You are on page 1of 15

Chapter 14

A Behavioral Perspective on Adult


Attachment Style, Intimacy, and
Relationship Health

Abigail K. Mansfield &:James V. Cordova


Clark University

Couples come to therapy in distress and seeking help for a variety ofproblems
in their relationships. Most ofthe problems couples encounter cannot be classified
in tenns ofDSM IV disorders. Instead, the symptoms of marital ill-health include
chronic relationship distress, thoughts of divorce, frequent bitter arguing or
withdrawal, and a sense of being either chronically at war with each other or
completely emotionally numb. From the perspective ofadult attachment theory,
these problems can result from the complex interplay between individual partners'
attachment histories and bids for nurturance and closeness within the current
relationship that have gone awry.
Given that the purpose of the current volume is to explore contemporary
behavioral perspectives on behavior disorders, this chapter will focus on disorders
ofattachment and intimacy in couple relationships. The goal of this chapter is to
explore the potential contributions to attachment theory that might be derived from
application of contemporary behavioral theory and research. Although early
attachment researchers clearly understood and identified the key role played by a
mother's responsiveness to a child's signals in the development of different
attachment styles, we argue that these researchers did not take full advantage of
behavioral theory and research to aid their understanding of attachment as a
developmental phenomenon. Drawing principallyon evolutionary-ethological and
psychodynamic theories, and more recently on cognitive and cognitive-behavioral
theories, attachment researchers have missed a potentially rich source ofinforma-
tion about the shaping of behavior provided by decades of basic operant research.
A behavior analytic (BA) perspective on attachment focuses on the develop-
ment ofthe person in the context ofherleaming history and the lawful development
of attachment repertoires shaped by that learning history. In contrast, traditional
attachment theory, while acknowledging individual history, focuses on internal
mechanisms as causal agents (i.e., cognitive maps and working models). We will not
wage the standard partisan battle here between contextualists and mechanists,
because in the case ofthe attachment literature, that debate is unlikely to be fruitful.
Rather than oppose traditional theory, we would like t.o suggest that because ofBA's
infants in Uganda and the United States. Through these observations, particularly
solid foundation in empirically observed behavioral phenomena, it can function to
those conducted in the United States, Ainsworth developed the laboratory based
provide a much more active dialogue with the data provided by Harlow, Bowlby,
"strange situation" procedure, which she used to characterize styles of attachment
Ainsworth, Main, and other attachment scientists. The potential outcome ofsuch
relationships. In the "strange situation," researchers observe the caregiver and child
a dialogue is a fresh perspective on attachment and new testable hypotheses.
in several different scenarios: with the caregiver and the child alone in an
In this chapter, we will explore how a behavioral perspective can foster a rich
observation room together, next with a stranger in the room with them, then with
understanding of adult attachment styles and how such styles can cOnlribule to
the baby alone with the stranger and then briefly reunited with the caregiver, and
distress between partners. We review the literature on attachment relationships and
finally, with the child alone in the room. After each shift, researchers noted how the
then spell out how a behavioral perspective can provide a compelling framework for
infant responded and whether he or she was distressed or apathetic. They also noted
understanding how attachment styles develop as well as how they influence adult
relationships. We then apply a behavioral perspective to understand how patterns how easily the infant was soothed by his or her caregiver.
Ainsworth and colleagues (1978) used their extensive observations of infant-
of vulnerability and intimacy in couples emerge and how some such patterns
mother interactions, both in naturalistic settings and in the "strange situation," to
become problematic for couples. Finally, We will offeran account ofhow Integrative
develop a three-tiered classification system ofattachment styles. They termed the
Behavioral Couples Therapy (IBCT) can be used to help couples move beyond the
difficulties they experience. three types of attachment relationship A, B, and C styles, each of which will be
described in tum.
Origins and Background of Attachment Theory Style A, sometimes referred to as an avoidant attachment style, is characterized
John Bowlby laid the foundation ofattachment theory with his three volume by infants who show practically no response to being alone in the "strange situation"
tome, Attad!mmt and Loss (1969, 1973, 1980). Influenced by the evolutionary (Ainsworth et al., 1978). When the caregiver leaves the room, the infant does not
biologist Konrad lorenz and by Harlow's work with rhesus macaque monkeys (e.g., appear distressed, and when the caregiver returns, the infant does not greet or
Harlow, 1958), Bowlby rejected the idea that attachment was a product of drive approach him or her. Similarly, when the stranger enters, infants with avoidant
reduction. More specifically, he rejected the idea that attachment between mother attachment styles either ignore him or her or are more friendly toward the stranger
and child developed because mothers meet their infants' need (drive) for food. than toward the caregiver. Furthermore,Ainsworth et aI. noted that infant avoidance
Harlow (1958) was instrumental in refuting the: drive-reduction theory of aUacll- is linked to caregiver behavior: Infants who display avoidant behavior tend to have
memo In experiments with macaque monkeys, he found that monkeys spent more caregivers who either directly reject attachment behavior (i.e., ignore bids for
time with a warm, soft dummy-mother that did not provide food than with a wire physical closeness, ignore infants' greetings, etc.) or report resentment at having to
cage dummy-mother that provided food. This finding validated Bowlby's sense that care for the infant.
attachment relationships were more complex than drive-reduction theory allowed. Attachment style B, often termed the "secure" attachment style, is characterized
With this information in tow, BOWlby proceeded to develop the argument that by infants who show distress at being separated from their caregivers, who greet or
attachment relationships have survival value because they keep infants close to approach caretakers after their return from separation, and who are easily comforted
caregivers who can provide protection in the face of danger (Bowlby, 1969, 1973, by caregivers when distressed by the separation (Ainsworth et aI., 1978). As with the
1980,1988). Thus, he came close to offering a functional analytic explanation of avoidant attachment style, Ainsworth et al. noted that caregiver behavior is linked
attachmentin which the function ofattachment eliciting behavior (e.g., crying, bids to the secure attachment style. Indeed, caregivers of securely attached infants are
for physical closeness) is ultimately the protection ofinfants. Had he embraced this likely to respond quickly and appropriately to their infant's needs and to be
position more fully, the field might not have taken the psychoanalytic tum that now comforting to the infant when the infant is in distress.
characterizes it. However, Bowlby had strong philosophical commitments to the Finally, attachment style C, also known as the resistant/ambivalent attachment
psychoanalytic paradigm and eschewed the idea that attachment theory was a style, is characterized by infants who appear to both seek and resist contact with the
version of behaviorism (Bowlby, 1988). As a result, the operant underpinnings of caregiver after reunions in the "strange situation." In addition, style C infants are
attachment theory were minimized and attachment became a subfield within often uncomfortable and distressed in the "strange situation" from the beginning,
psychodynamic theory. Within this subfield, many researchers have made valuable even before any separation from the caretaker has occurred (Ainsworth et aL, 1978).
contributions. When the caregiver leaves the room, the "resistant/ambivalent" infant tends to
Mary Ainsworth and her colleagues (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978) become very distressed, and when the caregiver attempts to soothe the infant, the
expanded on Bowlby's work through intensive observational studies ofmothers and infant usually continues to cry or fuss. Interestingly, Ainsworth et al. noted that this
style ofattachment is not related to maternal rejection, but rather to inconsistency
a~d insensitivity in the caregiver's ability to meet and respond to his or her infiml'H
for developing dissociative disorders (Main, 2000). In addition, she found that
infants exhibiting a disorganized attachment style tended to have either witnessed
bids for closeness and comfort. an event in which the caregiver was frightened due to a traumatic event or to have
. ~ary !"1ain and her colleagues (1985) posited that the attachment behavioCli been traumatized directly by the caregiver. Main used this information to expound
exhibited 10 the "strange situation" procedure become internalized as "workin", on Bowlby's theory that attachment has survival value. She posed the question:
models ofattachment." According to Main's theory, these representations ofwhnt What happens when the person an infant comes to in distress becomes "a source
a perso~ expects from rel~tionships are unconscious but modulate a person's of alarm?" (Hesse & Main, 2000). As with Bowlby's evolutionary theory of
expectations about the feelings, thoughts, and experiences that surround interper- attachment, there are hints ofan operant understanding ofattachment relationships
sonal relationships (Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985). While Main and other in Main's work, a point to which we shall return later in this chapter.
attachment theorists (e.g., Ainsworth et aI., 1978) concede thatattachmentstyles eRn Bartholomew and Horowitz (1991) contested the idea that attachment styles
~h~ge wh,en, forexamp,'e, severe ec~nomic strain oremotional difficulty drastically are categorical. They suggested that attachment is the product of an individual's
Itmll the time and qualtty of attentIon a caregiver can provide, they contend thnt positioning on two continua: the degree to which a person feels positively about
attachment styles tend to be stable over time. oneselfand the degree to which a person feels positively about other people. Feeling
. To examine the stability of attachment styles, Main et al. (1985) examined positive about oneself is much the same as the construct of self-esteem. Feeling
children at ages one an~ six and admi~isteredthe Adult Attachment Interview (AAlj positive about others refers to the degree to which an individual trusts or feels
Georg;,.Kaplan, & Ma.m, 1985) to their caregivers. The AAI was designed to measure confident in the ability ofothers to support, nurture, care for, or otherwise be present
adults 10ternal w~rking models of attachment and classifies adults into three in relationships. Bartholomew and Horowitz proposed four. distinct styles of
attachment categones: secure-autonomous, dismissing, and preoccupied (George ct attachment corresponding to the four quadrants formed by their two orthogonal
aI., 1985). Secure-a~tonomous adul.ts t~nd to have securely attached children. They continua. The first, referred to as secure attachment, results from a positive sense of
~e able to speak art1cul~tely and obJ.ectlvely about their attachment figures. Equally self and a positive sense of others in a relationship. The second style, called
Important, whether thetr own caregivers were consistent or appropriate is irrelevant preoccupied, results from a negative sense ofselfand a positive sense ofothers and
to whether an ,ad~lt is dassi~ed as secure-autonomous; it is the ability to speak is similar to the AAI's preoccupied style. People with this style tend to devote a great
clearly and obJectively, and 10 a way that values the importance of attachment deal ofenergy to thinking about their relationships. They tend to seek affirmation
~g.ure~, ~ha~ matters. Accor~ing to G~orge and colleagues, adults classified a~ and acceptance from others and to vest their hopes for security and safety in attaining
. dlsmls~mg tend to have children claSSIfied as avoidant. Their narratives concern- these things. The dismissing attachment style, which results from a positive view of
109 their own attachment experiences tend to minimize the importance of oneselfand a negative view ofothers in relationship, is similar to Main's dismissing
atta~~mentfigures and to ignore negative events. Such narratives often have a very style. People who exemplify this style of attachment tend to avoid closeness and
pos~~ve tone, bu.t whe~ p~obed, interviewees cannot explain what exactly was intimacy. In addition, they tend not to value dose relationships with others. Finally,
POSitive a~out theuupbnngmg or attachment relationships. Over the course oftheir Bartholomew and Horowitz suggested a fourth style, much like Main's (2000)
wor~, MalO and colleagues (1985) argued that the attachment behaviorof12-month- disorganized style. They termed this fourth attachment style '"fearful," and theorized
old mfa~ts becomes codified into an ·internal working model" of relationships that it results from a negative model of both selfand others in relationships. They
accountmg for the large association between the attachment styles ofchildren at Ii asserted that people with fearful attachment styles are highly dependent on
months and 6 years. ~hey. also found that adult working models ofattachment are affirmation and acceptance from others but that they avoid intimacy in order to
closely cela~~d to then children's working models of attachment. avoid pain. Thus, they are in the difficult position ofvaluing and wanting closeness
In addition to her work on '"internal working models" of relationships Main as well as avoiding it so as to protect themselves from rejection and pain.
