You are on page 1of 10

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/352065258

Comfort zone orientation: Individual differences in the motivation to move


beyond one's comfort zone

Article  in  Personality and Individual Differences · October 2021


DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111024

CITATIONS READS

0 5

2 authors, including:

Nona Kiknadze
University of Miami
3 PUBLICATIONS   0 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Nona Kiknadze on 02 June 2021.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Personality and Individual Differences 181 (2021) 111024

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Comfort zone orientation: Individual differences in the motivation to move


beyond one’s comfort zone
Nona C. Kiknadze , Mark R. Leary *
Duke University, United States of America

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Three studies examined the degree to which people value doing things that push them out of their comfort zone.
Comfort zone Study 1 showed that the boundaries of people’s comfort zones are related to their motives for engaging in a
Approach-avoidance behavior and how they expect to feel if they perform it. Study 2 involved the development of a measure of
Confidence
comfort zone orientation (CZO), the degree to which people value doing things outside of their comfort zone.
Neophilia
Predictors of CZO reflected psychological influences on the value people place on stepping outside their comfort
zone, including a desire for new and stimulating experiences, low anxiety, and self-efficacy. Study 3 was a
laboratory experiment that examined the relationship between CZO and responses to an anxiety-producing task.
Participants who valued pushing themselves out of their comfort zone were more confident that they could make
themselves perform tasks that fell outside their comfort zone.

People are sometimes encouraged to do things that make them un­ The interplay between the motives to approach and to avoid is
comfortable or afraid — to push themselves out of their “comfort zone.” complicated by three conflicts that arise in goal pursuit: (a) approach-
They are often told that exciting experiences, new opportunities, and approach conflicts between two potential gratifications (i.e., which
personal growth await if they can step across that line (Newmark, 2017). option will result in greater benefit?), (b) avoidance-avoidance conflicts
Although this advice resonates with many people, no research has between two potential threats (i.e., which option will result in the least
examined the construct of comfort zone in detail or explored how peo­ distress?), and (c) approach-avoidance conflicts between a potential
ple’s beliefs about comfort zones relate to personality or behavior. This gratification and threat (i.e., will the benefits of an action outweigh the
project was designed to take an initial look at how people think about costs?) (Corr & Krupić, 2017).
comfort zones. At their core, decisions whether to do something that lies outside
Approach and avoidance motivations lie at the heart of most one’s comfort zone involve an approach-avoidance conflict in which
behavior (Carver, 2006). In fact, the decision to approach or withdraw people weigh whether behaving in a manner that creates anxiety is
from stimuli may be the most basic behavioral choice for all organisms worth the potential benefits. People appear to regard a behavior as
(Corr & Krupić, 2017; Davidson, 1992; Kenrick & Shiota, 2008). Simply outside their comfort zone when they believe they must exert conscious
put, approach and avoidance lie at the heart of most responses, effort to override avoidance motivation based on fear of negative con­
reflecting motives that direct behavior toward positive or desirable sequences. Thus, when talking about behaviors that lie outside their
stimuli and outcomes and away from negative or undesirable ones comfort zone, people generally refer to behaviors they have difficulty
(Elliot, 2010). Conceptualizations of approach and avoidance appear in making themselves perform, such as risky activities (such as sky-diving
many motivational approaches in philosophy and psychology (Bentham, or mountain climbing), difficult interpersonal situations (e.g., giving a
1779/1879; Freud, 1915; Skinner, 1938; Thorndike, 1898), and the speech or dancing in public), or pursuing ambitious goals that might
nature of approach and avoidance has also been examined in research on result in failure (e.g., starting a business or running for office). In many
the neurological basis of the approach and avoidance systems, as in work cases, the perceived risks associated with behaviors deter or completely
on the behavioral inhibition and activation systems (BIS/BAS; Gray & inhibit such behaviors, whereas in other cases, people are able to
McNaughton, 2000), approach/withdrawal (Davidson, 1993), and perform such behaviors with resolve and effort.
appetitive and aversive systems (Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1997). Not all approach-avoidance decisions involve comfort zones. People

* Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708, United States of America.
E-mail address: leary@duke.edu (M.R. Leary).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.111024
Received 17 August 2020; Received in revised form 18 May 2021; Accepted 20 May 2021
0191-8869/© 2021 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
N.C. Kiknadze and M.R. Leary Personality and Individual Differences 181 (2021) 111024

