You are on page 1of 14

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/8940298

The impact of self-hypnosis and Johrei on lymphocyte subpopulations at


exam time: A controlled study

Article  in  Brain Research Bulletin · January 2004


DOI: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2003.09.014 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

57 220

6 authors, including:

Akira Naito Tannis M Laidlaw


Sussex Partnership NHS Foundation Trust 52 PUBLICATIONS   1,536 CITATIONS   
8 PUBLICATIONS   216 CITATIONS   
SEE PROFILE
SEE PROFILE

Linda Farahani Prabuddh Dwivedi


King's College Hospital NHS Foundation Trust NELFT NHS Foundation Trust
1 PUBLICATION   57 CITATIONS    15 PUBLICATIONS   446 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Johrei and self hypnosis project, Imperial College London View project

Dissemination and cross fertilisation View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Akira Naito on 07 January 2019.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Brain Research Bulletin 62 (2003) 241–253

The impact of self-hypnosis and Johrei on lymphocyte


subpopulations at exam time: a controlled study
Akira Naito a,b,∗ , Tannis M. Laidlaw a , Don C. Henderson b ,
Linda Farahani a , Prabudha Dwivedi a , John H. Gruzelier a
a Department of Cognitive Neuroscience and Behaviour, Imperial College London, Charing Cross Campus, St Dunstan’s Road, London W6 8RF, UK
b Department of Immunology, Imperial College London, Chelsea and Westminster Campus, 369 Fulham Road, London SW10 9NH, UK

Received 9 June 2003; received in revised form 26 September 2003; accepted 26 September 2003

Abstract

In a prospective randomised controlled trial, 48 students were randomly assigned to stress reduction training before exams with self-hypnosis,
Johrei or a mock neurofeedback relaxation control. Peripheral blood lymphocyte subpopulations and self-reported stress (Perceived Stress
Scale) were measured before training and 1–2 months later as exams approached. Absolute number and percentages of CD3+ CD4+ and
CD3+ CD8+ T lymphocytes, CD3− CD56+ Natural Killer cells (NK cells) and NK cell cytotoxic activity was measured from venous blood.
Stressed participants showed small but significant declines in both CD3− CD56+ NK cell percentages and NK cell cytotoxic activity levels
while CD3+ CD4+ T cell percentages increased, changes supported by correlations with perceived stress. The effects of stress were moderated
in those who learned Johrei at exam time; 11/12 showed increases in CD3− CD56+ NK cell percentages with decreased percentages of
CD3+ CD4+ T cells, effects not seen in the relaxation control group. Stress was also buffered in those who learned and practised self-hypnosis
in whom CD3− CD56+ NK cell and CD3+ CD4+ T cell levels were maintained, and whose CD3+ CD8+ T cell percentages, shown previously
to decline with exams, increased. The results compliment beneficial effects on mood of self-hypnosis and Johrei. The results are in keeping
with beneficial influences of self-hypnosis and provide the first evidence of the suggestive value of the Japanese Johrei procedure for stress
reduction, which clearly warrants further investigation.
© 2003 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Stress; Hypnosis; Visualisation; Healing; Lymphocytes; Natural Killer cell (NK cell)

1. Introduction the stress that falls in the period between alarm reaction and
resistance in Selye’s model. Sustained or chronic stress is
The question whether psychological interventions can the period from resistance to exhaustion. The perception by
benefit health by influencing immune function [2,18,19,21] the subject of the significance of the “stressor” [46] is now
is arguably best understood in the context of psychological recognised as essential when the word ‘stress’ can mean
stress. Stress, according to the pioneering endocrinologist either the stressor or its perception [33]. Stress perception is
Hans Selye, is “the non-specific response of the body to any thought to be influenced by two capacities of appraisal [48],
demand” [47]. His General Adaptation Syndrome (GAS) which can be modified by psychological interventions. Pri-
specified three phases of stress: (1) an alarm reaction to mar- mary appraisal is the perception of the threat or demand in
shal resources; (2) a stage of resistance when resistance to the situation, i.e. ranging from irrelevant or minor through
stress rises above normal; and (3) a stage of exhaustion when to stressful. Secondary appraisal is the evaluation of one’s
adaptation energy has been consumed [45]. This roughly ability to cope with the threat or demand.
equates to a two-stage model of stress classifying reactions Stress has deleterious effects on health in many organ
to stressors as acute in contrast to sustained or chronic. systems including the immune system. The immune sys-
Acute, momentary or short-lasting stress can be defined as tem is closely aligned with the autonomic nervous system
(ANS) and recently has been shown to interact closely with
∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +44-20-8846-7042; other systems as well, especially the central nervous and en-
fax: +44-20-8846-1670. docrinological systems [39]. Evidence shows that there are
E-mail address: akira.naito@imperial.ac.uk (A. Naito). bi-directional influences between the brain and the immune

