You are on page 1of 10

Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 121–130

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

Qualitatively different cognitive processing during online reading


primed by different study activities
Hector R. Ponce a,⇑, Richard E. Mayer b,1
a
Faculty of Management and Economics, University of Santiago of Chile, Av. L.B. O’Higgins, 3363 Santiago, Chile
b
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA 93106-9660, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This article uses eye-tracking technology to examine how study activities such as taking notes or filling in
Available online 29 August 2013 a graphic organizer affect cognitive processing during learning. College students read a computer-pre-
sented passage that compared the characteristics of eastern steamboats (top section) and western steam-
Keywords: boats (bottom section), either by reading it twice (read-only group), typing notes into a textbox on the
Graphic organizer right side of the screen (note-taking group), or typing characteristics of the two types of steamboats into
Note-taking a compare-and-contrast graphic organizer on the right side of the screen (graphic organizer group). Com-
Eye tracking
pared to the note-taking group, the graphic organizer group displayed more eye movements between the
Learning strategy
Reading
top and bottom of the passage (i.e., integrative saccades, d = 1.03), more eye movements between the text
and the type-in window on the right side (i.e., constructive saccades, d = 0.79), fewer constructive sac-
cades during initial reading (d = 0.64), and less time looking to the right side during initial reading
(d = 0.81); and scored higher on a comprehension test given afterwards (d = 1.17), although both study
groups outscored the read-only group. Results suggest that students in the note-taking group (and read-
only group) tended to use a linear learning strategy in which their eyes followed the text in the order pre-
sented whereas students in the graphic organizer group tended to use a generative learning strategy in
which their eyes searched for connections between specific information across the passage required to
make comparisons.
Ó 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Consider a student who reads an expository passage presented


on a computer screen, such as the steamboat passage—adapted
Being able to learn from expository text (i.e., text that describes from (Meyer & Poon, 2001)—shown in Fig. 1. For example, to
or explains) is a central skill in the development of literacy, and has enhance learning, some students could be asked to take notes by
received increased scrutiny because of its prominence in new cur- typing into a textbox on the right side of the screen during learn-
riculum frameworks such as the Common Core Standards in the US ing, such as shown in Fig. 2. In a different attempt to enhance
(Porter, McMaken, Hwang, & Yang, 2011) and in international learning, other students could be asked to complete a compare-
assessment programs such as the Programme for International and-contrast graphic organizer that enables them to compare the
Student Assessment (Organization for Economic Co-operation two types of steamboats along several key dimensions by typing
and Development, 2013). The goal of the present study is to com- in the name of each dimension as well as the corresponding attri-
pare the cognitive processes that are fostered by two different butes for each type of steamboat, as shown in the right side of
study strategies intended to improve learning from expository Fig. 3.
text—taking notes and filing in a graphic organizer—in order to Most previous research assesses the impact of different study
better understand how study activities can improve learning. strategies by examining learning outcomes, such as measured by
Consequently, the primary research question addressed in this comprehension tests or memory tests (Dunlosky, Rawson, Marsh,
study is whether these study activities—note taking and graphic Nathan, & Willingham, 2013; Mason, Pluchino, & Tornatora,
organizers—prime qualitatively different cognitive processing 2013; Mayer, 2008). In the present study we sought a more direct
during learning. measure of learners’ cognitive processing during learning primed
by different study strategies. Therefore, we used eye-tracking
methodology to examine students’ eye movements as they simply
⇑ Corresponding author. Tel.: +56 2 27180716.
read a text (read group), took notes by typing into a textbox as they
E-mail addresses: hector.ponce@usach.cl (H.R. Ponce), rich.mayer@psych.ucsb.
edu (R.E. Mayer).
read a text (note-taking group), or filled in a graphic organizer as
1
Tel.: +1 805 8932472. they read a text (graphic organizer group).

0747-5632/$ - see front matter Ó 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.07.054
122 H.R. Ponce, R.E. Mayer / Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 121–130

outcomes, while the product function (or external storage func-


tion) refers to studying the impact of reviewing notes on learning
outcomes (Kiewra, 1985). Although most research has focused on
note-taking during lectures, some general findings are relevant
for this study. The process of note-taking promotes encoding and
storage of information in long-term memory (Barnett, di Vesta, &
Rogozinski, 1981). The process of reviewing notes supports retriev-
ing information from long-term memory, and it facilitates remem-
bering and provides opportunities for deeper elaboration of the
presented material (Bohay, Blakely, Tamplin, & Radvansky, 2011).
The act of note-taking has been demonstrated to improve students’
performance on comprehension tests under appropriate circum-
stances (Kiewra, 1985; Peper & Mayer, 1978). However, research
shows that in general students take incomplete notes and do not
address critical points (Kiewra et al., 1991; Peverly, Brobst,
Graham, & Shaw, 2003).
A graphic organizer, on the other hand, is a spatial structure for
representing material (such as a matrix). Similar to note-taking, re-
search on graphic organizers has concentrated on its process and
product functions. The process function refers to researching the
impact of using graphic organizers as thinking procedures to assist
Fig. 1. The steamboat passage in the read-only condition. learners on the steps to carry out a cognitive operation (Beyer,
1997). A Venn diagram, for example, can support the development
2. Research on note-taking and graphic organizers of the compare-and-contrast cognitive strategy by guiding stu-
dents to establish the elements to be compared, to indicate their
A study activity refers to actions performed by a learner during common characteristics, and to figure out their differences. Asking
learning that are intended to improve learning, whereas a learning students to fill in graphic organizers during reading can improve
strategy refers to cognitive processing performed by a learner dur- students’ learning outcomes (Ponce, Lopez, & Mayer, 2012; Robin-
ing learning which can be primed by a study activity (Mayer, 2008; son, 1998). On the other hand, the product function of graphic
Weinstein & Mayer, 1985). The conventional view is that study organizers refers to researching the impact of studying filled-in
activities such as taking notes or filling in graphic organizers cause or teacher-made graphic organizers on learning outcomes (Stull
students to engage in more useful cognitive processing during & Mayer, 2007). Numerous studies have shown that using graphic
learning and thereby result in better memory for the material than organizers and note-taking appropriately employed in learning
when students do not engage in study activities (Dunlosky et al., contexts are effective text comprehension activities, and that
2013; Mayer, 2011). structured note-taking strategies such as with outline or matrix
Note-taking and graphic organizers are two well-known exam- format are more beneficial than linear recording of information
ples of study activities that are intended to support reading com- (Jairam & Kiewra, 2009; Kiewra et al., 1991; Piolat, Olive, & Kellogg,
prehension (National Institute of Child Health and Human 2005).
Development, 2000). Research on note-taking has concentrated Graphic organizers are tied to basic rhetorical structures (i.e.,
on its process and product functions: the process function (or common ways of structuring text), such as compare-and-contrast
encoding function) refers to the impact of taking notes on learning (e.g., matrix), sequence (e.g., flow chart), or hierarchy (e.g., tree

