You are on page 1of 10

0022-3018/02/19011–757 Vol. 190, No.

11
THE JOURNAL OF NERVOUS AND MENTAL DISEASE Printed in U.S.A.
Copyright © 2002 by Lippincott Williams & Wilkins

Childhood Sexual History of 20 Male Pedophiles vs. 24 Male Healthy


Control Subjects
LISA J. COHEN, PH.D.,1 PAMELA G. MCGEOCH, M.A.,2 SNIEZYNA WATRAS GANS, PH.D.,3
KONSTANTIN NIKIFOROV, M.D.,1 KEN CULLEN, M.S.W.,4 and IGOR I. GALYNKER, M.D., PH.D.1

Despite the widespread incidence of childhood sexual abuse, there is insufficient


investigation into the childhood sexual history of perpetrators. In addition, there is
little published on the specific similarities between childhood and adult sexual
histories. The present study investigates the incidence of childhood sexual abuse in
a carefully characterized sample of male pedophiles compared with a demograph-
ically similar control group. Concordance between and cognitive distortions about
characteristics of childhood abuse and pedophilic behavior are also studied. Twenty
men with pedophilia, heterosexual type were compared with 24 demographically
similar, healthy male control subjects on a questionnaire specifically designed to
assess childhood sexual history in pedophiles. Sixty percent of pedophiles com-
pared with 4% of control subjects reported adult sexual advances as a child.
Seventy-five percent of pedophiles and 22% of control subjects reported a first
sexual encounter before age 14 years. About 60% concordance was found between
acts experienced as a child and perpetrated as an adult. Finally, numerous incon-
sistencies throughout the questionnaire add preliminary support for the role of
cognitive distortions with regard to childhood and adult sexual history. The present
findings replicate the elevated rate of childhood sexual abuse found among pedo-
philes and are consistent with the notion of a causative relationship between early
childhood abuse and later pedophilic behavior.
—J Nerv Ment Dis 190:757–766, 2002

Despite the widespread incidence of child sexual Dougher, 1988). However, the evidence for such a
abuse, there is insufficient empirical investigation theory has been mixed.
into the childhood sexual abuse history of perpetra- Numerous reports do suggest an elevated rate of
tors. Systematic investigation into the precursors of child sexual abuse in the history of pedophile of-
pedophilic behavior can help elucidate etiological fenders (Araji and Finkelhor, 1985; Bagley et al.,
factors in pedophilia and as such addresses an im- 1994; Freund et al., 1990; Haywood et al., 1996).
portant clinical and public health issue. One etiolog- Moreover, the rate of childhood sexual trauma
ical theory of pedophilia holds that pedophiles are among pedophiles may be higher than that among
“abused abusers” who are sexually attracted to chil- other groups of sexual offenders (Freund and
dren because they themselves were subject to child- Kuban, 1994; Worling, 1995a). However, estimates of
hood sexual abuse (Araji and Finkelhor, 1985). Such the incidence of childhood sexual abuse among pe-
behavior has been posited to reflect an attempt at dophiles vary widely, ranging from 28.6% (Freund et
mastery of the trauma through repetition of the trau- al., 1990) to 93% (Knopp and Lackey, 1987). This is
matic act (Araji and Finkelhor, 1985). In this way, compared with rates such as 13.4% in a group of
they attempt to combat (or deny) their own feelings healthy control subjects (Freund and Kuban, 1994)
of powerlessness and humiliation by assuming the and 15.6% in a community sample of young men
role of the powerful victimizer (Garland and (Bagley et al., 1994). It also appears that the rates of
childhood sexual abuse histories may differ accord-
1
Beth Israel Medical Center, 6 Karpas, 1st Ave. and 16th St., ing to several factors, including the age and sex of
New York, New York 10003. Send reprint requests to Dr. Cohen. victim and of perpetrator.
2
New School for Social Research, New York, New York.
3
Montefiore Medical Center, Bronx, New York. In a study comparing pedophile offenders, nonpe-
4
CAP Behavior Associates, Brooklyn, New York. dophilic sexual offenders, and healthy control sub-
Supported in part by Singer grant # I-480-400 to Dr. Galynker. jects, Freund and Kuban (1994) found that 43.9% of
The authors thank Olga Poznansky, M.S.W., Sarah Acker, B.A.,
Elena Itskovich, M.D., and Marina Moskovitch, M.D., for their 83 pedophiles compared with 13.4% of 134 control
contributions. subjects reported childhood sexual abuse histories.
DOI: 10.1097/01.NMD.0000038171.34345.D8
757
758 COHEN et al.

