You are on page 1of 77

Barral, O., Aranyi, G., Kouider, S., Lindsay, A., Prins, H., Ahmed, I., Jacucci, G.

, Negri, P., Gamberini,


L., Pizzi, D., & Cavazza, M. (2014). Covert persuasive technologies: bringing subliminal cues to human-
computer interaction: 9th International Conference, PERSUASIVE 2014, Padua, Italy, May 21-23, 2014.
Proceedings. In Persuasive Technology (pp. -) https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-07127-5_1

1. .

Sidis, B.: The psychology of suggestion. D. Appleton, New York


(1898)Google Scholar

2. 2.

Fox, R., Herrmann, J.: Stochastic properties of binocular rivalry


alternations. Perception & Psychophysics 2, 432–436
(1967)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

3. 3.

Almeida, J., Mahon, B.Z., Nakayama, K., Caramazza, A.: Unconscious


processing dissociates along categorical lines. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105, 15214–15218
(2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

4. 4.

Breitmeyer, B.G., Ganz, L.: Implications of sustained and transient


channels for theories of visual pattern masking, saccadic suppression, and
information processing. Psychological Review 83, 1–36
(1976)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

5. 5.

Bouma, H.: Interaction effects in parafoveal letter recognition.


Nature 226, 177–178 (1970)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

6. 6.

Levi, D.M.: Crowding–an essential bottleneck for object recognition: a


mini-review. Vision Research 48, 635–654 (2008)CrossRefGoogle
Scholar

7. 7.

Pelli, D.G., Tillman, K.A.: The uncrowded window of object recognition.


Nature Neuroscience 11, 1129–1135 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

8. 8.
Whitney, D., Levi, D.M.: Visual crowding: a fundamental limit on
conscious perception and object recognition. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences 15, 160–168 (2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

9. 9.

Kouider, S., Berthet, V., Faivre, N.: Preference is biased by crowded facial
expressions. Psychological Science 22(2), 184–189 (2011)CrossRefGoogle
Scholar

10. 10.

Bonneh, Y.S., Cooperman, A., Sagi, D.: Motion-induced blindness in


normal observers. Nature 411, 798–801 (2001)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

11. 11.

Neisser, U.: The control of information pickup in selective looking. In:


Pick, A.D. (ed.) Perception and Its Development: A Tribute to Eleanor
Gibson, pp. 201–219 (1979)Google Scholar

12. 12.

Simons, D.J., Chabris, C.F.: Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional


blindness for dynamic events. Perception 28, 1059–1074
(1999)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

13. 13.

Rensink, R.A., O’Regan, J.K., Clark, J.J.: To See or not to See: The Need
for Attention to Perceive Changes in Scenes. Psychological Science 8,
368–373 (1997)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

14. 14.

Simons, D.J., Levin, T.: Change blindness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 1,


261–267 (1997)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

15. 15.

Raymond, J.E., Shapiro, K.L., Arnell, K.M.: Temporary suppression of


visual processing in an RSVP task: an attentional blink? Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 18, 849–
860 (1992)Google Scholar

16. 16.
Humphreys, G.W., Evett, L.J., Taylor, D.E.: Automatic phonological
priming in visual word recognition. Memory Cognition 10, 576–590
(1982)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

17. 17.

Kunst-Wilson, W.R., Zajonc, R.B.: Affective discrimination of stimuli that


cannot be recognized. Science 207, 557–558 (1980)CrossRefGoogle
Scholar

18. 18.

Faivre, N., Berthet, V., Kouider, S.: Nonconscious influences from


emotional faces: a comparison of visual crowding, masking, and
continuous flash suppression. Frontiers in Psychology 3, 129
(2012)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

19. 19.

Bargh, J.A., Chen, M., Burrows, L.: Automaticity of social behavior: direct
effects of trait construct and stereotype-activation on action. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 71, 230–244 (1996)CrossRefGoogle
Scholar

20. 20.

Devine, P.G.: Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled


components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 56, 5–18
(1989)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

21. 21.

Strahan, E.J., Spencer, S.J., Zanna, M.P.: Subliminal priming and


persuasion: Striking while the iron is hot. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology 38, 556–568 (2002)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
-References

Aarts, H., Chartrand, T., Custers, R., Danner, U., Dik, G., Jefferis, V., et al. (2005). Social stereotypes and
automatic goal pursuit. Social Cognition, 23, 465–490.

Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Marien, H. (2008). Preparing and motivating behavior outside of awareness.
Science, 319, 1639.

Abrams, R. (2008). Influence of category size and target set size on unconscious priming by novel words.
Experimental Psychology, 55(3), 189–194.
Bargh, J. A. (1990). Auto-motives: Preconscious determinants of social interaction. In E. T. Higgins & R.
M. Sorrentino (Eds.). Handbook of motivation and

cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 93–130). New York: Guilford Press.

Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., & Trotschel, R. (2001). The automated will:
Nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1014–1027.

Bermeitinger, C., Goelz, R., Johr, N., Neumann, M., Ecker, U. K. H., & Doerr, R. (2009). The hidden
persuaders break into the tired brain. Journal of Experimental

Social Psychology, 45(2), 320–326.

Bijleveld, E., Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2010). Unconscious reward cues increase invested effort, but do not
change speed-accuracy tradeoffs. Cognition, 115,

330–335.

Bijleveld, E., Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2011). Once the money is in sight: Distinctive effects of conscious
and unconscious rewards on task performance. Journal

of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 865–869.

Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1996). Automatic activation of social information processing goals:
Nonconscious priming reproduces effects of explicit

conscious instructions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 464–478.

Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2010). The unconscious will: How the pursuit of goals operates outside of
conscious awareness. Science, 329, 47–50.

Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec’H, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., et al. (1998).
Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395,

597–600.

Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and
subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends in

Cognitive Science, 10(5), 204–211.

Dijksterhuis, A., & Aarts, H. (2010). Goals, attention, and (Un)Consciousness. Annual Review of
Psychology, 16, 467–490.

Eitam, B., Hassin, R., & Schul, Y. (2008). Nonconscious goal pursuit in novel environments: The case of
implicit learning. Psychological Science, 19(3),

261–267.

Eitam, B., & Higgins, E. T. (2010). Motivation in mental accessibility: Relevance of a representation
(ROAR) as a new framework. Personality and Social
Psychology Compass, 4(10), 951–967.

Eriksen, C. W. (1960). Discrimination and learning without awareness: A methodological survey and
evaluation. Psychological Review, 67, 279–300.

Ferguson, M. (2008). On becoming ready to pursue a goal you don’t know you have: Effects of
nonconscious goals on evaluative readiness. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 95(6), 1268–1294.

Ferguson, M., & Bargh, J. (2004). Liking is for doing: The effects of goal pursuit on automatic evaluation.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(5),

557–572.

Fishbach, A., Friedman, R., & Kruglanski, A. (2003). Leading us not unto temptation: Momentary
allurements elicit overriding goal activation. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 296–309.

Fitzsimons, M., & Bargh, J. (2003). Thinking of you: Nonconscious pursuit of interpersonal goals
associated with relationship partners. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 84, 148–164.

Forster, K. I. (2004). Category size revisited: Frequency and masked priming effects in semantic
categorization. Brain and Language, 90, 276–286.

Greenwald, A., Klinger, M., & Schuh, E. (1995). Activation by marginally perceptible (‘‘subliminal”)
stimuli: Dissociation of unconscious from conscious

cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 22–42.

Hassin, R. T., Aarts, H., Eitam, B., Custers, R., & Kleiman, T. (2009a). Non-conscious goal pursuit and the
effortful control of behavior. In E. Morsella, J. A. Bargh,

& P. M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of human action (pp. 549–566). New York: Oxford University
Press.

Hassin, R. R., Bargh, J. A., & Zimerman, S. (2009b). Automatic and flexible: The case of nonconscious goal
pursuit. Social Cognition, 27, 20–36.

Higgins, E. T. (2011). Beyond pleasure and pain: How motivation works. New York: Oxford University
Press.

Higgins, E. T., & Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds.). (2000). Motivational science. Social and personality
perspectives. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.

Holender, D. (1986). Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal
vision, and visual masking: A survey and

appraisal. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 1–23.


Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Karremans, J., Stroebe, W., & Clauss, J. (2006). Beyond Vicary’s fantasies: The impact of subliminal
priming and brand choice. Journal of Experimental Social

Psychology, 42, 792–798.

Kiesel, A., Kunde, W., Pohl, C., & Hoffmann, J. (2006a). Priming from novel masked stimuli depends on
target set size. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 2(1),

37–45.

Kiesel, A., Wagener, A., Kunde, W., Hoffmann, J., & Fallgatter, A. (2006b). Unconscious manipulation of
free choice in humans. Christian Consciousness and

Cognition: An International Journal, 15(2), 397–408.

Kinoshita, S., & Hunt, L. (2008). RT distribution analysis of category congruence effects with masked
primes. Memory and Cognition, 36(7), 1324–1334.

Klauer, K., Eder, A., Greenwald, A., & Abrams, R. (2007). Priming of semantic classifications by novel
subliminal prime words. Consciousness and Cognition: An

International Journal, 16(1), 63–83.

Klapp, S., & Haas, B. (2005). Non-conscious influence of masked stimuli on response selection is limited
to concrete stimulus–response associations. Journal

of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(1), 193–209.

Kleiman, T., & Hassin, R. (2011). Non-conscious goal conflicts. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
47(3), 521–532.

Kouider, S., & Dehaene, S. (2007). Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: A critical review
of visual masking. Philosophical Transaction of the

Royal Society: B.

Kunimoto, C., Miller, J., & Pashler, H. (2001). Confidence and accuracy of near-threshold discrimination
responses. Consciousness and Cognition, 10, 294.

Kruglanski, A. W. (1996). Goals as knowledge structures. In P. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The
psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to

behavior (pp. 599–618). NY: Guilford.

Levy, B., Ariely, D., Mazar, N., Chi, W., Lukas, S., & Elman, I. (2008). Gender differences in the
motivational processing of facial beauty. Learning and

Motivation, 39, 136–145.

Merikle, P. M., & Daneman, M. (2000). Conscious vs. unconscious perception. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.),
The new cognitive neurosciences (2nd ed.,
pp. 1295–1303). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Naccache, L., & Dehaene, S. (2001). Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli.
Cognition, 80, 223–237.

Pratkanis, A. R. (1992). The cargo-cult science of subliminal persuasion. Skeptical Inquirer, 16, 260–272.

Pratte, M., & Rouder, J. (2009). A task-difficulty artifact in subliminal priming. Attention, Perception, and
Psychophysics, 71, 1276–1283.

Reingold, E., & Merikle, P. (1988). Using direct and indirect measures to study perception without
awareness. Perception and Psychophysics, 44, 563–575.

Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2004). Masked prime stimuli can bias ‘‘free” choices between response
alternatives. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11(3),

463–468.

Shidlovski, D., & Hassin, R. (2011). When pooping babies become more appealing: The effects of
nonconscious goal pursuit on experienced emotions.

Psychological Science, 22(11), 1381–1385.

Strahan, E., Spencer, S., & Zanna, M. (2002). Subliminal priming and persuasion: Striking while the iron is
hot. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38,

556–558.

Van den Bussche, W., den Noortgate, Van., & Reynvoet, B. (2009). Mechanisms of masked priming: A
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 452–477.

1240 M. Milyavsky et al. / Consciousness and Cognition 21 (2012) 1232–1241

Veltkamp, M., Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2011). Motivating consumer behavior by subliminal conditioning
in the absence of basic needs: Striking even while

the iron is cold. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21, 49–56.

Winkielman, P., Berridge, K., & Wilbarger, J. (2005). Unconscious affective reactions to masked happy
versus angry faces influence consumption behavior

and judgments of value. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 121–135.

-References

Andersen, S.A., G.B. Moskowitz, I.V Blair, and B.A. Nosek. 2007. Automatic

thought. In Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles, 2nd edition,

ed. by E.T. Higgins and A. Kruglanski, 138–175. New York: Guilford.

Bargh, J.A. 2002. Losing consciousness: Automatic influences on consumer

judgment, behavior, and motivation. Journal of Consumer Research


29(3):280–285.

———. 2006. What have we been priming all of these years? On the development, mechanisms, and
ecology of nonconscious social behavior.

European Journal of Social Psychology 36(2):147–168.

Bargh, J.A., and T. L. Chartrand. 2000. The mind in the middle: A practical

guild to priming and automaticity research. In Handbook of Research

Methods in Social and Personality Psychology, ed. by H.T. Reis and C.M.

Judd, 253–285. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Bargh, J.A., M. Chen, and L. Burrows. 1996. Automaticity of social behavior:

Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71(2):230–244.

Beatty, S.E., and D. Hawkins. 1989. Subliminal stimulation: Some new data

and interpretation. Journal of Advertising 18(3):4–8.

Carey, B. 2007. Who’s minding the mind? New York Times, August 30.

Available online at www.nytimes.com.

Cooper, J., and G. Cooper. 2002. Subliminal motivation: A story revisited.

Journal of Applied Social Psychology 32(11):2213–2227.

Dehaene S., and L. Naccache. 2006. Can one suppress subliminal words?

Neuron 52(3):397–399.

Dijksterhuis, A., H. Aarts, and P.K. Smith. 2005. The power of the subliminal: On subliminal persuasion
and other potential applications. In The

New Unconscious, ed. by R. Hassin, J.S. Uleman, and J.A. Bargh, 77–106.

New York: Oxford.

Dijksterhuis, A., T. L. Chartrand, and H. Aarts. 2007. Effects of priming and

perception on social behavior and goal pursuit. In Social Psychology and the

Unconscious: The Automaticity of Higher Mental Processes, ed. by J.A. Bargh,

51–131. New York: Psychology Press.

Dijksterhuis, A., J.A. Bargh, and J. Miedema. 2000. Of men and mackerels:

Attention, subjective experience, and automatic social behavior. In The


Message Within: The Role of Subjective in Social Cognition and Behavior, ed.

by H. Bless and J.P. Forgas, 37–51. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.

Dijksterhuis, A., P.K. Smith, R.B. van Baaren, and D.H.J. Wigboldus. 2005.

The unconscious consumer: Effects of environment on consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer


Psychology 15(3):193–202.

Dijksterhuis, A., R. Spears, T. Postmes, D.A. Stapel, W. Koomen, A. van

Knippenberg, and D. Scheepers. 1998. Seeing one thing and doing

another: Contrast effects in automatic behavior. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology 75(4):862–871.

Devine, P.G. 1989. Stereotype and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled

components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 56(1): 5–18.

Epley, N., K. Savitsky, and R.A. Kachelski. 1999. What every skeptic should

know about subliminal persuasion. SKEPTICAL INQUIRER 23(5):40–45, 58.

Glaser, J., and J.F. Kihlstrom. 2005. Compensatory Automaticity: Unconscious volition is not an
oxymoron. In The new unconscious, ed. by R.

Hassin, J.S. Uleman, and J.A. Bargh, 171–195. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Greenwald, A.G., E.R. Spangenberg, A.R. Pratkanis, and J. Eskenazi. 1991.

Double blind tests of subliminal self-help audiotapes. Psychological Science

2(2):119–122.

Karremans, J.C., W. Stroebe, and J. Claus. 2006. Beyond Vicary’s fantasies:

The impact of subliminal priming and brand choice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
42(6):792–798.

Macrae, C.N., and L. Johnston. 1998. Help, I need somebody: Automatic

action and inaction. Social Cognition 16(4):400–417.

Mather, R.D., and A. Romo 2007. Automaticity and Cognitive Control in

Social Behavior. Southlake, TX: Fountainhead Press.

Moore, T.E. 1992. Subliminal perception: Facts and fallacies. SKEPTICAL

INQUIRER 16(3):273–281.

Nisbett, R.E., and T.D. Wilson. 1977. Telling more than we can know: Verbal

reports on mental processes. Psychological Review 84(3):231–259.


Phelps, E.A. 2006. Emotion and cognition: Insights from studies of the

human amygdala. Annual Review of Psychology 24(57):27–53.

Pratkanis, A.R. 1992. The cargo-cult science of subliminal persuasion.

SKEPTICAL INQUIRER 16(3):261–272.

Pratkanis, A.R., and E. Aronson. 2001. Age of Propaganda: The Everyday Use

and Abuse of Persuasion. New York: Henry Holt and Company LLC.

Randolph-Seng, B., and M.E. Nielsen. 2007. Honesty: One effect of primed

religious representations. The International Journal for the Psychology of

Religion 17(4):303–315.

Satpute, A.B., and M.D. Lieberman. 2006. Integrating automatic and controlled processing into
neurocognitive models of social cognition. Brain

Research 1079:86–97.

Strahan, E.J., S.J. Spencer, and M.P. Zanna. 2002. Subliminal priming and

persuasion: Striking while the iron is hot. Journal of Experimental Social

Psychology 38(6):556–568.

Vallacher, R.R., and D.M. Wegner. 1987. What do people think they’re

doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological Review

94(1):3–15.

Wegner, D.M. 2005. Who is the controller of controlled processes? In The

New Unconscious, ed. by R. Hassin, J.S. Uleman, and J.A. Bargh, 19–36.

New York: Oxford University Press.

References

Balay, J. & Shevrin, H. (1988). The subliminal psychodynamic activation method: a critical review.
American

Psychologist, 43(3), 161-174.

Barber, T. X. & Calverley, D. S. (1963). „Hypnotic-like“ suggestibility in children and adults. Journal of

Abnormal & Social Psychology, 66(6), 589-597.