0361-9230/$ – see front matter © 2003 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2003.09.014
242 A. Naito et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 62 (2003) 241–253

system with two major pathways—Sympathetic Adrenal of the emotional distress of examinations in contrast to the
Modulation [12] and the Hypothalamus–Pituitary–Adrenal control mock neurofeedback relaxation condition. These
axis [9]. These involve soluble mediators such as cytokines advantages were unrelated to expectations about outcome
and hormones [36,41,54]. Clearly effects on immune param- before or after training.
eters will be complex because these pathways dynamically
interact with each other and with various homeostatic con-
trols through regulatory [13], buffering [37] and feedback 2. Methodology
mechanisms [42]. Furthermore, effects may vary depending
2.1. Subjects
on whether stress is acute or chronic.
Many aspects of the immune system are reported to be After approval from the Riverside Human Ethics Com-
altered by stress in healthy individuals, including T lympho- mittee, 48 healthy young participants were recruited with
cytes and Natural Killer cells (NK cells). In particular NK an age range 19–23 years with one participant of 37 years.
cells have been shown to be reduced by sustained stress [24] There were 22 males and 26 females of whom 39/48 were
in both number and in functional activity, and these two mea- medical students. Informed consent was obtained after they
sures have been found to be correlated [53]. As part of the were given detailed information sheets and then they were
stress response, CD3+ CD8+ T cell counts can also decrease randomly assigned to one of three interventions. Partici-
in parallel with CD3− CD56+ NK cells [21], while reports pants were given a simple breakfast after their blood had
vary as to the effects of stress on CD3+ CD4+ T cell counts: been drawn and paid £30 at the end of the study. In a
increased in chronic stress associated with burnout in nurses 1-month intervention period they attended weekly sessions
[7] or little change with the stress of exams in students [21]. to be trained in and subsequently to practice at home either
Acute stress has been mainly examined in experimental self-hypnosis or Johrei, or, as the control measure, to par-
settings in the laboratory using short-lasting anxiety reac- ticipate in eight ‘training’ sessions of ‘neurofeedback’. The
tions coupled with tasks such as mental arithmetic [56,59] data collected comprised baseline assessments from 47 par-
and public speaking [23]. Sustained stress reactions have of- ticipants: 16 each in hypnosis and Johrei, 15 in control (one
ten been examined in healthy populations using academic person’s blood collection missed due to non-attendance).
examinations as the stressor [8,16,18,20,21,27,28]. Post-training session I comprised 45 participants, 15 in
Among stress intervention techniques one approach has each group. Twenty-nine participants continued in the study
involved the use of self-hypnosis, which overall has shown for approximately another month and were re-examined at
considerable benefits in buffering the effects of stress that post-training session II (11 hypnosis, 10 Johrei, 8 control).
were manifested in control groups [21,28]. Hypnosis has By this time preparation for examinations, which were held
been widely used in clinical studies involving health and im- at various times over a 2-month period, had been expe-
mune function parameters [26,28], and it is arguably one of rienced by all participants. This variation in exam period
the more accepted therapies within complementary medicine meant that bloods from the exam condition were from ei-
[3]. Self-hypnosis will be applied in the present study along ther post-training sessions I or II. The distribution of the
with a Japanese laying on of hands method known as Johrei, participants according to intervention and exam period is
or ‘purification’, where the participant acts as both the prac- shown in Table 1. There were no demographic differences
titioner, who concentrates on the transmission of subtle en- between the groups, nor were there significant differences
ergies through the hand via the metaphor of light, and the for either hypnotisability (HGSHS:A, F = 0.64; P = ns)
recipient, who in turn visualises images of light received. or for absorption (the Jamieson version of the TAS, F =
Young and healthy students facing the stress of exams 1.61; P = ns) and further no post hoc differences using a
were chosen to examine the effects of self-hypnosis and least significant differences analysis. No participants in the
Johrei. For comparison purposes, a relaxation technique in- Johrei group had experienced or indeed heard of Johrei.
volving mock neurofeedback was chosen for the control Of those in the self-hypnosis group, many participants had
group, a procedure which has been shown to provide a valid heard lectures on hypnosis and several had experienced the
relaxation experience [11]. By controlling for expectations HGSHS:A in a classroom setting. None had been taught or
when the outcome of one intervention is compared with an- practiced self-hypnosis before. All participants in the mock
other, the placebo effect is effectively controlled. Similarly biofeedback control condition were naı̈ve to biofeedback.
as there is a relaxation component present in the mock neu- The proportion of participants in terms of exam versus
rofeedback technique as well as in self-hypnosis and Johrei, non-exam conditions, and between groups was almost equal.
relaxation is also effectively controlled. These two aspects Testing was done during the week prior to the participant’s
of the control condition, expectation and relaxation, enable first exam to provide data from the exam preparation period.
a stringent test to be made for self-hypnosis and Johrei stress
reduction practices. 2.2. Intervention groups
The effects of these psychological interventions on
mood have been published elsewhere [32]. In brief, both The self-hypnosis training, designed and run by an expe-
self-hypnosis and Johrei were successful in countering some rienced clinical hypnotist, T.L., consisted of 4 weekly ses-
A. Naito et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 62 (2003) 241–253 243

