You are on page 1of 13

912414

research-article2020
LDQXXX10.1177/0731948720912414Learning Disability QuarterlyAllen and Lembke

Original Research
Learning Disability Quarterly

The Effect of a Morphological Awareness


1­–13
© Hammill Institute on Disabilities 2020
Article reuse guidelines:
Intervention on Early Writing Outcomes sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/0731948720912414
https://doi.org/10.1177/0731948720912414
journals.sagepub.com/home/ldq

Abigail A. Allen, PhD CCC-SLP1 and Erica S. Lembke, PhD2

Abstract
The purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of a morphological awareness intervention on the spelling and
writing of second-grade (n = 16) and third-grade (n = 10) students at risk for learning disabilities. The intervention was
provided for 25 min per session 4 to 5 times per week for 5 weeks. Students were randomly assigned to the intervention
(n = 13) or comparison (n = 13) condition. Students were pre- and posttested using standardized tests of spelling and
writing and were progress monitored twice weekly with a curriculum-based measure of writing. Intervention effects were
measured using a univariate analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) on each outcome. Growth on the progress monitoring task
was measured using hierarchical linear modeling (HLM). Results indicated the intervention did not have a significant effect
on outcomes but a moderate positive effect was detected on writing. Practical applications including intervention revisions
and feasibility of use are discussed.

Keywords
writing, instructional strategies, at risk, language arts

The ability to communicate in writing is an essential skill for word’s meaning is contained in its morphological struc-
people in a literate society. However, almost 75% of ture (Carlisle & Fleming, 2003; Perfetti, 2007). The mor-
American students are not proficient on national tests of phological structure of a word can tell a writer the word’s
writing (National Center for Educational Statistics, 2011). acceptable forms and proper usage, and the ability to rec-
Although the performance of students with disabilities was ognize morphological relationships between words gives
not explicitly reported in the 2011 National Assessment of students access to different word choices when writing
Educational Progress (NAEP) writing results, previous text (Bowers et al., 2010; Green et al., 2003; Nagy et al.,
research has suggested students with disabilities and those 2014). Morphological awareness refers to the ability to
who struggle with writing typically produce shorter, less recognize and manipulate the smallest units of meaning in
complex, and lower quality writing compared with typically language (Apel & Werfel, 2014). Although experts ini-
developing peers (Graham et al., 2013). Students who strug- tially suggested that students do not have morphological
gle with writing and literacy during their K–12 education awareness until mid- to late-elementary grades, current
have a lack of opportunities and poorer outcomes in postsec- research has shown that students as young as kindergarten
ondary education and employment (National Commission and first grade have awareness of morphological word
on Writing for America’s Families, Schools, and Colleges, structure and this awareness affects how they use and spell
2004). Writing instruction has not kept up with societal words (Apel & Werfel, 2014; Wolter et al., 2009). For
demands. Researchers have found students write for only 20 example, young students spell words differently depend-
min per day in Grades 1 to 3 and teachers feel inadequately ing on the number of morphemes in the word (Rubin,
prepared to teach writing (Cutler & Graham, 2008). One 1988; Treiman & Cassar, 1996; Wolter et al., 2009). Rubin
way to address this instructional gap is to design effective (1988) found in words with one morpheme and a final
instruction that specifically targets written language skills. consonant blend (e.g., “bind”), students typically left out
At the earliest stages of writing, this instruction is typically one of the final consonants, but in words with multiple
provided in the form of transcription lessons.
1
Clemson University, Clemson, SC, USA
Morphological Contributions to 2
University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA
Spelling and Writing Corresponding Author:
Abigail A. Allen, Department of Education and Human Development,
For writers to find correct and precise words they want to Clemson University, 215 Holtzendorff Hall, Clemson, SC 29634, USA.
use, they must understand what words mean, and part of a Email: aaallen@clemson.edu
2 Learning Disability Quarterly 00(0)

morphemes that are pronounced as a final consonant blend time. Similarly, Coker (2006) found that in first grade,
(e.g., “rained”), students were more likely to write both students with language difficulties produced lower qual-
letters in the word ending. In addition, morphological ity writing initially but oral language did not account for
awareness has uniquely predicted spelling ability in participants’ growth in writing. Empirical work in vali-
Grades 2 to 3 beyond other linguistic variables like ortho- dating the connection between oral and written language
graphic knowledge (Apel et al., 2012). Morphological is ongoing and initial results indicate that oral language
awareness not only affects how students spell words but supports and influences writing quality and productivity,
how they compose written sentences. although not necessarily growth.
Students need to be able to use morphological knowl-
edge to create varied sentence structures (Nagy et al., Working Memory
2014). For example, knowing that teach is a verb but that
attaching -er to the end creates a noun dictates how that Writing is constrained by working memory, or the ability to
word is used within a sentence (“My teacher was absent” hold information in mind (Swanson & Berninger, 1996). To
instead of “My teach was absent”). McCutchen and Stull write successfully, one must put thoughts into words and
(2015) asked fifth graders to combine a series of short sen- sentences while using correct phonological, orthographic,
tences (e.g., “The snake was slow. The snake moved his and syntactic rules (Berninger & Amtmann, 2003).
coils down the tree.”). McCutchen and Stull (2015) noted Researchers found that working memory actually predicts
that students would have to rely on their morphological writing quality and productivity (Kim, Al Otaiba, et al.,
knowledge to accurately combine statements into a longer 2015; Swanson & Berninger, 1996). Kim and Schatschneider
complex sentence using phrases such as “moved slowly” (2017) concluded that while working memory is directly
or “the coiled snake.” They found fifth graders’ morpho- related to individual writing skills (e.g., spelling, grammar),
logical awareness predicted their accurate word produc- when writing skills are combined, working memory is no
tion and sentence combining. Ultimately, morphological longer directly related to writing proficiency. The relation-
knowledge influences word-level affix knowledge, under- ship between working memory and writing may be more
standing grammatical function of words, and how to use indirect than previously thought. Taken together, oral lan-
words in sentences (Nagy et al., 2014). In addition to mor- guage and working memory influence writing proficiency
phological awareness, other factors influence how young for young students and should be considered when design-
students write. ing writing interventions.