Begg, I. M., Needham, D. R. & Bokkbinder, M. (1993). Do backward messages unconsciously affect
listeners?

No. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47(1), 1-14.


Benes, K. M., Gutkin, T. B. & Decker, T. N. (1990). The effects of mellow and frenetic music on reported

cognitions resulting from auditory subliminal messages. Journal of General Psychology, 117(1), 83-89.

Benoit, S. C. & Thomas, R. L. (1992). The influence of expectancy in subliminal perception experiments.

Journal of General Psychology, 119(4), 335-341.

Bjorklund, D. F., Cassel, W. S., Bjorklund, B. R. & Brown, R. D. (2000). Social demand characteristics in

children‘s and adults‘ eyewitness memory and suggestibility: the effect of different interviewers on free

recall and recognition. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14(5), 421-433.

Borgeat, F. (1989). Psychophysiological responses to subliminal auditory suggestions for activation.


Perceptual

& Motor Skills, 69(3, Pt 1), 947-953.

Borgeat, F., Elie, R., Chaloult, L. & Chabot, R. (1985). Psychophysiological responses to masked auditory

stimuli. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 30(1), 22-27.

Borgeat, F. & Goulet, J. (1983). Psychophysiological changes following auditory subliminal suggestions
for

activation and deactivation. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 56(3), 759-766.

Bornstein, R. F. (1989). Subliminal techniques as propaganda tools: review and critique. Journal of Mind
&

Behavior, 10(3), 231-262.

Bornstein, R. F. (1990). Critical importance of stimulus unawareness for the production of subliminal

psychodynamic activation effects: a meta-analytic review. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46(2), 201-210.

Chakalis, E. & Lowe, G. (1992). Positive effects of subliminal stimulation of memory. Perceptual & Motor
Skills,

74(3, Pt 1), 956-958.

Chartrand, T. L. (2005). The Role of Conscious Awareness in Consumer Behavior. Journal of Consumer

Psychology, 15(3), 203-210.

Cheesman, J. & Merikle, P. M. (1985). Word recognition and consciousness. In D. Besner & T. G. Weller &

G. E. MacKinnon (Eds.), Reading research: advances in theory and practice (Vol. 5, pp. 311-352). New
York:

Academic Press.

dBSONIC. (2002). (Version 3.12) [Computersoftware for psychoacoustic analysis. For description see
http:
//www.cortex-instruments.de]. Regensburg: Neutrik Cortex Instruments GmbH.

Dijksterhuis, A., Smith, P. K., van Baaren, R. B. & Wigboldus, D. H. J. (2005). The unconscious consumer:

effects of environment on consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15(3), 193-202.

Elfont, H. & Kaplan, D. (2001). Josie and the pussycats (Director, Director). USA: Metro Goldwyn Mayer.

Friedman, R. S., McCarthy, D. M., Förster, J. & Denzler, M. (2005). Automatic effects of alcohol cues on

sexual attraction. Addiction, 100(5), 672-681.

Froufe & Schwartz, A. B. (2001). Subliminal messages for increasing self-esteem: placebo effect. Spanish

Journal of Psychology, 4(1), 19-25.

Fudin, R. & Benjamin, C. (1991). Review of auditory subliminal psychodynamic activation experiments.

Perceptual & Motor Skills, 73(3, Pt 2), 1115-1136.

Greenwald, A. G., Spangenberg, E. R., Pratkanis, A. R. & Eskenazi, J. (1991). Double-blind tests of

subliminal self-help audiotapes. Psychological Science, 2(2), 119-122.

44 Journal of Articles in Support of the Null Hypothesis. JASNH, 2006, Vol. 4, No. 2 Subliminal messages
in music? 45

Groenland, E., A.G. & Schoormans, J. P. L. (1994). Comparing mood-induction and affective conditioning

as mechanisms influencing product evaluation and product choice. Psychology & Marketing, 11(2), 183-

197.

Harris, J. L., Salus, D., Rerecich, R. & Larsen, D. (1996). Distinguishing detection from identification in

subliminal auditory perception: A review and critique of Merikle‘s study. Journal of General Psychology,

123(1), 41-50.

Henley, S. (1975). Cross-modal effects of subliminal verbal stimuli. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology,
16(1),

30-36.

Hughes, J. A., Sanders, L. D., Dunne, J. A., Tarpey, J. & Vickers, M. D. (1994). Reducing smoking. The
effect

of suggestion during general anaesthesia on postoperative smoking habits. Anaesthesia, 49(2), 126-128.

Kotzé, H. F. & Möller, A. T. (1990). Effect of auditory subliminal stimulation on GSR. Psychological
Reports,

67(3, Pt 1), 931-934.

Lazarus, R. S. & McCleary, R. A. (1951). Autonomic discrimination without awareness: a study of


subception. Psychological Review, 58, 113-122.

Merikle, P. M. (1988). Subliminal auditory messages: an evaluation. Psychology & Marketing, 5(4), 355-
372.

Merikle, P. M. (2000). Subliminal perception. In A. E. Kazdin (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychology (Vol. 7, pp.
497-

499). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Merikle, P. M. & Daneman, M. (1996). Memory for unconsciously perceived events: evidence from

anesthetized patients. Consciousness & Cognition: An International Journal, 5(4), 525-541.

Merikle, P. M. & Skanes, H. E. (1992). Subliminal self-help audiotapes: A search for placebo effects.
Journal

of Applied Psychology, 77(5), 772-776.

Milliman, R. E. (1982). Using background music to affect the behavior of supermarket shoppers. Journal
of

Marketing, 46(3), 86-91.

Milliman, R. E. (1986). The influence of background music on the behavior of restaurant patrons. Journal

of Consumer Research, 13(2), 286-289.

Mitchell. (1995). Effects of subliminally presented auditory suggestions of itching on scratching behavior.

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 80(1), 87-96.

Moore, T. E. (1995). Subliminal self-help auditory tapes: an empirical test of perceptual consequences.

Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 27(1), 9-20.

Moore, T. E. (1997). Scientific consensus & expert testimony: lessons form the Judas Priest trial.
American

Psychology Law Society News, 17(1), 3-14.

Möller, A. T., Kotzé, H. F. & Sieberhagen, K. J. (1993). Comparison of the effects of auditory subliminal

stimulation and rational-emotive therapy, separately and combined, on self-concept. Psychological


Reports,

72(1), 131-145.

Naccache, L., Blandin, E. & Dehaene, S. (2002). Unconscious masked priming depends on temporal

attention. Psychological Science, 13(5), 416-424.

North, A. C. (1996). The effects of music on responses to a dining area. Journal of Environmental
Psychology,
16(1), 55-64.

North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J. & McKendrick, J. (1997). In-store music affects product choice. Nature,

390(6656), 132.

North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J. & McKendrick, J. (1999). The influence of in-store music on wine
selections.

Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 271-276.

North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J. & McKendrick, J. (2000). The effects of music on atmosphere in a bank and

a bar. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(7), 1504-1522.

Oates, C., Blades, M. & Gunter, B. (2002). Children and television advertising: when do they understand

persuasive intent? Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 1(3), 238-245.

Ott, R. (1998). Subliminale akustische Beeinflussung - Fakten und Fiktionen [Subliminal auditory
influences:

facts and fictions]. Hypnose und Kognition, 15(1-2), 57-78.

Packard, V. (1960). The hidden persuaders. Harmondsworth: Penguin (original work published 1957).

Pratkanis, A. R. (1992). The cargo-cult science of subliminal persuasion. The Skeptical Inquirer, 16(3),
260-

272.

Rogers, M. & Smith, K. H. (1993). Public perceptions of subliminal advertising: why practitioners
shouldn‘t

ignore this. Journal of Advertising Research, 33(2), 10-18.

Russell, T. G., Rowe, W. & Smouse, A. D. (1991). Subliminal self-help tapes and academic achievement:
an

evaluation. Journal of Counseling & Development, 69(4), 359-362.

Schiller, U. (1990, 10 August). Man hört, was man hört [You hear, what you hear]. Die Zeit, pp. 50.

44 Journal of Articles in Support of the Null Hypothesis. JASNH, 2006, Vol. 4, No. 2 Subliminal messages
in music? 45

Schneider, R. U. (2004). Das Buch der verrückten Experimente [The book of crazy experiments].
München:

Bertelsmann.

Silverman, L. H. (1975). A clinical application of subliminal psychodynamic activation. Journal of Nervous


&

Mental Disease, 161(6), 379-392.


Sloboda, J. A., O‘Neill, S. & Ivaldi, A. (2001). Functions of music in everyday life: an exploratory study
using

the Experience Sampling Method. Musicae Scientiae, 5(1), 9-32.

Staum, M. J. & Brotons, M. (1992). The influence of auditory subliminals on behavior: a series of

investigations. Journal of Music Therapy, 29(3), 130-185.

Swart, L. C. & Morgan, C. L. (1992). Effects of subliminal backward-recorded messages on attitudes.

Perceptual & Motor Skills, 75(3, Pt 2), 1107-1113.

Theus, K. T. (1994). Subliminal advertising and the psychology of processing unconscious stimuli: a
review

of research. Psychology & Marketing, 11(3), 271-290.

Thorne, S. B. & Himelstein, P. (1984). The role of suggestion in the perception of satanic messages in
rockand-roll recordings. The Journal of Psychology, 116(2), 245-248.

Vokey, J. R. (2002). Subliminal messages. In J. Vokey, R. & S. W. Allen (Eds.), Psychological sketches (6th
ed., pp.

223-246). Lethbridge, Alberta: Psyence Ink.

Vokey, J. R. & Read, J. D. (1985). Subliminal messages: between the devil and the media. American
Psychologist,

40(11), 1231-1239.

Walker, M. W. (1985). Backward messages in commercially available recordings. Popular Music and
Society,

10(1), 2-13.

Walker, M. W. (1987). How to produce forward/backward recordings (without supernatural guidance).

Popular Music and Society, 11(2), 1-3.

Walls, K. C., Taylor, J. A. & Falzone, J. (1992). The effects of subliminal suggestions and music experience

on the perception of tempo in music. Journal of Music Therapy, 29(3), 186-197.

Wolpert, R. S. (2000). Attention to key in a nondirected music listening task. Music Perception, 18(2),
225-

230.

Yost, W. A. (2000). Fundamentals of hearing: an introduction (4th ed.). San Diego: Academic Press.

Zwicker, E. & Fastl, H. (1999). Psychoacoustics. Facts and models (2nd ed.). Berlin: Springer.
1. -Bargh, J. A., & Pietromonaco, P. (1982). Automatic information
processing and social perception: The influence of trait information
presented outside of conscious awareness on impression
formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 437–449.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.43.3.437
2. Bowden, E. M. (1997). The effect of reportable and unreportable hints
on anagram solution and the aha! experience. Consciousness and
Cognition, 6, 545–573. doi:10.1006/ccog.1997.0325

PubMed Article Google Scholar 

3. Burnham, C. A., & Davis, K. G. (1969). The nine-dot problem: Beyond


perceptual organisation. Psyochonomic Science, 17, 321–323.

Google Scholar 

4. Chronicle, E. P., MacGregor, J. N., & Ormerod, T. C. (2004). What


makes an insight problem? The role of heuristics, goal conception, and
solution recoding in knowledge-lean problems. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 14–
27. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.30.1.14

PubMed Article Google Scholar 

5. Chronicle, E. P., Ormerod, T. C., & MacGregor, J. N. (2001). When


insight just won't come: The failure of visual cues in the nine-dot
problem. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,
54A, 903–919. doi:10.1080/02724980042000471
6. Dijksterhuis, A., & Van Knippenberg, A. (2000). Behavioral indecision:
Effects of self-focus on automatic behavior. Social Cognition, 18, 55–74.
doi:10.1521/soco.2000.18.1.55
7. Duncker, K. (1945). On problem-solving. Psychological Monographs,
58(5), i-113. doi:10.1037/h0093599

Google Scholar 

8. Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1980). Analogical problem


solving. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 306–355.
Article Google Scholar 

9. Holender, D. (1986). Semantic activation without conscious


identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual
masking: A survey and appraisal. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 1–
23. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00021269

Article Google Scholar 

10. Holyoak, K., & Koh, K. (1987). Surface and structural similarity in
analogical transfer. Memory & Cognition, 15, 332–340.
doi:10.3758/bf03197035

Article Google Scholar 

11. Kaplan, C. A., & Simon, H. A. (1990). In search of insight. Cognitive


Psychology, 22, 374–419. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(90)90008-R
12. Karremans, J. C., Stroebe, W., & Claus, J. (2006). Beyond Vicary’s
fantasies: The impact of subliminal priming and brand choice. Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 792–798.
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.12.002

Article Google Scholar 

13. Kershaw, T. C., & Ohlsson, S. (2004). Multiple causes of difficulty in


insight: The case of the nine-dot problem. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 3–13.
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.30.1.3

PubMed Article Google Scholar 

14. Kunst-Wilson, W. R., & Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Affective discrimination of


stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science, 207, 557–558.
doi:10.1126/science.7352271
15. Légal, J.-B., Chappé, J., Coiffard, V., & Villard-Forest, A. (2012). Don't
you know that you want to trust me? Subliminal goal priming and
persuasion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 358–360.
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2011.06.006
Article Google Scholar 

16. Lockhart, R. S., Lamon, M., & Gick, M. L. (1988). Conceptual transfer in
simple insight problems. Memory & Cognition, 16, 36–44.
doi:10.3758/BF03197743

Article Google Scholar 

17. MacGregor, J. N., Ormerod, T. C., & Chronicle, E. P. (2001).


Information processing and insight: A process model of performance on
the nine-dot and related problems. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 176–201.
doi:10.1037//0278-7393.27.1.176

PubMed Article Google Scholar 

18. Maier, N. R. F. (1931). Reasoning in humans: II. The solution of a


problem and its appearance in consciousness. Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 12, 181–194. doi:10.1037/h0071361
19. Moss, J., Kotovsky, K., & Cagan, J. (2007). The influence of open goals
on the acquisition of problem-relevant information. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 876–
891. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.5.876

PubMed Article Google Scholar 

20. Nickerson, R. S. (1999). Enhancing creativity. In R. J. Sternberg


(Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 392–430). Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar 

21.Nishimura, Y., & Suzuki, H. (2006). Dousatsu mondai kaiketsu-no


seiyaku kanwa-ni okeru senzaiteki jouhou syori [Implicit information
processing of constraint relaxation in insight problem-
solving]. Cognitive Studies, 13, 136–138.

Google Scholar 
22. Ohlsson, S. (2011). Deep learning: How the mind overrides
experience. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar 

23. Radel, R., Sarrazin, P., Legrain, P., & Gobancé, L. (2009). Subliminal
priming of motivational orientation in educational settings: Effect on
academic performance moderated by mindfulness. Journal of Research
in Personality, 43, 695–698. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2009.02.011

Article Google Scholar 

24. Schooler, J. W., Ohlsson, S., & Brooks, K. (1993). Thoughts beyond
words: When language overshadows insight. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 122, 166–183. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.122.2.166
25. Schunn, C. D., & Dunbar, K. (1996). Priming, analogy, and awareness in
complex reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 24, 271–284.
doi:10.3758/BF03213292
26. Seifert, C. M., Meyer, D. E., Davidson, N. S., Patalano, A. L., &
Yaniv, I. (1995). Demystification of cognitive insight: Opportunistic
assimilation and the prepared-mind hypothesis. In R. J. Sternberg & J.
E. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 65–124). Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.

Google Scholar 

27. Siegler, R. S. (2000). Unconscious insights. Current Directions in


Psychological Science, 9, 79–83. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00065
28. Snodgrass, M., Bernat, E., & Shevrin, H. (2004). Unconscious
perception: A model-based approach to method and
evidence. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 846–867.
doi:10.3758/BF03194978
29. Spencer, R. M., & Weisberg, R. W. (1986). Context-dependent effects on
analogical transfer. Memory & Cognition, 14, 442–449.
doi:10.3758/bf03197019

Article Google Scholar 
30. Thomas, L. E., & Lleras, A. (2007). Moving eyes and moving thought:
On the spatial compatibility between eye movements and
cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 663–668.
doi:10.3758/BF03196818
31. Thomas, L. E., & Lleras, A. (2009). Covert shifts of attention function as
an implicit aid to insight. Cognition, 111, 168–174.
doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.01.005
32. Weisberg, R. W. (1995). Prolegomena to theories of insight in
problem solving: A taxonomy of problems. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E.
Davidson (Eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 157–196). Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.

Google Scholar 

33. Weisberg, R. W., & Alba, J. W. (1981). Gestalt theory, insight, and past
experience: Reply to Dominowski. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 110, 193–198. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.110.2.199
34. Zhong, C.-B., Dijksterhuis, A., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). The merits of
unconscious thought in creativity. Psychological Science, 19, 912–918.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02176.x

Bales, R. F. (1950). Interaction process analysis. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Bargh, J. A., & Pietromonaco, P. (1982). Automatic information processing and social perception: The
influence of trait information presented outside of conscious awareness on impression formation.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 437-449.

Bemporad, J. R. (1980). Review of object relations theory in light of

cognitive development. Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis, 8, 57-75.

Berlyne, D. E. (1970). Novelty, complexity and hedonic value. Perception and Psychophysics, 8,279-286.

Blatt, S. J. (1974). Levels of object representation in anaclitic and introjective depression. Psychoanalytic
Study of the Child, 29, 107-157.

