You are on page 1of 5

Personality and Individual Differences 147 (2019) 28–32

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Socio-cognitive biases are associated to belief in neuromyths and cognitive T


enhancement: A pre-registered study

Michiel van Elk
Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
Amsterdam Brain and Cognition Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: This study identified the psychological and cognitive factors that predispose people to believe in neuromyths
Neuromyths (e.g., the belief that we only use 10% of our brain) and in cognitive enhancement (e.g., the belief that brain
Cognitive enhancement stimulation devices can enhance cognition). A new scale to measure belief in neuromyths and cognitive en-
Cognitive biases hancement was developed and validated. Belief in neuromyths was related to a more intuitive (compared to an
Brain pictures
analytical) thinking style, a stronger need for cognitive closure, a simpler view of epistemic cognition, en-
Intuitive and analytical thinking
dorsement of a fixed mindset, and it was inversely related to scientific literacy. Belief in cognitive enhancement
was specifically related to the endorsement of a growth mindset. No clear effect of the presence or absence of
brain pictures on credibility ratings for neuroscientific explanations and intuitive thinking was found. These
findings suggest that belief in neuromyths and cognitive enhancement is a multifaceted phenomenon and that
interventions to foster public understanding of neuroscience could be aimed at affecting thinking styles.

1. Introduction neuroscience experts endorse neuromyths to a great extent (Macdonald,


Germine, Anderson, Christodoulou, & McGrath, 2017). Belief in neu-
Why do so many people believe that we only use a limited capacity romyths is partly supported and reinforced by popular media reports
of our brain? Consider the case of a man who uses a brain-training (Macdonald et al., 2017) that often refer to neuroscientific evidence in
application on his smartphone to activate his hidden mental potential. an illegitimate way. It has also been suggested that complex and col-
Or take the example of a woman who willingly subjects herself to a orful renderings of a human brain can increase the credibility and
commercially available brain stimulation device for cognitive en- perceived quality of scientific information (McCabe & Castel, 2008),
hancement. Why are people so mesmerized by the potential of new while others have argued that this effect might be small or even non-
neuroscience techniques that they are willing to subject themselves to existent (Michael, Newman, Vuorre, Cumming, & Garry, 2013;
extensive neurofeedback programs or even willing to sacrifice their Schweitzer, Baker, & Risko, 2013). Belief in cognitive enhancement can
autonomy for the promise that their brain will ultimately be enhanced? result in an excessive burden for parents to provide the optimal cir-
To answer these questions it is important to better understand why cumstances for their child's brain development (O'Connor & Joffe,
so many people believe in ‘neuromyths’, i.e., incorrect conceptions of 2013) and even in unwarranted advice regarding parenting practices
and beliefs about the human brain (e.g., the widespread belief that (e.g., the Mozart-effect; cf., Bangerter & Heath, 2004). Moreover, the
people use only 10% of their brain capacity, or the myth that the left popular use of brain-based explanations of human behavior increas-
hemisphere is for analytical and the right hemisphere for intuitive ingly raises ethical issues related to individual responsibility (e.g., ‘my
thinking; cf., Dekker, Lee, Howard-Jones, & Jolles, 2012; Furnham, brain made me do it’; Farah, 2002; Roskies, 2006), which becomes even
2018; Lilienfeld, Lynn, Ruscio, & Beyerstein, 2010, 2011; Pasquinelli, more prominent with the introduction of neurostimulation techniques
2012). It has been found for instance that between 50% and 70% of that may provide a perfect ‘excuse’ for the externalization of one's ac-
teachers in primary and secondary school believe in neuromyths tions (Hoogeveen, Schjoedt, & van Elk, 2018).
(Dekker et al., 2012; Düvel, Wolf, & Kopiez, 2017) and teachers apply Despite the wide media coverage of neuroscience findings and the
ideas related to neuroeducation in their teaching (i.e., by tailoring their large number of grants that have been awarded to brain-based projects
teaching style to the modality most suited to the student's preferred (e.g., the Human Brain Project; Markram, 2012), surprisingly little at-
learning style, e.g., the visual or auditory modality). But even tention has been paid to the biases underlying our expectations about


