You are on page 1of 14

Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 66–79

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Contemporary Educational Psychology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cedpsych

Empirical study

Spontaneous spatial strategy use in learning from scientific text


Logan Fiorella a,⇑, Richard E. Mayer b
a
University of Georgia, United States
b
University of California, Santa Barbara, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Two studies explored the role of the spontaneous use of spatial note-taking strategies (i.e., creating maps
Available online 5 January 2017 and drawings) and spatial ability in learning from a scientific passage. In Study 1, college students read
and took notes by hand on a 10-paragraph scientific passage about the human respiratory system.
Keywords: Students tended to use verbal strategies such as lists (on 48% of the paragraphs), outlines (29%) and run-
Learning strategies ning text (15%), but also used spatial strategies such as maps (28%) and drawings (11%). Regression anal-
Spatial ability yses indicated that spatial ability and the use of spatial strategies (maps or drawings) significantly
Scientific text
predicted learning outcomes, with spatial strategy use explaining additional variance beyond spatial abil-
Comprehension
Note taking
ity. In Study 2, students read the same scientific passage and took notes either by hand on paper (paper
group), by hand on a large whiteboard (whiteboard group), or on a laptop computer (computer group). A
similar general pattern as Study 1 was found for the paper group, but this pattern was not found for the
computer or whiteboard groups, suggesting that the relationships found in Study 1 might depend on the
note-taking medium. Results also indicated that students in the paper and whiteboard groups sponta-
neously used more spatial strategies, whereas the computer group tended to use verbal strategies (i.e.,
words only), suggesting that different note-taking contexts encourage different strategies.
Ó 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Objective 2. Rationale

Although much attention has been directed toward the role of There is now substantial evidence that individual differences in
spatial ability in learning in science, technology, mathematics spatial ability predict students’ pursuit, persistence, and achieve-
and engineering (STEM) disciplines, the present study examines ment in STEM disciplines (Wai, Lubinski, & Benbow, 2009). In a
the additional role that might be played by spatial learning strate- massive longitudinal study involving over 400,000 participants,
gies – specifically, the spontaneous use of spatial note-taking Wai et al. (2009) found that people who scored highly on measures
strategies while studying scientific text. The goals of the present of spatial ability as adolescents were much more likely to achieve
studies are to determine (a) the unique roles of the spontaneous advanced degrees and occupations in STEM fields than people with
use of spatial note-taking strategies (i.e., creating maps and draw- lower spatial ability. Subsequent research has found similar evi-
ings) and spatial ability in explaining learning from a scientific pas- dence linking spatial ability to STEM learning, likely because many
sage, and (b) whether the medium in with which students take STEM concepts involve representing and manipulating complex
notes (i.e., on paper, on a whiteboard, or on a computer) affects spatial relations (Uttal & Cohen, 2012), such as spatial configura-
the note-taking strategies that students spontaneously use and tions of molecules in chemistry or the structures and processes
the relationship between spatial note-taking strategies and learn- of the human respiratory system in biology.
ing outcomes. One implication of this relationship is to attempt to improve
students’ general spatial skills through training. If certain spatial
skills are important for STEM fields, then targeted training might
enhance underlying cognitive processes that ultimately lead to
better academic learning. A meta-analysis by Uttal and colleagues
(Uttal et al., 2013) found evidence that spatial skills can be
⇑ Corresponding author at: Department of Educational Psychology, University of improved through training (average Hedge’s g = 0.47) and can
Georgia, Athens, GA 30602, United States. transfer across different measures of spatial ability; however,
E-mail addresses: lfiorella@uga.edu (L. Fiorella), mayer@psych.ucsb.edu whether spatial training transfers to authentic STEM learning
(R.E. Mayer).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.01.002
0361-476X/Ó 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
L. Fiorella, R.E. Mayer / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 66–79 67

remains unclear. As the authors noted, ‘‘. . . the lack of studies that 3.1. Mapping
directly assess the effects of spatial training on performance in a
STEM discipline is disappointing” (p. 356). Furthermore, a long his- Learning by mapping involves creating an abstract spatial
tory of research on the transfer of learning suggests that transfer is arrangement of the text by connecting key ideas based on their
often highly domain-specific (e.g., Mayer & Wittrock, 2006; conceptual relationships (Fiorella & Mayer, 2015, 2016; Holley &
Salomon & Perkins, 1989; Thorndike & Woodworth, 1901). As Dansereau, 1984). In research on mapping, learners create maps
one example related to spatial cognition, a study by Sims and by establishing their own conceptual labels for the relationships,
Mayer (2002) found that playing the game Tetris improved mental or they might be provided with labels (e.g., ‘‘part of,” ‘‘type of,”
rotation ability only for Tetris-like shapes. In short, the extent to or ‘‘leads to”). A classic study by Holley, Dansereau, McDonald,
which spatial training yields educationally relevant benefits might Garland, and Collins (1979) demonstrates the benefits of teaching
be limited, and, at this point, such benefits have not been convinc- students to engage in learning by mapping. College students
ingly demonstrated (Stieff & Uttal, 2015). received training in creating knowledge maps for expository texts
This suggests an alternative approach might be appropriate for (mapping group) or they did not receive training (control group).
understanding the relationship between spatial ability and STEM Then students read a new text on geology while either creating a
learning, and ultimately, for developing methods to enhance STEM knowledge map or taking notes normally. The mapping group out-
instruction. One possibility is that general spatial ability is related performed the control group on recall and recognition tests, with
to the use of domain-specific learning and problem-solving the strongest effects for lower-achieving students. A subsequent
strategies. For example, Stieff, Dixon, Ryu, Kumi, and Hegarty study by Roberts and Dansereau (2008) found that mapping was
(2014) found that training college students on mental imagery more effective than creating verbal summaries for students with
and analytic problem-solving strategies in chemistry eliminated low verbal ability, but less effective for students with high verbal
gender differences in achievement. This finding suggests that ability. This suggests that using a spatial strategy such as mapping
focusing on effective strategy use in a STEM discipline can improve might be more effective than a verbal strategy for students with
academic performance, and further, that ‘‘achievement is low ability, although the study did not include a measure of spatial
dependent not only on spatial ability but also on strategy ability.
choice . . .” (p. 390). A related form of mapping is to create or fill in a graphic orga-
More broadly, a vast research literature demonstrates that nizer or matrix to spatially represent one’s notes, such as to repre-
learning outcomes largely depend on the quality of strategies stu- sent a compare-and-contrast structure (e.g., Kiewra et al., 1991;
dents use during learning (Fiorella & Mayer, 2015, 2016; Dunlosky, Robinson & Kiewra, 1995). For example, in a study by Jairam and
Rawson, Marsh, Nathan, & Willingham, 2013; Weinstein & Mayer, Kiewra (2010), college students read a text lesson presented on a
1985). Popular but suboptimal strategies include rereading, computer about different types of wildcats. Students were either
restudying, underlining, highlighting, and verbatim note taking asked to take linear notes using a window on the screen or to fill
(e.g., Bretzing & Kulhavy, 1979; Callender & McDaniel, 2009; in an onscreen matrix that helped students organize different char-
Fowler & Barker, 1974), whereas effective strategies involve deeper acteristics attributed to each type of wildcat. Results indicated that
generative processing, which require learners to actively organize students who filled in a matrix organizer during learning per-
and integrate the material with their existing knowledge (Fiorella formed better on subsequent retention and comprehension tests,
& Mayer, 2015, 2016; Wittrock, 1990) – such as by self-testing, compared to students who took notes normally. A more recent
self-explaining, explaining to others, creating a knowledge or con- study by Ponce and Mayer (2014) provides similar support for ask-
cept map, or drawing a diagram (e.g., Fiorella & Mayer, 2013; ing students to complete a matrix organizer when learning about
Roediger & Karpicke, 2006; Schwamborn, Mayer, Thillmann, different types of steamboats. Overall, research on learning by
Leopold, & Leutner, 2010). Importantly, students can be taught to mapping suggests that creating knowledge or concept maps, or
use such strategies effectively with relatively little explicit instruc- using matrix or graphic organizers when learning from text is gen-
tion (Fiorella & Mayer, 2015, 2016; Dunlosky et al., 2013; erally more effective than generating verbal summaries or using
Weinstein & Mayer, 1985). Thus, strategy use might be more mal- one’s own note-taking techniques, particularly for low-ability stu-
leable and transferrable to STEM contexts than general spatial abil- dents (Fiorella & Mayer, 2015; Nesbit & Adesope, 2006).
ity. The present study focuses on students’ spontaneous use of a
particular class of strategies – called spatial learning strategies
(Fiorella & Mayer, 2015, 2016; Holley & Dansereau, 1984) – which 3.2. Drawing
are especially useful in understanding STEM concepts.
Learning by drawing involves creating a concrete illustration
that spatially depicts the key ideas in the text (Fiorella & Mayer,
3. Spatial learning strategies 2015, 2016; Leutner & Schmeck, 2014; Van Meter & Garner,
2005). In a classic study by Alesandrini (1981), college students
Learning for understanding involves building a coherent read a lesson about the chemistry of electric batteries. Some stu-
mental model based on the material’s underlying structure dents were asked to create drawings to represent each of the four-
(Gentner & Stevens, 1983; Kintsch, 1998; Mayer, 2014; teen components described in the lesson (drawing group), whereas
Wittrock, 1990), such as understanding a cause-and-effect system other students were asked to use a verbal paraphrase strategy
or a hierarchy (Cook & Mayer, 1988). Spatial learning strategies (paraphrase group). The drawing group outperformed the para-
help students represent these structural features by making spa- phrase group on a subsequent test that included factual, compre-
tial relations among the material more explicit (Holley & hension, and transfer questions, providing early evidence for the
Dansereau, 1984; Pressley, 1990). This advantage is especially benefit of drawing over a purely verbal strategy.
important when learning from scientific texts, for which no In a more recent study, Leopold and Leutner (2012) compared
external spatial representations of the material are provided to the effects of creating drawings (drawing group), generating verbal
the learner (e.g., Leopold & Leutner, 2012). Two of the most summaries (summary group), or using no strategy (control group),
common forms of spatial strategies are creating knowledge or while learning from a scientific text about water molecules. Results
concept maps, and drawing diagrams. indicated that the drawing group outperformed the control group
68 L. Fiorella, R.E. Mayer / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 66–79

