You are on page 1of 10

Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 263–272

Relations among perceived parental rearing behaviors,


attachment style, and worry in anxious children
Amy M. Brown *, Stephen P. Whiteside
Department of Psychiatry & Psychology, Mayo Clinic, 200 First Street SW, Rochester, MN 55905, USA
Received 14 September 2006; received in revised form 9 February 2007; accepted 19 February 2007

Abstract
The present study extended the findings of Muris et al. [Muris, P., Meesters, C., Merckelbach, H., & Hulsenbeck, P. (2000).
Worry in children is related to perceived parental rearing and attachment. Behavior Research and Therapy, 38, 487–497] regarding
the relations between perceived parental rearing behaviors, self-reported attachment style, and worry in a community sample to a
clinical sample of anxious children. Sixty-four children and adolescents, aged 7–18 years, with a primary anxiety disorder
completed (a) the EMBU-C, a questionnaire measuring perceptions of parental rearing behaviors, (b) a single-item measure of
attachment style, and (c) an index of worry severity. Findings revealed that child rated parental rearing behaviors, particularly
parental rejection, were positively related to child worry. Self-reported attachment style was also related to worry, such that children
who classified themselves as ambivalently attached reported higher levels of worry than did children who classified themselves as
securely attached. Parenting style and attachment were found to make independent contributions to worry. The results are compared
to those from Muris et al.’s community study, and implications for future research are discussed.
# 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Childhood anxiety disorders; Worry; Attachment; Parental rearing behaviors

Current conceptualizations view anxiety disorders as characteristics, such as temperament, anxiety symp-
a combination of biological (Hettema, Prescott, Myers, toms, modeling of anxious behavior, and parental
crianza
Neale, & Kendler, 2005; Stoppel, Albrecht, Pape, & rearing behaviors, are thought to contribute to child-
Stork, 2006; Weissman, 1993) and psychosocial factors hood anxiety problems. To evaluate this model, studies
(Albano, Chorpita, & Barlow, 2003; see Rapee, 1997 for have utilized a variety of assessment strategies and
a review; Whaley, Pinto, & Sigman, 1999). Over the identified several parental rearing behaviors associated
past few decades, researchers have increasingly focused with childhood anxiety. The present study aimed to
on the role that parents play in the development and investigate the use of a self-report questionnaire
maintenance of anxiety disorders in children and measuring children’s perceptions of parental rearing
adolescents (e.g., Cobham, Dadds, & Spence, 1999; behaviors, the EMBU (Swedish acronym for ‘‘My
Siqueland, Kendall, & Steinberg, 1996). Parental memories of upbringing’’) for children (EMBU-C;
Castro, Toro, Van der Ende, & Arrindell, 1993; Gruner,
Muris, & Merckelbach, 1999), with clinically anxious
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 507 284 5849;
children. Although the EMBU-C has been successfully
fax: +1 507 284 4158. used to explore associations among children’s worry
E-mail address: brown.amy@mayo.edu (A.M. Brown). and perceptions of parental rearing behaviors in

0887-6185/$ – see front matter # 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.02.002
264 A.M. Brown, S.P. Whiteside / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 263–272

