GST interven-
tion group were able to consider the
influence of terrain on the spatial
pattern of cities.
The comparison group com-
pleted the same activity. Instead
of digital maps, however, they
used laminated paper maps and
rulers to measure distances
(requiring scale conversion). The
paper maps displayed the same
remote-sensed images and infor-
mation layers as the Google Earth
representations, but lacked the
dynamic interactive nature of
Google Earth. Students using the
nondigital maps could not change
scale, orientation, or view angle.
Likewise, they were not able to
turn layers on and off. Instead,
they viewed the layers as multiple
static paper maps. Table 2 com-
Figure 4. Spatial-orientation test item example.
pares differences in features of the
tool of representation used by the
GST digital intervention group
and the nondigital comparison group. repeated measure design prior to and then following the
intervention. Efficiency and accuracy scores were calcu-
RESULTS lated for each test. The test results were used to (1) deter-
mine starting levels of the participants’ spatial skills, (2)
Changes to Spatial-Thinking Skills quantify change to participants’ skills following the inter-
The tests were administered to the three participant vention, and (3) explore changes in the three groups’ skills
groups (control, comparison, and intervention) using a following the intervention.

26
Spatial Thinking Assists Geographic Thinking

The control, comparison, and


intervention group demonstrated
equal and moderate gains pre-
to post- in both 2-D and 3-D spa-
tial-visualization skills. The gains
were statistically significant at a
0.05 level for all groups for the
2-D spatial visualization and for
the intervention group for the
3‑D visualization. In contrast, the
groups demonstrated no change
in spatial-orientation skills. The
participants became more effi-
cient at orientation—they com-
pleted more items in the post-
than in the pretest—but demon-
strated no gains in accuracy.
This may indicate that spatial-
orientation skills are more resis-
tant or less malleable than spa-
tial-visualization skills. The
relatively equal performance in
accuracy for the three groups
also indicates that gains pre- to
post- were most likely due to the
test-retest effect and were not
affected by the instructional unit
or by the use of maps; digital or
otherwise (Fig. 6). An indepen-
dent samples T test was used to
compare means between males
and females. No statistical differ-
ences were found. Mean scores
on all measures of spatial skill
were essentially equal for male
and female participants.

Changes to Spatial-Relations
Content Knowledge
Results of the content tests were
coded as correct or incorrect,
quantified, and then analyzed
Figure 5. Spatial-relations content test item example. Correct answer for the content- using descriptive statistics and
independent item (left) is A. Correct answer for the content-dependent item (right) is D. comparison of means for the three
instructional groups. A significant
difference between the two treat-
For the three groups combined, accuracy for 2-D spatial ment groups’ and the control group’s scores was found
visualization increased significantly pre- to posttest (5.9% for the content-dependent and independent items. No sig-
gain, p < 0.00). Accuracy for 3-D spatial visualization also nificant difference was found for the task-based items;
increased significantly pre- to posttest (8.0% gain, p < each group demonstrated substantial loss in scores pre- to
0.00). Accuracy scores for spatial orientation decreased post- on the task-based items. Time on task was substan-
slightly, but a large significant gain in efficiency (10.7%, p tially longer for the task-based items in the posttest than
< 0.00) occurred. Maximum scores for all three spatial the pretest. The researcher did not have an adequate
skills tested were relatively high on the pretest, and either instrument (rubric) to create comparable difficulty level
at or near 100 percent in the posttests. In addition, meas- between pretest task-based items and posttest items. In
ures of variance (standard deviation scores) were large, retrospect, the observed decline in task-based scores pre-
especially for 3-D spatial visualization. to post- almost certainly occurred because the task-based