Fig. 2. The steamboat passage and the note editor.


H.R. Ponce, R.E. Mayer / Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 121–130 123

Fig. 3. The steamboat passage and the comparison graphic organizer.

diagram). Text comprehension performance can be improved structure consisting of two types of boats being compared along
when students receive training in how to outline based on basic several dimensions. In this case, the learner would use a generative
rhetorical structures (Cook & Mayer, 1988) or when signaling tech- learning strategy of reading the passage in an effort to build a ma-
niques such as headings are used to highlight the text’s rhetorical trix that compares the two steamboats on each dimension, so for
structure (Meyer & Poon, 2001). In this study, we used a compare- example, when reading that the eastern steamboats operated in
and-contrast structure as described in the previous section. In par- deep rivers the reader might scan down in the passage to see that
ticular, we focused on the steamboat passage as an example of an western steamboats operated in shallow waters. The goal of this
expository text with a common rhetorical structure (i.e., compare- study is examine techniques that encourage readers to change
and-contrast), as it is beyond the scope of this study to examine all from a linear strategy to a generative strategy.
major types of expository text. We examine the prediction that the study activity of note-tak-
The present study is inspired by a previous large-scale class- ing leads to a linear learning strategy whereas the study activity
room experiment, in which students who learned how to build of using graphic organizers leads to a generative learning strategy.
computer-based graphic organizers for on-screen text passages Table 1 compares a linear learning strategy which involves adding
showed improvements on reading comprehension tests (Ponce information to memory as presented versus a generative learning
et al., 2012). To complement that study conducted at the macro-le- strategy which involves building a coherent structure in which
vel, the present study focuses on the micro-level to pinpoint some the material is reorganized, as summarized in the first line of the
of the cognitive processes during learning that could contribute to table. The left side of the second line of Table 1 shows that cogni-
the findings. Eye-tracking measures are used as a way of gauging tive processes involved in a linear learning strategy are character-
learners’ cognitive processing during learning. ized in the cognitive theory of multimedia learning (Mayer, 2009)
as essential processing, in which learners select essential elements
from the lesson and mentally organize them in memory as a list
3. Theoretical framework and predictions in the order presented in the lesson. The result is the construction
of a surface-level representation, that is, a copy of the main ideas in
The present study extends the growing body of work on differ- the presented material, using the same structure as in the text, as
ences in the amount remembered caused by study activities, by shown in the left side of the third line of Table 1. We propose that
examining the further proposal that different study activities— asking students to engage in standard note-taking can promote
such as taking notes versus filling in a graphic organizer—can fos- essential processing, in which students simply write down facts
ter qualitatively different learning strategies, that is, qualitatively from the lesson in the order presented.
different cognitive processes during learning rather than differ- In contrast, the cognitive processes involved in a generative
ences solely in the quantity of learning. According to the cognitive learning strategy are characterized in the cognitive theory of multi-
theory of multimedia learning (Mayer, 2009, 2011), the quality of a media learning as generative processing, in which learners select
learning outcome depends on the kinds of the cognitive processes relevant material from the lesson, mentally reorganize it to fit into
that learners engage in during learning. a coherent structure, and integrate it with a relevant prior schema
Consider the learning strategies that readers could use in read- activated from long-term memory, as summarized in the right side
ing the steamboat passage shown in Fig. 1. A default approach is to of the second line of Table 1. The result is a deep-level representa-
view the steamboat passage as a list of facts to be remembered, in tion based on constructing an integrated structure (which can be
which case the learner would use a linear learning strategy of read- called a mental model) that has internal coherence (i.e., fits together
ing the sentences in order while trying to remember each fact in in a logical way) and external coherence (i.e., relates to existing
the order presented in the text. In contrast, a structured approach schemas), as shown in the right side of the third line of Table 1.
is to view the steamboat passage as a compare-and-contrast We propose that asking students to engage in filling in a graphic
124 H.R. Ponce, R.E. Mayer / Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 121–130

Table 1
Comparison of linear and generative learning strategies.

