You are on page 1of 14

This article was downloaded by: [Dicle University]

On: 14 November 2014, At: 00:51


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

The Journal of Positive Psychology: Dedicated to


furthering research and promoting good practice
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rpos20

The mind’s eye: A photographic method for


understanding meaning in people’s lives
ab a a a a
Michael F. Steger , Yerin Shim , Brenna R. Rush , Libby A. Brueske , Joo Yeon Shin & Leslie
a
A. Merriman
a
Department of Psychology, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO, USA;
b
North-West University, Vanderbijlpark, South Africa
Published online: 13 Sep 2013.

To cite this article: Michael F. Steger, Yerin Shim, Brenna R. Rush, Libby A. Brueske, Joo Yeon Shin & Leslie
A. Merriman (2013) The mind’s eye: A photographic method for understanding meaning in people’s lives, The
Journal of Positive Psychology: Dedicated to furthering research and promoting good practice, 8:6, 530-542, DOI:
10.1080/17439760.2013.830760

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2013.830760

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
The Journal of Positive Psychology, 2013
Vol. 8, No. 6, 530–542, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2013.830760

The mind’s eye: A photographic method for understanding meaning in people’s lives
Michael F. Stegera,b*, Yerin Shima, Brenna R. Rusha, Libby A. Brueskea, Joo Yeon Shina and Leslie A. Merrimana
a
Department of Psychology, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO, USA; bNorth-West University, Vanderbijlpark, South Africa
(Received 13 April 2012; accepted 24 July 2013)

Although research relying on self-report inventories has built an increased appreciation of the importance of meaning in
life, such research has strayed somewhat from the original promise of meaning in life research, which was to shed light
on the individual experience of meaning. Some research has focused on understanding people’s sources of meaning.
However, previous methods have relied on verbal ways of assessing sources of meaning in life. In recognition of the fact
that not everyone has highly developed verbal skills – and that even those who do can find it hard to articulate what life
means – we offer a new method for understanding individuals’ experiences with meaning in life. In this article, we
describe the use of photography to elicit information about people’s sources of meaning and provide inductive qualitative
Downloaded by [Dicle University] at 00:51 14 November 2014

analysis of a pilot study using this method. Photography holds great potential as a new method for seeing meaning
through another’s eyes.
Keywords: meaning; purpose

Several decades of research have created a rich land- Broadly speaking, meaning in life is an indicator of
scape of knowledge about meaning in life. Research on psychological and spiritual well-being, psychological
meaning in life covers a wide range of topics and has strengths, and positive development (Ryff, 1989; Steger
focused on many different dimensions along which peo- 2009, 2012). Research has documented associations
ple are thought to experience meaning. Most of this with happiness (Ryff & Keyes, 1995; Steger, Oishi, &
research has investigated people’s subjective sense of the Kashdan, 2009), a more positive affect balance (Steger
amount of meaning in their lives. In our research, we & Frazier, 2005), and life satisfaction (Keyes, Shmotkin,
have referred to this dimension as the presence of mean- & Ryff, 2002; Steger, Frazier, Oishi, & Kaler, 2006;
ing and have distinguished it from people’s efforts and Steger & Kashdan, 2007; Steger, Kashdan, Sullivan, &
interest in establishing and/or augmenting their experi- Lorentz, 2008). Meaning also appears to facilitate more
ence of meaning in life, which the field has referred to adaptive coping and better adjustment following stress
as ‘will for meaning’ or ‘search for meaning’ (e.g. (Park & Folkman, 1997; Thompson, Coker, Krause, &
Frankl, 1963; Maddi, 1970). Research on search for Henry, 2003). Research also links deficits in meaning
meaning is in an early stage. However, research on the with a greater likelihood of experiencing psychological
presence of meaning is fairly mature, dating back to distress, such as PTSD symptoms (Owens, Steger,
Crumbaugh and Maholick’s (1964) publication of the Whitesell, & Herrerra, 2009; Steger, Frazier, & Zaccha-
Purpose in Life Test in the 1960s. Subsequent research nini, 2008) and depression (Steger et al., 2006; Steger &
has used a large number of tools like the Purpose in Life Kashdan, 2009).
Test, meaning that most of what we know about mean- While we know a lot about the general status and
ing in life relies on the use of self-report questionnaires. outcome of believing one’s life is meaningful or meaning-
By using self-report questionnaires, we have learned a less, we generally are lacking a richer understanding of
great deal about the role of meaning in human function- what it means for someone to say, ‘yes, my life is mean-
ing. At the same time, meaning in life is intuitively a ingful!’ Research on sources of meaning helps us under-
personal, phenomenological pillar of human experience. stand where people tend to naturally find meaning, and, in
The question of whether having more or less meaning is turn, what they really mean by saying that life is meaning-
important is exciting, but an equally fascinating question ful to them. Sources of meaning refer to the specific
has – more or less – been left by the wayside: ‘How aspects and domains of life that people say they find
does one individual person answer the question of his or meaningful or say that they use as a resource from which
her individual meaning in life?’ In this paper, we to draw meaning. Some meaning in life theorists have
describe and present initial results from a new method argued that it is ideal for people to draw meaning from
for asking this sort of question about meaning in life. many sources (known as meaning in life breadth) and to

*Corresponding author. Email: michael.f.steger@colostate.edu.

© 2013 Taylor & Francis


The Journal of Positive Psychology 531

find that the relative contribution of each source of face of a broader pattern of sources of meaning, which
meaning is similar (known as meaning in life balance) were said to converge on the following eight categories:
(Wong, 2012). Researchers have used qualitative, mixed- (i) Relationships; (ii) Service; (iii) Belief; (iv) Obtaining;
method, and quantitative/sources of meaning measurement (v) Growth; (vi) Health; (vii) Life Work; and (viii)
approaches to identify where meaning in life comes from. Pleasure (Ebersole, 1998).
Other researchers used their own approaches to try to
identify people’s sources of meaning. O’Connor and
Qualitative approaches Chamberlain (1996) interviewed 38 adults in mid-life,
Although early meaning in life questionnaires often beginning with the question, ‘What do you think of as
included sections that requested participants to provide an important source of meaning in your life?’ They
short responses about their sources of meaning (e.g. identified six broad sources of meaning: (i) Relationship
Crumbaugh & Maholick, 1964), there is a certain level with People; (ii) Creativity; (iii) Religious and Spiritual;
of dedication required to engage in qualitative research (iv) Personal Development; (v) Social and Political; and
of this sort and such sections were almost invariably dis- (vi) Relationships with Nature. Baum and Stewart (1990)
carded from studies. By any account, the most successful focused their efforts on sources of meaning across the
qualitative research effort on sources of meaning was led lifespan. A sample of men and women was assorted into
by Ebersole, using his Meaning in Life Depth approach. five developmental stages – young adulthood, adulthood,
Downloaded by [Dicle University] at 00:51 14 November 2014

