You are on page 1of 9

Research Paper

British Journal of Occupational Therapy


1–9

Developmental coordination disorder is more ! The Author(s) 2017


Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
than a motor problem: Children describe the DOI: 10.1177/0308022617735046
http://journals.sagepub.com/home/bjot
impact of daily struggles on their quality of life

Jill G Zwicker1,2,3,4,5,6, Melinda Suto7, Susan R Harris8, Nikol Vlasakova9,10


and Cheryl Missiuna11,12

Abstract
Introduction: Affecting 5–6% of children, developmental coordination disorder is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by
poor motor coordination and difficulty learning motor skills. Although quantitative studies have suggested that children with
developmental coordination disorder experience reduced quality of life, no known qualitative studies have reported what daily life
is like from their perspective.
Method: Guided by an inductive realistic approach and using semi-structured, individual interviews, 13 children (8–12 years) were
asked to describe what life is like in their own words. Three researchers coded interviews manually to identify relevant content. An
experienced qualitative researcher conducted a second, in-depth thematic analysis using NVivo to identify patterns and themes.
Findings: Two themes – milestones as millstones and the perils of printing – illuminated participants’ challenges in completing
everyday activities at home and at school. The third theme – more than a motor problem – revealed the social and emotional impact
of these struggles and from being excluded from play. The fourth theme – coping strategies – described their efforts to be resilient.
Conclusion: Parents, educators, physicians, and therapists working with children with developmental coordination disorder must
recognize how their quality of life is affected by the physical and emotional toll of their efforts to participate successfully in daily
activities.

Keywords
Developmental coordination disorder, DCD, motor skills disorder, quality of life, school-aged children, thematic analysis

Received: 27 January 2017; accepted: 12 September 2017

1
Assistant Professor, Department of Occupational Science and Occupational
Therapy and Associate Member, Department of Pediatrics (Division of
Introduction Developmental Pediatrics), University of British Columbia, Vancouver,
Developmental coordination disorder (DCD) is a neuro- Canada
2
Associate Member, Department of Pediatrics (Division of Developmental
developmental disorder characterized by marked impair-
Pediatrics), University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
ment of motor coordination that significantly interferes 3
Investigator, Brain, Behaviour and Development, BC Children’s Hospital
with academic achievement, performance of activities of Research Institute, Vancouver, Canada
4
daily living, and engagement in play (American Clinician Scientist, Sunny Hill Health Centre for Children, Vancouver,
Psychiatric Association (APA), 2013). With a prevalence Canada
5
Research Associate, CanChild Centre for Childhood Disability Research,
of 5–6% (APA, 2013), DCD is one of the most common Hamilton, Canada
disorders affecting school-aged children. While the eti- 6
Affiliate Investigator, Kids Brain Health Network, Vancouver, Canada
ology of DCD is unknown, several brain imaging studies 7
Associate Professor Emerita, Department of Occupational Science and
have been conducted to suggest neurological underpin- Occupational Therapy, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
8
nings of the disorder, including under-activation of brain Professor Emerita, Department of Physical Therapy, University of British
Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
regions associated with motor learning and altered devel- 9
PhD student, Department of Occupational Science and Occupational
opment of motor and sensory pathways (Brown-Lum and Therapy, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
Zwicker, 2015). Children experience numerous functional 10
PhD student, Charles University in Prague, Czech Republic
11
difficulties related to their motor incoordination, including Professor, School of Rehabilitation Science, McMaster University,
difficulty with dressing, tying shoes, using utensils, riding a Hamilton, Canada
12
Scientist, CanChild Centre for Childhood Disability Research, Hamilton,
bike, catching a ball, handwriting, participating in phys-
Canada
ical play, and engaging in leisure activities (APA, 2013;
Corresponding author:
Zwicker et al., 2012). While it was once believed that chil- Dr. Jill Zwicker, BC Children’s Hospital, K3-180, 4480 Oak Street, Vancouver,
dren with DCD would outgrow their motor difficulties, BC, V6H 3V4, Canada.
evidence suggests that these difficulties persist into Email: jill.zwicker@ubc.ca
2 British Journal of Occupational Therapy 0(0)