University of Amsterdam, Department of Psychology, Nieuwe Achtergracht 129B, 1018WT Amsterdam, the Netherlands
E-mail address: m.vanelk@uva.nl.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.04.014
Received 23 February 2019; Received in revised form 6 April 2019; Accepted 11 April 2019
Available online 24 April 2019
0191-8869/ © 2019 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
M. van Elk Personality and Individual Differences 147 (2019) 28–32

brain research and its potential applications. Drawing a parallel with Table 1
research on cognitive precursors of supernatural beliefs and pseu- Scale used to assess belief in neuromyths, cognitive enhancement and brain
doscience (Gervais et al., 2017; Maij et al., 2017; Maij, van Schie, facts used in pre-test 3 and in the main study.
Ploeger, & van Elk, 2019; van Elk, 2013, 2015a, 2015b; van Elk & Neuromyths
Aleman, 2017; van Elk, Bekkering, & Friston, 2015; van Elk, Rutjens, 1. Learning problems associated with developmental differences in brain function
van der Pligt, & van Harreveld, 2015; van Leeuwen & van Elk, 2018), cannot be remediated by education.
2. Children are less attentive after consuming sugary drinks and/or snacks.
the present pre-registered study tested the hypothesis that neuroench-
3. Male brains are biologically better suited for math and science, female brains for
antment, i.e., people's beliefs in and expectations about the promises of empathy.
neuroscientific findings and techniques, is based on similar cognitive 4. Children must acquire their native language before a second language is learned. If
biases that play a role in religion and spirituality. The aim of this study they do not do so neither language will be fully acquired.
was thus to determine why some people are more prone to believe in 5. Brain development has finished by the time children reach secondary school.
6. We only use 10% of our brain.
neuromyths and in cognitive enhancement than others.
7. Some people are left-brained (logical) and some are right-brained (creative).
A large-scale survey was conducted in which participants were 8. Mental capacity is hereditary and cannot be changed by the environment or
presented with different questions related to brain myths (e.g., ‘people experience.
use only 10% of their brain capacity’; ‘the left hemisphere is for in- 9. Drinking (moderate) alcohol kills brain cells.
10. To uncover people's true thoughts, brain scans are more suitable than asking
tuitive thinking’) and to cognitive enhancement beliefs (e.g., ‘Cognitive
questions.
abilities can be boosted through brain stimulation techniques’). 11. Playing classical music to infants makes them smarter.
Building on previous research conducted on cognitive biases associated
Cognitive enhancement
with different belief systems (e.g., conspiracy beliefs; belief in alter- 12. Exercises that rehearse coordination of motor-perception skills can improve
native medicine; Maij, van Harreveld, et al., 2017; van Elk, 2013, literacy skills.
2015a, 2015b; van Elk, Karinen, Specker, Stamkou, & Baas, 2016), this 13. Playing commercial video games improves cognitive performance.
study assessed the relation between socio-cognitive biases and neu- 14. You can improve your working memory with yoga exercises.
15. Cognitive performance can be positively affected by using brain stimulation (e.g.,
roenchantment. All hypotheses, the pre-tests, the materials, the sam-
tDCS).
pling and analysis plan were pre-registered prior to the study on the 16. Doing crossword puzzles improves memory.
Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/639uk/; the preregistration
Brain facts
itself can be found at: https://osf.io/f8j9x/register/ 17. Individuals learn better when they receive information in their preferred learning
565fb3678c5e4a66b5582f67). style (e.g., auditory, visual, kinesthetic).
18. The anatomic structure of the brain changes through intensive practice of an
2. Methods instrument.
19. There are sensitive periods in childhood when it's easier to learn things.
20. Production of new connections in the brain can continue into old age.
2.1. Participants 21. We use our brains 24 h a day.
22. When a brain region is damaged other parts of the brain can take up its function.
Based on an expected moderate correlation of r = 0.33, α = 0.05
and a power of β = 0.80 at least 230 participants are required to detect
an effect. In total 400 participants were recruited through Amazon's reliabilities of the different subscales were as follows: neuromyths,
Mechanical Turk platform and participated in a survey assessing their α = 0.73, cognitive enhancement, α = 0.67 and brain facts, α = 0.61.
view and knowledge of the human brain and neuroscience. Participants The sumscore of the neuromyths and cognitive enhancement subscale
provided informed consent for their participation and received a 2$ were calculated and were used as the main dependent variables in the
reward for completing the survey. The study protocol was approved by present study. The rationale for including brain facts in the neuromyths
the local ethics committee (https://www.lab.uva.nl/lab/ethics; project questionnaire was to avoid response bias and to encourage participants
id = 2018-SP-8866). A priori exclusion criteria (as specified on the to think carefully about each statement. The items of the neuromyths
OSF) were exclusion of participants who did not complete the survey or questionnaire were presented in random order.
who completed the survey in < 5 min, resulting in a drop-out of 14
participants. 2.3. Demographic variables and cognitive measures