~a~e another major contribution to attachment theory: She explicated a 'fourth Hazan and Shaver (1987) linked adult romantic relationships with attachment
dlstmct style ofattachment. First recognized by Ainsworth and Eichberg in 1991 processes. Following Bowlby, they described romantic love as a biological process
~h~ fourt~ styl~ of atta~hment v.:as known as style D, which Main referred to a~ which functions to keep prospective and actual parents connected so that they will
dls~rganlze~. Acc~rd1Og to ~al~ (2000~, disorganized infants tend to respond to provide reliable nurturance and protection for their children. Hazan and Shaver
reumons dunng the ~trange situatIon" WIth contradictory and sometimes difficult found that insecurely attached adults reported more negative thoughts and
to u.nderstand b.eh~vl0rs, such as staring blankly, closing the eyes, engaging in experiences of love than their securely attached counterparts.
~ndlrected or mlsd1Cecte~ movements, freezing, or appearing apprehensive at the
Sight of. the pare~t. MalO ~~und that disorganized infants are most at risk for
developmg lapses m the ablltty to reason, becoming aggressive or disruptive, and
nurturance (broadly defined). From our perspective. Bowlby correctly asserted that
A Behavioral Conceptualization of Adult Attachment Relationsbips the history of the species has shaped an inclination for infants (and adults) to seek
Like Hazan and Shaver (1987), we contend that couple relationships orc attachment and for caregivers (including other adults) to respond by providing
contexts for attachment processes and take this as a foundation for developing our protection and nurturance. In addition, a BA perspective recognizes that the exact
own behavioral conceptualization of attachment and its function in couple topography of the attachment repertoire is shaped by each individual's unique
relationships. We believe that a behavioral perspective offers a compelling- reinforcement history. It is this individual-level reinforcement history that we are
theoretical base from which to conceptualize the existing empirical evidence and most interested in for the current analysis.
generate new, unique, and testable hypotheses. We have great respect for the
scientists who have done, and continue to do. pioneering work in attachment. and Attachment Transactions
we do not wish to challenge the quality of their work. Instead. it is our position that When we observe attachment transactions as they unfold in the natural
a behavioral perspective on attachment contributes to the field by highlighting the environment or in the strange situation. what do attachment-seeking behaviors look
basic behavioral processes through which attachment processes develop and like? Principally. they involve approaching or calling to the caretaker or partner for
proceed through time. In addition, we believe that a behavioral perspective 011 nurturance. We will equate attachment behaviorwith nurturance-seeking and define
attachment connects seemingly incompatible claims about attachment and allows the class of behavior as comprised of a range of behaviors including bids for
for an enriched understanding of how attachment styles play out in romantic closeness, companionship. comfort. protection. caretaking, reassurance. and suste-
relationships. Finally. a behavioral perspective posits the potential existence of nance. Each of these emerges from a particular state of deprivation or threat but
additional, and as yet unidentified, attachment styles. generally represents the inclination to seek protection and resource-sharing from
As we discussed earlier. we believe that Bowlby's work on attachment contained others assumed to have evolved over the history ofthe species. In short. we are born
underpinnings of an operant understanding of attachment. Bowlby described inclined to seek out and attach ourselves to those who protect and nurture us.
attachment behavior as an operant that functions to bring caregivers to the infant Bowlby. from his ethological perspective, focused more narrowly on the protection-
and thus protect the infant from danger. This view is compatible with a behavioral seeking function ofthe attachment-seeking repertoire. because evolutionary theory
perspective, and we will develop it further by exploring how attachment behavior stresses the survival ofthe individual in the service ofreproductive success. We have
can be differentially reinforced to yield distinct attachment styles. We contend that broadened the emphasis somewhat to take into account the range ofreinforcers that
each episode ofattachment behavior begins with the same operant class: a bid for are most likely associated with the overarching protection goal. Just as feelings of
nurturance. closeness. or protection. In order to explore this idea. we will investigate hunger and the experience offlavor are associated with the overarching goal ofeating
how different consequences and various reinforcement schedules allow for the to survive. feelings ofcloseness. comfort, reassurance, and desire for caretaking are
development of different attachment styles. It is important to note that although associated with the overarching goal of acquiring protection.
attachment pattems vary in a more continuous rather than categorical way. we are Given nurturance-seelcing. the key to exploring attachment transactions and the
most interested in clearly identifiable patterns ofbehavior. We therefore acknowl- resulting attachment repertoires from a behavior analytic perspective lies in
edge but do not get sidetracked by the fact that attachment behavior varies examining the variety ofways in which those bids fornurturance can be consequated
continuously and contextually. or completed between partners. To put it simply, bids for nurturance can be (a)
reinforced. (b) punished, or (c) ignored, and the different ratios ofthose three types
Extending Basic Principles of Behavior to Attachment Theory of consequences. including variability in schedules of reinforcement. should
The first step in extending basic principles ofbehavior to attachment theory is determine the attachment repertoire that the person develops in relation to actual
to identify the operant in question. In other words. what is the specific class of and potential attachment figures. Put another way, the manner in which an
behavior involved in attachment events? Attachment often refers to "internal attachment figure tends to respond to bids for nurturance can be represented as a
working models," presumably derived from histories of interactions with primary three-part ratio consisting of the probability that nurturance-seeking will be
caretakers. Bowlby (1969. 1973, 1980) referred to those behaviors that functioned reinforced. punished. or ignored. Each bid fornurturance is a reach into aprobability
to provide proximity between the infant and the caregiver. thus providing protection bagcontaining those three possible responses, with the expected odds ofpulling any
and. presumably. nurturance such that the infant was more likely to survive to pass particular response depending entirelyon that dyad's previous history ofattachment
on his or her own genes. Although originally discussed as a "behavioral system" transactions. The behavior that is shaped by that history constitutes the person's
(consisting ofthe innate attachment-relevant behaviors ofboth infant and mother) attachment repertoire. Depending on the historical mix of these three types of
based on Bowlby's ethological perspective. we wiIl frame attachment behavior responses. the person will develop a repertoire in relation to others (potential
specifically as a behavioral class shaped by its operant function of obtaining
attachment figures) that should resemble previously described attachment catego-
ries, while at the same time highlighting the ·spaces between" those categories. In easily. In fact, results from some ofMain's early work (as cited in Ainsworth et aI.,
the following sections, we will describe the various probability ratios formed by the 1978) suggest that more secure babies are more positively sociable with relatively
three possible consequences of nurturance bids and the repertoires most likely to unfamiliar adults than babies categorized as more insecure. Additionally, the
result from those ratios. literature on adult attachment has consistently reported a robust association
Rll/ioshighestin rtinjor«ment.lnitially, it should be noted that nurturance-seeking between secure attachment style and capacity to form close intimate relationships.
as a category of behavior will vary in strength depending on conditions of Although it is unlikely that any person has ever been raised in circumstances
deprivation and threat. In other words, people are not always strongly seeking that provided reinforcement for nurturance-seeking on a strictly continuous
nurturance from others. A person's need for nurturance will vary with the reinforcement schedule, schedules rich in reinforcement are apparently the most
circumstances. A person is more likely to seek nurturance after a period of probable. It is likely that all schedules of reinforcement for nurturance-seeking are
deprivation, separation, or threat than during times when that "need" has been essentially intermittent and vary in terms of the probability ofreinforcement over
satiated. punishmentor ignoring. Some circumstances may nearly approximate a continuous
Histories in which bids for nurturance are reinforced more frequently than they reinforcement schedule, while others may be increasingly intermittent. Ainsworth
are punished or ignored should result in a stable, well-defined, and secure et a1. (1978) noted that among mothers of securely attached infants, there are
attachment repertoire characterized by a tight association between deprivation, an mothers who respond verysensitively and consistently as well as mothers whom they
available attachment figure, and confident nurturance-seeking. Evidence from the describe as inconsistently sensitive, mixing in both periods of ignoring and
extensive attachment literature demonstrates that such secure attachment styles are interfering.