Linear learning strategy Generative learning strategy


Description Adding information sequentially in the order presented (such as a Building an organized structure (such as a matrix)
list)
Cognitive processes Essential processing (selecting and organizing information as Generative processing (selecting and reorganizing and integrating
presented) information)
Mental Surface-level representation (e.g., text base) Deep-level representation (i.e., mental model)
representation

organizer can promote generative processing, in which students strategy and the note-taking group is more likely to engage in lin-
use a compare-and-contrast schema primed from long-term mem- ear processing, then we predict that the graphic organizer group
ory and mentally reorganize the material in the lesson to fit within will produce a greater number of integrative saccades and con-
the compare-and-contrast schema. structive saccades than the note-taking group. We also expect
The distinction between surface and deep representations is the read-only group to display a linear learning strategy, indicated
consistent with Kintsch’s (1988) construction-integration theory by fewer integrative saccades than the graphic organizer group.
of prose comprehension, and its distinction between text-base Next consider what happens during the initial reading of the
and situation model representation of text. The distinction be- passage, which we estimate takes about 25 s based on observations
tween linear and generative learning strategies is consistent with of students in the read-only group. The third line in Table 2 high-
Wittrock’s (1989; Mayer & Wittrock, 2006) generative theory of lights the number of early constructive saccades, that is, the num-
reading comprehension, and its distinction between generative ber of constructive saccades that occur during the first 25 s. If a
and non-generative processes during learning. learner is engaging in linear processing, we would expect construc-
tive saccades to occur from the very start because the learner is
simply reading a bit and entering that information in the textbox,
4. Measures then reading the next bit and entering that information in the text-
box, and so on. If a learner is engaging in generative processing, we
Table 2 summarizes some key measures used in the study to would expect the learner to read over the entire passage initially as
evaluate the learning process and learning outcomes primed by the basis for figuring out how to organize it, and then go back and
different study activities. We used eye-tracking measures (Johnson start entering text into the textbox. Similarly, the fourth line of Ta-
& Mayer, 2012; Rayner, 1998) to gauge the process of learning, by ble 2 highlights the number of seconds spent looking at the textbox
breaking the screen (such as shown in Figs. 2 and 3) into three (area C) during the first 25 s of reading—that is, early non-text fix-
areas of interest (AOIs)—the top portion of the passage consisting ation time. We expect learners engaging in linear processing to
of 9 sentences describing eastern steamboats (area A), the bottom show greater early non-text fixation time, and learners who read
portion of the passage consisting of 11 sentences describing wes- over the entire text in order to figure out the overall structure to
tern steamboats (area B), and the right side of the screen contain- show less early non-text fixation time. Based on this analysis, we
ing a textbox for note-taking or for filling in the graphic organizer predict that the graphic organizer group will score lower on early
(area C). constructive saccades and early non-text fixation time than the
First, integrative saccades (or up-down saccades) are the num- note-taking group.
ber of times a learner’s eyes move between areas A and B. We The foregoing process measures are the main new focus of this
interpret the total number of integrative saccades as an indication study, but we included learning outcome measures to make con-
of a generative learning strategy, because they reflect an attempt to nections with previous work. The bottom portion of Table 2 sum-
make connections that go beyond simple linear order. marizes two learning outcome measures based on a summary
Second, constructive saccades (or left–right saccades) are the test, in which learners write a summary of the passage, and a cloze
number of times a learner’s eyes move between area C and com- test, in which learners fill in blanks corresponding to key facts in
bined areas A and B. We interpret the total number of constructive the passage. The summary test score consists of the number of cor-
saccades as an indication of a generative learning strategy, because rect comparisons (i.e., comparable information for both the eastern
they reflect an attempt to make connections to several portions of and western steamboat on a particular dimension) which indicates
the text rather than going in simple linear order. If the graphic generative processing, and the cloze test score consists of the
organizer group is more likely to engage in generative learning number of correctly filled in slots using the verbatim wording of

Table 2
Measures of the process and product of learning.

Name Description
Measures of learning process
Integrative saccades (up- Number of times eyes move from the top portion of the passage (area A) to the bottom portion of the passage (area B) or vice versa.
down) Indicates generative learning strategy
Constructive saccades Number of times eyes move from the passage (area A or B) to the note-taking textbox or graphic organizer (area C) or vice versa. Indicates
(left–right) generative learning strategy
Early constructive Number of constructive saccades in first 25 s (i.e., during initial reading of the passage). Indicates linear learning strategy
saccades
Early non-text fixation Number of seconds spent looking at note-taking textbox or graphic organizer (area C) during first 25 s (i.e., initial reading of the passage).
time Indicates linear learning strategy
Measures of learning product
Summary test score Number of key comparisons recalled. Indicates reading comprehension
Cloze test score Number of key verbatim terms filled in. Indicates rote memory
H.R. Ponce, R.E. Mayer / Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 121–130 125