Table 1 group, PEI diaries were used to record their practice fre-
Numbers of participants in each intervention group with respect to con- quency and collect daily mood data.
dition of exams
The control participant group had eight mock neurofeed-
Condition Post-training I Post-training II n back sessions over 1 month. This condition appeared to
Exams 18 17 35 be extremely high-tech, with two computers and electrodes
Non-exams 27 12 39 fixed to earlobes and the centre of the scalp, and with au-
n 45 29 ditory feedback sounds (babbling brook and ocean waves)
Hypnosis
heard over headphones. The sounds were supposedly repre-
Exams 5 7 12 senting alpha and theta-wave feedback from the participant’s
Non-exams 10 4 14 brain, both waves associated with relaxation. Actually, the
n 15 11 sounds were recorded from another subject’s training ses-
sion. This procedure has been shown to generate a valid re-
Johrei
Exams 8 4 12
laxation response when used as a control condition [11].
Non-exams 7 6 13
2.3. Psychological measures
n 15 10
Relaxation control A series of mood measures was taken at the three assess-
Exams 5 6 11 ment points assessed by questionnaire.
Non-exams 10 2 12
n 15 8 2.3.1. Psychological testing
The Perceived Stress Scale (PSS [5]) was completed at the
time of the blood draws. The PSS is a well-used assessment
sions. The participants learnt firstly a Spiegel-type eye-roll scale designed to measure appraisals of stress with higher
for ‘instant’ relaxation, which could then be combined with scores meaning that the subject is experiencing more stress.
specific immune imagery. They were then taught a slower The PSS data at post-training session I were used to classify
relaxation-type induction, again to be combined with the all subjects into non-stressed or stressed groupings.
immune imagery, and all were provided with a standard
2.3.2. Personalised Emotional Index
tape-recording using a relaxation induction that included the
Personalised Emotional Index (diary [29]) was to be filled
imagery description. Further to the basic immune imagery,
out at the end of each day so that practice and mood data
all participants were taught two anxiety management tech-
could be collected. Data were collected for the first 4–6
niques: how to use breathing control for acute anxiety and
weeks of the project, after which diary completion was spo-
the Interrupt Distraction Procedure (IDP) for worries and
radic.
belief change to be used when appropriate [30]. It was re-
Two related tests to measure hypnotisability and absorp-
quested that each participant used self-hypnosis three-times
tion were used:
a day to learn the technique if possible for the first fortnight
and once a day after that. Diaries (Personalised Emotional Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility (HGSHS:
Index: PEI) were used to record their practice frequency and A [49]): the most frequently used group assessment tool
to collect daily mood data [29]. for hypnotic susceptibility over several decades [31].
The training of the Johrei healing method was planned Tellegan’s Absorption Scale (TAS [52]): the Jamieson ver-
and run by a trained practitioner from Japan, A.N., who was sion [25].
also medically qualified, with materials and support from the
British Johrei Society. The four training sessions involved 2.4. Immunological measures
an introduction to Johrei philosophy which emphasises the
importance of awareness of spiritual well-being, harmony Venous blood samples were collected in Vacutainers (Bec-
and balance, aesthetics, and nature farming practices, and ton Dickinson, Oxford, UK). All samples were collected be-
the core principles needed to practise the Johrei techniques, tween 8.00 and 10.00 a.m. prior to breakfast to avoid diurnal
such as ‘healing oneself by healing others’. In essence, the effects [38].
practitioner imagines light entering his body being concen-
trated through their hands towards the recipient. The prac- 2.4.1. Lymphocyte profiles
titioner, without touching the recipient, slowly moves their The absolute lymphocyte count was derived from the
hands from head down to kidney area, front and back. The flow cytometric assessment of lymphocyte subsets using
procedure takes approximately 15 min and is mostly silent. two standard panels of fluorescently tagged antibodies, i.e.
The participants were requested to practise Johrei daily with CD45/CD3/CD4/CD8 and CD45/CD3/CD19/CD56 (Beck-
a partner, but at the end of training, self-Johrei was intro- man Coulter, Bedfordshire, UK). Ten microlitres of each
duced as a supplement so that participants could still prac- monoclonal antibody cocktail was added to 100 ␮l of whole
tice even if a partner was not available. As in the hypnosis blood according to the manufacturer’s instructions. Cell sur-
244 A. Naito et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 62 (2003) 241–253

Table 2
Methodological accountability when measuring lymphocyte profiles of CD4+, CD8+ (T) and CD56+ (NK) cells using a single volunteer (nine tubes
collected and averaged on three separate occasions)
CD4% (S.D.) CD8% (S.D.) CD56% (S.D.) CD4 count CD8 count CD56 count Total lymph
(S.D.) (S.D.) (S.D.) count (S.D.)
Time 1 48.1 (0.97) 29.6 (0.73) 9.7 (0.54) 438.4 (12.71) 270.0 (11.27) 94.8 (9.27) 894.3 (31.97)
Time 2 46.8 (0.89) 28.8 (0.33) 8.3 (1.31) 346.1 (10.69) 213.4 (9.79) 65.6 (12.57) 712.6 (36.17)
Time 3 44.4 (0.52) 28.2 (0.60) 14.3 (0.72) 381.0 (36.36) 249.3 (11.59) 130.3 (8.69) 859.2 (33.60)
Mean %C.V. 1.7 1.9 8.8 5.2 4.5 11.9 4.2
Mean percentages or counts of lymphocyte subpopulations, S.D., or mean of % coefficient of variation (%C.V.) are shown.