This rate was also substantially higher than that Finally, in a randomly selected sample of commu-
found in gynephilic sex offenders (sexual offenders nity males (N ⫽ 750), only .2% of men who reported
against women) where 17.6% of 34 reported such no sexual abuse in childhood had perpetrated sexual
histories. In an earlier study of 131 pedophilic of- contact with children aged 13 years and younger.
fenders, the incidence of reported child sexual This is compared with 7.7% of men who had re-
abuse was much higher among heterosexual (28.6% ported multiple events of sexual contact in their
of 77) and homosexual (25.9% of 54) pedophiles than own childhood (Bagley et al., 1994).
in nonpedophilic child molesters, i.e., those with an In summary, there is clear consensus of an ele-
erotic preference for adults (19.6% of 51); gynephilic vated rate of childhood sexual abuse in pedophiles.
offenders (10.7% of 36); and homosexual (11.8% of On its own, however, evidence of an increased inci-
51) and heterosexual (10.7% of 75) control subjects dence of childhood sexual abuse in male pedophiles
(Freund et al., 1990). Further, in a study of 151 male is insufficient to establish a causative relationship
perpetrators, 57% of the child molesters compared between childhood abuse and adult pedophilia. Ev-
with 23% of the rapists reported having been victims idence of concordance between age of abuse and
of child sexual abuse (Seghorn et al., 1987). On the age of victims or between acts experienced and acts
other hand, while Dhawan and Marshall (1996) perpetrated would provide additional evidence of
found that the rate of child sexual abuse was higher the etiological role of child sexual abuse in pedo-
among incarcerated pedophiles (50%, 8 of 16) than philia. Thus, also of interest is the degree of concor-
nonsexual offenders (20%, 4 of 20), it was not higher dance between the age of the victims chosen by the
than that reported among rapists, (62%, 18 of 29). perpetrator and the age at which the perpetrator’s
Moreover, Worling (1995a) found that the rate of own sexual abuse began. Similarly, the actual acts
child sexual abuse in a sample of adolescent male pedophiles perpetrate on victims may bear a striking
sexual offenders was approximately equal between relationship to the acts of victimization that they
those who had sexually assaulted children (24%, 7 of themselves endured as children. Only one case re-
port, to our knowledge, examined concordance of
29) and those who had assaulted postpubescent fe-
age and type of sexual act. Haapasalo et al. (1999)
males (26%, 7 of 27).
reported a case in which the sexual acts perpetrated
The rates of sexual victimization during childhood
and the ages of the victims were almost identical to
in female pedophiles are also high, possibly higher
those in the perpetrator’s own childhood experi-
than those of male pedophiles. Based on clinician
ences.
reports, the incidence of childhood sexual abuse in
In addition, there is wide variation in the rates
a sample of female perpetrators ranged from 93% to reported. The variation, in part, may relate to the age
100% across three age groups (Knopf and Lackey, and sex of victim and perpetrator, highlighting the
1987). Similarly, Johnson (1989) found that of 13 importance of such data in studies of childhood
girls who had sexually assaulted other children, sexual abuse. However, the accuracy of such figures
100% had been sexually abused themselves. How- has also been questioned, with some investigators
ever, Faller (1987) found that 47.5% (19) of 40 female suggesting the rates are overreported, as pedophiles
perpetrators reported abuse in their own childhood. may falsify the incidence or extent of abuse in an
Among adolescent offenders, the incidence of attempt to mitigate the legal consequences of their
childhood sexual abuse has also been reported to be behavior (Hanson and Slater, 1988; Hindman, 1988;
much higher than that in the general population. Langevin et al., 1989). Others suggest the rates are
Forty percent of 57 adolescents who had sexually substantially underreported, and even that perpetra-
assaulted children reported having been sexually tors may fear appearing guilty of their legal charges
abused compared with 29% of 38 adolescents who if they admit to sexual victimization (Hanson and
had sexually assaulted peers or adults (O’Brien, Slater, 1988). On the other hand, pedophiles may
1991; Worling, 1995b). In a study utilizing retrospec- underreport their own history of sexual abuse for
tive chart reviews, 42% of 90 male adolescent sexual purely psychological reasons. Due to the traumatic
offenders reported sexual abuse histories (Worling, nature of the sex abuse history, there may be an
1995b). These accounts were corroborated by exter- inability or failure to cognitively process the memo-
nal agencies in all but seven cases. Significant dif- ries. As such, the memories of childhood abuse are
ferences in the rate of sexual abuse were found denied or distorted. In other words, the cognitive
when grouping offenders by choice of victim. distortions operate at least partially outside of con-
Among those who had assaulted boys, the rate was scious control.
74%. However, among those who assaulted only fe- Cognitive distortions and the denial of the implica-
males of any age, the rate was only 25%. tions for pedophilic behavior have been widely dis-
CHILDHOOD SEXUAL HISTORY OF 20 PEDOPHILES 759
cussed (Fischer et al., 1999; Haywood and Grossman, child and was on probation for Internet trafficking
1994). Pedophiles routinely deny and minimize the of child pornography.
deviant nature of their behavior and its destructive Twenty-four demographically similar healthy
impact on the children involved. Thus, one possibility male control subjects were recruited through adver-
could be that, among pedophiles, the denial and cog- tising in a local newspaper. Exclusion criteria for
nitive distortions of pedophilic behavior may be etio- both groups included significant axis I psychiatric
logically related to the denial and cognitive distortions disorder (psychotic disorder, major affective disor-
of their own history of sexual abuse. der, serious anxiety disorder), substance abuse or
In the following study, 20 male pedophiles, het- dependence within the past 6 months, major medical
erosexual, nonexclusive type were compared with or neurological disease, and mental retardation. For
24 demographically similar healthy control subjects pedophiles, this was defined as any major mood
on a detailed measure of sexual history. Specifically, disorder (major depressive disorder, bipolar disor-
the present study assessed a) the incidence of child- der), psychotic disorder, or significantly debilitating
hood sexual abuse in pedophiles versus control sub- disorder requiring hospitalization. For control sub-
jects; and b) the nature of and concordance between jects, it was defined as any Structured Clinical In-
characteristics of childhood abuse and adult pedo- terview for DSM (SCID-I) diagnosis. Further exclu-
philic behavior. A secondary analysis assessed the sion criteria for control subjects included any
presence of cognitive distortions regarding child- history of pedophilia or of sexual contact with a
hood sexual history and pedophilic behavior child aged 14 years or younger after the age 16 years,
through examination of response discrepancies or with a child aged 16 years or younger after the age
throughout the questionnaire. 18 years. All subjects expressed sexual attraction to
women.