on tests of comprehension and transfer, and the summary group In summary, research is needed to systematically explore stu-
performed worse than the control group. Taken together with the dents’ spontaneous use of spatial strategies during learning. The
early findings from Alesandrini (1981), these results suggest that present study addressed this gap by closely examining the quality
when learning from a scientific text that describes complex spatial of students’ attempted note-taking strategies across ten sections of
relations, using a spatial learning strategy such as creating a draw- a scientific text about the human respiratory system. Strategies
ing is more effective than a verbal learning strategy such as writing were coded based on the frequency with which students used each
a summary. Overall, similarly to mapping, reviews of drawing of five strategies to represent the material in each section of the
(Ainsworth, Prain, & Tytler, 2011; Fiorella & Mayer, 2015; text: words only, list, outline, map, or drawing. The main focus of
Leutner & Schmeck, 2014; Van Meter & Garner, 2005) indicate that the present study was on students’ spontaneous use of spatial
creating illustrations to depict a scientific text generally results in strategies (i.e., maps and drawings). We also tested the role of spa-
better learning than generating verbal summaries or using one’s tial ability in predicting learning outcomes and spontaneous spa-
own strategies. tial strategy use. In short, the present study aimed to provide
insight into (a) the note-taking strategies students spontaneously
4. Spontaneous spatial strategy use use when studying scientific text and (b) relationships among spa-
tial ability, spontaneous spatial strategy use, and learning
It is important to note that the research discussed above outcomes.
involved situations in which learners were told which strategy to
use and often were provided with training in how to use the strat-
egy. Much less research has systematically investigated the sponta- 5. Theory and predictions
neous use of spatial note-taking strategies (such as mapping and
drawing) when learning from a scientific text. Spontaneous strat- According to generative learning theory (Fiorella & Mayer, 2015,
egy use has often been studied by analyzing verbal think-aloud 2016; Wittrock, 1990), meaningful learning depends on whether
protocols, such as the extent to which students generate self- students use appropriate strategies to select the most relevant
explanations during learning (e.g., Azevedo, Guthrie, & Seibert, material from a lesson, organize it into a coherent mental repre-
2004; Chi, Bassok, Lewis, Reimann, & Glaser, 1989); or by analyzing sentation, and integrate it with their prior knowledge – which
text-based note-taking or study strategies, such as the quantity we refer to as generative learning strategies. When learning from a
and quality of students’ lecture notes, or the information students scientific text, this often involves building a mental model of the
underline or highlight when reading a text (e.g., Kiewra & Benton, text to represent complex spatial relations, such as the structures
1988; Peper & Mayer, 1986; Wade & Trathen, 1989). Strategy use is and processes involved in the human respiratory system. Thus,
also often assessed via self-report questionnaires (e.g., Cromley & using spatial learning strategies such as creating a knowledge
Azevedo, 2006; Kardash & Amlund, 1991) or interviews (e.g., Van map or drawing a diagram should support the construction of an
Meter, Yokoi, & Pressley, 1994). Although there is experimental appropriate mental representation of the material, thereby result-
evidence that training students to engage in mapping or drawing ing in better learning outcomes. Study 1 tests the prediction that
can promote learning (as discussed above), strategies imposed by students who spontaneously employ strategies that make use of
an experimenter likely do not reflect the types of strategies stu- space to support knowledge construction (i.e., by creating maps
dents choose when learning on their own (Kardash & Amlund, or drawings) will show better learning outcomes than students
1991; Lahtinen, Lonka, & Lindblom-Ylanne, 1997; Wade & who primarily use text-based verbal strategies (i.e., words only,
Trathen, 1989). This is because students often report using ineffec- lists, or outlines).
tive strategies when studying on their own (Karpicke, Butler, & An additional theoretical consideration addressed in Study 1 is
Roediger, 2009), and without training, might employ effective the role of general spatial ability in explaining spatial strategy use
strategies inappropriately. Furthermore, studying spontaneous and learning. Students with high spatial ability tend to succeed in
strategy use requires students to select, employ, and monitor the STEM fields presumably because they have greater cognitive
use of multiple strategies based on their own judgments about capacity to mentally represent and manipulate complex spatial
the nature of the to-be-learned material and about which strate- concepts. However, it is unclear whether this relationship is medi-
gies they believe are most appropriate, given their perceived level ated by spatial strategy use, or whether spatial ability and spatial
of understanding. Thus, it is important to understand the types of strategy use might provide independent paths to learning in STEM.
strategies that successful students employ spontaneously, and the According to the mediator hypothesis, students with high spatial
factors contributing to effective spontaneous strategy use. ability are able to better represent scientific concepts because they
A study by Lahtinen et al. (1997) provides preliminary evidence are more likely and/or better able to use appropriate spatial strate-
that the spontaneous use of spatial learning strategies might sup- gies than students with low spatial ability. Alternatively, the inde-
port learning from text. Medical school applicants read text pas- pendent paths hypothesis posits that spatial strategy use predicts
sages about risk theories or about health hazards and were told learning even after accounting for the influence of spatial ability.
they could take notes however they preferred. Students’ notes dur- Study 1 directly tests these two hypotheses by analyzing the
ing learning were coded based on one of five categories: no notes, strategies that students use while taking notes on paper. If STEM
underlining only, verbatim notes, summary notes, or concept map- learning is affected independently by both domain-specific spatial
ping. Each student was classified based on the most dominant strategies and domain-general spatial abilities as specified in the
strategy used in his or her notes. Results indicated that students independent paths hypothesis, this suggests that spatial strate-
who spontaneously used generative strategies – taking summary gies—which might be relatively easier to train—might compensate
notes or creating concept maps – performed better on a subse- for spatial abilities.
quent comprehension test, compared to students who did not take Study 2 tests whether the relationships among spatial ability,
notes or who chose rote strategies (underlining or verbatim notes). spatial strategy use, and learning from a scientific text depend on
However, this study is limited because it combined the use of sum- the medium through which students take notes. According to situ-
mary notes or concept maps, and because it categorized students ated theories of cognition, human thought and action is shaped by
based on the most dominant strategy rather than taking into the specific context in which it occurs and the affordances offered
account that students might use multiple strategies during by one’s environment (Anderson, Reder, & Simon, 1996; Greeno &
learning. the Middle School Mathematics Through Applications Project
L. Fiorella, R.E. Mayer / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 66–79 69