Student-Level Factors That Influence Writing of Students With and At Risk


Writing Proficiency for Learning Disabilities (LDs)
Students in early elementary grades are still developing
Oral Language written language skills, and LDs in writing may not mani-
The act of writing is essentially translating spoken lan- fest clearly until later grades when the mismatch between
guage into text. Oral language ability is logically linked higher writing expectations and student performance is
to written language performance, particularly in early evident. However, young students can demonstrate defi-
elementary grades (Coker, 2006; Kim et al., 2011, 2014, cits in skills that are predictive or related to later writing
2018; Kim & Schatschneider, 2017). In typically devel- proficiency. Young writers with or at risk for LD often
oping children, oral language skills like expressive demonstrate weak phonological and orthographic knowl-
vocabulary, grammatical knowledge (Kim et al., 2011, edge, which manifests as poor spelling and handwriting
2014), listening comprehension, and oral narrative ability (Graham et al., 2013). Students may also have deficits in
(Kim et al., 2018) in concert with transcription skills working memory, which, coupled with transcription dif-
(Kim et al., 2011, 2018) are uniquely related to writing ficulty, means they frequently spend their cognitive
ability in Grades K–3. Speech and language impairments resources on the transcription task, leaving fewer
are associated with disorganized, shorter, and lower qual- resources for higher order tasks like organization, sen-
ity writing (Kim, Puranik, & Al Otaiba, 2015). A key tence construction, and revising (Kim et al., 2014). In
finding from Kim, Puranik, and Al Otaiba’s (2015) work addition, students with or at risk for LD often experience
with first graders with speech and language impairments difficulty with morphology of words, which further
was that although students with language impairments affects spelling and connected writing (Apel et al., 2012).
produced more disorganized writing than their peers, Overall, struggling writers typically produce shorter, less
those differences did not account for growth trajectories; complex, and lower quality writing compared with peers
that is, the gap between typically developing students and (Graham et al., 2013). One avenue for remediation is
students with language impairments did not widen over morphological awareness instruction.
Allen and Lembke 3

Differential Effects of Morphological those previously described (Apel et al., 2013; Apel & Diehm,
Interventions on Literacy Outcomes 2014; Brimo, 2016; Devonshire et al., 2013; Good et al.,
2015; Wolter & Dilworth, 2013). The results across morpho-
Five literature reviews (Bowers et al., 2010; Carlisle, 2010; logical awareness studies suggest that (a) morphological
Goodwin & Ahn, 2010, 2013; Reed, 2008) investigated the instruction can positively affect word-level spelling and read-
effect of morphological instruction on literacy outcomes. ing, (b) students in Grades K–2 and struggling learners derive
Studies used instructional strategies like teaching morpho- an even greater benefit from morphological interventions than
logical forms in oral and written words through listening peers, and (c) when measuring writing outcomes, it is critical
comprehension (e.g., orally identifying morphemes in spo- to incorporate instruction and practice opportunities for writ-
ken words and sentences), word discrimination (e.g., identi- ing with morphological targets. Despite the promising results
fying which word has a target affix), and word building cited here, there is a general lack of research conducted in
exercises (e.g., attach prefixes and suffixes to base words). morphological awareness instruction in early elementary
Morphological awareness interventions had a significant grades for students at risk for LD. Less than half the studies in
effect on word spelling (d = .20–.49), word reading (d = the reviews cited above included participants in Grades K–3
.41–.79), oral vocabulary (d = .34–.40), and reading compre- and most were conducted across multiple later grades (e.g.,
hension (d = .24) for students in Grades preK–12 (Bowers Grades 3–7). Further research is needed to investigate the
et al., 2010; Carlisle, 2010; Goodwin & Ahn, 2010, 2013; potential of morphological instruction as a method for improv-
Reed, 2008). Several specific studies are worth noting. Nunes ing the writing of students with or at risk for LD.
et al. (2003) found that students in Grades 2 to 3 demon-
strated gains in spelling only when provided practice oppor-
tunities to write words they learned. In addition, McCutchen Study Purpose
et al. (2013) found that fifth graders in a morphological inter- Morphological awareness interventions can have a positive
vention used significantly more morphologically complex effect on word spelling and reading, but to date, no studies
words when combining sentences (d = 1.21) and when writ- on morphological awareness have included connected writ-
ing extended response essays (d = .89). The only study to ing as an outcome measure in the early elementary grades.
find no effect on spelling or reading outcomes was Parel This study expands on the existing literature by measuring
(2006), who notes that the effectiveness of the intervention the effect of a morphological awareness intervention on
was likely limited due to scheduling constraints. spelling and sentence writing in early elementary grades
Certain subgroups of students appear to derive greater ben- while also measuring oral language and working memory.
efit from morphological awareness instruction. Specifically, We hypothesize that an intervention targeting morphologi-
larger effect sizes were found for students in early elementary cal skills would result in improved spelling accuracy in
grades (preK–2) for skills like identifying morphemes words and sentences, and improved construction of written
(d = .41–2.58), word reading (d = .28–.81), word spelling sentences for students with or at risk for LDs in writing, but
(d = .57–.2.08), and reading comprehension (d = .52–.1.49; effects may be constrained by participants’ oral language
Bowers et al., 2010; Goodwin & Ahn, 2013). Larger effect and working memory. The research questions are the fol-
sizes were also noted for the previously mentioned skills for lowing: (a) What are the effects of a morphological aware-
students classified as low or struggling readers (d = .35–.99), ness intervention on spelling and writing outcomes for
having a LD in reading (d = .17–.37), or a language impair- students with or at risk for LD in writing? and (b) What
ment (d = .77; Bowers et al., 2010; Goodwin & Ahn, student-level variables predict growth on spelling and writ-
2010, 2013; Reed, 2008). Interventions included explicit ing outcome measures?
instruction in identifying base words and affixes, rules for This study is an exploratory, underpowered randomized
combining morphemes into words, and morphosyntactic control trial. Because writing is an understudied area and
spelling rules in activities like identifying affixes in spo- educators generally feel unprepared to teach writing (Cutler
ken and written words, orally “chunking” multisyllabic & Graham, 2008), it is necessary to investigate applications
words containing affixes, and building new words using and future directions of writing intervention research. The
target affixes orally and in writing. Sessions were usually effect sizes and implications for intervention feasibility
25 to 45 min in length for 9 to 20 weeks. It appears from this study will be discussed in depth, regardless of the
younger and poorer performing readers gain more from statistical significance of the findings.
explicit morphological instruction than their older and
higher achieving peers.
Recent studies echo the results of these literature reviews. Method
Morphological intervention positively impacted morpheme
Participants and Setting
knowledge (d = .41–2.58), word spelling (d = .65–2.08),
word reading (d = .21–.81), and reading comprehension The study was conducted in one elementary school serv-
(d = .52–.1.49) in Grades K–3 using activities similar to ing Grades 2 to 5 in a Midwestern district in a small city.
4 Learning Disability Quarterly 00(0)