adolescence and adulthood (Cantell et al., 2003; Cousins adults reflecting on their teen years (Missiuna et al.,
and Smyth, 2003; Kirby et al., 2011). 2008) described being bullied, laughed at, and teased by
Children with DCD tend to avoid social and physical their peers because of their poor motor skills. The emo-
activities (Chen and Cohn, 2003; Missiuna et al., 2007) tional impact of DCD extends into adulthood, with adults
and are at higher risk for obesity (Cairney et al., 2010) reporting high levels of depressive symptoms and poor life
and coronary vascular disease (Faught et al., 2005). In satisfaction (Kirby et al., 2013; Tal-Saban et al., 2014).
addition to physical concerns, these children may experi- As no known qualitative studies of children with DCD
ence secondary psychosocial issues, including difficulty from their own perspectives have been published, little is
with social and peer relationships (Dewey et al., 2002; known about the QOL of younger children based on their
Poulsen and Ziviani, 2004), lower self-worth and self- subjective experiences and in their own voices. A greater
esteem (Piek et al., 2006; Zwicker et al., 2013), anxiety understanding of what life is like for these children will
and depression (Missiuna et al., 2014; Piek et al., 2006), increase awareness of the significant impact of DCD and
loneliness (Poulsen et al., 2007), and emotional/behavioral may lead to greater ability to support them at home, at
disorders (Green et al., 2006; Lingam et al., 2012). school, and in the community.
Quantitative evidence suggests that children with DCD
may experience lower quality of life (QOL) compared to
peers (Zwicker et al., 2013), but few qualitative studies
Method
have been conducted. Some studies have captured the per- This qualitative research study used thematic analysis to
spectives of parents (Mandich et al., 2003; Missiuna et al., explore the QOL of children with DCD from their own
2006, 2007; Segal et al., 2002) and of adolescents/young perspective (Braun and Clark, 2006). Using an inductive
adults with DCD (Kirby et al., 2013; Lingam et al., 2014; realistic approach, we focused on accessing the children’s
Missiuna et al., 2008), but no studies have documented personal experiences of their daily life. Children were
what life is like from the voices of school-aged children. recruited from caseloads of occupational or physical ther-
Thus, the purpose of this study was to interview school- apists and met the following inclusion criteria: (1) prob-
aged children with DCD to better understand the implica- able DCD based on clinician judgment and level of motor
tions of the disorder on their daily life and to deepen our impairment (416th percentile on the Movement
understanding about their QOL. Assessment Battery for Children – 2 (Henderson et al.,
2007)); (2) aged 8 years to 12 years; (3) able to speak
and understand English fluently; and (4) able to demon-
Literature review
strate awareness of their condition and/or motor difficul-
Although physical and psychosocial concerns related to ties in order to be information-rich cases for data
DCD have been well documented (Harris et al., 2015; collection. Parents completed the DCD Questionnaire
Zwicker et al., 2013), few studies have examined the (Wilson et al., 2007) to confirm that they had observed
health-related QOL of children with DCD. Health-related their child’s motor difficulties during activities at home.
QOL is a multi-dimensional construct encompassing three Children were excluded if they had other medical condi-
broad domains of functioning: physical, psychological, tions that might contribute to motor impairment (such as
and social (Rajmil et al., 2004). Health-related QOL ide- cerebral palsy) or if parents reported that their child had a
ally reflects the individual’s perception of their subjective condition that commonly co-occurs with DCD, such as
experience of QOL, but proxy reports are also considered attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, learning disability,
(Eiser and Morse, 2001). Two qualitative studies based on or autism spectrum disorder. Comorbidities were excluded
parental interviews suggest that DCD may be detrimental so that we could better understand the personal experience
to children’s QOL. Parents of children aged 7 to 12 years of children with DCD without confounding with other
with DCD reported QOL concerns such as their children disorders. This study received approval from the
‘‘com[ing] home from school in tears’’ because they did University of British Columbia Behavioural Research
not have the motor skills needed to play physical games Ethics Board. Parents provided written consent and all
with other children and their children’s low self-esteem in children assented to participate in the study.
trying to conduct simple motor activities such as tying Capitalizing on the relative verbal strength of children
shoes (Mandich et al., 2003: 589). Missiuna et al. (2007) with DCD (Alloway, 2007; Missiuna et al., 2004), individ-
interviewed parents of children with DCD and discovered ual semi-structured interviews were conducted. While it
a developmental trajectory of QOL-related issues across was once believed that young children could not provide
the childhood years: from motor and play concerns in accurate information in interviews, qualitative research
early childhood; to problems with self-care, school, and has demonstrated that children as young as 4 years old
peer interactions in middle childhood; and issues with can provide information regarding their daily lives and
self-esteem and emotional health in later childhood. health experiences (Irwin and Johnson, 2005). To facilitate
Parents expressed great concern about the emotional the interview, children received a disposable camera prior
impact of DCD – from children’s repeated experiences to the interview and were asked to take photos of activities
of failure to master everyday activities to negative experi- they enjoyed doing and activities that they wished they
ences at school (Mandich et al., 2003; Missiuna et al., could do. While other researchers have asked children to
2006). Adolescents (Lingam et al., 2014) and young draw pictures to initiate conversation (Green et al., 2009;
Zwicker et al. 3

Kortesluoma et al., 2003), we chose to use photographs to Table 1. Participant characteristics.