Group, 1998; Jarvenoja, Jarvela, & Malmberg, 2015). Thus, different what the terms ‘diastolic’ and ‘systolic’ mean,” and ‘‘I took
note taking environments might afford the use of different learning advanced biology courses in high school (AP, IB, Honors).” This
strategies, resulting in different learning outcomes (e.g., Bui, twelve-item checklist, from Leopold and Mayer (2015), was used
Myerson, & Hale, 2013; Mueller & Oppenheimer, 2014). For exam- as an indicator of students’ self-reported prior knowledge related
ple, whereas taking notes by hand on paper (as in Study 1) might to the human respiratory system (a = 0.57).
afford the spontaneous use of spatial learning strategies, taking The spatial ability tests consisted of the cube comparison test
notes on a laptop computer likely does not, and instead may and the paper folding test, both acquired from the ETS Kit of
encourage the use of primarily verbal strategies. This study also Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests by Ekstrom, French, Harman,
incorporates a third study environment that might more strongly and Dermen (1976). Both tests load on the Spatial Visualization
afford the use of space and encourage the use of spatial strategies (VZ) factor of spatial ability, which involves imagining and men-
– taking notes on a large whiteboard. Further, the note-taking envi- tally manipulating two- and three-dimensional spatial forms
ronment might moderate the influence of spatial ability on learn- (Carroll, 1993). Research suggests spatial visualization is particu-
ing, because situations that do not afford the use of space – such larly important in many disciplines of science, such as biology,
as taking notes on a computer – may rely less heavily on spatial geology, chemistry, and physics (Uttal & Cohen, 2012).
ability. Study 2 tests these predictions by analyzing the strategies The paper-folding test consisted of one sheet with 10 problems.
that students use while taking notes either on paper, on a white- Each item required imagining a paper being folded, punched with a
board, or on a laptop computer. This study helps determine hole, and reopened. The scoring procedure consists of awarding
whether the relationship between spatial strategies and learning one point for each correct answer, and subtracting one point for
outcomes found in Study 1 with paper-based note-taking depend each incorrect answer, for possible scores ranging from 10 to
on the medium with which students take notes. +10. The cube comparison test consisted of one sheet with 21 prob-
lems. Each item required determining whether two cubes, pre-
sented at different orientations and with letters written on each
6. Study 1
side, could be the same cube or must be different cubes. The scor-
ing procedure consists of awarding one point for each correct
The purpose of Study 1 was to explore the relationships among
answer, and subtracting one fifth of a point was for each incorrect
spatial ability, the spontaneous use of spatial learning strategies,
answer, for a total possible score of 21. As expected, the two spatial
and learning from a scientific text. Participants completed spatial
ability measures significantly positively correlated with each other
ability measures, studied and took notes by hand on a scientific
(r = 0.46, p < 0.001). Since there is no theoretical basis for expecting
text about the human respiratory system, and then completed
the two measures to differentially predict spatial strategy use or
learning outcome measures (including retention, transfer, and
learning outcomes, we created a composite spatial ability score
drawing items) on the material. Students’ notes were coded for
by averaging the z scores of the two measures.
each of ten sections of the lesson, based on the frequency at which
The lesson on the human respiratory system (adapted from
they used no strategy, words only, lists, outlines, maps, or draw-
Leopold & Mayer, 2015) contained 786 words and consisted of an
ings to represent the content of the lesson.
introductory part and nine main parts. Each of the ten parts of
the lesson was presented on a separate PowerPoint slide with
6.1. Method the following headings: (1) Introduction, (2) Structure of the Ner-
vous System, (3) Steps in the Nervous System to Control Breathing,
6.1.1. Participants and design (4) Structure of the Thoracic Cavity, (5) Structure of the Airway
Participants were 108 undergraduate students recruited from System, (6) Process of Inhaling, (7) Structure of the Exchange Sys-
the Psychology Subject Pool at a large western university, who tem, (8) Structure of the Circulatory System, (9) Process of
received course credit for their participation. The mean age was Exchanging, and (10) Process of Exhaling. Students clicked on each
19.0 (SD = 1.2), and there were 32 men and 76 women. Self- slide to move from one part to the next at their own pace. The
reported prior knowledge related to the human respiratory system mean study time was 978.2 (SD = 284.3) s. The complete lesson is
was generally low (M = 3.9; SD = 2.2), as reported on a question- presented in Appendix A.
naire with a maximum score of 12. The note-taking sheets consisted of five stapled sheets of paper,
with each sheet containing two labeled empty boxes correspond-
6.1.2. Materials ing to the ten parts of the lesson. Students recorded their notes
Materials consisted of a consent form, a demographics ques- in the corresponding boxes as they read each part of the lesson.
tionnaire, spatial ability tests, a lesson on the human respiratory Notes were coded by the type of strategy (or strategies) students
system, note-taking sheets, learning outcome tests, and a post- used for each part of the lesson:
experiment questionnaire. The consent form described the details
of the study and included a place for participants to sign. The  No strategy: The student did not write anything in the box.
demographics questionnaire asked participants to provide their  Words only: The student wrote text only, without showing evi-
age and gender. It also asked participants to place a check mark dence of using a list or an outline; see below). For example, a
next to each of the following twelve items that applied to them: student may write a string of words, a complete sentence, or
‘‘I have participated in science programs or fairs,” ‘‘Biology was a paragraph about the material. Fig. 1 presents a representative
my favorite subject in high school,” ‘‘I sometimes watch science example of a participant using the words only strategy.
documentaries about anatomy in my free time,” ‘‘I can name most  List: The student wrote text presented as a sequence, such as by
of the parts of the human heart from memory,” ‘‘I have taken a using bullet points or numbering. Thus, the list strategy differs
course in human anatomy or physiology,” ‘‘I attended a course from the words-only strategy in that it involves arranging one’s
on cardiopulmonary resuscitation (CPR),” ‘‘I can explain what pul- notes from top-to-bottom on the page. Fig. 2 presents a repre-
monary embolism means,” ‘‘I sometimes find myself on the Inter- sentative example of a participant using the list strategy.
net looking up biology related topics,” ‘‘I have watched an  Outline: The student wrote text that was arranged from top-to-
educational video on how the respiratory system works,” ‘‘I talked bottom as well as from left-to-right on the page. Generally,
to a doctor about the process of how respiration works,” ‘‘I know students wrote a main idea or heading at the top; then below,
70 L. Fiorella, R.E. Mayer / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 66–79

Example of Words-Only Strategy from Study 1 as a matrix organizer. Generally, students used maps by con-
necting elements of text with arrows, either linearly or as a
web.1 Fig. 4 presents a representative example of a participant
using the map strategy.
 Drawing: The student created a pictorial representation of the
material that represented appearance and location of specific
parts of the human respiratory system, such as the heart or
lungs. Fig. 5 presents a representative example of a participant
using the drawing strategy.

For each of the ten sections of the lesson, students received one
point for each type of strategy they attempted. Students could also
receive credit for multiple strategies on a given section. Thus,
scores for each type of strategy ranged from 0 (i.e., the strategy
was used on 0 sections of the lesson) to 10 (i.e., the strategy was
used on all ten sections of the lesson). A spatial strategy score
was calculated by adding the number of maps and diagrams scores,
Fig. 1. Example of words-only strategy from Study 1. for a total possible score of 20. Two raters scored the note-taking
sheets and any discrepancies were settled by consensus. Inter-
rater reliability on the first 25% of the data (n = 27) was generally
Example of List Strategy for Study 1
high across the two raters (no strategy: r = 0.99; words only:
r = 0.98, lists: r = 0.77, outlines: r = 0.83; maps: r = 0.93; drawings:
r = 0.98).
The learning outcome measures (from Leopold & Mayer, 2015)
consisted of one retention item, five transfer items, and two draw-
ing items. Each of the eight items was printed on its own sheet of
paper. Table 1 presents the test items and examples of acceptable
responses. For scoring the retention item, students received one
point for each correct idea unit included in their response (out of
76 possible idea units from the text). The participant did not have
to show the exact wording or correct spelling to receive credit for
an idea unit but had to express the correct idea.
For the five transfer items (a = 0.59), students received one
point for each acceptable response. For the two drawing items
(a = 0.51), students received two points for each correctly drawn
and labeled component, one point for each partially correctly-
Fig. 2. Example of list strategy for Study 1. drawn (and correctly labeled) component, and zero points for com-
ponents that were not correctly drawn or that were not labeled.
The drawing test was worth a total of 22 possible points. Inter-
Example of Outline Strategy for Study 1 rater reliability between total scores of two raters based on the first
25% of the data (n = 27) was r = 0.84 for the retention test, r = 0.79
for the transfer test, and r = 0.79 for the drawing test.
To maximize the reliability of our measure of learning out-
comes, we created a composite learning outcome score by averag-
ing the z scores of performance on the retention, transfer, and
drawing tests. Each of these tests significantly correlated with each
other (r’s > 0.47, p’s < 0.001). Further, a confirmatory factor analy-
sis suggested that a single factor represented an appropriate factor
structure among the eight items, accounting for 37% of the vari-
ance. The reliability of the composite learning outcome measure
is a = 66.
The post-experiment questionnaire was intended to assess stu-
dents’ perceived difficulty of the material, motivation to learn, per-
ceived understanding of the material, learning preferences/
strategies, and mental effort exerted to learning. Students were
asked to rate their level agreement, on a five-point scale ranging
Fig. 3. Example of outline strategy for Study 1.
(1) strongly disagree to (5) strongly agree, for each of seven state-
ments: ‘‘I felt that the subject matter was difficult,” ‘‘I enjoyed
learning from this lesson,” ‘‘I would like to learn more about the
students indented to the right and wrote supporting details.
Fig. 3 presents a representative example of a participant using
1
the outline strategy. Our operationalization of ‘map’ is broader than past research distinguishing
 Map: The student created an abstract spatial representation of between knowledge maps, concept maps, and graphic organizers (e.g., Holley &
Dansereau, 1984). In the present study, a map involved any abstract spatial
the material by using shapes, lines, arrows, or brackets to group arrangement attempting to connect or group elements of text. In general, if the
or connect multiple elements of text (words or phrases). Maps strategy organized the material beyond a typical outline but did not involve a
may be arranged linearly with arrows, as a web with lines, or concrete drawing, it was considered a map.
L. Fiorella, R.E. Mayer / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 66–79 71

Example of Map Strategy for Study 1 Table 1


Retention, transfer and drawing test items.

Question Question Example accepted responses


type
Retention Using what you have learned in Brain detects need for oxygen;
the session, please write and diaphragm contracts
explanation of how the human downward; thoracic cavity
respiratory system works contains the lungs
Transfer Although there is oxygen in the Heart not pumping; capillaries
lungs, the cells in the body do are not picking up oxygen
not get enough oxygen to make
energy. What could have
caused this problem?
Suppose you are a scientist who Increase number of capillaries
is trying to improve the human surrounding alveoli; increase
respiratory system for people volume/capacity of lungs
who climb high mountains
(where less oxygen is in the
air). What could be done to
Fig. 4. Example of map strategy for Study 1.
make the human respiratory
system more effective for
mountain climbing?
Example of Drawing Strategy for Study 1 Joe has small lungs and Moe Joe has more frequent
has big lungs, but both have the inhaling/exhaling; less oxygen
same body height and weight, in his lungs
and the same kind of lifestyle.
Is there a difference in their
respiration? Please explain
When Sue is afraid, her heart Frequency of exchange
rate increases. What happens to process increases; body/cells
her respiration? Why does this need more energy
happen?
What does the concept of Related to inhalation process
suction have to do with the of taking in air into the lungs
respiration process?
Drawing Please draw a picture of the Draw and label lungs and
exchange system and label the bronchial tubes
different parts
Please draw a picture of the Draw and label diaphragm
respiratory system when the moving down
person inhales and label the
different parts
Fig. 5. Example of drawing strategy for Study 1.