27
Sandra Metoyer and Robert Bednarz

items in the posttest were consider-


ably more difficult than the items in Table 2. Differences in the tools of representation.
the pretest, and thus, the changes in
scores do not provide a valid com-
Digital Group Nondigital Group
parison. Pre- and post- spatial-rela-
tions content scores were compared (Google Earth and GIS) (Paper maps and data tables)
for the three groups using an inde- Interactive: Students could adjust Noninteractive: Students could not
pendent T test. The variance was scale, altitude, perspective, and/or layers adjust scale, altitude, perspective,
similar for the groups, allowing use or layers
of the independent samples T test. Adjustable Scale: Students could easily Fixed Scale
Both treatment groups’ content zoom in and out
knowledge scores increased, but Layering: Students could turn on and No Layering:
only the intervention group showed view several layers displayed on Students viewed layers separately
statistically significant gains, a 15.0 one image on different maps
percent (p  0.01) increase in con- Dynamic: Images changed Static: Images did not change
Nonsequential: Students could explore Sequential and Linear: Students
tent-independent items and a 11.9
outside the region of interest and among viewed separate maps in a
percent (p  0.05) increase in con- the layers prescribed order
tent-dependent items. In addition to Digital Measurement: Students used Hand Measurement: Students used
testing for statistical significance, automated tools to measure distance a scale bar and a ruler to estimate
Cohen’s d was calculated to estimate and to determine scale distance and scale
effect sizes and to compare degree of
impact between the comparison
group and intervention group. The effect sizes for the Categories Based on Spatial-Skill Level
intervention group were larger than those for the compari- Based on their pretest spatial-skill accuracy scores, par-
son group: 0.54 and 0.30 for the content-independent and ticipants were placed into one of three spatial-skill groups:
0.47 and 0.26 for the content-dependent items, respec- low spatial, average spatial, and high spatial. The influ-
tively. The 2.5-week delay between the activity and the ence of starting spatial-skill level, sex, and method of
posttest strongly suggests that the content was acquired instruction on gains in content knowledge was explored,
and retained by the participants. Table 3 summarizes the using correlations and analysis of variance (ANOVA).
descriptive statistics and comparison of means by instruc- The scores for the three spatial-skill tests were posi-
tional group for the spatial-relations content test. tively correlated and results of pretests for the three
Gains in content knowledge were evident in both treat- groups were very similar with respect to sample size,
ment groups. Only the intervention group, however, dem- starting point, and variance. The pretest accuracy
onstrated significant gains. These
results suggest that using GST for
teaching and learning has a greater
positive effect on students’ content
knowledge than using an alternative,
more traditional approach. The large
standard deviations in each group,
however, suggest factors other than
choice of instructional method influ-
ence performance. The scores of
some individuals did not improve
using GST, and some did signifi-
cantly worse. Similar variation
occurred in the scores of the compari-
son group. This situation prompted
an analysis of the relationships
between the students’ spatial-visuali-
zation and spatial-orientation skills
and changes in skills and content
knowledge based on the instructional
Figure 6. Graphic comparison of mean scores by spatial-skill test and by group.
method in order to explore the best
*Statistically significant difference at the 0.05 level between the pretest and the
methods of practice and representa-
posttest mean scores.
tion for students with different levels
of spatial-thinking skills.

28
Spatial Thinking Assists Geographic Thinking