Methods
Procedure
This study was part of a larger, comprehensive
Subjects
investigation of neuropsychiatric function in pedo-
Twenty male subjects with a DSM-IV diagnosis of philes, which included PET and plethysmographic
pedophilia, heterosexual type, were recruited from methods. The 44 subjects who entered the current
CAP Behavior Associates, a facility specializing in study were paid $50.00 after they completed a bat-
outpatient treatment of court-referred sexual of- tery of psychometric tests.
fenders. CAP provides a comprehensive treatment
program that includes group psychotherapy incor-
Recruitment
porating cognitive-behavioral techniques, psychiat-
ric coverage, and coordination with the judicial sys- To recruit pedophiles, a research assistant visited
tem. At least 1 year of treatment is required. CAP Behavior Associates and spoke to the clients
Subjects were recruited within the first few weeks immediately prior to their group therapy appoint-
of treatment. ment. The research assistant first described the
All pedophilic subjects were diagnosed with pe- project in general to the assembled group of CAP
dophilia, heterosexual, nonexclusive, nonincest type Behavior Associates clients. Pedophilic patients
according to DSM-IV criteria by psychiatric evalua- who expressed interest in the project were inter-
tion at CAP Behavior Associates. Incest refers to viewed individually. Interested subjects were sched-
sexual activity with first-degree relatives. To reduce uled for an appointment at Beth Israel Medical Cen-
potential confounds of a heterogeneous subject pop- ter for further evaluation and further explanation of
ulation, we used strict inclusion and exclusion cri- study protocol. Control subjects were screened by
teria. All pedophilia subjects had been convicted of phone for initial eligibility. Of the 119 volunteers
a sexual offense against a prepubescent child (aged who were initially screened by telephone, 50 were
13 years or younger), which they committed when interviewed, 29 signed informed consent, and 24
aged at least 18 years or when 5 years older than completed the psychometric studies. Fourteen were
their victim, and were currently either on parole or excluded for admitting sexual attraction to girls
on probation. In addition, all subjects admitted to aged 13 years or younger. After primary inclusive
committing the act for which they were charged and criteria were met, subjects were interviewed in per-
had committed more than one act against more than son by research staff at Beth Israel Medical Center.
one victim, with the exception of one subject who Control subjects completed a SCID-I interview (First
denied having had direct sexual engagement with a et al., 1996) and a clinical interview by a psycholo-
760 COHEN et al.

gist. All interested subjects who met inclusion and sexual experiences on them and of their pedophilic
exclusion criteria were entered into the study after behavior on the child recipient. The 6-point scales
signing an informed consent form. The BIMC Com- ranged from 1 ⫽ very bad effect, 2 ⫽ bad effect, 3 ⫽
mittee on Scientific Activities (COSA) approved this slightly bad effect, 4 ⫽ none, 5 ⫽ positive effect, to
study. 6 ⫽ very positive effect.