The school was chosen as a convenience sample. During Study Design


the 2014–2015 academic year, the school reported enroll-
ment as 417 students, 93% White, 38% eligible for free/ We conducted a randomized control trial and randomly
reduced lunch, and 8% receiving special education ser- assigned participants to intervention (n = 13) and com-
vices (state average special education rate 12.7%). parison conditions (n = 13), blocking on grade level.
Participants were second-grade (n = 17) and third-grade Participants were assessed pre- and postintervention and
(n = 10) students. One second-grade student in the inter- were administered twice-weekly progress monitoring
vention condition moved away before posttesting was CBM-W assessments.
complete, for a final count of 26 students (attrition rate
4%). The final participant sample was 46% female, 89%
Measures
White, 7% Latino, 50% free/reduced lunch eligible, and
19% received special education services. For students Screening and progress monitoring CBM-W.  Each CBM-W
receiving special education, diagnoses were 60% lan- consisted of 12 pictures of common objects and actions
guage impairment, 20% mild intellectual disability, and paired with written words (e.g., “paper,” “walk,”). During
20% other health impairment. No students were desig- administration, the first author read each CBM-W item
nated as English learners. aloud while students followed along. Students were
Although no participants were officially diagnosed instructed to “Write one sentence for each picture in your
with a LD, they all demonstrated difficulties in written packet” and complete as many sentences as they could in
language (see screening procedures below) and could rea- 3 min. For screening, one form of the CBM-W was group-
sonably be considered “at risk” for a LD in writing. In administered to students in their classrooms 4 weeks pre-
addition, participants were relatively young (Grades 2–3) intervention. For progress monitoring, students were
and LD, particularly in writing, is often diagnosed in later given a different CBM-W form twice a week for 5 weeks.
grades when writing expectations are higher and difficul- Participants were given one final CBM-W 4 weeks pos-
ties become more obvious (Coker et al., 2017). Finally, tintervention for a total of 12 data points. All CBM-W
the school as a whole had a relatively low rate of special forms were given by the first author. Probes were scored
education diagnoses, so it was challenging to find low with correct word sequences (CWS, two adjacent words
performing young students with the LD label. Given the spelled and used correctly), which demonstrate evidence
exploratory nature of the study, there are still important of reliability (r ≥ .70) and criterion validity (r ≥ .60) in
applications to be examined for use with students with or Grades 2 to 3 (Lembke et al., 2003; Videen et al., 1982).
at risk for LD in writing. Picture-word demonstrates evidence of sensitivity to
Participants were identified using a two-gate screening growth within (Parker et al., 2011) and across elementary
system. All students in Grades 2 to 3 (N = 224; Grade 2: grades (McMaster et al., 2011). Total and mean CWS
117, Grade 3: 107) across 11 classrooms were first screened were used to score all CBM-W. Mean CWS allows for
using a sentence writing picture-word Curriculum-Based fairer assessment of slow writers as productivity is not a
Measurement task (CBM-W). Students who fell below the factor (Lembke et al., 2003).
25th percentile using norms developed from a previous
study (Allen et al., 2019) were identified for the second Spelling and writing. All students were given the Spelling
round of screening. Procedures for norm development are and Writing Samples subtests of the WJ-III ACH (Wood-
available upon request. cock et al., 2007). The Spelling subtest evaluated students’
Students who met the first round of screening criteria ability to spell single words from dictation. The Writing
with CBM-W (n = 74) were given the second screener, Samples subtest assessed students’ ability to write in
the Spelling and Writing Samples subtests of the response to a picture or verbal prompt. Test items start with
Woodcock–Johnson Test of Achievement, Third Edition one-word responses and progress to multisentence
(WJ-III ACH; Woodcock et al., 2007). Students who also responses. Both subtests were given by the first author 4
fell below the 25th percentile on either WJ-III ACH sub- weeks pre- and postintervention. The subtests demonstrated
test using age norms were eligible to participate in the evidence of sufficient reliability (Spelling subtest: r = .87,
study (n = 26 after attrition). Participants in the study Writing Samples subtest: r = .90) and criterion-related
were then given the Digit Span and Letter-Number validity with standardized writing tests (r = .57–.77; Wood-
Sequencing subtests of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for cock et al., 2007) and with CBM-W (r ≥ .60; Ritchey &
Children, Fourth Edition (WISC-IV; Weschler, 2003) as a Coker, 2014). On the Spelling subtest, total and mean cor-
working memory covariate measure, and the Following rect letter sequences (CLSs) was calculated (adjacent letters
Directions subtest of the Clinical Evaluation of Language correctly placed in a word, Deno et al., 1980) to more pre-
Fundamentals, Fifth Edition (CELF-V; Semel et al., 2013) cisely measure the effect of the intervention on student
as a language covariate measure. spelling.
Allen and Lembke 5

Working memory. The Digit Span and Letter and Number about what practices they used during writing instruction
Sequencing subtests of the WISC-IV (Weschler, 2003) were (adapted from Cutler & Graham, 2008). Teachers com-
used to measure working memory (reliability: .89, criterion pleted 19 items about time spent on writing and spelling
validity: .72). The Digit Span subtest required students to instruction, in various student groupings (e.g., whole
repeat sequences of numbers forward and backward. The group, small group) and frequency of use of evidence-
Letter and Number Sequencing subtest required students to based practices (e.g., graphic organizers and student goal-
repeat a mixed series of letters and numbers (e.g., 1-A-B). setting). Four open-ended questions asked teachers to
The WISC-IV subtests were administered to participants by describe their approach to teaching writing. Although there
the first author and two trained doctoral students 2 weeks were no questions asked about “morphological awareness”
before the intervention. The doctoral students were trained activities specifically, which is a weakness of the study,
in test administration and scoring by the first author, a questions were asked about practices related to the study
speech–language pathologist with experience with both intervention (e.g., teaching word families and prefixes/suf-
assessments. The doctoral students were blind to study con- fixes and sentence writing). Future studies should incorpo-
ditions of the participants and reached 100% fidelity of rate questionnaires with more explicit language around
administration on 20% of sessions as measured by a 20-item morphological awareness and specific morphological
observational checklist (available upon request). activities and teaching strategies.
All participating teachers delivered their regular class-
Receptive language.  The Following Directions subtest of the room reading and writing instruction to all student partici-
CELF-V (Semel et al., 2013) measures receptive semantic pants during the study. Students in the intervention condition
and syntactic abilities (reliability: .83–.90; criterion valid- received the study intervention during a 25-min school-
ity: .78–.91). It was given as a covariate measure to account wide intervention block delivered by the research team.
for maturational growth in language skills not explicitly Students in the comparison condition received a supple-
taught during the intervention to more precisely capture mental intervention in reading or mathematics during the
intervention effects. Students should demonstrate some same school-wide intervention block delivered by school
natural language growth in semantics and syntax, but not staff. We were unable to ascertain what supplemental inter-
significantly so and not as a result of the intervention (Apel ventions were being delivered to the comparison group,
et al., 2013). The Following Directions subtest required stu- which is a weakness of the study. Because all participants,
dents to listen to oral directions and point to pictures in a regardless of condition, continued to receive their regular
specific order. We chose to not directly assess oral morpho- classroom reading and writing instruction from their class-
logical skills given that the focus of the study was to deter- room teachers throughout the duration of the study, the
mine intervention effects on written language outcomes. teacher questionnaire was used to summarize writing
The CELF-V subtest was administered 2 weeks before and instruction in participating classrooms and to detect any dif-
4 weeks after the intervention by the first author and two- ferences in instruction across experimental groups or grade
trained doctoral students. The doctoral students reached levels that may have affected results.
100% fidelity of administration on 20% of sessions as mea-
sured by a 20-item observational checklist (available upon
request).
Intervention Description and Procedures
The intervention was provided by the first author and seven-
Inter-rater reliability (IRR) of scoring.  IRR of scoring was con- trained graduate students in special education 4 to 5 times
ducted on 20% of each assessment with a goal of at least per week during the school-wide intervention block in
90% agreement across scorers. WJ-III ACH and CBM-W groups of two to five students for 25 min per session for 5
were originally scored by the first author. IRR was con- weeks. Due to school scheduling constraints, Grade 2 stu-
ducted with an expert scorer who used WJ-III ACH in her dents received the intervention 5 times per week and Grade
work as a school psychologist and CBM-W as a project 3 students received it 4 times per week. However, because
coordinator for the larger screening study from which the of unexpected school cancelations, groups differed only by
CBM-W forms were drawn (Allen et al., 2019). The expert one total session. Any student who was absent did not
scorer was blind to study conditions of participants. IRR receive makeup instruction. Average attendance across stu-
reached 100% on the CBM-W and WJ-III ACH Spelling, dents was 93%.
and 85% on WJ-III ACH Writing Samples. First author The intervention was delivered in two groups during
retrained on scoring Writing Samples with the expert scorer each grade level’s intervention block and participants were
and reached 90%. randomly assigned to their instructional group. Third grade
(n = 5) had a group of two and a group of three while sec-
Teacher questionnaire.  All participating classroom teachers in ond grade (originally n = 9) had a group of five and a group
both conditions (n = 9) completed a 23-item questionnaire of four. The two-group structure was used in both grades for
6 Learning Disability Quarterly 00(0)