In this approach, participants were asked to discuss what middle adulthood, older middle adulthood, and older
made their lives meaningful. Responses were interpreted adulthood – and asked to report on their most
using Battista and Almond’s (1973) six life orientations meaningful events, the age that the event occurred, and
(interpersonal, service, understanding, obtaining, the reasons why it was meaningful. They reported seven
expressive, and ethical). Over the course of several categories: (i) Work; (ii) Love and Marriage; (iii) Births
studies, Ebersole and colleagues investigated sources of of Children; (vi) Independent Pursuits; (v) Accidents,
meaning in several different populations, including Illnesses, or Death; (vi) Separations and/or Divorces; and
young children (Taylor & Ebersole, 1993), young adoles- (vii) Major Purchases.
cents (DeVogler & Ebersole, 1983), college students
(DeVogler & Ebersole, 1980; Ebersole & DeVogler,
1981), adults (DeVogler & Ebersole, 1981), elderly Mixed-method approaches
nursing home residents (DePaola & Ebersole, 1995), Debats (1999) developed the Sources of Meaning Ques-
later life couples (Ebersole & DePaola, 1987), active and tionnaire (SOMQ) to implement a mixed-method
married elderly adults (Ebersole & DePaola, 1989). They approach to sources of meaning in samples of young
even extended their method to written works of eminent adults who were psychiatric patients and those who were
people (Ebersole & DeVogler-Ebersole, 1986). not. The two-part SOMQ first asked participants, ‘If you
In the study that established their method, DeVogler care to, please describe below in short the three most
and Ebersole (1980) asked college students to write important things that give meaning to your personal life
about the three most meaningful things in their lives and at the present moment.’ Next, participants provided a
rank the importance of those written meanings. They rating of how committed they were to each source of
derived eight categories from the essays which showed meaning listed (i.e. ‘Now, if you care to, please indicate
adequate inter-rater reliability: (i) Relationships; (ii) for each answer on the corresponding five-point scale to
Service; (iii) Growth; (iv) Belief; (v) Existential-Hedonis- what extent you are really committed to that particular
tic; (vi) Obtaining; (vii) Expression; and (viii) Under- meaning’). Responses were initially coded using seven
standing. To validate this categorization, they conducted categories: (i) Relationships; (ii) Lifework; (iii) Personal
another study with a college student sample, but this Well-being; (iv) Self-actualization; (v) Service; (vi)
time asking them to write an essay about, and provide Beliefs; and (vii) Materiality;and added an eighth
an example of one strongest meaning in their life (Eber- category; (viii) Future/Hope, which appeared frequently
sole & DeVogler, 1981). in participants’ responses. In this study, as in qualitative
As this method was used in samples other than research, Relationships were most frequently mentioned
college students, differences in the categories of meaning by participants.
in life began to appear. Research with an older sample
of adults (30–80 years old) prompted the addition of two
more categories, ‘Health’ and ‘Life Work’ (DeVogler & Quantitative approach/measures of sources of meaning
Ebersole, 1981), and a study conducted with adolescents Drawing on this qualitative and mixed-methods research,
generated three more categories: ‘Activities,’ ‘School,’ several researchers attempted to develop quantitative
and ‘Appearance’ (DeVogler & Ebersole, 1983). These measures of sources of meaning. Reker and Wong (1988)
differences were considered to be somewhat minor in the developed the Sources of Meaning Profile (SOMP),
532 M.F. Steger et al.

which instructed respondents to rate 13 sources of mean- Group differences in sources of meaning
ing in terms of each source’s meaningfulness. Factor Research on sources of meaning indicates that differ-
analysis indicated four factors: Self-transcendence, ences exist in the sources people find meaningful,
Collectivism, Individualism, and Self-preoccupation. In a depending on participants’ age, gender, culture, and other
17-item revised version of this scale, the SOMP-R, Reker sample characteristics. Several studies have found that
(1996) reworded the items and added four items (Human- sources of meaning vary across the lifespan. Research by
istic Concerns, Hedonistic Activities, Material Posses- Ebersole and colleagues (e.g. DePaola & Ebersole, 1995;
sions, and Relationship with Nature) to improve the DeVogler & Ebersole, 1981; Ebersole & DePaola, 1987)
factor structure of the original scale. This scale has been found that younger adults were more concerned with
used with samples from Canada, Australia, and Israel Belief than Health or Pleasure, while Health and Plea-
(Prager, 1996). Prager, Savaya, and Bar-Tur, (2000) later sure were the second and third most frequently men-
conducted focus groups and interviews with Jewish and tioned sources of meaning (after Relationships) reported
Arab Israelis in order to develop a culturally sensitive by the elderly. Schnell’s (2009) findings showed that
measure of sources of meaning. The final scale consisted Horizontal Self-transcendence and Order increased with
of 41 items that created 11 subscales: (i) Family and age.
Communal Values; (ii) Materialistic Concerns/Values; Gender differences also have been found in several
(iii) Life Satisfaction/Autonomy; (iv) Sense of Connect- studies. Baum and Stewart (1990) reported that men find
Downloaded by [Dicle University] at 00:51 14 November 2014