community studies, it has not yet been adequately difference regarding perceived parental overprotection
evaluated in a clinical population of anxious children. between the groups. This was surprising given the
numerous studies describing high levels of parental
1. Parental rearing behaviors overprotection in OCD patients (Merkel, Pollard,
Wiener, & Staebler, 1993; Turgeon et al., 2002). One
In a review of the literature on the relation between such study completed by Turgeon et al. compared
parenting and childhood anxiety, Wood, McLeod, outpatients with panic disorder with agoraphobia
Sigman, Hwang, and Chu (2003) concluded that (PDA), outpatients with OCD, and non-anxious con-
childhood anxiety is most consistently associated with trols. Findings were similar among patients with OCD
three parenting dimensions: parental control (i.e., and PDA, such that they were more likely to rate their
overprotectiveness), acceptance, and modeling of parents (both mothers and fathers) as more over-
anxious/avoidant behavior. Indeed, empirical research protective compared to healthy controls. In contrast, no
has found that parents of anxious children are more differences emerged between the anxious and non-
overprotective (Messer & Beidel, 1994; Silverman, anxious groups regarding perceived rejection or
Cerny, & Nelles, 1988), less accepting (Scott, Scott, & emotional warmth. Thus, results have been contra-
McCabe, 1991; Moore, Whaley, & Sigman, 2004), and dictory and often criticized for the retrospective nature
less granting of psychological autonomy (Barrett, of the data. Indeed, recollections of parental practices
Shortt, & Healy, 2002; Siqueland et al., 1996) than experienced during childhood are limited not only due
parents of non-anxious children. In particular, through to anxious symptoms but also the duration of time since
direct observation of parent–child interactions, Sique- childhood that may bias their recall.
land et al. found that parents of anxious children were While structured parent–child interaction tasks and
less tolerant of differences of opinion, demonstrated retrospective studies can provide useful information,
less respect for the child’s views, and exhibited more clinicians working with anxious children would benefit
judgmental or dismissive reactions, than parents of non- from the ease and clinical utility of child self-report
anxious children. In the study by Moore et al., mothers questionnaires that assess children’s perceptions of
of anxious children, regardless of their own level of parental rearing behaviors. Such information would
anxiety, were less warm compared to mothers of non- have significant implications for the treatment of
anxious children. The authors concluded that maternal childhood anxiety at the family level. To date, few
criticism and rejection play a significant role in the self-report measures have been developed (e.g., EMBU-
development of childhood anxiety. Other studies, such C; Castro et al., 1993; Gruner et al., 1999; e.g., CRPBI-
as those by Barrett and colleagues (see Dadds & Barrett, 30; Schludermann & Schludermann, 1970), and
1996 for a review), found that parents of anxious empirical data to support their use with clinically
children model anxious behaviors and maladaptive anxious children are sparse (Bogels, van Oosten, Muris,
problem solving strategies that lead to behavioral & Smulders, 2001).
avoidance in children (Barrett, Rapee, Dadds, & Ryan, In an effort to offer a child report measure of parental
1996; Chorpita, Albano, & Barlow, 1996). rearing behaviors, the adult version of the EMBU
To date, most research examining anxious indivi- (Perris, Jacobsson, Lindstrom, von Knorring, & Perris,
duals’ perceptions of their parents’ rearing behaviors 1980), a retrospective measure pertaining to the
has been retrospective. While most studies have childhoods of adults, was modified for use with
demonstrated differences in perceived parental rearing children. The EMBU-C (Castro et al., 1993; Gruner
behaviors between individuals with and without anxiety et al., 1999) is a child-report questionnaire assessing
disorders, specific findings have often been contra- four parental rearing behaviors: emotional warmth,
dictory (Alonso et al., 2004; Turgeon, O’Connor, overprotection/control, anxious rearing, and rejection.
Marchand, & Freeston, 2002). For example, Alonso Community studies by Muris et al. using the EMBU-C
et al. recently employed the EMBU to assess recall of have generally demonstrated significant and positive
parental rearing behaviors among OCD adult out- associations between perceived parental anxious rear-
patients and healthy controls. Patients with OCD ing, rejection, and control, on the one hand, and
reported higher levels of rejection from their fathers children’s anxiety symptoms, on the other hand (Gruner
during their childhood as compared to healthy controls. et al., 1999; Muris & Merchelbach, 1998).
In addition, low levels of parental emotional warmth Conversely, studies with clinical samples of children
partially predicted the presence of hoarding symptoms have not found significant differences. For instance, no
among OCD patients. The authors did not find a associations were found between parental rearing
A.M. Brown, S.P. Whiteside / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 263–272 265

behaviors and children’s internalizing problems among as well as associated psychosocial problems (e.g.,
a diverse clinical sample of children with internalizing behavior problems, social withdrawal; Kirsh & Cassidy,
and externalizing disorders (Muris, Bogels, Meesters, 1997; Rubin & Mills, 1991). Similarly, retrospective
van der Kamp, & van Oosten, 1996). Notably, this study research found that anxious adults are more likely than
had a small sample of children with anxiety disorders non-anxious adults to describe themselves as insecurely
(n = 14) and combined the EMBU-C into two factors, attached to their caregivers during childhood (Myhr,
positive and negative rearing behavior, rather than Sookman, & Pinard, 2004). Further, based on long-
examining the original four subscales. In another study itudinal data, Warren, Huston, Egeland, and Sroufe
that compared a diverse clinical group of high socially (1997) found that adolescents who were classified as
anxious children to a low socially anxious clinical group anxiously and resistantly (i.e., ambivalently) attached as
and a healthy control group, Bogels et al. (2001) infants were two times more likely to meet criteria for
revealed differences, although not statistically signifi- an anxiety disorder (e.g., social phobia, generalized
cant, in the expected direction on parental rejection and anxiety disorder) than adolescents previously classified
emotional warmth from the EMBU-C. Again, this study with secure or avoidant attachment. Further, ambivalent
was comprised of a diverse clinical group (e.g., anxiety attachment was found to be a stronger predictor for the
disorder, ADHD, mood disorder) and relatively small presence of an anxiety disorder in adolescence than both
group sizes (n = 20). Therefore, further research using maternal anxiety symptoms and infant temperament,
the EMBU-C with children diagnosed with anxiety suggesting that ambivalent attachment influenced the
disorders is warranted in order to determine the clinical likelihood of an anxiety disorder to a greater degree
utility of this questionnaire and to guide interpretation than these individual factors.
of scores among this population. In an effort to combine the literature on attachment
and parenting in anxiety, Muris, Meesters, Merck-
1.1. Attachment and parenting elbach, and Hulsenbeck (2000) examined the relation-
ship between perceived parental rearing behaviors and
One way in which parental rearing behaviors affects children’s reported attachment style, on the one hand,
children’s worry and anxiety is through the parent–child and children’s level of worry, on the other hand.
attachment relationship (Bowlby, 1973; Cassidy, 1994). Findings from a community sample of school-aged
Children are characterized as securely or insecurely children revealed that children who classified them-
attached based on the extent to which parent–child selves as avoidantly or ambivalently attached reported
interactions bestow a sense of security and confidence higher levels of worry than children who classified
in the child (Thompson, 2004). With secure parent– themselves as securely attached. Similarly, children’s
child attachment, infants feel confident in their rating of parental rejection and anxious rearing were
caregiver’s ability to respond to their needs, while with related to higher levels of worry. Moreover, parental
insecure attachment, infants experience a sense of behaviors and attachment style contributed indepen-
uncertainty and anxiety about the caregiver’s avail- dently to child worry. This study demonstrated how
ability and assistance (Cassidy & Berlin, 1994; family environmental factors contribute to children’s
Thompson, 1999, 2004). Attachment theorists (Ains- anxiety symptoms. To date, these relations have not
worth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; Bowlby, 1973) been studied among clinical samples of anxious
have described two main types of insecure attachment: children.
avoidant and ambivalent attachment styles. Avoidantly
attached infants and young children tend to have 1.2. The present study
mothers who frequently reject them when they seek
contact. This leads the child to purposely avoid contact The current study sought to replicate and extend
with their caregivers upon reunion as a means to cope findings by Muris, Meesters, et al. (2000) by testing
with the rejection. Infants with ambivalent attachment whether the relations between two family environ-
relationships tend to have mothers who provide mental factors, children’s attachment style and per-
inconsistent or intrusive caregiving. These infants tend ceived parental rearing behaviors, on the one hand, and
to express both anxious and angry behaviors toward children’s self-reported level of child worry, on the
their caregiver. other hand, endure within a clinical sample of anxious
Empirical research demonstrates that children with children. This study contributes to the field by (a)
insecure attachments, either avoidant or ambivalent, are providing a greater understanding of the relationship
at greater risk for heightened levels of fear and anxiety between worry and family variables within a population
266 A.M. Brown, S.P. Whiteside / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 263–272