Statistical Analysis
Materials
Chi-square analyses were used with frequency
Sexual History Questionnaire. A detailed 72-item variables to compare scores across the subject
self-report questionnaire assessing childhood sexual groups, and t-test statistics were calculated for con-
experiences, sexual interest, and pedophilic behav- tinuous variables. When homogeneity of variance
ior was developed for this study. Sexual age prefer- was not supported by Levene test for equal variance,
ences were assessed in a grid-format checklist, with t-tests for unequal variance were reported. Because
three columns of the grid labeled “sexually attracted years of education significantly differed across
to,” “have had sexual encounters with,” and “have groups (see below), it was entered as a covariate in
had legal trouble for sexual activity with.” The 10 all of the analyses of childhood sexual history. In
rows included five categories of age groups for first addition, because of the large number of compari-
girls and then boys, ranging from infancy through sons in the self-reported sexual preferences data,
adulthood (18 years or more). Control subjects com- Bonferroni corrections were applied. Analysis was
pleted a shortened form, which collapsed the young- performed using SPSS for Windows, version 10.0.
est age categories into one category of 12 years and
younger. To increase the reliability of subjects’ re- Results
ported sexual abuse history, childhood sexual his-
tory was assessed with multiple questions. At the Subjects
beginning of the questionnaire, subjects are asked to Pedophiles and control subjects did not signifi-
list the age of their first sexual experience, with cantly differ on age, marital status, ethnicity, or
whom it occurred, and the age of their first partner. religion. Pedophiles had fewer years of education
Later in the questionnaire, they are asked a series of compared with control subjects (12.00 ⫾ 3.2 vs.
questions such as: “When you were a child did any 14.75 ⫾ 2.1, t ⫽ ⫺3.43, df ⫽ 42, p ⫽ .001) and a lower
adult ever make sexual advances at you?” and “How level of employment [65% vs. 92%, chi-square ⫽ 4.8,
old were you when it started?” They are also asked df ⫽ (1), p ⫽ .035]. Twenty percent of pedophiles
the number of people who made advances toward were European American, 25% African American,
them, their gender, ages, and whether they were 45% Hispanic, and 5% Asian. Thirty-five percent were
family members. single, 20% married, 20% separated, and 25% di-
Last, possible concordance between acts experi- vorced. Twenty-nine percent of control subjects
enced in childhood and pedophilic acts is assessed were European American, 21% were African Ameri-
by asking subjects to check from a list of eight can, 46% Hispanic, and 4% Asian. Fifty percent were
sexual practices any acts performed on them as a single, 29% married, 8% separated, and 13% divorced.
child by an adult: fondling, kissing, tongue kissing,
masturbating adult, adult touching child’s genitals,
Pedophilic Age Preferences
fellatio on adult, sexual intercourse, and anal inter-
course. They were then asked to check any of their All pedophiles and control subjects admitted at-
adult pedophilic behaviors from a separate list with traction to adult women, aged 18 years or more
the same eight categories of sexual acts. (Table 1). While 3% of control subjects also admitted
Cognitive distortions were evaluated indirectly attraction to teenage girls aged 13 to 17 years, none
through assessment of inconsistencies in the ques- reported being attracted to girls aged younger than
tionnaire. Age of first reported sexual contact was 13 years, and no control subjects reported any sex-
compared with age at which an adult made a sexual ual activity with girls aged 12 years and younger. In
advance. Patterns of admitted attraction to, sexual contrast, 50% of pedophiles report attraction to girls
activity with, and legal trouble involving children of aged 13 to 17 years, 30% to girls aged 10 to 12 years,
different age groups were also examined for incon- and 5% to girls aged 6 to 9 years. Of note, only 30%
sistencies. Finally, 9 of the 20 pedophiles were given admitted being attracted to girls aged 12 years and
an amended version of the questionnaire, with two younger, yet 40% admitted to sexual encounters with
questions about the effect of their own childhood them, and 60% of pedophiles admitted to legal trou-
CHILDHOOD SEXUAL HISTORY OF 20 PEDOPHILES 761
TABLE 1
Self-Reported Sexual Age Preferences
Age Group Sexual History Pedophiles Controls Statistic ␹2 (df) Significance
Women, 18⫹ Attracted to 20 (100%) 24 (100%) ns
Encounters 17 (85%) 24 (100%) 3.86 (1,44) .049
Legal trouble 0 (0%) 0 (0%) ns
Girls 13–17 Attracted to 10 (50%) 3 (13%) 7.37 (1,44) .007
Encounters 8 (40%) 3 (13%) 4.40 (1,44) .036
Legal trouble 7 (35%) 0 (0%) 9.99 (1,44) .002
Girls 10–12 Attracted to 6 (30%) 0 (0%) 8.34 (1,44) .004
Encounters 8 (40%) 0 (0%) 11.73 (1,44) .001*
Legal trouble 10 (50%) 0 (0%) 15.53 (1,44) ⬍.001*
Girls 6–9 Attracted to 1 (5%) 0 (0%) ns
Encounters 3 (15%) 0 (0%) 3.86 (1,44) .049
Legal trouble 5 (25%) 0 (0%) 6.77 (1,44) .009
Girls 3–5 Attracted to 0 (0%) 0 (0%) ns
Encounters 1 (5%) 0 (0%) ns
Legal trouble 1 (5%) 0 (0%) ns
Men 18⫹ Attracted to 0 (0%) 1 (5%) ns
Encounters 0 (0%) 0 (0%) ns
Legal trouble 0 (0%) 0 (0%) ns
Boys 13–18 Attracted to 0 (0%) 0 (0%) ns
Encounters 0 (0%) 0 (0%) ns
Legal trouble 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
Boys 10–12 Attracted to 1 (5%) 0 (0%) ns
Encounters 1 (5%) 0 (0%) ns
Legal trouble 1 (5%) 0 (0%)
Boys 6–9 Attracted to 1 (5%) 0 (0%) ns
Encounters 1 (5%) 0 (0%) ns
Legal trouble 1 (5%) 0 (0%) ns
*Reaches significance at Bonferroni corrected level of p ⱕ .001