2.2. Development of the neuromyths & cognitive enhancement questionnaire Background variables that were measured included age, gender,
education, occupation, perceived socio-economic status and media ex-
The questionnaire to assess belief in neuromyths and belief in cog- posure (i.e., reading of popular science and exposure to primary sci-
nitive enhancement was developed and validated by using three in- entific articles). Media exposure was measured by asking participants to
dependent pre-tests that are described in more detail on the OSF indicate how often they read each of the following: (1) Popular scien-
(https://osf.io/639uk/ under ‘Pretest and Validation of Neuromyths tific texts (e.g. blogs, articles, journals, magazines, online platforms)
questionnaire’). In a first pretest 50 participants completed 25 items and (2) Primary scientific literature (e.g. peer reviewed articles) on a
related to neuromyths, cognitive enhancement and brain facts that were continuous scale ranging from 0 (never) to 100 (often).
derived from previous studies (Dekker et al., 2012). An exploratory The Need for Cognitive Closure scale was included, consisting of 15
factor analysis was conducted, indicating that most but not all items items (e.g., ‘I don't like situations that are uncertain.’) that were com-
loaded positive on the expected construct. Next, in a second pretest we pleted on a 6 point scale ranging from 1 = completely disagree to
asked 12 neuroscience experts from the Brain and Cognition pro- 6 = completely agree; Cronbach's alpha = 0.92 (cf., Roets & Van Hiel,
gramme group at the University of Amsterdam to classify each of the 25 2011). Epistemic cognition and participants' view on the simplicity vs.
items as a ‘neuromyth’, as ‘cognitive enhancement’ or as a ‘brain fact’. complexity of neuroscientific knowledge was measured using a mod-
Based on both pre-tests ambiguous items were removed from the ified version of the Discipline-focused Epistemological Beliefs scale
questionnaire and the classification of the remaining 22 items to the (e.g., ‘In the field of neuroscience, most work has only one right an-
different subscales was established (see Table 1). In a third pre-test 100 swer.’; cf., Hofer, 2000). Items were completed on a 7-point scale ran-
participants completed the survey and by conducting a confirmatory ging from 1 = completely disagree to 7 = completely agree, α = 0.82.
factor analysis (CFA) the expected factor structure was validated, as the Intuitive thinking was measured by using the faith in Intuition scale,
CFA indicated a good model fit. Thus for the present study we used the consisting of 12 items (e.g., ‘I trust my initial feelings about people.’)
22-item version of the neuromyths scale as presented in Table 1. The that were completed on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 = completely

29
M. van Elk Personality and Individual Differences 147 (2019) 28–32

false to 5 = completely true, α = 0.91 (Epstein, Pacini, Denes-Raj, & different variables are presented in Table 3.
Heier, 1996). Scientific Literacy was measured using 8 items (e.g., ‘It is
the father's gene that decides whether the baby is a boy or a girl.’), 3.2. Confirmatory analyses
probing participant's general scientific knowledge (Kahan, 2012). Par-
ticipants had to indicate for each statement whether it was true or false 3.2.1. Regression analyses: predicting belief in neuromyths
and the sumscore of the number of correct answers was taken as a When predicting belief in neuromyths, the first model including the
measure of scientific literacy. Finally, endorsement of a growth vs. a demographic variables was only marginally significant, F(4,
fixed mindset was measured using the implicit theory of intelligence 381) = 2.38, p = .051, R2 = 0.024 (see Table 4). When including the
scale, consisting of 14 items (Abd-El-Fattah & Yates, 2006). Seven items different cognitive biases in Step 2, the model was significant, F(9,
were related to belief in a fixed mindset (e.g., ‘You are born with a fixed 376) = 12.96, p < .001, R2 = 0.24. As can be seen in Table 4, a sim-
amount of intelligence’), and 7 items to belief in a growth mindset (e.g., pler epistemological understanding of neuroscience, more intuitive
‘You can develop your intelligence if your really try’). The items were thinking and endorsement of a fixed mindset predicted a stronger belief
combined in a continuous scale, with low values indicating a fixed view in neuromyths. Increased scientific literacy was related to a reduced
of intelligence and high values a fluid view of intelligence. belief in neuromyths.