consistency and is a potential limitation of the study. To instruction. To detect any significant differences in class-
account for teacher effects, all lesson plans were scripted room writing instruction between experimental groups, a
and instructors alternated groups to prevent students from series of independent samples t-tests were used with a
having the same instructor each day. For logistical reasons, Bonferroni correction (α = .05/19 = .003) on the 19
student groups remained the same throughout the interven- numerical questionnaire items. No statistically significant
tion. One lesson target (e.g., plural “-s”) was taught over 2 differences were found. All teachers reported using both a
days, with a review session after every two lessons. “process-based” approach (e.g., students guided through
planning, writing, and editing) and a “skills-based”
Intervention activities. Each session included four activities approach (e.g., students taught discrete skills). All teachers
(Apel & Diehm, 2014): (a) listening activity, (b) word sort, (c) reported teaching spelling using word families or letter–
“say it another way,” and (d) affix writing book. The listening sound correspondence. Students in both groups received an
activity required students to give a “thumbs up” when they average of 30 min per day of writing instruction. Teachers
heard the target affix read aloud in a short story. In word sort, reported using “mini-lessons” on specific skills, teacher–
students were presented with a word card and were told to student conferences, and providing writing models daily to
“say it, spell it, say it again” and sort the cards according to weekly. Other instructional practices such as peer confer-
whether the word had the target affix. Words were paired with ences and graphic organizers were used monthly or less
pictures only during Lessons 1 to 3. During “say it another often. Full questionnaire results are available upon request.
way,” instructors presented a written base word and asked the
question, “How can I say [BLANK] another way?” Students Data Analysis
orally responded with a modified word using the target affix
(e.g., “dogs”) and wrote the affix next to the base word. The All data were entered into SPSS v. 24.0 and hierarchical
affix book was a cumulative booklet where students circled linear model(ing) (HLM) 7 for analyses. Descriptive statis-
words containing the target affix in a list, orally defined the tics (mean, SD, range, skewness, and kurtosis) were calcu-
words, and wrote sentences with the words. Instructors pro- lated for all measures. Age-normed standardized scores
vided students with feedback and error correction through were used for all norm-referenced tests. To answer the first
verbal prompts to spell and use target words correctly. research question, what are the effects of a morphological
awareness intervention, a series of univariate analyses of
Order of lessons. The order of affixes taught followed the covariance (ANCOVAs) was conducted to determine
developmental progression of morpheme acquisition and between-group differences on the outcome measures.
included derivational and inflectional morphemes (Apel & Covariate measures were CELF-V Following Directions
Diehm, 2014). Affixes were taught in spoken and written subtest and WISC-IV Working Memory Index. Given the
forms in the following order: plural -s, third person -s, past number of individual comparisons being made a Bonferroni
tense -ed, present progressive -ing, prefix un-, prefix re-, correction was used to account for Type I error (.05/6 =
suffix -er (person), suffix -ly, and suffix -er/-est (compari- .008). Effect sizes (Hedges’ g; M1–M2/SDpooled) were calcu-
son). Base words were taken from the Dolch (1936) word lated to determine the magnitude of effect of the interven-
lists. None of the words used on the assessments were tion and were interpreted using Cohen’s (1988) standards
directly taught during the intervention. (small 0.2, medium 0.5, large .80).
To answer the second research question, what student-
Fidelity of intervention implementation.  Fidelity of implemen- level variables predict growth, an HLM growth curve analysis
tation was recorded through direct observation with a was used to determine if students’ rate of growth on CBM-W
20-item checklist (e.g., modeling skills, providing immedi- was significantly different from zero and what student-level
ate corrective feedback) by the authors. Intervention ses- variables (e.g., working memory, receptive language) may
sions (22%) were observed for fidelity and instructors have shaped growth rates. HLM is a method used in previous
achieved a mean 97% fidelity (range 89%–100%). research when investigating CBM-W slopes (McMaster et al.,
2011). Test data observations (Level 1) were nested within
students (Level 2) and were collected biweekly during the
Comparison Group Instruction duration of the intervention. Two models were used to inves-
Students in the comparison group were receiving supple- tigate growth and differences between students.
mental interventions in either reading or mathematics dur-
ing the school-wide intervention block based on their Results
individual needs identified by the school. Results from the
teacher questionnaire about classroom instruction indicated
Descriptive Data
there was no published writing curriculum being followed in Descriptive statistics are reported in Table 1. Visual
either grade or experimental group during regular literacy inspection of the data and skewness and kurtosis values
Allen and Lembke 7

Table 1.  Descriptive Statistics for Outcome and Covariate Measures.