ble for their sexual activity with those girls. One pedophiles reported sexual abuse by one or more
pedophile subject (5%) admitted to sexual attrac- men, six (30%) reported abuse by one or more
tion, sexual activity, and legal trouble with boys women, and three (15%) reported abuse by a family
aged 10 to 12 years, and one other (5%) admitted to member. One control subject reported being sexu-
the same with boys aged 6 to 9 years. This is in direct ally abused by an aunt at age 12 years.
contrast to the initial psychiatric evaluations at CAP, As sexual abuse may be underreported, we also
in which all pedophiles were diagnosed as hetero- calculated age of earliest sexual encounter, which
sexual. Statistically, groups only differed in admis- was defined as the younger of either first reported
sion of sexual encounters with women, and with sexual experience or age at which an adult made a
girls aged 13 to 17 years, 10 to 12 years, and 6 to 9 sexual advance. The reported age of earliest sexual
years; sexual attraction to girls aged 13 to 17 years encounter occurred significantly earlier among the
and 10 to 12 years; and legal trouble with girls aged pedophilic group, at an average age of 11.10 ⫾ 5.2
13 to 17 years, 10 to 12 years, and 6 to 9 years. years compared with 16.13 ⫾ 2.42 years in the con-
However, after Bonferroni corrections, only sexual trol group (t ⫽ ⫺3.97, df ⫽ 25.73, p ⫽ .001). There
encounters and legal trouble with girls aged 10 to 12 was also a notable difference in the proportion of
years maintained significance (Table 1). subjects whose earliest sexual encounter occurred
at age 13 years or younger. Seventy-five percent of
pedophiles compared with only 21.7% of control
Childhood Sexual History
subjects reported their first sexual encounter when
Pedophiles relative to control subjects reported a younger than age 13 years [chi-square ⫽ 12.2 (1),
much higher incidence of child sexual abuse (Table p ⫽ .000]. The age difference between self and first
2). Twelve (60%) of the pedophiles compared with partner averaged 14.40 ⫾ 9.7 years among the pedo-
only one (4%) control subject reported adult sexual philic group and only 2.33 ⫾ 7.3 years for control
advances in childhood [chi-square ⫽ 16.34 (1), p ⱕ subjects (t ⫽ 4.60, df ⫽ 34.81, p ⫽ .000). All of these
.001]. Age of reported abuse for pedophiles averaged comparisons retained statistical significance when
7.83 ⫾ 3.74 years (range: 3 to 16 years). Eight (40%) covaried by education (Table 2).
762 COHEN et al.
TABLE 2
Self-Reported Sexual History of Male Pedophiles
Pedophiles (20) Controls (23) Statistic Signif*
Age of 1st sexual encounter 11.10 ⫹ 5.2 16.13 ⫹ 2.42 t ⫽ ⫺3.97 (25.73) .001
Age of 1st partner 25.50 ⫹ 7.8 18.46 ⫹ 6.7 t ⫽ 3.18 (37.7) .003
Age difference 14.40 ⫹ 9.7 2.33 ⫹ 7.3 t ⫽ 4.60 (34.81) ⬍.001
1st sex at age 13 and under 15 (75%) 5 (21.7%) ␹2 ⫽ 12.2 (1,43) ⬍.001
Adult advances during childhood 12 (60%) 1 (4%) ␹2 ⫽ 16.34 (1,44) ⬍.001
By more than one adult 5 (25%) 0 (0%) ␹2 ⫽ 6.77 (1,44) .009
Number of men ␹2 ⫽ 11.73 (3,44) .008
0 12 (60%) 24 (100%)
1 6 (30%) 0
2 1 (%) 0
3 1 (%) 0
Number of women ␹2 ⫽ 5.62 (3,44) ns
0 14 (70%) 23 (96%)
1 4 (20%) 1 (4%)
4 1 (5%) 0
20 1 (5%) 0
Perpetrator was family member 3 (15%) 1 (4%) ␹2 ⫽ 1.55 (1,44) ns
*All significant comparisons maintained significance when covaried by education.