instructional groups tabulated by


Table 3. Summary of spatial relations content test by instructional group. spatial-skill category is illustrated
in Figure 7.
n Premean Postmean Mean Difference Sig.
Spatial Thinking and Geographic
(std) (std)
Thinking. Starting spatial-skill level is
Content-independent Control * 20 53.9 (28.6) 43.3 (34.7) ¡12.7 0.11 related to change (gain or loss) in spatial
Comparison 41 57.1 (25.2) 65.0 (27.8) 8.5 0.13 skills. The low spatial-skill group had
Intervention ⱡ 41 56.7 (25.0) 71.5 (29.4) 15.0 0.01 the largest significant gains pre- to post-
Content-dependent Control * ᵻ 20 36.2 (21.7) 25.0 (23.3) ¡27.0 0.05 for 2-D and 3-D visualization skills. The
Comparison 41 39.3 (17.6) 44.9 (24.8) 5.9 0.19 average spatial-skill group also had sta-
Intervention ⱡ 41 39.5 (23.9) 51.7 (28.3) 11.9 0.05 tistically significant gains for 2-D and 3-
D visualization skills. None of the three
*Statistically significant difference at the 0.05 level between the control group and the other two
groups evidenced improvement in spa-
groups.
ᵻStatistically significant negative difference at the 0.05 level between the pretest and the posttest mean tial orientation. The high spatial group
scores. made very little, if any, gains in accu-
ⱡStatistically significant positive difference at the 0.05 level between the pretest and the posttest mean racy on any of the three spatial-skills
scores. tests. A ceiling effect may have limited
their gains. Based on these results, start-
ing spatial-skill level does influence the
scores for spatial skills, therefore, could be combined impact of training on spatial skills. The low spatial-skill group
and an average score used to divide individuals into had much more room for gain, and through the practice pro-
spatial-skill categories. This is not to argue that two- vided by testing and retesting, improved their spatial skills
dimensional visualization, three-dimensional visualiza- significantly.
tion, and spatial orientation are a single skill. An Starting spatial-skill level also influences gains in spa-
emerging body of evidence from behavioral sciences tial-relations content knowledge. Removing the control
and neuroscience suggests that these processes are group members, all three spatial-skill groups’ content-
physiologically and behaviorally distinct (Zacks and independent test scores increased. Only the high spatial-
Tversky 2005; Hegarty et al. 2006; Gersmehl and Gers- skill group, however, demonstrated significant gains. In
mehl 2007). In this study, however, the scores were the content-dependent item category, no change was evi-
aggregated to represent one measure for spatial skills dent for the low and only a small gain was made by the
because, in general, a relatively higher than average average spatial-skill participants. The high spatial group,
score on one measure was associated with a relatively however, demonstrated significant gains for both content-
higher than average score on the other two. Compari- independent and content-dependent scores.
son of the number of participants in the three This phenomenon was analyzed further by dividing the
spatial-skill groups into instructional
groups. Analysis revealed that the
high spatial-skill participants outper-
formed the low and average regard-
less of instructional strategy. High
spatial-skill individuals, males and
females, consistently outperformed
low and average individuals, and
the advantage was larger in the GST
than in the comparison group
(Table 4). These results suggest that
an individual’s starting spatial-skill
level influences change to spatial-
relations content knowledge espe-
cially for high spatial-skill individu-
als using GST, implying that
thinking geographically is closely
linked to thinking spatially.
High spatial-skill level positively
affects gains in spatial-relations con-
Figure 7. Participants in instructional group by spatial-skill category.
tent knowledge. Gains in spatial
skills were obtained through training