2.4. Experimental manipulation 3.2.2. Regression analyses: predicting belief in cognitive enhancement
When predicting belief in cognitive enhancement, the first model
We conducted an experimental manipulation to investigate whether including the demographic variables was significant, F(4,
brain pictures increase the credibility of a popular science article and 381) = 6.345, p < .001, R2 = 0.05: media exposure was a significant
make people more prone to intuitive compared to analytical thinking. predictor of belief in cognitive enhancement (see Table 5). When in-
To this end we presented participants with a short story on the neural cluding the different cognitive biases in Step 2, the model was sig-
correlates of the LSD experience (Carhart-Harris et al., 2014): one group nificant, F(9, 376) = 8.36, p < .001, R2 = 0.17. As can be seen in
of participants read the story accompanied by an illustrative and col- Table 5, more intuitive thinking, viewing neuroscientific knowledge as
orful brain picture, while the other group read the story without a simple and endorsement of a growth mindset predicted a stronger belief
picture. in cognitive enhancement.
Following the story participants were asked to what extent they
found the neuroscientific explanation of the LSD experience credible 3.2.3. Experimental manipulation: effect of presence or absence of brain
and we measured intuitive thinking, by using a variant of the cognitive pictures on credibility and analytical thinking
reflection task (CRT-2; Thomson & Oppenheimer, 2016). Participants When including all 386 participants in the analysis we found that
were randomly assigned to one of the two different experimental con- participants in the brain picture condition tended to rate the entropic
ditions. The presentation of the different questionnaires was also ran- brain hypothesis as somewhat more credible (mean = 3.40, SD = 1.00)
domized throughout the study, but the experimental manipulation and than participants in the control condition (mean = 3.20, SD = 1.00),
the subsequent dependent measures were presented at the end of the although this difference was non-significant, t(384) = −1.94, p = .053,
survey. d = −0.197. Participants in the brain picture condition also tended to
give more intuitive answers on the CRT (mean = 3.00, SD = 1.48)
2.5. Analysis compared to the control condition (mean = 3.29, SD = 1.34), t
(354) = 1.93, p = .054. When using a Bayesian analysis for the cred-
Two separate regression analyses were conducted with the belief in ibility ratings, we found a BF10 of 0.684, indicating that the data was
neuromyths and the cognitive enhancement scales as dependent vari- almost two times as likely under H0 compared to H1. Similarly, for the
able. To analyze the effect of the experimental manipulation on the CRT, we found a BF10 of 0.70, indicating that the data was more likely
credibility rating and intuitive vs. analytical thinking a MANOVA was under H0 compared to H1.
conducted with group as between-subjects factor, and the credibility When including faith in intuition as a covariate in the analysis, we
rating and the summed CRT score as dependent variables. did not find the predicted interaction between condition and intuitive
thinking on the credibility ratings, (F1, 382) = 0.49, p = .49.
3. Results Participants scoring high on intuitive thinking tended to give higher
credibility ratings to the story overall – irrespective of condition, F(1,
3.1. Descriptives 382) = 7.09, p = .008, η2 = 0.02. Similarly, for the overall CRT score
the interaction between condition and intuitive thinking was not sig-
An overview of the different variables, including demographics and nificant, F(1, 352) = 0.17, p = .68. As one might expect, participants
the scales is presented in Table 2. The correlations between the scoring high on intuitive thinking had lower scores (i.e., more intuitive
responses) on the CRT task, F(1, 352) = 6.41, p = .012, η2 = 0.02.
Table 2
Descriptives for the different variables that were included in the study. 3.3. Exploratory analyses