Concordance dophiles admitted being attracted to girls aged 10 to


12 years, 40% admitted to having had sexual encoun-
Pedophiles also report a wide range of sexual acts
ters with them, and 50% to having had legal trouble
experienced as children and performed on children
for this behavior. Similarly, while only 5% of pedo-
as adults. They reported a mean of 1.2 ⫾ .2 types of
philes admitted attraction to girls aged 6 to 9 years,
acts experienced as a child with an adult and a mean
15% admitted to sexual encounters with them, and
of 2.95 ⫾ .2 types of acts performed on a child as an
25% had undergone legal trouble for those actions.
adult. The concordance between childhood experi-
With regard to older age groups, i.e., women and
ence of a given sexual act and pedophilic perfor-
girls aged 13 to 17 years, the pattern was reversed,
mance of such an act averaged 58%. For seven of the
with attraction being the most frequently endorsed,
eight particular sexual acts, 66% of pedophiles on
sexual encounters the next, and legal trouble the
average (range: 50% to 100%) admitted to repeating
least. One subject denied any attraction, sexual in-
the particular act in their adult pedophilic behavior.
volvement, or legal trouble with children aged less
The one exception was anal intercourse, which was
than 18 years despite a documented history of pe-
not reported as having been repeated. However, of
dophilic offenses. In contrast, there is no discrep-
note, almost twice the number of subjects reported
ancy among control subjects regarding their re-
pedophilic performance of most sexual acts than
ported age preferences.
childhood experience of the same acts. To assess
the concordance between age of earliest sexual con- Of the 12 pedophiles who reported experiences of
tact and age of victim, a rough estimate of victim age adult sexual advances as a child, 9 (75%) reported a
was calculated by recoding the youngest age prefer- different age of first sexual contact (mean ⫽ 11.94 ⫾
ence listed (involving sexual encounters or legal 3.83 years) than age of adult sexual advances (7.83 ⫾
trouble with) into a 5-point scale, ranging from 1 ⫽ 3.74 years). The one control subject who reported a
age 3 to 5 years to 5 ⫽ adults only. This was then sexual abuse history reported the same age of first
correlated with the earliest age of pedophiles’ own sexual contact as age of adult advance (12 years).
sexual contact. The correlation was significant (r ⫽ Moreover, of the 20 pedophiles, 9 answered the
.511, p ⫽ .021, N ⫽ 20). question about the effect of their pedophilic behav-
ior on the involved children. One (11%) responded a
very positive effect, two (22%) reported no effect,
Cognitive Distortions
one (11%) answered slightly bad effect, and five
Response discrepancies throughout the question- (56%) answered very bad effect. Of the 12 pedo-
naire were also examined as a preliminary estimate philes who reported adult sexual advances in child-
of propensity toward cognitive distortions. Pedo- hood, 6 answered the questions about the effect of
philes demonstrated numerous response discrepan- their own childhood sexual history. Two (33%) an-
cies in reports of adult pedophilic behavior and swered no effect, one (17%) answered slightly bad
childhood sexual experience. While only 30% of pe- effect, two (33%) answered bad effect, and one (17%)
CHILDHOOD SEXUAL HISTORY OF 20 PEDOPHILES 763
answered very bad effect. Hence, a sizable fraction of The methods of assessing sexual abuse histories
the pedophiles did not consider childhood sexual may also affect results. Our assessment method in-
abuse to have a strongly negative effect on a child. volves a retrospective, self-report questionnaire that
uses several questions to assess the variables. Other
studies differ in assessment measures. Very few use
Discussion questionnaires specifically designed to assess child-
hood history of sexual abuse (Bagley et al., 1994;
The present study assessed the childhood sexual
Dhawan and Marshall, 1996), although Dhawan and
history and pedophilic practices of 20 pedophilic
Marshall (1996) used a semistructured interview in
men compared with 24 healthy male control sub-
addition to a questionnaire. Many studies used data
jects. Major findings were as follows: A number of
culled from more general assessment instruments,
variables converge to support the finding of a dis-
for example, with a few questions about sexual
proportionately high rate of childhood sexual abuse
abuse history embedded in a sexual preference
histories in male pedophiles. Sixty percent of pedo-
questionnaire (Freund and Kuban, 1994; Freund et
philes compared with 4% of control subjects report
al., 1990), semistructured interviews (Faller, 1987;
experiencing adult sexual advances as a child. Pe-
Worling, 1995a, 1995b), or in intake evaluations
dophiles’ reported average age of first sexual en-
(Johnson, 1989). Several studies utilized retrospec-
counter was 11.10 years compared with 16.13 years
tive data from clinical charts (Seghorn et al., 1987)
for control subjects. The proportion of pedophiles
or treatment providers (Knopf and Lackey, 1987).
who reported a first sexual encounter at age 13
On the other hand, corroborative legal or clinical
years or younger was 75% versus 22% for control
data were used in a study of adolescent offenders
subjects. Forty percent report sexual advances in
(Worling, 1995b).
childhood by at least one man and 30% by at least
However, in all retrospective studies of childhood
one woman; 25% report sexual advances by more
sexual abuse, assessment methods pose significant
than one adult. A 58% concordance rate was found
challenges to the reliability and validity of the re-
between reported acts experienced as a child and
sults. Subjects’ willingness to disclose such sensitive
perpetrated as an adult. Finally, numerous inconsisten-
material, their ability to recognize and label child-
cies throughout the questionnaire support the role of
hood experiences, and the accuracy of their memory
cognitive distortions with regard to present pedophilic
for distant events cannot be assumed. In the absence
behavior and also childhood sexual history.
of objective or at least corroborative data, struc-
tured assessment instruments can best approximate
an accurate assessment. Nonetheless, that the high
Incidence of Childhood Sexual Abuse
incidence of sexual abuse is so widely replicated
The elevated rate of admitted childhood sexual supports the robustness of the present findings.
abuse in the present study is consistent with numer-
ous reports of elevated rates of childhood sexual
Characteristics of Childhood Sexual Abuse
abuse among pedophiles (Dhawan and Marshall,
1996; Freund et al., 1990; Freund and Kuban, 1994; Our study differed from many other studies, how-
Seghorn et al., 1987). Other studies of adult male pe- ever, in that we also asked questions about the age
dophiles report incidences of 27% (Freund et al., 1990), and gender of the adult sexual abusers. It is of note
43.9% (Freund and Kuban, 1994), 50% (Dhawan and that the proportion of subjects who report child-
Marshall, 1996), and 57% (Seghorn et al., 1987). Hence, hood sexual encounters with women (30%) does not
our rate is somewhat higher than those found in sim- significantly differ from those who report such en-
ilar populations. That our subjects were outpatients counters with men (40%). This intriguing finding
currently in group treatment may have increased their either suggests that sexual abuse by female perpe-
readiness to disclose their sexual abuse history. This is trators is much more frequent than generally known
in contrast to the incarcerated status of several of the or that victims of female sexual perpetrators are
previous studies’ populations (Dhawan and Marshall, disproportionately represented in samples of male
1996; Seghorn et al., 1987; Worling, 1995a). However, pedophiles.
our incidence is lower than those found in samples of Regarding the concordance between childhood
girls who abuse other children (100%; Johnson, 1989; sexual history and adult pedophilic behavior, there
Knopf and Lackey, 1987) and adult female pedophiles is very little literature. Johnson (1989) noted concor-
(93%; Knopf and Lackey, 1987). This may reflect gen- dance of relationship status in child perpetrators. Of
der differences either in self-disclosure patterns or in the 11 children who were abused by a family mem-
sexual abuse histories of male and female pedophiles. ber, 91% subsequently abused a relative. Regarding
764 COHEN et al.