29
Sandra Metoyer and Robert Bednarz

and were evident in this study


from a test-retest effect. Gains in Table 4. Summary of mean spatial relations content test scores (%) by instructional group and
spatial skills are feasibly corre- spatial skill group.
lated to gains in spatial-relations
content knowledge. In other
n Pre Post Mean Diff. Sig. d
words, individuals who demon- (std) (std)
strated gains in spatial skills may
also have developed an advan- Comparison Content-independent Low 15 58.7 (25.6) 64.4 (38.8) 5.7 0.60 0.18
tage in geographic thinking. Average 12 50.0 (24.9) 63.9 (17.2) 13.9 0.13 0.66
This possible correlation was High 14 61.3 (25.6) 66.7 (22.6)x 5.4 0.48 0.22
explored using a scatterplot and Content-dependent Low 15 43.3 (20.7) 36.0 (24.1)* ¡7.3 0.30 ¡0.33
Pearson correlation coefficient. Average 12 30.6 (15.6)x 43.3 (25.3) 12.7 0.19 0.62
High ⱡ 14 42.2 (13.9) 55.7 (22.4)* 13.5 0.05 0.74
First, participants in the two
treatment groups that had Intervention Content-independent Low 14 50.7 (28.1) 61.9 (34.2)* 11.2 0.29 0.36
improved scores pretest to post- Average 16 62.5 (25.2) 70.8 (29.5) 8.3 0.26 0.30
test on combined spatial-skills High ⱡ 11 56.7 (20.6) 84.8 (17.4)*x 28.1 0.01 1.48
accuracy by 10 percent or more Content-dependent Low 14 37.8 (22.2) 45.7 (26.5) 7.9 0.41 0.32
were identified. Considering only Average 16 46.9 (23.7)x 46.3 (28.0) ¡0.6 0.94 ¡0.02
High ⱡ 11 31.9 (25.1) 67.3 (27.2) 35.4 0.01 1.35
individuals who had demon-
strated gains in spatial skills of 10 *Statistically significant difference at the 0.05 level between the high spatial and the low spatial group in the
percent or more and had received same instructional group for the same test.
instruction on central place the- xStatistically significant difference at the 0.05 level between the intervention and comparison group in the
same spatial skill group for the same test.
ory, correlation between gains in ⱡStatistically significant positive difference at the 0.05 level between the pretest and the posttest mean scores.
spatial skills and gains in spatial
relations were explored.
No relationship existed (r D ¡0.17, p D 0.457). Students high level of spatial skill prior to instruction performed
with the greatest gains in spatial skill had little to no gains better on the spatial-relations content test. Furthermore,
in spatial-relations content knowledge. The greatest gains students with a high level of spatial skill performed better
in spatial relations were achieved by a few students with when using GST than when using paper maps. This was
the smallest spatial-skill gain considered for this group (10 especially true for males. Using GST to teach a spatially
percent). Spatial-relations content scores for the majority dependent concept is most beneficial to students who pos-
of individuals (fourteen out of twenty-one) either sess well-developed spatial skills. The link between spatial
decreased or remained unchanged regardless of changes thinking and geographic thinking is apparent. Higher lev-
in their spatial skills. This may be attributed to a lag time els of spatial thinking are correlated with superior geo-
between gains in skills and subsequent gains in the appli- graphic thinking.
cation of those skills. Or, it may be attributed to the diffi- Assumptions, informed by the literature, were made for
culties involved with knowledge transfer. Several the theoretical framework of this study. First, the use of
exposures or opportunities for applying a new skill or GST was assumed to have a positive impact on spatial
strategy is often required before it can be transferred to skills. This assumption was not supported. Use of GST
novel problem-solving situations. had no effect on students’ spatial skills. All groups had
similar gains in spatial-visualization skills, including the
DISCUSSION control group. Retesting did have a positive effect on spa-
In conclusion, the first research question asked, what tial skills. Improvement from training spatial skills is well
is the effect of using GST on students’ spatial-thinking supported in the literature. Therefore, the gains seen in
skills? This study found that instructional use of geo- spatial skills were most likely due to a training effect
spatial technologies did not improve students’ spatial- established by the repeated testing.
thinking skills. Gains in spatial skills were assumed to be positively cor-
The second research question asked, what is the effect of related with gains in content knowledge. This assumption
using GST on students’ content knowledge of a spatially was not supported. Gains in spatial skills had no relation-
dependent concept? Students utilizing GST demonstrated ship to gains in content knowledge. It was also assumed
greater gains in content knowledge than the students uti- that high spatial skills would correlate with better perfor-
lizing paper-and-pencil maps. Use of GST in this context mance on the spatial-relations content knowledge test.
was a more effective method for teaching a spatially This assumption was supported. In fact, only the high spa-
dependent concept than the traditional paper maps. tial group had significant gains on the spatial-relations
The third research question asked, what is the relation- content test.
ship between spatial thinking and geographic thinking? Sex differences were assumed to be present in spatial
Two notable relationships were evident. Students with a skills and in content test outcomes. More specifically,