Intervention (n = 13) Comparison (n = 13)


Pretest Posttest Pretest Posttest

Measure M (SD) Range M (SD) Range M (SD) Range M (SD) Range


WJ-III ACH Spelling 78.8 (7.0) 65–89 83.5 (6.8) 70–92 85.2 (6.0) 76–97 87.5 (6.1) 77–100
Total CLS 61.5 (20.4) 27–91 72.6 (21.0) 45–107 71.3 (23.6) 33–119 81.2 (17.9) 46–105
Mean CLS 4.0 (0.5) 2.8–4.6 4.0 (0.4) 3.3–4.4 3.5 (0.4) 2.9–4.3 3.7 (0.4) 3–4.3
WJ-III ACH Writing 94.5 (11.7) 74–118 94.2 (14.0) 63–114 92.5 (7.0) 77–102 87.6 (11.4) 66–109
CBM-W Total CWS 17.2 (7.6) 2–31 25.2 (12.1) 4–43 15.5 (6.8) 4–30 27.5 (10.7) 12–47
Mean CWS 3.5 (1.4) 1.0–5.5 3.5 (1.2) 1.3–6 4.1 (1.2) 2.2–6.3 4.1 (1.1) 2.3–6
CELF-V 8.0 (2.1) 4–11 8.5 (3.9) 2–15 8.5 (2.5) 4–11 8.7 (2.7) 3–12
WISC-IV 84.3 (13.1) 62–110 n/a n/a 85.2 (11.3) 65–104 n/a n/a

Note. Age-normed standardized scores were used for all standardized tests (WJ-III ACH, CELF-V, and WISC-IV). WJ-III ACH = Woodcock–Johnson
Tests of Achievement, Third Edition; CLS = correct letter sequences; CBM-W = curriculum-based measurement in writing; CWS = correct word
sequences; CELF-V = Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, Fifth Edition; WISC-IV = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition
(Working Memory Index).

indicated the data were approximately normally distrib- large-positive effects found on the WJ-III ACH Spelling
uted. Collinearity of the outcome and covariate measures subtest in mean CLS (g = .75) and on the WJ-III ACH
was calculated with Pearson product–moment correla- Writing Samples subtest (g = .52). Negative effect sizes
tion. Almost all coefficients were significant (p < .05) were found for the WJ-III ACH Spelling subtest total score
and were small to moderate (p = .11–.65). Due to space (g = −.60), total CLS score (g = −0.44), and CBM-W total
constraints, skewness, kurtosis, and collinearity values CWS (g = −0.20) and mean CWS (g = −0.52). These nega-
are available upon request. tive effects indicate that the comparison group outscored the
At pretest, the groups were statistically significantly dif- intervention group at posttest on these measures.
ferent on WJ-III ACH Spelling t(24) = 2.47, p = .02; there-
fore pretest WJ-III ACH Spelling total scores were used as
HLM Growth Modeling
a covariate in the ANCOVAs along with language and
memory scores. Groups were not statistically significantly Unconditional model. All HLM results are in Table 3. An
different on any other measure. Students with special edu- unconditional growth model was analyzed first with time
cation diagnoses were distributed equally across interven- (testing occasion) as the only variable at Level-1 and CWS
tion and comparison groups through random assignment. pretest score as the outcome variable. Deviance testing indi-
Both groups increased their mean scores from pre- to post- cated that a random slopes model was more appropriate
test on every measure except the WJ-III ACH Writing than a fixed slopes model (χ2 = 8.01, df = 2, p = .001). The
Samples subtest, where the comparison group decreased linear slope model resulted in a better fit over an intercept-
from pre to post (see Table 1). Neither group grew signifi- only model, χ2 = 7.30 (2), p = .02. Next, a quadratic term
cantly on the CELF-V from pre- to posttest, intervention: was added to the model to see if any growth was nonlinear.
t(12) = −.49, p = .64; comparison: t(13) = −.30, p = .77. Deviance testing indicated that random slopes were more
appropriate than fixed slopes for this model as well (χ2 =
22.50, df = 5, p = .001). The addition of a quadratic term
Intervention Effects resulted in a better fit over the linear slope model (χ2 =
A series of ANCOVAs was used with a Bonferroni correc- 22.50, df = 5, p = .001). Because the quadratic term was
tion (.05/6 = .008) to determine if the intervention had an significant, it was retained in the model. Students varied
effect on each of the six spelling and writing outcome mea- significantly in their total CWS pretest scores (intercepts)
sures for students in the intervention condition (see Table 2). before the intervention (r0 = 53.50, p = .002). Linear slope
Levene’s test of equality of variances was not significant for (2.96 CWS per testing occasion or 5.92 CWS per week; p =
any of the measures, indicating the assumption of equality of .001) and nonlinear (quadratic) acceleration rate (−.17 CWS
variances was met (p < .05). In short, the intervention did per testing occasion or −.34 CWS per week; p = .002) were
not have a significant effect on any of the outcome measures significantly different from zero.
after controlling for baseline receptive language, working
memory, and spelling ability (see Table 2; all p > .05). Conditional model. Several variables were added to the
Although not statistically significant, there were medium- to model as Level-2 predictors: intervention group (coded as
8 Learning Disability Quarterly 00(0)

Table 2.  ANCOVA Results.

Levene’s testa
Type III
Measure (Posttest) F p Sum of Squares F p Hedges’ g
WJ-III ACH Spelling 1.80 .19 0.04 0.002 .97 −0.60
Total CLS 4.06 .06 4.00 0.02 .90 −0.44
Mean CLS 0.50 .49 0.18 1.10 .31 0.75
WJ-III ACH Writing Samples 1.82 .19 568.92 4.31 .05 0.52
CBM-W Total CWS 0.40 .53 7.76 0.07 .80 −0.20
Mean CWS 0.49 .49 0.39 0.29 .60 −0.52

Note. WJ-III ACH = Woodcock–Johnson Tests of Achievement, Third Edition; CLS = correct letter sequences; CBM-W = curriculum-based
measurement in writing; CWS = correct word sequences.
a
df1 = 1, df2 = 24.

Table 3.  HLM Growth Curve Analysis Results.

Coefficients

Variables Unconditional Conditional


Fixed Effects
  Intercept (π0)
   Mean Pretest CWS (β00) 16.67*** 11.56***
  Mean Pretest × Grade (β01) — 13.78***
  Linear Slope (π1)
   Mean Linear Growth Rate (β10) 2.83*** 2.83***
   Mean Growth Rate × Pretest WJ-III ACH Spelling (β11) — 0.20***
  Nonlinear Growth (π2)
   Mean Quadratic Growth Rate (β20) −0.16** −0.14**
   Mean Growth Rate × Grade (β21) — −0.06**
  Mean Growth × Pretest WJ-III ACH Spelling (β22) — −0.01**
Random Effects
  Mean Pretest CWS Error (r0i) 55.73** 3.32
  Mean Linear Slope Error (r1i) 5.33** 1.88*
  Mean Non-linear Growth Error (r2i) 0.03* 0.14*
Proportion of Variance
  Level 1 Within Student (R12) — .003
  Level 2 Between Student (R22) — .80

Note. HLM = hierarchical linear modeling; CWS = correct word sequences; WJ-III ACH = Woodcock–Johnson Tests of Achievement, Third Edition.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