face marker expression was analysed on a Coulter Epics The instrument was set for two-colour analysis using FAC-
Profile II Flow Cytometer. Scomp software in conjunction with Calibrite beads (Beck
In order to assess methodological variation in lym- Dickinson). Data were collected in list mode and analysis
phocyte profiles, three preliminary blood samples from was performed using CellQuest software (Beck Dickinson).
one volunteer were assessed. Nine sample tubes of blood A region was set on a dot plot of SSC versus fluorescence 1
were taken at each time point, and assessed for per- (fl 1) around the cluster of target cells identified in the con-
centages of CD45+ CD3+ CD4+ (henceforth CD4+) and trol sample tubes and percentages of PI-stained cell number
CD45+ CD3+ CD8+ (CD8+) T cells and CD45+ CD3− in the region were counted. NK cell cytotoxic activity was
CD56+ (CD56+) NK cells. Means and ranges were within calculated by subtraction of target total from individual to-
normal limits and all standard deviations were less than 1% tals. All samples were tested in duplicate. Due to technical
with inter-assay coefficient variations (%C.V.) of 1.7, 1.9, difficulties no NK cell activity data were available at base-
8.8%, respectively (Table 2). line. NK cell cytotoxic activity was examined by flow cy-
tometry using Godoy-Ramirez’s method [17]. However, it
2.4.2. Natural Killer cell cytotoxic activity should be noted that the NK cell activity numbers here ap-
A flow cytometric method was used [17]. Effector cells pear to be relatively lower than noted in her paper.
(Peripheral Blood Mononuclear Cells (PBMCs)): PBMCs
were separated by density gradient centrifugation using 2.5. Statistics
Lymphoprep (Sigma, Dorset, UK). The effector cells were
maintained in Tissue Culture Medium (TCM: RPMI1640) The results of the three groups were compared with
supplemented with l-glutamine, penicillin, streptomycin, repeated-measures ANOVA followed by paired compar-
Hepes buffer, essential amino acids, and sodium pyruvate isons with non-parametric tests [50]. These were undertaken
(Sigma, Dorset, UK). Viability was assessed by dye exclu- from two viewpoints: First, the effects of the exam stressor
sion and the cell concentration adjusted to 5 × 106 cells/ml. were compared with baseline by pooling subjects according
Ten microlitres of anti-human CD45 monoclonal antibody to their exam period from post-training sessions I and II. In
(Becton Dickinson, Oxford, UK) directly conjugated with the ANOVA, the between-subject factor was Intervention
fluorescein isothiocyanate (FITC) was added to each tube (Hypnosis, Johrei, Control) and the within-subject factor
containing 100 ␮l of effector cells. was Session (Baseline, Exam). Secondly, the interventions
Target cells (K562 cell line): K562 cells were maintained were compared for their effects at post-training I with sub-
in long-term culture at 2 × 105 cells/ml in TCM supple- jects subdivided as high or low on perceived stress accord-
mented with 10% foetal calf serum (FCS). Viability was ing to a median split on the PSS. Those perceiving more
assessed by dye exclusion and cell concentrations were ad- stress (n = 20) had PSS scores at or above 22 and those
justed to 1 × 105 cells/ml. perceiving less stress (n = 21) with scores below 22. The
Assay procedure: Effector and target cells, a graded num- ANOVA included two between-subject factors, Intervention
ber of fresh PBMCs and K562 cells, were added in dupli- (3) and perceived stress (2), and a within-subject factor
cate in 12 mm × 75 mm round-bottom tubes (Beck Dickin- of Session (2). Intervention effects were not examined at
son, Oxford, UK) to yield the Effector to Target (E:T) ratio post-training session II due to the reduction in participant
of 50:1, 25:1, 12.5:1. Control tubes including only target or numbers.
effector cells were assayed to determine spontaneous cell All analyses were performed with the Statistical Package
death. Tubes were mixed by gently tapping, and incubated for Social Sciences, version 11.0 (SPSS 11.0® ) [14].
at 37 ◦ C for 3 h. After incubation, tubes were placed on ice
until analysed. Twenty microlitres of Propidium Iodide (PI)
(Sigma) was added to each tube 15 min before acquisition 3. Results
in order to stain dead K562 cells.
Flow cytometric acquisition: Flow cytometry was per- Means and standard deviations for the three groups are
formed with a FACS Calibur cytometer (Becton Dickinson). shown in Table 3 for the various immune variables at base-
A. Naito et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 62 (2003) 241–253 245

Table 3
Mean total lymphocyte counts which is labelled as total counts, and mean percentages of CD4+ T cells, CD8+ T cells and CD56+ NK cells (S.D.)
Baseline Post-training I Post-training II Exams
All Total counts 1990.7 (549) 2055.7 (596) 2060.0 (536) 2062.1 (558.2)
CD4+% 46.0 (5.8) 46.1 (7.9) 45.3 (7.7) 45.2 (6.8)
CD8+% 26.1 (5.4) 26.8 (5.5) 27.3 (5.9) 27.5 (5.5)
CD56+% 9.7 (4.5) 9.6 (5.2) 8.8 (4.8) 8.9 (4.6)
n 47 45 29 33
Hypnosis Total counts 1959.2 (408) 1923.5 (545) 1958.4 (556) 2056.8 (500.6)
CD4+% 44.4 (5.9) 45.8 (8.2) 45.8 (8.3) 45.6 (8.4)
CD8+% 25.5 (6.6) 26.7 (5.8) 27.8 (7.0) 28.0 (6.4)
CD56+% 10.7 (4.4) 9.3 (5.7) 8.2 (4.2) 9.1 (4.9)
n 16 15 11 11
Johrei Total counts 2123.7 (788) 2213.7 (709) 2243.1 (631) 2195.5 (642.5)
CD4+% 47.0 (5.9) 44.6 (8.5) 44.8 (8.9) 43.1 (9.0)
CD8+% 25.7 (5.0) 26.5 (6.3) 26.2 (6.1) 27.6 (5.5)
CD56+% 8.9 (5.3) 11.7 (5.6) 10.0 (6.3) 10.8 (4.6)
n 15 15 10 11
Relaxation Total counts 1882.5 (334) 2029.9 (519.7) 1950.7 (347) 1934.5 (550.0)
CD4+% 46.4 (5.6) 48.0 (7.0) 45.2 (6.3) 46.9 (7.0)
CD8+% 27.0 (4.3) 27.3 (4.7) 28.2 (4.6) 26.9 (4.9)
CD56+% 9.4 (3.7) 7.7 (3.6) 7.9 (3.2) 6.7 (3.4)
n 15 15 8 11
Counts are expressed as cells per microlitre of blood.