Mean SD Range
Participants who read the page representing the entropic brain ex-
Age 37.27 11.77 18–70 planation in < 40 s were excluded in an exploratory analysis, as this
Education 4.70 1.13 2–7 interval was considered too short to read the complete text. The text
SES 4.50 1.94 0–10 consisted of 365 words, resulting in an estimated reading time of about
Media exposure 38.12 24.48 0–100
1.5 min. Following this exclusion criterion, 144 participants were in-
Belief in neuromyths 41.59 14.29 0.73–96.91
Cognitive enhancement 61.43 14.15 1.60–100.00 cluded in the condition without a brain picture and 132 participants in
Brain facts 71.45 12.99 18.00–100.00 the condition with a brain picture. The credibility ratings were higher
Need for cognition 3.92 0.89 1.00–5.80 for participants who were presented with a brain picture (mean = 3.42,
Epistemological understanding 3.50 0.74 1.36–6.07 SD = 1.00) compared to the group without a brain picture
Faith in intuition 3.49 0.74 1–5
(mean = 3.17, SD = 0.97), t(274) = −2.12, p = .035, d = −0.26.
Growth vs. fixed mindset 0.82 0.72 −1.14–3.00
Scientific literacy 5.91 1.18 3.00–7.00 There was no effect of the experimental manipulation on CRT scores
(p = .453).

30
M. van Elk Personality and Individual Differences 147 (2019) 28–32

Table 3
Correlations between the different variables, related to belief in neuromyths and cognitive enhancement and the socio-cognitive biases.
Pearson correlations

Neuromyths CE ME NFC EU FI GvFM

⁎⁎⁎
Belief in cognitive enhancement 0.187 –
⁎ ⁎⁎⁎
Media exposure 0.128 0.227 –
⁎⁎⁎
Need for cognitive closure 0.176 −0.06 −0.096 –
⁎⁎⁎ ⁎⁎⁎ ⁎⁎⁎
Epistemological understanding 0.299 0.098 0.168 0.269 –
⁎⁎⁎ ⁎⁎⁎ ⁎⁎⁎ ⁎⁎
Faith in intuition 0.279 0.245 0.068 0.211 0.164 –
⁎⁎⁎ ⁎⁎⁎ ⁎ ⁎⁎⁎ ⁎⁎⁎ ⁎⁎
Growth vs. fixed mindset −0.23 0.258 0.106 −0.171 −0.23 0.136 –
⁎⁎⁎ ⁎ ⁎⁎ ⁎⁎ ⁎⁎⁎
Scientific literacy −0.258 −0.106 −0.139 −0.094 −0.141 −0.201 0.013


p < .05.
⁎⁎
p < .01.
⁎⁎⁎
p < .001.

Table 4 stronger belief in paranormal phenomena (Aarnio & Lindeman, 2005;