invoke the notion of comfort zone only when a behavior they desire to scale (1 = in my comfort zone, 2 = slightly out of my comfort zone,
perform evokes anxiety because of potentially negative outcomes. Peo­ 3 = moderately out of my comfort zone, 4 = way out of my comfort zone,
ple may be motivated to avoid behaviors for other reasons – for example, 5 = entirely out of my comfort zone). Participants then rated their confi­
because they are boring, time-consuming, or expensive – and they may dence that they could perform the behavior (1 = I can do it with confi­
have no desire to perform other behaviors that seem dangerous, irra­ dence, 2 = I am willing to do it despite being uncomfortable, 3 = I am not able
tional, or otherwise undesirable. People invoke the construct of comfort to make myself do the behavior), followed by their interest in performing
zone only when they otherwise desire to perform a behavior but are the behavior (1 = no interest in doing it at all, 5 = extremely interested in
deterred by avoidance motives. doing it).
Nor is the concept of comfort zone isomorphic with simply being Participants who indicated they had some interest in performing the
afraid versus unafraid to perform a particular behavior. Although people behavior (ratings of 2–5) were prompted to explain why they wished
colloquially talk about certain behaviors or activities being “inside” or they could perform this behavior. Participants then rated how they
“outside” their comfort zone, implying that they are making a binary believed they would feel while performing this behavior or activity on
judgment, experiences of threat or hesitancy lie on a continuum of 24 items (1 = not at all, 5 = extremely): nervous, scared; distressed, upset;
anxiety and avoidance. But research has not examined the factors that invigorated, energized; reckless, careless; happy, glad; inspired, enthu­
influence where along this continuum people draw the line that de­ siastic; bold, brave; irrational, stupid; curious, interested; confident, self-
marcates the boundary of their comfort zone or determine which assured; good, positive feelings; calm, relaxed; bad, negative feelings;
anxiety-producing events fall inside vs. outside people’s comfort zones. safe, secure; satisfied, pleased; powerless, helpless; empowered, strong;
People seem to distinguish most clearly between “low-risk” activities hesitant, uncertain; determined, purposeful; discouraged, dejected;
that clearly lie inside their comfort zone and “high risk” activities that lie impulsive, spontaneous; vulnerable, at-risk; pleased with myself, proud;
so far outside their comfort zone that they regard the behaviors as and incompetent, inept. These items were generated by examining
impossible. Yet, this binary distinction ignores a “discomfort zone” be­ measures such as the PANAS-X (Watson & Clark, 1994) and the Adjec­
tween these extremes. Whereas the comfort zone involves behaviors tive Check List (Gough & Heilbrun Jr., 1983) for terms reflecting af­
people can perform with little or no anxiety or avoidance, the discomfort fective reactions and self-views that might arise when people attempt to
zone consists of desired behaviors that require tenacity and conscious do things outside of their comfort zone. Finally, participants explained
effort to override avoidance motivation. In the discomfort zone, people why they found this behavior or activity difficult to perform.
must decide whether behaving in a manner that creates anxiety is worth
the potential benefits. 1.1.2.2. Comfort zone attitudes. Participants rated how important it was
This project involved three studies. Study 1 examined how people for them to push themselves to do things that were outside of their
conceptualize behaviors or activities that lie in and out of their comfort comfort zone (1 = not at all, 5 = extremely) and explained their answer in
zone. Based on the results of Study 1, Study 2 examined factors that a phrase. Participants then rated how often they try to push themselves
predict the degree to which people value stepping outside their comfort out of their comfort zone (1 = never or almost never, 6 = as much as I can)
zone. Study 3 was a lab experiment in which participants faced tasks that and how their comfort zone compares in size to other people’s (1 = my
fell outside their comfort zone. comfort zone is much smaller, 7 = my comfort zone is much larger). Par­
ticipants then rated themselves on nine characteristics: outgoing,
1. Study 1: a “proof of concept” investigation dominant, bold, active, cautious, open-minded, responsible, rational,
easy-going (1 = not at all, 5 = extremely).
The primary goals of Study 1 were simply to determine whether
people can articulate the degree to which actions are in or out of their 1.1.2.3. Behavior coding. The behaviors that participants identified as
comfort zone and, if so, to identify the kinds of behaviors and activities falling outside their comfort zone were coded into seven categories that
for which the concept of comfort zone is relevant. The results of Study 1 reflected physical danger/fear (i.e., skydiving, bungee jumping), fear of
were then used in the design of Studies 2 and 3. negative evaluation (i.e., public speaking, meeting strangers), being
alone (i.e., eating alone at a restaurant, traveling alone), fear of failure
(i.e., starting a business and failing), behaving in a way contrary to one’s
1.1. Method values (i.e., lying, having indiscriminate sex), socially difficult situations
(i.e., ending a relationship), and “other.” Responses could be classified
1.1.1. Participants into multiple categories. A second rater coded 50% of the responses, and
An a priori power analysis using G*Power indicated that a minimum inter-rater agreement was 98%.
sample of 194 was needed to detect a bivariate correlation of 0.20 with Participants’ reasons for wanting to perform these behaviors were
α = 0.05 and β = 0.80 (2-tailed test). Three-hundred and nine in­ coded into four categories: believing life would improve for themselves
dividuals (162 male, 137 female) were recruited from Amazon’s Me­ (i.e., I would become more confident or outgoing), enjoyment (i.e.,
chanical Turk (MTurk) to complete a 10-min online study, for which enjoying the rush of skydiving), and believing that life would improve
they received $2.00 USD. Participants ranged in age from 18 to 77 for other people (i.e., being able to advocate more effectively for a
(M = 34.16, SD = 10.71), but age was not related to any of the results. cause); participants who indicated that they were not interested in
performing the target behavior were classified into a fourth, no-interest
1.1.2. Materials and procedure category. Responses could be included in multiple categories. Inter-rater
reliability, based on an independent coding of 50% of the responses, was
1.1.2.1. Behavior-specific questions. To be certain that we obtained the 94%.
broadest possible range of behaviors that fall outside people’s comfort Participants’ explanations for why the behavior was difficult to
ones, participants were randomly assigned to one of two sets of in­ perform were coded into five categories: fear of injury or death, negative
structions. Half of the participants were asked to provide an example of a social evaluation, avoidance of discomfort, fear of failure, and “other.”
behavior or activity that was so far outside their comfort zone that they Responses could be classified into multiple categories. Inter-rater reli­
must work very hard to do it, and half were asked to identify a situation ability, based on an independent coding of 50% of the responses, was
that was so far outside of their comfort zone they could never imagine 94%.
themselves doing it.
In both conditions, participants described this specific behavior or
activity and rated how far it was out of their comfort zone on a 5-point