line, post-training sessions I and II, with the post-training that the change of percentages also reflected the individual
sessions also subdivided according to exams. lymphocyte subset absolute count change.
All lymphocyte counts from these healthy students were
within normal ranges; see also Table 2. There were no sta- 3.1. Intervention effects on exam stress: baseline versus
tistical differences in mean lymphocyte counts (total lym- exams
phocytes, CD4+, CD8+, CD56+ cells) between the three
intervention groups at baseline. Nor were there statistical 3.1.1. Natural Killer cells
changes in numbers of total lymphocyte counts from base- Change scores for all participants are shown in Fig. 1.
line to post-training I or to the exam condition. Accordingly, Johrei appeared to buffer the decline in CD56+ NK cells
all the changes that occurred were proportional changes of where there were no consistent effects with the other inter-
the various lymphocyte subpopulations within the total so ventions. This was confirmed by ANOVA where there was

14

12

10

8
Mean +- 1 SE

6 NKC% Baseline

4 NKC% Exams
N= 11 11 11 11 11 11
Hypnosis Johrei Relaxation control

GROUP

Fig. 1. Means (S.E.) of CD56+ Natural Killer cell percentages from baseline to exams.
246 A. Naito et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 62 (2003) 241–253

20 20 20
NK cell percentages

10 10 10

0 0 0
NKC% Baseline NKC NKC %1 NKC NKC% Baseline NKC

Johrei Hypnosis Relaxation Control

Fig. 2. Effect of exam stress on peripheral blood CD56+ Natural Killer cell percentages in individual practising various stress reduction regimens with
respect to each intervention group.

no main effect of session but instead there was a significant P = 0.063), while paired group comparisons showed that
Intervention × Session effect (F(1, 33) = 5.86; P = 0.007). the extent of change was greater in the hypnosis group than
This differential pattern of change was confirmed by paired in the relaxation group (z = 2.40; P = 0.016), but not
comparisons using Wilcoxon Signed Ranks tests. Only the when the hypnosis group was compared to the Johrei group
Johrei group showed an increase in CD56+ NK cell percent- (z = 1.60). Exploratory non-parametric tests within groups
ages (mean change = 3.22; z(1, 11) = 2.94; P = 0.003), also confirmed that the change in the hypnosis group was
whereas the hypnosis group maintained levels (mean = significant (z(1, 11) = 2.13; P = 0.033).
−0.36; z(1, 11) = 0.22) and the control group showed a
non-significant decline (mean = −2.14; z(1, 11) = 1.42). 3.1.3. CD4+ T cells
The advantages for Johrei were further demonstrated by As can be seen in Fig. 3, there was no main effect of ses-
Mann–Whitney U tests comparing the extent of the changes sion but there was a differential pattern of group changes
from baseline in the Johrei group over both the relaxation from baseline to exams. This was confirmed by a signifi-
group (z = 2.73; P = 0.005) and the hypnosis group (z = cant Intervention×Session effect in an ANOVA (F(2, 32) =
2.37; P = 0.016). It is noteworthy that Johrei buffered the 4.71; P = 0.016) where the groups were ordered as fol-
decline in CD56+ NK cell percentages in all subjects bar lows: Control > Hypnosis > Johrei. The relaxation group
one, whereas there was no consistent direction of change in showed a non-significant increase in CD4+ T cell percent-
the other groups, as illustrated in Fig. 2. 14/15 Johrei sub- ages (mean = 1.63; z = 1.25), there was little change with
jects showed an increase in their NK cell percentages over hypnosis (mean = 0.10; z = 0.13), whereas with Johrei
the course of the intervention (binomial test < 0.001), with there was a marginal decline in CD4+ T cells (mean =
the one outlier 4.7 standard deviations below the mean, thus −3.44; z(1, 11) = 1.82, P = 0.068). Mann–Whitney U tests
influencing results. Accordingly, all statistics were computed comparing groups for the extent of change confirmed that
with this outlier changed to just below the other subjects due to the increase in CD4+ T cell percentages, the control
[50]. group differed from the Johrei group (z = 2.07, P = 0.040)
although not from the hypnosis group (z = 0.77).
3.1.2. CD8+ T cells
The hypnosis group showed an increase (mean change = 3.1.4. Perceived stress
2.26) with little change in the other groups (Table 3). A repeated measures ANOVA on the perceived stress
This was confirmed in an ANOVA using change scores. scores indicated that participants were significantly more
A group effect approached significance (F(1, 33) = 3.02; stressed (F(1, 44) = 4.55; P = 0.045) during the exam
A. Naito et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 62 (2003) 241–253 247

50

48

46

44

42
Mean +- 1 SE

CD4% Baseline
40

38 CD4% Exams
N= 11 11 11 11 11 11

Hypnosis Johrei Relaxation control

GROUP
Fig. 3. Mean (S.E.) percentages of CD4+ T cells at baseline and exams for each group.