• the most commonly occurring. Ainsworth et al. (1978) noted that the mothers of Like Bartholomew and Horowitz (1991), the BA perspective proposes that one
more securely attached infants "tend to be substantially more sensitive, accepting, result ofvariance in the probability ofreinforcement is that attachment repertoires
cooperative, and psychologically accessible to the babies" (p. 146). In addition, are continuous or dimensional rather than strictly categorical. For example, within
Ainsworth's studies found that the mothers of more securely attached children the broad category of"securely attached," there should exist a continuum spanning
responded more promptly to their babies' cries, handled their babies more the range from ratios that approximate continuous reinforcement schedules to those
affectionately and tenderly, and responded to their babies' c~es more contingently. that mix in substantially higher probabilities of ignoring (but little or no punish-
In those circumstances in which reinforcement of nurturance-seeking is substan- ment).
tially more probable than eitherpunishment or ignoring, nurturance-seeking under Theoretically, therefore, within the broad category of securely attached indi-
the appropriate conditions of deprivation or threat, and in the presence of an viduals, there should be some vadability with regard to how well individuals tolerate
appropriate discriminative stimulus (i.e., attachment figure), should occur consis- periods in the relationship in which the partner is less responsive to attachment bids.
tently and lawfully. In other words, a person wiU develop a strong repertoire of The closer a reinforcement schedule is to continuous (the larger the ratio of
nurturance-seeking from identifiable attachmen t figures, and the person will reliably reinforcement to ignoring), the more vulnerable is the resulting repertoire to
seek nurturance when he or she needs it. Histories rich in reinforcement for extinction. The smaller the variable ratio is the more extinction-resistant it becomes.
nUrturance-seeking teach aperson that attachment figures are responsive and can be Thus, within the range ofsecurely attached individuals, some will be more resistant
counted On and that the world ofclose others is stable and reliable. From a behavior to the extinction of their nurturance-seeking repertoires than others. For example,
analytic perspective, a secure attachment style is a repertoire for nurturance-seeking ifa person has been raised by parents that respond very consistently every time she
that results from a reliable reinforcement history. More technically, such highly approaches them seekingnurturance and then as an adult she becomes involved with
contingent responding by the mothers to their infants' bids for nurturance should a partner that tends to ignore her bids for nurturance, she is likely to stop seeking
result in strong stimulus control by the mother over the infant's nurturance-seeking nurturance from that partner fairly quickly (following perhaps a brief extinction
behavior under conditions of deprivation. This relationaUy situated stimulus burst). In other words, she will be unlikely to stay with someone that tends to ignore
control constitutes the infant's attachment to her "attachment figure. It
her attachment needs. On the other hand, ifa person has been raised by parents who
often provided nurturance on demand but also ignored those bids from time to time,
Although stimulus control under such circumstances is extremely strong, a
related characteristic of rich reinforcement contingencies is that the resulting that person would learn to tolerate some lack ofresponsiveness from an attachment
behavior tends to generalize rather well to similar circumstances. Accordingly, one partner. Thus, as an adult, that person would be more likely to tolerate the occasional
would expect people raised with high degrees ofresponsiveness to nUrturance bids lackofresponsiveness from a partnerwithout ceasing bids for attachment altogether.
to form positive attachment relationships with sufficiently similar others fairly BA theory predicts that some otherwise securely attached individuals should be
fairly extinction-vulnerable and relatively more likely to leave a partner that does
~ot.c?nsistently respond to bids for nurturance, whereas other securely attached they can turn their behavior to other priorities. However, for individuals such as
IOdlvlduals should be much more tolerant of occasional or intermittent those described here, for whom the history ofreinforcement for nurturance-seeking
nonresponsiveness. Speculatively, the more extinction-resistant secure individuals has been very inconsistent, so much time and energy becomes devoted to seeking
may be those that are more capable offorming stable relationships with avoidantly reassurance and nurturance (with little post-reinforcement pause), that little time
attached partners, w~ereas the more extinction-vulnerable secure individuals may and energy remains for engaging effectively with the rest of the environment. As a
~ m~ch more s~le~t1~e of consistently bid-sensitive partners. Regardless. neither result, the focus of attachment behavior narrows to only one time-eonsuming
typ~ of ~ecure IOdlv!dual should be particularly prone to developing disordered
i relatIonshIps. In fact, 10 adult couples, the most frequent pairing is of two securely
relationship. In addition to being potentially relationaIly dysfunctional, this type of
repertoire is also likely to be depressogenic (Scott & Cordova, 2002). Narrow and
attached partners, .and the next most common pairings are between one securely rigid repertoires have been argued to create a particular vulnerability to developing
II a.ttached and one Insecurely attached individual (Brennan Be Shaver, 1995). This depression specifically because they provide few. and easily disrupted, opportuni-
I' sImple frequency, along with other evidence from the literature, suggests that not ties for engaging in effective behavior (Cordova Be Gee, 2001). In terms of
only are. more securely attached individuals less susceptible to relationship relationship dysfunction, the preoccupation and dinginess that characterize a
dysfu~ctJon, but they may actually function as a stabilizing force in pairings with preoccupied style can generate a great deal ofstress within a relationship because
more IOsecurely attached individuals. it feeds demand-withdrawal patterns between partners, emphasizes differences in
~atios h,~hest in reinforcement, but also high in ignoring. As implied by the needs for closeness versus distance, and generally contributes to an underlying
~ontlnuum discussed abo~e, ra~os involving mostly reinforcement can theoretically chronic sense of dissatisfaction with the amount of nurturing provided by the
mclude large amounts oflgnonng, up to and beyond the point where the result is partner. Preliminary evidence does suggest, however, that t.hese repertoires can be
greater~tin~tion-re~istancewithin a primarilysecure attachment style. At the point modified so that more preoccupied adults can form more securely attached
where l.gnorlOg be~ to be almost as probable as reinforcement, the resulting relationships with adult partners (Scott, 2002). At the same time, repertoires are
repertoIre should b~gIn t~ look more and more relentless and dingy, with bids for built, not replaced, and so it is likely that in the absence ofopportUnities to build
nurturance becomlOg fairly constant and invariable. Such large variable ratio new repertoires, such individuals will maintain an enduring vulnerability predispos-
schedu.les tend to produce behavior that occurs at a very high rate with virtually no ing them to preoccupied behavior.
post-remforcement pause. Thus, a person whose bids for nurturance resulted about The general principle at work in building such "preoccupied" repertoires is that
eq~ally in ignoring as reinforcement would be expected to develop a repertoire in the higher the proportion ofnonresponsive to reinforcing responses, the greater the
which he would tend to make bids for nurturance at a fairly high rate and would be tolerance or extinction resistance the person develops to being ignored by an
un~e~erred fro~ makin~ those. bids, regardless ofa partner's nonresponsiveness. In attachment figure. On the secure end of the spectrum, this presents few problems
addl~on,.even gIven a remforcmg response, the person would be likely to continue and may actually confer some strength to the secure attachment style by making it
~aking bl~s for nurtur~ce with little sign ofa post-reinforcement pause. It is also more resilient to periods ofneglect that may characterize difficult periods in an adult
likely that It.would ~e difficult for that person to feel satiated for long by nurturing long-term relationship. The transition to parenthood, with its associated decrease
respo~ses, glv~n a hIstory of r~mforcementthat taught him that nurturing is fairly in time available for nurturing the parents' relationship, is one such example. On
unrel~able: ThiS style of pers~stent nurturance and reassurance seeking has been the preoccupied end of the spectrum, increased extinction-resistance also comes
de~cnbed 10 the attac~men~ Itterature as a "preoccupied" attachment style charac- with an anxiety-driven persistence or clinginess that is chronically uncomfortable
tenzed by p~eoccupatJon WIth the attachment partner, dinginess, and a pervasive for the person experiencing it and difficult to tolerate for the partner.
sense of anxiety about the av~il~bility ofcontinued nurturance (Main, 2000). Ratioshighest in ranforcemmt, but also highinpunishment. Following from the same
One expectable charactenstJc ofa large variable ratio schedule as noted above general principle discussed above, if a person's bids for nurturance are sometimes
is that it ~rod~ces ~ very high rate ofbehavior with little post-reinforcement pause: reinforced and sometimes punished, that person will learn to tolerate punishment
thus leaVIng httle time and energy for other behaviors. One of the central facets of to get his nurturance needs met. The punitiveness involved need not necessarily be
Bowlby's (1969, 1973, 1980) attachment theory is that an infant must feel relatively abusive, but can, and most likely often does, include noxious interactions with a
assured of the responsiveness of an attachment figure before he or she will feel caregiver around occasions of nurturance. For example, Ainsworth and colleagues
comfortable exp!oringand learning from the rest ofthe environment. Among infants (1978) describe mothers of ambivalent infants as sometimes engaging in inappro-
and adults classified as securely attached, exploration of the environment occurs priately controlling behavior and, for example, "resist[ingJ any effort the baby made
regularly and consistently. Such individuals have learned that their attachment to feed himself. The babies who were treated thus tended to rebel, so that feedings
partners will respond with contingent nurturing ifand when needed, and therefore were unhappy and occasions for struggle" (p. 146). In this example, nurturance and
punitiveness ar~ occ~rringsimultaneously i~ response to the child's bid to be shown when alone may also characterize fearfully-avoidant individuals due to the punitive
care. The aff~ctIvesIde effect should be anxiety about punishment or reciprocAted nature of their history with attachment figures. as we will discuss later.
an?er. Thus. Ifa perso? cannot predict whether a bid for nurturance will result in Another important characteristic of punishment, repeatedly demonstrated in
beang nurtured or pUnJ~hed, then simply needing nurturance should elicit anxiely the basic BA literature. is that punishment suppresses behavior without extinguish-
and. ~ear. and approachmg the attachment figure should intensify that feeling. In ing it. Put another way, the individual remains just as motivated to seek nurturance
additIOn. the three .standard responses to applied punishers are (a) aggression, (b) but suppresses the behavior associated with that need in order to avoid punishment.
escape, and (c) avoidance. So, theoretically, given a need to be nurtured and the The implication is that the experienced desire for nurturance remains strong in
presence ofan attachment ~gure, the person should feel (a) fear, (b) anger, (c) a desjre anxious-ambivalent individuals and the behavior, bids for nurturance, is likely to
to approach. ~d (d) a desire to run away. That exact mix of motivations has been rebound strongly at any signal that reinforcement (nurturance) might be available.
obse;""ed b>:, Ainsworth and others in children they have classified as "anxious- This would account for the high degree of vigilance in anxious-ambivalent
ambIValent. ~rom the BA perspective, anxious-ambivalent attachment styles individuals, as well as for their potentially high tolerance for the punishment of
s,hould result dlre~t1y from histories in which bids for nurturance are almost equally nurturance bids.
likely to be met.wlth punishment as reinforcement. As we will discuss later, as more An additional principle that has been derived from the work ofbehavioranalysts
and. more ~unJshers are a?ded., the repertoire should move from looking like regarding punishment is that punishment does not generalize well. Indeed.