2
N.C. Kiknadze and M.R. Leary Personality and Individual Differences 181 (2021) 111024

1.2. Results b = 0.240, 95% CI[0.073, 0.408], t(296) = 2.83, p = 0.005, sr = 0.13;
Factor 3 (security), b = − 0.449, 95% CI[− 0.605, − 0.293], t
1.2.1. Descriptive statistics (296) = − 5.65, p < 0.001, sr = − 0.26; Factor 4 (recklessness), b = 0.529,
Of the 309 participants, 299 were included in the final analysis. 95% CI[0.367, 0.691], t(296) = 6.43, p < 0.001, sr = 0.29. Controlling
Three participants were eliminated because they indicated they could do for interest in performing the behavior, the four factors still predicted
the target behavior with confidence, and seven participants were the degree to which the behaviors or activities fell in or out of partici­
excluded for not answering the questions properly. pants’ comfort zones, F(4, 295) = 16.90, p < 0.001, ΔR2 = 0.19.
On average, participants reported that the behavior or activity was Participants’ ratings of how far the behavior or activity fell outside
far out of their comfort zone (M = 4.20, SD = 0.84) and rated their their comfort zone correlated with ratings of the relative size of their
confidence in their ability to perform the behavior as relatively low comfort zone compared to other people, r = − 0.19, t(295) = − 2.56,
(M = 2.56, SD = 0.52), falling midway between “I am willing to do it p = 0.011. Participants who believed that their comfort zone was bigger
despite being uncomfortable” and “I am not able to make myself do the than other people’s tended to rate their target behavior or activity as less
behavior.” Participants rated their interest in performing the behavior as far out of their comfort zone.
moderate (M = 2.91, SD = 1.40).
The percentage of behaviors or activities classified into each of the 1.2.3. Personality characteristics
seven categories were, in descending order: social evaluation (45.2%), A principal axis factor analysis was conducted on participants’ rat­
danger/fear (30.4%), difficult social situations (8.4%), fear of failure ings of their personal characteristics, revealing three factors
(7.4%), miscellaneous responses (“other,” 6.0%), performing a behavior (eigenvalues = 3.06, 1.71, and 1.00, with the fourth eigenvalue = 0.78).
alone (5.4%), and violation of values (4.0%). When asked why the The pattern matrix (oblimin rotation) showed that Factor 1 reflected
behavior was difficult, the categories in which the responses fell were: characteristics associated with boldness, including bold (0.82), domi­
fear of negative evaluation (47.8%), fear of injury or death (28.8%), nant (0.80), outgoing (0.42), and active (0.42). Factor 2 reflected a
avoidance of discomfort (17.4%), fear of failure (9.0%), and “other” reasoned, open-minded, non-emotional response to decisions; the
(3%). highest loading items were rational (0.73), responsible (0.63), easy-
Participants’ reasons for wanting to perform the behaviors were going (0.46), and open-minded (0.39). Factor 3 reflected cautiousness,
classified into three categories: believing that life would improve for with cautious (0.54) and outgoing (− 0.47) loading highly. The three
themselves (44.1%), enjoyment of the behavior or experience (37.8%), factors accounted for 64.2% of the item variance.
and believing that life would improve for other people (6.4%); 23.7% of Standardized factor scores were created for each of the factors. A
participants responded that they did not want to perform the behavior. regression analysis was conducted using the three factor scores to pre­
dict the importance that participants placed on pushing themselves out
1.2.2. Feelings associated with behaving outside one’s comfort zone of their comfort zone. Only Factor 3 (cautiousness) predicted the degree
A principal axis factor analysis with an oblimin rotation was con­ to which participants thought it was important to push themselves
ducted on participants’ ratings of how they would feel performing the beyond their comfort zone, b = − 0.299, 95% CI[− 0.482, − 0.116], t
behavior or activity. Eigenvalues indicated four factors (300) = 48.64, p < 0.001, sr = − 0.24.
(eigenvalues = 10.08, 3.26, 1.78, and 1.28; the next highest
eigenvalue = 0.80). The rotated pattern matrix showed that Factor 1 1.2.4. Effects of instruction prompt
reflected positive emotions: items with the highest loadings (>0.40) were As noted, participants were instructed to identify a behavior or ac­
pleased (0.90), satisfied (0.83), happy (0.82), inspired (0.80), deter­ tivity that was so far outside of their comfort zone that either they must
mined (0.79), curious (0.76), good (0.75), bold (0.71), empowered work very hard to do it or they could never imagine doing it. A 2
(0.71), invigorated (0.69), and confident (0.49). Factor 2 reflected low (prompt) by 2 (gender) analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted on
self-efficacy, with ratings of feelings such as discouraged (0.73), participants’ ratings of (a) how far the target behavior was outside their
incompetent (0.71), bad (0.60), distressed (0.54), powerless (0.51), and comfort zone, (b) confidence in their ability to perform the behavior,
hesitant (0.41) loading highly. Factor 3 reflected security on which the and (c) their interest in performing the behavior.1 A main effect of in­
highest loading ratings were safe (0.77), calm (0.76), nervous (− 0.67), struction was obtained on ratings of both how far the behavior was out
vulnerable (− 0.59), and hesitant (− 0.51). Factor 4 reflected reckless­ of one’s comfort zone, F(1, 292) = 14.62, p < 0.001, and confidence in
ness, which involved items such as reckless (0.78) and impulsive (0.47). one’s ability to perform the behavior, F(1, 292) = 24.11, p < 0.001.
These four factors accounted for 68.36% of the variance in the items. Participants who responded to the “cannot do” prompt rated the
A simultaneous multiple regression analysis using standardized fac­ behavior as farther out of their comfort zone (M = 4.39, SE = 0.07, 95%
tor scores for these four factors as predictors accounted for 18.4% of the CI [4.26, 4.52]) than those in the “hard-to-do” condition (M = 4.03,
variance in ratings of how far the behaviors or activities were out of SE = 0.07, 95% CI [3.90, 4.16]). Participants in the “hard-to-do” con­
participants’ comfort zone, F(4, 296) = 16.71, p < 0.001. Factor 3 (se­ dition (M = 2.71, SE = 0.04, 95% CI [2.63, 2.79]) rated their confidence
curity) was most strongly related to ratings of the degree to which the that they could perform the behavior higher than those in the “cannot
behaviors were out of participants’ comfort zones, b = 0.264, 95% CI do” condition (M = 2.43, SE = 0.04; 95% CI [2.35, 2.51]). (Higher
[− 0.368, − 0.159], t(296) = 4.97, p < 0.001, sr = − 0.26. Significant values indicated lower confidence.) The relationship between instruc­
effects were also obtained for Factor 2 (low self-efficacy), b = 0.131, tion prompt and interest in performing the behavior was not significant,
95% CI[0.019, 0.242], t(296) = 2.30, p = 0.022, sr = 0.12, and Factor 4 indicating that whether participants believed they could successfully
(recklessness), b = − 0.124, 95% CI[− 0.233, − 0.016], t(296) = 2.27, perform the behavior was not related to their interest in doing it.
p = 0.024, sr = − 0.12, but not Factor 1 (positive emotions). In addition, women rated the behavior as farther out of their comfort
A second simultaneous regression analysis examined the relationship zone (M = 4.35, SE = 0.07, 95% CI [4.22, 4.49]) than men (M = 4.06,
between the four factors and participants’ interest in performing the SE = 0.06, 95% CI [3.94, 4.19]), F(1, 292) = 9.37, p = 0.02, and rated
behavior. The factors accounted for 38.5% of the variance in partici­ their confidence in performing the behavior lower as well
pants’ ratings of how much they wished they could do the behavior, F(4, (Mwomen = 2.66, SE = 0.04, 95% CI [2.58, 2.74]; Mmen = 2.47, SE = 0.04,
296) = 46.17, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.385. All four emotional factors
accounted for unique variance in participants’ interest in performing the
behavior. Factor 1 (positivity) was the strongest predictor of interest, 1
Gender was included in the analysis because males tend to engage in more
b = 1.037, 95% CI[0.880, 1.194], t(296) = 12.99, p < 0.001, sr = 0.59. risky behaviors than females (Byrnes, Miller, & Schafer, 1999), raising the
Factors 2, 3, and 4 were also significant: Factor 2 (low self-efficacy), possibility that gender may moderate the effects of comfort zone orientation.