respiratory system in the future,” ‘‘I feel like I have a good under- the human respiratory system and would later be asked to answer
standing of how the respiratory system works,” ‘‘I prefer to learn some questions about what they learned. They were told that the
visually,” ‘‘I formed mental pictures about the text content in order lesson consisted of ten parts presented on individual slides and
to understand it,” and ‘‘I felt motivated to try to understand the that they should ‘‘put anything on the sheets in front of you that
material.” Students also rated their level of effort exerted during you think will help you learn and remember the material.” Finally,
learning on a scale from (1) very low to (5) very high. Data from students were told that they would have time to review what they
the post-experiment questionnaire were not analyzed in this study put on their sheets of paper before being tested on the material.
because they are not relevant to our primary research questions. Students studied the 10 slides in the lesson sequentially and at
The apparatus consisted of five Dell computers with 17-in. their own pace. The total amount of time students spent studying
screens. the lesson was recorded.
After studying the lesson, students were given 3 min to review
what they had recorded on their note-taking sheets without refer-
6.1.3. Procedure ring back to the lesson. Then, the experimenter collected the note-
Students were tested in groups of two to five per session. Each taking sheets and administered the learning outcome measures.
student was seated at an individual cubicle in front of a computer Students were given 5 min for the retention item, and they were
and out of sight from the other participants. First, the experimenter given 2.5 min for each of the transfer and drawing items. Finally,
provided a brief oral introduction to the experiment, passed out students completed the post-experiment questionnaire at their
the consent form to sign, and collected the consent forms. Second, own pace and then were debriefed and thanked for their participa-
the experimenter passed out the demographics questionnaire and tion. The study lasted a total of approximately 60 min.
collected them when participants were finished. Third the experi-
menter administered the two spatial ability tests: the cube com-
parison test and the paper-folding test. For each test, participants
first read an instructions sheet and then were given 3 min to work 6.2. Results
on the test sheet.
After participants completed the spatial ability tests, the exper- The primary research questions for Study 1 concern the types of
imenter passed out the note-taking sheets and asked students to strategies students use spontaneously during learning and the
read instructions presented on the computer screen. The instruc- relationships among spatial ability, spatial strategy use, and learn-
tions informed students that they would be studying a lesson on ing outcomes.
72 L. Fiorella, R.E. Mayer / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 66–79

6.2.1. What types of learning strategies do students use beyond spatial ability, consistent with the independent paths
spontaneously? hypothesis.
Table 2 reports the descriptive statistics for the frequency of To test whether spatial strategy use partially mediates the rela-
each type of strategy used across the ten sections of the lesson. tionship between spatial ability and learning outcomes, we con-
To compare the frequency of each strategy type, paired-samples t ducted a mediation analysis using bootstrapping, based on
tests were conducted with Bonferroni corrections applied, result- guidelines by Hayes and Preacher (2014). First, the direct effect
ing in a significance level of p < 0.005. Results indicated that stu- of spatial ability on learning outcomes was statistically significant
dents used significantly more lists than each of the other four (b = 0.28; SE = 0.09), t(106) = 3.22, p = 0.002. Second, the path from
strategies – words only (p < 0.001, d = 0.88), outlines (p < 0.001, spatial ability to spatial strategy use was not statistically signifi-
d = 0.37), maps (p < 0.001, d = 0.43), and drawings (p < 0.001, cant (b = 0.19; SE = 0.11), t(106) = 1.73, p = 0.085, but the path from
d = 0.91). Students also used significantly more maps than words spatial strategy use to learning outcomes was statistically signifi-
only (p < 0.001, d = 0.44), and they used more outlines (p < 0.001, cant (b = 0.18; SE = 0.07), t(106) = 2.38, p = 0.019. Finally, using a
d = 0.51) and maps (p < 0.001, d = 0.65) than drawings. These data 95% confidence interval of the indirect effects obtained from
suggest that when studying a scientific text, students tend to lar- 5000 bootstrap resamples, the analysis indicated that the indirect
gely rely on creating verbal lists, and they create relatively few effect of spatial ability on learning outcomes through spatial strat-
drawings. egy use was not statistically significant (b = 0.03; SE: 0.03;
Regarding spatial strategy use, 82% of participants (n = 89) used CI = 0.001 to 0.101). Fig. 6 depicts the results of the mediation
at least one map but only 39% of participants (n = 42) used at least analysis.
one drawing. Participants’ use of maps and drawings was not sig- Overall, these results provide evidence in support of the inde-
nificantly correlated (r = 0.14, p = 0.139). Overall, 89% of partici- pendent paths hypothesis and against the mediator hypothesis –
pants (n = 96) created at least one map or drawing across the ten that is, rather than mediating the relationship between spatial abil-
sections of the lesson. The bottom row of Table 2 presents the ity and learning outcomes, spatial strategy use appears to indepen-
descriptive statistics for the use of spatial strategies (i.e., the sum dently predict learning outcomes. Given the independent
of maps and diagrams). This composite spatial strategies score relationship between spatial strategy use and learning outcomes
was used in the following analyses. for STEM material, an important avenue for future research
involves techniques for improving students’ spatial strategies
(which might be more trainable and more transferable to STEM
6.2.2. How are spatial ability, strategy use, and learning outcomes
learning) rather than spatial abilities (which might be less train-
related?
able and less transferable to STEM learning).
Table 3 reports the correlation coefficients among the key vari-
ables of interest – spatial ability, spatial strategy use (i.e., use of
maps and drawings), and learning outcomes. Results indicate that
7. Study 2
spatial ability significantly correlated with learning outcomes
(r = 0.33, p < 0.001) but not with spatial strategy use (r = 0.17,
The purpose of Study 2 was to experimentally test whether dif-
p = 0.085). Further, spatial strategy use significantly correlated
ferent note-taking environments cause students to spontaneously
with learning outcomes (r = 0.27, p = 0.005). In contrast, the use
choose different learning strategies. Previous research comparing
of verbal strategies (words only, lists, and outlines) was not signif-
note-taking media (e.g., computer vs. hand-written notes) has
icantly correlated with learning outcomes (r = 0.06, p = 0 0.556).
focused on the use of verbal strategies and its effects on learning
These data indicate that spatial ability and spatial strategy use
outcomes, showing somewhat mixed results (e.g., Bui et al.,
may be somewhat independently related to learning outcomes.
2013; Mueller & Oppenheimer, 2014). Bui et al. (2013) found that
transcribing notes on a computer led to better immediate recall
6.2.3. What is the role of spatial ability and spatial strategy use in from a lecture than taking notes by hand. However, Mueller and
explaining learning outcomes? Oppenheimer (2014) found that students who took notes by hand
The mediator hypothesis predicts that spatial strategy use during a lecture performed better on conceptual questions than
mediates the relationship between spatial ability and overall learn- students who took notes on a computer, likely because students
ing outcomes – that is, students with high spatial ability perform taking notes on a computer tended to take verbatim notes,
better on learning outcome measures because they use more spa- whereas students taking notes by hand tended to summarize the
tial learning strategies. Alternatively, the independent paths material into their own words. Study 2 focused on how note-
hypothesis predicts that spatial ability and spatial strategy use taking mediums influence the spontaneous use of spatial learning
independently predict learning outcomes. To test these hypotheses strategies and the relationships among spatial ability, strategy
directly, we conducted (a) a hierarchical regression analysis to use, and learning outcomes.
determine the extent to which spatial ability and spatial strategy Participants studied the human respiratory system lesson and
use uniquely predict learning outcomes, and (b) a mediation anal- took notes either by writing on paper (paper group), writing on a
ysis to test whether spatial strategy use partially mediates the rela- whiteboard (whiteboard group), or typing on a laptop computer
tionship between spatial ability and learning outcomes. (computer group). The paper group most resembles how students
First, a hierarchical regression analysis was conducted, with took notes in Study 1, although there were not separate boxes for
learning outcomes as the outcome variable and spatial ability (Step the notes on each part of the lesson. As the study by Mueller and
1) and spatial strategy (Step 2) entered as predictor variables. Anal- Oppenheimer (2014) demonstrated, different note-taking medi-
ysis of the residuals of indicated that the assumption of normality ums can encourage students to use different types of strategies
was met. Step 1 of the model was statistically significant, F(1, 106) (such as verbatim or summary notes). This is in line with a situated
= 12.92, p < 0.001, with spatial ability explaining 10.9% of the vari- view of cognition and learning (Anderson et al., 1996; Greeno,
ance in learning outcomes (b = 0.33). In Step 2, spatial strategy use 1998; Jarvenoja et al., 2015), which posits that the affordances
explained an additional 4.6% of the variance in learning outcomes and limitations of one’s environment influences how one repre-
beyond spatial ability (b = 0.22). The change from Step 1 to Step 2 sents and solves problems. Study 2 extended this basic idea to
was statistically significant, F(1, 105) = 5.67, p = 0.019. This sug- the spontaneous use of spatial note-taking strategies that might
gests that spatial ability uniquely predicts learning outcomes be afforded by different note-taking environments. Specifically,
L. Fiorella, R.E. Mayer / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 66–79 73

Table 2
Strategy use frequency across ten sections of the lesson in Study 1.

Strategy Frequency Skewness Kurtosis


M SD Statistic SE Statistic SE
No strategy 0.5 1.2 3.7 0.2 16.1 0.5
Words only 1.5 1.7 2.1 0.2 7.2 0.5
List 4.3 2.5 0.1 0.2 0.6 0.5
Outline 2.9 2.4 0.9 0.2 0.2 0.5
Maps 2.8 2.1 0.4 0.2 0.3 0.5
Drawings 1.1 1.9 2.0 0.2 3.8 0.5
Spatial strategiesa 4.0 3.0 0.8 0.2 <0.1 0.5
a
Total frequency of maps and diagrams.