avoid taxing the children with DCD with drawing, given
Pseudonym Age (years) Sex DCDQ score (interpretation)
their poor motor skills. The film was developed prior to
the interview so that the photographs could be used solely Grant 8 Male 35 (indication of DCD)
to initiate discussion; photographs were not analyzed. Adam 9 Male 25 (indication of DCD)
Children were asked to describe their photographs and Cindy 9 Female 30 (indication of DCD)
were prompted to describe the thoughts and feelings that Jack 9 Male 19 (indication of DCD)
the pictures evoked. The interviewer (JZ) used follow-up Peter 9 Male 35 (indication of DCD)
probes and asked questions from the interview guide Sarah 9 Female 42 (suspect DCD)
(Appendix). The guide was comprised of 10 questions Thomas 9 Male 31 (indication of DCD)
developed by investigators (JZ, SH, CM) based on a Nicolas 10 Male 39 (suspect DCD)
review of pediatric QOL and DCD literature. Interviews Tristan 10 Male 32 (indication of DCD)
were 25 to 80 minutes long and occurred in a quiet room in Alison 11 Female 48 (suspect DCD)
a university setting, child health center, or participant’s Bill 12 Male 24 (indication of DCD)
home; occasionally a parent was present. Data were col- Jason 12 Male 25 (indication of DCD)
lected using a digital voice recorder and transcribed ver- Philip 12 Male 34 (indication of DCD)
batim by a professional transcriptionist who replaced DCDQ: Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire.
participants’ names with pseudonyms.
Four researchers contributed to the inductive thematic
data analysis, following the process suggested by Braun and
Clarke (2006). We adopted a realistic approach that focuses
Findings
on the text itself, analyzing what was actually said.
Consistent with this, we first generated codes for the data, Three girls and 10 boys ranging in age from 8 to 12 years
and then conducted thematic analysis at the semantic level, were recruited and participated in this study. They lived in
with themes being identified based upon explicit or surface three different urban cities in British Columbia, Canada.
meanings, rather than interpretation. These methodo- Demographic information appears in Table 1. Four inter-
logical choices arose from the exploratory nature of the related themes provide insight about the experience of
research, the number and length of interviews, and the abil- living with DCD, from participants’ perspectives: (1) mile-
ity of young participants to articulate their experiences. stones as millstones: struggling to perform ordinary activ-
Initially, three team members (JZ, SH, NV) coded all ities; (2) the perils of printing: schooling as hard work; (3)
interviews manually to identify chunks of relevant content. more than a motor problem: left out of left field; and (4)
An experienced qualitative researcher (MS) conducted a coping strategies: thinking differently and emphasizing
second, more in-depth analysis using NVivo10 to aid with strengths. Consistent with the aim of understanding sub-
coding and finding patterns in the data. She identified 27 jective experiences, quotes remain unedited except where
codes that included fatigue, feelings, sports, and tricky clarity was needed. Further, the voices of participants are
things about eating, dressing, and grooming. An iterative emphasized, whereas the researchers’ interpretation
process of eliminating duplicate data and reading the appears in the Discussion section of this article.
interviews several times helped crystallize the study find-
ings into themes that were negotiated with team members. Milestones as millstones: struggling to perform
A reflexive process enabled team members to gauge the
influence that their assumptions, knowledge, and beliefs
ordinary activities
had on the data analysis. The first author (JZ) has extensive Developmental milestones mark progression through
experience as an occupational therapist working with chil- childhood and reflect skills that, once acquired, support
dren with DCD, as well as research training in rehabilita- independence. Participants’ descriptions of performing
tion and familiarity with literature on DCD and quality of ordinary activities (for example using utensils, getting
life. These experiences contributed to the first author dressed, riding a bicycle) are noteworthy because of the
developing astute interview questions. The second author early onset of the difficulties and the impact on their daily
(MS) has no experience with children with DCD but is lives. Describing tricky aspects of mealtime routines, 9-
aware of research that shows how environments affect chil- year-olds reported eating with spoons but struggling to
dren’s participation regardless of individual factors asso- manipulate knives and forks successfully. Cindy’s inability
ciated with diagnoses. The third author (SH) is a seasoned to coordinate a knife and fork was apparent from her
pediatric physiotherapist who has worked extensively with photo, in which she described positioning her elbow awk-
children with many types of disabilities. The fourth author wardly and gripping her fork hard. Sarah’s mealtime
(NV) is a graduate student interested in DCD. The fifth description shows how her coordination difficulties con-
author (CM), a DCD expert, was involved in study tribute to messes at the table:
design and question creation but not data analysis.
Recognition of the lenses through which we analyzed the When I eat breakfast, like say – cereal this morning – I
data helped to assure that the findings reflected partici- sometimes, um. I describe something [using her hands]
pants’ experiences rather than our biases or expectations. for her [mother] and then I spilled my, um, cup over
4 British Journal of Occupational Therapy 0(0)