3
N.C. Kiknadze and M.R. Leary Personality and Individual Differences 181 (2021) 111024

95% CI [2.40, 2.55], with higher values indicating lower confidence), F 2.1. Method
(1, 292) = 10.57, p = 0.01. The relationship between gender and interest
in performing the behavior was not significant. 2.1.1. Participants
An a priori power analysis using G*Power indicated that a sample of
1.3. Discussion 194 was needed to detect a bivariate correlation of 0.20 with α = 0.05
and β = 0.80 (2-tailed). Two-hundred participants (100 women) were
Study 1 demonstrated that participants’ answers to questions about recruited from MTurk to complete a 20-min online questionnaire that
the degree to which behaviors and activities fall out of their comfort assessed attitudes toward pushing oneself out of one’s comfort zone and
zone related to their feelings, motives, and self-ratings in conceptually relevant personality variables. Participants received $2.50.
meaningful ways. Participants clearly understood the concept of “com­
fort zone” and provided meaningful data about their own comfort zone. 2.1.2. Materials and procedure
The fact that participants’ ratings of how they expected to feel when
performing the target behavior accounted for variance in ratings of how 2.1.2.1. Comfort zone orientation. The authors developed 16 statements
far the behavior was out of their comfort zone raises the possibility that that, on the face of it, appeared to assess the degree to which people
people make inferences about what is inside versus outside of their value pushing themselves out of their comfort zone, items such as “I
comfort zone by imagining how they would feel if they performed the value being the kind of person who pushes myself to do things even if
behavior. The reasons that participants gave for wanting to perform they make me anxious or uncomfortable.” Participants rated these items
behaviors out of their comfort zone—believing life would improve for on a 5-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree). The order of
themselves or other people, or that they would enjoy the behavior or items was randomized for each participant.
activity—sheds light onto the motivations underlying the approach-
avoidance conflict that informs comfort zone decisions. Although per­ 2.1.2.2. Personality measures. Participants then completed the
sonality was assessed with only a few items, the results provided pre­ following scales in randomized order.
liminary evidence that personal characteristics, specifically The Brief Sensation-Seeking Scale (8 items; Hoyle, Stephenson,
cautiousness, are associated with the degree to which people think it is Palmgreen, Lorch, & Donohew, 2002) assesses the degree to which
important to push themselves beyond their comfort zone. people enjoy unpredictable and risky activities and situations (e.g., I
would love to have new and exciting experiences, even if they are illegal;
2. Study 2: comfort zone orientation I prefer friends who are excitingly unpredictable). The 5-point response
scale ranged from strongly disagree to strongly agree.
Study 1 showed that people differ not only in the behaviors that they The Behavioral Activation System/Behavioral Inhibition System
regard as being out of their comfort zone but also in their beliefs about Scale (BIS/BAS Scale; 24 items; Carver & White, 1994) assesses the
the importance of pushing themselves to do things beyond their comfort degree to which people regulate aversive motives in order to move away
zone. To study these differences further requires a measure that assesses from things that are unpleasant (BIS) and appetitive motives in order to
the value that people place on pursuing potentially uncomfortable ex­ move toward things that are desired (BAS). The 4-point response scale
periences. Building from the results of Study 1, the goals of Study 2 were ranges from very true for me to very false for me.
to develop a measure of comfort zone orientation—the value that people The Brief Trait Anxiety Scale (10 items; Spielberger, Gorsuch, &
place on pushing themselves out of their comfort zone—and to take an Lushene, 1970) measures the degree to which people experience anxi­
initial look at characteristics that may predict people’s comfort zone ety. Self-descriptive items such as “worry about things” and “am afraid
orientation. of many things” are rated on a 5-point scale (1 = very inaccurate and
Specifically, we identified five constructs that seem conceptually 5 = very accurate.)
related to the degree to which people value going outside their comfort The Generalized Self-Efficacy Scale (10 items; Schwarzer & Jerusa­
zone. First, we assessed sensation-seeking, which involves pursuing lem, 1995) measures how well people believe they can cope with daily
“varied, novel, complex, and intense sensations and experiences, and the challenges and adapt to stressful life events (e.g., I am confident that I
willingness to take physical, social, legal, and financial risks for the sake could deal efficiently with unexpected events; It is easy for me to stick to
of such experience” (Zuckerman, 1994, p. 27), which often requires my aims and accomplish my goals). Items are answered on a 4-point
people to do things outside their comfort zone. Conversely, people with scale (1 = not at all true, 4 = exactly true).
an active behavioral inhibition system (Gray & McNaughton, 2000) or The Neophilia Scale (12 items; Walker & Gibbins, 1989) assesses the
who are high in trait anxiety (Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970) degree to which people seek novelty in various facets of their lives (e.g.,
should be less likely to engage in behaviors that may lead to aversive People who know me seem to think I am into strange and unusual
outcomes. We also thought that a higher general sense of self-efficacy things). The response scale ranges from 1 = strongly disagree to
might be associated with comfort zone orientation because people 5 = strongly agree.
who believe that they can deal with challenging and stressful situations Three scales measured the degree to which people are willing to try
should be more willing to step outside their comfort zone (Schwarzer & new foods and take risks in what they eat: the Food Neophobia Scale (10
Jerusalem, 1995). Finally, neophilia – the tendency to enjoy and seek items; Pliner & Hobden, 1992), Food Variety-Seeking Scale (6 items;
out novel experiences – should be related to comfort zone orientation Van Trijp & Steenkamp, 1992), and Willingness to Try Foods Scale (10
(Walker & Gibbins, 1989). items, created for this study). The Food Neophobia Scale (e.g., “I am
Although we predicted that these characteristics were related to constantly sampling new and different foods”) and the Food Variety-
comfort zone orientation, we expected that the value that people place Seeking Scale (“I think it is fun to try out food items one is not
on pushing themselves out of their comfort zone reflects a distinct familiar with”) are answered on 5-point scales. The Willingness to Try
construct that predicts unique variance in people’s comfort zone be­ Foods Scale measures willingness to try unfamiliar foods (e.g., Wicker­
haviors. To determine whether comfort zone orientation has incre­ flow; Frickers). Items are measured on a 6-point scale (1 = I would refuse
mental validity beyond the Big 5 personality traits, we also administered to try it; 6 = I would try it).
a measure of the Big 5. The Big Five Inventory-2 (BFI-2) contains 60 items, with 12-item
subscales that assess extraversion, emotional stability, agreeableness,
conscientiousness, and openness to experience (Soto & John, 2017).
Items were answered on a 5-point scale (1 = disagree strongly; 5 = agree

4
N.C. Kiknadze and M.R. Leary Personality and Individual Differences 181 (2021) 111024

strongly). Table 2
Personality predictors of comfort zone orientation (Study 2).
2.1.2.3. Behavior-specific questions. As in Study 1, participants provided Mean SD Cronbach’s Correlation with
an example of a behavior or activity that falls outside of their comfort alpha CZO
zone and rated how far it was out of their comfort zone (1 = This behavior Extraversion 2.97 0.82 0.89 0.53*
is in my comfort zone, 5 = This behavior is entirely out of my comfort zone). Negative emotionality 2.74 0.98 0.84 − 0.42*
Participants rated their confidence in their ability to perform the Agreeableness 3.73 0.65 0.89 0.23*
Conscientiousness 3.66 0.75 0.93 0.24*
behavior (1 = absolutely certain that I can make myself do it, 5 = absolutely
Openness 3.85 0.74 0.88 0.39*
certain that I cannot make myself do it) and explained why they found this Trait anxiety 2.89 0.96 0.93 − 0.44*
behavior or activity difficult. Participants also rated their interest in Sensation-seeking 2.88 0.83 0.82 0.47*
performing the target behavior (1 = no interest in doing it at all, Self-efficacy 3.02 0.55 0.91 0.50*
5 = extremely interested in doing it). Participants who rated their interest 2 BAS — drive 2.72 0.65 0.82 0.33*
BAS — fun-seeking 2.71 0.62 0.74 0.45*
to 5 were prompted to explain why they wished they could perform the BAS — reward 3.25 0.50 0.77 0.28*
behavior. responsiveness
Participants then rated how they would feel while performing this BIS 2.90 0.64 0.85 − 0.41*
behavior or activity on the 24 reactions used in Study 1. Items were Note. n = 213.
presented in a random order to each participant. *
p < 0.001.