preparation time (mean = 26.2) than a month before or after ing a beneficial effect of Johrei independent of perceived
(mean = 21.5). stress, while in the control group there were no significant
changes (stressed: n = 6, z = 0.67; non-stressed: n = 6,
3.2. The influence of perceived stress on intervention z = 0.52).
effects: baseline versus post-training session I
3.2.2. Natural Killer cell cytotoxic activity
The means and standard deviations are shown in Table 3. The measures were taken post-training due to techni-
Groups were compared with ANOVA with stress (2) in- cal difficulties with baseline assays. The pattern of results
tervention (3) and session (2) as factors. We note that all post-training was similar to that of the CD56+ NK cell per-
three-way interaction results with intervention groups sub- centages. In an ANOVA with stress (2) and intervention (3)
divided by stress need to be interpreted with caution due to as between-group factors, there was a significant stress ef-
small sample sizes. fect (F(1, 40) = 5.29; P = 0.028); post-training the stressed
group had lower NK cell activity (mean = 5.75) compared
3.2.1. Natural Killer cell percentages with the non-stressed group (mean = 11.96). There was no
In an ANOVA, there was a tendency towards an inter- interaction with intervention (F = 0.04). Exploratory com-
action between stress and session (F(1, 39) = 3.27; P = parisons within intervention groups with Wilcoxon Signed
0.079) whereby those who were more stressed (henceforth Rank tests indicated a significant difference in the hypnosis
the stressed group) showed decreased CD56+ NK cell per- group with the non-stressed group (n = 6, mean = 14.5)
centages (n = 20, mean = −0.96, F = 1.62) and those who having significantly more NK cytotoxic activity than the
were less stressed (the non-stressed group) showed an in- stressed group (n = 6, mean = 3.4) (t = 2.25, P = 0.025).
crease (n = 20, mean = 1.32). There was also a significant
interaction between intervention and session (F = 5.76; 3.2.3. NK cell percentage to NK cytotoxic activity ratio
P = 0.007) and a tendency towards a further interaction The actual cytotoxic activity per NK cell was calculated
with intervention, session and stress (F = 2.70; P = 0.082). using the ratio of NK cell activity to CD56+ NK cell per-
As confirmed by Wilcoxon Signed Rank tests, the hypnosis centages. As can be seen in Fig. 4, the stressed group had a
group showed a decline in CD56+ NK cell percentages in lower NK cell activity to NK cell percentage ratio (mean =
the stressed group (n = 8, z = 2.52, P = 0.008) with no 0.64, S.E. = 0.19) compared with the non-stressed group
significant change in the non-stressed group (n = 7, z = (mean = 1.44, S.E. = 0.2), a significant difference (t =
0.85), implying no moderation of perceived stress in those 2.38, P = 0.022).
who were more highly stressed. With Johrei there were in- There was a significantly positive correlation between the
creases in both stressed (n = 6, z = 2.02, P = 0.062), and percentage of CD56+ NK cells and NK cell activity (r =
non-stressed (n = 7, z = 2.37, P = 0.014) groups, suggest- 0.52, P = 0.001, n = 44).
248 A. Naito et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 62 (2003) 241–253

1.8 are shown in Fig. 5. Paired comparisons disclosed that the


stressed group showed increased CD4+ T cell percentages
1.6 post-training (mean = 1.79, F(1, 19) = 4.14; P = 0.056),
Mean +- 1 SE NKC Activity / NKC% ratio

whereas the non-stressed group showed a non-significant de-


1.4 crease (F = 1.54). There was also a significant interaction
between intervention and session (F = 3.35; P = 0.047)
1.2 and a further interaction tendency between stress, interven-
tion and session (F = 2.78; P = 0.077). As confirmed by
1.0
Wilcoxon Signed Rank tests, the hypnosis group showed
little change in those who were less stressed (n = 7, z =
.8
1.36) although there was a tendency for a rise in CD4+ T
cell percentages in those who were more stressed (n = 8,
z = 1.89, P = 0.065). The opposite effects were found
.6
in the control group with a significant increase in the less
stressed group (n = 6, z = 2.20, P = 0.027) and no change
.4
in the more stressed group (n = 6, z = 0.63). With Johrei
.2 there was a fall in the less stressed group (n = 7, z = 1.86,
N= 20 21 P = 0.072) and no change in the stressed group (n = 7,
Non-stressed Stressed z = 1.36).
PSS midline split
Consistent with these findings Perceived Stress scores
were positively correlated with the increase in CD4+ T cell
Fig. 4. NK cell activity per cell (calculated as a ratio: NK cell activity/ percentages (n = 40, r = 0.443, P = 0.004), due primarily
CD56+ NK cell percentage). to the correlation found in the hypnosis group (r = 0.816,
P < 0.000).
3.2.4. CD8+ T cells
In the ANOVA, there were no significant main effects or 3.3. Hypnotisability and absorption
interactions with respect to stress, intervention and session.
There were no correlations for the group as a whole
3.2.5. CD4+ T cells between the change scores for the various immunological
In the ANOVA, there was a significant interaction be- variables and either the HGSHS:A or TAS Summary of
tween stress and session (F = 4.35; P = 0.044). Effects Sub-scales scores.

50

48

46

44
Mean +- 1 SE

42 CD4% Baseline

40 CD4% Post-training 1
N= 20 20 20 20
Non-stressed Stressed

PSS midline split


Fig. 5. Changes in CD4+ T cell percentages by stress grouping based upon PSS regardless of treatment group.
A. Naito et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 62 (2003) 241–253 249