of clinically anxious children, and (b) being the first were completed by the children prior to or during their
study to our knowledge to use the EMBU-C with a initial evaluation.
clinical sample comprised solely of anxious children.
Consistent with Muris et al., it was hypothesized that 2.3. Materials
children who classify themselves as insecurely attached
(i.e., avoidant, ambivalent) will report a higher level of 2.3.1. Diagnostic status
worry compared to children who classify themselves as Diagnoses were determined by the interviewing
securely attached. Furthermore, children who perceive psychologist for all patients. All diagnoses were made
their parents as engaging in rejecting, controlling, and consistent with the criteria outlined in the Diagnostic
anxious rearing behaviors will report a higher level of and Statistical Manual-IV-TR (APA, 2000).
worry. Finally, it was hypothesized that step-wise
regressions will reveal that both insecure attachment 2.3.2. Parental rearing
and negative parental rearing behaviors explain The modified version of the EMBU-C (Castro et al.,
significant and separate proportions of the variance of 1993; Gruner et al., 1999) was completed by the
anxious children’s worry. children to assess their current perceptions of their
parents’ rearing behaviors. The questionnaire consists
2. Method of 40 items, which are collapsed into four subscales
representing domains of parental rearing behaviors:
2.1. Participants emotional warmth, overprotection, rejection, and
anxious rearing. Examples of items include: ‘‘When
Sixty-four children (44 males and 20 females) who you are unhappy, your parents console you and cheer
were evaluated in a Child & Adolescent Anxiety you up’’ (emotional warmth), ‘‘When you come home,
Disorders Clinic in a large Midwestern medical center you have to tell your parents what you’ve been doing’’
participated in the present study. All children presented (overprotection), ‘‘If something happens at home,
with an anxiety disorder as their principal diagnosis. you’re the one who gets blamed for it’’ (rejection),
Twenty-four (36.9%) had a principal diagnosis of OCD, and ‘‘Your parents are afraid when you do something on
13 (20%) had GAD, 8 (12.3%) had social phobia, 6 your own’’ (anxious rearing). Items are answered using
(9.4%) had separation anxiety disorder, 4 (6.2%) had a four-point Likert scale (1 = No, never; 2 = Yes, but
specific phobia, 1 (1.5%) had panic disorder, and 8 seldom; 3 = Yes, often; 4 = Yes, most of the time). For
(12.2%) had other anxiety disorders (e.g., anxiety each EMBU-C item, children answered based on their
disorder, NOS). Axis I comorbidity was relatively current perceptions of both parents’ rearing behaviors
common (60.9%), and many children had multiple combined. Cronbach’s alphas for this study were as
anxiety diagnoses (37.5%) and/or an additional follows: emotional warmth = .89, overprotection = .64,
diagnosis of a mood disorder (15.6%). Children ranged rejection = .83, anxious rearing = .81.
from 7 to 18 years old (M = 13.45 years, S.D. = 3.06). A
majority of the children’s parents were married (89.3%) 2.3.3. Children’s worry
and had at least a 2-year college degree (89.3% mothers, The Penn-State Worry Questionnaire for Children
76.8% fathers). The sample was predominantly (PSWQ-C; Chorpita, Tracey, Brown, Collica, &
Caucasian (92.9%). Barlow, 1997) was completed by children to assess
their level of worry. The questionnaire consists of 14
2.2. Setting and procedure items using a 4-point Likert scale (0 = never, 1 = some-
times, 2 = often, 3 = always) with a total score ranging
All children were evaluated in a child anxiety between 0 and 42. Higher scores reflect greater levels of
disorders center within a large Department of Psy- worry.
chiatry and Psychology. The parents and children were
evaluated by a Ph.D.-level psychologist or a Master’s- 2.3.4. Attachment
level therapist who specializes in providing both Taken from Muris, Meesters, et al. (2000), a simplified
consultative and treatment/follow-up services. Children version of Hazan and Shaver’s (1987) prototypic
were referred from a wide variety of sources including descriptions of attachment patterns was employed to
self-referral, physicians, and mental health profes- assess children’s attachment styles. Descriptions given to
sionals with the goal of obtaining diagnostic clarifica- children included: (a) ‘‘I find it easy to become close
tion and treatment recommendations. Questionnaires friends with other children. I trust them and I am
A.M. Brown, S.P. Whiteside / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 263–272 267