so . . . And when and also when, when I just took a Learning to ride a two-wheeled bicycle is a rite of pas-
cereal and I ate it then it missed my mouth. sage for most children, offering physical freedom and
opportunities to socialize with friends. Some participants
The children aged 10 to 12 years reported better fork use described bike-riding as easy and some recalled learning to
but struggled to cut food with a knife. They persisted in ride by about age 6 years. However, many children, such
mastering this skill despite limited success and seemed as Cindy and Sarah, learned or were still learning to ride a
perplexed by their difficulties. Nicolas shared this bicycle at 9 years old and they communicated ambivalence
experience: and fear of bicycling. This differed from Thomas’ enthu-
siasm and pride: ‘‘Well, I always really want to do it
Well, I don’t really know how to hold them very well. mostly. I felt pretty good because it was hard for me to
Like the fork is fine but like the knife. This is my, learn how to ride it.’’
I can’t do very much with this hand. My right Few participants described biking with friends, riding
hand or I can only use this one. And I usually use to school, or having two-wheeled adventures. Participants
that for the knife but then the fork, it’s kind of hard were afraid of falling, sometimes banged into parked cars,
if I use this one with the knife and this one with the and used training wheels longer than their peers. Nicolas
fork. And I’ve been trying to get it right but I haven’t was unable to bike with friends yet, explaining: ‘‘My dad is
quite yet. trying to teach me how to ride a bike . . . And it’s a bit hard
because I never know if I’ll fall or not . . . But I’ve been
Bill, Philip, and Jason identified knife use as a problem doing okay but it’s hard.’’
and, like younger participants, still accepted their parents’ Falling off a bicycle happens to most children when
help. Whereas Nicolas and Tristan felt ‘sort of bad’ they learn to ride, but these participants described their
that they could not use a knife, 9-year-olds like Sarah efforts to overcome bike-riding fears, and seemed to spend
described being pleased to have parental assistance. longer and work harder to master this typically enjoyable
Parents helped their children further by supplying lunch childhood activity.
foods, such as sandwiches and cheese and crackers, that Overall, this theme reflects the efforts and difficulties
fitted the child’s skills and containers with large tabs for that children experienced with everyday activities, such
easier opening. as dressing, eating, and tying shoes, and highlights how
Not surprisingly, poor coordination and body aware- children perceive their physical functioning QOL domain
ness impeded participants’ ability to dress. Their descrip- to be affected by DCD.
tions of dressing reveal bewildering experiences. Jack
explained:
The perils of printing: schooling as hard work
Um, it’s sort of hard for me to get dressed . . . My pants, Difficulty with printing/handwriting was an obstacle to
um, are quite hard because my feet get stuck inside enjoying and succeeding in school. Participants needed
because they’re always inside, they’re always inside to write notes, copy from the board, take tests, and
out and sometimes they get stuck inside the legs. write reports. Printing (handwritten print letters) problems
were widespread and involved messiness, slowness, and
Bill’s dressing difficulty is atypical for his age and evokes difficulty with letter formation. Tristan explained: ‘‘Um,
the disconnection between intention and results: my normal printing, most people can’t read it; well it’s like
my dad can probably read it and my mum can probably.
Well, like when I pull on my pants like I put the leg and My brother could probably but he’d say he couldn’t and
it goes into the wrong socket like when I put on my that it was really horrible.’’ Philip’s experiences revealed
shorts. My right leg goes into the left socket and it just an unwelcome result: ‘‘Sometimes if there’s a time limit I
really annoys me. like, have to rush and I get stuff wrong because I’m rush-
ing because I don’t want to take forever. I might end up
Similarly, Jack described confusion: ‘‘Um, like here it’s not finishing the test.’’ Other participants reported that
hard because I can’t see what I’m doing outside my body- their impulse to skip over items arose from hand strain.
. . . it’s sort of dark and I don’t know where my hands are.’’ Thomas explained why printing was hard: ‘‘When I, I’m
Participants of all ages reported difficulty in tying shoe- writing some words sometimes I forgot what the letter is
laces, whereas younger children also described problems supposed to look like.’’ Jack adds, ‘‘and um, the curving
buttoning shirts and managing zippers. Some participants and drawing that it takes.’’
who were still learning to tie wore shoes with Velcro clos- Printing problems that were identified as such led to
ures. Adam’s description of shoe-tying typified children’s strategies using technological and human resources.
perseverance: Participants obtained some benefits from using computers,
tablets, and typing programs to address printing and
I keep trying to learn knots and I try and I try and I try handwriting issues. However, what interfered with their
and I just keep forgetting and still doing it quite, I, and printing also interfered with effective keyboarding. Cindy
this hand thing is one of the problems that stops me noted: ‘‘Um, the computer it’s hard because you have to
from tying a perfect knot. control all the keys like this so I just poke them with my
Zwicker et al. 5

fingers.’’ Despite considerable practice, some children responses to their unsatisfying involvement in team
found typing to be difficult. Alison reported: sports. Cindy said: ‘‘If I run like a long time then my
feet start to hurt and then, and then I walk. Sometimes
I’ve done three, two, um, typing things, typing pro- when I can’t do the same things as other friends and kids
grams both, the whole thing at my home and I’ve and sometimes it bothers me.’’ Participants often con-
done the same typing program almost twice in school nected their fatigue and limited physical coordination to
and it’s still, I’m still looking down and I can’t really negative feelings about team sports involvement. In the
type fast. context of tiring easily and feeling ‘‘bad, annoyed, and
frustrated,’’ Bill said: ‘‘Like, when we’re playing at kick-
In contrast to some participants’ frustrations with tech- ball I usually don’t do well, I just like quit on it.’’ Jack
nology, others appreciated help from parents and teaching revealed how physical limitations affect him: ‘‘I always
staff. When describing how he keeps up in school, Jason think I’m a loser and um, you know feel kind of sad for
said: ‘‘I’ve got a, um, I forget what they’re called, the quite a long time but I’ll get over it. It’s really sad and you
person who helps you and she like, well, she writes, she don’t think you can do it. And you stop trying.’’
writes for me and she types for me because I’m also very Participants’ reports of feeling frustrated, sad, and/or
bad at typing.’’ These participants’ experiences were spe- left out reveal the impact that repeated academic and
cific to printing, but handwriting problems also abounded. play failures had on their future involvement.
Both limitations prompted negative feelings. Some partici- Participants told stories about being left out of gym
pants like Alison, however, accepted their abilities: ‘‘I activities (voluntary or otherwise) that helped to explain
could be practicing writing as much as I want all day their sadness and frustration. Nicolas linked feeling
and stuff and I’d still, it still would be practically the ‘‘depressed or sad’’ to ‘‘having not to do what other kids
same.’’ Taking a different attitude than Alison’s, Philip can do like run fast or, um, or do a lot of fun stuff that I
was highly critical of the limited awareness and compas- can’t do.’’ This depth of feeling was unsurprising given
sion he perceived from teachers: Nicolas’ experience of basketball drills: ‘‘Nobody usually
passes to me so I just run around and try to get the ball
It’s harder than it should be. And then most of my and maybe pass it to someone else. And that’s okay but I
teachers except for one were stupid and like, didn’t don’t usually get to have the ball.’’ Thomas revealed a
notice . . . I lost a lot of marks in school because I had pattern of exclusion from play:
messy handwriting which I didn’t think was
fair . . . They said oh, I’m just lazy . . . So how would No one ever passes the ball to me and when I’m playing
they feel if someone called them lazy when they’re hockey, no one passes me the puck. Ringette no one
working their hardest. Or when you make a mistake ever passes the hoop to me. I’d rather have the thing
and they point it out to you every waking moment. sometimes . . . But I can’t get to them as fast as everyone
else.
This theme demonstrates that participants tried to
master the fine motor skills needed for scholastic success Reflecting what happened to many participants who
but that their efforts often came with emotional costs. This described fatigue and low fitness levels, Peter stated:
theme indicates the overlap of physical and psychological ‘‘I’m getting left out at running activities.’’
QOL domains that can be affected in children with DCD. Some participants described feeling afraid of balls and
other play equipment and getting hit frequently during
games like dodge ball. Recess was challenging as play typ-
More than a motor problem: left out of left field ically involved monkey bars, four-square games,1 and
A word cloud of unhappy feelings permeated descriptions other activities requiring continuous movement. Sarah
of participants’ motor problems at school. Children often summarized her feelings about physical activities:
identified their emotions precisely; other times, negative
feelings were inferred from words like ‘‘bothered’’ and Well for the skipping rope it’s, it’s I might trip on it and
‘‘not good.’’ Tristan’s comment reflects the frustration fall and hurt myself. Monkey bars I lose my grip, fall.
participants felt when trying to print, handwrite, and key- And with soccer I’m afraid I might kick it but instead
board: ‘‘Lots of times when I write stuff, I just, I get mad of kicking it put my foot on the ball and then slip when
but just like. Once I got so mad I just took the pencil and the ball goes. And this, I’m afraid I might fall and hurt
went bam, split it in two.’’ Although participants myself. So is mostly afraid.
expressed a range of feelings about not meeting self-care
and writing expectations, their strongest negative feelings Sarah’s inability to use playground equipment led to her
arose over school sports and social activities. being alone and, like other children, feeling lonely. She
Sports activities that participants either had to do, or explained:
wanted to do, required them to run, catch, throw, and
balance, and sometimes to use all these skills simultan- I have this corner as I have no one to play with at
eously. The description of feeling fatigued and having recess, and I just sit there and color or anything and I
sore muscles offered a perspective on the emotional do something and wait for the recess bell so I can go
6 British Journal of Occupational Therapy 0(0)