2.2. Results
(r = − 0.41), p’s < 0.001.
2.2.1. Comfort zone orientation Although these correlations are consistent with the conceptualiza­
Item analyses were conducted on the 16 items that assessed the value tion of comfort zone orientation and support the construct validity of the
that participants placed on pushing themselves out of their comfort measure, it is possible that CZO is nothing more than a particular
zone. Based on corrected item-total correlations, 10 items were retained configuration of basic traits and offers no additional predictive power in
for the final measure. Each item correlated at least 0.50 with the sum of its own right. To assess whether the CZO Scale provides incremental
the other items, and Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.88. Notably, the validity in predicting the degree to which people do things outside their
item with the highest item-total correlation (r = 0.71) reflects the comfort zone, a multiple regression analysis was conducted. On Step 1,
conceptualization of comfort zone orientation most clearly: “I value extraversion, emotional stability, agreeableness, conscientiousness,
being the kind of person who pushes myself to do things even if they openness, trait anxiety, sensation-seeking, self-efficacy, BAS, and BIS
make me anxious or uncomfortable.” After reverse-scoring the reverse- scores were entered as predictors of participants’ ratings of how often
worded items, participant’s ratings were summed as an index of their they try to push themselves out of their comfort zone, accounting for
comfort zone orientation. The Comfort Zone Orientation (CZO) Scale is 39.7% of the variance, F(12, 199) = 10.903, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.397.
shown in Table 1. When CZO scores were added in Step 2, they accounted for an additional
11% of the variance in how often people try to push themselves out of
2.2.2. Correlations with personality measures their comfort zone, b = 0.833, 95% CI[0.590, 1.076], F(1,
Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was acceptable (α > 0.70) for all per­ 199) = 45.647, p < 0.001, ΔR2 = 0.113.
sonality scales (Table 2). Of the Big Five traits, scores on the CZO Scale Furthermore, with all 11 measures in the regression equation, CZO
correlated most highly with extraversion (r = 0.53), negative emotion­ was the only scale that predicted unique variance in participants’ rat­
ality (r = − 0.42), and openness (r = 0.39), p’s < 0.001. Correlations ings. b = 0.833, t(198) = 6.756, p < 0.001, sr2 = 0.113, although
were smaller between CZO and agreeableness (r = 0.23) and conscien­ sensation-seeking approached significance, b = 0.224, t(198) = 1.860,
tiousness (r = 0.24), p’s < 0.001. p = 0.064, sr2 = 0.009 (p’s for all other predictors > 0.15).
As predicted, CZO scores correlated positively with sensation-
seeking (r = 0.47), self-efficacy (r = 0.50), and all three subscales of 2.2.3. Correlations with behaviors outside One’s comfort zone
the Behavioral Activation Scale (drive, r = 0.33; fun seeking, r = 0.45; Cronbach’s alpha exceeded 0.70 for the four neophilia measures. As
reward responsiveness, r = 0.28), p’s < 0.001. CZO scores correlated shown in Table 3, CZO correlated positively with both the willingness to
negatively with trait anxiety (r = − 0.44) and scores on the BIS Scale try new and different types of foods (food neophobia, r = 0.36; food
variety-seeking, r = 0.35; willingness to try new foods, r = 0.27) and a
Table 1 general preference for novel things (neophilia, r = 0.27).
Comfort Zone Orientation Scale.
1. I make myself to do things that make me nervous because it’s important that I face
my fears. 2.3. Discussion
2. I value being the kind of person who pushes myself to do things even if they make
me anxious or uncomfortable. The CZO Scale appears to be a potentially useful measure of the value
3. I rarely do things that would take me out of my comfort zone. (R)
4. I try not to let fear discourage me from doing something that I want to do.
5. Feeling anxious or uncomfortable about something is an indication that I probably Table 3
shouldn’t do it. (R) Manifestations of comfort zone orientation (Study 2).
6. Even when there is a risk of danger or embarrassment, making myself do things that
Mean SD Cronbach’s Correlation with
are out of comfort zone is often rewarding.
alpha CZO
7. I don’t feel the need to do things that make uncomfortable just for the sake of
pushing myself beyond my comfort zone. (R) Food neophobia 3.58 0.94 0.93 0.36*
8. I have no desire to do things that take me out of my comfort zone. (R) Food variety-seeking 3.54 0.92 0.90 0.35*
9. Purposefully pushing myself beyond my comfort zone helps me grow as a person. Willingness to try new 2.91 1.24 0.92 0.27*
10. I don’t understand people who like doing things that take them out of their comfort foods
zone. (R) Neophilia 3.06 0.56 0.75 0.27*

Note. Response scale: 1 = strongly disagree, 2 = moderately disagree, Note. n = 213.


*
3 = neither agree nor disagree, 4 = moderately agree, 5 = strongly agree. Items p < 0.001.
marked (R) are reverse-scored before summing.

5
N.C. Kiknadze and M.R. Leary Personality and Individual Differences 181 (2021) 111024

that people place on stepping outside of their comfort zones. The pat­ 3.1.2. Procedure
terns of correlations between the personality measures and CZO Scale
are consistent with its conceptualization and support the construct 3.1.2.1. Personality measures. Participants completed the 10-item CZO
validity of this scale. measure described in Study 2, followed by the following scales in ran­
The predictors of CZO appear to fall into three broad categories that domized order: trait anxiety (Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970),
might reflect three distinct psychological influences on the degree to sensation seeking (Hoyle et al., 2002), self-efficacy (Schwarzer & Jeru­
which people value stepping out of their comfort zone. The first is the salem, 1995), and BIS/BAS (Carver & White, 1994). These measures
degree to which people enjoy and seek stimulating and novel experi­ were administered as in Study 2.
ences, as reflected in correlations with sensation-seeking, BAS, extra­
version, and openness. People who are motivated to do things outside 3.1.2.2. Task description. Because people differ in the activities that fall
their comfort zone may enjoy the pleasure or adrenalin rush associated outside their comfort zones, we identified 11 laboratory tasks based on
with doing so, find many otherwise frightening experiences rewarding, the domains of the examples participants provided in Study 1, namely
and/or be open to new experiences. tasks that involved danger/fear, negative evaluation, failure, and
The second set of predictors involves measures of negative affect, avoidance of discomfort. The tasks were also chosen to be analogous to
anxiety, and avoidance – specifically, emotional stability (neuroticism), situations one might encounter in everyday life, such as approaching a
BIS, and trait anxiety – or, conversely, a low tendency to experience threatening animal, trying an unknown food, or revealing personal in­
threat. Not surprisingly, people who are less likely to experience anxiety formation. We assumed that all participants would regard some of the
are more inclined to do new and potentially threatening things. tasks as outside their comfort zone.
Third, the correlations between CZO, self-efficacy, and extraversion The 11 tasks, with the verbatim descriptions presented to partici­
may reflect the possibility that people are more likely to step outside of pants, were:
their comfort zone if they believe that they can handle challenging sit­
uations. Along these lines, extraverts tend to be higher in overall self- • Karaoke. You will sing karaoke to a popular song. You will be given
efficacy (Rottinghaus, Lindley, Green, & Borgen, 2002). Although pop­ the lyrics to the song and sing along with a recorded backing track as
ular notions paint comfort zone as one-dimensional—essentially the if you are on stage.
ability to face one’s fears—the patterns of correlations suggest that the • Personal struggle. You will talk about a personal struggle you have
approach-avoidance judgments that define people’s comfort zones are had.
dynamic and may involve at least three distinct processes. Importantly, • Dancing. You will dance to popular music.
scores on the CZO Scale involve more than simply whether or not people • Cockroach. You will put your hand in a cage with a hissing cockroach.
do risky or novel things. Although the cockroach is large, it is harmless.
• Solving problems. You will solve challenging anagram problems.
3. Study 3: behavioral evidence • Eating insects. You will eat fried insects.
• Odors. You will smell exceptionally disgusting odors.
Study 3 was designed with two goals in mind. First, it examined the • Compliment in mirror. You will compliment yourself out loud while
relationship between CZO and people’s reactions to performing behav­ staring at yourself in a mirror.
iors that fall outside of their comfort zone. Participants were confronted • Photographs. You will look at disturbing photographs (of car crashes
with the prospect of engaging in potentially challenging tasks such as and dead animals, for example).
eating fried insects, dancing in front of other people, and handling a • Dark box. You will place your hand into the opening of a dark box,
large cockroach. Although we expected that CZO would relate to par­ touch the object inside, and describe what you think it is to the
ticipants’ ability to perform these tasks, it was not clear whether CZO researcher.
would also be related to how people felt about doing these tasks. Are • Blindfold. You will be blind-folded and sit quietly in the lobby of the
people with a positive comfort zone orientation more comfortable doing lab as people come and go.
things outside their comfort zone, or are they just as distressed as low
CZO people yet more willing to try? Participants were told they would perform two of these tasks for
The second goal was to test the hypothesis that performing a chal­ 2 min each, so the length of the tasks would not influence their judg­
lenging task that lies within one’s “discomfort zone” – a task people can ments of task difficulty. Participants were presented with each task in
do despite feeling uncomfortable – increases their confidence in their random order and asked to rate how they would feel if they had to
ability to subsequently perform a task they view as even more chal­ perform the task (1 = extremely comfortable; 9 = extremely uncomfortable)
lenging and uncomfortable. The boundaries of people’s comfort zones and how easy it would be to perform the task (1 = extremely easy;
are based both on the perceived risk of the behavior and people’s beliefs 9 = extremely difficult). Participants then rated all tasks (1 = I could do
about their ability to handle the associated risk. Presumably, these this behavior easily, 4 = I could not make myself do this behavior).
boundaries are influenced by people’s beliefs about their capabilities to Participants were randomly assigned to one of two conditions. In the
behave successfully in specific contexts—that is, by their self-efficacy easy task condition, participants were assigned to perform one of the
(Bandura, 1994). Performing an action that falls in one’s “discomfort tasks they had rated “1,” indicating they could do the behavior easily. In
zone” might increase people’s confidence in performing an action that the difficult task condition, participants were assigned one of the tasks
falls further outside their comfort zone. they had rated “2,” indicating they could do this behavior despite being
uncomfortable.