Table 3 cating that participants in the paper group were older than partic-
Correlations among spatial ability, strategy use, and learning outcomes for Study 1. ipants in the whiteboard group. However, follow-up analyses
Variable Spatial ability Spatial strategy use indicated that age was not a significant covariate of the primary
Spatial ability dependent measures and adding it as a covariate did not affect
Spatial strategy use .17y the results of the analyses reported below.
Learning outcomes .33** .27**
**
p < 0.01. 7.1.2. Materials
y
p < 0.10.
The materials were mostly identical to Study 1, with the excep-
tion of how the human respiratory system lesson was presented to
students. Rather than presenting each of the ten parts of the lesson
taking notes by typing on a computer should lead students to use
on individual PowerPoint slides (as done in Study 1), each part of
more verbal strategies (such as words only), given that typing does
the lesson was printed on individual 3-inch by 5-inch flashcards.
not easily afford the use of spatial strategies (such as creating maps
This allowed each of the experimental groups to use their respec-
and drawings). In contrast, writing notes by hand (either on paper
tive medium solely for taking notes on the lesson – a whiteboard, a
or on a whiteboard) may more easily allow students to attempt to
laptop, or on paper by hand. The 10 flashcards were bound
represent material spatially. Although somewhat unconventional,
together so that participants had to study and take notes on one
the whiteboard group was included because the availability of a
flashcard at a time. Participants were instructed not to return to
large open space on which to take notes might especially afford
a previously studied part of the lesson.
the use of spatial strategies, compared to more conventional
hand-written notes on paper. Overall, Study 2 tested whether stu-
dents could be induced to use different strategies during learning, 7.1.3. Procedure
and how such strategy differences related to spatial ability and The procedure was mostly identical to Study 1, with the excep-
learning outcomes. tion of how participants studied and took notes on the human res-
piratory system lesson. Participants were randomly assigned to a
7.1. Method treatment condition. Students in the computer group were asked
to take notes using a blank Word document on a laptop computer.
7.1.1. Participants and design Students in the whiteboard group were asked to take notes on a
Participants were 94 undergraduate students from a large west- large whiteboard attached to the wall, and therefore had to stand
ern university. The mean age was 19.6 (SD = 1.6), and there were while they studied and took notes. Students in the paper group
41 men and 53 women. Thirty-one students served in the com- were asked to takes notes on blank, unlined sheets of paper. Thus,
puter group, 31 served in the whiteboard group, and 32 served in unlike in Study 1, students in Study 2 did not use note-taking
the paper group. The groups did not differ in terms proportion of sheets that separated the ten parts of the lesson into separate
women and men, spatial ability, or prior knowledge of the human boxes, and instead used a blank Word document, whiteboard, or
respiratory system. The groups did differ significantly in terms of sheet of paper. All other aspects of the procedure for Experiment
mean age, F(2, 93) = 5.23, p = 0.007, with Tukey post hoc tests indi- 2 were the same as in Experiment 1.

Mediation Analysis of Spatial Ability, Spatial Strategy Use, and Learning Outcomes in Study 1

Spatial strategy
.19 use .18*

Learning
Spatial ability
outcomes
.28**

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.001

Fig. 6. Mediation analysis of spatial ability, spatial strategy use, and learning outcomes in Study 1. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.001.
74 L. Fiorella, R.E. Mayer / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 66–79

7.2. Results not differ significantly from each other (p = 0.353, d = 0.35). For
the transfer test, the computer and whiteboard groups both out-
The primary research questions for Study 2 concern the rela- performed the paper group (p < 0.001, d = 1.49; and p = 0.011,
tionships among spatial ability, spatial strategy use, and learning d = 0.74, respectively), but did not significantly differ from each
outcomes across note-taking environments, and the influence of other (p = 0.162, d = 0.44). Finally, for the drawing test, the com-
note-taking environment on strategy use and learning outcomes. puter group outperformed the whiteboard group (p = 0.010;
Due to a technical issue, we do not have data for one participant’s d = 0.81), but did not significantly differ from the paper group
notes from the computer group. This participant’s data is excluded (p = 0.231, d = 0.40), and the whiteboard group did not significantly
from the relevant analyses presented below. differ from the paper group (p = 0.363; d = 0.33).
We also compared the three groups in terms of overall learning
7.2.1. Did the groups differ on learning strategy use? outcomes, based on the average of the z scores across the reten-
Table 4 reports descriptive statistics of strategy use frequency tion, transfer, and drawing items for each of the three groups.
across the ten sections of the lesson for each of the three groups. The ANOVA was statistically significant, F(2, 91) = 11.19,
To compare strategy frequency across groups, one-way ANOVAs p < 0.001, g2 = 0.20, and Tukey post hoc tests indicated that the
were conducted, with group serving as the independent variable computer group outperformed the paper group (p < 0.001;
and number of each strategy used serving as dependent variables. d = 1.25) and the whiteboard group (p = 0.004; d = 0.81), but the
When assumptions of homogeneity of variance were not met (as whiteboard and paper groups did not significantly differ from each
indicated by Levene’s test), Welch’s test was used. other (p = 0.442, d = 0.31).
The analysis indicated significant differences across groups in Taken together, these data indicate somewhat surprising differ-
the use of words only, F(2, 56.20) = 4.89, p = 0.011, outlines, F ences across the three groups on learning outcomes. Despite using
(2, 59.12) = 11.59, p < 0.001, maps, F(2, 90) = 4.87, p = 0.010, fewer spatial learning strategies, students who took notes using
g2 = 0.10, and total spatial strategies, F(2, 53.14) = 11.29, the laptop generally outperformed students who took notes by
p < 0.001. Post-hoc tests were then conducted to further explore hand, either on a whiteboard or on paper. In short, different
these effects. First, the computer group used the words only strat- note-taking environments led to differences in both strategy use
egy significantly more than the whiteboard group (p = 0.008; and learning outcomes.
d = 0.80). Second, the paper group used significantly more outlines
than the computer group (p = 0.004; d = 0.85) and the whiteboard
group (p < 0.001; d = 1.22). Third, the whiteboard group used sig- 7.2.3. How are spatial ability, strategy use, and learning outcomes
nificantly more maps than the computer group (p = 0.008; related for each of the groups?
d = 0.88). Finally, consistent with predictions, the paper group To relate the findings to Study 1, we explored the patterns of
(p = 0.014, d = 1.07) and the whiteboard group (p < 0.001, relationships among spatial ability, spatial strategy use, and learn-
d = 0.74) both used significantly more spatial strategies overall ing outcomes for each of the three groups. It is important to note
than the computer group. These data support the hypothesis that that given the relatively low sample size for this type of analysis
taking notes by hand (either on a computer or a whiteboard) leads (n  30 per group), the statistical power needed to detect signifi-
students to use more spatial strategies than taking notes on a cant relationships was relatively low. Table 6 presents the correla-
computer. tion coefficients among the three key variables of interest – spatial
We also analyzed the total number of words that appeared in ability, spatial strategy use, and learning outcomes – across each of
students’ notes and the total amount of time students spent study- the three groups. As can be seen, notable patterns emerge across
ing. Regarding the word count of students’ notes, Levene’s test the three groups. First, the positive correlation coefficients among
indicated heterogeneity of variance across conditions; thus, spatial ability, spatial strategy use, and learning outcomes for the
Welch’s test was used. Welch’s test was statistically significant, F paper group presented in Table 6 are consistent with the pattern
(2, 57.92) = 8.76, p < 0.001. Games-Howell post hoc tests indicated of relationships found in Study 1 (presented in Table 3). In contrast,
that students in the computer group (M = 215.7; SD = 134.3) the correlation coefficients for the computer group are either close
included significantly more words in their notes than the white- to zero or negative. Finally, for the whiteboard group, spatial ability
board group (M = 98.5; SD = 87.4; p = 0.001, d = 1.03), but not sig- was significantly positively related to learning outcomes (r = 0.47,
nificantly more than the paper group (M = 159.0; SD = 95.1; p = 0.007), but the other relationships are non-significant (and neg-
p = 0.147, d = 0.49). Students in the paper group also included sig- ative). Although the present data do not warrant strong conclu-
nificantly more words in their notes than the whiteboard group sions, they do suggest that the relationships among spatial
(p = 0.029; d = 0.66). Despite these differences in word count, an ability, spatial strategy use, and learning outcomes might depend
ANOVA indicated no significant differences across groups in total on the medium with which students take notes.
study time, F(2, 91) = 0.32, p = 0.728.

7.2.2. Did the groups differ on learning outcomes? 8. Discussion


Table 5 reports the means and standard deviations of the learn-
ing outcome tests for each of the three groups. Levene’s test indi- 8.1. Empirical contributions
cated that the variance across groups was homogeneous. One-
way ANOVAs were conducted, with group serving as the indepen- The present study provides insight into the relationships among
dent variable and performance on the retention, transfer, and spatial ability, the spontaneous use of spatial learning strategies
drawing tests, serving as the dependent measures. The analysis during note taking and learning from a scientific text. Study 1
indicated statistically significant differences across groups for the results indicated that both spatial ability and the spontaneous cre-
retention test, F(2, 91) = 9.37, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.17, transfer test, F ation of maps and drawings during note taking independently pre-
(2, 91) = 11.89, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.21, and drawing test, F(2, 91) dicted learning outcomes – providing support for the independent
= 4.48, p = 0.014, g2 = 0.09. Tukey post hoc tests indicated that paths hypothesis and against the mediator hypothesis. That is,
for the retention test, the computer group significantly outper- even after accounting for spatial ability, students who sponta-
formed the whiteboard group (p = 0.015, d = 0.69) and the paper neously used more spatial learning strategies while taking notes
group (p < 0.001, d = 1.13). The whiteboard and paper groups did on paper showed better learning outcomes.
L. Fiorella, R.E. Mayer / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 66–79 75

Table 4
Strategy use frequency across ten sections of the lesson for the three groups in Study 2.