Regression analysis predicting belief in neuromyths, including demographic Lindeman & Svedholm-Häkkinen, 2016), conspiracy beliefs (Brotherton
variables in Step 1 and the different cognitive biases in Step 2. & French, 2014), skepticism of climate change, vaccination, and genetic
B SE β p-Value modification (Rutjens, Sutton, & van der Lee, 2017), endorsement of
pseudoscience (Majima, 2015) and even acceptance of pseudo-pro-
Step 1 40.36 4.74 0.01 found ‘bullshit’ (Pennycook, Cheyne, Barr, Koehler, & Fugelsang, 2015).
Gender 1.46 1.47 0.05 .32
Belief in a variety of different phenomena, such as conspiracy beliefs or
Age 0.01 0.06 0.01 .85
Education −0.95 0.65 −0.08 .14 paranormal beliefs, are also strongly interrelated (Lobato, Mendoza,
Media exposure 0.04 8.96 0.14 .01 Sims, & Chin, 2014). As such the observed relationships may reflect a
Step 2 18.92 more general propensity or gullibility to accept epistemically un-
Gender 2.51 1.34 0.09 .06 warranted beliefs (Pennycook, Fugelsang, & Koehler, 2015). Our
Age 0.09 0.06 0.07 .13
finding that intuitive thinking, scientific literacy and need for cognitive
Education −0.08 0.59 −0.01 .89
Media exposure 0.06 0.03 0.10 .04 closure predict belief in neuromyths likely reflects a general proclivity
Need for cognitive closure 0.78 0.79 0.05 .32 to answer in a confirmatory fashion to profound-looking pseudo-sci-
Epistemological understand 3.22 0.97 0.17 .001 entific statements. We note however, that we observed differential ef-
Faith in intuition 4.63 0.93 0.24 .000
fects for belief in neuromyths and belief in cognitive enhancement,
Mindset −4.16 0.95 −0.21 .000
Scientific literacy −2.23 0.58 −0.18 .000 indicating that we measured related – though distinct – constructs
(which was also confirmed by the CFA of our scale).
Our findings also extend previous results, by highlighting the role of
Table 5 domain-specific epistemic cognition and people's views on intelligence.
Regression analysis predicting belief in cognitive enhancement, including de- Some people tend to see neuroscientific knowledge as simple – as un-
mographic variables in Step 1 and the different cognitive biases in Step 2. changing facts that are handed down by authority. In contrast, people
B SE β p-Value
with more complex levels of epistemic understanding are aware of the
fact that conflicting opinions may exist on certain issues, although they
Step 1 64.12 4.60 < 0.001 acknowledge that empirical evidence and the use of the scientific
Gender −2.58 1.43 −0.09 .07 method may ultimately lead to more legitimate positions (Kuhn,
Age −0.02 0.06 −0.01 .79
Education −0.70 0.63 −0.06 .79
Cheney, & Weinstock, 2000). We found that viewing neuroscientific
Media exposure 0.13 0.03 0.23 .000 knowledge as simple is associated with a stronger endorsement of
Step 2 44.59 8.05 0.000 neuromyths and belief in cognitive enhancement. People who are
Gender −1.66 1.38 −0.06 .23 convinced that neuroscience provides straightforward solutions and
Age 0.04 0.06 0.03 .51
answers are more likely to subscribe to popular scientific conceptions of
Education −0.19 0.61 −0.02 .75
Media exposure 0.09 0.03 0.16 .001 the brain. They are also more willing to believe in the potential to
Need for cognitive closure −1.46 0.82 −0.09 .08 unleash the hidden powers of the mind through brain stimulation
Epistemological understand 2.16 1.00 0.113 .03 techniques.
Faith in intuition 3.67 0.97 0.19 .000 In addition we found that one's view of intelligence as being fixed or
Mindset 4.29 0.98 0.22 .000
Scientific literacy −0.50 0.60 −0.04 .41
fluid (Dweck, 2006) was associated with respectively belief in neuro-
myths and with belief in neuroenhancement. People who viewed in-
telligence and human performance in general as fixed tended to believe
4. Discussion less in the potential of cognitive enhancement – though they endorsed
neuromyths to a greater extent. The view that intelligence is fixed from
The data from our study confirmed most of the pre-registered hy- one's birth on fits well with the contents of neuromyths that also imply
potheses (https://osf.io/639uk/ and: https://osf.io/f8j9x/register/ a deterministic view on human cognitive performance (e.g., ‘Learning
565fb3678c5e4a66b5582f67). Belief in neuromyths was predicted by problems associated with developmental differences in brain function
a high need for cognitive closure, a simple (vs. a complex) view of cannot be remediated by education.’). In contrast, endorsement of a
scientific knowledge, an intuitive (compared to an analytical) thinking growth mindset, i.e., the view that intelligence is a source that can be
style, increased scientific literacy and a fixed mindset. Belief in cogni- cultivated, was positively related to belief in cognitive enhancement,
tive enhancement was predicted by a simple view of neuroscientific while it was negatively related to belief in neuromyths. This illustrates
knowledge, intuitive thinking and a growth mindset. Previous studies that belief in the potential of neurotechnologies, strongly hinges on
have demonstrated a relationship between these cognitive biases and a one's view that the human mind can be ‘hacked’ and improved. The
observation of a differential effect of mindset for belief in neuromyths