the lesson which may be supported by linear processing but by & Poon, 2001) in which two types of steamboats (i.e., eastern-style
generative processing as well. If study activities are effective, we and western-style) are compared along several dimensions (e.g.,
expect the two study activity groups (note-taking and graphic river depth, cargo storage and engine type). We chose this text
organizer) to outperform the read-only group. If the graphic orga- because it facilitated to measure the eye-tracking variables used
nizer group is more likely to engage in generative processing then in this study.
we predict better performance on the summary test, which is a As can be seen in Fig. 1, the top portion of the passage (i.e., first
measure of comprehension, but not necessarily on the cloze test, 9 sentences) describes characteristics of eastern-style steamboats,
which is a rote memory test. whereas the bottom portion (i.e., last 11 sentences) describes cor-
responding characteristics of western-style steamboats. There
should be a lot of eye movements between the top and bottom por-
5. Method
tions of the passage if readers use a generative reading strategy but
not if they use a linear reading strategy.
5.1. Participants and design
As shown in Fig. 1, the read-only version presented the text
titled ‘‘The Steamboat’’ as the only window on the screen. The
The participants were 64 students recruited from the Psychol-
read-only application was implemented as a PowerPoint presenta-
ogy Subject Pool at the University of California, Santa Barbara. In
tion with the steamboat text in similar format to the one presented
a between-subjects design, 21 participants served in the graphic
for the other treatments. In the read-only version, we asked partic-
organizer group, 22 participants were in the note-taking group,
ipants to read through the text twice in order to observe a linear
and 21 participants were in the read-only group. Data from three
reading pattern and to compare it with those generated in the
participants (1 in the note-taking group and 2 in the graphic orga-
note-taking and graphic organizer conditions.
nizer group) were excluded in the eye-tracking measures analysis
As shown in Fig. 2, the note-taking version presented the text in
due to the low quality of eye-tracking data for these subjects. The
a window on the left side and also provided a textbox on the right
average age of participants was 18.44 years (SD = 1.11), the propor-
side for typing in notes. The note-taking strategy was implemented
tion of females was 0.58, and the proportion of freshmen was 0.77.
as a PowerPoint presentation by integrating a textbox object that
The groups did not differ significantly (at p < .05) on mean age, pro-
worked as a basic text editor. The note-taking application did not
portion of females and males, or self-rated knowledge of
allow students to copy and paste text into the text box. In the
steamboats.
note-taking condition, we asked participants to read the text and
take notes using the embedded text editor.
5.2. Materials and apparatus As shown in Fig. 3, the graphic organizer version presented the
text in a window on the left and also provided an interactive gra-
The materials consisted of an informed consent sheet, summary phic organizer tool on the right with a compare-and-contrast gra-
test sheet, cloze test sheet, participant questionnaire, and three phic organizer. The graphic organizer treatment was created by
computer-based versions of the steamboat passage. The informed integrating into a PowerPoint presentation an Adobe Flash object
consent form described the study and human subject protections. that implemented a comparison graphic organizer (Almarza,
The summary test was computer-based and consisted of a blank Ponce, & Lopez, 2011). This interactive graphic organizer applica-
textbox that students had to use to write a summary of the lesson tion allowed students to add and delete text boxes for each com-
without access to the original text and notes. The summary was parison, and for each comparison to type in the name of the
scored by counting the number of complete comparisons made dimension to be compared and to type in the respective attri-
based on a blind-scoring process. From the original text, it is pos- butes for the two types of boats. An empty organizer was pre-
sible to create a maximum of nine comparisons associated with sented with only one set of boxes. If students wanted to add
the following attributes: steamboat type, rivers travelled, river attributes, they had to use the respective function in the applica-
depth, hull type, cargo storage, number of floors, engine type, en- tion. The graphic organizer application did not allow students to
gine efficiency, and engine safety. One point was given to each copy and paste text into the organizer. In this condition, we asked
complete comparison made in the summary (e.g., ‘‘the engines participants to read the text and complete the embedded graphic
were low-pressure in eastern boats and high pressure in western organizer.
boats’’), either made in one sentence or on separate sentences The eye-tracker allows the generation of areas of interest (AOI)
within the summary; yielding a total possible of 9 points. Partial so they can be analyzed as independent elements within the total
comparisons indicating a characteristic for one type of steamboat area of eye-movement recordings. In our study, we defined three
but not for the other (‘‘the engines were low-pressure in eastern areas of interest: (1) the top section of the text (i.e., the first 9 sen-
boats’’) were not given any points. The cloze test sheet was pa- tences, which described eastern-style steamboats) in the window
per-based and consisted of the steamboat passage (as shown in on the left side of the screen, (2) bottom section of the text (i.e.,
Fig. 1) with the following words eliminated and replaced with bottom 11 sentences, which described western-style steamboats)
blank spaces: Hudson, deep (three times), below, low-pressure, in the window on the left side of the screen, and (3) graphic orga-
shallow, Missouri, Ohio, Mississippi, flat, on, high-pressure. One nizer section (for the graphic organizer group) or note editor text-
point was given for each exact word that was filled in correctly, box section (for the note taking group) represented as a window in
yielding a total possible of 13 points. Students were also asked to the right side of the screen. As indicated previously, the top and
complete a paper-based questionnaire that asked for ratings on a bottom sections of the text correspond to the description of the
5-point scale for previous knowledge about steamboats, difficulty eastern-style and western-style steamboats, respectively. In the
of the task, effort exerted on the task, and the degree to which they read-only condition, only the top and bottom sections of the text
liked using the application. were defined as areas of interest.
The learning materials consisted of three computer-based ver- If readers focus on building a mental representation based on
sions of a 123-word expository text, titled ‘‘The Steamboat,’’ which the structure of the text (i.e. compare-and-contrast), then they
was originally used by Meyer and Poon (2001). The text summa- should produce more saccades between the top and bottom sec-
rized the differences between eastern-style and western-style tions of the text, which we call integrative saccades. For instance,
steamboats, and is presented in Fig. 1. The rhetorical structure of when contrasting the hulls, it is possible to read the sentences
the passage is compare-and-contrast (Cook & Mayer, 1988; Meyer ‘‘. . .the deep hulls of the eastern steamboat’’ and ‘‘. . .Their hulls
126 H.R. Ponce, R.E. Mayer / Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 121–130

Fig. 4. Gaze plot showing a generative learning strategy.