concordance of age of victim, there are no reported tory when reporting their age of first sexual experi-
data in the literature. However, as discussed earlier, ence, and only one of six reported such a history to
there is a fair amount of evidence that pedophiles have had a strongly negative effect. Given the wealth
have a higher rate of childhood sexual abuse than do of data supporting the pervasive and pernicious ef-
gynephilic offenders. As to concordance of type of fect of early sexual abuse, such findings are more
sexual act, to our knowledge, there are no data. likely to reflect pedophiles’ own response biases
Several studies assess in detail either the specific than the genuinely benign effect of their childhood
acts experienced in childhood sexual abuse (Bagley sexual experiences. That our subjects appear to
et al., 1994) or those perpetrated on child victims more readily admit the negative impact of their pe-
(Johnson, 1989), but no study to date has attempted dophilic behavior on their victim than of their own
to correlate such data. That our current study found childhood experiences on themselves is intriguing
a 58% concordance rate between type of pedophilic but may reflect the focus of their outpatient treat-
act sustained in childhood and perpetrated in adult- ment. We would expect that increased treatment
hood adds support to the notion of concordance time would decrease cognitive distortions. Future
between childhood experience of sexual abuse and studies could control for the effect of length of time
adult pedophilic behavior. However, clearly other in treatment.
However, it is also possible that such response in-
factors influence choice of pedophilic acts. These
consistencies reflect intentional attempts to deceive,
may include situational factors, e.g., expedience or
associated with widely documented antisocial traits
risk of discovery and sexual preferences developed
(Black et al., 1997; Raymond et al., 1999). Certainly,
in adulthood. Although we did not systematically
pedophiles may be motivated to bias their responses to
assess concordance between age of youngest victim
protect their own self-interest within the criminal jus-
and earliest age of victimization, our post hoc cor- tice system. Many other authors, however, have dis-
relation supports such a relationship. This finding cussed the central role of such cognitive distortions in
can be seen as consistent with the notion of a caus- the development and perpetuation of pedophilic be-
ative relationship between early childhood abuse havior, above and beyond the obvious self-serving
and later pedophilic behavior. functions. The current findings add preliminary sup-
port to the notion that pedophiles may distort their
understanding of their own childhood sexual abuse,
Cognitive Distortions perhaps in an attempt to regulate the associated dis-
Despite multiple studies on pedophiles’ cognitive tress of such traumatic memories. This distortion pre-
distortions about pedophilic behavior (Blumenthal vents the effective processing of the trauma and leaves
et al., 1999; Keenan and Ward, 2000; McGrath et al., the person vulnerable to the development of maladap-
tive coping mechanisms, such as compulsive repetition
1998), there is very little information on their distor-
of the trauma via assumption of the active role. Such
tions of their own childhood sexual abuse and none,
an interpretation is certainly consistent with many
to our knowledge, on both. Although we did not
models of trauma and the maladaptive sequelae of
study cognitive distortions with previously vali-
trauma (Bagley et al., 1994; Courtois, 1988; Worling,
dated, structured instruments, our study found mul-
1995a, 1995b). Of course, any such model must be seen
tiple inconsistencies within the data, suggestive of as highly speculative, given the preliminary nature of
such cognitive distortions. Pedophiles appeared to the data about not only the incidence of childhood
slant their reports of sexual age preferences in the sexual abuse in pedophiles but also the role of cogni-
direction of social desirability. This implies that they tive distortions in the processing of such histories.
understand the social stigma associated with pedo- Finally, even if robust evidence supporting the current
philic behavior. Nonetheless, only five of nine stated model is demonstrated, it is unlikely to wholly explain
that their behavior had a very negative effect on the etiology of pedophilia. In all but two studies, a
their victims, suggesting a minimization of the im- significant fraction of child sexual molesters denied
pact of their behavior. This is consistent with the any childhood history of sexual abuse. Moreover, the
multiple reports of pedophiles minimizing and ratio- vast majority of sexual abuse victims do not become
nalizing pedophilic behavior (Blumenthal et al., abusers (Bagley et al., 1994).
1999; Keenan and Ward, 2000; McGrath et al., 1998). The present study investigated the incidence of
Of interest, however, our study also provided pre- childhood sexual abuse in a sample of male, hetero-
liminary data of similar minimization with regard to sexual, nonexclusive pedophiles compared with a
their own history of childhood sexual abuse. Many demographically similar control group. By applying
disregarded their early childhood sexual abuse his- fairly rigorous inclusion and exclusion criteria and a
CHILDHOOD SEXUAL HISTORY OF 20 PEDOPHILES 765
structured instrument specifically designed to study roborative data, if possible, are also important given
the topic of this article, the present findings manage the validity problems of retrospective reports. Sys-
to avoid many of the potential confounds found in tematic assessments of the degree and nature of
other studies. Furthermore, our findings add sup- cognitive distortions are also needed. Pedophilic di-
port to the extant literature and contribute new agnoses could be corroborated with plethysmo-
information on the characteristics of childhood sex- graphic or other objective measures of sexual
ual abuse in male pedophiles and its concordance arousal patterns. Given the pervasive and devastat-
with characteristics of adult pedophilic behavior. ing effects of childhood sexual abuse, such investi-
Nonetheless, the current study also presents several gations are unquestionably indicated, if not overdue.
important limitations. The study sample was small,
and the pedophilic subjects were restricted to an