in. . . . I’m not really strong enough to say ‘‘can I play everything will be all right, yeah man.’’ Taking a similar
with you?’’ approach, Alison said: ‘‘I think, whatever, I’m still just
me. . . . I’m not gonna let it ruin my life, it’s going to con-
Self-exclusion and engagement in quiet play were tinue.’’ These perspectives helped participants who were
benign outcomes compared to the stories that male par- facing limited skills, feeling frustrated, or being sidelined
ticipants told about being excluded and bullied. attribute these difficulties to the motor impairment rather
Classmates responded to participants’ different abilities than define them.
and vulnerabilities and none of these left children feeling Participants chose solitary activities that engaged their
good. Thomas explained: imagination such as writing stories, reading, and drawing.
The physical activities they selected were less competitive,
There’s this guy in my class who’s really good at things required fewer people, and aligned with their interests and
so when I’m on the same team as him, he tells to get the skills. For example, Sarah chose individual activities with
team so they can make up a plan, he never includes me others where she could exercise some control over the out-
in the plan. come; thus, ballet lessons, Jazzercise, choir, and swimming
were good fits. In situations where participants felt fru-
Nicolas described being teased but recognized the injustice strated or recognized their limitations, they did something
of being blamed for sports outcomes he could not control. different to feel better. Cindy reported: ‘‘Sometimes I wish
I could, um, play tag better but, um, I feel okay when I
There are a few kids in my class who say ‘‘you didn’t do walk around by myself at recess.’’
anything, you didn’t do anything.’’ Well, it’s because I Participants were creative and distracted and/or dis-
don’t usually have the ball, not because I don’t get to tanced themselves emotionally when experiencing prob-
do much [but] because I don’t get the ball, nobody lems. Thomas took this approach when sidelined for
passes it to me. being ‘‘out’’ early in team sports: ‘‘Luckily, my head is
full of things to think about. . . . I write little songs in my
The male participants described instances of hassling and head.’’ Nicolas described feeling frustrated when a peer
bullying ranging from critical remarks about their dancing repeatedly tricked him in four-square games and said:
ability to physical altercations. Peter offered this story: ‘‘But I usually have, have a laugh or have a bit of fun
maybe. I just laugh along maybe.’’ These strategies reflect
And everybody laughs at me when I try to run on the participants’ agency, although their words hinted at an
grass . . . They sometimes bully me and chase me and all unspoken emotional burden. Despite the challenges of
that. And it’s not making me feel that good. When I’m DCD, participants like Grant (‘‘I’m really good at being
playing out on the field with my friends and they come a goalie’’) and Bill (‘‘I have a very good imagination’’)
up and push me around, and I push them back and were able to describe their talents and anticipated a posi-
because they’re making me really mad and I, sometimes tive future. Nicolas captured this hopefulness: ‘‘There’s
I can’t control my madness. always something that you can do that other kids
can’t. . . . Like your talent, everyone has a talent.’’ These
This description of bullying demonstrates the social attitudes and activity choices offer an alternative to the
and emotional implications of DCD that may be second- typically negative connotation of a motor ‘‘difference,’’
ary to an individual’s difficulties with motor skills. As suggesting that for some participants, their differences
Philip concluded: ‘‘It kind of makes you feel different. do not negatively influence their QOL.
Not in a good way, though.’’ This theme illuminates
types of exclusion and accompanying negative feelings
for participants whose abilities do not meet age expect-
Discussion and implications
ations, and shows how DCD affects QOL across three The four themes stemming from this work resonate with
domains of functioning – physical, psychological, and existing literature. The first two themes have been noted in
social. The final theme depicts participants’ agency in many other studies and, in fact, the struggle to succeed in
managing the challenges of living with DCD. everyday self-care and academic activities is one of the
diagnostic criteria of DCD (Criterion B) (APA, 2013).
Coping strategies: thinking differently and Children with DCD are also known to participate less in
physical and social activities than typically developing
emphasizing strengths peers (Zwicker et al., 2013). Further, difficulty with
Participants developed various strategies to cope with school-related tasks is a problem that arises in almost all
their experience of difference that arose from not meeting literature about children with DCD. While handwriting is
age-related expectations. Children described cognitive often presented as the dominant concern, strategies to sup-
strategies that contributed to their self-esteem and contin- port children with DCD, such as using a computer, may
ued participation in activities that were important to them. initially be as challenging for them as handwriting.
Jason reframed his situation: ‘‘DCD isn’t like the whole Struggles at school extend far beyond handwriting and
thing ruling your life or anything . . . You, you’ll still be do not seem to improve with age: a qualitative study
very, very good at everything, just get a scribe and of 11 teenagers revealed that all reported difficulty
Zwicker et al. 7