Fig. 5. Gaze plot showing a linear learning strategy.

were flat. . .’’ relationally. In the eye-tracker the relation between The apparatus consisted of a Tobii T-60 eye-tracking system and
these two sentences should be recorded as a series of fixations Dell computer for controlling the eye-tracking system.
on the first sentence (top section), a saccade to the second sentence
(bottom section) and a series of fixations on the later sentence. We 5.3. Procedure
interpret integrative saccades as an indication that the reader is
using a generative reading strategy. In particular, we are interested Participants were randomly assigned to one of the three groups,
in whether students in the graphic organizer group engage in qual- and were tested individually. They were told that they would par-
itatively different reading strategies than students in the note-tak- ticipate in a study whose aim was to understand eye-movement
ing group, as indicated by differences in the amount of integrative patterns while reading a text. Participants received a short expla-
saccades. nation of the eye-tracker at the beginning of the session and then
H.R. Ponce, R.E. Mayer / Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 121–130 127

Table 3
Means and standard deviations on four eye-tracking measures for each group.

Eye-tracking measure Graphic-organizer (N = 20) Note-taking (N = 20) Read-only (N = 21) GO vs. NT


M SD M SD M SD d
Integrative saccades (up-down) 20.40 7.41 13.75 5.38 5.86 3.35 1.03
Constructive saccades (left–right) 64.25 18.94 49.30 19.07 – – .79
Early constructive saccades (left–right) 2.40 3.45 5.45 5.74 – – .64
Early non-text fixation time 1.00 1.37 3.13 3.46 – – .81

they were asked to complete a form with some basic demographic A secondary prediction is that studying with a graphic organizer
information and to sign a consent form. For the read-only group, primes a generative learning strategy that is reflected in a higher
the next step was to calibrate the eye-tracker for each participant, number of constructive saccades (i.e., left–right saccades) whereas
and then present the steamboat passage. Participants in the read- studying with note-taking primes a linear learning strategy that is
only group were asked to read the text twice while the eye-tracker reflected in a lower number of constructive saccades. The second
recorded their eye movements and to tell the researcher when they row of Table 3 summarizes the mean number of constructive sac-
had completed the task, which stopped the recording. In the note- cades (and SDs) during studying. Consistent with this prediction, a
taking group, each participant was introduced to the note-taking t-test found that the graphic organizer group made significantly
application, using a practice text. For training purposes, partici- more constructive saccades than the note-taking group,
pants in the note-taking group were asked to take some notes on t(38) = 2.49, p = .017, d = .79.
the practice text so they could familiarize themselves with the edi- Next, to conduct a somewhat more fine-grained analysis, we fo-
tor; they were told that it was not possible to copy-and-paste from cused on the first 25 s of reading the text, which is the average time
the original text. After this training, the calibration of the eye- taken by the participants in the read-only group to read completely
tracker and the recording of the note-taking session followed. Par- through the text the first time (considering this group was asked to
ticipants were told that they had five minutes to complete the task; read the text twice). Focusing on the first 25 s gives some insight
in case they finished before the allocated time, they were asked to into the learning strategies taken by participants in each group,
tell the researcher in order to stop the recording, otherwise the re- particularly the degree to which they started taking notes or filling
searcher told the participants when the time was up. In the graphic in the graphic organizer before reading the text completely which
organizer condition, the procedure was the same as for the note- reflects a linear learning strategy. The third row in Table 3 summa-
taking condition, except in the training session participants were rizes the mean number of constructive saccades in the first 25 s
taught how to use the interactive graphic organizer embedded in (and SDs) for the note-taking and graphic organizer groups, and
the application. The training sessions took about 5 min. shows that the note-taking group made significantly more early
For each condition, immediately after each participant com- constructive saccades than the graphic organizer group,
pleted studying the steamboat passage and the recording was fin- t(38) = 2.04, p = .049, yielding a medium effect size (d = .64).
ished, the participant was asked to write a short summary of the The fourth row of Table 2 summarizes the mean fixation time on
passage by typing directly into a textbox window using the MS- either the graphic organizer or text editor (and SDs) during the first
Word editor; the participant was informed that the maximum time 25 s, and shows that the note-taking group spent significantly
to complete the summary was 4 min. The participant was not al- more time looking on the side of the screen for typing in words
lowed to review notes or the original text to write the summary. (i.e., showed greater early non-text fixation time) than the graphic
After completing the summary, the paper-based cloze test sheet organizer group, t(38) = 2.56, p = .015, with a large effect size
was administered with a time limit of 2 min. Finally, the partici- (d = .81). Overall, these two pieces of evidence suggest that par-
pant was asked to complete the paper-based questionnaire, which ticipants in the note-taking group were more likely than those in
solicited information about previous knowledge on steamboats, the graphic organizer group to begin writing before they had com-
difficulty and effort during the task, and satisfaction with the pletely read over the passage, which is consistent with a linear
application. learning strategy.
Overall, the eye-tracking data summarized in Table 3 are con-
sistent with the idea that the graphic organizer study activity is
6. Results more likely to encourage a generative learning strategy whereas
the note-taking study activity is more likely to encourage a linear
6.1. Do different study activities foster qualitatively different learning learning strategy (which also appears to be the default strategy
strategies? used in the read-only group). In short, the eye-tracking data pro-
vides evidence for qualitatively different learning strategies fos-
The primary prediction is that studying with a graphic organizer tered by different study strategies.
primes a generative learning strategy (reflected by a high number
of integrative or up-down saccades see Fig. 4) whereas studying
with note-taking or with no aids primes a linear learning strategy 6.2. Do different study activities foster different learning outcomes?
(reflected by a low number of integrative saccades see Fig. 5). The
top row of Table 3 summarizes the mean number of integrative The preceding section focused on measures of the process of
saccades (and standard deviations) during studying for each group. learning based on eye tracking data, which is the main focus of this
An analysis of variance (ANOVA) showed that the groups differed research, indicating that the graphic organizer group was more
significantly, F(2, 58) = 34.71, p < .001, and follow-up post hoc likely than the other groups to use a generative learning strategy
comparisons using a Tukey test (with p < .05) demonstrated that during reading. This section focuses on measures of learning out-
the graphic organizer group made significantly more integrative comes based on the summary test and cloze test. First, we examine
saccades than the note-taking group (d = 1.03) and read-only group the prediction that the graphic organizer group will outperform the
(d = 2.55), and that the note-taking group also made significantly others on the summary test, which is the best measure of reading
more integrative saccades than the read-only group (d = 1.77). comprehension available in this study, because the graphic
128 H.R. Ponce, R.E. Mayer / Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 121–130