3.4. Beliefs about the efficacy of the interventions CD45 [35], and both CD16 and CD56 were included as NK
cells [7,55]. In keeping with random assignment to the inter-
After the intervention, subjects were asked how they rated vention groups no statistical differences existed at baseline.
their belief in/attitude towards the efficacy of their interven- Differences became apparent after training in all three inter-
tion before it started and again when they finished. These vention methods—self-hypnosis, Johrei and the neurofeed-
beliefs did not vary significantly between the three groups back control, with each intervention having a characteristic
either before or after the intervention, although there was profile, as shown in Fig. 6. Further, those participants who
some swing from belief to non-belief and vice versa in all perceived themselves to be stressed displayed a different
groups. No correlations could be found between belief rat- profile from those who were comparatively unstressed.
ings before or after the intervention and results in any im-
munological or psychological outcome measure for any of 4.1. Perceived stress
the three groups analysed separately. Interestingly, although
belief before their intervention was uncorrelated with any First, considering perceived stress, this was associated
other variables, belief in their intervention after experiencing with the following profile after a month or more of train-
their training was positively correlated with hypnotisability ing/practice:
in the hypnosis group (r = 0.66; P = 0.008), and absorp-
(a) a decline in the percentage of CD56+ Natural Killer
tion in the Johrei (r = 0.61, P = 0.013) and the relaxation
cells, lower NK cell cytotoxic activity and a lower ratio
control groups (r = 0.65, P = 0.015).
between CD56+ NK percentages and cytotoxic activity
in the stressed group compared with the non-stressed
group,
4. Discussion (b) an increase in CD4+ T cell percentages together with
a positive correlation with percentage of CD4+ T cells,
At baseline, the lymphocyte subpopulation for all 48 and a higher mean percent in the stressed than the
healthy young volunteers were within the normal reference non-stressed group (Fig. 5),
ranges for counts and percentages. The degree of changes (c) few differences in the CD8+ T cell percentages.
in percentages following training and at exam time, were
small, but comparable with similar studies in the literature The results with perceived stress and CD56+ NK cells
using healthy students and examinations as a naturalistic were in line with expectations, given there is substantive evi-
stressor [19,21,22,27,35,55]. Other stress studies finding dence for more stressed participants having significantly less
similar fluctuations used slightly different immunopheno- CD3− CD16+ /CD3− CD56+ NK cells in their bloodstream
typing: CD2 was used to gate T and NK cells instead of compared to those with less perceived sustained stress [7].

-2
Exam: CD4 % change

Exam: CD8 % change


Mean

-4 Exam: NKC % change


Hypnosis Johrei Relaxation Control

GROUP
Fig. 6. Profile of change scores for the hypnosis, Johrei and relaxation control groups from baseline to the time of exams showing unique profiles for
all three groups.
250 A. Naito et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 62 (2003) 241–253

Similarly, the NK cell cytotoxic activity and also cytotoxic 4.2.2. Self-hypnosis
activity per NK cell, calculated as a NK cell percentage to In the case of those practising self-hypnosis, the results
NK cell activity ratio, were found to be lower with per- were perhaps less interesting than those with Johrei. In terms
ceived stress. NK cell percentages and cytotoxic activity of the perceived stress score, hypnosis did buffer the decline
are often together or separately used as stress benchmarks in CD56+ NK cells in the non-stressed participants but not
[10,34,44,58]. in the stressed group. On average hypnosis buffered the de-
Considering perceived stress and the T-lymphocyte mark- cline in CD56+ NK cells at exam time, in keeping with pre-
ers CD4+ and CD8+, given the small changes discernible vious studies [20,21], though here without producing the in-
in healthy people, there is no clear picture of what indicates crease in CD56+ NK cell percentages that followed Johrei
a ‘strong and effective’ immune system, other than in com- training. On the other hand CD8+ T cell percentages were
parison to that of an ill person. The picture is further clouded not only maintained with hypnosis in replication of previous
when stress is involved, as stress differentially affects the results [15,20,21], but here there was a significant increase
numbers and percentages of the various lymphocytes. Our in the paired t-test results. In previous reports, the benefi-
CD4+ T cell results may illustrate this. Those participants cial effects of hypnosis on levels of lymphocytes were se-
expressing a higher level of perceived stress had a signifi- lective either to NK cells (labelled as CD3− CD56+ or/and
cantly higher CD4+ T cell percentage at the second blood CD3− CD16+ ) only [4], or to both CD56+ NK cells and
draw. This suggests that perceiving a situation as stressful CD8+ T cells [21].
could be expressed immunologically by releasing CD4+ T However, one apparent discrepancy between the self-
cells into the circulation, perhaps an alarm reaction or a stage hypnosis group results here and an earlier exam stress study
of resistance using Selye’s classifications. The increase in must be noted. Whereas the positive relation with CD4+ T
CD4+ T cell percentages in stressed participants is consis- cell percentages and perceived stress was stronger in the
tent with previous studies, and could suggest that the ampli- hypnosis group, in the earlier study an increase following
fication is of activated CD4+ T cells, i.e. CD25+ CD4+ T self-hypnosis training in CD4+ T cells at exam time cor-
cells, as was found in nurses with chronic work stress [7]. related with the increase in calmness ratings in those with
In summary, the ecological validity of the examina- self-hypnosis training. One implication was that hypnosis
tions acting as a stressor was supported by both the sig- may alter relations between stress and immune variables
nificant increase in PSS scores when compared with the [18,21] just as it may alter relations between cortical elec-
pre-examination period baseline, and the profile of immune trophysiological responses to pain and ratings of distress
results. A decrease in CD56+ NK cells and NK cytotoxic [6] presumably through effects on hormones. The reason
activity was associated with an increase in perceived stress, for the disparate results in the hypnosis group here is un-
a result in keeping with the well-documented decline in NK clear, though the hypnosis training in the earlier studies
cells under conditions of exam stress. CD4+ T cell counts consisted of imagery training of the immune system and
on the other hand increased, and the increase correlated ego strengthening without any mention of stress, whereas
positively with perceived stress. here hypnotic stress reduction techniques were also taught
in a more eclectic approach. Subjects were given a tape
4.2. Intervention profiles with identical wording to that used in previous studies,
but they could use non-tape self-hypnosis for stress if they
4.2.1. Johrei so wished. Thus, they could emphasise the stress reduc-
In terms of the intervention profiles, given the very clear tion aspects and this may have been at the expense of the
relation between stress and NK cells found here, one which immune-imagery training aspect of the intervention. The
is well documented in the literature [7,8,16,21,22,27], per- latter is important because we have previously demonstrated
haps the most interesting result from this study was the in- that relatively small changes in the wording of hypnosis
crease in the percentage of CD56+ NK cells in the Johrei instructions result in quite different immune profiles; for
group at exam time. This result, summarised in Figs. 1 and example, immune imagery appeared superior to relaxation
2, was apparent within the group over time, and it was also imagery in the series of exam stress studies [18].
evident when the Johrei percentages were compared to the
self-hypnosis and relaxation control groups. At the same 4.2.3. Control
time, although statistically significant, caution should be ex- The relaxation control group in comparison to those
pressed in that the changes are relatively small, and a test practising Johrei experienced higher levels of CD4+ T cell
sample of blood processed in separate batches will produce percentages. Overall however, the relaxation control group
some fluctuation of results, as shown in Table 2. Neverthe- showed fewer significant results than either the Johrei or
less, 14 of 15 Johrei participants showed raised percentages self-hypnosis groups. This is all the more interesting because
of CD56+ NK cells in their peripheral blood. In concert with of the complex nature of the control group. It was set up to
the increase in CD56+ NK cells, the Johrei group showed control for expectations and the placebo effect by the use
a decline in CD4+ T cell percentages, also congruent with of an elaborate and pseudo-scientific apparatus to achieve
a possible stress reduction relationship. relaxation, yet using a procedure that has been shown to
A. Naito et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 62 (2003) 241–253 251