0 = comparison group, 1 = intervention group), working Level-2 model


memory score, oral language pretest score, WJ-III ACH (between students): π0i = β00 + β01*(GRADEi) + r0i
Spelling and Writing Samples pretest scores, and grade    π1i = β10 + β11*(PREWJSi) + r1i
level (coded as 0 = second grade, 1 = third grade). Because     π2i = β20 + β21*(GRADEi) +
the study sample was rather homogeneous, demographic β22*(PREWJSi) + r2i
variables were not used as predictors. Each predictor that
was not significant was removed from the model one at a Participation in the intervention did not predict growth
time based on which had the highest p-value. The final on the CBM-W task, which also reflects the ANCOVA
conditional model was as follows: results. The intervention did not have a significant effect on
participant performance. Rather, linear slopes were signifi-
Level-1 model cantly predicted by pretest WJ-III ACH Spelling subtest
CBMti = π0i + π1i*(TIMEti) +
(within-student):  score and nonlinear acceleration rate was predicted by
π2i*(TIME2ti) + eti grade-level and pretest Spelling. Younger students and
Allen and Lembke 9

poorer spellers initially scored lower but grew faster than spelling and identification of morphological spelling rules
their peers in the study. For linear slope, a one-unit increase and strategies, they did not always spontaneously utilize or
in Spelling score was associated with a .38 increase in CWS transfer those strategies to other types of words (Devonshire
per week (p = .02). For nonlinear growth, one-unit increases et al., 2013; Wolter & Dilworth, 2013).
in grade and Spelling score resulted in statistically signifi-
cant although negligible increases in CWS (see Table 3).
Writing Performance
The HLM results do not provide any further support for the
effectiveness of the intervention. Although previous work in the area of morphological
instruction and writing is scant, the lack of significant
growth on the writing measures in this study is in conflict
Discussion with one previous study that found positive effects of mor-
The purpose of this study was to determine the effective- phological awareness instruction on sentence writing
ness of a morphological awareness intervention on the writ- (McCutchen et al., 2013). Although this study attempted to
ing outcomes of students in Grades 2 to 3 at risk for LD in reflect McCutchen et al.’s findings by using sentence writ-
writing. First, findings are discussed in terms of partici- ing practice with the morphological targets in each lesson,
pants’ spelling and writing performance, then the growth the intervention may not have been explicit or powerful
patterns evident in the CBM-W progress monitoring data, enough to obtain measurable effects in 5 weeks. The writing
then practical applications, limitations of the study, and instruction portion of the intervention likely needed to be
future directions. the focus of each lesson to produce detectable performance
gains on the writing measures. A moderate positive effect
was found on the Writing Samples subtest (g = .52), how-
Spelling Performance ever, similar to the Spelling subtest, this is likely because
Although previous research has illustrated that morphologi- the comparison students decreased from pre- to posttest
cal awareness instruction can produce large, positive gains rather than the intervention having a significant effect on
in spelling (Apel et al., 2013; Apel & Diehm, 2014; the performance of the experimental students. In addition,
Devonshire et al., 2013; Wolter & Dilworth, 2013), this the McCutchen et al. (2013) study was with fifth graders; it
study did not detect any significant between-group differ- may be necessary when working with younger at-risk stu-
ences on the WJ-III ACH Spelling subtest, even after con- dents who are still developing basic writing skills to spend
trolling for pretest Spelling scores. Although the effects on more time on the sentence writing aspect rather than the
total score and total CLS were negative, a moderate positive activities devoted to identifying morphological targets in
effect was found for mean CLS (g = .75). Upon closer oral and written language.
inspection, this is likely because the comparison group The lack of significant growth on the WJ-III ACH
decreased in mean CLS from pre- to posttest rather than the Writing Samples subtest could also be due to the way the
intervention having a large effect on the experimental subtest was administered and scored. The Writing Samples
group’s spelling. It is possible that any potential gains in subtest did not reward or penalize spelling, capitalization,
spelling from the intervention were attenuated when con- or punctuation on most of the test items, so even if stu-
trolling for working memory and receptive language in the dents improved in these skills as a result of the interven-
ANCOVA. Previous research has shown that spelling and tion, this would not be reflected in their Writing Samples
writing ability is influenced by oral language (Kim et al., scores. In addition, the Writing Samples subtest required
2011, 2014, 2018) and working memory, either directly or students to write to a specific prompt or idea, sometimes
indirectly (Kim & Schatschneider, 2017). No previous mor- with multiple sentences, whereas the intervention activi-
phological intervention studies measured working memory ties required students use a target word while writing sin-
and oral language together; had previous studies controlled gle sentences about a topic of their choice, so it could be
for either of these constructs, the impact of the interventions that the Writing Samples tasks were more difficult than or
on spelling could have been smaller. not aligned closely enough with the intervention activities
Another potential influence on the lack of significant and therefore student growth was not detected. Similar to
results is the distal nature of the outcome measure. The the Spelling subtest, maybe the Writing Samples subtest is
WJ-III ACH Spelling subtest may not have been sensitive not an adequate measure to capture increased morphologi-
enough to detect growth in morphological awareness specifi- cal knowledge and does not provide adequate opportuni-
cally. Even if students improved their morphological spelling ties for students to apply their morphological knowledge
knowledge, they may not have been able to transfer that to writing.
knowledge in a way that was detected by the WJ-III ACH It is important to acknowledge the lack of information
Spelling subtest or the CLS scoring metric. Other researchers we have about the supplemental intervention the compari-
have found that even when participants improved their son group students were receiving during the school-wide
10 Learning Disability Quarterly 00(0)