Bornstein, R. F. (1987). Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968-1987.
Manuscript submitted for publication.

Bornstein, R. F., Galley, D. J., & Leone, D. R. (1986). Parental representations and oraltiy. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 50, 80-89.

SUBLIMINAL MERE EXPOSURE EFFECTS 1079

Bornstein, R. F., & Masling, J. M. (1984). Subliminal psychodynamic


stimulation: Implications for psychoanalytic theory and therapy. International Forum for Psychoanalysis,
1, 187-204.

Bornstein, R. F, & Masling, J. M. (1985). Orality and latency of volunteering to serve as experimental
subjects. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 306-310.

Bornstein, R. F., Masling, J. M., & Poynton, F. G. (1987). Orality as a

factor in interpersonal yielding. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 4, 161-

170.

Dixon, N. F. (1981). Preconscious processing. New York: Wiley.

Freud, S. (1905). Three essays on the theory of sexuality. Standard Edition VII, London: Hogarth.

Freud, S. (1919). The interpretation of dreams. Standard Edii.jn IV &

V. London: Hogarth. (Original work published 1900)

Freud, S. (1923). The ego and the id. Standard Edition XIX. London:

Hogarth.

Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley.

Harrison, A. A. (1968). Response competition, frequency, expectancy

behavior and liking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9,

363-368.

Harrison, A. A., & Crandall, R. (1972). Heterogeneity and homogeneity

of exposure sequences and the attitudinal effects of exposure. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 234-238.

Kunst-Wilson, W. R., & Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Affective discrimination

of stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science, 207,557-558.

Lazarus, R. S. (1984). On the primacy of cognition. A merican Psychologist, 39, 124-129.

Masling, J. M., Bornstein, R. F., Poynton, F. G., Reed, S. D., & Katkin,

E. S. (1987). Subliminal stimulation and electrodermal responding.

Manuscript submitted for publication.

McConnell, J. V, Cutler, R. L., & McNeil, E. B. (1958). Subliminal

stimulation: An overview. American Psychologist, 13, 229-242.

Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know:
Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231-

259.

Piaget, J. (1937). The construction of reality in the child. New York:

Basic Books.

Poetzl, O. (1917). The relationship between experimentally induced

dream images and indirect vision [Monograph No. 7). Psychological

Issues, 2, 41-120.

Saegert, S. C, & JeUison, J. M. (1970). Effects of initial level of response

competition and frequency of exposure on liking and exploratory behavior. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 16, 553-558.

Seamon, J. G., Brody, N., & Kauff, D. M. (1983a). Affective discrimination of stimuli that are not
recognized: Effects of shadowing, masking,

and cerebral laterally Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 544-555.

Seamon, J. G., Brody, N., & Kauff, D. M. (1983b). Affective discrimination of stimuli that are not
recognized: II. Effect of delay between

study and test. Bulletin of the Psychometric Society, 21, 187-189.

Seamon, J. G., Marsh, R. L., & Brody, N. (1984). Critical importance

of exposure duration for affective discrimination of stimuli that are

not recognized. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,

Memory, and Cognition, 10, 465-469.

Silverman, L. H. (1966). A technique for the study of psychodynamic

relationships: The effects of subliminally-presented aggressive stimuli

on the production of pathological thinking in a schizophrenic population. Journal ofConsulting


Psychology, 30, 103-111.

Silverman, L. H. (1977). Ethical considerations and guidelines in the

use of subliminal psychodynamic activation. Unpublished manuscript, New York University, Research
Center for Mental Health.

Silverman, L. H. (1983). The subliminal psychodynamic activation

method: Overview and comprehensive listing of studies. In J. Masling

(Ed.), Empirical studies of psychoanalytic theories (pp. 69-100).


Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Spence, D. P. (1964). Conscious and preconscious influences on recall.

Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 92-99.

Spitzer, R. L., Cohen, J., Fliess, J. L., & Endicott, J. (1967). Quantification of agreement in psychiatric
diagnosis. Archives of General Psychiatry. 17, 83-87.

Triesman, A. M. (1969). Strategies and models of selective attention.

Psychological Review, 76, 282-299.

Vanderplas, J. M., & Garvin, E. A. (1959). The association value of

random shapes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 5 7, 147-154.

Winson, J. (1984). Brain and psyche: The biology of the unconscious.

New York: Doubleday.

Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology Monograph. 9(2, Pt. 2), 1-27.

Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35,
151-175.

Zajonc, R. B. (1984). On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist,

39, 117-123.
References

Coles, M. G. H. (1989). Modern mid-brain reading: Psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition.


Psychophysiology, 26, 251–269.

Damian, M. F. (2001). Congruity effects evoked by subliminally presented primes: automaticity rather
than semantic processing. Journal

of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 154–165.

Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le ClecH, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., et al. (1998).
Imaging unconscious semantic

priming. Nature, 395, 597–600.

Dell Acqua, R., & Grainger, I. (1999). Unconscious semantic priming from pictures. Cognition, 73, B1–
B15.

Eimer, M. (1998). The lateralized readiness potential as an on-line measure of central response
activation processes. Behavior research

methods, instruments, computers, 30, 146–156.


Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (1998). Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: Behavioral and
electrophysiological evidence.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1737–1747.

Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact.
Electroencephalography and

Clinical Neurophysiology, 55, 468–484.

A. Kiesel et al. / Consciousness and Cognition 15 (2006) 397–408 407

Greenhouse, S. W., & Geisser, S. (1954). On methods in the analysis of profile data. Psychometrika, 24,
95–112.

Greenwald, A. G., Draine, S. C., & Abrams, R. L. (1996). Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic
activation. Science, 273,

1699–1702.

Holender, D. (1986). Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal
vision, and visual masking: A

survey and appraisal. The behavioral and brain sciences, 9, 1–66.

Huynh, H., & Feldt, L. S. (1976). Estimation of the Box correction for degrees of freedom from sample
data in the randomized block and

split-plot designs. Journal of Statistics Education, 1, 69–82.

Jasper, H. H. (1958). Report of the committee on methods of clinical examination in


electroencephalography. Electroencephalography and

Clinical Neurophysiology, 10, 370–375.

Keller, P. E., Wascher, E., Prinz, W., Waszak, F., Koch, I., & Rosenbaum, D.A. (in press). Differences
between intention-based and

stimulus-based actions. Journal of Psychophysiology.

Klapp, S. T., & Haas, B. W. (2005). Nonconscious influence of masked stimuli on response selection is
limited to concrete stimulus–

response associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31,
193–209.

Klapp, S. T., & Hinkley, L. B. (2002). The negative compatibility effect: Unconscious inhibition influences
reaction time and response

selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 255–269.

Klotz, W., & Neumann, O. (1999). Motor activation without conscious discrimination in metacontrast
masking. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 976–992.

Kunde, W. (2003). Sequential modulations of stimulus–response correspondence effects depend on


awareness of response conflict.

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 198–205.

Kunde, W., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). Conscious control over the content of unconscious
cognition. Cognition, 88, 223–242.

Lleras, A., & Enns, J. T. (2004). Negative compatibility or object updating. A cautionary tale of mask-
dependent priming. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 475–493.

Mordkoff, J. T., & Gianaros, P. J. (2000). Detecting the onset of the lateralized readiness potential: A
comparison of available methods

and procedures. Psychophysiology, 37, 347–360.

Neumann, O., & Klotz, W. (1994). Motor responses to nonreportable, masked stimuli: Where is the limit
of direct parameter specification?

In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV. Conscious and nonconscious
information processing

(pp. 123–150). Cambridge: MIT Press.

Pratkanis, A. R. (1992). The cargo-cult sciece of subliminal persuation. Skeptical Inquirer, 16.

Quintana, S. M., & Maxwell, S. E. (1994). A Monte Carlo comparison of seven e-adjustment procedures
in repeated measures designs with

small sample sizes. Journal of Statistics Education, 19, 57–71.

Reingold, E. M., & Merikle, P. M. (1993). Theory and measurement in the study of unconscious
processes. In M. Davies & G. Humphreys

(Eds.). Consciousness: Psychological and philosophical essays. Readings in mind and language (Vol. 2, pp.
40–57). Malden, MA, US:

Blackwell Publishers.

Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2004). Masked prime stimuli can bias ‘‘free’’ choices between response
alternatives. Psychonomic Bulletin

& Review, 11, 463–468.

Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2002). Motor activation with and without inhibition: Evidence for a
threshold mechanism in motor

control. Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 148–162.


Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-prime users guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology
Software Tools.

Shanks, D. R., & John, M. F. (1994). Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems. Behavioral &
Brain Sciences, 17, 367–447.

Verleger, R., Jaskowski, P., Aydemir, A., van der Lubbe, R. H. J., & Groen, M. (2004). Qualitative
differences between conscious and

nonconscious processing? On inverse priming induced by masked arrows. Journal of Experimental


Psychology: General, 133, 494–515.

Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T., & Schwarzbach, J. (2003). Different time courses for
visual perception and action

priming. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 6275–
6280.

Waszak, F., Wascher, E., Keller, P. E., Koch, I., Aschersleben, G., Rosenbaum, D. A., et al. (2005).
Intention-based and stimulus-based

mechanisms in action selection. Experimental brain research, 162, 346–356.

-Avant, L. L. and Thieman, A. A. 1985. On visual access to letter case and lexical/semantic

information. Memory and Cognition 13: 393–404.

Balota, D. A. 1983. Automatic semantic activation and episodic memory. Journal of verbal

Learning and Verbal Behavior 22: 88–104.

Bauer, R. M. 1984. Autonomic recognition of names and faces in prosopagnosia: A

neuropsychological application of the Guilty Knowledge Test. Neuropsychologia 224:

457–469.

Baars, B. J. 1994. A thoroughly empirical approach to consciousness. PSYCHE 16.

Cheesman, J. and Merikle, P. M. 1984. Priming with and without awareness. Perception and

Psychophysics 36: 387–395.

Cheesman, J. and Merikle, P. M. 1986. Distinguishing conscious from unconscious perceptual

processes. Canadian Journal of Psychology 40: 343–367.

Dixon, L. F. 1971. Subliminal Perception: The Nature of a Controversy. New York: McGraw

Hill.

Farah, M. J. 1989. Mechanisms of imagery-perception interaction. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 15: 203–211.


Farah, M. J. 1994. Specialisation within visual object recognition: Clues from prosopagnosia and alexia.
In: M. J. Farah and F. Ratcliff (eds), The Neuropsychology of HighLevel Vision: Collected Tutorial Essays,
pp. 133–146. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum

Associates.

Fendrich, R., Wessinger, C. M. and Gazzaniga, M. S. 2000. Speculations on the neural basis

of islands of blindsight. Progress in Brain Research 134: 353–366.

Fowler, C. A., Wolford, G., Slade, R. and Tassinary, L. 1981. Lexical access with

and without awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 110: 341–

362.

Georgeson, M. 1997. Vision and action: You ain’t seen nothin’ yet .... Perception 26

(http://www.perceptionweb.com/perc0197/editorial.html).

Groeger, J. 1988. Qualitatively different effects of undetected and unidentified auditory primes.

Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 40A: 323–339.

Hines, D., Czerwinski, M., Sawyer, P. K. and Dwyer, M. 1986. Automatic semantic priming:

Effect of category exemplar level and word association level. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 12: 370–379.

Jack, A. I. and Shallice, T. 2001. Introspective physicalism as an approach to the science of

consciousness. Cognition 79: 161–196.

Kemp-Wheeler, S. M. and Hill, A. B. 1988. Semantic priming without awareness: Some

methodological considerations and replications.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 40A: 671–


692.

Kihlstrom, J. F. 1987. The cognitive unconscious. Science 237: 1145–1151.

Kolb, F. C. and Braun, J. 1995. Blindsight in normal observers. Nature 377 (6547): 336–

338.

Kotze, H. F. and Moller, A. T. 1990. Effect of auditory subliminal stimulation on GSR. Psychological
Reports 67: 931–934.

22 THOMAS ZOEGA RAMS ¨ ØY AND MORTEN OVERGAARD

Kunst-Wilson, W. R. and Zajonc, R. B. 1980. Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot

be recognized. Science 207: 557–558.

Lutz, A. 2002. Toward a neurophenomenology as an account of generative passages: A first


empirical case study. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 1 (2): 133–167.

McCauley, C., Parmelee, C. M. and Sperber, R. D. 1980. Early extraction of meaning from

pictures and its relation to conscious identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology:

Human Perception and Performance 62: 265–276.

Mangan, B. 2001. Sensation’s Ghost. The Non-Sensory “Fringe” of Consciousness. PSYCHE

718 (October).

Marcel, A. J. 1983. Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking

and word recognition, Cognitive Psychology 15: 197–237.

Marcel, A. J. 1998. Blindsight and shape perception: deficit of visual consciousness or of visual

function? Brain 121 (Part 8): 1565–1588.

Merikle, P. M. and Daneman, M. 1998a. Memory for unconsciously perceived events: Evidence

from anesthetized patients. Consciousness and Cognition 5: 525–541.

Merikle, P. M. and Daneman, M. 1998b. Psychological investigations of unconscious perception. Journal


of Consciousness Studies 5: 5–18.

Merikle, P. M. and Daneman, M. 2000. Conscious vs. unconscious perception. In: M. S.

Gazzaniga (ed), The New Cognitive Neurosciences, 2nd edn., pp. 1295–1303. Cambridge,

MA: MIT Press.

Merikle, P. M. and Joordens, S. 1997a. Measuring unconscious influences. In: J. D. Cohen and

J. W. Schooler (eds), Scientific Approaches to Consciousness, pp. 109–123. Mahwah, NJ:

Erlbaum.

Merikle, P. M. and Joordens, S. 1997b. Parallels between perception without attention and

perception without awareness. Consciousness and Cognition 6: 219–236.

Merikle, P. M., Smilek, D. and Eastwood, J. D. 2001. Perception without awareness: Perspectives

from cognitive psychology. Cognition 79: 115–134.

Norman, E. 2002. Subcategories of “fringe consciousness” and their related nonconscious contexts.
PSYCHE 815 (October) (http://psyche.cs.monash.edu.au/v8/psyche-8–15-

norman.html).

Overgaard, M., Kauffmann, O. and Ramsøy, T. Z. 2001. Consciousness and introspection.

Journal of Consciousness Studies: Consciousness Research Abstract, 182.


Overgaard, M. and Sørensen, T. A. (submitted). Introspection as a process distinct from first

order experiences. Journal of Consciousness Studies.

Peirce, C. S. and Jastrow, J. 1884. On small differences of sensation. Memoirs of National

Academy of Sciences 3: 73–83.

Ptito, A., Fortin, A. and Ptito, M. 2001. ‘Seeing’ in the blind hemifield following hemispherectomy.
Progress in Brain Research 134: 367–378.

Purcell, D. G., Stewart, A. L. and Stanovich, K. E. 1983. Another look at semantic priming

without awareness. Perception and Psychophysics 34: 65–71.

Rosenthal, D. 2000a. Introspection and self-interpretation. Philosophical Topics 28 (2): 201–

233.

Rosenthal, D. 2000b. Metacognition and higher-order thoughts. Consciousness and Cognition

9 (2): 231–242.

Schooler, J. W. 2002. Re-representing consciousness: Dissociations between experience and

meta-consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6 (8): 339–344.

Sidis, B. 1898. The Psychology of Suggestion. New York: Appleton and Company.

Stambrook, M. and Martin, D. G. 1983. Brain laterality and the subliminal perception of facial
expression.International Journal of Neuroscience 18 (1/2): 45–

58.

INTROSPECTION AND SUBLIMINAL PERCEPTION 23

Stroh, M. A., Shaw, M. and Washburn, M. F. 1908. A study of guessing. American Journal of

Psychology 19: 243–245.

Tranel, D. and Damasio, A. R. 1985. Knowledge without awareness: an autonomic index of

facial recognition by prosopagnosics. Science 228 (4706): 1453–1454.

Weiskrantz, L. 1986. Blindsight: A Case Study and Implications. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Zeki, S. and Ffytche, D. H. 1998. The Riddoch syndrome: Insights into the neurobiology of

conscious vision. Brain 121: 25–45

- Aarts H Custers R Marien H. Preparing and motivating behavior outside of awareness.


Science2008;319:1639. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1150432.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed
Abadie M Waroquier L Terrier P. Gist memory in the unconscious-thought effect. Psychol
Sci2013;24:1253–9. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612470958.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Addante RJ. A critical role of the human hippocampus in an electrophysiological measure of implicit
memory. NeuroImage2015;109:515–28. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.069.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Arzi A Holtzman Y Samnon P , et al. . Olfactory aversive conditioning during sleep reduces cigarette-
smoking behavior. J Neurosci2014;34:15382–93. http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2291-14.2014.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Arzi A Shedlesky L Ben-Shaul M , et al. . Humans can learn new information during sleep. Nat
Neurosci2012;15:1460–5. http://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3193

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Atas A Faivre N Timmermans B , et al. . Nonconscious learning from crowded sequences. Psychol
Sci2013. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613499591.