comfortable depending on them. I do not worry about significant age difference for the EMBU-C over-
being abandoned or about another child getting too close protective scale, with younger children reporting more
friends with me’’ (secure attachment), (b) ‘‘I am overprotective behaviors from parents than older
uncomfortable to be close friends with other children. children, t(1, 62) = 4.03, p < .001. Second, with regard
I find it difficult to trust them completely, difficult to to gender, no significant gender effects emerged in t-
depend on them. I get nervous when another child wants tests assessing the EMBU-C scales and the PSWQ-C
to become close friends with me. Friends often come total score or for the chi-square test assessing children’s
more close to me than I want them to’’ (avoidant attachment style, all p’s > .05. Given that, with the
attachment), and (c) ‘‘I often find that other children do exception of the overprotective scale of the EMBU-C,
not want to get as close as I would like them to be. I am analyses did not yield child gender or age effects, these
often worried that my best friend doesn’t really like me factors were not included as covariates in further
and wants to end our friendship. I prefer to do everything analyses.
together with my best friend. However, this desire
sometimes scares other children away’’ (ambivalent 3.2. Group means
attachment). Children were provided these descriptions
and instructed to choose the description that applied best Single sample t-tests were conducted to compare the
to them. means for the present study’s sample to those for the
sample in Muris, Meesters, et al. (2000) on the PSWQ-C
3. Results and the EMBU-C. As expected, a significant difference
emerged on the PSWQ-C, such that the mean for the
3.1. Preliminary analyses present study was significantly higher than that for the
community sample in Muris et al., t(1, 61) = 8.26,
Prior to analyzing the main hypotheses of the present p < .001. Prior to comparing means on the EMBU-C,
study, several analyses were conducted to determine an average of the means for each of the subscales of the
whether child gender and age significantly related to EMBU-C in Muris et al. was calculated in order to
children’s perceived parental rearing, worry, and combine children’s separate reports of maternal and
attachment style. First, given the large age range paternal parenting behaviors. Using these averages,
(i.e., 7–18 years old), statistical tests were conducted to results revealed significantly higher means for over-
assess differences between younger children (7–12 protective parenting, t(1, 63) = 5.86, p < .001, anxious
years old, n = 30) and older children (13–18 years old, rearing, t(1, 62) = 2.31, p < .05, and parental rejection,
n = 34). Specifically, results of t-tests revealed no age t(1,63) = 2.05, p < .05, on the EMBU-C for the present
differences for three of the four EMBU-C scales or for study as compared to those for the sample in Muris
the PSWQ-C total score; further, a chi-square test did et al.. No significant difference emerged for emotional
not reveal an age effect for children’s attachment style, warmth across samples. See Table 1 for means and
all p’s > .05. Not surprisingly, a t-test revealed a standard deviations.

Table 1
Means and standard deviations for PSWQ-C, EMBU-C, and attachment measure from Muris, Meesters, et al. (2000) and the present study
Muris et al., total (N = 159) Present study, total (N = 65)
PSWQ-C total 14.65 (5.44) 24.40 (9.15)**
EMBU-C
Anxious rearing 20.90 22.56 (5.75)*
Overprotection 20.06 23.65 (4.83)**
Emotional warmth 30.91 32.34 (6.18)
Rejection 14.70 15.89 (4.55)*
Attachment styles
Secure 130 (81.8%) 42 (70%)
Avoidant 8 (5.0%) 8 (11.7%)
Ambivalent 21 (13.2%) 12 (18.3%)
Note. Means for the EMBU-C from Muris et al. are averages of children’s reports of maternal and paternal parental rearing behaviors.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .001.
268 A.M. Brown, S.P. Whiteside / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 263–272