anxIOus-avOIdance to looking like what has been described as a "disorganized" punishment tends to be rather tightly associated with the specific source of
attachment style.
punishment and the punished behavior quickly reemerges when the source of the
~e BA ~rsp.ective suggests that current attachment theory may meIge two punishment changes or is removed. Extending that principle to .attachme?t theory
potentIally qUJt~ dl~erent a~chmen~ styles, preoc~p.ied and ambivalent. into the suggests that individuals whose bids for nurturance are frequently pUOlshed are
one category of anxtous-ambJvalent. At the same tIme. it should also be noted that likely to associate punishment with the specific person who has applied punishment
early attachment r~e~chers, induding Ainsworth. made distinctions between and with the specific circumstances under which punishment occurred. If a new
several subgroups wlthm ~ac.h.ofthe origin~l three attachment styles and recognized person enters the scene, then nurturance-seeking behaviorshould quickly reemerge
that t~ere was lawful ~an~bdlty that remalOed to be discerned and studied. A BA to the extent that the new person is perceptibly different from the punishing person.
analYSIS suggests that Ifremforcement is most probable, even ifonly slightly more Ainsworth et aI. (1978) noted that the avoidant infants in their sample were quite
probabl~ than other responses, the individual will learn to tolerate whatever else is willing to respond positively to the close bodily contact of the visitor-observer in
10 the m.1X, rega~dless ofwhether the rest ofthat mix consists mostly ofbeing ignored the strange situation. despite the fact that those same infants generally avoided
or ofbel?g pUnJs~ed. Ifhaving nurturance bids ignored is a significant probability contact with their own mothers. One may also see this phenomenon in the
and pUOJsbme~t IS not. th~n the person should develop an attachment style that particularly passionate relationships formed by individuals with abusive histories.
~ooks p.re~cupI~dand arnuously persistent. Ifpunishment is a significant probabil- In addition, even given the same punishing partner, ifthe circumstances surrounding
Ity(agam WJth reinforcement bein~ most probabl~),then the person should develop punishment for nunurance-seeking cbange, then nurturance-seeking should again
~ ~ttac.hment style ~at looks anxIOUS and genumely ambivalent. Thus, a genuine quickly reemerge. depending on how different the circumstances are and whether
dlstmctJon should exist between a "preoccupied" attachmentstyle and an "anxt' s- or not those specific circumstances have been previously associated with punish-
am b'IvaI " attachment style, two styles that are potentially confounded in current
ent au
ment.
attach~enttheory. Th~ difference should be the source ofthe experienced anxiety. Ratioshighestinpunishment. In those circumstances where a child's caretakers are
One mlgh,t expect anxtously-ambivalent individuals to report an approach-avoid- frequently punitive or abusive, bids for nurturance are more likely to result in
a.nce conflict about att~chmentfigures and preoccupied individuals to report a more punishment than either nurturance or neglect. Ainsworth et al, (1978) noted that,
slm~le sense of yeamlOg for attachment figures. In other words, the anxiously- "a highly rejecting mother frequently feels angry and resentful toward her baby, She
ambivalent should genuinely experience and be able to report the ambivalence may grumble that he interferes unduly with her life. or she may show her rejection
about att.ach?'~t.figures that the category teem has always implied, whereas by constantly opposing his wishes or by a generally pervasive mood ofscolding or
preoccupied IOdlvJduals should experience a much less ambivalent craving for irritation" (p. 142). Ainsworth·s studies also showed that these mothers demon-
nurturance from .others. In addition, because of that strong sense of ambivalence strated a strong aversion to physical contact with their babies, despite the fact that
about others, anxJously-ambivalent individuals may report that they only genuinely they tended to hold their babies as much as other mothers (usually toward
!eel at ease when ~J?ne, wh.ereas preoccupied individuals may report that they easily instrumental ends determined by the mother). Ainsworth et aI. (1978) noted that
lose themselves In relatIOnships. A sense of only being genuinely comfortable such mothers "provide their babies with unpleasant, even painful experiences
associated with close bodily contact" (p. 151). Under such circumstances, according In other words, punitive responses do not have to be abusive in order for them to
to basic behavioral principles, the individual willieam to avoid, escape from, and/ serve their function as punishers in shaping an individual's attachment repertoire.
oraggress against those punishers, resulting in either fearful oraggressive attachment At the same time, punishers do vary in intensity from mild to severely abusive.
styles. Currently, the effects ofthat variability in severity on the formation ofattachment
On the one hand, an individual may develop a "fearful-avoidant" attachment repertoires are unclear. However, it is assumed that more noxious punishers result
repertoire, such as that described by Bartholomew and Horowitz (1991), character- in greater aversion.
ized by fearful avoidance ofothers, along with a chronic desire for nurturance. On Ratios highest in ignoringor noncontingentresponding. Ignoring leads to extinction
the other hand, an individual may develop what might be called an "aggressive" ofattachmentseeking. Thus, high probabilities ofignoring place nunurance-seeking
attachment repertoire, characterized by defensively aggressive nurturance-seeking. on an extinction schedule, theoretically resulting in the actual removal of
Such a person might act out their nurturance needs by being domineering, nurturance-seeking from the repertoire. Perhaps the most dramatic circumstances in
possessive, threatening, and generally abusive in response to the assumed aversiveness which ignoring is the most likely response to bids for nurturance is in orphanage
ofattachment transactions. In other words, because someone who has developed an institutions. As noted by Ainsworth et aI. (1978):
aggressive attachment repertoire has leamed that attachment figures are usually ...an infant reared for a long period..., in an institutional environment in
punitive, and becausepunishment does notextinguish nurturance-seeking, he orshe which he has so little consistent interaction with anyone potential
may react to needing nurturance by lashing out in response to expected or perceived attachment figure that he fails to form an attachment may, when subse-
threat from available attachment figures. quentlyfostered and thus given an opportunity to attach himself, be unable
If punishment is the most probable contingency, but the probability of to attach himself to anyone (p. 9).
reinforcement is also relatively high, then the resulting repertoire may be akin to that However, given that the inclination to seek nurturance is fundamental to the
described by Hesse and Main (2000) as "disorganized," or one in which the species, extinction or near-extinction schedules are unlikely to remove nurturance-
individual, in response to the need for nurturance, becomes stuck in an approach- seeking from the repertoire altogether. Rather, they are likely to drastically weaken
avoidance response. Ainsworth et al. (1978) noted that even the most rejecting nurturance-seeking. More commonly, ignoring occurs within normal family settings
mothers in their studies at least occasionally interacted with their babies in tender characterized by unresponsiveness. Ainsworth et al. (1978) noted that "the inacces-
and affectionate ways, providing their babieswith ahistory ofnurturance mixed with sible or ignoring mother is often so preoccupied with her own thoughts and activities
both punishers and reinforcers. Under circumstances in which an individual does that she does not even notice her baby, let alone acknowledge his signals" (p. 143).
not know whether a bid for nurturance is going to be punished or reinforced, the The resultingattachment repertoire should be akin to the "dismissive avoidant" style
resulting behavior is likely to be a confusing mix of approach, avoidance, and described by Bartholomew and Horowitz (1991), characterized by a virtUal absence
aggression. The availability of periodic nurturance should maintain nurturance- ofnurturance-seeking, but by little, ifany, anxiety about others. The person leams
seeking by the individual, but since punishment is more likely than reinforcement, that otherpeople do not provide nurturance and therefore does notseek it from them.
that nurturance-seeking should come with agreat deal offear and anger. In addition, This likely creates a self-fulfilling prophecy that follows individuals into adult life,
the individual would learn that punishment has to be tolerated in order to receive providing them with little opportunity to receive nurturance from their social
any nurturance at all. The person genuinely becomes attached to her punitive network. Again, this style should exist on acontinuum, with higherlevels ofignoring
caretaker, and expectations for what nurturing relationships should look like may resulting in greater dismissiveness.
generalize into adult relationships,setting the stage for the person to tolerate abusive Even though a rigid extinction schedule should remove nurturance-seeking
relationships. Ainsworth et al. noted that "most family-reared infants do become from an individual's repertoire, it will not destroy nurturance as a powerful
attached, even to unresponsive or punitive mother figures" (p.18). reinforcer. In fact, one would expect such individuals to exist in a constant state of
Punishers can vary in intensity and severity. When we refer to punishers as nurturance deprivation that may not only be palpable but chronically aversive and
contingent responses to bids for nurturance, we do not usually make reference to the distracting. In the same way that certain circumstances extinguish an individual's
severity of those punishers. The technical definition of punisher is that it is a efforts to find food without extinguishing the basic survival value of food or
contingent response that decreases the probability ofsubsequent occurrences ofthe "hunger." other circumstances can extinguish an individual's efforts to find
target behavior. More informally, we generally think ofpunishers as noxious events nurturance without changing the survival value ofnurturance-seeking.
contingent upon a particular behavior. For the most part, in the context ofnaturally Ainsworth et al. (1978) described some ignoring mothers as nOl ignoring bids
occurring responses to bids for nurturance, the punishers that most peopleencounter for nurturance, but instead as providing nurturance according to the mother's
are likely to be mild, including signs ofirritation, frustration, anger, and rejection. schedule and terms, rather than in response to the baby's signals (noncontingent
nurturing). Undersuch circumstances, onewould expect the child's nurturance bids Ratloof . Resul11Dg DescrtPtJoa UIIdy bdlaWir ID couple rdatklllsblp
to extinguish (since they are not contingently responded to) and for the child to Reinforcement to altachment
Jportlllllo repertolr&'5lyl~
eitherdeveJop a passive nurturance-seeking repertoire, or asuperstitious nurturance- I'\m.IsIJJDmt
seeking repertoire, given that noncontingent reinforcement has been shown to shape Indivicllaals witb tbis Generally form positive l\WlCIlment
HiBb mnfOlUment Secute.
superstitious behavior. Ainsworth et al. noted that "because the C2 infant [a Low ignoring ClllillClion auaebmenl repenoiJe tend ID relaliomhips. and b:lve htalthy
wlnerable have secure relationships relationships. The c:\oser the
subcategory of ambivalently attached] could rarely experience a consequence Low punishment
with close olben. They tend mnfortemenl schedule wi!b the prim:uy
contingent on his own behavior, it is not surprising that he behaved very passively 10 UUSI ilWlcluJleol figwca ClIregiver WlIS 10 eontinuollJ, lIIe leu
and 10 have fai!b in tbellL lolcrallt of iplOring mpDlI5CS these
both in the strange situation and at home" (p. 237). 1bey abo tend to have individuals will be in couple
Although the effects ofnoncontingent responding should be similar to simple children who are more likely relationships. Thus, individuals wi!b tbis
10 be secwe In their styl: lend ID b:lve liule IOIaance for
nonresponding, there are relatively few naturally occurring cases in which attach- a1l11clllru:m relAtionships. pannm who are inattentive, and are
ment figures are genuinely nonresponding in a neglectful and abusive sense. Instead, likely to SlOp seeking
1lIIIlIII'3IlCe10SlCllCS$ or CVCllID end
the type ofattachment relationships observed to give rise to ambivalent attachment n:lationships !bal are IlOl adequately
styles were not necessarily neglectful (the mothers did provide adequate care for responsive.