3.1. Method 3.1.2.3. First task performance. Participants were taken to another room
in which materials needed for the tasks were visible to emphasize that
3.1.1. Participants the participant would actually have to complete the assigned tasks. After
An a priori power analysis using G*Power indicated that a minimum completing the first task, participants rated how uncomfortable they had
sample of 102 was needed to detect a moderate effect (d = 0.50) with felt (1 = extremely comfortable; 9 = extremely uncomfortable) and how
alpha = 0.05 and power = 0.80 (1-tailed test). Participants were 102 difficult it was to perform the task (1 = extremely easy; 9 = extremely
university students (76 women, 22 men, 4 not recorded) who completed difficult).
a 30-min laboratory experiment. Participants received either $7.00 or
required class credit for participating.

6
N.C. Kiknadze and M.R. Leary Personality and Individual Differences 181 (2021) 111024

3.1.2.4. Second task performance. All participants were then assigned a Table 5
task they had previously rated “3” in difficulty, indicating they found it Tasks ranked from least to most uncomfortable (Study 3).
difficult to do (i.e., it was out of their comfort zone). Participants rated Task Discomfort Difficulty
how comfortable they expected to feel while performing the task
Mean SD Correlation Mean SD Correlation
(1 = extremely comfortable; 9 = extremely uncomfortable) and how easy it with CZO with CZO
would be to make themselves perform the task (1 = extremely easy;
Anagrams 3.17 1.76 − 0.25** 3.13 2.12 − 0.26**
9 = extremely difficult). Participants also rated the likelihood that they Talk about 4.68 2.32 − 0.08 4.28 2.31 − 0.17
could make themselves do the task (1 = I am absolutely certain that I can self
make myself do this task, 5 = I am absolutely certain that I cannot make Dance 4.79 2.54 − 0.17 4.30 2.47 − 0.20*
myself do this task). After answering these questions, participants were Compliment 4.89 2.50 − 0.11 4.02 2.24 − 0.21*
in mirror
told that they would not perform the second task and were fully
Blindfold in 5.08 2.35 − 0.17 3.49 2.17 − 0.21*
debriefed. lobby
Hand in dark 5.54 2.25 − 0.14 4.56 2.35 − 0.30**
box
3.2. Results Photographs 5.58 2.33 − 0.17 4.32 2.44 − 0.21*
Karaoke 6.00 2.44 − 0.19 5.46 2.51 − 0.23*
3.2.1. CZO and personality Smell odors 6.18 2.09 − 0.14 5.23 2.42 − 0.14
Eat fried 6.90 2.37 0.14 6.61 2.54 0.14
Cronbach’s alpha was sufficiently high for all measures (α’s > 0.70). − −
insects
As shown in Table 4, correlations between CZO and the personality Cockroach 7.36 2.04 − 0.06 6.74 2.38 − 0.30**
measures replicated the patterns in Study 2. CZO correlated positively
Note. n = 108.
with sensation-seeking (r = 0.46), self-efficacy (r = 0.58), and the three *
p < 0.05.
subscales of the Behavioral Activation Scale (drive, r = 0.34; fun
seeking, r = 0.51; reward responsiveness, r = 0.42), p’s < 0.001. CZO **
p < 0.01.
correlated negatively with trait anxiety (r = − 0.37) and scores on the
Behavioral Inhibition Scale (r = − 0.29), p’s < 0.001. CZO scores did not
differ by gender F(1, 96) = 0.042, p = 0.837.
(M = 4.09, 95% CI [3.37, 4.81]) than in the easy task condition, M = 2.53,
95% CI [1.96, 3.21]), F(1, 95) = 11.82, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.11.
3.2.2. Initial task ratings
However, after performing Task 1, participants in the difficult task
CZO correlated with the sum of participants’ initial ratings of the
condition moderated their difficulty ratings, viewing the task as less
discomfort they expected to feel while doing the 11 tasks (r = − 0.28)
difficult than they had expected, M = 3.41, 95% CI [2.56, 4.26], whereas
and the sum of their ratings of how difficult it would be for them to do
ratings in the easy task condition increased, M = 2.92, 95% CI[2.18,
the tasks (r = − 0.39). Importantly, when both task discomfort and dif­
3.68]. As a result, after participants performed the first task, the dif­
ficulty were entered simultaneously into a multiple regression analysis
ference in ratings of difficulty between conditions was no longer sig­
predicting CZO scores, difficulty ratings were related to CZO, b = − 0.19,
nificant, F(1, 95) = 0.61, p = 0.435, R2 = 0.01. This finding suggests
95% CI[− 0.310, − 0.070], t(105) = − 3.14 p = 0.002, but discomfort
that, although the task assignment was based on participants’ prior
ratings were not, b = 0.03, 95% CI[− 0.099, 0.162], t(105) =0.48
expectations of task difficulty, those in the difficult task condition did not
p = 0.63. Thus, CZO predicted expected difficulty but not expected
end up perceiving the first task as significantly more difficult. As a result,
discomfort.
the study does not provide an adequate test of the hypothesis that per­
Table 5 shows mean ratings of discomfort and difficulty for the 11
forming a difficult first task will increase people’s ability to perform a
tasks, along with correlations between CZO and the discomfort and
more challenging second task. However, it does suggest that people’s
difficulty ratings of each task. CZO correlated with only one of the 11
expectations regarding the difficulty of threatening tasks may be
discomfort ratings but with 8 of the 11 difficulty ratings, again sug­
inflated.
gesting that CZO involves the perceived difficulty of making oneself
As expected given that all participants were assigned a subjectively
engage in a challenging behavior rather than simply expected discom­
equivalent task for Task 2, participants in the easy and difficult task
fort or anxiety.
conditions did not differ in how difficult they expected the second task to
be, F(1, 95) = 0.39, p = 0.530, R2 = 0.00.
3.2.3. Ratings of Task 1
A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was conducted in which
3.2.4. Ratings of Task 2
CZO score (mean-centered), experimental condition (dummy-coded),
To test the effects of the effects of the experimental manipulation on
and the interaction of CZO and condition served as predictors of par­
ratings of the second, more difficult task, regression analyses were
ticipant’s a priori ratings of the difficulty of the first task. Only the effect
conducted in which CZO (mean-centered), condition (dummy-coded),
of condition was significant, showing that, as planned, participants rated
and the interaction of CZO and condition were entered as predictors of
the task they were assigned as more difficult in the difficult task condition
ratings of (a) expected discomfort while doing the second task, (b) ex­
pected difficulty of the second task, and (c) whether participants
Table 4 thought they could make themselves do the task.
Personality correlates of comfort zone orientation (Study 3). For ratings of how uncomfortable participants expected to feel, none
Mean SD Cronbach’s Correlation with of the effects were significant: CZO, b = 0.054, 95% CI[− 0.566, 0.674],
alpha CZO score F(1, 96) = 0.03, p = 0.864; condition, b = − 0.412, 95% CI[− 1.072,
Trait anxiety 2.93 0.79 0.88 − 0.37 0.248], F(1, 95) = 1.54, p = 0.218; CZO by condition interaction,
Sensation-seeking 3.34 0.77 0.81 0.46 b = − 0.093, 95% CI[− 1.340, 1.155], F(1, 94) = 0.02, p = 0.883.
Self-efficacy 3.14 0.41 0.88 0.58
Similarly, none of the effects were significant for participants’ ratings of
BAS — drive 2.81 0.55 0.77 0.34
BAS — fun-seeking 2.96 0.50 0.63 0.51 how difficult it would be to perform the task: CZO, b = 0.230, 95% CI
BAS — reward 3.46 0.40 0.71 0.42 [− 0.457, 0.918], F(1, 96) = 0.44, p = 0.508; condition, b = 0.233, 95%
responsiveness CI[− 0.503, 0.970], F(1, 95) = 0.40, p = 0.530; interaction, b = − 0.002,
BIS 3.11 0.50 0.78 − 0.29 95% CI[− 1.394, 1.389), F(1, 94) = 0.001, p = 0.997.
Note. n = 108. All correlations are significant, p < 0.01. However, on participants’ ratings of the extent to which they thought