3.3. Parental rearing behaviors and worry ambivalently attached children, p < .01. Findings did not
yield significant differences between worry scores for
Pearson correlations were computed to examine the avoidantly and ambivalently attached children or for
relations between children’s perceived parental rearing securely and avoidantly attached children. See Table 3 for
behaviors and their level of worry. Results revealed a means.
significant correlation between children’s perceived
parental rejection and worry (r = .34, p < .01). Worry 3.5. Parental rearing behaviors, attachment status,
was not significantly related to any of the other three and worry
scales, although there were trends for perceived anxious
rearing (r = .23, p = .08) and overprotection (r = .21, To examine the relations between the two family
p = .10) to be positively related to children’s worry. environment factors, a series of one-way ANOVAs was
Taken together, this suggests that higher levels of conducted with attachment style as the between-group
rejection and, to a lesser degree, anxious rearing and factor and EMBU-C scales as the dependent variables
overprotection are associated with higher levels of (see Table 3 for means and standard deviations by
worry in clinically anxious children. See Table 2 for attachment style). Results indicated that the attachment
correlations. styles differed with respect to perceived parental
warmth, F(2, 58) = 3.34, p < .05, and anxious rearing,
3.4. Attachment style and worry F(2, 57) = 3.26, p < .05. More specifically, post-hoc
analyses with least significant differences criteria
Similar to Muris, Meesters, et al. (2000), more indicated that ambivalently attached children reported
children endorsed the secure attachment item than less parental warmth than securely attached children,
avoidant and ambivalent attachment items: (a) secure = p < .05. There also was a trend suggesting that
42 (70%, 30 boys, 12 girls), (b) avoidant = 7 (11.7%, 5 avoidantly attached children reported less parental
boys, 2 girls), and (c) ambivalent = 11 (18.3%, 7 boys, 4 warmth than securely attached children, p = .06.
girls). To examine the effect of attachment style on worry, Additionally, ambivalently attached children reported
a one-way ANOVA was conducted. Results yielded a more anxious rearing behaviors than both securely
significant main effect of attachment style, F(2, ( p < .05) and avoidantly attached children ( p < .05).
56) = 5.30, p < .01. Post-hoc analyses with least sig- No difference emerged between securely and avoi-
nificant differences criteria indicated that securely dantly attached children on reports of anxious rearing
attached children endorsed lower levels of worry than behaviors.
Finally, to determine the contributions of both family
Table 2 environment factors to children’s worry, a stepwise
Pearson correlations among PSWQ-C total score and EMBU-C scales regression analysis was conducted with EMBU-C
1 2 3 4 5 scales and attachment status (i.e., transformed into
1. PSWQ-C – .23 .21 .16 .34 *
dummy variable with secure attachment = 0 and
2. Anxious rearing – .61* .16 .09 avoidant/ambivalent attachment = 1) as predictors and
3. Overprotection – .34* .00 PSWQ-C as the dependent variable. Findings indicated
4. Emotional warmth – .58 * that parental rejection (b = .26, p < .05) and insecure
5. Rejection – attachment (b = .33, p < .01) explained modest but
*
p < .01. significant and separate proportions of the variance of
worry scores. See Table 4 for regression statistics.
Table 3
Means and standard deviations for EMBU-C and PSWQ-C by attach- Table 4
ment style Results of regression analysis with EMBU-C scales and attachment
status as predictors and PSWQ-C as the dependent variable
Secure Avoidant Ambivalent
a Dependent Predictors R R2 B (S.E.) b
PSWQ-C 22.02 (8.72) 27.57 (11.30) 31.18 (6.77)b
Anxious rearing 21.98 (6.30) a 19.86 (1.95) a 26.18 (4.77)b PSWQ-C 1. Insecure attachment 0.38 0.15 6.67 0.33**
Overprotection 23.76 (5.13) 20.86 (2.67) 24.82 (5.25) (2.47)
Emotional warmth 33.55 (6.01) a 28.71 (6.13) 28.82 (5.44)b 2. Parental rejection 0.46 0.21 0.53 0.26*
Rejection 15.36 (4.51) 17.57 (6.00) 17.64 (3.82) (0.25)
*
Note. Means in each row with different superscripts differ signifi- p < .05.
**
cantly, p < .05. p < .01.
A.M. Brown, S.P. Whiteside / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 263–272 269

4. Discussion 4.2. Attachment style

This study represents a clinical replication of the With regard to attachment style, findings partially
study by Muris, Meesters, et al. (2000) to determine confirmed our hypotheses. In particular, it was
whether relations between family environmental hypothesized that an insecure attachment style, either
factors (i.e., parental rearing, attachment style) and avoidant or ambivalent, would be associated with higher
children’s worry would be maintained among a levels of worry compared to a secure attachment style.
clinical sample of anxious children. Consistent with While results revealed that children with secure
findings by Muris et al. and with theories of child attachment reported less worry than children with
development and psychopathology (e.g., Bowlby, avoidant or ambivalent attachment, the difference was
1973; Wood et al., 2003), results of the present only significant for the group with ambivalent attach-
study implicate the contribution of family environ- ment. These findings differed from those in Muris
mental factors to children’s worry. Findings were et al.’s study which revealed significantly higher worry
comparable to those of Muris et al., but specific scores for both ambivalent and avoidant attachment
relations among variables differed slightly from their styles as compared to secure attachment. The lack of
community sample. significant difference between the secure and avoidant
attachment groups may have resulted from insufficient
4.1. Parental rearing behaviors power. Indeed, there was a limited number of children
who endorsed having an avoidant attachment style
As hypothesized, the results demonstrated associa- (n = 7). However, as discussed by Warren et al. (1997),
tions between anxious children’s perceived parental it is also possible that avoidantly attached children
rearing behaviors and their level of worry. Overall, ‘‘displace’’ their anxious feelings and, in turn, are less
findings were most evident for parental rejection, aware of and less likely to report worries than
such that children with anxiety disorders who ambivalently attached children. Furthermore, it is
perceived higher levels of parental rejection reported interesting to speculate that an ambivalent attachment
more worries. There also were marginal relations is more closely associated with worry and anxiety than
between perceived anxious rearing and overprotec- avoidant attachment. Indeed, in the study by Warren
tion, on the one hand, and children’s worry, on the et al., only ambivalent attachment during childhood and
other hand. Perceived emotional warmth was not not avoidant attachment was predictive of anxiety
substantially associated with children’s worry. In disorders during adolescence. As discussed by Warren
general, these findings are consistent with those of et al. and consistent with attachment theory (Bowlby,
Muris, Meesters, et al. (2000) who found that non- 1973; Cassidy, 1995), children with ambivalent attach-
anxious children who perceived parents as engaging ment may be especially prone to worry and anxiety
in more rejecting and anxious rearing reported higher problems as a result of the uncertainty about their
levels of worry. Results are also consistent with caregiver’s availability and ability to protect them in
numerous studies identifying significant relations times of trouble. Additionally, these children may worry
between parental rejection and anxiety (Gruner about their own ability to effectively cope with their
et al., 1999; Moore et al., 2004; Alonso et al., environment.
2004) and between parental overprotection and
anxiety (Bogels & van Melick, 2004; Hudson & 4.3. Parental rearing behaviors, attachment style,
Rapee, 2005; Muris, Meesters, & von Brakel, 2003). and worry
The fact that only trends emerged for anxious rearing
and overprotection was somewhat surprising; how- Although it was hypothesized that insecure attach-
ever, this supports previous research suggesting that ment styles would be related to negative parental
parental criticism and negativity (i.e., rejection) may rearing behaviors more than a secure attachment style,
play a more salient role in the development of findings demonstrated that only an ambivalent attach-
childhood anxiety disorders (e.g., Hudson & Rapee, ment style, and not an avoidant attachment style, was
2001; Moore et al.). Thus, although parents of significantly related to children’s perceived parental
anxious children may engage in anxious and over- rearing behaviors. Specifically, ambivalently attached
protective parenting, it may be that rejection is most children reported less parental emotional warmth as
strongly associated with worry and childhood anxiety compared to children who endorsed a secure attachment
disorders. style. Ambivalently attached children were also more
270 A.M. Brown, S.P. Whiteside / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 263–272