Google Scholar

Bergström F Eriksson J. The conjunction of non-consciously perceived object identity and spatial
position can be retained during a visual short-term memory task. Conscious
Res2015;1470.http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01470

Google Scholar

Bermeitinger C Goelz R Johr N , et al. . The hidden persuaders break into the tired brain. J Exp Soc
Psychol2009;45:320–6. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.10.001

Google ScholarCrossref

Bettman JR Luce MF Payne JW. Constructive consumer choice processes. J Cons Res1998;25:187–217.
http://doi.org/10.1086/209535

Google ScholarCrossref

Capa RL Cleeremans A Bustin GM , et al. . Long-lasting effect of subliminal processes on cardiovascular


responses and performance. Int J Psychophysiol2011;81:22–30.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.04.001

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Chen JCW Li W Lui M , et al. . Left-frontal brain potentials index conceptual implicit memory for words
initially viewed subliminally. Brain Res2009;1285:135–47. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2009.05.085

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed
Chong TTJ Husain M Rosenthal CR. Recognizing the unconscious. Curr Biol2014;24:R1033–5.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.09.035

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Chun MM Phelps EA. Memory deficits for implicit contextual information in amnesic subjects with
hippocampal damage. Nat Neurosci1999;2:844–7. http://doi.org/10.1038/12222

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Daltrozzo J Claude L Tillmann B , et al. . Working memory is partially preserved during sleep. PLoS
ONE2012;7:e50997.http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0050997

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Dew ITZ Cabeza R. The porous boundaries between explicit and implicit memory: behavioral and neural
evidence. Ann NY Acad Sci2011;1224:174–90. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05946.x

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Dienes Z. Understanding Psychology as a Science: An Introduction to Scientific and Statistical Inference.


New York: Palgrave Macmillan, 2008.

Google Scholar

Dienes Z. Using Bayes to get the most out of non-significant results. Quant Psychol
Meas2014;5:781.http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00781

Google Scholar

Dijksterhuis A Bos MW Nordgren LF , et al. . On making the right choice: the deliberation-without-
attention effect. Science2006;311:1005–7. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1121629.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Dijksterhuis A Nordgren LF. A theory of unconscious thought. Perspect Psychol Sci2006;1:95–109.


http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00007.x.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Duss SB Oggier S Reber TP , et al. . Formation of semantic associations between subliminally presented
face-word pairs. Conscious Cogn2011;20:928–35. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.03.018.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Duss SB Reber TP Hänggi J , et al. . Unconscious relational encoding depends on hippocampus.


Brain2014;137:3355–70. http://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awu270.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Farooqui AA Manly T. Anticipatory control through associative learning of subliminal relations: invisible
may be better than visible. Psychol Sci2015;26:325–34. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614564191.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed
Ferrand L. The masked repetition priming effect dissipates when increasing the inter-stimulus interval:
evidence from word naming. Acta Psychologica1996;91:15–25. http://doi.org/10.1016/0001-
6918(95)00010-0.

Google ScholarCrossref

Forster K Booker J Schacter DL , et al. . Masked repetition priming: lexical activation or novel memory
trace?Bull Psychon Soc1990;28:341–5. http://doi.org/10.3758/BF03334039.

Google ScholarCrossref

Gaillard R Cohen L Adam C , et al. . Subliminal words durably affect neuronal activity.
NeuroReport2007;18:1527–31. http://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f0b6cd.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

García-Orza J Damas-López J Matas A , et al. . “2 x 3” primes naming “6”: evidence from masked
priming. Atten Percept Psychophys2009;71:471–80. http://doi.org/10.3758/APP.71.3.471

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Greene AJ Gross WL Elsinger CL , et al. . An fMRI analysis of the human hippocampus: inference,
context, and task awareness. J Cogn Neurosci2006;18:1156–73.
http://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.7.1156

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Greene AJ Gross WL Elsinger CL , et al. . Hippocampal differentiation without recognition: an fMRI


analysis of the contextual cueing task. Learn Mem2007;14:548–53. http://doi.org/10.1101/lm.609807.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Greenwald AG Draine SC Abrams RL. Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation.
Science1996;273:1699–702. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.273.5282.1699

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Greenwald AG Klinger MR Schuh ES. Activation by marginally perceptible (“subliminal”) stimuli:


dissociation of unconscious from conscious cognition. J Exp Psychol-Gen1995;124:22–42.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Hannula DE Greene AJ. The hippocampus reevaluated in unconscious learning and memory: at a tipping
point? Front Hum Neurosci 2012;6. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00080.

Google Scholar

Hannula DE Ranganath C. The eyes have it: hippocampal activity predicts expression of memory in eye
movements. Neuron2009;63:592–9. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.025.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed
Harand C Bertran F La Joie R , et al. . The hippocampus remains activated over the long term for the
retrieval of truly episodic memories. PLoS
ONE2012;7:e43495.http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0043495.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Hassin RR. Yes it can: on the functional abilities of the human unconscious. Perspect Psychol
Sci2013;8:195–207. http://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612460684.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Hassin RR Ferguson MJ Shidlovski D , et al. . Subliminal exposure to national flags affects political
thought and behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA2007;104:19757–61.
http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0704679104.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Henke K. A model for memory systems based on processing modes rather than consciousness. Nat Rev
Neurosci2010;11:523–32. http://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2850.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Henke K Buck A Weber B , et al. . Human hippocampus establishes associations in memory.


Hippocampus1997;7:249–56. http://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1997)7:3<249::AID-
HIPO1>3.0.CO;2-G.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Henke K Mondadori CRA Treyer V , et al. . Nonconscious formation and reactivation of semantic
associations by way of the medial temporal lobe. Neuropsychologia2003;41:863–76.
http://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00035-6.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Henke K Reber TP Duss SB. Integrating events across levels of consciousness. Front Behav
Neurosci2013;7:68.http://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00068.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Hesselmann G Moors P. Definitely maybe: can unconscious processes perform the same functions as
conscious processes? Conscious Res 2015;584. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00584.

Google Scholar

Holdstock JS Mayes AR Isaac CL , et al. . Differential involvement of the hippocampus and temporal lobe
cortices in rapid and slow learning of new semantic information. Neuropsychologia2002;40:748–68.
http://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00192-0.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Karpinski A Yale M Briggs JC. Unconscious arithmetic processing: a direct replication. Eur J Soc
Psychol2016; n/a-n/a. http://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2175.
Google Scholar

Karremans JC Stroebe W Claus J. Beyond Vicary’s fantasies: the impact of subliminal priming and brand
choice. J Exp Soc Psychol2006;42:792–8. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.12.002.

Google ScholarCrossref

Kawakami N Yoshida F. Perceiving a story outside of conscious awareness: when we infer narrative
attributes from subliminal sequential stimuli. Conscious Cogn2015;33:53–66.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.12.001.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Kiesel A Wagener A Kunde W , et al. . Unconscious manipulation of free choice in humans. Conscious
Cogn2006;15:397–408. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.10.002.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Klapp ST Haas BW. Nonconscious influence of masked stimuli on response selection is limited to
concrete stimulus-response associations. J Exp Psychol-Hum Percept Perform2005;31:193–209.
http://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.31.1.193.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Lau HC Passingham RE. Unconscious activation of the cognitive control system in the human prefrontal
cortex. J Neurosci2007;27:5805–11. http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4335-06.2007.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Levy BR Pilver C Chung PH , et al. . Subliminal strengthening: improving older individuals’ physical
function over time with an implicit-age-stereotype intervention. Psychol Sci2014;2525:2127–35.
http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614551970.

Google ScholarCrossref

Lowery BS Eisenberger NI Hardin CD , et al. . Long-term effects of subliminal priming on academic


performance. BASP2007;29:151–7. http://doi.org/10.1080/01973530701331718.

Google Scholar

McClelland JL McNaughton BL O’reilly RC. Why there are complementary learning systems in the
hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of
learning and memory. Psychol Rev1995;102:419–57.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Mealor AD Dienes Z. Conscious and unconscious thought in artificial grammar learning. Conscious
Cogn2012;21:865–74. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2012.03.001.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Miller J. Measurement error in subliminal perception experiments: simulation analyses of two


regression methods. J Exp Psychol-Hum Percept Perform2000;26:1461–77.
Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Monahan JL Murphy ST Zajonc RB. Subliminal mere exposure: specific, general, and diffuse effects.
Psychol Sci2000;11:462–6. http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00289.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Moscovitch M. Recovered consciousness: a hypothesis concerning modularity and episodic memory. J


Clin Exp Neuropsychol1995;17:276–90. http://doi.org/10.1080/01688639508405123.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Mudrik L Breska A Lamy D , et al. . Integration without awareness: expanding the limits of unconscious
processing. Psychol Sci2011;22:764–70. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611408736.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Mudrik L Faivre N Koch C. Information integration without awareness. Trends Cogn Sci2014;18:488–6.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.009.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Mudrik L Koch C. Differential processing of invisible congruent and incongruent scenes: a case for
unconscious integration. J Vis2013;13:24.http://doi.org/10.1167/13.13.24.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Negash S Kliot D Howard DV , et al. . Relationship of contextual cueing and hippocampal volume in
amnestic mild cognitive impairment patients and cognitively normal older adults. J Int Neuropsychol
Soc2015;21:285–96. http://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617715000223.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Newell BR Shanks DR. Unconscious influences on decision making: a critical review. Behav Brain
Sci2014;1–19. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12003214.

Google Scholar

Nieuwenstein MR Wierenga T Morey RD , et al. . On making the right choice: a meta-analysis and large-
scale replication attempt of the unconscious thought advantage. Judgm Decis Mak2015;10:1–17.

Google Scholar

Ocampo B. Unconscious manipulation of free choice by novel primes. Conscious Cogn2015;34:4–9.


http://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.03.007.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Olsen RK Moses SN Riggs L , et al. . The hippocampus supports multiple cognitive processes through
relational binding and comparison. Front Hum
Neurosci2012;6:146.http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00146.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed
Parkinson J Haggard P. Subliminal priming of intentional inhibition. Cognition2014;130:255–65.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.005.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Reber TP DoLam A Elger CE , et al. . Intracranial EEG correlates of implicit relational inference within the
hippocampus. Hippocampus2016;26:54–66. http://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.22490.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Reber TP Henke K. Integrating unseen events over time. Conscious Cogn2012;21:953–60.


http://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2012.02.013

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Reber TP Luechinger R Boesiger P , et al. . Unconscious relational inference recruits the hippocampus. J
Neurosci2012;32:6138–48. http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5639-11.2012.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Reder LM Park H Kieffaber PD. Memory systems do not divide on consciousness: Reinterpreting
memory in terms of activation and binding. Psychol Bull2009;135:23–49.
http://doi.org/10.1037/a0013974.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Reuss H Kiesel A Kunde W , et al. . Unconscious activation of task sets. Conscious Cogn2011;20:556–67.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.02.014.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Ruby P Caclin A Boulet S , et al. . Odd sound processing in the sleeping brain. J Cognitive
Neurosci2008;20:296–311. http://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20023.

Google ScholarCrossref

Ruch S Koenig T Mathis J , et al. . Word encoding during sleep is suggested by correlations between
word-evoked up-states and post-sleep semantic priming. Front
Psychol2014;5:1319.http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01319.

Google ScholarPubMed

Schlaghecken F Eimer M. Masked prime stimuli can bias “free” choices between response alternatives.
Psychon Bull Rev2004;11:463–8. http://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196596.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Shanks DR. Learning: from association to cognition. Annu Rev Psychol2010;61:273–301.


http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100519.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed
Simonson I. In defense of consciousness: the role of conscious and unconscious inputs in consumer
choice. J Cons Psychol2005;15:211–17. http://doi.org/10.1207/s15327663jcp1503_5.

Google ScholarCrossref

Sklar AY Levy N Goldstein A , et al. . Reading and doing arithmetic nonconsciously. Proc Natl Acad Sci
USA2012. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1211645109.

Google Scholar

Squire LR Zola SM. Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. Proc Natl
Acad Sci USA1996;93:13515–22.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Strahan EJ Spencer SJ Zanna MP. Subliminal priming and persuasion: striking while the iron is hot. J Exp
Soc Psychol2002;38:556–68. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00502-4

Google ScholarCrossref

Tononi G. An information integration theory of consciousness. BMC


Neurosci2004;5:42.http://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-5-42.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Tulving E. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev Psychol2002;53:1–25. http://doi.org/Article

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Vadillo MA Kostopoulou O Shanks DR. A critical review and meta-analysis of the unconscious thought
effect in medical decision making. Conscious Res2015;636.http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00636.

Google Scholar

Van den Bussche E Van den Noortgate W Reynvoet B. Mechanisms of masked priming: a meta-analysis.
Psychol Bull2009;135:452–77. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0015329.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

van Gaal S De Lange FP Cohen MX. The role of consciousness in cognitive control and decision making.
Front Hum Neurosci2012;6:121.http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00121.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Van Opstal F de Lange FP Dehaene S. Rapid parallel semantic processing of numbers without
awareness. Cognition2011;120:136–47. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.03.005.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Verwijmeren T Karremans JC Bernritter SF , et al. . Warning: You are being primed! The effect of a
warning on the impact of subliminal ads. J Exp Soc Psychol2013;49:1124–9.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.06.010.

Google ScholarCrossref
Verwijmeren T Karremans JC Stroebe W , et al. . The workings and limits of subliminal advertising: the
role of habits. J Cons Psychol2011;21:206–13. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2010.11.004.

Google ScholarCrossref

Vlassova A Donkin C Pearson J. Unconscious information changes decision accuracy but not confidence.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA2014;111:16214–8. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1403619111.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed

Weinberger J Westen D. RATS, we should have used Clinton: subliminal priming in political campaigns.
Polit Psychol2008;29:631–51. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00658.x.

Google ScholarCrossref

Züst MA Colella P Reber TP , et al. . Hippocampus is place of interaction between unconscious and
conscious memories. PLoS ONE2015;10:e0122459.http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0122459.

Google ScholarCrossrefPubMed
to consciousness, top-down processing has been suggested as a key feature (Dehaene & Naccache, 2001). Here,
we argue thatit is not crucial because these processes can occur outside consciousness. Also, the current study
demonstrates that top-down processing can be differentiated from ‘mental effort’, another feature that is
proposed to be critical to consciousness(Dehaene et al., 1998): Top-down modulation changed unconsciously, so it
can hardly be qualified as effortful. This does not,of course, imply that other features of consciousness are not
crucial either. For example, recent research demonstrates that‘seriality’ may be crucial in the sense that chaining
mental operations requires consciousness (Sackur & Dehaene, 2009; VanOpstal, de Lange, & Dehaene, 2011).A
theory of consciousness should clarify which features are crucial for consciousness. Developing such a theory is
obvi-ously beyond the scope of the current paper, but the paper does show that a feature which standardly has
been assumed tobe crucial, actually is not. As such, it underscores the power of unconscious
processes.AcknowledgmentF.V.O. is a Postdoctoral Fellow of the Research Foundation–Flanders (FWO-
Vlaanderen). F.V.O. and T.V. were supported byGOA B/09019/02.References Anderson, B. L., & Winawer, J. (2005). Image
segmentation and lightness perception.Nature, 434, 79–83.Bodner, G. E., & Masson, M. E. J. (2003). Beyond spreading activation: An influence
of relatedness proportion on masked semantic priming.PsychonomicBulletin & Review, 10, 645–652.Bodner, G. E., & Masson, M. E. J. (2004).
Beyond binary judgments: Prime validity modulates masked repetition priming in the naming task.Memory &Cognition, 32, 1–11.Bodner, G. E.,
Masson, M. E. J., & Richard, N. T. (2006). Repetition proportion biases masked priming of lexical decisions.Memory & Cognition, 34, 1298–
1311.Boles, D. B., & Clifford, J. E. (1989). An upper- and lowercase alphabetic similarity matrix, with derived generation similarity
values.Behavioral ResearchMethods, Instruments, and Computers, 21, 579–586.Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2011). Learning of predictive relations
between events depends on attention, not on awareness.Consciousness and Cognition, 20,368–378.Dehaene, S., & Naccache, L. (2001).
Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework.Cognition, 79, 1–37.Dehaene, S., Naccache, L.,
Cohen, L., Le Bihan, D., Mangin, J.-F., Poline, J.-B., et al (2001). Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious
repetitionpriming.Nature Neuroscience, 4, 752–758.Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Cleq’h, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., et
al (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming.Nature, 395,597–600.Eiser, J. R. (1990).Social judgment. Pacific Grove, CA:
Brooks/Cole.Heinemann, A., Kunde, W., & Kiesel, A. (2009). Context-specific prime-congruency effects: On the role conscious stimulus
representations for cognitivecontrol.Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 966–976.Kiesel, A., Kunde, W., Pohl, C., Berner, M. P., & Hoffmann, J.
(2009). Playing chess unconsciously.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory andCognition, 35, 292–298.Kinoshita, S., Forster, K.
I., & Mozer, M. I. (2008). Unconscious cognition isn’t that smart: Modulation of masked repetition priming effect in the word
namingtask.Cognition, 107, 623–649.Laming, D. R. J. (1984). The relativity of ‘‘absolute’’ judgements.British Journal of Mathematical and
Statistical Psychology, 37, 152–183.Lau, H., & Passingham, R. E. (2007). Unconscious activation of the cognitive control system in the human
prefrontal cortex.Journal of Neuroscience, 27,5805–5811.Moors, A., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity: A theoretical and conceptual
approach.Psychological Bulletin, 132, 297–326.Parducci, A. (1954). Learning variables in the judgment of single stimuli.Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 48, 24–30.Sackur, J., & Dehaene, S. (2009). The cognitive architecture for chaining of two mental operations.Cognition, 111, 187–
211.Shin, E., Fabiani, M., & Gratton, G. (2006). Multiple levels of stimulus representation in visual working memory.Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 18,844–858.Stewart, N., Brown, G. D. A., & Chater, N. (2005). Absolute identification by relative judgment.Psychological Review,
112, 881–911.Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice.Science, 211, 453–458.Van den
Bussche, E., Segers, G., & Reynvoet, B. (2008). Conscious and unconscious proportion effects in masked priming.Consciousness and Cognition,
17,1345–1358.van Gaal, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Fahrenfort, J. J., Scholte, H. S., & Lamme, V. A. (2008). Frontal cortex mediates unconsciously
triggered inhibitory control.Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 8053–8062.Van Gaal, S., Ridderinkhof, R. K., Scholte, S. H., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2010).
Unconscious activation of the prefrontal no-go network.Journal of Neuroscience, 30,4143–4150.Van Opstal, F., de Lange, F. P., & Dehaene, S.
(2011). Rapid parallel semantic processing of numbers without awareness.Cognition, 120, 136–147.Van Opstal, F., Gevers, W., Osman, M., &
Verguts, T. (2010). Unconscious task application.Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 999–1006.Van Opstal, F., Moors, A., Fias, W., & Verguts, T.
(2008). Offline and online automatic number comparison.Psychological Research, 72, 347–352.Van Opstal, F., Reynvoet, B., & Verguts, T.
(2005). How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, & Hoffmann (2003).Cognition, 97,89–97.Vuilleumier, P., Henson, R.
N., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Multiple levels of visual object constancy revealed by event-related fMRI of repetition priming.Nature
Neuroscience, 5, 491–499

-References

[1] Astolfi, L., Toppi, J., Fallani, F.D., Vecchiato, G., Cincotti, F. et al. 2011. Imaging the social brain by

simultaneous hyperscanning during subject interaction. IEEE Intelligent Systems, 26(5): 38–45. DOI:

10.1109/MIS.2011.61

[2] Axelrod, V., Bar, M., Rees, G. 2015. Exploring the unconscious using faces. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences.