at school – academically, socially, and in sports (Lingam across a broad spectrum of children who have varied
et al., 2014). In many jurisdictions, students with DCD do motor skills. This suggests that universal programs that
not receive any support at school, further perpetuating the are designed to support motor skill development could
negative trajectory and psychosocial problems. benefit many children, particularly those with DCD.
While some of the findings from this study are not Service delivery models such as Partnering for Change
surprising given a recent systematic review of the literature may be a first step in putting resources in place to support
of QOL domains affected in DCD (Zwicker et al., 2013), the participation of children with DCD, and all children
this is the first study to specifically ask children about their who are experiencing motor challenges, in the school con-
own subjective experience of living with DCD. The quota- text (Missiuna et al., 2012). Consideration must also be
tions from the children highlight the extensive impact of given to ensuring that social supports are established on
DCD on their QOL and reaffirm that DCD is more than a the playground and that bullying and leaving children out
motor impairment. of play situations is actively discouraged (Missiuna and
Another novel contribution that came through the Campbell, 2014).
voices of children in this qualitative study was the extent Therapeutic interventions for children with DCD are
to which they highlighted the emotional toll of attempting often provided individually; a recent systematic review
to perform daily activities, without success. The and meta-analysis concluded that there was lack of evi-
Elaborated Environmental Stress Hypothesis (Cairney dence for impairment-oriented interventions but that there
et al., 2013) is a recently introduced conceptual model, was evidence to support task-oriented approaches (for
based upon literature about children with DCD, that example Neuromotor Task Training, Cognitive
posits that DCD is a primary stressor that leads to a var- Orientation to daily Occupational Performance (CO-
iety of secondary psychosocial stressors, including peer OP)), which help children improve their ability to perform
victimization and low social support; these secondary selected daily activities (Smits-Engelsman et al., 2013). The
stressors eventually result in children and adolescents perceptions shared by the children in this study would
with DCD experiencing internalizing problems, such as support this emphasis on individual mastery of daily activ-
anxiety and depression. The voices of the children pre- ities. In fact, a recent quasi-experimental study of 20 boys
sented in this study support a direct link between DCD with DCD showed that a 10-week program using the CO-
and psychosocial stressors and highlight the many reasons OP approach resulted in improvements in daily life activ-
why children may withdraw from physical activities. In the ities and overall participation, based on child-selected
third theme, children noted their inability to keep up and goals (Thornton et al., 2016). The impact of these
participate in many of the structured and unstructured improvements on participation in school or community
sports and games that are typical of outdoor play at sports activities is not known.
school. There were also poignant descriptions of the emo-
tional impact of being left out, teased, or bullied as a
result. On a more positive note, the fourth theme in this
Limitations
study also highlighted factors that could potentially mod- Children who were interviewed in this study were aware of
erate the psychosocial outcomes. The model proposes that their motor challenges and were able to provide relatively
increasing social resources (including peer and parental rich descriptions of their daily struggles. The experiences
support) and increasing personal resources (including a may not be the same for children who are unaware that
child’s sense of mastery and improved self-esteem) could they have motor coordination difficulties, or those who
moderate the relationship between the known psycho- have commonly co-occurring conditions such as attention
social stressors and internalizing problems. Children in deficit hyperactivity disorder. As QOL is a difficult con-
this study described the benefits of having a scribe, cept for children to understand, we opted to ask questions
having access to technology, or of having a parent persist related to the domains of QOL: physical, psychological,
with trying to teach bike-riding. While there was limited and social functioning. Second interviews with each par-
evidence of social resources, ways of increasing personal ticipant may have deepened our analysis; however, this is
resources were definitely identified. Children described the first known study to explicitly ask school-age children
choosing to engage in individual sports activities in the what life is like from their own perspective. While the
community, such as swimming and dance. In these types findings appear to provide additional evidence in support
of activities, children are not in competition with others of the elaborated environmental stress hypothesis, this
but are encouraged through verbal teaching to learn study was cross-sectional and it is not known whether
motor skills sequentially and to gradually increase the effi- any participants went on to experience internalizing
ciency of the performance as they work toward mastery. problems.
Joining a choir, writing little songs in their head or having
a laugh to block the feeling of being left out were other
ways that children protected their self-esteem.
Conclusion
More recently, Mancini and colleagues (2016) summar- Parents, educators, physicians, therapists, and others
ized evidence showing that this negative relationship working with children with DCD need to listen closely
between motor skills and symptoms of internalizing dis- to recognize the physical and emotional effort exerted by
orders holds not only for children with DCD but also children just to participate in everyday life activities.
8 British Journal of Occupational Therapy 0(0)