likely to report more anxious rearing as compared to et al., 1997). Moreover, our findings support those of
children with either secure or avoidant attachment Warren et al. suggesting that insecure attachment has a
styles. Again, this finding is consistent with attachment unique contribution to anxious symptoms. Overall,
research demonstrating that children with ambivalent findings propose an additive model where both parental
attachment, which is believed to result from incon- rejection and insecure attachment uniquely contribute to
sistent and intrusive parenting, perceive less emotional children’s worry.
support (e.g., Cassidy, 1995) and more anxiety
(Manassis, Bradley, Goldberg, Hood, & Swinson, 4.4. Limitations and future research directions
1994) in their relationship with their parent. A parent
who interacts with their child in inconsistent and While the present study represents an important step
uncertain ways is not consistently meeting their child’s in evaluating family environmental factors, including
emotional needs and may come across as less certain children’s attachment and perceived parental rearing
and more anxious in the way they interact with their behaviors, among a clinical sample of anxious children,
child. Moreover, these parenting dimensions (i.e., high several limitations exist. To begin with, the study relied
anxious rearing and low emotional warmth) may have on child report and lacked observational and parent report
actually contributed to the development of an ambiva- data. These methods of assessment would have provided
lent attachment style in these children; however, the excellent comparisons to evaluate whether children’s
cross-sectional nature of these data does not allow for a expectations of parental rearing behaviors are similar to
conclusion regarding causality. those observed clinically and reported by parents.
Interestingly, there were no differences between Additionally, relying solely on child report data provided
perceived rearing behaviors for secure and avoidant a potential source of measurement error due to rater bias.
attachment styles. This is in contrast to the study by Given that the same respondent provided information on
Muris, Meesters, et al. (2000) who found avoidant all variables, it is possible that the magnitude of
attachment to be associated with perceived parental associations was inflated because of shared method
rejection. While findings from this study may have been variance. Children’s responses also may have been
partly due to the limited number of children who endorsed influenced by their current anxious symptoms. Thus,
an avoidant attachment style, findings imply that an future studies would benefit from using a multiple
ambivalent attachment style for anxious children is more informants approach (e.g., Bogels & van Melick, 2004)
closely associated with negative parental rearing beha- to reduce the likelihood of these potential sources of
viors than either secure or avoidant attachment styles. error. Another limitation pertains to the attachment
Finally, findings supported hypotheses that both measure used. As stated in the study by Muris, Meesters,
attachment and parental rearing are related to children’s et al. (2000), use of a single-item instrument to assess
worry. In the present study, results of the regression children’s attachment may have resulted in an under-
analysis indicated that the relative contribution of estimate of children’s attachment styles. Future research
parental rearing behaviors to children’s worry is largely would benefit from utilizing a more sophisticated
explained by parental rejection. Although studies have assessment of children’s attachment. Finally, given that
highlighted other parenting behaviors, including anxious Muris et al. studied a Dutch sample, there are probable
and overprotective rearing, this finding is consistent with cultural differences that limit conclusions that can be
the extensive research demonstrating the significance of drawn from comparing our samples. Although differ-
rejection to children’s anxious symptoms (Gruner et al., ences were in the expected directions, we can only
1999; Muris et al., 2003; Hudson & Rapee, 2001) and speculate about the ability of the EMBU-C to
provides further evidence that parental rejection plays an differentiate between parents of anxious and non-anxious
important role in the development and maintenance of children. In addition, although the current study provides
childhood anxiety problems. Furthermore, the contribu- initial support for using the EMBU-C to describe parents
tion of children’s attachment style was somewhat in general, rather than each independently, the difference
stronger than that of parental rejection and explained a in instructions between the two studies limits the
separate and unique part of the variance. This is comparability of the results. Further, using the EMBU-
consistent with past studies indicating that insecurely C to describe parents in general may neglect important
attached children and adolescents are more likely to differences between mothers and fathers. Thus, future
report higher levels of anxiety and worry as compared to studies should examine the value of combining mother
their securely attached counterparts (Muris, Mayer, & and father information versus providing separate ratings
Meesters, 2000; Muris, Meesters, et al., 2000; Warren for each parent.
A.M. Brown, S.P. Whiteside / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 263–272 271