theirchildren) but provided care only on the caretaker's terms and schedule and not HiBb reinfcm:cment Secure. Same as above. G:nerally form heallhy relationships as
adullS. These individuals IeDd ID be man:
in response to bids from the child. Under such circumstances, a person learns that ModtI'IIIC igDDring extinction
toleranl of periods of inallention from
Low punishment resislant
how and when they are provided nurturance is out oftheir control, resulting in the their panners and !bus tend 10 cope well
wilh n:lationships in which their bids for
extinction ofagreat deal ofthe nurturance-seeking repertoire and agenerally passive cJoscn:sslrwmarasu:e are usually mel but
relationship to attachment figures. One might imagine such individuals passively sometimes igoom1.
falling into adult attachment relationships (because they have been sources of PmlcaIpicd Preocxupied style Polelllially dysfunctional and
tlillh n:infcm:cmenl
noncontingent nurturance in the past), but subsequently providing attachment HiBb ignoring chamclerizcd by a high I'lItC dcpresogcnic because almost all of a
Low punishment ofbids for IIWlIIJ1tIlCC wi!b person's energy i$ focused on
partners with few, ifany, cues about their own attachment needs, passivelyexpecting little post.mnfon:emtlll mainl:lining a seewe conneaion. al the
to be nurtured without knowing how to ask for it. This repertoire would likely pause tr:t_ bids. Tend 10 e.qmtSC of devoting encrzy ID olher
relationships or activities. Individuals
yearn for atlemion from
manifest itselfnot as active avoidance ofclose adult relationships, but as a passive caregivetS. IeDd to be jealoUS, fearful, and clinsy·
nonpursuit of intimacy accompanied by a normal desire to be nurtured. Demand-withdraw pauems may be
common.
An analysis from a BA perspective comes to the same general conclusion as that
argued by Bartholomew and Horowitz (1991) regarding avoidant attachmentstyles: HiBb reinfcm:cmenl Anxious- Anxious-amblvalClll slyle in Approach·avoidance pauems are
Ambivalent which penon may feel fear, common. NurtunllltC-seeldng behavior
Low ignoring
that the original category ofavoidant attachment (or style A children) described by High punisbmenl anger, and a desire 10 Nn has been panhtlly suppressed but is likely
away Iiom pOlCmiaJ 10 re-cmerge Slnlogly gi_
Ainsworth and colleagues likely includes children shaped by two distinguishable providers of IlIII1IIl3lICIC cIlJe mnfOlCelQCll1. Vigilan1 monilOrina of
histories, one in which frequent punishment suppresses approach behavior(fearful 10 a hislOty of rccclving bolb panmr's bellavior is common. Slum-
punishntelll and term passiOIlllle n:lalionships occur
avoidance in Bartholomew and Horowitz's terms) and one in which frequent rein1'ortemem for nultUllllICe- frequenlly, but tend 10 end early due 10
ignoring essentially extinguishes approach behavior (dismissive avoidance). In seeking. Bec:mse of the fearful or anlll'Y n:spoll5CS 10
millCll1 mnfon:em:nt aDd imerpcrsonal hun.
addition, an adult style that may look insecure, much like an avoidant attachment punishntelll hililOry. people
style, may actually be characterized by a learned, passive nonpursuit ofattachment may rqKln feeling al casc
only when alone.
relationships that are actively desired, rather than by either fear of potential
attachment figures or a conviction that others are not nurturing (quite the opposite, Low mnfon:ement Avoidant- This style is cIuIracleriud by Individuals b:lvc learned lhal other
Dismissive a virtual ab!CIICC of people do IIDt provide nunu~ and
in fact). HiBb iplOring thcreI'ore they do IlOl seek it 0lIl. This can
Low punishmenl nurturanCC seeking. The
individull1 has lcamed thai lead 10 a self.fulfilling prophecy in which
Summary of Attachment Repertoires bids for nl1ltU/'alll:e arc almost individuals do nOI expect nuntlring and
never mtI. and thcreI'on:: thcrefon:: do 1t0l1ry 10 awUn iL AI a
In sum, an examination of attachment phenomenon from a contemporary nurtur1IIIl»seeking behavior n:sull. such individuals are oftm IlOl in
is plllt1lcally exUoct. couple relatiolUhips lIS adllllS, and may
behavioral perspective suggests a variety ofpossible attachment repertoires devel- beloMly.
oping out of histories in which bids for nurturance are responded to with
combinations ofcontingent reinforcement, punishment, ignoring, and noncontin-
gent responding. See Table 14.1 for a summary ofratios ofreinforcement and the Table 14.1. Description ofAttachment Styles.
resultant attachment styles.
Halloo' ResuIUDg DesatpUon
Relntofte1llellt 10 otlac:bmeat U1leIy beIlamr In couple rdnlllIRshlp stability of adult relationships during times of decreased responsiveness. For
IgaorfJl« 10 repertofre'style example, one might predict that more extinction-resistant secure repertoires wo~ld
Punfsllment
be more resilient during periods in which partners are forced to be apart (e.g., dunng
low 10 IDOCIer.uc
reinfOltel!lCtll (lion-
AWlidanI- IllIIividuaJs wiCh Chis As adullS, Ihe# individuals may be military deployment). One might also expect people with more extinc~ion­
Passi~ n:infon:emclll history may be
contingent lonely bec:ausc Ihey )'Cam for closeness. vulnerable repertoires to be more selective ofmates, and therefore, to move qUJckly
inlen!Sted in intillllll:y bUl bllt do nollulow bow to pursue it. These
nurtW:IIICe)
"ish isnorins
often haW! no praaical illllividuaJs may also be in re/alionships out of relationships that are not adequately responsive. In general, however, one
repertoire for ptIIIUing il. where !hey passi-ely CXpccllIUnlll'lltlCC
Low punishmenl Their lIltaehmentligllreS would expect secure attachment repertoires to confergreaterrelationship health and
but in which chey are rebtively poor
IeIlded 10 respond IIOn- IIllItUm's. stability.
coDtingemJy 10 their bids for
nlll1lll:lm:e. so Ihey have Iinle What have traditionally been categorized as ambivalent attachment styles
experience Ie'Cldng out should, according to the current analysis, consist oftwo distinguishable repertoires.
IIUlI1U1IIIl:e but do expect
othen 10 provide it The first should be characterized primarily by a preoccupation with attachment-
low reinfon:ement FeaJful·Avoidanl This scyle appe3J'S gemnlly
seeking and anxiety about abandonment, and the second should be characterized
Mocletate Ignoring Tend to bave "eat difficulty fonning and by fearful approach-avoidance. The first type ofrepertoire is theoretically shaped by
avoidant of 3IladImenl maimaini"8 close couple relationships.
High punishment relationships. Bids for They sometimes move qUickly from histories consisting mostly of ignoring with some low-ta-moderate probability of
nul1U/'atIce were most often rellllioltShip to relationship in punuil of
eilher ignored or punished, nlll'lUring but leave qllic:kly in order to
contingent reinforcement for nurturance-seeking. As adults, ~dividuals ~th this
and as a resull. illllividuaJs avoid pain. NW'lUranoe-seeking has been type of repertoire should be dingy, fearful of abandonment, Jealous, and In great
Iearo to avoid close others in SUJlprused and is likely to tdlound
order 10 minimize Che th=t Sli'Ongly when reinforced. However. as
need of reassurance. However, there is some preliminary evidence that these
Chey pose. Ihe re/alionship becomes more individuals can form more secure styles as adults if they find contingently nurturing
emotionally chreateniag. the person is
likely to re-suppress l\lII'tlmUIce.seeking. partners (Scott, 2002). . ...
creating a situation in whleh Ihc person The second, more genuinely amb1Valent, type of repertoue IS theoretically
may wilhdraw from Che re/alionship
sucIdcuIy. shaped by histories consisting of a somewhat equal mix of punishment and
Modcr.uc Fearful. Bids for nunurancc are reinforcement for nurturance-seeking. As adults, individuals with this type of
reinforcement Individuals wiCh llIis history lend 10
Disorganized occasionally reinfon:ed, but repertoire are constantly faced with an approach-avoidance dilemma in attachment
low ignoring CJIIlericnce Ihe pRlSJlC:Cl of nurtur:lnce as
often punished. The both noxious and appealing. Numuance-
High punishment availability of periodic relationships. On the one hand they want and seeknurturance and on the other hand,
seeking behavior comes wilb a"eat deal
reinforcement mailllains offear and an~. wtUch often n:sull$ in a they are afraid ofand therefore avoid closeness. Ultimately, fearfulness should limit
nurtl1l1lJlCe Ie'Cking but
involves fear.
powerfuJ approach-avoidanc:e Jl:Ittml. intimacy within adult relationships, creating a chronic, if mild-to-moderate, sense
Individuals may IOIer.ile abusive
The resulting behavioral trea1JI1eIIl in adull couple relationships of dissatisfaction and disconnection for both partners. Again, the repertoire will
repertoire includes approach. and may also be abusive in relationships.
avoicbnoe. and aggression. continue to be shaped by transactions with current attachment partners, either
low reinfon:emenl F=fu\.
toward more secure or more insecure styles. Even among those shaped toward more
~Ie wllb Ibis history of Tend to fonu abusive relationships in
low ignoring Aggressive reinforcement bave Icarned to which !hey bebave aggressively and are
attachment security, those with a history ofambivalent attachmentshould continue
Hish punishmenl associate seeldng nlll1Ur.lltl:e created aggressively. Since Ibcse to have a unique vulnerability to developing approach-avoidance relationships.
wilb punishment They individuals have not Ieamcd to _late
Ihcrefore often relate to close nlll1Ul'allcc.seeking wilb relnfon:emenr.