Strategy Frequency Skewness Kurtosis


M SD Statistic SE Statistic SE
Computer group
No strategy 1.4 2.1 2.6 0.4 9.3 0.8
Words only 3.7 2.8 0.4 0.4 0.7 0.8
List 3.6 2.8 0.5 0.4 0.9 0.8
Outline 1.0 1.5 1.6 0.4 1.2 0.8
Maps 0.8 1.2 2.3 0.4 6.4 0.8
Drawings – – – – – –
Spatial strategiesa 0.8 1.2 2.3 0.4 6.4 0.8
Whiteboard group
No strategy 2.9 3.4 1.1 0.4 0.9 0.8
Words only 1.9 1.5 0.2 0.4 0.8 0.8
List 3.6 2.5 <0.1 0.4 0.9 0.8
Outline 0.4 1.4 4.9 0.4 25.4 0.8
Maps 2.2 1.9 0.5 0.4 0.7 0.8
Drawings 0.7 1.3 2.1 0.4 3.7 0.8
Spatial strategiesa 2.9 2.5 0.4 0.4 0.9 0.8
Paper group
No strategy 1.4 1.8 2.0 0.4 3.5 0.8
Words only 2.3 1.9 1.2 0.4 1.2 0.8
List 3.5 2.3 0.7 0.4 0.4 0.8
Outline 2.5 2.0 0.4 0.4 0.9 0.8
Maps 1.7 2.1 2.3 0.4 6.6 0.8
Drawings 0.8 1.9 4.2 0.4 20.3 0.8
Spatial strategiesa 2.5 3.0 1.9 0.4 3.3 0.8
a
Total frequency of maps and diagrams.

Table 5
Learning outcomes of three groups for Study 2.

Strategy Computer group Whiteboard group Paper group


M SD M SD M SD
Retention test 20.6 7.5 15.1 8.4 12.4 7.0
Transfer test 9.7 2.2 8.5 3.2 6.5 2.1
Drawing test 13.0 3.8 9.9 3.9 11.3 4.6

Table 6 2014) and others supporting computer-based notes (e.g., Bui


Correlations among key variables for three groups in Study 2. et al., 2013). One possibility is that the college students in the
present study were more experienced taking computer-based
Group Variable Spatial ability Spatial strategies
notes than taking hand-written notes and were able to use their
Computer group Spatial ability
own verbal strategies effectively with this medium. Also, in the
Spatial strategy use 0.05
Learning outcomes 0.03 0.19
paper group, using paper both to present information (as a book-
let of note cards) and for taking notes (on sheets of paper) may
Whiteboard group Spatial ability
Spatial strategy use 0.19
have caused interference unlike taking hand-written notes from
Learning outcomes 0.47** 0.13 a face-to-face lecture or computer-presented lesson. In short,
Paper group Spatial ability
Study 2 demonstrates how different note taking environments
Spatial strategy use 0.32 can shape the way in which students represent material from
Learning outcomes 0.27y 0.35y a scientific text, ultimately resulting in differential learning
y
p < 0.10.
outcomes.
**
p < 0.01. Study 2 also indicated that the relationships among spatial abil-
ity, spatial strategy use, and learning outcomes might depend on
the medium though which students take notes, although this find-
Study 2 indicated that spontaneous strategy use and learning ing warrants further research with more participants. The pattern
outcomes depended on the medium with which students took of relationships for the paper group were highly similar to those
notes. As expected, taking notes on a whiteboard or on paper found in Study 1, suggesting that spatial ability, spatial strategy
led students to use more spatial strategies (maps and drawings). use, and learning are generally positively associated with each
Given this finding, it was somewhat surprising that students in other. However, the whiteboard and computer groups did not
the computer group generally outperformed the other two groups appear to show this same pattern, with many of the relationships
on the learning outcome measures. However, it is important to either near zero or negative. This raises the possibility that the
note that previous research testing the relationship between affordances offered by different note taking environments might
note-taking medium and learning is mixed, with some studies moderate the role of spatial ability and spatial strategy use in pre-
supporting hand-written notes (e.g., Mueller & Oppenheimer, dicting learning.
76 L. Fiorella, R.E. Mayer / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 66–79

8.2. Theoretical contributions important contributor to the effects of note taking on learning
(Kiewra, 1989).
Findings from the present study are generally consistent with Findings from Study 2 also suggest potential boundary condi-
predictions made by generative learning theory (Fiorella & tions of the independent paths hypothesis, which was supported
Mayer, 2015, 2016; Wittrock, 1990) and situated theories of cogni- in Study 1. Specifically, the role of spatial ability and spatial strat-
tion (Anderson et al., 1996; Greeno, 1998). In Study 1, when stu- egy use in predicting learning appears to depend on whether stu-
dents took notes on paper, the spontaneous use of spatial dents take notes in situations that afford the use of spatial
learning strategies predicted learning outcomes even after control- strategies. For students who took notes on paper, a similar general
ling for spatial ability. This finding is consistent with generative pattern of data was observed as in Study 1. For students who took
learning theory, which posits that effective learning occurs when notes on a computer, neither spatial ability nor spatial strategy use
students select strategies that support organizing the material into appeared to contribute to learning outcomes.
a coherent mental model based on the text’s underlying structure. Finally, for the whiteboard group, spatial ability strongly pre-
Creating knowledge maps and drawings support the construction dicted learning, but spatial strategy use did not. Closer analysis
of learners’ mental representation of the structures and processes of students’ note taking strategies might help explain this pattern.
involved in the human respiratory system. By comparison, text- Consistent with predictions, students in the whiteboard group gen-
based strategies such as words only, listing, or outlining, did not erally used more spatial strategies than the computer group, yet
appear to support the construction of an appropriate mental model they took fewer notes overall – that is, students in the whiteboard
during learning when students took notes on paper. group showed more instances of not taking notes on a given sec-
Study 1 also clarifies the relationship among spatial ability, tion of the lesson, and their notes contained far fewer words. It is
spatial strategy use, and learning outcomes for when students possible that students were not as comfortable or familiar with this
take notes on paper. The results suggest that the use of spatial method of note taking including having to stand up while studying,
strategies while taking notes on paper does not depend on the especially compared to taking notes on paper or on a laptop, which
student’s level of spatial ability; rather, spatial strategy appears is common among college students. Further, given that students
to predict learning independently of spatial ability – providing used a large whiteboard to display their notes, there might have
support for the independent paths hypothesis. In other words, been an observer effect, such that students felt more as if the
regardless of one’s level of spatial ability, some students acquire experimenter was evaluating them, compared to students in the
and use spatial strategies to help them represent the material to other groups who could take more concealed notes. This might
support learning, whereas other students rely primarily on verbal have led participants to take fewer notes on the board and conse-
strategies. quently place a higher demand on processing the material
Findings from Study 2 demonstrate how the affordances offered internally.
by one’s note-taking environment can influence spontaneous strat-
egy use and learning outcomes. Regarding strategy use, the results 8.3. Practical contributions
supported predictions that note-taking environments that afford
the use of space (taking notes on paper or on a whiteboard) led The present study indicates that general spatial ability and
to greater use of spatial strategies than a note-taking environment domain-specific strategy use serve somewhat independent roles
that constrains the use of space (taking notes on a computer). This in learning from a scientific text. This distinction provides impor-
suggests that strategy choice is influenced not only by students’ tant practical implications for instruction, as it suggests two differ-
strategic knowledge but on the specific context in which that ent routes – ability-based and strategy-based – to learning in STEM
knowledge is to be employed. disciplines. Given the link between spatial ability and STEM
The use of different learning strategies by the three groups also achievement (Wai et al., 2009), much research has focused on
subsequently led to different learning outcomes, with the com- attempts to train generalizable spatial skills (Uttal et al., 2013).
puter group generally outperforming the other two groups. Analy- However, this approach ultimately depends on achieving transfer
sis of students’ study behaviors might help explain the computer from performance on general psychometric tests of spatial ability
group’s superior performance in the present study. As mentioned to improved understanding of STEM concepts in authentic settings.
above, the students who took notes on a laptop generally took A long history of research on transfer suggests that transfer tends
more notes than the other two groups, and they also did so in to be highly context-dependent.
the same amount of time – the groups did not significantly differ Focusing on domain-specific strategy use provides a potentially
on total study time. Given that typing is generally faster than tak- more malleable route to improved learning in STEM, which the
ing notes by hand, students in the paper and whiteboard groups present study suggests does not depend on whether students have
likely spent a greater proportion of their study time dealing with low or high spatial ability. Indeed, there is a vast literature demon-
the mechanics of writing compared to students in the computer strating that teaching students to use specific strategies that
group. Further, past research indicates that using spatial strategies involve organizing the material into a coherent structure and inte-
might require more time and cognitive effort than using verbal grating it with existing knowledge can promote learning (Fiorella &
strategies (e.g., Leutner et al., 2009; Van Meter, 2001). This sug- Mayer, 2015, 2016; Dunlosky et al., 2013). In short, if spatial strate-
gests that students who took notes on a whiteboard or on paper gies can compensate for spatial ability in promoting STEM learning
might have experienced greater extraneous cognitive processing and are more malleable, there is reason to focus on helping stu-
during learning compared to students who took notes on a dents develop effective spatial learning strategies.
computer. Study 2 suggests that the affordances of different note taking
Another important consideration is that students were given situations can influence both the quality and the quantity of stu-
time to review their notes without access to the lesson before dents’ notes. Spatial strategies are more easily afforded from taking
being tested on the material. This review period might have con- notes on paper or on a whiteboard, whereas students are likely to
tributed to the benefits of taking notes on a computer. Since use primarily verbal strategies when taking notes on a computer. A
students in the computer group generally took more notes, there computer also affords taking more notes in the same amount of
was more information for them to review, compared to the white- time, providing students with more information to review after
board group and the paper group. Although the present study did viewing a lesson. In Study 2, the ability to take more notes and sub-
not focus on the effects of note taking review, it can be an sequently review more material led to superior learning outcomes.
L. Fiorella, R.E. Mayer / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 66–79 77