31
M. van Elk Personality and Individual Differences 147 (2019) 28–32

and in cognitive enhancement, furthermore demonstrates that our C. Lüthy, & E. van Wolde (Eds.). The challenge of chance. Springer.
cognitive enhancement items capture a specific proclivity for belief in van Elk, M., Karinen, A., Specker, E., Stamkou, E., & Baas, M. (2016). Standing in awe’:
The effects of awe on body perception and the relation with absorption. Collabra:
human enhancement – rather than a general gullibility to buy into Psychology, 2(1).
popular science explanations. van Elk, M., Rutjens, B. T., van der Pligt, J., & van Harreveld, F. (2015). Priming of
We failed to replicate previously reported effects regarding the supernatural agent concepts and agency detection. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 6(1),
4–33.
presence of brain pictures on the perceived credibility of a neuroscience Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual differences in in-
explanation (Weisberg, Keil, Goodstein, Rawson, & Gray, 2008). By tuitive–experiential and analytical–rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and
using a Bayesian analysis found no evidence for an effect of brain pic- Social Psychology, 71(2), 390–405.
Farah, M. J. (2002). Emerging ethical issues in neuroscience. Nature Neuroscience, 5(11),
tures on credibility ratings. We also could find no evidence for the 1123–1129.
hypothesis that an intuitive thinking style moderated the relationship Furnham, A. (2018). Myths and misconceptions in developmental and neuro-psychology.
between the effects of brain pictures on credibility ratings. Thus, a Psychology, 9(2), 249–259.
Gervais, W., Xygalatas, D., McKay, R. T., van Elk, M., Buchtel, E. E., Aveyard, M., ...
higher-powered study is needed to provide more conclusive evidence
Bulbulia, J. (2017). Global evidence of extreme intuitive moral prejudice against
regarding the presence or absence of an effect of brain pictures on atheists. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(4), 543–556.
perceived credibility (based on the observed effect sizes of d = 0.2, at Hofer, B. K. (2000). Dimensionality and disciplinary differences in personal epistemology.
least 1082 participants would need to be included). Future studies Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(4), 378–405.
Hoogeveen, S., Schjoedt, U., & van Elk, M. (2018). Did I do that? Expectancy effects of
should also consider including additional conditions (e.g., a black-white brain stimulation on error-related negativity and sense of agency. Journal of Cognitive
instead of a colored rending of the brain; a colorful picture of a different Neuroscience, 1–14.
object) to control for other factors that could potentially contribute to Kahan, D. (2012). Why we are poles apart on climate change: The problem isn't the
public's reasoning capacity; it's the polluted science-communication environment that
an effect on the perceived credibility of messages. drives people apart. Nature, 488(7411), 255–256.
Kuhn, D., Cheney, R., & Weinstock, M. (2000). The development of epistemological un-
5. Conclusions derstanding. Cognitive Development, 15(3), 309–328. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-
2014(00)00030-7.
van Leeuwen, N., & van Elk, M. (2018). Seeking the supernatural: The interactive re-
Thinking style and different socio-cognitive biases selectively pre- ligious experience model. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 1–31.
dicted belief in neuromyths and in cognitive enhancement. Changing Lilienfeld, S. O., Lynn, S. J., Ruscio, J., & Beyerstein, B. L. (2010). 50 great myths of popular
psychology. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.
people's beliefs in the potential promises and pitfalls of neuroscience Lilienfeld, S. O., Lynn, S. J., Ruscio, J., & Beyerstein, B. L. (2011). 50 great myths of popular
thus requires paying careful attention to people's thinking styles. At the psychology: Shattering widespread misconceptions about human behavior. Hoboken, NJ:
same time, belief in cognitive enhancement (and a growth mindset) John Wiley & Sons.
Lindeman, M., & Svedholm-Häkkinen, A. M. (2016). Does poor understanding of physical
may hold great promise to exert beneficial effects in different neuro-
world predict religious and paranormal beliefs? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 30(5),
cognitive treatments and expectancy manipulations (Hoogeveen et al., 736–742.
2018). Lobato, E., Mendoza, J., Sims, V., & Chin, M. (2014). Examining the relationship between
conspiracy theories, paranormal beliefs, and pseudoscience acceptance among a
university population. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(5), 617–625.
Acknowledgments Macdonald, K., Germine, L., Anderson, A., Christodoulou, J., & McGrath, L. M. (2017).
Dispelling the myth: Training in education or neuroscience decreases but does not
I would like to thank Jasmijn Kruissink, Laura Vermande and eliminate beliefs in neuromyths. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(1314).
Maij, D. L. R., van Harreveld, F., Gervais, W., Schrag, Y., Mohr, C., & van Elk, M. (2017).
Marissa Woudstra for their help with setting up the study, their creative Mentalizing skills do not differentiate believers from non-believers, but credibility