19(1): 35-45. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.11.003

[3] Babiloni, C., Miniussi, C., Babiloni, F., Carducci, F., Cincotti, F. et al. 2004. Sub-second ‘‘temporal
attention’’

modulates alpha rhythms.A high-resolution EEG study. Cognitive Brain Research, 19(3): 259 – 268. DOI:

10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.12.010

[4] Bacik, R., Gavurora, B., Fedorko, I. 2015. The analysis of the impact of selected marketing
communication

factors on the online consumer behavior. Journal of Applied Economic Sciences, Volume X, Winter,
37(7):

999-1004.

[5] Balconi, M., Ferrari, C. 2012. Subliminal and supraliminal processing of facial expression of emotions:
Brain

oscillation in the left/right frontal area. Brain Sciences, 2: 85-100. DOI: 10.3390/brainsci2020085

[6] Bensmann, W., Vahid, A., Beste, C., Stock, A. 2019b. The intensity of early attentional processing, but
not

conflict monitoring, determines the size of subliminal response conflicts. Frontiers in Human
Neuroscience, 13:

1-18. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00053

[7] Bensmann, W., Zink, N., Mückschel, M., Beste, C., Stock, A. 2019a. Neuronal networks underlying the
conjoint
modulation of response selection by subliminal and consciously induced cognitive conflicts. Brain
Structure

and Function, 224: 1697–1709. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-019-01866-0

[8] Brooks, S., Savov, V., Allzén, E., Benedict, C., Fredriksson, R., Schiöth, H. 2012. Exposure to subliminal

arousing stimuli induces robust activation in the amygdala, hippocampus, anterior cingulate, insular
cortex and

primary visual cortex: A systematic meta-analysis of fMRI studies. NeuroImage, 59: 2962–2975. DOI:

10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.077

[9] Broyles, S.J. 2006. Subliminal advertising and the perpetual popularity of playing to people’s
paranoia. The

Journal of Consumer Affairs, 40(2): 329–406. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6606.2006.00063.x

[10] Button, K.S., Ioannidis, J.P., Mokrysz, C., Nosak, B.A., Flint, J., Robinson, E.S., Munafò, M.R. 2013.
Power

failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,
14:

365–376.

[11] Cacioppo, S., Grafton, S.T. and Bianchi-Demicheli, F. 2012. The speed of passionate love, as a
subliminal

prime: A high-density electrical neuroimaging study. NeuroQuantology, 10(4): 715-724. DOI:

10.14704/nq.2012.10.4.509

[12] Chalfoun, P., Frasson, C. 2011. Subliminal cues while teaching: HCI technique for enhanced learning.

Advances in Human-Computer Interaction, Article ID 968753, 15 p. DOI:10.1155/2011/968753. Available


at:

https://www.hindawi.com/journals/ahci/2011/968753/

[13] Charles, L., Opstal, F.V., Marti, S., Dehaene, S. 2013. Distinct brain mechanisms for conscious versus

subliminal error detection. Neuroimage, 73: 80–94. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.054

[14] Cruz, A.L., Arango-Muñoz, S., Volz, K.G. 2016. Oops, scratch that! Monitoring one’s own errors
during mental.

Cognition, 146: 110–120. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.005

Journal of Applied Economic Sciences

814
[15] Dall’Acqua, T., Begliomini, C., Motta, R., Miotto, D., and Castiello, U. 2018. Effects of intentionality
and

subliminal information in free-choices to inhibit. Neuropsychologia, 109: 28–38. DOI:

10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.11.035

[16] Dimoka, A., Banker, R.D., Benbasat, I., Davis, F.D., Dennis, A.R. et al. 2012. On the use of
neurophysicological

tools in IS research: Developing a research agenda for neuroIS. MIS Quarterly, 36(3): 679–702

[17] Doyen, S., Klein, O., Simons, D.J., Cleeremans, A. 2014. On the other side of the mirror: Priming in
cognitive

and social psychology. Social Cognition, 32: 12-32. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2014.32.supp.12

[18] Elci, M., Sert, A. 2017. Subliminal advertising and its ethical dimensions in the social media age. In I.
R.

Association, Advertising and Branding: Concepts, Methodologies, Tools, and Applications, 91–110 pp.

Hershey, PA: IGI Global. DOI: 10.4018/978-1-4666-8125-5.ch020

[19] Elgendi, M., Kumar, P., Barbic, S., Howard, N., Abbott, D., Cichocki, A. 2018. Subliminal priming -
State of the

art and future perspectives. Behavioral Sciences, 8: 54. DOI: 10.3390/bs8060054

[20] Farnsworth, B. 2017. EEG vs. MRI vs. fMRI – What are the differences? Available at:
https://imotions.com/blog/

eeg-vs-mri-vs-fmri-differences

[21] Gabrieli, J. 2011. Introduction to Psychology. Available at:


https://theopenacademy.com/content/lecture-4-

brain-ii-methods-research.

[22] Gao, Y., Zhang, H. 2014. Unconscious processing modulates creative problem solving: Evidence from
an

electrophysiological study. Consciousness and Cognition, 26: 64–73. DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.03.001

[23] Garner, W.R., Hake, H.W., Eriksen, C.W. 1956. Operationism and the concept of perception.
Psychological

Review, 63(3): 149–159. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0042992

[24] Giattino, C.M., Alam, Z.M., Woldorff, M. G. 2018. Neural processes underlying the orienting of
attention without

awareness. Cortex, 102: 14-25. DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.07.010


[25] Gibbons, H. 2009. Evaluative priming from subliminal emotional words: Insights from event-related
potentials

and individual differences related to anxiety. Consciousness and Cognition, 18: 383–400. DOI:

10.1016/j.concog.2009.02.007

[26] Gillath, O., Canterberry, M. 2012. Neural correlates of exposure to subliminal and supraliminal
sexual cues.

Scan, 7: 924-936. DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsr065

[27] Grau, C., Ginhoux, R., Riera, A., Nguyen, T.L., Chauvat, H. et al. 2014. Conscious brain-to-brain

communication in humans using non-invasive technologies. Plos One, 9(8): e105225. DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0105225

[28] Harris, P., Murawski, C. 2010. Unconscious brand reactions influence financial decision-making.
Advances in

Consumer Research, 37: 810–812

[29] Hartmann, P., Apaolaza, V., Alija, P. 2013. Nature imagery in advertising. International Journal of
Advertising,

23(2): 183–210. DOI: https://doi.org/10.2501/IJA-32-2-183-210

[30] Hashimoto, T., Umeda, S., Kojima, S. 2011. Neural substrates of implicit cueing effect on prospective
memory.

NeuroImage, 54: 645–652. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.047

[31] Hollenstein, M., Koenig, T., Kubat, M., Blaser, D., Perrig, W.J. 2012. Non-conscious word processing
in a

mirror-masking paradigm causing attentional distraction: An ERP-study. Consciousness and Cognition,


21:

353–365. DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.01.005

[32] Hsu, M. 2017. Neuromarketing: Inside the mind of the consumer. California Management Review,
59(4): 5–

22. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0008125617720208

[33] Ibáñez, A., Hurtado, E., Lobos, A., Escobar, J., Trujillo, N. et al. 2011. Subliminal presentation of
other faces

(but not own face) primes behavioral and evoked cortical processing of empathy for pain. Brain
Research,

1398: 72–85. DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.05.014


Journal of Applied Economic Sciences

815

[34] Ikpefan, O.A., Olaolu, R., Omankhanlen, E. A., Osuwa, G.O., Evbuomwan, G. 2018. Impact of
marketing of

deposit money bank services on customers’ patronage and loyalty. Empirical study of five deposit
money banks

in Nigeria, Journal of Applied Economic Sciences, Volume XIII, 62(8): 2446-2459.

[35] Jiang, Y., Wu, X., Saab, R., Xiao, Y., Gao, X. 2018. Time course of influence on the allocation of
attentional

resources caused by unconscious fearful faces. Neuropsychologia, 113: 104–110. DOI:

10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.001

[36] Jiménez-Ortega, L., Espuny, J., de Tejada, P.H., Vargas-Rivero, C., Martín-Loeches. M. 2017.
Subliminal

emotional words impact syntactic processing: Evidence from performance and event-related brain
potentials.

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11: 192. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00192

[37] Kasai, K., Fukuda, M., Yahata, N., Morita, K., Fujii, N. 2015. The future of real-world neuroscience:
Imaging

techniques to assess act brains in social environments. Neuroscience Research, 90: 65–71. DOI:

10.1016/j.neures.2014.11.007

[38] King, J.A., Korb, F.M., Egner, T. 2012. Priming of control: Implicit contextual cuing of top-down
attentional set.

The Journal of Neurosciencs, 32(24): 8192– 8200. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0934-12.2012

[39] Kiss, M., Eimer, M. 2008. ERPs reveal subliminal processing of fearful faces. Psychophysiology, 45(2):
318–

326. DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00634.x

[40] Kongthong, N., Minami, M., Nakauchi, N. 2013. Semantic processing in subliminal face stimuli: An
EEG and

tDCS study. Neuroscience Letters, 544:141–146. DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.04.002

[41] Kongthong, N., Minami, M., Nakauchi, N. 2013. Semantic processing in subliminal face stimuli: An
EEG and

tDCS study. Neuroscience Letters, 544:141–146. DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.04.002


[42] Košíková, K., Pilárik, Ľ. 2012. Unconscious visual perception in the decision making process. Studia

Psychologica, 54(4): 261–270.

[43] Kouider, S., Dehaene, S., Bihan, D.L. 2007. Cerebral bases of subliminal and supraliminal priming
during

reading. Cerebral Cortex, 17: 2019–2029. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhl110

[44] Lau, H.C., Passingham, R.E. 2007. Unconscious activation of the cognitive control system in the
human

prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(21): 5805–5811. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4335-


06.2007

[45] LeDoux, J.E. 1998. The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life. New York,
NY:

Simon & Schuster. ISBN: 978-0684836591, 384 p.

[46] Lia, T-T, Lu, Y. 2014. The subliminal affective priming effects of faces displaying various levels of
arousal: An

ERP study. Neuroscience Letters, 583:148-153. DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.09.027

[47] Lieberman, M.D. 2007. Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes. Annual Review of

Psychology, 58: 259–289. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085654

[48] Lowery, B., Eisenberger, N., Hardin, C., Sinclair, S. 2006. Long-term effects of subliminal priming on
academic

performance. DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.937950. Available at: https://www.gsb.stanford.edu/faculty-


research/workingpapers/long-term-effects-subliminal-priming-academic-performance

[49] Meltzer, J.A., Kielar, A., Panamsky, L., Deschamps, T., Leigh R.C. 2017. Electrophysiological
signatures of

phonological and semantic maintenance in sentence repetition. Neuroimage, 156: 302-314. DOI:

10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.030

[50] Mladenović, Ž., Marković, Z., Ljajić, S. 2016. The Use of subliminal messages in marketing:
Experimental

examination. Socioeconomica, 5(9): 39–52. DOI: 10.12803/SJSECO.59005

[51] Mnegusso, P., Tsakiris, M., Schioth, H.B., Stein, J.D., Brooks, S.J. 2014. Subliminal versus supraliminal
stimuli

activate neural responses in anterior cingulate cortex, fusiform gyrus and insula: A meta-analysis of fMRI

studies, 2(1): 52. DOI: 10.1186/s40359-014-0052-1


[52] Mohan, D. M., Kumar, P., Mahmood, F., Wong, K. F., Agrawal, A. et al. 2016. Effect of subliminal
lexical priming

on the subjective perception of images: A machine learning approach. Plos One, 11(2). DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0148332

Journal of Applied Economic Sciences

816

[53] Morsella, E., Godwin, C.A., Jantz, T.J., Krieger, S.C.,Gazzaley, A. 2016. Homing in on consciousness in
the

nervous system: An action-based synthesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39: e168. DOI:

10.1017/S0140525X15000643

[54] Newell, B.R., Shanks, D.R. 2014. Unconscious influences on decision making: A critical review. The
Behavioral

And Brain Sciences, 37(1): 1–19. DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X12003214

[55] Ochsner, K.N., Lieberman, M.D. 2001. The emergence of social cognitive neuroscience. American

Psychologist, 56(9): 717–734. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.9.717

[56] O'Connel, B., Walden, S., Pohlmann, A. 2011. Marketing and neuroscience. What drives customer
decisions?

Chicago, IL: American Marketing Association, White Paper. Available at: http://www.hoffman
marcom.com.

ama/white-papers/White%20Paper%20Neuroscience%20what%20drives%20cust%20descis ions.pdf

[57] Parkinson, J., Garfinkel, S., Critchley, H., Dienes, Z., Seth, A.K. 2016. Don’t make me angry, you
wouldn’t like

me when I’m angry: Volitional choices to act or inhibit are modulated by subliminal perception of
emotional

faces. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 17(2): 252-268. DOI: 10.3758/s13415-016-0477-5

[58] Pavone, E.P., Marzi1, C.A., Girelli, M. 2009. Does subliminal visual perception have an error-
monitoring

system?. European Journal of Neuroscience, 30: 1424–1431. DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06908.x

[59] Pegna, A.J., Landis, T., and Khateb, A. 2008. Electrophysiological evidence for early non-conscious

processing of fearful facial expressions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 70: 127–136. DOI:

10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.08.007

[60] Pessiglione, M., Petrovic, P., Daunizeau, J., Palminteri, S., Dolan, R.J., and Frith, C.D. 2008, Subliminal
instrumental conditioning demonstrated in the human brain. Neuron, 59: 561–567. DOI:

10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.005

[61] Pichon, S., Guex, R., Vuilleumier, P. 2016. Influence of temporal expectations on response priming
by

subliminal faces. Plos One,11(10): e0164613. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0164613

[62] Pozharliev, R., Verbeke, W.J., Bagozzi, R.P. 2017. Social consumer neuroscience: Neurophysiological

measures of advertising effectiveness in a social context. Journal of Advertising, 46(3): 351–362. DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.2017.1343162

[63] Pradeep, A. 2011. The Buying brain: Secrets to selling to the unconscious mind. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley
ISBN:

9780470601778, 252 p.

[64] Prochnow, D., Kossack, H., Brunheim, S., Müller, K., Wittsack, Markowitsch, H-J, Seitz, R.J. 2013.
Processing

of subliminal facial expressions of emotion: A behavioral and FMRI study. Social Neuroscience, 8(5):
448–461.

DOI: 10.1080/17470919.2013.812536

[65] Rodilla, C.C., Beauducel, A., Leue, A. 2016. Error-related negativity and the misattribution of state-
anxiety

following errors: On the reproducibility of Inzlicht and Al-Khindi. Human Neuroscience, 10: 475. DOI:

10.3389/fnhum.2016.00475

[66] Ruch, S., Zust, M.A., and Henke, K. 2016. Subliminal messages exert long-term effects on decision-
making.

Neuroscience of Consciousness, 1: 1-9. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1093/nc/niw013. Available at:


https://academic.oup.com/nc/article/2016/1/niw013/2757133

[67] Salgado-Montejo, A., Leon, I.T., Elliot, A.J., Salgado, C.J., and Spence, C. 2015. Smiles over frowns:
When

curved lines influence product preference. Psychology & Marketing, 32(7): 771–781. DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.20817

[68] Scherer, K.R., Shorr, A., Johnstone, T. 2001. Appraisal Processes in Emotion: Theory, Methods,
Research.

Canary, NC: Oxford University Press, ISBN: 9780195351545. 496 p.


[69] Shalgi, S., Deouell, L.Y. 2013. Is there any electrophysiological evidence for subliminal error
processing?

Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7: 150. DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00150

[70] Sidarus, N., Vuorre, M., Haggard, P. 2017. How action selection influences the sense of agency: An
ERP

study. NeuroImage, 150: 1–13. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.02.015

Journal of Applied Economic Sciences

817

[71] Sidorchuk, R. 2017. Study of the relationship between value orientations and consumer preferences
of young

consumers in Russia. Journal of Applied Economic Sciences, XII, Winter, 7(53): 2012-2027.

[72] Simmons, J.P., Nelson, L.D., Simonsohn, U. 2011. False-positive psychology: undisclosed flexibility in
data

collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science, 22(11): 1359–
1366.

DOI: 10.1177/0956797611417632

[73] Smărăndescu, L., Shimp, T. 2015. Drink coca-cola, eat popcorn, and choose powerade: Testing the
limits of

subliminal persuasion. Marketing Letters, 26(4): 715–726. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-014-


9294-1

[74] Smedt, B.D. 2014. Advances in the use of neuroscience methods in research on learning and
instruction.

Frontier Learning Research, 2(4): 7-14. DOI: https://doi.org/10.14786/flr.v2i4.115

[75] Smelser, N.J., Baltes, P.B. 2001. International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences.
Ontario:

Elsevier Science. ISBN: 9780080548050

[76] Sweeny, T.D., Grabowecky, M., Suzuki, S., and Paller, K. A. 2009. Long-lasting effects of subliminal
affective

priming from facial expressions. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(4): 929–938. DOI:

10.1016/j.concog.2009.07.011

[77] Teuchies, M., Demanet, J., Sidarus, N., Haggard, P., Stevens, M.A., Brass, M. 2016. Influences of
unconscious

priming on voluntary actions: Role of the rostral cingulate zone. NeuroImage, 135: 243–252. DOI:
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.036

[78] Theus, K. T. 1994. Subliminal advertising and the psychology of processing unconscious stimuli: A
review of

research. Psychology & Marketing, 11(3): 271–290. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mar.4220110306

[79] Trappey, C. 1996. A meta-analysis of consumer choice and subliminal advertising. Psychology &
Marketing,

13(5): 517–553. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1520-6793(199608)13:5<517::AID-MAR5>3.0.CO;2-


C

[80] Turner, B.O., Paul, E.J., Miller, M.B., Barbey, A.K. 2018. Small sample sizes reduce the replicability of
taskbased fMRI studies. Communications Biology, 1: 62. DOI: 10.1038/s42003-018-0073-z

[81] Uono, S., Satoa, W., Sawada, R., Kochiyama, T.,Toichi, M. 2018. Spatiotemporal commonalities of
frontoparietal activation in attentional orienting triggered by supraliminal and subliminal gaze cues: An
event-related

potential study. Biological Psychology, 136: 29–38. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.05.004

[82] Venkatraman, V., DimokaI, A., Pavlou, P.A., Vo, K., Hampton, W., Bollinger, B., Hershfield, H. E.,
Ishihara, M.,

Winer, R.S. 2015. Predicting advertising success beyond traditional measures: New insights from

neurophysiological methods and market response modeling. Journal of Marketing Research, 53(4): 436–
452.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1509/jmr.13.0593

[83] Wang, L., Feng, C., Mai, X., Jia, L., Zhu, X., Luo, W., and Lou, Y. 2016. The impact of perceptual load
on the

non-conscious processing of fearful faces. Plos One, 11(5): e0154914. DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0154914

[84] Wang, Y., Minor, M. 2008. Validity, reliability and applicability of psychophysiolgical techniques.
Psychology &

Marketing, 25(2): 197–232. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.20206

[85] Wokke, M. E., van Gaal, S., Scholte, H.S., Ridderinkhof, K.R., Lamme, A.F.L. 2011. The flexible nature
of

unconscious cognition. Plos One, 6(9): e25729. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025729

[86] Yokoyama, T., Noguchi, Y., Kita, S. 2013. Unconscious processing of direct gaze: Evidence from an
ERP

study. Neuropsychologia, 51(7): 1161-1168. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.04.002


[87] Zani, A., Biella, G., Proverbio, A.M. 2012. Brain imaging techniques: Invasiveness and spatial and
temporal

Resolution. Available at: https://erplabcnr.files.wordpress.com/2012/02/appendixf_edited-2012.pdf.

[88] Zurawicki, L. 2010. Nueromarketing, Exploring the Brain of the Consumer. London, UK: Springer.
ISBN: 978-

3-540-77829-5, 273p.

-References

1. Barutchu, A.; Spence, C.; Humphreys, G.W. Multisensory enhancement elicited by unconscious visual

stimuli. Exp. Brain Res. 2018, 236, 409–417. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

2. Kawakami, N.; Miura, E.; Nagai, M. When you become a superman: Subliminal exposure to death-
related

stimuli enhances men’s physical force. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 221. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

3. Lourenco, S.F.; Ayzenberg, V.; Lyu, J. A general magnitude system in human adults: Evidence from a

subliminal priming paradigm. Cortex 2016, 81, 93–103. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

4. Dehaene, S.; Cohen, L. The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends Cogn. Sci.
2011, 15,

254–262. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

5. Murphy, S.T.; Zajonc, R.B. Affect, cognition, and awareness: Affective priming with optimal and
suboptimal

stimulus exposures. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1993, 64, 723. [CrossRef]

6. Erdelyi, M.H. Subliminal perception and its cognates: Theory, indeterminacy, and time. Conscious.
Cogn.

2004, 13, 73–91. [CrossRef]

Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 54 16 of 23

7. Greenwald, A.G.; Draine, S.C.; Abrams, R.L. Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic
activation.

Science 1996, 273, 1699–1702. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

8. Saari, T.; Ravaja, N.; Laarni, J.; Turpeinen, M.; Kallinen, K. Psychologically targeted persuasive
advertising

and product information in e-commerce. In Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on


Electronic
Commerce, Delft, The Netherlands, 25–27 October 2004; ACM: Delft, The Netherlands, 2004; pp. 245–
254.

9. Ortells, J.J.; Kiefer, M.; Castillo, A.; Megías, M.; Morillas, A. The semantic origin of unconscious
priming:

Behavioral and event-related potential evidence during category congruency priming from strongly and

weakly related masked words. Cognition 2016, 146, 143–157. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

10. Li, B.; Gao, C.; Wang, W.; Guo, C. Processing fluency hinders subsequent recollection: An
electrophysiological

study. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

11. Ferrand, L.; New, B. Semantic and associative priming in the mental lexicon. In Mental Lexicon: Some
Words

to Talk about Words; Nova Science Publisher: Hauppauge, NY, USA, 2003; pp. 25–43.

12. Bar, M.; Biederman, I. Subliminal visual priming. Psychol. Sci. 1998, 6, 464–469. [CrossRef]

13. Bartram, D. The role of visual and semantic codes in object naming. Cogn. Psychol. 1974, 6, 325–356.

[CrossRef]

14. Biederman, I.; Cooper, E.E. Evidence for complete translational and reflectional invariance in visual
object

priming. Perception 1991, 20, 585–593. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

15. Becker, S.; Moscovitch, M.; Behrmann, M.; Joordens, S. Long-term semantic priming: A
computational

account and empirical evidence. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1997, 23, 1059. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

16. Cave, C.B. Very long-lasting priming in picture naming. Psychol. Sci. 1997, 8, 322–325. [CrossRef]

17. Francken, J.; van Gaal, S.; de Lange, F. Immediate and long-term priming effects are independent of
prime

awareness. Conscious. Cogn. 2011, 20, 1793–1800. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

18. Yoshimoto, S.; Imai, H.; Kashino, M.; Takeuchi, T. Pupil response and the subliminal mere exposure
effect.

PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e90670. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

19. Kwan, L.Y.Y.; Yap, S.; Chiu, C.-Y. Mere exposure affects perceived descriptive norms: Implications for

personal preferences and trust. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2015, 129, 48–58. [CrossRef]

20. Stafford, T.; Grimes, A. Memory enhances the mere exposure effect. Psychol. Mark. 2012, 29, 995–
1003.
[CrossRef]

21. Kopp, B.; Mattler, U.; Goertz, R.; Rist, F. N2, p3 and the lateralized readiness potential in a nogo task

involving selective response priming. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1996, 99, 19–27.
[CrossRef]

22. Klotz, W.; Wolff, P. The effect of a masked stimulus on the response to the masking stimulus.
Psychol. Res.

1995, 58, 92–101. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

23. Klotz, W.; Neumann, O. Motor activation without conscious discrimination in metacontrast masking.
J. Exp.

Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1999, 25, 976–992. [CrossRef]

24. Vorberg, D.; Mattler, U.; Heinecke, A.; Schmidt, T.; Schwarzbach, J. Different time courses for visual

perception and action priming. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2003, 100, 6275–6280. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

25. Schmidt, T.; Vorberg, D. Criteria for unconscious cognition: Three types of dissociation. Percept.
Psychophys.

2006, 68, 489–504. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

26. De Groot, A.M. Primed lexical decision: Combined effects of the proportion of related prime-target
pairs

and the stimulus-onset asynchrony of prime and target. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1984, 36, 253–280. [CrossRef]

27. Beiderman, I.; Cooper, E.E. Size invariance in visual object priming. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept.
Perform.

1992, 18, 121–133. [CrossRef]

28. Jolicoeur, P. A size-congruency effect in memory for visual shape. Mem. Cogn. 1987, 15, 531–543.
[CrossRef]

29. Besner, D. Visual pattern recognition: Size preprocessing reexamined. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1983, 35A,
209–216.

[CrossRef]

30. Bundesen, C.; Larsen, A. Visual transformation of size. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1975,
1,

214–220. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

31. Ellis, R.; Allport, D.A.; Humphreys, G.W.; Collis, J. Varieties of object constancy. Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
1989, 41A,

775–796. [CrossRef]
32. Jolicoeur, P.; Besner, D. Additivity and interaction between size ratio and response category in the
comparison

of size-discrepant shapes. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1987, 13, 478–487. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]

Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 54 17 of 23

33. Larsen, A. Pattern matching: Effects of size ratio, angular difference in orientation, and familiarity.

Percept. Psychophys. 1985, 38, 63–68. [CrossRef]

34. Larsen, A.; Bundesen, C. Size scaling in human pattern recognition. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept.
Perform.

1978, 4, 1–20. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

35. Kosslyn, S.M. Aspects of a cognitive neuroscience of mental imagery. Science 1987, 240, 1621–1626.
[CrossRef]

36. Ullman, S. Aligning pictorial descriptions: An approach to object recognition. Cognition 1989, 32,
193–254.

[CrossRef]

37. Vaidya, C.J.; Gabrieli, J.D.; Monti, L.A.; Tinklenberg, J.R.; Yesavage, J.A. Dissociation between two
forms of

conceptual priming in alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 1999, 13, 516. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

38. Reisberg, D. Cognition: Exploring the Science of the Mind; WW Norton: New York, NY, USA, 2007.

39. Mayr, S.B.; Axel, B. Negative priming as a memory phenomenon: A review of 20 years of negative
priming

research. J. Psychol. 2007, 215, 35–51. [CrossRef]

40. Bentin, S.; McCarthy, G.; Wood, C.C. Event-related potentials, lexical decision, and semantic priming.

Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1985, 60, 343–355. [CrossRef]

41. Tipper, S.; Driver, J. Negative priming between pictures and words in a selective attention task:
Evidence for

semantic processing of ignored stimuli. Mem. Cogn. 1988, 16, 64–70. [CrossRef]

42. Neill, W.T.V.; Leslie, A.; Terry, K.M.; Gorfein, D.S. Persistence of negative priming: Ii. Evidence for
episodic

trace retrieval. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1992, 18, 993–1000. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

43. Stanovich, K.E.W.; Richard, F. On priming by a sentence context. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1983, 112, 1–
36.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]

44. Berger, J.; Meredith, M.; Wheeler, S.C. Contextual priming: Where people vote affects how they
vote. Proc.

Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2008, 105, 8846–8849. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

45. Cook, S.; Fallon, N.; Wright, H.; Thomas, A.; Giesbrecht, T.; Field, M.; Stancak, A. Pleasant and
unpleasant

odors influence hedonic evaluations of human faces: An event-related potential study. Front. Hum.
Neurosci.

2015, 9, 661. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

46. Cook, S.; Kokmotou, K.; Soto, V.; Wright, H.; Fallon, N.; Thomas, A.; Giesbrecht, T.; Field, M.; Stancak,
A.

Simultaneous odour-face presentation strengthens hedonic evaluations and event-related potential


responses

influenced by unpleasant odour. Neurosci. Lett. 2018, 672, 22–27. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

47. Williams, M.A.; Morris, A.P.; McGlone, F.; Abbott, D.F.; Mattingley, J.B. Amygdala responses to
fearful

and happy facial expressions under conditions of binocular suppression. J. Neurosci. 2004, 24, 2898–
2904.

[CrossRef] [PubMed]

48. Fazio, R.H.; Olson, M.A. Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use.

Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2003, 54, 297–327. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

49. Milders, M.; Sahraie, A.; Logan, S. Minimum presentation time for masked facial expression
discrimination.

Cogn. Emot. 2007, 22, 63–82. [CrossRef]

50. Pessoa, L.; Japee, S.; Ungerleider, L.G. Visual awareness and the detection of fearful faces. Emotion
2005, 5,

243–247. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

51. Potter, M.C.; Wyble, B.; Hagmann, C.E.; McCourt, E.S. Detecting meaning in rsvp at 13 ms per
picture.

Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2014, 76, 270–279. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

52. Karremans, J.; Stroebe, W.; Claus, J. Beyond vicary’s fantasies: The impact of subliminal priming and
brand

choice. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2006, 42, 792–798. [CrossRef]


53. Mohan, D.M.; Kumar, P.; Mahmood, F.; Wong, K.F.; Agrawal, A.; Elgendi, M.; Shukla, R.; Ang, N.;
Ching, A.;

Dauwels, J.; et al. Effect of subliminal lexical priming on the subjective perception of images: A machine

learning approach. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0148332. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

54. Kumar, P.; Mahmood, F.; Mohan, D.M.; Wong, K.; Agrawal, A.; Elgendi, M.; Shukla, R.; Dauwels, J.;

Chan, A.H. On the Effect of Subliminal Priming on Subjective Perception of Images: A Machine Learning

Approach. In Proceedings of the 2014 36th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in

Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), Chicago, IN, USA, 26–30 August 2014; pp. 5438–5441.

55. Lang, S.F.; Nelson, C.A.; Collins, P.F. Event-related potentials to emotional and neutral stimuli.

J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 1990, 12, 946–958. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 54 18 of 23

56. Matsukawa, J.; Snodgrass, J.G.; Doniger, G.M. Conceptual versus perceptual priming in incomplete
picture

identification. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 2005, 34, 515–540. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

57. Battig, W.F.M.; William, E. Category norms of verbal items in 56 categories a replication and
extension of the

connecticut category norms. J. Exp. Psychol. 1969, 80, 1–46. [CrossRef]

58. Snodgrass, J.G.; Vanderwart, M. A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement,
image

agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. Mem. 1980, 6, 174–215.
[CrossRef]

59. Shelton, J.R.; Martin, R.C. How semantic is automatic semantic priming? J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem.
Cogn.

1992, 18, 1191. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

60. Perea, M.; Rosa, E. The effects of associative and semantic priming in the lexical decision task.
Psychol. Res.

2002, 66, 180–194. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

61. Neely, J.H. Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings
and

theories. In Basic Processes in Reading; Routledge: London, UK, 2012; pp. 272–344.

62. Ansorge, U.; Klotz, W. Manual and verbal responses to completely masked (unreportable) stimuli:
Exploring
some conditions for the metacontrast dissociation. Perception 1998, 27, 1177–1189. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]

63. Ansorge, U.; Neumann, O.; Becker, S.I.; Kälberer, H.; Kruse, H. Sensorimotor supremacy: Investigating

conscious and unconscious vision by masked priming. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 2007, 3, 257–274. [CrossRef]

[PubMed]

64. Schmidt, T. The finger in flight: Real-time motor control by visually masked color stimuli. Psychol. Sci.
2002,

13, 112–118. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

65. Schmidt, T.; Niehaus, S.; Nagel, A. Primes and targets in rapid chases: Tracing sequential waves of
motor

activation. Behav. Neurosci. 2006, 120, 1005–1016. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

66. Schmidt, T.; Schmidt, F. Processing of natural images is feedforward: A simple behavioral test.

Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2009, 71, 594–606. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

67. Mattler, U. Flanker effects on motor output and the late-level response activation hypothesis. Q. J.

Exp. Psychol. 2005, 58A, 577–601. [CrossRef]

68. Leuthold, H.; Kopp, B. Mechanisms of priming by masked stimuli: Inferences from event-related
brain

potentials. Psychol. Sci. 1998, 9, 263–269. [CrossRef]

69. Eimer, M.; Schlaghecken, F. Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: Behavioral and

electrophysiological evidence. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1998, 24, 1737–1745. [CrossRef]

[PubMed]

70. Eimer, M.; Schlaghecken, F. Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming. Biol. Psychol.
2003,

64, 7–26. [CrossRef]

71. Mattler, U. Delayed flanker effects on lateralized readiness potentials. Exp. Brain Res. 2003, 151,
272–288.

[CrossRef] [PubMed]

72. Vath, N.; Schmidt, T. Tracing sequential waves of rapid visuomotor activation in lateralized readiness

potentials. Neuroscience 2007, 145, 197–208. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

73. Postman, L.; Keppel, G. Norms of Word Association; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 2014.

74. Voss, J.L.; Schendan, H.E.; Paller, K.A. Finding meaning in novel geometric shapes influences
electrophysiological correlates of repetition and dissociates perceptual and conceptual priming.
NeuroImage

2010, 49, 2879–2889. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

75. Brown, C.M.; Hagoort, P. The Cognitive Neuroscience of Language; Brown, C.M., Hagoort, P., Eds.;

The Neurocognition of Language; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1999; Volume 6.