edness; (v) Communal Consciousness/Awareness; (vi) meaning mostly in Work (72%), Love and Marriage
Attainment of Tranquility/Peace; (vii) Leisure Pursuits; (61%), and Independent Pursuits (54%) while women
Self-Development; (viii) Family Relationships; (xi) find meaning mostly in Births of Children (78%), Love
Leisure Activities Away From Home; (x) Enjoyment and Marriage (77%), and Work (72%). Schnell (2009)
from Animals; and (xi) Relationship with Partner. found that females reported more Vertical Self-transcen-
Wong (1998) developed the Personal Meaning Profile dence, Well-being, and Relatedness than males, whereas
(PMP) based on what he called people’s ‘implicit males reported more Self-actualization than females.
theories’ about what constructs an ideally meaningful life. Differences among ethnic and cultural groups also
Initial reports detailed several rounds of revision that have been reported. Jenerson-Madden, Ebersole, and
failed to yield a consistent factor structure. The final Romero (1992) compared the types of meaning generated
version included 57 items which loaded on seven factors: by first-generation Mexican-Americans to that of Euro-
(i) Achievement; (ii) Relationship; (iii) Religion; (iv) pean-Americans of similar age. They found that signifi-
Self-transcendence; (v) Self-Acceptance; (vi) Intimacy; cantly more Mexican-American adults (70%) named
and (vii) Fair Treatment (Wong, 1998). More recently, Relationships as being central compared to European-
Wong developed the PMP-B which is a short form of the American adults (47%). Prager et al. (2000) found that
original PMP that includes all seven subscales, but only cultural differences between Israeli Arabs and Jews inter-
three items for each, resulting in a total of 21 items. acted with age. For young Israelis, Family and Communal
Sources of meaning measures continue to be Values and Communal Consciousness/Awareness were
developed. Fegg, Kramer, L’hoste, and Borasio (2008) more important among Arabs than among Jews. For older
developed the Schedule for Meaning in Life Evaluation Israelis, Materialistic Concerns/Values and Leisure Pur-
which assessed 13 sources: (i) Family; (ii) Work/study- suits/Self-Development were more important among
ing; (iii) Friends; (iv) Partner; (v) Leisure time; (vi) Arabs than among Jews. In contrast, Life Satisfaction/
Religiosity; (vii) Health; (viii) Animals/nature; (xi) Autonomy and Connectedness/Belonging and Attainment
Finances; (x) Well-being; (xi) Hedonism; (xii) Altruism; of Tranquility/Peace were more important for Jews com-
and (xiii) Home/garden. Schnell’s (2009) Sources of pared to Arabs, especially among older Israelis.
Meaning and Meaning in Life Questionnaire assesses 26 Sources of meaning may differ according to other,
sources of meaning assorted into four dimensions: (i) non-demographic factors as well. A sample of typical
Self-transcendence (Vertical: explicit religiosity; spiritual- adults valued Relationships more than did eminent peo-
ity; Horizontal: social commitment, unison with nature, ple, who placed greater value on Life Work (Ebersole &
self-knowledge, health, generativity); (ii) Self-actualiza- DeVogler-Ebersole, 1986). Student volunteers in a ser-
tion (challenge, individualism, power, development, vice organization reported Growth as a way to find
achievement, freedom, knowledge, creativity); (iii) Order meaning more often than non-volunteers (Braunstein &
(tradition, practicality, morality, reason); and (iv) Ebersole, 1992). Finally, psychiatric patients viewed
Well-being and Relatedness (community, fun, love, com- Self-actualization as a more important source of meaning
fort, care, attentiveness, harmony). These sources were than did non-patients, while non-patients saw Personal
identified using structured interviews, in which partici- Well-being as more important than patients (Debats,
pants indicated whether they agreed or disagreed with 1999).
each item as a source of meaning in their lives.
The Journal of Positive Psychology 533

Despite these differences, there seems to be a what really makes life meaningful for people, leaving
general consensus about the domains from which them no opportunity to update their written assessment
people most often draw meaning. Emmons (2003) if they realize a certain aspect of their lives is mean-
argued that a consensus could be reached, based on ingful during their daily lives.
three studies (Ebersole, 1998; Emmons, 1999; Wong, Due to these limitations of research solely relying
1998) that used diverse methodologies (rating scales, on written expression, we propose a new method to
surveys, and interviews) in heterogeneous populations. explore sources of meaning using visual methods – in
He suggested that four major categories of life mean- particular, using the medium of photography. Visual
ing encompass the most typical sources of meaning: research methods have been used widely in fields such
Achievements/Work, Relationships/Intimacy, Religion/ as anthropology, ethnography, and sociology and are
Spirituality, and Self-transcendence/Generativity. Among becoming more popular in other social sciences such as
these categories, Relationships seem to be the most social psychology (e.g. Dollinger, 2002; Ziller & Lewis,
frequent and consistent source of meaning reported across 1981), health (e.g. Riley & Manias, 2004; Wang &
studies (e.g. DeVogler & Ebersole, 1981; McCarthy, 1983, Burris, 1997), and organizational studies (e.g. Buchanan,
1985). 2001). Photographic data, which is one form of visual
data, provide a more direct record of the phenomena of
interest than any other data collection method used by
Downloaded by [Dicle University] at 00:51 14 November 2014

A new approach to sources of meaning social science researchers and also capture the immedi-
Despite the breadth of the consensual categories, there ate moment when taken by the photographer (Grady,
is still a lot of variability from one study to the next. 2008). Photographs are easy for participants to produce,
It is likely that many factors shape people’s sources of requiring no training or sustained effort. But beyond
meaning, including age, gender, culture, and life experi- convenience, photographs are used by many researchers
ences. It could be that individual variation in these because they can convey emotion. Rose delineates two
factors accounts for different results. Or, it could be ways photographs can be used in social science research
that methodological factors affect study findings. based on how they each function as a part of a research
Qualitative research has inherent limitations, including project. Supporting methods use photographs to try to
difficulty with generalizability and obstacles to gauging answer specific research questions. Supplemental
effect sizes or testing causality. Qualitative research methods focus more on the specific visual qualities of
also poses challenges to efforts to synthesize results the photographs and treat them as stand-alone docu-
across multiple research programs. In the case of ments separate from any specific research questions. We
sources of meaning research, another potential problem used the supporting method to evaluate previous efforts
is the reliance on verbal expressiveness on the part of to answer the question, ‘From where do people draw
research participants. Verbalizing what makes life meaning in their lives?’ To develop this method, we
meaningful can be a challenging task for people. In drew upon two photographic methods – auto-photogra-
addition, people may have differing levels of verbal phy and photo-elicitation.
ability, and differential ability to describe an experience Auto-photography is a research method that
of meaning in life. Using written responses compounds, instructs participants to take pictures from their envi-
these problems altering the content of people’s ronment of things or representations of things that are
responses, at least relative to verbal responses (e.g. important to them. In an examination of the ‘self-
Esterling, Antoni, Fletcher, Margulies, & Schneiderman, concept,’ Ziller (1990) used this approach to allow
1994). Additionally, participants are typically asked to participants to represent themselves in their own con-
write about their sources of meaning in a laboratory text and to become involved in the research process.
setting, at one point in time. This means that there is Ziller (1990) argued that the auto-photography method
an absence of cues from their own lives that can allows communication across different cultures, in con-
prompt their responses, which might lead to memory trast to verbal methods, such as those used in previous
biases, such as the availability heuristic (e.g. Schwartz sources of meaning research. Another aspect of auto-
et al., 1991). Further, the act of writing or talking can photography is that it frees participants from any
become a social-cognitive experience, in which people constraints of categories generated by researchers.
may imagine their audience and frame their responses Noland reported two case studies in self-esteem and
to meet expectations. These two factors might lead to identity research using auto-photography, one exploring
an over-reliance on culturally stereotyped sources of self-esteem of adolescent Latina teenage girls living in
meaning, poorly specified and verbalized responses, or Los Angeles and one studying identities of South
satisficing in the face of the verbal demands of the Asian immigrant women living in a small town in the
task. Finally, writing about sources of meaning during Midwest of the US. He concluded that adopting the
a single session may derive an incomplete snapshot of method of auto-photography generated more authentic
534 M.F. Steger et al.