insight and ideas and their help with validating the neuromyths ques- enhancing displays do. PLoS One, 12(8), e0182764.
tionnaire. This work was made possible by a grant from the BIAL Maij, D. L., van Schie, H. T., & van Elk, M. (2019). The boundary conditions of the hy-
persensitive agency detection device: An empirical investigation of agency detection
Foundation (grant # 076/2016). in threatening situations. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 9(1), 23–51.
Majima, Y. (2015). Belief in pseudoscience, cognitive style and science literacy. Applied
References Cognitive Psychology, 29(4), 552–559.
Markram, H. (2012). The human brain project. Scientific American, 306(6), 50–55.
McCabe, D. P., & Castel, A. D. (2008). Seeing is believing: The effect of brain images on
Aarnio, K., & Lindeman, M. (2005). Paranormal beliefs, education, and thinking styles. judgments of scientific reasoning. Cognition, 107(1), 343–352. https://doi.org/10.
Personality and Individual Differences, 39(7), 1227–1236. 1016/j.cognition.2007.07.017.
Abd-El-Fattah, S. M., & Yates, G. C. R. (2006). Implicit theory of intelligence scale: Testing for Michael, R. B., Newman, E. J., Vuorre, M., Cumming, G., & Garry, M. (2013). On the
factorial invariance and mean structure. Paper presented at the Australian Association for (non)persuasive power of a brain image. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(4),
Research in Education Conference. 720–725. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0391-6.
Bangerter, A., & Heath, C. (2004). The Mozart effect: Tracking the evolution of a scientific O'Connor, C., & Joffe, H. (2013). How has neuroscience affected lay understandings of
legend. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 43(4), 605–623. personhood? A review of the evidence. Public Understanding of Science, 22(3),
Brotherton, R., & French, C. C. (2014). Belief in conspiracy theories and susceptibility to 254–268.
the conjunction fallacy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(2), 238–248. Pasquinelli, E. (2012). Neuromyths: Why do they exist and persist? Mind, Brain, and
Carhart-Harris, R. L., Leech, R., Hellyer, P. J., Shanahan, M., Feilding, A., Tagliazucchi, E., Education, 6(2), 89–96.
& Nutt, D. (2014). The entropic brain: A theory of conscious states informed by Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Barr, N., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2015). On the
neuroimaging research with psychedelic drugs. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, reception and detection of pseudo-profound bullshit. Judgment and Decision making,
8(20). 10(6), 549–563.
Dekker, S., Lee, N. C., Howard-Jones, P., & Jolles, J. (2012). Neuromyths in education: Pennycook, G., Fugelsang, J. A., & Koehler, D. J. (2015). Everyday consequences of
Prevalence and predictors of misconceptions among teachers. Frontiers in Psychology, analytic thinking. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(6), 425–432.
3, 249. Roets, A., & Van Hiel, A. (2011). Item selection and validation of a brief, 15-item version
Düvel, N., Wolf, A., & Kopiez, R. (2017). Neuromyths in music education: Prevalence and of the Need for Closure Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(1), 90–94.
predictors of misconceptions among teachers and students. Frontiers in Psychology, Roskies, A. (2006). Neuroscientific challenges to free will and responsibility. Trends in
8(629). Cognitive Sciences, 10(9), 419–423.
Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. Random House Incorporated. Rutjens, B. T., Sutton, R. M., & van der Lee, R. (2017). Not all skepticism is equal:
van Elk, M. (2013). Paranormal believers are more prone to illusory agency detection Exploring the ideological antecedents of science acceptance and rejection. Personality
than skeptics. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(3), 1041–1046. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44(3), 384–405.
van Elk, M. (2015a). Perceptual biases in relation to paranormal and conspiracy beliefs. Schweitzer, N. J., Baker, D. A., & Risko, E. F. (2013). Fooled by the brain: Re-examining
PLoS One, 10(6), e0130422. the influence of neuroimages. Cognition, 129(3), 501–511. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
van Elk, M. (2015b). The self-attribution bias and paranormal beliefs. Consciousness and cognition.2013.08.009.
Cognition, 49, 313–321. Thomson, K. S., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2016). Investigating an alternate form of the
van Elk, M., & Aleman, A. (2017). Brain mechanisms in religion and spirituality: An in- cognitive reflection test. Judgment and Decision making, 11(1), 99–113.
tegrative predictive processing framework. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, Weisberg, D. S., Keil, F. C., Goodstein, J., Rawson, E., & Gray, J. R. (2008). The seductive
73, 359–378. allure of neuroscience explanations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(3),
van Elk, M., Bekkering, H., & Friston, K. (2015). The experience of coincidence: An in- 470–477.
tegrated psychological and neurocognitive perspective. In N. ter Berg, K. Landsman,

32

You might also like