organizer group is more likely to use a generative learning strategy. 6.3. Do different study activities foster different levels of self-reported
The first line of Table 4 summarizes the means and standard devi- effort, difficulty, and motivation?
ations on the summary test, along with effect sizes (based on Co-
hen’s d) for all three pair-wise comparisons. Consistent with Asking students to engage in study activities such as filling in a
predictions, an ANOVA showed a significant difference among graphic organizer as they read an expository passage is expected to
the three groups on the summary score, F(2, 61) = 19.59, p < .001, require more mental effort during learning (which reflects in-
and a follow-up post hoc analysis using a Tukey test (with alpha creased generative processing as compared to the read-only condi-
at .05) revealed that the graphic organizer group significantly out- tion) but lower perceived difficulty (which reflects decreased
scored the note-taking group (d = 1.17) and the read-only group extraneous processing as compared to the read-only condition).
(d = 1.94), whereas the difference between the note-taking and The top two lines of Table 5 show the mean ratings and standard
read-only groups reached marginal significance (p = .061, d = 0.73). deviations (on a 5-point scale) of each group on difficulty and ef-
Second, we examine the prediction that active study tech- fort, respectively. Consistent with predictions, an ANOVA revealed
niques such as note-taking and using graphic organizers will re- significant differences among the groups on difficulty, F(2,
sult in better performance than the read-only group on the 61) = 4.77, p = .012, and effort, F(2, 61) = 5.92, p = .004, with pair-
cloze test, which is a measure of rote memory. If the cloze test wise Tukey tests (with alpha less than .05) showing that the
is mainly a measure of rote memory, then we can assume that read-only group reported significantly higher difficulty and lower
it is less sensitive to the effects of using a generative learning effort as compared to the graphic organizer group and the note-
strategy than is the summary test, and therefore it might not taking group, which did not differ significantly with each other.
show a superiority of the graphic organizer group over the Engaging in study activities such as filling in a graphic organizer
note-taking group. Consistent with predictions, an ANOVA while reading an expository passage is expected to be more moti-
showed a significant difference among the three groups on the vating than simply reading the passage. The third row of Table 5
cloze score, F(2, 61) = 14.48, p = .000, and a follow-up post hoc shows the mean agreement ratings and standard deviations (on a
analysis (with alpha at .05) showed that the graphic organizer 5-point scale) for the three groups on the item, ‘‘I would like more
group significantly outscored the read-only group (d = 1.69) and lessons like this one’’, which is intended as a measure of an aspect
the note-taking group significantly outscored the read-only group of motivation, namely, persistence on a learning task. Consistent
(d = 1.12), but the graphic organizer group did not significantly with predictions, an ANOVA revealed a significant difference
outscore the note-taking group (d = 0.52). among the groups, F(2, 61) = 5.30, p = .008, with pair-wise Tukey
Overall, the learning outcome data show strong effects for add- tests (with alpha less that .05) showing only that read-only group
ing study techniques to a reading task, and furthermore, the gra- gave a significant lower rating than the graphic organizer group.
phic organizer group excels over the note-taking group when a The fourth line of Table 5 summarizes the mean ratings and
measure of reading comprehension is used. standard deviations for the groups, on an item aimed at measuring

Table 4
Means and standard deviations on summary test and cloze test for each group.

Test Graphic-organizer (N = 21) Note-taking (N = 22) Read-only (N = 21) GO vs. NT GO vs. RO NT vs. RO
M SD M SD M SD d d d
Summary 4.86 1.82 2.73 1.83 1.43 1.72 1.17 1.94 .73
Cloze 7.81 2.87 6.32 2.82 3.48 2.20 .52 1.69 1.12

Note: GO: Graphic-organizer, NT: Note-taking, RO: read-only.

Table 5
Mean ratings and standard deviations on effort, difficulty, and motivation for three groups.

Item Graphic-organizer Note-taking Read-only GO vs. NT GO vs. RO NT vs. RO


M SD M SD M SD d d d
Difficulty 2.38⁄ .59 2.27⁄ .70 2.95 .97 .17 .71 .81
Effort 3.33⁄ .66 3.27⁄ 1.08 2.52 .75 .07 1.15 .80
Motivation 3.86⁄ .57 3.45 .67 3.19 .75 .66 1.01 .37
Satisfaction 3.81 .81 3.91 .68 3.43 .68 .13 .51 .71

Note: Asterisk indicates significant difference from read-only group, GO: Graphic-organizer, NT: Note-taking, RO: read-only. Higher number indicates higher levels of
difficulty, effort, motivation, or satisfaction, respectively.