data because it allowed participants to select and Method


record images of their identity which they feel Participants
represent them the best from their own view of what
Eighty-six participants were recruited from an under-
is important. A further advantage of auto-photography
graduate introductory psychology course at a large
was the provision to participants of sufficient space
University in the Western United States. The mean age
and time to contemplate the topic of study and how
of the participants was 19.3 years (SD = 1.9). Most
they want to represent it, rather than having to come
(73.3%) of the participants were female, and the major-
up with on-the-spot answers. As such, a photographic
ity of the sample was European-American (82.6%).
approach should yield richer data which is more
Participants received course credit for their participation
authentically representative of the participant’s perspec-
in the study.
tive than on-the-spot verbal or written approaches.
Photo-elicitation is a technique developed by
Collier (1957) and Collier and Collier (1986) in which Materials
photographs are used to prompt responses during an Participants used a Kodak digital camera with 8.2 mega-
interview. Collier’s research demonstrated that photo- pixels. Using only the camera’s built-in memory, partici-
elicitation interviews elicited longer and more compre- pants were limited to a maximum of 12 photographs.
hensive responses than verbal interviews and also
Downloaded by [Dicle University] at 00:51 14 November 2014

helped participants overcome fatigue during the inter-


view (Collier, 1957). Collier (1957) asserted that state- Procedure
ments derived from photographic interviews were more After consent procedures, participants were given a
directly tied to the visual stimuli, whereas verbal inter- battery of well-being, mental health, and personality
views tended to be governed by the mood of the questionnaires to complete. Participants were then given
informants. As Harper (2002) describes, photo-elicita- instructions and a brief training for using the camera.
tion ‘mines deeper shafts into a different part of Participants were instructed to ‘take photos of the things
human consciousness than do words-alone interviews’ that make your life feel meaningful.’ Participants were
(p. 22–23). informed that they could take photographs of abstract/
Photo-elicitation interviews could be conducted symbolic things (e.g. souvenirs, objets d’art, photographs
with researcher-only, participant-only, archival-only, or of other photographs depicting things that were not prox-
a hybrid approach of photographic production. In imal) because they would be given the opportunity to
researcher-only photographic production, the researcher describe the picture and what it meant at the end of the
(or research team) produces the photographs which week. Participants scheduled their next session exactly
they want to use in the interview. In contrast, partici- one week after their first one. During the intervening
pant-only photographic production involves the partici- week, participants were given reminders of how many
pant being asked to take their own photographs to be days they had left until their second appointment. When
later used as stimuli during the interview (Clark, they returned for their second appointment, the photo-
1999; VanAuken, Frisvoll, & Stewart, 2010). Whereas graphs were downloaded from the camera and inserted
auto-photography typically solicits written descriptions into an electronic presentation document that provided
of the photographs, photo-elicitation elicits spoken one-half page for the photograph, and one-half page for
responses, which may prompt more in-depth responses the participants to write a description of the photograph
as well as provide participants with opportunities to and why it was meaningful to them. Each photograph
clarify or further expand on responses. received its own slide, and each slide was printed on a
Overall, both auto-photography and photo-elicita- color printer. While the photograph rating sheets were
tion seem to be effective tools for better understand- being printed, participants completed a brief survey
ing what people find meaningful and from what inquiring about their current mood that included state
sources they draw meaning in their lives. Using pho- measures of meaning in life, depression, and anxiety.
tography may uncover information about sources of Participants then were handed the printed sheets and
meaning that has been missed by previous verbal wrote their description of the photographs they took.
research. In our research, we have conducted studies Participants were asked to rank each photograph in terms
using auto-photography and photo-elicitation. In the of its importance to their meaning in life and to stack all
present article, we provide our findings from an auto- of the photographs in that same order. When they were
photography study and describe an ongoing photo- finished, they completed a duplicate form of the brief
elicitation study. mood survey, as well as a duplicate form of the original
questionnaire battery. Following this, participants were
debriefed.
The Journal of Positive Psychology 535

Data analysis Leisure; (iii) Relationships; (iv) Pets; (v) Possessions;


Photographic data were analyzed using an analytic (vi) Everyday Necessities; (vii) Religion; (viii) Values;
inductive coding approach by two trained undergraduate (ix) Education; (x) Technology; (xi) Organizations/Activ-
research assistants (B.R. and L.B.) and a graduate ities; (xii) Physical Environment; (xiii) Future Aspira-
researcher (Y.S.) from November 2011 to April 2012. A tions; (xiv) Occupation/Work; (xv) Self; and (xiv)
constant comparative analysis method based on grounded Miscellaneous. Several of these categories correspond
theory (Strauss & Corbin, 1990) was adopted as the cod- with previous research, including Relationships, Hobby/
ing approach. In constant comparative analysis, the codes Leisure, Religion, Values, Animals and Nature, and Pets
are induced from the participants’ responses without any (cf. Ebersole, 1998; Fegg et al., 2008; Prager et al.,
a priori codes. This method was selected because it 2000). In addition, the Possessions, Technology, and
allows for new information within the data to surface Everyday Necessities categories could correspond to
during analysis. Since the purpose of this study was not Materialistic Concerns (Prager et al., 2000) or Obtaining
to develop a theory, but to determine representative cate- (Ebersole, 1998) or perhaps Finances (Fegg et al.,
gories of sources of meaning, only the two initial stages 2008). However, a greater number of categories were
of coding – open coding (developing categories for derived from this study than previous studies using ver-
information) and axial coding (interconnecting catego- bal response methods. In addition, participants in the
ries) – were conducted. In the open coding phase, the present study made distinctions among some categories
Downloaded by [Dicle University] at 00:51 14 November 2014