outpatient clinic specializing in sex offenders and,
References
thus, perhaps were representative neither of incar-
cerated pedophiles nor those living in the commu- Abel GG, Becker JV, Mittelman M, et al. (1987) Self-reported sex
nity. Two subjects admitted to bisexual pedophilic crimes of non-incarcerated paraphiliacs. J Interpers Violence
2:3–25.
activity despite being previously classified as hetero- Araji S, Finkelhor D (1985) Explanations of pedophilia: Review
sexual by psychiatric evaluation. of empirical research. Bull Am Acad Psychiatry Law 13:17–37.
The question of diagnostic certainty is intrinsic to Bagley C, Wood, M, Young L (1994) Victim to abuser: Mental
this population. Due to their propensity toward un- health and behavioral sequels of child sexual abuse in a com-
munity survey of young adult males. Child Abuse Neglect
reliable self-reports, it is difficult to ascertain 18:683– 690.
whether pedophilic behavior reflects a consistent Black DW, Kehrberg LLD, Flumefelt DL, Schlosser SS (1997)
sexual orientation or an opportunistic encounter, Characteristics of 36 subjects reporting compulsive sexual
behavior. Am J Psychiatry 154:243–249.
i.e., whether our subjects are true pedophiles or Blumenthal S, Gudjonsson G, Burns J (1999) Cognitive distor-
nonpedophilic child molesters. In our sample, all tions and blame attribution in sex offenders against adults and
pedophile subjects admitted to more than one pedo- children. Child Abuse Neglect 23:129 –143.
philic encounter and more than one victim. More- Courtois CA (1988) Healing the incest wound: Adult survivors
in therapy. New York: W.W. Norton & Company.
over, it is most likely that pedophilic behavior in Dhawan S, Marshall WL (1996) Sexual abuse histories of sexual
convicted child molesters is significantly underre- offenders. Sex Abuse 8:7–15.
ported rather than overestimated. In a report by Faller KC (1987) Women who sexually abuse children. Violence
Abel et al. (1987), using a federal guarantee of con- Vict 2:263–276.
Fischer D, Beech A, Browne K (1999) Comparison of sex offend-
fidentiality, convicted child molesters admitted to an ers to non offenders on selected psychological measures. Int J
average 240 pedophilic acts, far greater than the Offender Ther Compar Criminol 43:473– 491.
rates reported in most studies. It is also possible that First M, Gibbon M, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW (1996) Structured
our stringent criteria for control subjects may exag- clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders (research ver-
sion). New York: Biometrics Research Dept., New York State
gerate the distinctions between pedophiles and con- Psychiatric Institute.
trol subjects. Further research with community sam- Freund K, Watson, R, Dickey R (1990) Does sexual abuse in
ples could be conducted to address this potential childhood cause pedophilia: An exploratory study. Arch Sex
Behav 19:557–568.
confound. Nevertheless, it is important to note that Freund K, Kuban M (1994) The basis of the abused abuser theory
only 14 of the 119 potential control subjects were of pedophilia: A further elaboration on an earlier study. Arch
rejected for admitting sexual attraction to girls aged Sex Behav 23:553–563.
less than 14 years. Furthermore, the questionnaire Garland RJ, Dougher MJ (1988) The abused/abuser hypothesis of
child sexual abuse: A critical review of theory and research. In
was developed for the present study and thus has no JR Feirerman (Ed), Adult human sexual behavior with chil-
previously demonstrated psychometric properties. dren and adolescents: Biosocial dimensions. New York: Al-
The accuracy of pedophile subjects’ responses is dine de Gruter.
often questionable, as discussed above, thus compli- Haapasalo J, Puupponen M, Crittenden P (1999) Victim to vic-
timizer: The psychology of isomorphism in a case of a recidi-
cating the interpretation of the findings. Finally, the vist pedophile in Finland. J Child Sex Abuse 1999:97–115.
assessment of response discrepancies was per- Hanson RK, Slater S (1988) Sexual victimization in the history of
formed only as a secondary analysis and thus not sexual abusers. Ann Sex Res 1:485– 499.
supported by structured measures of cognitive dis- Haywood TW, Dravitz HM, Wasyliw OE, Galberg J, Cavanaugh JL
(1996) Cycle of abuse and psychopathology in cleric and non-
tortions. cleric molesters of children and adolescents. Child Abuse
Further research is needed with rigorous assess- Neglect 20:1233–1243.
ment methods, such as structured interviews and Haywood TW, Grossman LS (1994) Denial of deviant sexual
arousal and psychopathology in child molesters. Behav Ther
questionnaires specifically designed to assess child- 25:327–340.
hood histories of sexual abuse, including the types Hindman J (1988) Research disputes assumptions about child
of acts, perpetrators, and ages of victimization. Cor- molesters. NDAA Bull 7:1–3.
766 COHEN et al.
Johnson TC (1989) Female child perpetrators: Children who (Ed), Family sexual abuse: Frontline research and evalua-
molest other children. Child Abuse Neglect 13:571–585. tion. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Keenan, T, Ward T (2000) A theory of mind perspective on Raymond NC, Coleman E, Ohlerking F, Christenson GA, Miner M
cognitive, affective and intimacy deficits in child sexual of- (1999) Psychiatric comorbidity in pedophilic sex offenders.
fenders. Sex Abuse 12:49 – 60. Am J Psychiatry 156:786 –788.
Knopf F, Lackey L (1987) Female sexual abusers: A summary of Seghorn TK, Prentky RA, Boucher RJ (1987) Childhood sexual
data from 44 treatment providers. Orwell, Vermont. abuse in the lives of sexually aggressive offenders. J Am Acad
Langevin R, Wright P, Handy L (1989) Characteristics of sex Child Adolesc Psychiat 26:262–267.
offenders who were sexually victimized as children. Ann Sex Worling JR (1995a) Adolescent sex offenders against females:
Res 2:227–253. Differences based on the age of their victims. Int J Offender
McGrath M, Cann S, Konopasky R (1998) New measures of Ther Compar Criminol 39:276 –293.
defensiveness, empathy and cognitive distortions for sexual Worling JR (1995b) Sexual abuse histories of adolescent male
offenders against children. Sex Abuse 10:25–36. sexual offenders: Differences on the basis of the age and
O’Brien MJ (1991) Taking sibling incest seriously. In MQ Patton gender of their victims. J Abnorm Psychol 104:610 – 613.

You might also like