Table 6
Means and standard deviations on total fixation time (in s).

Areas of Interest Graphic-organizer (N = 20) Note-taking (N = 20) Read-only (N = 21)


M SD M SD M SD
All areas of interest 199.49 42.41 144.65 37.12 62.94 13.19
Top 64.36 19.28 47.19 12.57 32.00 7.06
Bottom 63.03 18.37 47.54 14.67 30.94 7.66
GO or NT 72.10 25.18 49.92 26.53 – –

Note: GO: Graphic-organizer and NT: Note-taking.


H.R. Ponce, R.E. Mayer / Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 121–130 129

overall affect for the lesson (i.e., ‘‘I like this program’’). An ANOVA the multimedia theory of learning (Mayer, 2009), in the sense that
shows that the groups did not differ significantly, F(2, 61) = 2.59, the organization embedded in the graphic organizer primes the
p = .083, indicating that liking is not the same as learning. selection of information in the text, resulting in a new spatial rep-
resentation that is integrated in long term memory not as list of
6.4. Do the groups differ on basic eye-movement measures? facts but as compare-and-contrast structure. This integration
process was observed through the significantly different ways stu-
The foregoing results constitute the main empirical focus of the dents wrote their summaries in the three experimental conditions.
study, concerning the idea that different study activities prime The read-only and note-taking groups wrote their summaries
qualitatively different cognitive processing during learning. This mostly as list of facts, remembering aspects of one element but
section examines whether the groups differ on basic eye-move- not necessarily relating it to the other. On the contrary, the graphic
ment measures. Table 6 shows the total study time for the whole organizer group tended to write sentences that related both ele-
lesson (all areas of interest) and on each area of interest separately. ments through the appropriate categories. These two ways of
The means for total fixation times (in seconds) were significantly approaching the writing of the summaries can be interpreted as
different among the three groups, as revealed by a one-way ANO- expressions of Kintsch’s (1988) text-base and situation model of
VA, F(2, 58) = 88.18, p = .000, with post hoc Tukey tests (with alpha the text.
at .05) indicating that each of the groups differed significantly from
each other. The read-only group was asked to read the text twice, 7.3. Practical and methodological contribution
whereas the GO and NT groups had up to 5 min to complete their
study activities. However, participants using the graphic organizer These findings have instructional implications, favoring the use
spent significantly more time focused on typing into the graphic of graphic organizers for promoting deep understanding of expos-
organizer than the note-taking group spent focused on typing into itory text. This conclusion is consistent with Beyer’s (1997) recom-
the textbox, t(38) = 4.35, p < .001. In order to determine whether mendation to use graphic organizers that highlight a clear spatial
study time in each condition had an influence on learning out- structure such as comparison, sequence, classification, and cause-
comes, we ran two one-way ANCOVAs with total fixation time as and-effect. He also emphasizes the need to teach students proce-
a covariate and summary score and cloze score as independent dures for organizing information based on appropriate graphic
variables, respectively. Similar results were observed as those ob- organizers, showing them the cognitive operations involved. From
tained through one-way ANOVAs for the summary test and cloze our study, we can additionally recommend that teachers show stu-
test as reported in the previous section; suggesting that study time dents how their reading patterns are improved when processing
did not have a major influence on the results. information through graphic organizers, and that some study
activities can facilitate deeper processing.
7. Discussion This study shows that eye-tracking methodology can be suc-
cessfully extended to research on study skills. If we had used solely
7.1. Empirical contribution conventional measures of learning outcome that focus on differ-
ences in the amount learned, we would have concluded that
The main empirical contribution of this study comes from the note-taking and graphic organizers are each effective study activi-
eye-tracking analysis, in which the graphic organizer group pro- ties that improve learning as compared to read-only conditions.
duced more integrative saccades (i.e., up-and-down within the When we conduct eye-tracking analyses, we find otherwise
passage) and constructive saccades (i.e., left and right between unavailable evidence that the two study activities prime qualita-
the passage and the notes or graphic organizer) but fewer early tively different cognitive processing during learning.
constructive saccades and less early non-text fixation time than
the note-taking group. Secondarily, the graphic organizer group 7.4. Limitations and future directions
outscored the note-taking group on a test of reading comprehen-
sion (i.e., the summary test) but not on a test of verbatim memory One limitation of the study is that we used only one type of
(i.e., the cloze test), and both groups outperformed the read-only expository text—a comparison-and-contrast text. The text on
group. steamboats is well structured which naturally facilitated not only
its processing and comprehension but also the eye-tracking mea-
7.2. Theoretical contribution sures used in this study. Less structured text may require more fix-
ations and saccades and may show other reading patterns. In this
The pattern of results supports the idea that the study activities case, a text processing strategy such as graphic organizers may
of taking standard notes and filling in a graphic organizer prime play a more important role on a deeper analysis of the text than
qualitatively different cognitive processes during learning (i.e., a less structured strategy such as note-taking. Therefore, one pos-
qualitatively different learning strategies). Specifically, note-taking sible research avenue is to vary the organizational quality of texts.
is more likely to prime a linear learning strategy in which learners Another research avenue is to incorporate other types of exposi-
add facts to memory in the order presented, whereas graphic orga- tory text structures (such as process or hierarchy) or other types
nizers are more likely to prime a generative learning strategy in of text genres (such as narratives) along with appropriate graphic
which learners mentally reorganize the facts to fit within a struc- organizers. A further option is to incorporate other commonly used
ture (such as a compare-and-contrast matrix) activated from text processing strategies (such as highlighting and questioning)
long-term memory. Rather than viewing study strategies as affect- and observe eye movement behaviors. Additional research is re-
ing the amount of cognitive processing during learning, this study quired as well in terms of analyzing reading patterns and the cog-
shows that different study strategies can prime differences in the nitive operations involved when using more than one strategy to
quality and type of cognitive processing during learning. comprehend a text—for example, using a graphic organizer to-
In this particular case, we have observed that the use of the gether with a highlighting strategy. Finally, under some circum-
graphic organizer assisted learners in carrying out the compare- stances note taking may serve as a generative activity (Cook &
and-contrast cognitive operation required to have a deeper Mayer, 1988; Peper & Mayer, 1978), such as when learners are
understanding of the steamboat passage. This is consistent with experienced in taking structured notes, so more work is needed
130 H.R. Ponce, R.E. Mayer / Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 121–130