three coders independently examined each photograph that are often merged in other research. For instance,
and its participant-generated description to draw seg- Work is distinguished from both Leisure and Education,
ments of the data which were informative of participants’ which is not always the case in other research. Further,
sources of meaning. These were then classified into two the distinctions among Pets, Nature, and Physical Envi-
hierarchical levels of coding – a main category and a ronment – as well as the distinctions among Possessions,
subcategory (if needed to describe the photograph and Technology, and Everyday Necessities – suggest that
description). Multiple categories were allowed for each participants in our sample made finer, non-verbal distinc-
photograph if necessary. The last level of analysis tions about the aspects of their world that hold meaning
involved determining whether the participant attached a than was suggested by previous research using verbal
specific meaning to the photograph within the apparent methods.
category and subcategory. In vivo codes (Creswell, 2007) The verbal descriptions of sources of meaning which
were often used to label the meanings which were were obtained in this study allowed participants to identify
directly taken from the participants’ narratives. After the the personal meaning of each photograph. These descrip-
initial, independent coding, the three researchers com- tions made possible a breadth of content to be collected, as
pared and discussed their findings and came to a mutual well as a depth of personal relevance. Figure 2 shows the
agreement on the main category, subcategory (if applica- meanings participants attached to photographs in the main
ble), and the meaning for each photograph. As the cod- categories and provides some examples of photographs. In
ing process evolved, a list of core categories with their many cases, the specific meaning of what is shown in the
subcategories and meanings was compiled. Throughout photographs could not be guessed without participant
the coding process, and after it was completed, axial descriptions. For example, participant 33’s photograph of
coding was conducted by the researchers, which food is quite baffling without the description.
involved a process of merging categories when possible Examining this selection of photographs and mean-
and consolidating the list of meanings to ensure that the ings – drawn from roughly 1000 photographs – shows
categories were non-redundant. New categories were that verbal descriptions of what makes life meaningful
added to the list when no previous category or meaning seem more prone to coalesce around a limited number of
could represent the data. This process was repeated until ‘Big Themes’. In participants’ descriptions, emergent
the list of categories reached ‘saturation,’ a state where themes included comfort, relationships, goals, health,
no new information emerges from the data that requires religion, heritage, work, and helping. These themes
a new code. cross-categories of photographs. A poster for a charitable
cause shares roots with a tractor and a man at a custodial
job because all mean providing for others to the photog-
Results
rapher. A mountain and a drawing share roots with a
Figure 1 shows the main categories and subcategories dog and a cupboard of food because all signify some
that emerged from the constant comparative analysis of kind of comfort. Thus, the photographic method revealed
participants’ photographs assisted by their descriptions a seemingly endlessly varied set of pictures that
of the photographs. The main categories show some nonetheless revealed 16 main categories, many more
convergence with previous, verbal investigations. We subcategories, and unique paths to some well-known
identified 16 sources of meaning: (i) Nature; (ii) Hobby/ themes of the meaningful life. As in previous research,
536 M.F. Steger et al.

Main Sub-Category Definition


Category
Nature Mountains, Flowers, Seasons, Body All elements of the natural world.
of water
Hobby/ Leisure Art, Photography, Music, Travel, Any activity that is pursued for
Reading, Cooking, Sports, pleasure or relaxation and is not their
Exercise, Outdoor Activities, Being occupation.
Outdoors, Social Interaction,
Events/ Performances, Fashion,
Fandom, Video Games, Gardening
Relationships Family, Friends, Romantic Partner, Any emotional connection,
Child, Co-Workers involvement, or association between
the participant and another person(s),
as identified by the participant.
Pets (no sub-categories identified) Domestic animals that are kept as
companions.
Possessions Car, Bike, House, Clothing, Gift, Material objects identified as
Educational supplies, Water Bottle, particularly important and meaningful
Calendar, Scrapbook, Souvenir, to the participant. Ownership.
License Plate, Stuffed animal,
Downloaded by [Dicle University] at 00:51 14 November 2014

Everyday Food, Money, Sleep Any material or act that is identified as


Necessities needed for everyday survival.
Religion Faith, God, Guiding Discipline, An identified specific set of beliefs and
Community practices followed by the participant.
The following of a specific religion.
Values Love, Fun, Laughter, Happiness, A belief, principle, or characteristic that
Humor, Humility, Integrity, is identified as important to the
Strength, Harmony, Care, Life, participant either personally or to
Knowledge/Learning, society.
Organization, Perfection, Health,
Individuality, Freedom, Privacy,
Diversity, Environmentalism, Gay
Rights, Politics, Military,
Education Major, School Pride, Academic Involvement in higher education.
Success Actively pursuing knowledge.
Technology Cell phone, Laptops The use of modern technology as tools
in everyday life
Organization/ Sorority, Fraternity, Community Any identified involvement in a
Activities Service, Marching Band, Sports specific group or activity that adds
team meaning or purpose to the participant’s
life.
Physical Church, Library, Campus, Any specific location identified by the
Environment Neighborhood, House, Hometown, participant that is frequented often
Room/Dorm and/or holds special meaning.
Future (no sub-categories identified) Future plans, goals, or careers
Aspirations identified by the participant as actively
pursuing.
Occupation/ (no sub-categories identified) A current occupation of the participant.
Work
Self (no sub-categories identified) The participant themselves.
Miscellaneous Holidays, Children (non-family), Any topic discussed by a participant
Experience, Farming, Past that does not fit into a Main Category
or Sub-Category, or a topic that was
only discussed by one participant.

Figure 1. Main categories and subcategories identified in participants’ photographs.

Relationships emerge as the most common source of Discussion


meaning (Table 1), and half or more of our sample also Meaning in life research has been conducted for
photographed Hobby/Leisure, Nature, and Education decades, yet it seems to be enjoying a burst of energy
sources of meaning. and excitement. Research consistently affirms that
The Journal of Positive Psychology 537

Main Category Common Meanings Examples Photo Examples


Nature Beauty, Relaxation, Participant 22: “This
Mood Elevation, picture represents the
Reminder of beauty of the world.
God/Faith, Escape, Stopping and taking it
Experience, Comfort, all in helps make life
Adventure, meaningful.”
Appreciation

Hobby/ Leisure Self- expression, Participant 56: “This is a


Stress relief, picture of me drawing.
Relaxation, This is meaningful
Motivation, Learning, because it lets me be
Enjoyment, Passion, creative; and helps me
Mood Enhancement, stay calm.”
Exploration

Relationships Support, Guidance, Participant 36:” This is a


Realization of values, drawing of my family
Value relationship, because they live
Downloaded by [Dicle University] at 00:51 14 November 2014

Encouragement, somewhere else. They


Understanding, Love, are very meaningful to
Care, Reason to live, me because they have
Inspiration, Having a been here for me my
fun time together, whole life. I can depend
Self- development on them and am loved
by them. They always
support me and teach me
valuable things. “
Pets Responsibility, Participant 38:” She is a
Companionship, great comfort and safety
Fondness, mechanism. She also
Unconditional love, gives me something to
Safety, Attachment, take care of, she depends
Constant presence, on me-so through her I
Loyalty, Calming gain some sense of
meaning.”

Figure 2. Main category, meanings held by participants regarding that category, and examples of photographs (this photograph was
intentionally distorted to protect the identity of the research participant).

meaning is a cornerstone of the Good Life. Beyond The results of our study show that there is both vast
the basic notion that meaning is good, though, our complexity and familiarity in the sources from which
understanding of the sources from where meaning is people draw meaning. Categories like Relationships,
derived is limited. This is especially true when it Religion, Values, Nature, and Work echo previous verbal
comes to probing the existential emphasis on the idea research (e.g. Ebersole, 1998). Yet, many categories and
that each of us, independently, uniquely forges our dozens of subcategories emerged from the photographic
own idiosyncratic meaning in our lives. Previous data, seeming to arise from much finer distinctions than
research, in which people write about what makes life previous research had revealed. Photographs seemed to
meaningful, or endorse various lists of sources of provide strong testimony about the many different ways
meaning that are provided to them, has identified a in which people find answers to existential questions.
variable but reasonably consistent set of sources of When participants were given the opportunity to describe
meaning. The reliance of these methods on verbal their photographs, the underlying themes were often sur-
descriptions of sources of meaning could place con- prising given the obvious content of the images, but also
straints on the richness and completeness of data reflected common, broad themes. The photographic
derived from these studies. Therefore, we sought to method seemed to yield nearly fractal-like data, in which
develop a photographic method for understanding what new variations on larger themes are encountered. It is
makes someone’ life meaningful that did not rely because of this ‘bottomless yet meaningful’ sense of
solely on verbal filtering and instead literally showed meaning that emerged that we are pursuing an extension
what meaning looked like for participants. to this work using photo-elicitation.
538 M.F. Steger et al.