School is a daily challenge; not just coping with academic Brown-Lum M and Zwicker JG (2015) Brain imaging increases
expectations, but dealing with the social ramifications of our understanding of developmental coordination disorder:
having a motor disorder that affects participation in sports A review of literature and future directions. Current
Developmental Disorders Reports 2(2): 131–140.
activities. Resilience may be fostered and self-esteem built
Cairney J, Hay JA, Veldhuizen S, et al. (2010) Trajectories of
through participation in activities that are successful for relative weight and waist circumference among children with
the child and that align with their strengths. and without developmental coordination disorder. Canadian
Medical Association Journal 182(11): 1167–1172.
Key findings Cairney J, Rigoli D and Piek J (2013) Developmental coordin-
ation disorder and internalizing problems in children: The
. Children with DCD struggle to perform everyday environmental stress hypothesis elaborated. Developmental
activities. Reviews 33(3): 224–238.
Cantell MH, Smyth MM and Ahonen TP (2003) Two distinct
. Physical efforts are complicated by secondary emo-
pathways for developmental coordination disorder:
tional and social challenges.
Persistence and resolution. Human Movement Science
. Encouraging a positive attitude and recognizing 22(4–5): 413–431.
strengths may foster resilience. Chen HF and Cohen ES (2003) Social participation for children
with developmental coordination disorder: Conceptual, evalu-
ation and intervention considerations. Physical &
What the study has added Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics 23(4): 61–78.
Qualitative research findings reflecting the quality of Cousins M and Smyth MM (2003) Developmental coordination
life of children with DCD have deepened our under- impairments in adulthood. Human Movement Science 22(4–5):
433–459.
standing of the impact of physical challenges on their
Dewey D, Kaplan BJ, Crawford SG, et al. (2002) Developmental
social participation and the resulting emotional toll. coordination disorder: Associated problems in attention,
learning, and psychosocial adjustment. Human Movement
Acknowledgments Science 21(5–6): 905–918.
Eiser C and Morse R (2001) A review of measures of quality of
We thank the children and families who participated in this study and
Dr. Anne Klassen for her contributions to the methodology. life for children with chronic illness. Archives of Disease in
Childhood 84(3): 205–211.
Faught BE, Hay JA, Cairney J, et al. (2005) Increased risk for
Research ethics coronary vascular disease in children with developmental
Ethics approval was obtained from the University of British coordination disorder. Journal of Adolescent Health 37(5):
Columbia Behavioural Research Ethics Board, 27 November 2008. 376–380.
Parents provided written consent and all children assented to partici- Green AL, McSweeney J, Ainley K, et al. (2009) Comparing
pate in the study. parents’ and children’s views of children’s quality of life
after heart transplant. Journal for Specialists in Pediatric
Declaration of conflicting interests Nursing 14(1): 49–58.
Green D, Baird G and Sugden D (2006) A pilot study of psycho-
The authors confirm that there is no conflict of interest.
pathology in developmental coordination disorder. Child:
Care, Health, and Development 32(6): 741–750.
Funding Harris SR, Mickelson EC and Zwicker JG (2015) Diagnosis and
This research was funded by the Michael Smith Foundation for management of developmental coordination disorder.
Health Research. Dr. Zwicker is funded by the Michael Smith Canadian Medical Association Journal 187(9): 659–665.
Foundation for Health Research, BC Children’s Hospital Research Henderson SE, Sugden DA and Barnett A (2007) Movement
Institute, Sunny Hill Foundation, Canadian Child Health Clinician Assessment Battery for Children – Second Edition. London:
Scientist Program, and Canadian Institutes for Health Research. Dr. Psychological Corporation.
Missiuna holds the John & Margaret Lillie Chair in Childhood Irwin LG and Johnson J (2005) Interviewing young children:
Disability Research. Explicating our practices and dilemmas. Qualitative Health
Research 15(6): 821–831.
Kirby A, Edwards L and Sugden D (2011) Emerging adulthood
Note
in developmental coordination disorder: Parent and young
1. Four-square is a ball game played on a court with four equal- adult perspectives. Research in Developmental Disabilities
sized, numbered squares that contain each player. The goal is to 32(4): 1351–1360.
advance to square number 4 through one’s skilled ball-handling
Kirby A, Williams N, Thomas M, et al. (2013) Self-reported
and hand–eye coordination errors made by one’s opponents.
mood, general health, wellbeing and employment status in
adults with suspected DCD. Research in Developmental
References Disabilities 34(4): 1357–1364.
Alloway TP (2007) Working memory, reading, and mathematical Kortesluoma R-L, Hentinen M and Nikkonen M (2003)
skills in children with developmental coordination disorder. Conducting a qualitative child interview: Methodological con-
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 96(1): 20–36. siderations. Journal of Advanced Nursing 42(5): 434–441.
American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Lingam R, Jongmans MJ, Ellis M, et al. (2012) Mental health
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed. – text revision difficulties in children with developmental coordination dis-
ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. order. Pediatrics 129(4): e882–e891.
Braun V and Clarke V (2006) Using thematic analysis in psych- Lingam RP, Novak C, Emond A, et al. (2014) The importance of
ology. Qualitative Research in Psychology 3(2): 77–101. identity and empowerment to teenagers with developmental
Zwicker et al. 9