intervention block. On the surface, an unknown interven- of language to help students apply their morphological
tion in literacy for the comparison group could possibly knowledge to their writing (Devonshire et al., 2013;
influence the lack of significant results. However, when McCutchen et al., 2013). To reflect previous morphologi-
looking at the descriptive data, the comparison group’s cal awareness interventions, we devoted time in our les-
mean score on the WJ-III ACH Writing Samples subtest sons to morpheme identification, discrimination, and word
decreased from pre- to posttest, so it is unlikely the com- building to support spelling, and sentence writing practice
parison students were receiving a highly effective writing to support writing, but sentence construction likely needed
intervention during the study. to be taught more explicitly. The interventionists, as part of
the lesson scripts, provided models for sentences that used
the target words from each lesson, but explicit instruction
Growth in Writing
in syntax, sentence types, and building sentence complex-
As indicated by our previous analyses, the intervention did ity was not a focus. Particularly for young at-risk students,
not influence student growth on the CBM-W task. Clearly, the process for how to construct a sentence from scratch is
if the intervention did not affect static performance at post- important and was probably not adequately addressed in
test, it will not affect growth either. However, there are the current intervention. Although modeling writing is an
aspects of the modeling results worth exploring. The HLM important evidence-based practice (Cutler & Graham,
analysis indicated that students did grow on CBM-W over 2008), a more direct approach to sentence writing could
the 12 progress monitoring data points, but only due to a have produced more significant results.
combination of their grade (or rather, their maturity and In addition, longer sessions may be needed to adequately
school experience) and baseline spelling ability, which address both spelling and sentence writing, or perhaps
makes sense as we would expect students with more school improved spelling and sentence writing should be treated as
experience and better spelling to perform higher initially, two separate goals within future iterations. The assumption
and younger and poorer performing students have more in this study was that morphological instruction would
room to grow. Interestingly, working memory and oral lan- improve word spelling (e.g., Nunes et al., 2003). We also
guage were not related to the growth rates of the sample, assumed that students would transfer that improved spelling
which is in conflict with some previous studies showing into their sentences and that the sentence writing practice
connections between early writing, oral language (Kim would boost their overall writing performance (e.g.,
et al., 2018), and working memory (Kim & Schatschneider, McCutchen et al., 2013). It could be that in a 25-min ses-
2017), but reflects past work showing that language ability sion, there is not enough time to devote to each goal.
did not influence writing growth rates (Coker, 2006; Kim, Previous morphological intervention studies were delivered
Al Otaiba, et al., 2015). The conflicting results could be due for at least 9 weeks in 25- to 45-min sessions (e.g., Apel
to the fact that previous studies measured expressive oral et al., 2013; Bowers et al., 2010; Goodwin & Ahn, 2013), so
vocabulary and grammatical knowledge (Kim et al., 2018), longer sessions may be needed to address both spelling
whereas this study only measured receptive oral semantic words and writing sentences with multimorphemic words.
and syntactic knowledge. Alternatively, perhaps two versions of the intervention
In terms of working memory, Kim and Schatschneider could be created, with one focusing on word spelling as a
(2017) concluded that after accounting for oral language foundational piece and a more advanced version focusing
and transcription skills, working memory had a more indi- on sentence writing.
rect relationship to writing proficiency than previously Although not directly assessed in this study, the inter-
thought. Perhaps we did not find working memory to have vention appeared to be feasible to implement. The trained
an impact on growth rates because either working memory graduate student instructors achieved high fidelity of imple-
may not directly affect writing growth or including a lan- mentation after a brief training. It is likely that other educa-
guage measure attenuates the influence of working memory tors would be able to incorporate the intervention activities,
on writing growth. Although the modeling results in this or the lessons in their entirety, into their classroom instruc-
study do not provide evidence of the effectiveness of the tion, particularly if the lessons are scripted. We know that
intervention, they contribute to our knowledge of how teachers often feel underprepared to teach writing and often
spelling, writing, and maturity may intersect. do not spend much time during the school day addressing
writing (Cutler & Graham, 2008), so a scripted and struc-
tured set of lessons may be a welcome addition to many
Practical Applications
elementary classrooms. The interventions can also be
As stated previously, the lack of significant results could be adapted and modified to meet the needs of various types of
due to the way the intervention was designed and deliv- students; for example, specific targets could be eliminated
ered. Previous research has shown it is important to extend or added to the lesson structure, and activities could be sim-
word-level morphological instruction to the sentence level plified (e.g., fewer items, more modeling) or enhanced
Allen and Lembke 11

(e.g., paragraph writing, more challenging targets) for stu- writing. Students who struggle with written language early
dents at different ability levels. on have the greatest potential for growth and need to start
receiving interventions as soon as possible.
Limitations and Future Directions
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The most notable limitation of this study was the small,
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
homogeneous sample (n = 26) and the short intervention to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
duration (5 weeks) with varying group sizes (two to five
students each). The participating school requested that the
Funding
intervention be shortened to 5 weeks in order for the study
to be completed before the state high-stakes testing win- The author(s) received no financial support for the research,
dow opened. In addition, the school intervention blocks authorship, and/or publication of this article.
were scheduled differently between the two grade levels,
which necessitated varying group sizes. It is possible that ORCID iD
a larger, more diverse sample and a longer duration and a Abigail A. Allen https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5135-3709
more consistent schedule and groupings would produce a
larger effect on spelling and writing outcomes (Parel, References
2006). Another limitation was the alignment of the mea- Allen, A. A., Jung, P.-G., Poch, A. L., Brandes, D., Shin, J.,
sures to the intervention tasks. Although using standard- Lembke, E. S., & McMaster, K. M. (2019). Technical ade-
ized assessments like the WJ-III ACH is important for quacy of curriculum-based measures in writing in grades
having a distal outcome measure with evidence of reliabil- 1–3. Reading & Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning
ity and validity, having researcher created assessments Difficulties. Advance online publication. doi:10.1080/10573
that more precisely measure morphological knowledge in 569.2019.1689211
spelling and writing tasks is important to pinpoint where Apel, K., Brimo, D., Diehm, E., & Apel, L. (2013). Morphological
gains did or did not occur and to further refine the inter- awareness intervention with kindergartners and first- and sec-
ond-grade students from low socioeconomic status homes: A
vention. Future iterations of the intervention might need to
feasibility study. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in
spend more time on morphological instruction paired with Schools, 44(2), 161–173.
strategies for sentence construction using multimorphe- Apel, K., & Diehm, E. (2014). Morphological awareness inter-
mic words and less time spent on the identification and vention with kindergarteners and first and second grade stu-
discrimination portions of the lessons. Future iterations dents from low SES homes: A small efficacy study. Journal
could also incorporate teacher feedback and input into the of Learning Disabilities, 47, 65–75.
intervention design to better address the needs of educa- Apel, K., & Werfel, K. (2014). Using morphological aware-
tors and students with or at risk for LD in writing. Finally, ness instruction to improve written language skills.
the lack of specific information on the supplemental read- Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 45(4),
ing or mathematics interventions the comparison students 251–260.
received during the school-wide intervention block is a Apel, K., Wilson-Fowler, E. B., Brimo, D., & Perrin, N. A. (2012).
Metalinguistic contributions to reading and spelling in second
limitation. Although unlikely, it is possible that compari-
and third grade students. Reading and Writing, 25(6), 1283–
son students received some literacy-based intervention 1305. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9317-8
during the study that improved their overall writing Berninger, V., & Amtmann, D. (2003). Preventing written expres-
performance. sion disabilities through early and continuing assessment
and intervention for handwriting and/or spelling problems:
Research into practice. In H. L. Swanson, K. Harris, & S.
Conclusion Graham (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 345–
There is a critical need to provide writing interventions to 363). Guilford.
struggling students, especially early in a child’s academic Bowers, P. N., Kirby, J. R., & Deacon, S. H. (2010).
career to prevent later failure. This study adds to the grow- The effects of morphological instruction on literacy
ing body of evidence that morphological awareness plays a skills: A systematic review of the literature. Review
of Educational Research, 80(2), 144–179. https://doi
role in students’ ability to understand, spell, and use words
.org/10.3102/0034654309359353
correctly in writing and that explicitly teaching morpho- Brimo, D. (2016). Evaluating the effectiveness of a morphologi-
logical awareness can produce gains in spelling and writing. cal awareness intervention: A pilot study. Communication
Results indicated that teaching the morphological structure Disorders Quarterly, 38(1), 35–45.
of words alone is not enough to transfer to improved sen- Carlisle, J. F. (2010). Effects of instruction in morphological
tence writing; instruction and practice with sentence con- awareness on literacy achievement: An integrative review.
struction is a necessary component of improving student Reading Research Quarterly, 45(4), 464–487.
12 Learning Disability Quarterly 00(0)