Possessions Transportation, Gift Participant 8:” This is


from loved one, my car. It is meaningful
Pride, cause it allows me to
Accomplishment, travel and get around. I
Memories of home, rely on it every day and
reliability, Fun, it is very meaningful to
Comfort me.”

Everyday Memories, Nostalgia, Participant 33: “My food


Necessities Survival, Energy, keeps me fueled and
Comfort, Enjoyment, energized. It is also there
Forming bonds, in times for comfort.”
Importance

Religion Guidance, Maintain Participant 43: “It shows


values, Morals, my Bible and a packet I
Devotion, Core did. My faith is the most
values, Purpose, important thing in my
Reliance, Influence, life because God created
Downloaded by [Dicle University] at 00:51 14 November 2014

Heritage me and has already


decided a purpose for
my life and He will lead
me through everything
in my life.”
Values Motivation, Life Participant 15: “Health:
principle, Enjoyment, it’s important to workout
Lifestyle, most days of the week
Importance, and eat healthy because
Influence, Activism, it keeps the balance and
Pride, Aspiration, makes me feel good
Admiration about myself. “

Education Pathway to future Participant 9: “This is


goals, Achievement, the Ram logo. It
Pride, Opportunity, represents my
Learning, Pursuit of opportunity to
Knowledge, experience all these
Independence, great things in Colorado.
Personal Growth, It also is meaningful
Importance, because it is where I
Enjoyment, work hard at school. It
Motivation, School will help me achieve my
pride, Gratitude goal to become an
occupational therapist.”
Technology Communication, Participant 66: “This is a
picture of my cell phone
Escape, Connection, and laptop. These are
Resource ways that help me to
stay connected with my
family even though we
are miles apart.”

Figure 2. (Continued)

Limitations and future research participants to clarify what they actually meant to
Though our findings from the auto-photography study represent (Blinn & Harrist, 1991). Having a chance to
generated rich, informative responses which also discuss the photographs with the participant in detail
captured the subtle meanings of the sources, some will provide the additional benefit of increasing the
photographs were difficult to interpret even with the trustworthiness of the findings through a ‘member
written descriptions. One of the advantages of photo- checking’ strategy. Furthermore, during the interview,
elicitation is that it offers an opportunity for the the photographs may ‘elicit’ deeper feelings and
The Journal of Positive Psychology 539

Organizations/ New Experiences, Participant 50: “This


Activities Helping others, picture represents
Friendship, “giving back to the
Achievement, Social community” and
interaction, volunteer work. This is
Teamwork, meaningful in my life
Commitment, because it is something
Influence that I enjoy doing.
Helping other gives my
life meaning. “
Physical Shelter, Childhood Participant 14: “This is
Environment memories, Safety, where I currently live. It
Relaxation, represents a place of
Community, Escape, relaxation and shelter
Privacy, Stomping from all the stresses of
ground, Nostalgia, everyday life. It also
Gratitude, Ownership represents
independence.”
Future Passion, Enjoyment, Participant 61: “This is
Aspirations Accomplishments, my DJ gear. This helps
Motivation, bring meaning to my life
Downloaded by [Dicle University] at 00:51 14 November 2014

Contribution, because it is a goal I


Direction strive for. I want to
become a big name DJ
and it’s something I’m
working on.”
Occupation/Work Pride, Providing for Participant 24: “This is
family, Source of my work at the Lory
income, Passion, Student Center. Though
Responsibility, Effort I am a custodian, I am
proud to be one. This is
the first job that will not
get me in trouble, I am
proud because the job
pays for my family.”
Self1 Support, Self-Love Participant 20: “This is a
picture of me. I am my
best support and I love
being me. “

Miscellaneous Influence, Hard Participant 19: “The


work, Heritage, main focus is a tractor. I
Means of living, picked it because I
Innocence, Honesty, wanted to show how
Love, Trust, Family farming was a large
traditions, Memories influence in my life. It
shows that there are still
people who work hard
just to put food on the
table. And that those are
my roots.”

Figure 2. (Continued)

expand thoughts about the sources of meaning partici- In addition to future research that seeks more depth,
pants have rated, which provides a chance for the future research should also seek more breadth by using
participants to reflect on things they may have never this photographic method to explore meaning in the lives
thought about. Therefore, we are conducting on- of diverse populations. The present study was limited by
going research which combines the methods of the its reliance on college students. Qualitative research on
present study with the addition of photo-elicitation sources of meaning has already shown that the emphasis
interviews to deepen and extend the findings of this people from different groups place on specific sources of
pilot study. meaning vary quite a bit (e.g. Prager et al., 2000). By
540 M.F. Steger et al.

Table 1. Frequency with which participants took photographs cheap digital cameras, future research may not need to
within each category. provide participants with cameras. Instead, participants
Number of Percentage of could be solicited through a variety of means, including
participants participants submitting internet methods such as Mechanical Turk, and asked to
Main submitting matching matching photograph use their own means of taking digital photographs,
category photograph (%) which could then be uploaded to the researchers.
Relationships 77 89.5 Each of the sources of meaning identified in the
Hobby/ 61 70.9 present study and similar research holds possibilities for
Leisure future research. Research could dig deeper into each
Nature 50 58.1 category in an effort to understand what it is about pet,
Education 43 50.0
hobbies, or possessions that holds such meaning for
Possessions 37 43.0
Physical 30 34.9 people. For example, people who experience meaning
environment around pets might do so because they appreciate some
Pets 28 32.6 entity needing them, which gives them a sense of signif-
Values 26 30.2 icance and contribution. Alternatively, they might also
Religion 19 22.1
find that pets facilitate their social experiences, enhanc-
Technology 15 17.4
Everyday 13 15.1 ing their sense of belongingness. For one person, a
Downloaded by [Dicle University] at 00:51 14 November 2014