76. Castle, P.C.; Van Toller, S.; Milligan, G.J. The effect of odour priming on cortical eeg and visual erp
responses.

Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2000, 36, 123–131. [CrossRef]

77. Watson, N.V.; Breedlove, S.M. The Mind’s Machine: Foundations of Brain and Behavior; Sinauer
Associates:

Sunderland, MA, USA, 2012.

78. Gibbons, H. Evaluative priming from subliminal emotional words: Insights from event-related
potentials

and individual differences related to anxiety. Conscious. Cogn. 2009, 18, 383–400. [CrossRef] [PubMed]

79. Bruner, J.S.; Postman, L. Emotional selectivity in perception and reaction. J. Personal. 1947, 16, 69–
77.

[CrossRef]

-References
 Abrams, R. L., & Greenwald, A. G. (2000). Parts outweigh the whole (word) in unconscious analysis of
meaning. Psychological Science, 11, 118–124.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Anderson, A. K. (2005). Affective influences on the attentional dynamics supporting
awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 258–281.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Anderson, A. K., Christoff, K., Panitz, D. A., De Rosa, E., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2003). Neural correlates
of the automatic processing of threat facial signals. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 5627–5633.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Anderson, A. K., & Phelps, E. A. (2001). Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception
of emotionally salient events. Nature, 411, 305–309.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Aquino, J. M., & Arnell, K. M. (2007). Attention and the processing of emotional words: Dissociating
effects of arousal. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 430–435.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Arnell, K. M., Killman, K. V., & Fijavz, D. (2007). Blinded by emotion: Target misses follow attention
capture by arousing distractors in RSVP. Emotion, 7, 465–477.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Gulikers, L. (1995). The Celex Lexical Database[CD-ROM].
Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia.
o View In Article
 Bahrami, B., Carmel, D., Walsh, V., Rees, G., & Lavie, N. (2008a). Unconscious orientation processing
depends on perceptual load. Journal of Vision, 8, 1–10.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Bahrami, B., Carmel, D., Walsh, V., Rees, G., & Lavie, N. (2008b). Spatial attention can modulate
unconscious orientation processing. Perception, 37, 1520–1528.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Bahrami, B., Lavie, N., & Rees, G. (2007). Attentional load modulates responses of human primary
visual cortex to invisible stimuli. Current Biology, 17, 509–513.
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Bernat, E., Bunce, S., & Shevrin, H. (2001). Event-related brain potentials differentiate positive and
negative mood adjectives during both supraliminal and subliminal visual processing. International Journal of
Psychophysiology, 42, 11–34.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang. P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation I:
Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion, 1, 276–298.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Carrasco, M., & McElree, B. (2001). Covert attention accelerates the rate of visual information
processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 98, 5363–5367.
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Dijksterhuis, A., & Aarts, H. (2003). On wildebeests and humans: The preferential detection of
negative stimuli. Psychological Science, 14, 14–18.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Dijksterhuis, A., Corneille, O., Aarts, H., Vermeulen, N., & Luminet, O. (2004). Yes, there is a
preferential detection of negative stimuli: A response to Labiouse. Psychological Science, 15, 571–572.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Etkin, A. Klemenhagen, K., Dudman, J., Rogan, M., Hen, R., Kandel, E. R., et al. (2004). Individual
differences in trait anxiety predict the response of the basolateral amygdala to unconsciously processed fearful
faces. Neuron, 44, 1043–1055.
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Gaillard, R., Del Cul, A., Naccache, L., Vinckier, F., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2006). Nonconscious
semantic processing of emotional words modulates conscious access. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences, USA, 103, 7524–7529.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Hamann, S. B., Ely, T. D., Grafton, S. T., & Kilts, C. D. (1999). Amygdala activity related to enhanced
memory for pleasant and aversive stimuli. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 289–294.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Huang, Y., Baddeley, A., & Young, A. W. (2008). Attentional capture by emotional stimuli is modulated
by semantic processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 328–
339.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Jackson, M. C., Wolf, C., Johnston, S. J., Raymond, J. E., & Linden, D. E. J. (2008). Neural correlates
of enhanced visual short-term memory for angry faces: An fMRI study. Public Library of Science One, 3, 1–10.
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Jackson, M. C., Wu, C–Y., Linden, D. E. J., & Raymond, J. E. (2009). Enhanced visual short-term
memory for angry faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 363–
374.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Keil, A., & Ihssen, N. (2004). Identification facilitation for emotionally arousing verbs during the
attentional blink. Emotion, 4, 23–35.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Kihara, K., & Osaka, N. (2008). Early mechanism of negativity bias: An attentional blink
study. Japanese Psychological Research, 50, 1–11.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Labiouse, C. L. (2004). Commentary: Is there a real preferential detection of negative stimuli?
Psychological Science, 15, 364–365.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Lang, P. J. (1980). Behavioral treatment and bio-behavioral assessment: Computer applications. In J.
B. Sidowski, J. H. Johnson, & T. A. Williams (Eds.), Technology in mental health care delivery systems (pp.
119–137). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 451–468.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 9, 75–82.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Lavie, N., & Cox, S. (1997). On the efficiency of attentional selection: Efficient visual search results in
inefficient rejection of distraction. Psychological Science, 8, 395–398.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Lavie, N., & Fox, E. (2000). The role of perceptual load in negative priming. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1038–1052.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar
 Lavie, N., Hirst, A., De Fockert, J. W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and
cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 339–354.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Lavie, N., & Tsal, Y. (1994). Perceptual load as a major determinant of the locus of selection in visual
attention. Perception and Psychophysics, 56, 183–197.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 McGinnies, E. M. (1949). Emotionality and perceptual defense. Psychological Review, 56, 471–482.
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 McKenna, F. P., & Sharma, D. (1995). Intrusive cognitions: An investigation of the emotional Stroop
task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 1595–1607.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Milders, M., Sahraie, A., Logan, S., & Donnellon, N. (2006). Awareness of faces is modulated by their
emotional meaning. Emotion, 6, 10–17.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Morris, J. S., Ohman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (1998). Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the
human amygdala. Nature, 393, 467–470.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and awareness: Affective priming with optimal
and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 723–739.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Naccache, L., Gaillard, R., Adam, C., Hasboun, D., Clémenceau, S., Daulac, M., et al. (2005). A direct
intracranial record of emotions evoked by subliminal words. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
USA, 102, 7713–7717.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Ogawa, T., & Suzuki, N. (2004). On the saliency of negative stimuli: Evidence from attentional
blink. Japanese Psychological Research, 46, 20–30.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Ohman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the
grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466–478.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar
 Pessoa, L. (2005). To what extent are emotional stimuli processed without attention and awareness?
Current Opinion of Neurobiology, 15, 188–196.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Pessoa, L., Japee, S., Sturman, D., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2006). Target visibility and visual awareness
modulate amygdala responses to fearful faces. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 366–375.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Pessoa, L., McKenna, M., Gutierrez, E., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). Neural processing of emotional
faces requires attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 99, 11458–11463.
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Pleyers, G., Corneille, O., Luminet, O., & Yzerbyt, V. (2007). Aware and (dis)liking: Item-based
analyses reveal that valence acquisition via evaluative conditioning emerges only when there is contingency
awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 130–144.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social
information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 380–391.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., & Arnell, K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in
an RSVP task: An attentional blink?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Performance, 18, 849–860.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Shapiro, K. L., Raymond, J. E., & Arnell, K. M. (1997). The attentional Blink. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 1, 291–296.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Silvert, L., Delplanque, S., Bouwalerh, H., Verpoort, C., & Sequeira, H. (2004). Autonomic responding
to aversive words without conscious valence discrimination. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 53,
135–145.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Snodgrass, L. L., & Harring, K. E. (2005). Right hemisphere positivity bias in preconscious processing:
Data from five experiments. Current Psychology: Developmental, Learning, Personality, Social, 23, 318–335.
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Thomas, S. J., Johnstone, S. J., & Gonsalvez, C. J. (2007). Event-related potentials during an
emotional Stroop task. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 63, 221–231.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Toglia, M. P., & Battig, W. F. (1978). Handbook of semantic word norms. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Clarke, K., Husain, M., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Neural
responses to emotional faces with and without awareness: Event-related fMRI in a parietal patient with visual
extinction and spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia, 40, 2156–2166.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Effects of attention and emotion on
face processing in the human brain: An event-related fMRI study. Neuron, 30, 829–841.
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Whalen, P. J., Rauch, S. L., Etcoff, N. L., McInerney, S. C., Lee, M. B., et al. (1998). Masked
presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. Journal of
Neuroscience, 18, 411–418.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article
 Zeelenberg, R., Wagenmakers, E. J., & Rotteveel, M. (2006). The impact of emotion on perception:
Bias or enhanced processing?Psychological Science, 17, 287–291.
o PsycINFO 
o Google Scholar 
o View In Article

This research was supported by a Wellcome Trust award to N. Lavie.M. Nasrallah and D. Carmel
contributed equally to this work.Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed
toDavid Carmel, Department of Psychology and Center for Neural Science, New York University, 6
Washington Place, New York, NY 10003

Email: davecarmel@nyu.edu

Received April 8, 2008

Revision received March 26, 2009

Accepted April 21, 2009

References

Aarts H, Custers R, Marien H. Preparing and motivating behavior

outside of awareness. Science 2008;319:1639. http://doi.org/10.

1126/science.1150432.

Abadie M, Waroquier L, Terrier P. Gist memory in the

unconscious-thought effect. Psychol Sci 2013;24:1253–9. http://

doi.org/10.1177/0956797612470958.
Addante RJ. A critical role of the human hippocampus in an electrophysiological measure of implicit
memory. NeuroImage

2015;109:515–28. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.069.

Arzi A, Holtzman Y, Samnon P, et al. Olfactory aversive conditioning during sleep reduces cigarette-
smoking behavior. J

Neurosci 2014;34:15382–93. http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.

2291-14.2014.

Arzi A, Shedlesky L, Ben-Shaul M, et al. Humans can learn new

information during sleep. Nat Neurosci 2012;15:1460–5. http://

doi.org/10.1038/nn.3193

Atas A, Faivre N, Timmermans B, et al. Nonconscious learning

from crowded sequences. Psychol Sci 2013. http://doi.org/10.

1177/0956797613499591.

Bergstro¨m F, Eriksson J. The conjunction of non-consciously perceived object identity and spatial
position can be retained during a visual short-term memory task. Conscious Res 2015;1470.

http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01470

Bermeitinger C, Goelz R, Johr N, et al. The hidden persuaders

break into the tired brain. J Exp Soc Psychol 2009;45:320–6.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.10.001

Bettman JR, Luce MF, Payne JW. Constructive consumer choice processes. J Cons Res 1998;25:187–217.
http://doi.org/10.1086/209535

Capa RL, Cleeremans A, Bustin GM, et al. Long-lasting effect of

subliminal processes on cardiovascular responses and performance. Int J Psychophysiol 2011;81:22–30.


http://doi.org/10.1016/

j.ijpsycho.2011.04.001

Chen JCW, Li W, Lui M, et al. Left-frontal brain potentials index

conceptual implicit memory for words initially viewed subliminally. Brain Res 2009;1285:135–47.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.

brainres.2009.05.085

Chong TTJ, Husain M, Rosenthal CR. Recognizing the unconscious. Curr Biol 2014;24:R1033–5.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.
2014.09.035

Chun MM, Phelps EA. Memory deficits for implicit contextual information in amnesic subjects with
hippocampal damage. Nat

Neurosci 1999;2:844–7. http://doi.org/10.1038/12222

Daltrozzo J, Claude L, Tillmann B, et al. Working memory is partially preserved during sleep. PLoS ONE
2012;7:e50997. http://

doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0050997

Dew ITZ, Cabeza R. The porous boundaries between explicit and

implicit memory: behavioral and neural evidence. Ann NY

Acad Sci 2011;1224:174–90. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.

2010.05946.x

Dienes Z. Understanding Psychology as a Science: An Introduction to

Scientific and Statistical Inference. New York: Palgrave Macmillan,

2008.

Dienes Z. Using Bayes to get the most out of non-significant results. Quant Psychol Meas 2014;5:781.
http://doi.org/10.3389/

fpsyg.2014.00781

Dijksterhuis A, Bos MW, Nordgren LF, et al. On making the right

choice: the deliberation-without-attention effect. Science

2006;311:1005–7. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1121629.

Dijksterhuis A, Nordgren LF. A theory of unconscious thought.

Perspect Psychol Sci 2006;1:95–109. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-

6916.2006.00007.x.

Duss SB, Oggier S, Reber TP, et al. Formation of semantic associations between subliminally presented
face-word pairs.

Conscious Cogn 2011;20:928–35. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.

2011.03.018.

Duss SB, Reber TP, Hanggi J, € et al. Unconscious relational encoding depends on hippocampus. Brain
2014;137:3355–70. http://

doi.org/10.1093/brain/awu270.
Farooqui AA, Manly T. Anticipatory control through associative

learning of subliminal relations: invisible may be better than

visible. Psychol Sci 2015;26:325–34. http://doi.org/10.1177/

0956797614564191.

Ferrand L. The masked repetition priming effect dissipates when

increasing the inter-stimulus interval: evidence from word

naming. Acta Psychologica 1996;91:15–25. http://doi.org/10.1016/

0001-6918(95)00010-0.

Forster K, Booker J, Schacter DL, et al. Masked repetition priming:

lexical activation or novel memory trace? Bull Psychon Soc

1990;28:341–5. http://doi.org/10.3758/BF03334039.

Gaillard R, Cohen L, Adam C, et al. Subliminal words durably affect neuronal activity. NeuroReport
2007;18:1527–31. http://doi.

org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f0b6cd.

Garcıa-Orza J, Damas-Lopez J, Matas A, et al. “2 x 3” primes naming “6”: evidence from masked priming.
Atten Percept

Psychophys 2009;71:471–80. http://doi.org/10.3758/APP.71.3.471

Greene AJ, Gross WL, Elsinger CL, et al. An fMRI analysis of the

human hippocampus: inference, context, and task awareness.

J Cogn Neurosci 2006;18:1156–73. http://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.

2006.18.7.1156

Greene AJ, Gross WL, Elsinger CL, et al. Hippocampal differentiation without recognition: an fMRI
analysis of the contextual

cueing task. Learn Mem 2007;14:548–53. http://doi.org/10.1101/

lm.609807.

Greenwald AG, Draine SC, Abrams RL. Three cognitive markers

of unconscious semantic activation. Science 1996;273:1699–702.

http://doi.org/10.1126/science.273.5282.1699

Greenwald AG, Klinger MR, Schuh ES. Activation by marginally

perceptible (“subliminal”) stimuli: dissociation of unconscious


from conscious cognition. J Exp Psychol-Gen 1995;124:22–42.

Hannula DE, Greene AJ. The hippocampus reevaluated in unconscious learning and memory: at a tipping
point? Front Hum

Neurosci 2012;6. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00080.

Hannula DE, Ranganath C. The eyes have it: hippocampal activity predicts expression of memory in eye
movements. Neuron

2009;63:592–9. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.025.

Harand C, Bertran F, La Joie R, et al. The hippocampus remains

activated over the long term for the retrieval of truly episodic

memories. PLoS ONE 2012;7:e43495. http://doi.org/10.1371/jour

nal.pone.0043495.

Hassin RR. Yes it can: on the functional abilities of the human

unconscious. Perspect Psychol Sci 2013;8:195–207. http://doi.org/

10.1177/1745691612460684.

Hassin RR, Ferguson MJ, Shidlovski D, et al. Subliminal exposure

to national flags affects political thought and behavior. Proc

Natl Acad Sci USA 2007;104:19757–61. http://doi.org/10.1073/

pnas.0704679104.

Henke K. A model for memory systems based on processing

modes rather than consciousness. Nat Rev Neurosci

2010;11:523–32. http://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2850.

Subliminal messages—effects on decision-making | 7

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/nc/article/2016/1/niw013/2757133 by guest on 02


November 2020

Henke K, Buck A, Weber B, et al. Human hippocampus establishes associations in memory. Hippocampus
1997;7:249–56.

http://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1997)7:3<249::AIDHIPO1>3.0.CO;2-G.

Henke K, Mondadori CRA, Treyer V, et al. Nonconscious formation and reactivation of semantic
associations by way of the

medial temporal lobe. Neuropsychologia 2003;41:863–76. http://


doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00035-6.

Henke K, Reber TP, Duss SB. Integrating events across levels of

consciousness. Front Behav Neurosci 2013;7:68. http://doi.org/10.

3389/fnbeh.2013.00068.

Hesselmann G, Moors P. Definitely maybe: can unconscious

processes perform the same functions as conscious processes? Conscious Res 2015;584.
http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.

2015.00584.

Holdstock JS, Mayes AR, Isaac CL, et al. Differential involvement

of the hippocampus and temporal lobe cortices in rapid and

slow learning of new semantic information. Neuropsychologia

2002;40:748–68. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00192-0.