Carlisle, J. F., & Fleming, J. (2003). Lexical processing of morpho- Kim, Y.-S., Al Otaiba, S., Wanzek, J., & Gatlin, B. (2015).
logically complex words in the elementary years. Scientific Toward an understanding of dimensions, predictors, and the
Studies of Reading, 7(3), 239–253. gender gap in written composition. Journal of Educational
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural Psychology, 107(1), 79–95.
sciences (2nd ed.). Academic Press. Kim, Y.-S., Gatlin, B., Al Otaiba, S., & Wanzek, J. (2018).
Coker, D. L. (2006). Impact of first-grade factors on the Theorization and an empirical investigation of the compo-
growth and outcomes of urban schoolchildren’s primary- nent-based and developmental text writing fluency construct.
grade writing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(3), Journal of Learning Disabilities, 51(4), 320–335.
471–488. Kim, Y.-S., Puranik, C., & Al Otaiba, S. (2015). Developmental
Coker, D. L., Ritchey, K. D., Uribe-Zarain, X., & Jennings, A. S. trajectories in writing skills in first grade: Examining the
(2017). An analysis of first-grade writing profiles and their effects of SES and language and/or speech impairments. The
relationship to compositional quality. Journal of Learning Elementary School Journal, 115(4), 593–613.
Disabilities, 51(4), 336–350. Kim, Y.-S., & Schatschneider, C. (2017). Expanding the develop-
Cutler, L., & Graham, S. (2008). Primary grade writing instruc- mental models of writing: A direct and indirect effects model
tion: A national survey. Journal of Educational Psychology, of developmental writing (DIEW). Journal of Educational
100(4), 907–919. Psychology, 109(1), 35–50.
Deno, S. L., Mirkin, P. K., & Marston, D. (1980). Relationships Lembke, E., Deno, S. L., & Hall, K. (2003). Identifying an indica-
among simple measures of written expression and perfor- tor of growth in early writing proficiency for elementary school
mance on standardized achievement tests (No. IRLDRR-22). students. Assessment for Effective Intervention, 28(3–4), 23–35.
Institute for Research on Learning Disabilities, University of McCutchen, D., & Stull, S. (2015). Morphological awareness and
Minnesota. children’s writing: Accuracy, error, and invention. Reading
Devonshire, V., Morris, P., & Fluck, M. (2013). Spelling and reading and Writing, 28(2), 271–289.
development: The effect of teaching children multiple levels of McCutchen, D., Stull, S., Herrera, B. L., Lotas, S., & Evans, S.
representation in their orthography. Learning and Instruction, (2013). Putting words to work: Effects of morphological instruc-
25, 85–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.11.007 tion on children’s writing. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
Dolch, E. W. (1936). A basic sight vocabulary. Elementary School 47(1), 86–97. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509969
Journal, 36, 456–460. McMaster, K. L., Du, X., Yeo, S., Deno, S. L., Parker, D., & Ellis,
Good, J. E., Lance, D. M., & Rainey, J. (2015). The effects of T. (2011). Curriculum-based measures of beginning writing:
morphological awareness training on reading, spelling, and Technical features of the slope. Exceptional Children, 77(2),
vocabulary skills. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 185–206.
36(3), 142–151. Nagy, W. E., Carlisle, J. F., & Goodwin, A. P. (2014).
Goodwin, A. P., & Ahn, S. (2010). A meta-analysis of morpholog- Morphological knowledge and literacy acquisition.
ical interventions: Effects on literacy achievement of children Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(1), 3–12. https://doi.
with literacy difficulties. Annals of Dyslexia, 60(2), 183–208. org/10.1177/0022219413509967
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-010-0041-x National Center for Education Statistics. (2011). The nation’s
Goodwin, A. P., & Ahn, S. (2013). A meta-analysis of mor- report card: Writing 2011 (NCES 2012–470). Institute of
phological interventions in English: Effects on literacy out- Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.
comes for school-age children. Scientific Studies of Reading, National Commission on Writing for America’s Families, Schools,
17(4), 257–285. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2012 and Colleges. (2004). Writing: A ticket to work. . .or a ticket
.689791 out: A survey of business leaders. The College Board.
Graham, S., Harris, K. R., & McKeown, D. (2013). The writing Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Olsson, J. (2003). Learning morphologi-
of students with learning disabilities, meta-analysis of self- cal and phonological spelling rules: An intervention study.
regulated strategy development writing intervention studies, Scientific Studies of Reading, 7(3), 289–307.
and future directions: Redux. In L. Swanson, K. R. Harris, Parel, R. (2006). The impact of training in morphological analy-
& S. Graham (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities sis on literacy in the primary grades. International Journal of
(pp. 405–438). Guilford Press. Learning, 13(1), 19–128.
Green, L., McCutchen, D., Schwiebert, C., Quinlan, T., Eva- Parker, D. C., McMaster, K. L., Medhanie, A., & Silberglitt, B.
Wood, A., & Juelis, J. (2003). Morphological development in (2011). Modeling early writing growth with curriculum-based
children’s writing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(4), measures. School Psychology Quarterly, 26(4), 290–304.
752–761. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.752 Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehen-
Kim, Y.-S., Al Otaiba, S., Folsom, J. S., Greulich, L., & Puranik, sion. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(4), 357–383.
C. (2014). Evaluating the dimensionality of first-grade writ- Reed, D. K. (2008). A synthesis of morphology interventions
ten composition. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing and effects on reading outcomes for students in grades K-12.
Research, 57(1), 199–211. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 23(1), 36–49.
Kim, Y.-S., Al Otaiba, S., Puranik, C., Folsom, J. S., Greulich, Ritchey, K. D., & Coker, D. L., Jr. (2014). Identifying writing difficul-
L., & Wagner, R. K. (2011). Componential skills of begin- ties in first grade: An investigation of writing and reading mea-
ning writing: An exploratory study. Learning and Individual sures. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 29(2), 54–65.
Differences, 21(5), 517–525. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lin- Rubin, H. (1988). Morphological knowledge and early writing
dif.2011.06.004 ability. Language and Speech, 31(4), 337–355.
Allen and Lembke 13

Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. A. (2013). Clinical evalua- Weschler, D. (2003). Wechsler intelligence scale for children (4th
tion of language fundamentals (5th ed.). The Psychological ed.). The Psychological Corporation.
Corporation. Wolter, J. A., & Dilworth, V. (2013). The effects of a multilin-
Swanson, H. L., & Berninger, V. W. (1996). Individual differences guistic morphological awareness approach for improving
in children’s working memory and writing skill. Journal of language and literacy. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
Experimental Child Psychology, 63(2), 358–385. 47(1), 76–85.
Treiman, R., & Cassar, M. (1996). Effects of morphology on Wolter, J. A., Wood, A., & D’zatko, K. W. (2009). The influence
children’s spelling of final consonant clusters. Journal of of morphological awareness on the literacy development of
Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 141–170. first-grade children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services
Videen, J., Deno, S., & Marston, D. (1982). Correct word in Schools, 40(3), 286–298.
sequences: A valid indicator of proficiency in ­written expres- Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, F. A., Mather, N., & Schrank, F. A.
sion (No. IRLDRR-84). University of Minnesota, Institute (2007). Woodcock-Johnson tests of achievement (3rd ed.).
for Research on Learning Disabilities. Riverside Publishing.

You might also like