From the current perspective, attachment styles traditionally categorized as
o!hers in an aur=ive Ihey lend 10 Jack a repertoire of muturing avoidant may contain two distinguishable dismissive attachment repertoires and
fashjon. behavior.
three distinguishable fearful attachment styles. Dismissive attachment styles may
include both genuinely dismissive repertoires resulting from the near-extinction of
Table 14.1. conl'i Description ofAttachment S{yle!. nurturance-seelcing and passive attachment repertoires resulting from noncontin-
gent nurturing. Within adults, the dismissive attachment repertoire should be
characterized by a genuine disinterest in intimate relationships with little anxiety
. M?stly s~cure attachment repertoires should theoretically vary in terms oftheir about other people. The passive attachment repertoire should be characterized by
extmctJon resIstances. Such variance should depend on the ratio ofreinforcement
a genuine interest in and longing for intimacy but no effective repertoire for actively
~ersu~ ignoring responses to bids for nurturance, with greater probabilities of pursuing it. Both repertoires are likely to result in loneliness and difficulty finding
Jgnonng building greater extinction resistance. As noted earlier, mostly secure
and maintaining intimate relationships.
attachment repertoires have been shown to be associated with greater relationship . Fearful attachment styles may include three distinguishable repertoires. First,
health, and the distinction we draw here should theoretically have its impact on a genuinely fearful repertoire should result from histories consisting primarily of
punitive consequences for nurturance-seeking and should be characterized in adults Attachment Difficulties in Relationships
as consistent fearful avoidance of intimate relationships accompanied by genuine Insecure attachment interfereswith the intimacy process through several routes.
longing for closeness. This type offearful repertoire is the most likely to be sustained Histories of punishment for nurturance-seeking (fearful, disorganized, aggressive,
by negative reinforcement, since avoidance offeared others functions to eliminate and ambivalent attachment repertoires) makenurturance-seeking dramatically more
a threat. Consequently, one might expect individuals with fearful-avoidant reper- interpersonally vulnerable than is the case for more securely attached individuals.
toires to have great difficulty forming and maintaining intimate relationships. One As a result, fearfully and ambivalently attached individuals have more difficulty
might also predict that individuals with fearful-avoidant repertoires might move initiating intimate events because interpersonal vulnerability is substantially more
quickly in and out of emotionally intense relationships, because suppressed threatening for them. In addition, the deepening of intimacy necessarily results in
nurturanc~seeking is likely to rebound strongly when the person is made to feel safe. more frequent intimate hurts because intimate partners are more vulnerable to each
Similarly, nurturance-seeking is likely to resuppress as the relationship becomes other and more capable of being both intentionally and unintentionally hurtful
more emotionally threatening, akin to the behavior of those with borderline (Cordova & Scott, 2001). As a result, maintaining close intimate connections
personality disorder. requires an ability to tolerate occasional emotional hurt in non-relationship-
The next type offearful repertoire is shaped by a history consisting ofa mix of destructive ways. However, these individuals are much more likely to respond to the
punishing and of reinforcing consequences, with punishing consequences being inherent slings and arrows of intimacy with powerful urges to withdraw or
more probable than reinforcing ones. The resulting repertoire should be like an counterattack. Thus. individuals with punitive attachment histories are not only less
ambivalent attachment style in that it is characterized by an approach-avoidance likely to initiate intimate events, they are also more likely to terminate the intimacy
conflict. In some cases, when the history consists primarily of punishment, the process.
resulting behavior may look much more disorganized. In this case, the level of Individualswith histories ofbavingnurturance-seekingignored ornoncontingently
deprivation required to elicit an approach would have to be quite powerful to responded to are substantially less likely to initiate intimate events because active
overcome a strong inclination to avoid a feared attachment figure. As adults, nurturance-seeking has nearly been extinguished from their overall repertoire. The
individuals with this type of history likely experience the desire for nurturance as genuinely dismissive are not only less likely to initiate intimate events. but are also
both attractive and noxious, creating an approach-avoidance paradox much more unlikely to reinforce the interpersonally vulnerable behaviorofothers and therefore
powerful than that experienced by the ambivalentlyattached, resulting in emotional are unlikely to culminate intimate events. Depending on how thoroughly nurtUrance
chaos. These individuals may be particularly susceptible to finding themselves in and nurturancNeeking have been removed from the repertoire, the dismissively
abusive relationships because they have learned to associate nurturance with attached may either fail to form intimate relationships entirely or may form
punitiveness. relationships that consist ofvery little intimacy. In contrast, the passively attached
Finally, an aggressive attachment repertoire is also a likely outcome ofhistories may be unlikely to initiate intimate events but may be more capable ofculminating
consisting of large probabilities of punishment for nurturance-seeking. This may them simply by virtue ofthe fact that they are much more comfortable and desirous
constitute a unique repertoire or it may be a facet of all attachment repertoires ofintimacy in general. Although they may be prone to depriving their partners of
shaped by some mix ofpunitive consequences. Regardless, some individuals with sufficient nurturing, they themselves may be perfectly content as long as they have
histories ofbeingpunished regularly for seekingnurturance may develop repertoires partners who are willing to actively pursue closeness with them.
in which they relate to potential intimates in a uniquely aggressive fashion. Having Finally, the preoccupied and ambivalently attached, due to the clingy and
learned to associate nurturance with punitiveness, they may be particularly hyper-vigilant nature oftheirattachment bids, are likely to overwhelm even the most
susceptible to forming abusive relationships. Whether an individual develops a securely attached partners at times with their need for reassurance. The preoccupied
disorganized style or an aggressive style may be culturally mediated. may be so focused on getting their own intimacy needs met that they themselves
make poor intimate partners and their relatively extreme vulnerability to feeling
Addressing Adult Attachment Insecurities in Couple Therapy
abandoned may contribute to something ofa self-fulfilling prophecy. Ambivalent
Although attachment histories cannot be erased, they can be shaped by the partners, by contrast, may withdraw at the first sign ofrejection or hurt, leaving their
addition ofnew learning opportunities; for many people, stable responsive adult partners with the task to figuring out what went wrong and how to repair the
intimate relationships provide just the type of new learning experiences that can relationship.
shape more secure attachment styles. At the same time. the enduring vulnerabilities
created by insecure attachment repertoires are likely to manifest themselves in adult Dysfunctional Relationship Themes
intimate relationships in fairly predictable forms. Three common '"themes" Oacobson & Christensen, 1996) in distressed couples
can be accounted for by attachment styles that interfere with the intimacy process.
For the purpose ofthis paper, intimacy refers to a process in couples' interactions seeking driven by achronic fear that their partner is angry. This pattern emerges from
in which behavior made interpersonally vulnerable by a history ofpunishment by an attachment history in which punitiveness was common. For both ofthese types
others is elicited and reinforced by another person (technically, an intimate event; ofstyles, nurturance is not perfectly satiating because attachment figures signal both
Cordova, 2002; Cordova & Scott, 2001). The classic example of interpersonally sources ofnurturance and abandonment or rejection. The resulting dinginess and
vulnerable behavior is the sharing of past hurts, humiliations, failures, and other reassurance-seeking can overwhelm even securely attached partners, increasing the
painful emotional experiences. Additional examples include most of those behav- probability of falling into a closeness-distance pattern.
iors that We refer to as "just being ourselves" which generally include behavior that Chronically distancing partners, for their part, are likely to have avoidant
we do not usually share with others because they are interpersonally risky (e.g., attachment repertoires, characterized bydismissiveness or fear ofcloseness. Because
caressing,1cissing, and holding, as well as burping, cussing, and acting cranky). Being their attachment histories have taught them that attachment figures are either
reinforced for interpersonally vulnerable behavior increases the probabilityofwhole generally nonresponsive or mostly punitive, maintaining closeness is either outside
classes ofinterpersonally vulnerable behavior in relation to that reinforcing person. their repertoire or just plain scary. Individuals with avoidant repertoires often deprive
The reinforcing person gains stimulus control over our interpersonally vulnerable even securely attached partners ofoptimal levels ofcloseness, thus these styles also
behavior. thereby beginning to establish him or herselfas an intimate partner. Most increase the odds of setting in motion a c1oseness-distance pattern.
fundamentally, the process results in the development ofa relationship in which we It should be noted that fearfully avoidant adults have had their nurturance-
are, more often than not, safe "just being ourselves." Thus, intimate events are the seeking behavior suppressed, notextinguished, and thus although closeness-seeking
building blocks of intimate partnerships that become characterized by open may be particularly scary for them, that repertoire may still be made available for
interpersonal vulnerability and feelings of safety, acceptance, and comfort. This shaping by a more reinforcing partnership. The genuinely dismissive, however,
occurs if the intimacy process unfolds well and is not derailed by other factors. having had their nurturance-seeking repertoire extinguished (to varying degrees),
Below, we describe three common relationship themes resulting from insecure may have the most difficulty forming intimate adult relationships without specific
attachment styles' effects on the intimacy process. training in how to effectively seek and provide nurturance. The passively avoidant
The closeness/distance theme. In the closeness/distance theme. one member ofa should fare relatively well with partners that actively pursue closeness with them
couple typically wants more contact and closeness than the other member. This because the passively attached genuinely want closeness and do not actively avoid
theme often arises out ofdifferences between partners in their desires for closeness, it. They may. however, benefit from specific training in how to elicit and provide
with one partner wanting more closeness and the other wanting more distance. The nurturance to their partners.
closeness-see1cing memberofthe couple often complains that the otherpartnerdoes TheparaOdbulseparalelivespatlem. Anothercommon pattern evident in distressed
notspend enough time with him ordoes not want to spend time togetherin the same couples is that of two people living separate lives in the same living quarters.