30
Spatial Thinking Assists Geographic Thinking

males were expected to outperform females. This assump- ubiquitous and location matters now more than ever. The
tion was not supported. Sex differences were neither pres- widespread availability of GST, however, does not imply
ent in the pretest scores nor in the posttest scores. In that teachers can integrate these technologies effectively or
addition, no significant interaction effects were present that students can use them competently. Society is awash in
between sex and instructional method. However, differen- spatial data and GST yet lacks the spatial thinking needed to
ces in score gains between males and females on the con- use GST to solve problems, make decisions, or affect policy
tent test were noticeable. Although not statistically (Metoyer, Bednarz, and Bednarz 2015). Thus, the recom-
significant, high spatial males showed more of an inclina- mendation here is that K–12 geography education should
tion for the GST method than high spatial females. This advance an integrated model of instruction that emphasizes
implies males and females may have interacted with the teaching content with GST for the purpose of promoting
content and instructional method differently. In general thinking spatially and, consequently, thinking
however, males did not perform better than females on geographically.
spatial-skills or content-knowledge tests.
Caution in interpreting these results is necessary. The
sample group was small and the categories compared
within the sample group were smaller. Even with statisti- NOTE
cally significant results, the correlations could be random. 1. For more detail on the simplifying assumptions and
The groups were too small with too much variation to cap- generalizations of Christaller’s central place theory, see
ture generalizable trends or patterns. The trends identified Favier (2011).
were possibly random and due to chance. A follow-up
scaled study is recommended that considers issues of rep-
resentativeness and replicability.
REFERENCES
Implications for K–12 geography education are many.
Albert, W. S., and R. G. Golledge. 1999. The use of spatial cog-
Spatial thinking is an important, yet unrecognized, cogni-
nitive abilities in geographic information systems: The
tive skill that has a substantial influence on student learn-
map overlay operation. Transactions in GIS 3 (1): 7–21.
ing. Students with high spatial-thinking skills are better
prepared to learn spatially dependent concepts. Yet, even Baenninger, M., and N. Newcombe. 1989. The role of expe-
for motivated and academically gifted students such as rience in spatial test performance: A meta-analysis.
those represented in this sample from geography honor Sex Roles 20 (5/6): 327–344.
classes, the ability to think spatially varies greatly. In addi-
Brosnan, M. J. 1998. Spatial ability in children’s play with
tion, spatial thinking is an amalgam of skills and pro-
LEGO blocks. Perceptual and Motor Skills 87 (1): 19–28.
cesses. It cannot be taught through traditional drills or
repetition. Geospatial technologies can extend students’ Burton, L. 2003. Examining the relation between visual
abilities to think spatially and understand geographic phe- imagery and spatial ability tests. International Journal
nomena, but GST is also an amalgam of tools and pro- of Testing 3 (3): 277–291.
cesses. It should not be assumed that GST, in itself, will
Committee on Support for Thinking Spatially, National
improve spatial thinking or content knowledge without
Research Council. 2006. Learning to Think Spatially: GIS
paying explicit attention to spatial-thinking strategies and
as a Support System in the K–12 Curriculum. Washing-
integration of geographic inquiry.
ton, D.C.: National Academies Press.
Based on evidence from this study, GST should be
integrated into instruction of spatially dependent con- Cooke-Simpson, A., and D. Voyer. 2007. Confidence and
cepts. However, in addition to teaching with GST, gender differences in the mental rotations test. Learn-
teachers should scaffold the instruction into an authen- ing and Individual Differences 17 (2): 181–186.
tic problem-solving activity utilizing reform-based
Dasen, P. R., and R. C. Mishra. 2010. Development of Geocen-
teaching methods. Examples may include one or more
tric Spatial Language and Cognition. New York: Cam-
of the following: collaborative teams of students, delib-
bridge University Press.
erate practice of spatial skills needed for success using
GST (for example, a minilesson on orientation), refer- Ekstrom, R., J. French, and H. Harmon. 1976. Manual for
ence guides for spatial vocabulary, or structured Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests. Office of Naval
assignments to promote metacognition about the activ- Research. Princeton, New Jersey: Educational Testing
ity and students’ own spatial thinking during the activ- Service.
ity (van Asselen, Fritschy, and Postma 2006; Cooke-
Eliot, J. 1987. Models of Psychological Space: Psychometric,
Simpson and Voyer 2007). Students learn in a variety
Developmental, and Experimental Approaches. New York:
of ways and require an astounding amount of cognitive
Springer-Verlag.
tools to become more effective learners.
Spatial thinking is but one cognitive tool, but an increas- Favier, T. 2011. Geographic information systems in inquiry-
ingly important one. Geospatial technologies have become based secondary geography education. Ph.D. diss., VU