Of course, this does not necessarily mean that taking notes on a material and that intelligence can be enhanced through effort.
computer is inherently more effective; nonetheless, it might mean These different beliefs can influence how students approach learn-
that students need different types of strategy instruction depend- ing and whether they invest the effort to use effective strategies
ing on the medium through which they take notes. that promote meaningful learning. Future research should account
for individual differences in students’ motivation and beliefs when
8.4. Limitations and future directions exploring spontaneous note taking strategy use.
Another limitation of the present study concerns the measures
One limitation of the present study is that students’ notes were of spatial ability and learning outcomes. First, although the two
coded and analyzed based on the types of strategies they measures of spatial ability (the Cube Comparison Test and the
attempted to use, not necessarily whether they used each strategy Paper Folding Test; Ekstrom et al., 1976) theoretically load on the
effectively. For example, students received credit for a map if they spatial visualization sub-dimension of spatial ability (Carroll,
attempted to spatially arrange and build connections among text, 1993), they were only moderately correlated with each other in
regardless of the number of connections the map contained or of the present study. Second, the measures might not have ade-
the relevance of the information represented in the map. Similarly, quately captured the same types of cognitive processing involved
using a words-only strategy might have consisted of verbatim in using spatial learning strategies such as mapping. That is, the
notes from the lesson or a more elaborated summary involving spatial ability measures involve mentally rotating and transform-
the student’s own words. Past research suggests that students ing iconic representations, whereas mapping involves creating a
might need explicit guidance or training to achieve the most ben- symbolic spatial representation. This might in part explain why
efit from spatial strategies (e.g., Schwamborn et al., 2010; Van spatial ability was not more strongly associated with spatial strat-
Meter, Aleksic, Schwartz, & Garner, 2006). We also were unable egy use in the present study. Further work is needed to clarify dif-
to distinguish between the influence of different spatial strategies ferences among measures of spatial ability and how these might
(maps and drawings), given the relatively low frequency of using differentially be related to strategy use and STEM learning out-
drawings. This is an important consideration for further research comes. Regarding the learning outcome measures, the retention,
because each type of spatial strategy might be differentially related transfer, and drawing tests showed somewhat low reliability sep-
to spatial ability or might be useful for representing different types arately and thus were combined into a composite learning out-
of information. Nonetheless, the method used in the present study come score. More reliable learning measures would have allowed
was able to detect clear individual differences in the spontaneous us to examine the relationships among strategy use and qualita-
use of space to represent material from a scientific text and its rela- tively different learning outcomes.
tionship with learning outcomes, which was the primary purpose The present study focused on spontaneous strategy use while
of the study. students learned from a scientific text consisting of a cause-and-
More generally, another limitation of using note-taking effect structure. Additional research should explore the generaliz-
behavior as a window into cognitive processing is that it is lim- ability of these findings across different educational contexts, such
ited to what students choose to represent externally. There are as learning from lectures, learning from multimedia lessons, and
likely important individual differences in how students internally learning within other STEM domains. In the present study, we used
represent the material that are not captured in students’ notes. a text-based lesson describing a highly spatial causal system so
For example, some students might have a preference to verbalize that students needed to create their own spatial representation
the material, whereas others might mentally visualize the of the material, such as in the form of maps or diagrams. Past
material (Mayer & Massa, 2003). A recent study by Leopold research suggests that students benefit when they are instructed
and Mayer (2015) involving the same learning materials as the to use spatial strategies such as drawing (Leopold & Leutner,
present study found that prompting students to mentally image 2012) or imagining (Leopold & Mayer, 2015) when learning from
the structures and processes of the respiratory system led to scientific texts describing spatial processes. However, it is possible
better learning. that students would be more likely to spontaneously use spatial
Further research should address additional factors that might strategies if the lesson also provided students with a spatial repre-
influence or covary with students’ strategy use and learning out- sentation. Similarly, further work should consider the role of differ-
comes, such as prior knowledge, motivation, beliefs about learning, ent text structures (e.g., sequential, hierarchical, comparative) in
or self-regulation ability. Exploring these potential covariates is spontaneous strategy use. Research should also incorporate the
especially important given that strategy use was spontaneous in role of note taking review in the effects of spontaneous strategy
the present study, and thus does not allow causal inferences link- use, as well as how note taking strategies during learning interact
ing strategy use and learning outcomes. The present study con- with other study strategies such as retrieval practice.
sisted of students with relatively low prior knowledge about the Finally, research is needed to better understand how strategy-
human respiratory system (as indicated by the self-report ques- based instruction can be incorporated within authentic STEM set-
tionnaire). However, domain-specific knowledge likely influences tings. The present study provides evidence of the types of strate-
the quality of strategies that students use during learning, as indi- gies used spontaneously under different note taking situations.
cated by previous research on expertise (Chi, Feltovich, & Glaser, By closely analyzing the quality of strategy use among successful
1981). Domain-specific knowledge can also override the impor- students – including the extent to which they use space – instruc-
tance of general spatial ability, in line with the circumvention-of- tion can target the use of specific strategies that are most appropri-
limits hypothesis (Hambrick et al., 2012). ate for representing the to-be-learned material.
The beliefs students hold about the nature of learning and
knowledge can also influence the effort they invest and the types Acknowledgement
of strategies they employ during learning (e.g., Blackwell,
Trzesniewski, & Dweck, 2007). Some students possess unproduc- This research was supported by grant N000140810018 from the
tive beliefs about learning, such as that learning involves rote Office of Naval Research. We thank Amanda Seaborne, Danielle
memorization or that intelligence is a fixed trait, whereas other Ewart, Jason Burton, and Arie Koster for their assistance with data
students believe that learning involves making sense of the collection and coding.
78 L. Fiorella, R.E. Mayer / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 66–79

Appendix A. Lesson on the human respiratory system capillaries connect larger blood vessels called veins to the air sacs
and are represented as blue because they contain an abundance of
1. Introduction carbon dioxide.
Respiration is the process that moves air in and out of the lungs.
Through respiration, oxygen is delivered to where it is needed in 8. Structure of the circulatory system
the body and carbon dioxide is removed from the body. Respiration
involves three phases: Inhaling, exchanging, and exhaling. The res- Arteries (red blood vessels) run one-way from the lungs,
piratory process is controlled by the nervous system. through the heart, which is somewhat below the lungs, to the cells
of the body. Arteries transport oxygen, which is used by the cells of
2. Structure of the nervous system the body to make energy. Veins (blue blood vessels) run one-way
in the opposite direction from the cells of the body, through the
The respiratory center is located in the rear, bottom part of the heart, to the lungs. Veins transport carbon dioxide, which is a
brain, near the back of the neck. The respiratory center of the brain waste gas produced in the cells of the body. The heart is a pump
is connected to a pathway of nerves that leads down from the that keeps the blood flowing in the veins and arteries.
spinal cord to connect with muscles controlling the diaphragm
and rib cage. 9. Process of exchanging

3. Steps in the nervous system to control breathing The exchange of oxygen and carbon dioxide takes place in the
connection between air sacs and capillaries. Oxygen molecules in
When the brain detects the need for more oxygen in the blood- the inhaled air move to the capillaries running nearby, and carbon
stream, the respiratory center in the brain sends out a signal to dioxide molecules move from the capillaries into the air sacs in the
inhale. The signal moves along the pathway of nerves to muscles lungs. The capillaries carry the oxygen to arteries, which transport
controlling the diaphragm and rib cage. When the brain detects it, through the heart, to the cells of the body. At the same time, car-
the need for less carbon dioxide in the bloodstream, the respiratory bon dioxide travels in veins from the cells of the body, through the
center in the brain terminates the signal to inhale. The signal to heart, to capillaries running next to the air sacs.
inhale stops moving along the pathway of nerves to the muscles
controlling the diaphragm and rib cage. 10. Process of exhaling