7
N.C. Kiknadze and M.R. Leary Personality and Individual Differences 181 (2021) 111024

they could make themselves do the task, the main effect of CZO was through negative relationships with negative emotionality, BIS (Carver
significant, showing that participants who valued doing things outside & White, 1994), and trait anxiety (Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene,
their comfort zone believed they were more likely to make themselves 1970); and (c) a belief in one’s ability to handle challenging situations (i.
do the second task, b = − 0.323, 95% CI[− 0.602, − 0.043], F(1, e., self-efficacy; Schwarzer & Jerusalem, 1995). The fact that CZO is
96) = 5.25, p = 0.024, sr = − 0.23. Neither the main effect of condition, related to these three clusters of characteristics suggests that judgments
b = 0.140, 95% CI[− 0.159, 0.438], F(1, 95) = 0.86, p = 0.355, nor the involving one’s comfort zone involve more than whether people do risky
interaction of CZO and condition was significant, b = 0.267, 95% CI or novel things. These pathways represent further areas for research into
[− 0.294, 0.829], F(1, 94) = 0.90, p = 0.347. Thus, although CZO was the process and determinants of comfort zone formulation. And, the
not related to how uncomfortable participants felt or how difficult the Comfort Zone Orientation Scale appears to offer a useful measure of
second task seemed, it was related to whether participants believed that individual differences in how people think about comfort zones.
they could make themselves do it. Study 3 demonstrated the relevance of CZO for predicting how
people respond to real situations. Although CZO was not fundamentally
3.3. Discussion related to the perceived difficulty or discomfort associated with a dis­
comfiting task, it was clearly related to the degree to which people
Study 3 confirmed the utility of CZO in understanding people’s re­ believed they could make themselves perform such tasks. The more
actions to performing behaviors outside of their comfort zone. CZO was people value being the kind of person who pushes themselves to do
unrelated to ratings of how difficult and uncomfortable the tasks were, things outside their comfort zone, the more they think they can do so.
yet participants high in CZO indicated that they were more likely to
make themselves do the tasks. So, although a task may seem equally 5. Limitations and directions for future research
challenging regardless of the value one places on going outside one’s
comfort zone, CZO is clearly related to people’s perception that they can As the first examination of comfort zone orientation, this research is
perform potentially distressing behaviors. necessarily limited in scope and coverage, and we do not wish to over­
Although ratings of discomfort and difficulty are naturally corre­ state our conclusions. Furthermore, although Studies 2 and 3 provided
lated, when their relationships to CZO were examined simultaneously, strong support for the validity of the new measure of CZO used in this
only perceived difficulty was significant. The key distinction between research, more psychometric work is needed. In addition, all measures
people who are low versus high in CZO lies in the perceived difficulty of in this research were self-reports, which, while essential, have short­
doing tasks and not in how uncomfortable they feel. This finding is comings. Although Study 3 actually confronted participants with tasks
consistent with the notion that CZO is not about being unafraid of doing that fell outside their comfort zones, all outcomes involved self-reports,
certain things but rather involves people’s efforts to act in spite of their owing to the ethical issues involved in requiring research participants to
fear. try to do things that they report make them extremely anxious. Future
Although we hypothesized that first performing a difficult task research will need to develop creative behavioral measures to study
would increase confidence in people’s ability to face a second, harder comfort zones.
task, the differences between conditions were not significant. However, Given that this is the first research to focus expressly on the construct
analyses revealed that the difficulty ratings after performing the first of comfort zone orientation, many questions call out for additional
task did not differ between conditions as intended, which might explain study. Perhaps the most pressing is to locate CZO within the nomological
the failure to obtain an effect of condition on reactions to the second network of conceptually-related constructs. For example, people’s be­
task. Because participants in the difficult task condition did not perceive liefs about the value of stepping outside their comfort zone is likely
the first task as more difficult than those in the easy task condition – related to their coping styles (and possibly to interactions among task-
even though they had initially rated the tasks more difficult – the study oriented, emotion-oriented, and avoidance-oriented coping; Endler,
does not provide an adequate test of the self-efficacy hypothesis. 1997), ways of maximizing satisfaction with their behavioral decisions
(i.e., utility maximization), promotion and prevention regulatory focus,
4. General discussion and risk-taking.
Second, research is needed on the degree to which comfort zones –
These studies offer several initial insights into the psychology of and the value people place on transcending them – can be changed.
comfort zones and suggest ideas for future research. In addition to Although Study 3 attempted to address whether comfort zones can be
exploring how people conceptualize behaviors and activities that lie affected by increasing self-efficacy, more research is needed to deter­
outside their comfort zone (Study 1), the studies showed that the value mine whether being pushed out of one’s comfort zone can change
people place on pushing themselves out of their comfort zone is related comfort zone boundaries by either making people more comfortable
to aspects of personality (Study 2) and that comfort zone orientation with similar types of activities (i.e., practicing public speaking to
relates to people’s reactions to performing a task that lies outside their become a more confident speaker) or expanding people’s comfort zones
comfort zone (Study 3). using tasks from other domains, thereby increasing their overall confi­
Study 1 showed that participants were able articulate the degree to dence in their ability to handle challenging situations (i.e., skydiving
which behaviors and activities fall in or out of their comfort zone, and might increase one’s confidence to face unrelated fears about public
their answers related to their feelings, motives, and self-ratings. speaking).
Furthermore, the results suggested that the boundaries of people’s One of the most fascinating topics for further study involves the
comfort zones may be determined, in part, by how they expect to feel if downstream correlates and outcomes of CZO, such as career choices,
they perform a particular behavior. The diversity of the behaviors and leisure activities, and life satisfaction as research could test the scientific
activities that participants identified as relevant to their comfort zone merit of popular advice that encourages people to push themselves out
shows that comfort zone represent a broad psychological framework of their comfort zone. Such claims would be supported if future studies
that people apply to a wide range of experiences. showed that people who frequently push out of their comfort zone are
Study 2 involved the development of a measure of the degree to more successful, knowledgeable, or happier than those who do not.
which people value doing things outside their comfort zone. The pre­ Although facing challenging situations may have certain benefits,
dictors of CZO fell into three broad categories that involved: (a) enjoying constantly living in a zone of discomfort is probably not viable, so people
and seeking stimulating and novel experiences, such as sensation- must choose when they will and will not try to do things outside their
seeking (Hoyle et al., 2002), BAS (Carver & White, 1994), extraver­ comfort zone, raising questions about how they make these choices.
sion, and openness; (b) low tendency to experience threat, as shown People appear to use approach versus avoidance strategies flexibly