necessities possession like a truck may be the means through which


Organization/ 11 12.8 she is able to work and support her dream of graduating
Activities from college; for another, a truck might be a status
Future 5 5.8
symbol that boosts self-esteem and helps him obtain
aspirations
Occupation/ 3 3.5 romantic partners. Very little research has gone beyond
Work the question of asking ‘how much meaning’ to ask
Self 2 2.3 ‘where from,’ and even less has gone even further to
Miscellaneous 7 8.1 ask ‘why.’
Gaining a detailed picture of individuals’ unique
experience of meaning offers the compelling advantage
side-stepping a complete reliance on written and verbal of enabling the development of better meaning interven-
abilities, this photographic method holds great potential tions. Elsewhere, we describe the results of a pilot
for being transportable to nearly any population. People study showing that people’s meaning in life increased
who have been incarcerated could use photography to after a week of taking sources of meaning photographs
share their world and their way of finding meaning. Sim- and also after reviewing and describing the photographs
ilarly, people enduring lengthy medical or psychiatric that were taken (Steger, Shim, Barenz, & Shin, in
hospital stays may find different routes to meaning. Poor, press). This seems like a natural, ipsative way of mak-
illiterate, homeless, or migrant people may be most ing meaning more salient and tangible for people. More
likely to benefit from a photographic research method. research is needed, but there is a distinct possibility that
Insight into their experiences would benefit meaning in through the act of trying to understand meaning in peo-
life research tremendously, yet these populations have ple’s lives through photography, researchers may give
been almost completely neglected. people a way to gain greater appreciation of their
Future research should strive to cross-cultural meanings in life.
divides. Photographic methods of exploring sources of
meaning might succeed where linguistic bridges fail, as
photographs hold communicative power that is distinct Conclusion
from narrative. Even in our small, university sample, sur- Meaning in life research has made great progress using
prising subcultural nuances seemed to appear. For exam- self-report questionnaires and surveys. Regardless of
ple, participant 19 takes pride in the hard work of how sound the psychometric properties of a survey may
farming. Among people who do farm work, what is it be, however, surveys may only provide a ‘snapshot’ of
precisely that fosters a sense of meaning in life and does perceived meaning in life, at a certain point in time,
the sources of meaning vary, or function similarly, across under influences of potential errors. Complementing sur-
cultural context? Getting a photographic tour of the vey research with qualitative investigations into meaning
meaning in the life of a Maasai herdsman, or a Peruvian in life offers a way to remediate the limitations of survey
miner, or a Cambodian river merchant could open up research, by allowing examinations of people’s meaning
new worlds of insight. of meaningfulness in greater depth, as it is experienced
Given the rapid proliferation of personal, photogra- and lived. By balancing the sizeable quantitative litera-
phy-enabled technology, such as cell phones, tablets, and ture about meaning in life with qualitative approaches,
The Journal of Positive Psychology 541

we may reach a more clear understanding of the multi- Ebersole, P., & DeVogler-Ebersole, K. (1986). Meaning in life
faceted nature of meaningfulness itself, including what of the eminent and the average. Journal of Social Behavior
and Personality, 1, 83–94.
makes meaning meaningful.
Emmons, R. A. (1999). The psychology of ultimate concerns:
Motivation and spirituality in personality. New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
References Emmons, R. A. (2003). Personal goals, life meaning, and virtue:
Wellsprings of a positive life. In C. L. M. Keyes & J. Haidt
Battista, J., & Almond, R. (1973). The development of (Eds.), Flourishing (pp. 105–128). Washington, DC: APA.
meaning in life. Psychiatry, 36, 409–427. Esterling, B. A., Antoni, M. H., Fletcher, M. A., Margulies, S.,
Baum, S. K., & Stewart, R. B., Jr. (1990). Sources of meaning & Schneiderman, N. (1994). Emotional disclosure through
through the lifespan. Psychological Reports, 67, 3–14. writing or speaking modulates latent Epstein-Barr virus
Blinn, L., & Harrist, A. W. (1991). Combining native instant antibody titers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
photography and photo-elicitation. Visual Anthropology, 4, Psychology, 62, 130–140.
175–192. Fegg, M. J., Kramer, M., L’hoste, S., & Borasio, G. D. (2008).
Braunstein, S., & Ebersole, P. (1992). Categories of life mean- The Schedule for Meaning in Life Evaluation (SMiLE):
ing for service organization volunteers. Psychological Validation of a new instrument for meaning-in-life research.
Reports, 70, 281–282. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 35, 356–364.
Buchanan, D. A. (2001). The role of photography in organiza- Frankl, V. E. (1963). Man’s search for meaning: An
tion research: A reengineering case illustration. Journal of introduction to logotherapy. New York, NY: Washington
Management Inquiry, 10, 151–164.
Downloaded by [Dicle University] at 00:51 14 November 2014

Square Press.
Clark, C. D. (1999). The autodriven interview: A photographic Grady, J. (2008). Visual Research at the Crossroads. Forum:
viewfinder into children’s experiences. Visual Sociology, Qualitative Social Research, 9, Art. 38. Retrieved from
14, 39–50. http://nbnresolving.de/urn:nbn:de:0114-fqs0803384
Collier, J. (1957). Photography in anthropology: A report on Harper, D. (2002). Talking about pictures: A case for photo
two experiments. American Anthropologist, 59, 843–859. elicitation. Visual Studies, 17, 13–26.
Collier, J., & Collier, M. (1986). Visual anthropology: Photog- Jenerson-Madden, D., Ebersole, P., & Romero, A. M. (1992).
raphy as a research method. Albuquerque, NM: University Personal life meaning of Mexicans. Journal of Social
of New Mexico Press. Behavior & Personality, 7, 151–161.
Creswell, J. W. (2007). Qualitative inquiry and research Keyes, C. L. M., Shmotkin, D., & Ryff, C. D. (2002).
design: Choosing among five approaches (2nd ed.) Thou- Optimizing well-being: The empirical encounter of two
sand Oaks, CA: Sage. traditions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Crumbaugh, J. C., & Maholick, L. T. (1964). An experimental 82, 1007–1022.
study in existentialism: The psychometric approach to Maddi, S. R. (1970). The search for meaning. In M. Page
Frankl’s concept of noogenic neurosis. Journal of Clinical (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 137–186).
Psychology, 20, 200–207. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Debats, D. L. (1999). Sources of meaning: An investigation McCarthy, S. V. (1983). Geropsychology: Meaning in life for
of significant commitments of in life. Journal of Humanis- adults over seventy. Psychological Reports, 53, 497–498.
tic Psychology, 39, 30–57. doi:10.1177/0022167899394003 McCarthy, S. V. (1985). Geropsychology: Meaning in life for
DePaola, S. J., & Ebersole, P. (1995). Meaning in life catego- Elderhostelers. Psychological Reports, 56, 351–354.
ries of elderly nursing home residents. International Jour- Owens, G. P., Steger, M. F., Whitesell, A. A., & Herrera, C. J.
nal of Aging and Human Development, 40, 227–236. (2009). Relationships among posttraumatic stress disorder,
DeVogler, K. E., & Ebersole, P. (1980). Categorization of col- guilt, and meaning in life for military veterans. Journal of
lege students’ meaning of life. Psychological Reports, 46, Traumatic Stress, 22, 654–657.
387–390. doi:10.2466/pr0.1980.46.2.387 O’Conner, K., & Chamberlain, K. (1996). Dimensions of life
DeVogler, K. E., & Ebersole, P. (1981). Adults’ meaning in meaning: A qualitative investigation at mid-life. British
life. Psychological Reports, 49, 87–90. Journal of Psychology, 87, 461–477.
DeVogler, K. E., & Ebersole, P. (1983). Young adolescents’ Park, C. L., & Folkman, S. (1997). Meaning in the context
meaning in life. Psychological Reports, 52, 427–431. of stress and coping. Review of General Psychology, 1,
Dollinger, S. J. (2002). Physical attractiveness, social connect- 115–144.
edness, and individuality: An autographic study. The Jour- Prager, E. (1996). Exploring personal meaning in an age-differ-
nal of Social Psychology, 142, 25–32. entiated Australian sample: Another look at the Sources of
Ebersole, P. (1998). Types and depth of written life meaning. Meaning Profile (SOMP). Journal of Aging Studies, 10,
In P. T. P. Wong & P. S. Fry (Eds.), The human quest for 117–136.
meaning: a handbook of psychological research and clini- Prager, E., Savaya, R., & Bar-Tur, L. (2000). The development
cal applications (pp. 179–191). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence of a culturally sensitive measure of sources of life meaning.
Erlbaum. In G. T. Reker & K. Chamberlain (Eds.), Exploring exis-
Ebersole, P., & DePaola, S. J. (1987). Meaning in life catego- tential meaning (pp. 123–136). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
ries of later life couples. The Journal of Psychology, 121, Reker, G. T. (1996). Manual of the sources of meaning profile-
185–191. doi:10.1080/00223980.1987.9712656 revised (SOMP-R). Peterborough, ON: Student Psychologist
Ebersole, P., & DePaola, S. J. (1989). Meaning in life depth in Press.
the active married elderly. Journal of Psychology: Interdis- Reker, G. T., & Wong, P. T. P. (1988). Aging as an individual
ciplinary and Applied, 123, 171–178. process: Toward a theory of personal meaning. In J. E. Bir-
Ebersole, P., & DeVogler, K. L. (1981). Meaning in life: ren & V. L. Bengston (Eds.), Emergent theories of aging
Category self-ratings. The Journal of Psychology, 107, (pp. 214–246). New York, NY: Springer.
289–293.
542 M.F. Steger et al.