coordination disorder. Child: Care, Health, and Development Tal-Saban M, Ornoy A and Parush S (2014) Young adults with
40(3): 309–318. developmental coordination disorder: A longitudinal study.
Mancini VO, Rigoli D, Cairney J, et al. (2016) The Elaborated American Journal of Occupational Therapy 68(3): 307–316.
Environmental Stress Hypothesis as a framework for Thornton A, Licari M, Reid S, et al. (2016) Cognitive orientation
understanding the association between motor skills and inter- to (daily) occupational performance intervention leads to
nalizing problems: A mini-review. Frontiers in Psychology 7: improvements in impairments, activity and participation in
239. children with developmental coordination disorder.
Mandich AD, Polatajko HJ and Rodger S (2003) Rites of pas- Disability and Rehabilitation 38(10): 979–986.
sage: Understanding participation of children with develop- Wilson BN, Crawford SG and Roberts G (2007) The Developmental
mental coordination disorder. Human Movement Science Coordination Disorder Questionnaire 2007 (DCDQ ’07).
22(4–5): 583–595. Available at: http://dcdq.ca (accessed 26 January 2017).
Missiuna C and Campbell W (2014) Psychological aspects of Zwicker JG, Harris SR and Klassen AF (2013) Quality of life
DCD: Can we establish causality? Current Developmental domains affected in children with developmental coordination
Disorder Reports 1(2): 125–131. disorder: A systematic review. Child: Care, Health, and
Missiuna C, Cairney J, Pollock N, et al. (2014) Psychological Development 39(4): 562–580.
distress in children with developmental coordination disorder Zwicker JG, Missiuna C, Harris SR, et al. (2012) Developmental
and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Research in coordination disorder: A review and update. European Journal
Developmental Disabilities 35(5): 1198–1207. of Paediatric Neurology 16(6): 573–581.
Missiuna C, Moll S, King G, et al. (2008) Life experiences of
young adults who have coordination difficulties. Canadian
Journal of Occupational Therapy 75(3): 157–166. Appendix
Missiuna C, Moll S, King S, et al. (2007) A trajectory of troubles:
Parents’ impressions of the impact of developmental coordin- Interview guide
ation disorder. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics
27(1): 81–101. Children will take photographs of activities they enjoy and
Missiuna C, Moll S, Law M, et al. (2006) Mysteries and mazes: activities they wish they could or would like to do; these
Parents’ experiences of children with developmental coordin- photographs will be used to initiate conversation.
ation disorder. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy
73(1): 7–17. 1. Tell me about the activities you like to do. Why do you
Missiuna CA, Pollock NA, Levac DE, et al. (2012) Partnering for like these activities?
change: An innovative school-based occupational therapy ser-
2. Tell me about the activities you wish you could or would
vice delivery model for children with developmental coordin-
ation disorder. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy
like to do. What has stopped you from doing these
79(1): 41–50. activities?
Missiuna C, Rivard L and Pollock N (2004) They’re bright but 3. Some people use the name ‘‘developmental coordination
can’t write: Developmental coordination disorder in school disorder’’ or ‘‘DCD’’ to describe children who have diffi-
aged children. Teaching Exceptional Children Plus 1(1): 3. culty with motor skills, such as handwriting, riding a
Available at: http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ966505.pdf bike, or tying shoes.
(accessed 26 January 2017).. a. Is that term familiar to you? [Probe: ask if there is a
Piek JP, Baynam GB and Barrett NC (2006) The relationship
different term or phrase they would like to use, for
between fine and gross motor ability, self-perceptions and
example ‘‘difficult to do certain things.’’]
self-worth in children and adolescents. Human Movement
Science 25(1): 65–75. b. What activities do you find hard to do? Why? [Probe:
Poulsen AA and Ziviani JM (2004) Can I play too? Physical self-care (such as dressing, tying shoes, using utensils);
activity engagement of children with developmental coordin- school activities (such as handwriting, gym class);
ation disorders. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy recreation/leisure activities (such as sports, games,
71(2): 100–107. playing a musical instrument).]
Poulsen AA, Ziviani JM, Cuskelly M, et al. (2007) Boys with 4. Please describe a typical day and where DCD may impact
developmental coordination disorder: Loneliness and team
your daily routine. Let’s start from when you get up in
sports participation. American Journal of Occupational
the morning until you go to bed at night. [Probe for
Therapy 61(4): 451–462.
Rajmil L, Herdman M, Fernandez de Sanmamed M-J, et al. thoughts and feelings.]
(2004) Generic health-related quality of life instruments in 5. Has having DCD affected what you do with your friends?
children and adolescents: A qualitative analysis of content. If so, how?
Journal of Adolescent Health 34(1): 37–45. 6. Has having DCD affected how you feel about yourself?
Segal R, Mandich A, Polatajko H, et al. (2002) Stigma and its 7. Do you think DCD has affected how others feel about
management: A pilot study of parental perceptions of the you? How? [If necessary, ask if he/she has been teased or
experiences of children with developmental coordination dis-
bullied, if they have lots of friends, etc.]
order. American Journal of Occupational Therapy 56(4):
8. What is the hardest or most difficult thing about having
422–428.
Smits-Engelsman BC, Blank R, van der Kaay AC, et al. (2013) DCD?
Efficacy of interventions to improve motor performance in 9. If you were talking to another child who just found out
children with developmental coordination disorder: A com- that they have DCD, what would you want him or her to
bined systematic review and meta-analysis. Developmental know? What have you learned?
Medicine & Child Neurology 55(3): 229–237. 10. Any final thoughts about what life is like with DCD?

You might also like