way. For example, a closeness-seeking partner might not consider watching Although the couple lives together, their lives do not appear otherwise intertwined;
television together to be "together time." She might long instead for more frequent they may spend their leisure time separately, do household chores separately, and
conversation and physical touch. Closeness-seeking partners often complain that when home at the same time, are rarely to be found together. Such couples often
theydo not feel fully appreciated orvalidated by their partners and that they typically state that they feel distant from each other or that they feel more like housemates
have to be the one to initiate contact. Distance-seeking partners, by contrast, tend than intimate partners. Working collaboratively as a couple is often a challenge for
to feel overwhelmed by their c1oseness-seeking partners. They often report feeling these couples, as their indination is to divide up tasks and perform them
as though no matter how much time they spend with their partners, it is never individually, not as a couple.
enough. Sadly, the closeness-seeking behavior of one partner often becomes the From an attachment perspective, couples who find themselves in this pattern
discriminative stimulus for the other partner's distancing, which in tum serves as the are likely to have learning histories that involve having had bids for closeness
discriminative stimulus for the first partner's increased demand for closeness, ignored or punished. As aresult. when they perceive trouble in the relationship, they
creating a self-perpetuating dysfunctional pattern. tend to withdraw and tum inward. Not surprisingly, people who have avoidant
What might be going on here, and how might an understanding ofattachment attachment styles are particularly susceptible to this relationship pattern. Once the
theory be helpful? One way closeness/distance patterns can originate is through the pattern has been established, it often seems to gather momentum, and breaking out
mismatch ofattachment styles. An adult with a preoccupied attachment style has ofit to seek contact or approach the otherpartner becomes less and less likely. When
a learning history which has taught him or her that others' nurturance is unpredict- one or both partners have a history ofhaving been punished for seeking closeness,
able and that closeness needs wilJ not always be met. Such individuals engage in the pattern is even more unlikely to be broken without professional help. Treatment
high frequency bids for closeness with little post-reinforcement pause. Adults with for avoidance will be discussed at greater length later in this chapter, but the general
ambivalent attachment styles are also likely to engage in a great deal ofreassurance- approach to helping couples who experience the "parallel lives" pattern is to
decrease avoidance by increasing comfort with intimate interactions. This is usually communicating or solving problems together. For these particularly difficult
accomplished through acceptance, which wiIJ be discussed later. It is also important couples, Christensen and Jacobson (1991) developed a new approach to therapy,
to note that this pattern may emerge for nonattachmenHelated reasons. One designed especially for couples who have difficulty collaborating ormoving beyond
common trigger for this theme is the transition to parenthood, when partners have damaging patterns. The primary mechanism of change is, paradoxically,learning
less time and energy to devote to their relationship. It seems likely that securely greater acceptance and developing deeper understanding of those aspects of the
attached couples are most likely to recover easily from such a situationally induced partnership that are unlikely to change. Although IBCT is arelatively new treatment,
pattern,while more insecurely attached couples are likely to struggle with the pattern initial data suggest that it is more effective than traditional behavioral couples
for longer periods of time. therapy in improving relationship satisfaction (Cordova,Jacobson, at Christensen,
The blamingormutuaI!J aggressivepat/ern. Couples who are caught in a blaming 1998; Jacobson, Christensen, Prince, Cordova, Be Eldridge, 2000).
pattern tend to have frequent arguments which quickly devolve into mutual blame, Using Acceptance Work in Therapy
retribution seeking, grudge holding, and avoidance. Over time, even attempts at
reconciliation or closeness may be met with a punitive response. Couples in which Acceptance has been described with terms ranging from complete rejection of
one or both members have learning histories that involve a history of receiving an event, through tolerance, to enthusiastic embracing (Cordova, 2001). Implicit in
punishment when making bids for closeness may be especiallyprone to this pattern; all definitions ofacceptance, however, is the idea ofgiving up the struggle to change
this applies especially to those who rarely experienced reinforcing responses to their that which cannot be changed. Much like the oft repeated "serenity prayer" of
requests for closeness or comfort. As noted earlier, besides avoidance and escape, Alcoholics Anonymous, the goal of acceptance work is to "accept the things (one)
aggression is the most predictable response to contact with aversive stimuli. For cannot change." As Christensen andJacobson (2000) note, however, acceptance can
fearfully and ambivalently attached individuals. attachment and closeness have be much more radical than simply tolerating that which one does not have the power
acquired aversive qualities from histories ofpunitive r~sponses to attachment bids. to change. The ultimate form ofinterpersonal acceptance is empathy and compas-
In addition, nurturance-seeking is particularly interpersonafly vulnerable for such sion, the ability to notice one's partner's aversive behavior and to understand the
individuals, and any hint ofrejection or punitiveness by an intimate partner is likely lovable and sometimes even well-intentioned reasons behind it. As Christensen and
to be especially painful, again eliciting either aggression or withdrawal. In short, Jacobson (2000) explain, "acceptance ... is tolerating aversive behavior because you
individuals with punitive attachment histories arc particularlyvulnerable to reactive see that behavior as part ofa larger context ofwho your partner is and who you are"
aggressiveness and blaming in their adult intimate relationships and are particularly (p. 124).
likely to fall into mutual blame and mutual aggressiveness with even securely The goals ofIBCT in relation to insecure attachment issues are compassionate
attached partners. Unfortunately, anger and aggression can escalate when a person acceptance and active coping. Given the assumption that insecure attachment styles
feels blamed, so this pattern has the potential to become dangerous. The pattern of remain enduring vulnerabilities within intimate relationships (Karney Be Bradbury,
trading punishment (blaming responses) either in response to aversive stimuli or in 1995), even when secure attachment between partners is achieved, we believe that
self-defense creates a cycle in which punishment becomes the most probable it is necessary for both partners to thoroughly understand the roots of insecure
outcome ofinteraction. Since neither member of the couple is reinforced in most attachment styles and the understandable ways in which those styles are likely to
interactions, it is difficult for either member to provide reinforcing responses to the manifest themselves in intimate relationships. The theory is that compassion for
other. both the selfand the partner, as well as a willingness to collaborate on coping with
enduring vulnerabilities, emerges out of genuine understanding by and for each
An Approach to Couples Therapy member ofa partnership. Attachment insecurities are treated as faultless, naturally
We believe that Integrative Behavioral Couples Therapy (IBCT) is uniquely occurring "flaws in the fabric" ofa couple's relationship from which both partners
suited for working with couples struggling with relationship dysfunction caused by suffer and toward which both partners can orient as a team with mutual compassion
insecure attachment styles. Developed byJacobson and Christensen (1996), IBel' and active coping. How does a therapist help a couple achieve acceptance? Cordova
wasdesigned for use with couples who hadspecial difficulty in workingcollaboratively. (2001) suggests three strategies for fostering acceptance: targeting the discriminative
Christensen andJacobson (1991) found that traditional behaviortherapy for coupleH stimulus, targeting the aversion behavior, and targeting the consequences of
did not work well for couples who were very polarized because, although they aversIOn.
sometimes could follow the rules prescribed by the therapist for problem solvin~ Targeting the discriminative stimulus. Targeting the discriminative stimulus in-
and communication, they stopped following the rules almost as soon as they Jell volves shifting the stimulus value ofan initially aversive stimulus from aversive to
therapy. In some cases, couples were so polarized that even with the help of II more appetitive. For example, the avoidantly attached individual's inclination to
therapist, they could not collaborate well enough to attempt new techniques or avoid intimacy is often quite aversive and blameworthy to his partner when the
~o~ple ?rst ~nters therapy. One approach to facilitating greater acceptance of that toward a more intimacy-producing response. Whereas targeting aversion behavior
~c mauon IS to help b~th partners more thoroughly understand the attachment relies on exposure, targeting the consequences relies on reinforcing a new, more
hlS.tory.that shaped avoidance and the underlying fearful vulnerability that charac- intimacy facilitative repertoire. For example, in the above scenario with the fearfully
temes Jt. The goal, foJIow~ng fro~ relational frame theory (e.g., Hayes. Barnes- avoidantpartner, new, naturally occurring approaches for nurturance are likely to be
Hol~es, & Roch~, 2001~, IS to S~lft the stimulus value of withdrawal from the weak and difficult to discern. but the therapist and both partners can be taught to
aversweness associated WIth descnptors like ·cold" and ·d· t . .. th
• I" • , IS ancmg to e more be vigilant for any behavior approximating interpersonal vulnerability and to
~ompass~on e ICthng stimulus value of·scared" and ·vulnerable." A successful shift increase the ampl itude ofnaturally occurring reinforcing responses (e.g., "when you
m th~ stlJ?ulus value along this gradient should result in both partners actually told me how you felt about that, it made me feel closer to you."). As those initially
expenencmg the first partner's struggle with avoidance as more tolerable under- tentative attempts at intimate approach are naturally reinforced, the stimulus value
standable, and approachable, thus facilitating the intimacyprocess for both ~fthem ofbids for closeness should shift from mostly fearful toward greater intimate safety.
T~~ ~erapeutic pro~ess involves the repeated sharing of these more compassio~
ehcltlng re:uons, whl~h allows for the gradual shifting of stimulus value.