Riley, R. G., & Manias, E. (2004). The uses of photography in seeking and experiencing meaning. Journal of
clinical nursing practice and research: A literature review. Personality, 76, 199–228.
Journal of Advanced Nursing, 48, 397–405. Steger, M. F., Mann, J., Michels, P., & Cooper, T. (2009).
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Meaning in life, anxiety, depression, and general health
Explorations of the meaning of psychological well-being. among smoking cessation patients. Journal of Psychoso-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1069–1081. matic Research, 67, 353–358.
Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of well- Steger, M. F., Oishi, S., & Kashdan, T. B. (2009). Meaning in
being revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- life across the life span: Levels and correlates of meaning
ogy, 69, 719–727. in life from emerging adulthood to older adulthood.
Schnell, T. (2009). The sources of meaning and meaning in Journal of Positive Psychology, 4, 43–52.
life questionnaire (SoMe): Relations to demographics and Steger, M. F., Shim, Y., Barenz, J., & Shin, J. Y. (in press)
well-being. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4, 483– Through the windows of the soul: A pilot study using pho-
499. tography to enhance meaning in life. Manuscript under
Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer- review.
Schatka, H., & Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1990). Basics of qualitative research:
information: Another look at the availability heuristic. Jour- Grounded theory procedures and techniques. Newbury
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 195–202. Park, CA: Sage.
Steger, M. F. (2009). Meaning in life. In S. J. Lopez (Ed.), Oxford Taylor, S. J., & Ebersole, P. (1993). Meanings in life in young
handbook of positive psychology (2nd ed., pp. 679–687). children. Psychological Reports, 73, 1099–1104.
Oxford: Oxford University Press. Thompson, N. J., Coker, J., Krause, J. S., & Henry, E. (2003).
Downloaded by [Dicle University] at 00:51 14 November 2014

Steger, M. F. (2012). Experiencing meaning in life: Optimal Purpose in life as a mediator of adjustment after spinal cord
functioning at the nexus of spirituality, psychopathology, injury. Rehabilitative Psychology, 48, 100–108.
and well-being. In P. T. P. Wong, & P. S. Fry (Eds.), The VanAuken, P. M., Frisvoll, S. J., Stewart, S. I. (2010). Visualis-
human quest for meaning (2nd ed., pp. 165–184). New ing community: Using participant-driven photo-elicitation
York, NY: Routledge. for research and application. Local Environment, 15, 373–
Steger, M. F., & Frazier, P. (2005). Meaning in life: One link 388.
in the chain from religion to well-being. Journal of Wang, C., & Burris, M. A. (1997). Photovoice: Concept,
Counseling Psychology, 52, 574–582. methodology, and use for participatory needs assessment.
Steger, M. F., Frazier, P., Oishi, S., & Kaler, M. (2006). The Health Education & Behavior, 24, 369–387.
meaning in life questionnaire: Assessing the presence of Wong, P. T. P. (1998). Implicit theories of meaningful life and
and search for meaning in life. Journal of Counseling the development of the Personal Meaning Profile. In P. T.
Psychology, 53, 80–93. P. Wong, & P. S. Fry (Eds.), The human quest for mean-
Steger, M. F., Frazier, P., & Zacchanini, J. L. (2008). Terrorism ing: A handbook of psychological research and clinical
in two cultures: Traumatization and existential protective applications (pp. 111–140). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erl-
factors following the September 11th attacks and the baum.
Madrid train bombings. Journal of Trauma and Loss, 13, Wong, P. T. P. (2012). Meaning-in-life measures and develop-
511–527. ment of a brief version of the personal meaning profile. In
Steger, M. F., & Kashdan, T. B. (2007). Stability and P. T. P. Wong (Ed.), The human quest for meaning: Theo-
specificity of meaning in life and life satisfaction over one ries, research, and applications (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
year. Journal of Happiness Studies, 8, 161–179. Routledge.
Steger, M. F., & Kashdan, T. B. (2009). Depression and every- Ziller, R. C. (1990). Photographing the self: Methods for
day socialactivity, intimacy, and well-being. Journal of observing personal orientations. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Counseling Psychology, 56, 289–300. Sage.
Steger, M. F., Kashdan, T. B., Sullivan, B. A., & Lorentz, Ziller, R. C., & Lewis, D. (1981). Orientations: Self, social,
D. (2008). Understanding the search for meaning in life: and environmental percepts through auto-photography.
Personality, cognitive style, and the dynamic between Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7, 338–343.

You might also like