A primary objective of the present study was to Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Volume 2. Separation: anxiety
evaluate the use of the EMBU-C with a clinical sample and anger. New York: Basic Books.
Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: influences of attachment
of anxious children. While these findings provide an relationships. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
initial exploration, further research is warranted. First, Development, 59, 228–283.
research is needed to establish North American norms Cassidy, J. (1995). Attachment and generalized anxiety disorder. In:
for the EMBU-C. Without norms, we are unable to D. Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.), Emotion, cognition, and represen-
tation: Rochester Symposium on Developmental Psychopathology
deduce the significance of the scores in the present
VI. New York: University of Rochester Press.
study. Second, research is needed to establish the Cassidy, J., & Berlin, L. J. (1994). The insecure/ambivalent pattern of
validity of the EMBU-C. At this point, it is uncertain attachment: Theory and research. Child Development, 65, 971–981.
whether the EMBU-C provides a valid reflection of Castro, J., Toro, J., Van der Ende, J., & Arrindell, W. A. (1993).
parenting styles. To this end, the next stage of research Exploring the feasibility of assessing perceived parental rearing
should involve evaluating the ability of the EMBU-C to styles in Spanish children with the EMBU. International Journal
of Social Psychiatry, 39, 47–57.
differentiate between parents of anxious and non- Chorpita, B. F., Albano, A. M., & Barlow, D. H. (1996). Cognitive
anxious children, in addition to comparing competent processing in children: relation to anxiety and family influences.
parents to those who may be more likely to exhibit poor Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 25, 170–176.
parenting behaviors (e.g., parents referred to the county Chorpita, B. F., Tracey, S. A., Brown, T. A., Collica, T. J., & Barlow,
for neglect or physical abuse reasons). Additionally, D. H. (1997). Assessment of worry in children and adolescents: an
adaptation of the Penn state worry questionnaire. Behavior
research should assess the extent to which the EMBU-C Research & Therapy, 35, 569–581.
relates to other measures of parenting or family Cobham, V. E., Dadds, M. R., & Spence, S. H. (1999). Anxious
functioning to establish concurrent and divergent children and their parents: what do they expect? Journal of
validity (e.g., parent report questionnaires, observa- Clinical Child Psychology, 28, 220–231.
Dadds, M. R., & Barrett, P. M. (1996). Family processes in child
tional assessment strategies). Finally, treatment out-
and adolescent anxiety and depression. Behavior Change, 13,
come research for parent interventions that compares 231–239.
findings of the EMBU-C before and after treatment Gruner, K., Muris, P., & Merckelbach, H. (1999). The relationship
would also assist in our understanding of the clinical between anxious rearing behaviors and anxiety disorders symp-
utility of the EMBU-C. tomatology in normal children. Journal of Behavior Therapy and
Experimental Psychiatry, 30, 27–35.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an
References attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
52, 511–524.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical Hettema, J. M., Prescott, C. A., Myers, J. M., Neale, M. C., & Kendler,
manual of mental disorders (4th ed., Text Revision). Washington, K. S. (2005). The structure of genetic and environmental risk
factors for anxiety disorders in men and women. Archives of
DC: Author.
Ainsworth, M. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). General Psychiatry, 62, 182–189.
Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange Hudson, J. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2001). Parent–child interactions and
anxiety disorders: an observational study. Behavior Research and
situation. Oxford, England: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Alonso, P., Menchon, J. M., Mataix-Cols, D., Pifarre, J., Urretaviz- Therapy, 39, 1411–1427.
caya, M., Crespo, J., et al. (2004). Perceived parental rearing style Hudson, J. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2005). Parental perceptions of
in obsessive–compulsive disorder: relation to symptom dimen- overprotection: specific to anxious children or shared between
siblings? Behavior Change, 22, 185–194.
sions. Psychiatry Research, 127, 267–278.
Albano, A. M., Chorpita, B. F., & Barlow, D. H. (2003). Childhood Kirsh, S. J., & Cassidy, J. (1997). Preschoolers’ attention to and
anxiety disorders. In: E. Mash & R. Barkley (Eds.), Child psycho- memory for attachment-relevant information. Child Development,
pathology (2nd ed., pp. 279–329). New York, NY: Guilford Press. 68, 1143–1153.
Manassis, K., Bradley, S., Goldberg, S., Hood, J., & Swinson, R. P.
Barrett, P. M., Rapee, R. M., Dadds, M. M., & Ryan, S. M. (1996).
Family enhancement of cognitive style in anxious and aggressive (1994). Behavioral inhibition, attachment, and anxiety in children
children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, 187–203. of mothers with anxiety disorders. Canadian Journal of Psychia-
try, 40, 87–92.
Barrett, P., Shortt, A., & Healy, L. (2002). Do parent and child
behaviors differentiate families whose children have obsessive- Merkel, W. T., Pollard, C. A., Wiener, R. L., & Staebler, C. R. (1993).
compulsive disorder from other clinic and non-clinic families? Perceived parental characteristics of patients with obsessive-com-
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 597–607. pulsive disorder, depression and panic disorder. Child Psychiatry
& Human Development, 24, 49–57.
Bogels, S. M., van Oosten, A., Muris, P., & Smulders, D. (2001).
Familial correlates of social anxiety in children and adolescents. Messer, S. C., & Beidel, D. C. (1994). Psychosocial correlates of
Behavior Research and Therapy, 39, 273–287. childhood anxiety disorders. Journal of the American Academy of
Bogels, S. M., & van Melick, M. (2004). The relationship between Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 33, 975–983.
Moore, P. S., Whaley, S., & Sigman, M. (2004). Interactions between
child-report, parent self-report, and partner report of perceived
parental rearing behaviors and anxiety in children and parents. mothers and children: impacts of maternal and child anxiety.
Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1583–1596. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 471–476.
272 A.M. Brown, S.P. Whiteside / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22 (2008) 263–272