Conclusion
Ta"!eltng Ihe aversion behavior. Targeting the aversion behavior means directly In this chapter, we have reviewed the literature on attachment theory and have
address:ng ho~ a partne~ responds to an aversive stimulus, most often his or her suggested that a behavioral analytic perspective provides a generative theoretical
partner s behaVIOr. AveCSlon usually takes the form ofavoidance (e.g., withdrawing framework from which to understand and approach this body ofresearch. We have
from an argumen~), so the therapeutic goal is most often to promote exposure and used BA to explain the function of attachment behavior and to account for the
re.sponse prevention. J?e goal in targeting aversion behaviors is to change the variability in attachment ·styles." Our account reframes attachment styles as
stlf~~lu~ value o~ the dIScriminative stimulus from aversive to more tolerable by repertoires that have been shaped by individual histories coinposed of different
faclhtat~g expen~ces that undermine negatively reinforced avoidance and rein- ratios ofreinforcement, ignoring, and punishment in response to bids for nurturance.
force actively making ~ontact. As with exposure and response prevention in the By extension, attachment repertoires are evoked in the context of adult intimate
treatment ofgerm phob~a, exposur~ to ~e aversive stimulus (e.g., dirt) in the absence relating. Many common relationship distress patterns can be understood as
of ~y harm sho~ld ulbmately extm~lSh the avoidance response. In addition, if originating from attachment repertoires that are not conducive to intimacy. Indeed,
maIcU:tg c~ntact With .na!Ural1y occumng consequences is actually reinforcing (e.g., insecure adult attachment styles can contribute to common painful relationship
expenencmg greater mbmate safety), then not only is avoidance extinguished but patterns, and fBCT can be useful in helping such couples to create new, more
an ap~roach repertoire is established. As Cordova (2001) noted, the topography of rewarding patterns with each other. By helping couples to gain a deeper understand-
the s~mulus does not change, but its stimulus value does. Put another way the ing about what has shaped their behavior in close relationships (histories of
aversl~e ~ent that triggers avoidance does not change. Instead, the avoidant punishment, being ignored. etc.), couples therapists can help partners come to a
bhe~ftsaV1£r0r IS made. to change, ~d through the resulting exposure, the stimulus value deeper understanding of themselves and their panners. Such an understanding,
s I om aversive to appetlbve. called ·acceptance," leads to intimacy-eonducive behaviors and can both reduce
For exa.mple, for fearfully attached individuals, making bids for nurtUrance is relationship distress and improve relationship satisfaction. We hope that our
sc~ ~d IS t.herefore avoided. Greater acceptance can be facilitated by (a) interpretation of attachment repertoires will lead the field to reconsider how
facJ1ltatmg a climate ofcompassionate understanding in therapy and (b) attachmentstyles have traditionally been understood and to. undertake new research
. th aki f b'd ' encour-
based on these new interpretations. In addition, we hope that ourre-eonceptualization
agmg e m ng 0 I S for nurturance and facilitating the partner's reinforcing
response to tho~e bids b.oth in therapy and at home. The ultimate goal is for the of attachment will help guide clinical fonnulations and interventions, both in
naturally o~cumng conbngencies to take hold and begin the process ofshaping a research and in practice.
more effecbve a~achment repertoire. As Cordova (2001) noted, such exposure and References
re~pon~ st:ategl~ rely on the fact that no harm comes from engaging in them. In
Ainswonh, M. D.. Blehar, M. C., Waters, E.., at Wall, S. (1978). PtIltemsofiIItlldnntnt: A psychologiazl
a sItuatIon .m whIch abuse or harm is possible, such techniques should not be used. sttuly of the slTll1Igt situation, Hillsdale, NJ: ErIbaum.
T~tmg the «mstJfumas ofaversion. The third way to promote acceptance in Ainsworth, M. D., lit Eichberg. C. G. (1991). Effects on infant-mother attachment of mother's
coupl~s IS through t~8eting the consequences of aversion. This means drawing unresolved loss of an attachment 6gu~ on other traumatic life experience, In P. Marris. J.
attentIon to the negative consequences ofaversion behavior while reinforcing efforts Stevenson-Hinde:, at C. Parks (Eds.), Alllldnntni aaoss t1N /fit gde (pp.16Q.183). New York:
:m
to approach d maintain COnt~ct. Borrowed from Functional Analytic Psycho-
Roudedge.
Bartholomew, K., at Horowitz, L M. (1991). Attachment slY1C$ among young adults: A test ofa four-
ther~py(F~. Kohl~nberg& TS31, 1991), the goal ofthis strategy is to help shift the category modd. jOtmlal of /'monality ami Sotial PsydJoIolY. 61. 226-244.
conbngenclcs of relOforcement away from an automatic avoidance response and Bowlby, J. (l969). Alllldnntnt anJ IoSi. Vol I: AUildlmmt. London: Hoganh Pn:ss.
Bowlby, J. (1973). AlIaOJmml tmd [all, Vol ll· SrptlTll/ion. New York; Basic Books.
Bowlby,). (1980). AtlaOJmmt and klSl, Vol Ill· Loss. New York: Basic Books. Chapter 15
Bowlby,). (1988). A S«Itrr bast: PlUtllt-dJildmadnnmt anti hubhy hlDPt4n dtTJdopmmt. New York: Basic
Books.
Brennan, K. A, ac Shaver, P. R. (1995). Dimensions of attachment, affect regulation, and romantic
relationship functioning. Pm01lll1ity & Social Psychology BlIlletin, 21, 267-283.
Christensen, A., ac Jacobson, N. S. (1991). /n1tgrtIJiw IJtlurtJitJra/(ollple therapy. Unpublished treatment
Implications of Verbal Processes
manual, University of Washington.
Christensen, A., ac Jacobson, N. S. (2000). RmmcilAblt dilftmrUs. New York: Guilford. for Childhood Disorders:
Cordova, J. V. (2001). Acceptance in behavior therapy: Understanding the process of change. 17Je
BtlJllViqr AnA!Ysl, 24, 213-226. Tourette's Disorder, Attention Deficit
Cordova,). V. (2002). Behavior analysis and the scientific study ofcouples. TIN Be/JlrtJi",Ana[yst ToJay,
J, 412-420.
Cordova, J. V., ac Gee. C. B. (2001). Couple therapy for depression: Using healthy relationships to
Hyperactivity Disorder, and Autism
treat depression. In S. R. H. Bmh (Ed.), MtDil4J antifamily {11OmSt1 in tkpmsion (pp. 185.204).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Cordova,). V.,Jacobson. N. S., ac Christensen, A (1998). Acceptance vs. change in behavioral couples Michael P. Twohig, Steven C. Hayes, & Nicholas M. Berens
therapy: Impact on client communication processes in the therapy session. jollT7llZ1 of Marital
/lila Family 77mtlJ1:Y, 24, 437-455. Universi~ ofNevada, Reno
Cordova, J. V., ac Scott, R. L (2001). Intimacy; A behavioral interpretation. TIN &hmor Atrlllysl. 24,
74-86.
GeoJge, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1985). 17Je mJlIk tIJItldnnmt intmJinD. Unpublished manuscript, Direct contingency principles have, for understandable historical and ~rag­
University of California at BeJkcley. matic reasons dominated in the analysis ofchildhood problems from a behaVIoral
Harlow, H. F. (1958). The nature of love. A",mCillf Psychologist, 13, 673-685. perspective, ~d cognitive or verbal procc:sses have often been ignored..N.ew
Hayes, S. C., Barnes·Holmes. D., Be Roche. B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A precis. In S. C. generations ofbehavioral concepts that are linked to verbal processes are recelvmg
Hayes, D. Barnes·Holmes, 8( B. Roche (Eds.). lW4Jiotrlllftamt thtory: A post·S/citmtri4n ,,"ount
ofbllm41ll4ngllllU and cognition (pp. 141.156). New York: K1uwcr Academic!PIenum Publishers. more attention in the field, particularly Relational Frame Theory (~;. H~yes,
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. ]ollT7llZ1 of Barnes-Holmes, & Roche, 2001). This chapter examines some of the unplIcatl~ns
Pmona/ity anti Social Psychology, 53, 511-524. of verbal processes for contemporary behavioral models of chil~hood beh~v~or
Hesse, E., & Main, M. (2000). Disorganized infant. child, and adult attachment: Collapse in behavioral problems, specifically Tourette's Disorder (fD). Attet.ttion DefiC1~ Hyperactivity
and attentional strategies. jottmal of the AmtriCillf Psyc1Joan4fytk Associ4Jion, 48. 1097.1127.
Jacobson, N. S., 8c Christensen, A. (1996). Acaptana and chiDIge in couple therapy. New York: W. W. Disorder (ADHO), and Autism. This chapter Wilt consider the pOSSIble benefits of
Norton Be Company. incorporating these data and the principles the~ imply into o~r. ~8tjn8 bcha~jornl
Jacobson, N. 5., Christensen, A, Prince, S. E.. Cordova,). V., ac Eldridge, K. (2000). Integrative conceptualizations ofa set ofchildhood behaVIor problems. 1hIS chapter wdll?ot
behavioral couple therapy: An acceptance-based, promising new treatment for couple discord. stand alone, and assumes some degree of familiarity with this set ofconcepts, whu:h
jOllmtd of Constdting anti Cliniuzl PsydJoIogy, 28, 351.355.
Karney. B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1995). The longitudinal course of marital quality and stability: A are covered in the initial chapters ofthe book and elsewhere (e,g" Haycs ct nl., 2001).
review of theoty, methods, and research. A)'choiogitJsJ BtJldin, 118, 3-34. Tourette's Disorder
KoblenbeJg, R. }.• ac Tsai, M. (1991). Funaiotrlll iDlalylic psythotherl1py: Crrll/ing intmlt and CIlrtIJiw
thtrllJltlllic rr!4tionships. New York: Plenum. Tics are defmed as sudden, repetitive, stereotyped movements and vocolhm·
Main, M. (2000). The oJganized categories ofinfant, child, and adult attachment: Flexible vs. inflexible tions (American Psychiatric Association. 2000). Children with tic disor~crs Jl1~Y
attention under attachment-related streSS. Jollmlll of tht Amtrian Psycholl1Uliytic Assodation, 48, present with simple or complex tics. Simple tics are relatively sh~rt lind discrete In
1055-1096.
Main, M., Kapl3JI, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood and adulthood: A move duration such as a cough or a hard eye blInk. whereas complex bCS are much more
to the level of representation. Monographs oftht SocidyjOr &su.rth in Child Dt1Jtlopmmt, 50. 66- prolonged and may include tics such as twirling or sin~g. Tic. disorders ore
104.
categorized by tic type (motor or vocal), ~d ~e leng~h oftime the tiCS are present.
Scott, R. L (2002). 17Jt association Ittlftltm mJllllil/tlldmrml anti il/tribillionsfor romanticpllTtnm'ntgdliw The tic disorder is considered to be transient If the tics have been present for less
bthllfJior. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois at Urbana.Champaign.
Scott. R., ac Cordova,}. V. (2002). The influence ofadult attachment styles on the association between than one year, and chronic ifthe tics are present for over a year. I!both motor and
marital adjustment and depressive symptoms. jollT7llZ1 of Family Psychology, 16, 199-208. vocal tics are present, a diagnosis of TO is warranted. Motor ucs are those .t~at
involve contractions ofmuscle groups and vocal tics are those that involve repetitive
sounds or vocalizations, although the distinction is imprecise because vocal tics
certainly involve contractions of muscles necessary for vocalizations.

You might also like