4. Structure of the thoracic cavity The carbon-dioxide-rich air in the air sacs is drawn out of the
lungs by exhaling. When the brain turns off the signal to inhale,
The thoracic cavity is the space in the chest that contains the the diaphragm and the rib muscles relax. The dome of the dia-
lungs. It is surrounded by the rib cage, which can move slightly phragm moves upward again and the ribs move slightly inward.
inward or outward, and has the diaphragm on the bottom, which As a result, the thoracic cavity becomes smaller creating less room
has a dome that can move downward. The main muscles involved for the lungs. Air containing carbon dioxide is forced out of the
in respiration are the diaphragm and the rib muscles. The dia- lungs through the bronchial tubes and windpipe to the nose and
phragm is located underneath the lungs. It lines the lower part of mouth, where it leaves the body.
the thoracic cavity, sealing it off air-tight from the rest of the body.
The rib muscles are attached to the ribs, which in turn encircle the References
lungs. When in the relaxed position, the ribs are slightly inward
and the diaphragm dome curves upward. Ainsworth, S., Prain, V., & Tytler, R. (2011). Drawing to learn in science. Science, 333,
1096–1097.
Alesandrini, K. L. (1981). Pictorial-verbal and analytic-holistic learning strategies in
5. Structure of the airway system science learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 358–368.
Anderson, J. R., Reder, L. M., & Simon, H. A. (1996). Situated learning and education.
From the nose and mouth, the windpipe leads to the bronchial Educational Researcher, 25(4), 5–11.
Azevedo, R., Guthrie, J. T., & Seibert, D. (2004). The role of self-regulated learning in
tubes, which branch off into the right and the left lung. There they fostering students’ conceptual understanding of complex systems. Journal of
branch off into finer tubes. Educational Computing Research, 30(1–2), 87–111.
Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of
intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal
6. Process of inhaling study and an intervention. Child Development, 78, 246–263.
Bretzing, B. H., & Kulhavy, R. W. (1979). Notetaking and depth of processing.
During inhaling, a signal from the brain to inhale causes the Contemporary Educational Psychology, 4, 145–153.
Bui, D. C., Myerson, J., & Hale, S. (2013). Note-taking with computers: Exploring
dome of the diaphragm to contract downward and the rib cage alternative strategies for improved recall. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105
to move slightly outward creating more space in the thoracic cav- (2), 299–309.
ity into which the lungs can expand. Air is drawn in through the Callender, A. A., & McDaniel, M. A. (2009). The limited benefits of rereading
educational texts. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 30–41.
nose or mouth, moves down through the windpipe and bronchial
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor analytic studies. New
tubes to tiny air sacs in the lungs. York: Cambridge University Press.
Chi, M. T. H., Bassok, M., Lewis, M. W., Reimann, P., & Glaser, R. (1989). Self-
7. Structure of the exchange system explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve
problems. Cognitive Science, 13, 145–182.
Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of
Tiny grape-like air sacs, called alveoli, are grouped together in physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5, 121–152.
the lungs at the bronchial tubes. Each air sac is surrounded by tiny Cook, L. K., & Mayer, R. E. (1988). Teaching readers about the structure of scientific
text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 448–456.
blood vessels called capillaries. On one side of the air sac the sur- Cromley, J. G., & Azevedo, R. (2006). Self-report of reading comprehension
rounding capillaries carry oxygen and on the other side they carry strategies: What are we measuring? Metacognition and Learning, 1, 229–247.
carbon dioxide. Oxygen-carrying capillaries connect air sacs to lar- Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, D. T. (2013).
Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques: Promising
ger blood vessels called arteries and are represented as red because directions from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychological Science in
they contain an abundance of oxygen. Carbon-dioxide-carrying the Public Interest, 14(1), 4–58.
L. Fiorella, R.E. Mayer / Contemporary Educational Psychology 49 (2017) 66–79 79

Ekstrom, R. B., French, J. W., Harman, H. H., & Dermen, D. (1976). Manual for kit of Mayer, R. E., & Wittrock, M. C. (2006). Problem solving. In P. H. Winne & P. A.
factor-referenced cognitive tests. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service. Alexander (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 287–303). New York:
Fiorella, L., & Mayer, R. E. (2013). The relative benefits of learning by teaching and Routledge.
teaching expectancy. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38(4), 281–288. Mueller, P. A., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2014). The pen is mightier than the keyboard:
Fiorella, L., & Mayer, R. E. (2015). Learning as a generative activity: Eight learning Advantages of longhand over laptop notetaking. Psychological Science, 1–10.
strategies that promote understanding. New York: Cambridge University Press. Nesbit, J. C., & Adesope, O. O. (2006). Learning with concept and knowledge maps: A
Fiorella, L., & Mayer, R. E. (2016). Eight ways to promote generative learning. meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 76, 413–418.
Educational Psychology Review, 28(4), 717–741. Peper, R. J., & Mayer, R. E. (1986). Generative effects of note-taking during science
Fowler, R. L., & Barker, A. S. (1974). Effectiveness of highlighting for retention of text lectures. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 34–38.
material. Journal of Applied Psychology, 59(3), 358–364. Ponce, H. R., & Mayer, R. E. (2014). Qualitatively different cognitive processing
Gentner, D., & Stevens, A. L. (Eds.). (1983). Mental models. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. during online reading primed by different study activities. Computers in Human
Greeno, J. G. the Middle School Mathematics Through Applications Project Group. Behavior, 30, 121–130.
(1998). The situativity of knowing, learning, and research. American Pressley, M. (1990). Cognitive strategy instruction that really improves children’s
Psychologist, 53(1), 5–26. academic performance. Brookline Books.
Hambrick, D. Z., Libarkin, J. C., Petcovic, H. L., Baker, K. M., Elkins, J., Callahan, C. N., ... Roberts, F. W., & Dansereau, D. F. (2008). Studying strategy effects on memory,
LaDue, N. D. (2012). A test of the circumvention-of-limits hypothesis in attitudes, and intentions. Reading Psychology, 29, 552–580.
scientific problem solving: The case of geological bedrock mapping. Journal of Robinson, D. H., & Kiewra, K. A. (1995). Visual argument: Graphic organizers are
Experimental Psychology: General, 141(3), 397–403. superior to outlines in improving learning from text. Journal of Educational
Hayes, A. F., & Preacher, K. J. (2014). Statistical analysis with a multicategorical Psychology, 3, 455–467.
independent variable. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, Roediger, H. L., III, & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory
67, 451–470. tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17(3), 249–255.
Holley, C. D., & Dansereau, D. F. (Eds.). (1984). Spatial learning strategies. Orlando, FL: Salomon, G., & Perkins, D. N. (1989). Rocky roads to transfer: Rethinking
Academic Press. mechanisms of a neglected phenomenon. Educational Psychologist, 24(2),
Holley, C. D., Dansereau, D. F., McDonald, B. A., Garland, J. C., & Collins, K. W. (1979). 113–142.
Evaluation of a hierarchical mapping technique as an aid to prose processing. Schwamborn, A., Mayer, R. E., Thillmann, H., Leopold, C., & Leutner, D. (2010).
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 4, 227–237. Drawing as a generative activity and drawing as a prognostic activity. Journal of
Jairam, D., & Kiewra, K. A. (2010). Helping students soar to success on computers: Educational Psychology, 102(4), 872–879.
An investigation of the SOAR study method for computer-based learning. Sims, V. K., & Mayer, R. E. (2002). Domain specificity of spatial expertise: The case of
Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 601–614. video game players. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 97–115.
Jarvenoja, H., Jarvela, S., & Malmberg, J. (2015). Understanding regulated learning Stieff, M., Dixon, B., Ryu, M., Kumi, B. C., & Hegarty, M. (2014). Strategy training
in situative and contextual frameworks. Educational Psychologist, 50(3), eliminates sex differences in spatial problem solving in a STEM domain? Journal
204–219. of Educational Psychology, 106(2), 390–402.
Kardash, C. A. M., & Amlund, J. T. (1991). Self-reported learning strategies and Stieff, M., & Uttal, D. (2015). How much can spatial training improve STEM
learning from expository text. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 16, achievement? Educational Psychology Review, 27, 605–615.
117–138. Thorndike, E. L., & Woodworth, R. S. (1901). The influence of improvement in one
Karpicke, J. D., Butler, A. C., & Roediger, H. L. (2009). Metacognitive strategies in mental function upon the efficiency of other functions. Psychological Review, 8
student learning: Do students practice retrieval when they study on their own? (4), 384–395.
Memory, 17(4), 471–479. Uttal, D. H., & Cohen, C. A. (2012). Spatial thinking and STEM education: When, why,
Kiewra, K. A. (1989). A review of note-taking: The encoding-storage paradigm and and how? Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 57, 147–181.
beyond. Educational Psychology Review, 1, 147–172. Uttal, D. H., Meadow, N. G., Tipton, E., Hand, L. L., Alden, A. R., Warren, C., &
Kiewra, K. A., & Benton, S. L. (1988). The relationship between information- Newcombe, N. S. (2013). The malleability of spatial skills: A meta-analysis of
processing ability and notetaking. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 13, 33–44. training studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139(2), 352–402.
Kiewra, K. A., BuBois, N. F., Christian, D., McShane, A., Meyerhoffer, M., & Roskelley, Van Meter, P. (2001). Drawing construction as a strategy for learning from text.
D. (1991). Note-taking functions and techniques. Journal of Educational Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 808–820.
Psychology, 83(2), 240–245. Van Meter, P., Aleksic, M., Schwartz, A., & Garner, J. (2006). Learner-generated
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge drawing as a strategy for learning from content area text. Contemporary
University Press. Educational Psychology, 31, 142–166.
Lahtinen, V., Lonka, K., & Lindblom-Ylanne, S. (1997). Spontaneous study strategies Van Meter, P., & Garner, J. (2005). The promise and practice of learner-generated
and the quality of knowledge construction. British Journal of Educational drawing: Literature review and synthesis. Educational Psychology Review, 17,
Psychology, 67, 13–24. 285–325.
Leopold, C., & Leutner, D. (2012). Science text comprehension: Drawing, main idea Van Meter, P., Yokoi, L., & Pressley, M. (1994). College students’ theory of note-
selection, and summarizing as learning strategies. Learning and Instruction, 22, taking derived from their perceptions of note-taking. Journal of Educational
16–26. Psychology, 86(3), 323–338.
Leopold, C., & Mayer, R. E. (2015). An imagination effect in learning from scientific Wade, S., & Trathen, W. (1989). Effect of self-selected study methods on learning.
text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(1), 47–63. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(1), 40–47.
Leutner, D., Leopold, C., & Sumfleth, E. (2009). Cognitive load and science text Wai, J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2009). Spatial ability and STEM domains:
comprehension: Effects of drawing and mentally imagining text content. Aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its
Computers in Human Behavior, 25, 284–289. importance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(4), 817–835.
Leutner, D., & Schmeck, A. (2014). The drawing principle in multimedia learning. In Weinstein, C. E., & Mayer, R. E. (1985). The teaching of learning strategies. In M. C.
R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 433–488). Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching and learning (3rd ed.,
New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. pp. 315–327). New York, NY: Macmillan.
Mayer, R. E. (Ed.). (2014). The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (2nd ed. Wittrock, M. C. (1990). Generative processes of comprehension. Educational
Cambridge University Press: New York. Psychologist, 24(4), 345–376.
Mayer, R. E., & Massa, L. J. (2003). Three facets of visual and verbal learners:
Cognitive ability, cognitive style and learning preference. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 95(4), 833–846.

You might also like