8
N.C. Kiknadze and M.R. Leary Personality and Individual Differences 181 (2021) 111024

depending on the situation (Monni et al., 2020), and future research Davidson, R. J. (1992). Prolegomenon to the structure of emotion: Gleanings from
neuropsychology. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 245–268.
could explore how comfort zone orientation moderates this process.
Davidson, R. J. (1993). Parsing affective space: Perspectives from neuropsychology and
Understanding the factors that underlie the approach-avoidance psychobiology. Neuropsychology, 7, 464–475.
decisions inherent in comfort zone deliberations also has the potential Elliot, A. (2010). Approach and avoidance motivation and achievement goals.
to improve outcomes in situations in which anxiety inhibits needed Educational Psychologist, 34, 169–189.
Endler, N. S. (1997). Stress, anxiety and coping: The multidimensional interaction model.
action or effective performance. This would have implications in several Canadian Psychology, 38(3), 136–153.
areas, including clinical cases in which changing the way people view Freud, S. (1915). Repression. Complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 19).
anxiety-provoking situations can improve their ability to function London: Hogarth, 1957.
Gough, H. G., & Heilbrun, A. B., Jr. (1983). Adjective check list manual. Palo Alto, CA:
(Parekh, 2017). A better understanding of comfort zones might also help Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.
people live more rewarding lives by helping them pursue important Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2000). In 2nd ed. (Ed.), The neuropsychology of anxiety:
goals or perform desired behaviors in the face of risk. An enquiry into the function of the septo-hippocampal system. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Finally, future investigations could examine whether cultural dif­ Hoyle, R. H., Stephenson, M. T., Palmgreen, P., Lorch, E. P., & Donohew, R. L. (2002).
ferences play a role in CZO. People in different cultures likely differ in Reliability and validity of a brief measure of sensation seeking. Personality and
both the degree to which they value pushing themselves to do things Individual Differences, 32, 401–414.
Kenrick, D. T., & Shiota, M. N. (2008). Approach and avoidance motivation(s): An
outside their comfort zone as well as the specific behaviors that fall in evolutionary perspective. In Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation. New
and out of the comfort zone. York: Psychology Press.
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). Motivated attention: Affect,
activation, and action. In Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational processes
6. Conclusion
(pp. 97–135). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Monni, A., Olivierb, E., Morin, A. J. S., Olivetti Belardinellia, M., Mulvihilld, K., &
Comfort zones involve psychological mechanisms that influence Scalase, L. F. (2020). Approach and avoidance in Gray’s, Higgins’, and Elliot’s
people’s decisions about whether performing anxiety-provoking be­ perspectives: A theoretical comparison and integration of approach-avoidance in
motivated behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 166.
haviors is worth the potential benefits. Although popular conceptuali­ Newmark, A. (2017). Chicken soup for the soul: Step outside your comfort zone: 101 stories
zations paint comfort zones as dichotomous go/no-go decisions, comfort about trying new things, overcoming fears, and broadening your world. Cos Cob, CT:
zones are, in fact, dynamic in the sense that they involve people’s Chicken Soup for the Soul.
Parekh, R. (Ed.). (2017, January). What are anxiety disorders? Retrieved April 18, 2018,
willingness to engage in tasks that make them uncomfortable, which is from https://www.psychiatry.org/patients-families/anxiety-disorders/what-are-
affected by the value they place in doing so. This research demonstrates anxiety-disorders.
that comfort zones show promise as a relevant construct for under­ Pliner, P., & Hobden, K. (1992). Development of a scale to measure the trait of food
neophobia in humans. Appetite, 19, 105–120.
standing many approach-avoidance conflicts in everyday life. Rottinghaus, P. J., Lindley, L. D., Green, M. A., & Borgen, F. H. (2002). Educational
aspirations: The contribution of personality, self-efficacy, and interests. Journal of
CRediT authorship contribution statement Vocational Behavior, 61, 1–19.
Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1995). Generalized self-efficacy scale. In J. Weinman,
S. Wright, & M. Johnston (Eds.), Measures in health psychology: A user’s portfolio.
Nona Kiknadze: Conceptualization, Methodology, Investigation, Causal and control beliefs (pp. 35–37). Windsor, UK: NFER-NELSON.
Formal analysis, Writing – original draft. Mark R. Leary: Conceptuali­ Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. New York:
Appleton-Century.
zation, Methodology, Investigation, Formal analysis, Writing – review &
Soto, C. J., & John, O. P. (2017). The next Big Five Inventory (BFI-2): Developing and
editing, Supervision. assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance bandwidth, fidelity, and
predictive power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113, 117–143.
References Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual for the state-trait
anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative
Bandura, A. (1994). Self-efficacy. In V. S. Ramachaudran (Ed.), Vol. 4. Encyclopedia of processes in animals. Psychological Monographs, 8.
human behavior (pp. 71–81). New York: Academic Press. Van Trijp, H. C. M., & Steenkamp, J. B. (1992). Consumers’ variety seeking tendency
Bentham, J. (1779/1879). Introduction to the principles of morals and legislation. Oxford: with respect to foods. European Review of Agricultural Economics, 19(2), 181–195.
Clarendon Press. Walker, I., & Gibbins, K. (1989). Expecting the unexpected: An explanation of category
Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: A width? Perceptual and Motor Skills, 68(3), 715–724.
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 367–383. Watson, D., & Clark, A. L. (1994). The PANAS-X: Manual for the positive and negative affect
Carver, C. S. (2006). Approach, avoidance, and the self-regulation of affect and action. schedule — Expanded form. University of Iowa: Department of Psychological & Brain
Motivation and Emotion, 30, 105–110. Sciences Publications. https://doi.org/10.17077/48vt-m4t2.
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. New
affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. York: Cambridge University Press.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333.
Corr, P. J., & Krupić, D. (2017). Motivating personality: Approach, avoidance, and their
conflict. In A. J. Elliot (Ed.), Vol. 4. Advances in motivation science (pp. 39–90). New
York: Academic Press.

View publication stats

You might also like