Muris, P., Bogels, S., Meesters, C., van der Kamp, N., & van Oosten, Silverman, W. K., Cerny, J. A., & Nelles, W. B. (1988). The familial
A. (1996). Parental rearing practices, fearfulness, and problem influence in anxiety disorders: studies on the offspring of patients
behavior in clinically referred children. Personality & Individual with anxiety disorders. In: Lahey, B., & Kazdin, A. Eds. Advances
Differences, 21, 813–818. in clinical child psychology. Vol. 11 (pp.223–248). New York:
Muris, P., Mayer, B., & Meesters, C. (2000a). Self-reported attach- Plenum Press.
ment style, anxiety, and depression in children. Social Behavior & Siqueland, L., Kendall, P. C., & Steinberg, L. (1996). Anxiety
Personality, 28, 157–162. in children: Perceived family environments and observed
Muris, P., Meesters, C., & von Brakel, A. (2003). Assessment of family interaction. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 25,
anxious rearing behaviors with a modified version of ‘‘Egna 225–237.
Minnen Betraffande Uppfostran’’ questionnaire for children. Jour- Stoppel, C., Albrecht, A., Pape, H. C., & Stork, O. (2006). Genes and
nal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 25, 229–237. neurons: molecular insights to fear and anxiety. Genes, Brain &
Muris, P., Meesters, C., Merckelbach, H., & Hulsenbeck, P. (2000b). Behavior, 5, 34–47.
Worry in children is related to perceived parental rearing and Thompson, R. A. (1999). Early attachment and later development. In:
attachment. Behavior Research and Therapy, 38, 487–497. J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: theory,
Muris, P., & Merchelbach, H. (1998). Perceived parental rearing research, and clinical applications (pp. 265–286). New York:
behavior and anxiety disorders symptoms in normal children. Guilford Press.
Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 1199–1206. Thompson, R. A. (2004). Development in the First Years of Life. In: E.
Myhr, G., Sookman, D., & Pinard, G. (2004). Attachment security and F. Zigler, D. G. Singer, & S. J. Bishop-Josef (Eds.), Children’s
parental bonding in adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder: a play: the roots of reading (pp. 15–31). Washington, DC: National
comparison with depressed outpatients and healthy controls. Acta Center for Infants, Toddlers and Families.
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 109, 447–456. Turgeon, L., O’Connor, K. P., Marchand, A., & Freeston, M. H.
Perris, C., Jacobsson, L., Lindstrom, H., von Knorring, L., & Perris, H. (2002). Recollections of parent–child relationships in patients
(1980). Development of a new inventory for assessing memories with obsessive-compulsive disorder and panic disorder with agor-
of parental rearing behavior. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 61, aphobia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 105, 310–320.
265–274. Warren, S. L., Huston, L., Egeland, B., & Sroufe, L. A. (1997). Child
Rapee, R. M. (1997). Potential role of childrearing practices in the and adolescent anxiety disorders and early attachment. Journal of
development of anxiety and depression. Clinical Psychology the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36,
Review, 17, 47–67. 637–644.
Rubin, K. H., & Mills, R. S. (1991). Conceptualizing developmental Weissman, M. M. (1993). Family genetic studies of panic disorder.
pathways to internalizing disorders in childhood. Canadian Jour- Journal of Psychiatric Research, 2, 69–78.
nal of Behavioral Science, 23, 300–317. Whaley, S. E., Pinto, A., & Sigman, M. (1999). Characterizing
Schludermann, S., & Schludermann, S. (1970). Replicability of interactions between anxious mothers and their children. Journal
factors in children’s report of parent behavior. The Journal of of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 826–836.
Psychology, 76, 239–249. Wood, J., McLeod, B. D., Sigman, M., Hwang, W., & Chu, B. C.
Scott, W. A., Scott, R., & McCabe, M. (1991). Family relationships (2003). Parenting and childhood anxiety: theory, empirical find-
and children’s personality: A cross-cultural, cross-source compar- ings, and future directions. Journal of Child Psychology and
ison. British Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 1–20. Psychiatry, 44, 134–151.

You might also like