to pinpoint the conditions that determine the learning strategies Mayer, R. E. (2011). Applying the science of learning. Boston, MA: Pearson/Allyn &
Bacon.
primed by note-taking.
Mayer, R. E., & Wittrock, M. C. (2006). Problem solving. In P. A. Alexander & P. H.
Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 287–303). New York, NY:
References Routledge.
Meyer, B. J. F., & Poon, L. W. (2001). Effects of structure strategy training and
Almarza, F. A., Ponce, H. R., & Lopez, M. J. (2011). Software component development signaling on recall of text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(1), 141–159.
based on the mediator pattern design: the interactive graphic organizer case. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.141.
IEEE Latin America Transactions, 9(7), 1105–1111. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/ National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (2000). Report of the
tla.2011.6129710. National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: an evidence-based
Barnett, J. E., di Vesta, F. J., & Rogozinski, J. T. (1981). What is learned in note taking? assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications
Journal of Educational Psychology, 73(2), 181–192. for reading instruction: Reports of the subgroups (NIH Publication No. 00-
Beyer, B. K. (1997). Improving student thinking: a comprehensive approach. Boston, 4754). Washington, DC.
MA: Allyn and Bacon. Peper, R. J., & Mayer, R. E. (1978). Note taking as a generative activity. Journal of
Bohay, M., Blakely, D. P., Tamplin, A. K., & Radvansky, G. A. (2011). Note taking, Educational Psychology, 70(4), 514–522.
review, memory, and comprehension. The American Journal of Psychology, Peverly, S. T., Brobst, K. E., Graham, M., & Shaw, R. (2003). College adults are not
124(1), 63–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.124.1.0063. good at self-regulation: a study on the relationship of self-regulation, note
Cook, L. K., & Mayer, R. E. (1988). Teaching readers about the structure of scientific taking, and test taking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(2), 335–346. http://
text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 448–456. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.2.335.
0022-0663.80.4.448. Piolat, A., Olive, T., & Kellogg, R. T. (2005). Cognitive effort during note taking.
Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, D. T. (2013). Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19(3), 291–312. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques: promising acp.1086.
directions from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychological Science in Ponce, H. R., Lopez, M. J., & Mayer, R. E. (2012). Instructional effectiveness of a
the Public Interest, 14(1), 4–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1529100612453266. computer-supported program for teaching reading comprehension strategies.
Jairam, D., & Kiewra, K. A. (2009). An investigation of the SOAR study method. Computers & Education, 59(4), 1170–1183. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
Journal of Advanced Academics, 20(4), 602–629. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ j.compedu.2012.05.013.
1932202x0902000403. Porter, A., McMaken, J., Hwang, J., & Yang, R. (2011). Common core standards: the
Johnson, C. I., & Mayer, R. E. (2012). An eye movement analysis of the spatial new US intended curriculum. Educational Researcher, 40(3), 103–116. http://
contiguity effect in multimedia learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: dx.doi.org/10.3102/0013189x11405038.
Applied, 18(2), 178–191. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033- Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years
2909.124.3.37210.1037/a0026923. of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372–422. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
Kiewra, K. A. (1985). Investigating notetaking and review: a depth of processing 0033-2909.124.3.372.
alternative. Educational Psychologist, 20(1), 23. Robinson, D. H. (1998). Graphic organizers as aids to text learning. Reading Research
Kiewra, K. A., DuBois, N. F., Christian, D., McShane, A., Meyerhoffer, M., & Roskelley, and Instruction, 37(2), 85–105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19388079809558257.
D. (1991). Note-taking functions and techniques. Journal of Educational Stull, A. T., & Mayer, R. E. (2007). Learning by doing versus learning by viewing:
Psychology, 83(2), 240–245. three experimental comparisons of learner-generated versus author-provided
Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: a graphic organizers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(4), 808–820. http://
construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95(2), 163–182. dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.80.4.448.
Mason, L., Pluchino, P., & Tornatora, M. C. (2013). Effects of picture labeling on Weinstein, C. E., & Mayer, R. E. (1985). The teaching of learning strategies. In M. C.
science text processing and learning: evidence from eye movements. Reading Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (3rd ed., pp. 315–327). New
Research Quarterly, 48(2), 199–214. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/rrq.41. York: Macmillan.
Mayer, R. E. (2008). Learning and instruction (2nd ed.). Upper Sadle River, N.J.: Wittrock, M. C. (1989). Generative processes of comprehension. Educational
Pearson Merrill Prentice Hall. Psychologist, 24(4), 345.
Mayer, R. E. (2009). Multimedia learning (2nd ed.). Cambridge, New York: Cambridge
University Press.

You might also like