31
Sandra Metoyer and Robert Bednarz

University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands: methods of secondary education. Journal of Geography
Ipskamp. ISBN No. 978-94-6190-105-7. 102 (3): 128–137.
Forer, P., and D. Unwin. 1999. Enabling progress in Kompridis, N. 2000. So we need something else for reason
GIS and education. In Geographical Information Sys- to mean. International Journal of Philosophical Studies 8
tems: Management Issues and Applications, Vol. 2, (3): 271–295.
ed. P. A. Longley, M. F. Goodchild, D. J. Maguire, Kyllonen, P. C., and J. Gluck. 2003. Spatial ability: Intro-
and D. W. Rhind, pp. 747–756. New York: John duction to the special edition. International Journal of
Wiley & Sons. Testing 3 (3): 215–217.
Gersmehl, P. J., and C. A. Gersmehl. 2007. Spatial thinking Lee, J. 2005. The effect of GIS learning on spatial ability. Ph.D.
by young children: Neurologic evidence for early diss., Department of Geography, Texas A&M University.
development and “educability.” Journal of Geography
106 (5): 181–191. Lee, J., and R. Bednarz. 2009. Effect of GIS learning on spa-
tial thinking. Journal of Geography in Higher Education
Golledge, R. G. 2002. The nature of geographic knowledge. 33 (2): 183–198.
Annals of the Association of American Geographers 92 (1):
Levine, S. C., M. Vasilyeva, S. F. Lourenco, N. S. New-
1–14.
combe, and J. Huttenlocher. 2005. Socioeconomic sta-
Golledge, R. G., and R. Stimson. 1997. Spatial Behavior: A tus modifies the sex difference in spatial skill.
Geographic Perspective. New York: The Guilford Press. Psychological Science 16 (11): 841–845.
Hall-Wallace, M. K., and C. M. McAuliffe. 2002. Design, Lloyd, R., D. Patton, and R. Cammack. 1996. Basic-level
implementation, and evaluation of GIS-based learning geographic categories. The Professional Geographer 48
materials in an introductory geoscience. Journal of Geo- (2): 181–194.
science Education 50 (1): 5–14. Metoyer, S. K., S. W. Bednarz, and R. S. Bednarz. 2015.
Halpern, D. 2000. Psychosocial hypothesis part I. In Sex Spatial thinking in education: concepts, development,
Differences in Cognitive Abilities, 3rd ed., pp. 229–276. and assessments. In Geospatial Technologies and Geogra-
Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum Associates. phy Education in a Changing World, ed. O. Muniz Solari,
A. Demicri, and J. van der Schee, pp. 21–33. Philadel-
Hegarty, M., D. R. Montello, A. E. Richardson, T. Ishi- phia: Springer US.
kawa, and K. Lovelace. 2006. Spatial abilities at differ-
Mills, R. 1991. A new understanding of self: The role of affect,
ent scales: Individual differences in aptitude-test
state of mind, self-understanding, and intrinsic motiva-
performance and spatial-layout learning. Intelligence
tion. Journal of Experimental Education 60 (1): 67–81
34 (2): 151–176.
Pallrand, G., and F. Seeber 1984. Spatial abilities and
Hegarty, M., and D. Waller. 2004. A dissociation between
achievement in introductory physics. Journal of
mental rotation and perspective-taking spatial abili-
Research in Science Teaching 21 (5): 507–516.
ties. Intelligence 32 (2): 175–191.
Quaiser-Pohl, C. 2003. The mental cutting test “Schnitte” and
Hsi, S., M. Linn, and J. Bell. 1997. The role of spatial reasoning the picture rotation test—Two new measures to assess
in engineering and the design of spatial instruction. Jour- spatial ability. International Journal of Testing 3 (3): 219–231.
nal of Engineering Education 86 (2):151–158.
Sanchez, C., and J. Wiley. 2010. Sex differences in science
Janelle, D. G., and M. F. Goodchild. 2009. Location across learning: Closing the gap through animations. Learn-
disciplines: Reflections on the CSISS experience. In ing and Individual Differences 20 (3): 271–275.
Geospatial Technology and the Role of Location in Science,
Shin, E. K. 2007. Using GIS technology to enhance elemen-
ed. H. J. Scholten, R. Velde, and N. Manen, pp. 15–29.
tary students’ geographic understanding. Theory and
Philadelphia: Springer US.
Research in Social Education 35 (2): 231–255.
Jo, I., and S. W. Bednarz. 2009. Evaluating geography text-
Talley, L. H. 1973. The use of three-dimensional visualiza-
book questions from a spatial perspective: Using con-
tions as a moderator in higher cognitive learning of
cepts of space, tools of representation, and cognitive
concepts in college level chemistry. Journal of Research
processes to evaluate spatiality. Journal of Geography
in Science Teaching 10 (3): 263–269.
108 (1): 4–13.
Terlecki, M. 2004. The effects of long-term practice and
Kali, Y., and N. Orion. 1996. Spatial abilities of high-school
training on mental rotation. Ph.D. diss., Department
students in the perception of geological structures.
of Psychology, Temple University.
Journal of Research in Science Teaching 33 (4): 369–391.
Thorndike, E. L. 1921. On the organization of intellect. Psy-
Kerski, J. 2003. The implementation and effectiveness of
chological Review 28 (2): 141–151.
geographic information systems technology and

32
Spatial Thinking Assists Geographic Thinking

van Asselen, M., E. Fritschy, and A. Postma. 2006. The direction giving and sex differences. Environment and
influence of intentional and incidental learning on Behavior 18 (2):192–213.
acquiring spatial knowledge during navigation. Psy-
Wright, R., W. L. Thompson, G. Ganis, N. S. New-
chological Research 70 (2): 151–156.
combe, and S. M. Kosslyn. 2008. Training general-
Voyer, D., S. Voyer, and M. P. Bryden. 1995. Magnitude of ized spatial skills. Psychometric Bulletin & Review
sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis 15 (4): 763–771.
and consideration of critical variables. Psychological
Zacks, J., and B. Tversky. 2005. Multiple systems for
Bulletin 117 (2): 250–270.
spatial imagery: Transformations of objects and
Ward, S. L., N. Newcombe, and W. F. Overton. 1986. Turn bodies. Spatial Cognition and Computation 5 (4):
left at the church, or three miles north. A study of 271–306.

33

You might also like