Karpinski A, Yale M, Briggs JC. Unconscious arithmetic processing: a direct replication. Eur J Soc Psychol
2016; n/a-n/a. http://

doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2175.

Karremans JC, Stroebe W, Claus J. Beyond Vicary’s fantasies:

the impact of subliminal priming and brand choice. J Exp

Soc Psychol 2006;42:792–8. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.

12.002.

Kawakami N, Yoshida F. Perceiving a story outside of conscious

awareness: when we infer narrative attributes from subliminal

sequential stimuli. Conscious Cogn 2015;33:53–66. http://doi.

org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.12.001.

Kiesel A, Wagener A, Kunde W, et al. Unconscious manipulation

of free choice in humans. Conscious Cogn 2006;15:397–408.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.10.002.

Klapp ST, Haas BW. Nonconscious influence of masked stimuli

on response selection is limited to concrete stimulus-response

associations. J Exp Psychol-Hum Percept Perform 2005;31:193–209.


http://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.31.1.193.

Lau HC, Passingham RE. Unconscious activation of the cognitive

control system in the human prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci

2007;27:5805–11. http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4335-06.

2007.

Levy BR, Pilver C, Chung PH, et al. Subliminal strengthening:

improving older individuals’ physical function over time

with an implicit-age-stereotype intervention. Psychol Sci

2014;2525:2127–35. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614551970.

Lowery BS, Eisenberger NI, Hardin CD, et al. Long-term effects of

subliminal priming on academic performance. BASP

2007;29:151–7. http://doi.org/10.1080/01973530701331718.

McClelland JL, McNaughton BL, O’reilly RC. Why there are complementary learning systems in the
hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist

models of learning and memory. Psychol Rev 1995;102:419–57.

Mealor AD, Dienes Z. Conscious and unconscious thought in artificial grammar learning. Conscious Cogn
2012;21:865–74.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2012.03.001.

Miller J. Measurement error in subliminal perception experiments: simulation analyses of two regression
methods. J Exp

Psychol-Hum Percept Perform 2000;26:1461–77.

Monahan JL, Murphy ST, Zajonc RB. Subliminal mere exposure:

specific, general, and diffuse effects. Psychol Sci 2000;11:462–6.

http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00289.

Moscovitch M. Recovered consciousness: a hypothesis concerning modularity and episodic memory. J


Clin Exp Neuropsychol

1995;17:276–90. http://doi.org/10.1080/01688639508405123.

Mudrik L, Breska A, Lamy D, et al. Integration without awareness:

expanding the limits of unconscious processing. Psychol Sci

2011;22:764–70. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611408736.
Mudrik L, Faivre N, Koch C. Information integration without

awareness. Trends Cogn Sci 2014;18:488–6. http://doi.org/10.

1016/j.tics.2014.04.009.

Mudrik L, Koch C. Differential processing of invisible congruent

and incongruent scenes: a case for unconscious integration. J

Vis 2013;13:24. http://doi.org/10.1167/13.13.24.

Negash S, Kliot D, Howard DV, et al. Relationship of contextual

cueing and hippocampal volume in amnestic mild cognitive

impairment patients and cognitively normal older adults.

J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2015;21:285–96. http://doi.org/10.1017/

S1355617715000223.

Newell BR, Shanks DR. Unconscious influences on decision making: a critical review. Behav Brain Sci
2014;1–19. http://doi.org/

10.1017/S0140525X12003214.

Nieuwenstein MR, Wierenga T, Morey RD, et al. On making the

right choice: a meta-analysis and large-scale replication attempt of the unconscious thought advantage.
Judgm Decis Mak

2015;10:1–17.

Ocampo B. Unconscious manipulation of free choice by novel

primes. Conscious Cogn 2015;34:4–9. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.con

cog.2015.03.007.

Olsen RK, Moses SN, Riggs L, et al. The hippocampus supports

multiple cognitive processes through relational binding and

comparison. Front Hum Neurosci 2012;6:146. http://doi.org/10.

3389/fnhum.2012.00146.

Parkinson J, Haggard P. Subliminal priming of intentional inhibition. Cognition 2014;130:255–65.


http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogni

tion.2013.11.005.

Reber TP, DoLam A, Elger CE, et al. Intracranial EEG correlates of

implicit relational inference within the hippocampus.


Hippocampus 2016;26:54–66. http://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.22490.

Reber TP, Henke K. Integrating unseen events over time.

Conscious Cogn 2012;21:953–60. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.

2012.02.013

Reber TP, Luechinger R, Boesiger P, et al. Unconscious relational

inference recruits the hippocampus. J Neurosci

2012;32:6138–48. http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5639-11.

2012.

Reder LM, Park H, Kieffaber PD. Memory systems do not divide

on consciousness: Reinterpreting memory in terms of activation and binding. Psychol Bull 2009;135:23–
49. http://doi.org/10.

1037/a0013974.

Reuss H, Kiesel A, Kunde W, et al. Unconscious activation of task

sets. Conscious Cogn 2011;20:556–67. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.

concog.2011.02.014.

Ruby P, Caclin A, Boulet S, et al. Odd sound processing in the

sleeping brain. J Cognitive Neurosci 2008;20:296–311. http://doi.

org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20023.

Ruch S, Koenig T, Mathis J, et al. Word encoding during sleep is

suggested by correlations between word-evoked up-states and

post-sleep semantic priming. Front Psychol 2014;5:1319. http://

doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01319.

Schlaghecken F, Eimer M. Masked prime stimuli can bias “free”

choices between response alternatives. Psychon Bull Rev

2004;11:463–8. http://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196596.

Shanks DR. Learning: from association to cognition. Annu Rev

Psychol 2010;61:273–301. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.

093008.100519.

Simonson I. In defense of consciousness: the role of conscious


and unconscious inputs in consumer choice. J Cons Psychol

2005;15:211–17. http://doi.org/10.1207/s15327663jcp1503_5.

8 | Ruch et al.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/nc/article/2016/1/niw013/2757133 by guest on 02


November 2020

Sklar AY, Levy N, Goldstein A, et al. Reading and doing arithmetic

nonconsciously. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012. http://doi.org/10.

1073/pnas.1211645109.

Squire LR, Zola SM. Structure and function of declarative and

nondeclarative memory systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

1996;93:13515–22.

Strahan EJ, Spencer SJ, Zanna MP. Subliminal priming and persuasion: striking while the iron is hot. J Exp
Soc Psychol

2002;38:556–68. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00502-4

Tononi G. An information integration theory of consciousness.

BMC Neurosci 2004;5:42. http://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-5-42.

Tulving E. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev

Psychol 2002;53:1–25. http://doi.org/Article

Vadillo MA, Kostopoulou O, Shanks DR. A critical review and

meta-analysis of the unconscious thought effect in medical

decision making. Conscious Res 2015;636. http://doi.org/10.3389/

fpsyg.2015.00636.

Van den Bussche E, Van den Noortgate W, Reynvoet B.

Mechanisms of masked priming: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull

2009;135:452–77. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0015329.

van Gaal S, De Lange FP, Cohen MX. The role of consciousness in

cognitive control and decision making. Front Hum Neurosci

2012;6:121. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00121.

Van Opstal F, de Lange FP, Dehaene S. Rapid parallel semantic


processing of numbers without awareness. Cognition

2011;120:136–47. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.03.005.

Verwijmeren T, Karremans JC, Bernritter SF, et al. Warning: You

are being primed! The effect of a warning on the impact of subliminal ads. J Exp Soc Psychol
2013;49:1124–9. http://doi.org/10.

1016/j.jesp.2013.06.010.

Verwijmeren T, Karremans JC, Stroebe W, et al. The workings

and limits of subliminal advertising: the role of habits. J Cons

Psychol 2011;21:206–13. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2010.11.

004.

Vlassova A, Donkin C, Pearson J. Unconscious information

changes decision accuracy but not confidence. Proc Natl Acad

Sci USA 2014;111:16214–8. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.

1403619111.

Weinberger J, Westen D. RATS, we should have used Clinton:

subliminal priming in political campaigns. Polit Psychol

2008;29:631–51. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.

00658.x.

Zu¨ st MA, Colella P, Reber TP, et al. Hippocampus is place of interaction between unconscious and
conscious memories. PLoS

ONE 2015;10:e0122459. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.

0122459.s

References

1. Baars, B. (1997) In the Theater of Consciousness: The Workspace of the Mind. New York:

Oxford University Press, 2001 reprint.

2. Buckley, K. W. (1982) The selling of a psychologist: John Broadus Watson and the

application of behavioral techniques to advertising. Journal of the History of the

Behavioral Sciences, 18, pp. 207-221.

3. Byrne, D. (1959). The effect of a subliminal food stimulus on verbal responses. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 43 (no.4), pp. 249-251.

4. Bermeitinger, C. R., Goelz, N., Johr, M., Neumann, U.K.H., Ecker, R. Doerr (2009) The

hidden persuaders break into the tired brain. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,

45, pp. 320-326.

5. Champion, J.M., W.W. Turner (1959) An experimental investigation of subliminal

perception. Journal of Applied Psychology, 43, no. 6, pp. 382-384.

6. Chessman, J., Merikle, P. M. (1984) Priming with and without awareness. Perception and

Psychophysics 36 (4), pp. 387–395.

Željko Mladenović et al.

The use of subliminal messages in marketing: experimental examination

Page52

7. Cooper, J., G. Cooper (2002) Subliminal motivation: a story revisited. Journal of Applied

Social Psychology, 32, no. 11, pp. 2213-2227.

8. Dunlap, K. (1900) The effect of imperceptible shadows on the judgment of distance.

Psychological Review, 7, pp. 435-453.

9. Eskenazi, J., & Greenwald, A.G., Pratkanis, A.R. (1990). What you expect is what you

believe (but not necessarily what you get): On the ineffectiveness of subliminal self-help

audiotapes. Unpublished manuscript. University of California. Santa Cruz.

10. Freud, S. (1913). The Interpretation of Dreams, Third Edition. Trans. by A. A. Brill. New

York: The Macmillan Company, Bartleby.com, 2010.

11. Freud, S. (1915). The unconscious. SE.

12. Freud, S. (1923). The ego and the id. SE.

13. Hollingworth, H. L. (1913). Advertising and Selling. New York: D. Appleton.

14. Karremans, J. C., Stroebe, W., & Claus, J. (2006) Beyond Vicary's fantasies: The impact

of subliminal priming and brand choice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42,

792−798.

15. McConnell, J.V., R.L. Cutler, E.B. McNeil (1958) Subliminal stimulation: an overview.

American Psychologist, 13, no. 5, pp. 229-242.

16. Merikle, P. M. (2000) Subliminal Perception. In Encyclopedia of Psychology (Vol. 7).


New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 497-499.

17. Merikle, P. M., Smilek, D., and Eastwood, J. D. (2001) Perception without awareness:

perspectives from cognitive psychology. Cognition 79, pp. 115–134.

18. Moore, T.E. (1982) Subliminal advertising: what you see is what you get. Journal of

Marketing, 46, no. 2, pp. 38-47.

19. Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Schumann, D. (1983) Central and peripheral routes to

advertising effectiveness: The moderating role of involvement. Journal of Consumer

Research, 10, pp. 134-148.

20. Packard, V. (1957). The hidden persuaders. New York: D. McKay Co.

21. Potter, C. M.,Wyble, B., Hagmann C.E., McCourt, & S. E., (2014) Detecting meaning in

RSVP at 13 ms per picture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, no.2, pp. 270-279.

22. Rogers, M., K.H. Smith (1993) Public perceptions of subliminal advertising: why

practitioners shouldn’t ignore this issue. Journal of Advertising Research, pp. 10-18.

23. Snodgrass, M., Bernat, E. & Shevrrin, H. (2004) Unconscious perception at the objective

detection threshold exists. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, no. 5, pp.888-889.

24. Scripture, E.W. (1897). The New Psychology. London: Walter Scott, LTD.

25. Strahan, E.J., S.J. Spencer, M.P. Zanna (2002) Subliminal priming and persuasion: striking

while the iron is hot. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, pp. 556-568.

26. Trappey, C. (1996) A meta-analysis of consumer choice and subliminal advertising.

Psychology & Marketing, 13, no. 5, pp. 517-530.

27. Watson, J. B. (1922). What cigarette are you smoking and why? The J. Walter Thompson

News Bulletin, 88, pp. 1-17.

28. Wheat, K.L., Cornelissen, P.L., Frost, S.J., & Hansen, P.C. (2010) During visual word

recognition phonology is accessed within 100 ms and may be mediated by a speech

production code: Evidence from MEG. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 5229-5233.

29. Zanot, E.J., Pincus, J.D. Lamp, E.J. (1983) Public perceptions of subliminal advertising.

Journal of Advertising, 12, no. 1, pp. 39-45.

-REFERENCES

AVSEC, A., 2012, Do emotionally intelligent


individuals use more adaptive decision making

styles? Studia Psychologica, 54, 3, 209-220.

BALOTA, D.A., 1983, Automatic semantic activation and episodic memory encoding. Journal of

Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 88-104.

DAMASIO, A.R., 2000, Descartův omyl: Emoce,

rozum a lidský mozek, Praha: Mladá fronta.

DEBNER, J.A., JACOBY, L.L., 1994, Unconscious perception: Attention, awareness, and control. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 20, 2, 304-317.

DELL’ACQUA, R., GRAIGNER, J., 1999, Unconscious semantic priming from pictures. Cognition, 73, B1-
B15.

DRAINE, S.C., GREENWALD, A.G., 1998, Replicable unconscious semantic priming. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 3, 286-

303.

FOWLER, C.A., WOLFORD, G., SLADE, R.,

TASSINARY, L., 1981, Lexical access with and without awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 110, 341-362.

ISEN, A.M., DAUBMAN, K.A., NOWICKI, G.P.,

1987, Positive affect facilitates creative problem

solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 6, 1122-1131.

KARREMANS, J.C., STROEBE, W., CLAUS, J.,

2006, Beyond Vicary’s fantasies: The impact of

subliminal priming and brand choice. Journal of

Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 792-798.

KIHLSTROM, J.F., MULVANEY, S., TOBIAS,

B.A., TOBIS, I.P., 2000, The Emotional Unconscious. In: E. Eich (Ed.), Cognition and Emotion.

New York: Oxford University Press.

KUNST-WILSON, W.R., ZAJONC, R.B., 1980,

Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot be

recognized. Science, 207, 4430, 557-558.

MCCAULEY, C., PARMELEE, C.M.,


SPERBER, R.D., CARR, T.H., 1980, Early extraction of meaning from pictures and its relation to

conscious identification. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,

6, 265-276.

MERIKLE, P.M., DANEMAN, M., 1998, Psychological investigations of unconscious perception. Journal of
Consciousness Studies, 5, 1, 5-

18.

MERIKLE, P.M. REINGOLD, E.M., 1991,

Comparing direct (explicit) and indirect (implicit)

measures to study unconscious memory. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and

Cognition, 17, 224-233.

MERIKLE, P.M., REINGOLD, E.M., 1998, On

demonstrating unconscious perception: Comment

on Draine and Greenwald. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: General, 127, 3, 304-310.

MERIKLE, P.M., 2000, Subliminal perception.

In: A.E. Kazdin (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Psychology

(Vol. 7). New York: Oxford University Press.

MURPHY, S.T., ZAJONC, R.B., 1993, Affect,

cognition, and awareness: Affective priming with

optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 5,

723-739.

270 STUDIA PSYCHOLOGICA, 54, 2012, 4

PILÁRIK, Ľ., SARMÁNY-SCHULLER, I., 2009,

Emotional intelligence and decision making of female students of social work in Iowa Gambling Task.

Studia Psychologica, 51, 4, 319-328.

PILÁRIK, Ľ., SARMÁNY-SCHULLER, I., 2011,

Personality predictors of decision making of


medical rescuers. Studia Psychologica, 53, 2, 175-

184.

SARMÁNY-SCHULLER, I., 2010, Decision

making under time pressure in regard to preferred cognitive style (analytical-intuitive) and

study orientation. Studia Psychologica, 52, 4, 285-

290.

SARMÁNY-SCHULLER, I., KURAČKA, P.,

2012, Dimension of the cognitive style “analytical-intuitive” and the successful problem solving

in the “IGT” experimental situation. Studia

Psychologica, 54, 2, 95-109.

ŠPOK, D., 2007a, Nevědomá percepce: Metody

a koncepce. Dizertačná práca, Katedra psychologie,

Fakulta sociálních studií, Masarykova Univerzita,

Brno, Česká republika.

ŠPOK, D., 2007b, Výzkum nevědomé percepce

metodou analýzy nesprávných odpovedí: Identifikace číslic. Československá Psychologie, 51, 5, 489-

502.

ŠVECOVÁ, V., SARMÁNY-SCHULLER, I., 2005,

Vybrané aspekty podprahovej stimulácie - kritické

poznámky a experiment. Diplomová práca,

Univerzita Konštatntína Filozofa, Nitra.

ZAJONC, R.B., 2000, Feeling and thinking: Closing the debate over the independence of affect. In:

J.P. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: The role of

affect in social cognition (pp. 31-58). Cambridge,

UK: Cambridge University Press.

ZAJONC, R.B., 2001, Mere exposure: A gateway

to the subliminal. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 6, 224-228.

WINKIELMAN, P., BERRIDGE, K.C.,

WILBARGER, J.L., 2005, Unconscious affective


reactions to masked happy versus angry faces influence consumption behavior and judgments of

value. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,

31, 1, 121-135.

You might also like