actually produce a good relaxation response as measured by dices of stress. Those who learned self-hypnosis increased
Thayer activation and deactivation self-report ratings [11]. their CD8+ T cell percentages; shown to decline with exam
Interestingly, although the CD56+ NK cell percentages of stress in previous studies, and on average maintained their
the relaxation control group were lower than the hypnosis CD56+ NK cell and CD4+ T cell percentages over exams
group, the decline in CD56+ NK cell percentages did not and all results are in line with other studies of students expe-
reach significance, suggesting that relaxation could be con- riencing the stress of exams. On comparison, the differences
tributing to the maintenance of NK cell percentages during between the groups were sufficient to suggest that Johrei
exams as has been found in other studies [27]. does not appear to be an elaborate form of hypnosis. Further,
there is more to both hypnosis and Johrei than relaxation.
4.3. Beliefs and hypnotisability/absorption The mock neurofeedback relaxation control condition pro-
vided a control against the placebo effect, so that any com-
In the hypnosis group, belief in their intervention was parisons between results from Johrei or self-hypnosis to the
correlated with hypnotisability scores, possibly reflecting relaxation controls that are significant are in all probability
the positive sensations experienced during hypnosis for over and above placebo. Moreover, expectations about the
those with higher susceptibility. Absorption was correlated efficacy of the various interventions either before or after
with belief scores for Johrei and the relaxation control training were unrelated to mood or lymphocyte subpopula-
group. Again, as those with higher absorption abilities par- tion outcomes.
ticipated in their interventions, belief in their intervention These results coupled with the benefits for mood [32]
possibly reflects their conscious perception of becoming are suggestive that there is a possible role for Johrei and
absorbed. self-hypnosis in moderating the effects of a stressor and,
There were a number of limitations to the study. Subject in effect, acting as tools for stress management. Although
numbers dropped off by the time of the second set of ex- self-hypnosis has been studied in the past, this is the first
ams leaving a total of 35, who fortunately were distributed time that lymphocyte subpopulation results from a non-touch
evenly across the intervention groups. Although our student healing method have been put to the test. Johrei may be
subjects claimed to be practicing their interventions, it was rightly considered as a positive agent for change. Other ap-
not possible to monitor this by post-training session II as proaches similar to Johrei include distant healing [51], exter-
many had ceased completing their diaries. A further lim- nal Qi Gong [40], Therapeutic Touch [43], Reiki [57], and
itation occurred with the assessment of NK cell activity. Spiritual healing [1]. However, Johrei may be unique in that
Godoy-Ramirez’s assessment method [17] was chosen be- participants appear to benefit when acting either as practi-
cause it avoids having to use a radioactive substance in the tioner or receiver. More research on the efficacy of Johrei
laboratory; the usual 51 Cr method is radioactive. Because and hypnosis in respect to immune and endocrine dynamics,
Godoy-Ramirez’s paper suggested there was no difference i.e. stress hormone and other cellular and humoral molecular
between a 3 and 4-h incubation time we opted for 3-h incu- interactions, is needed to test this intriguing result further.
bation. However, given that we obtained smaller values than
expected with the 3-h method, perhaps 4 h may be prefer-
able. It appears that more work needs to be done if this use- Acknowledgements
ful non-radioactive methodology can be fully accepted as a
reliable measurement of NK cell activity that is comparable Appreciation to our student subjects and especially for all
to the radio-assay. the work contributed by the other B.Sc. student researchers
Finally, despite other life stressors that may beset student on this project: Catriona Lynch, Nick Enzor, Christine Brin-
life and which have sometimes compromised other attempts cat, Tom French and Helena Marconell.
to use examinations as a stressor (reviewed in [19]), in this
study there is clear evidence that examinations did act as a
real life stressor over and above other life vicissitudes [32]. References
One criticism of all studies that use exam stress as a natural-
istic stressor is that anxieties are considered to be synony- [1] N.C. Abbot, E.F. Harkness, C. Stevinson, F.P. Marshall, D.A. Conn,
mous with a negative experience, and patently this is untrue E. Ernst, Spiritual healing as a therapy for chronic pain: a randomized,
clinical trial, Pain 91 (2001) 79–89.
for all students. Using perceived stress rather than relying
[2] R. Ader, On the development of psychoneuroimmunology, Eur. J.
on a naturalistic stressor such as exams, though subjective Pharmacol. 405 (2000) 167–176.
is arguably a useful supplement. [3] N. Afari, D.M. Eisenberg, R. Herrell, J. Goldberg, E. Kleyman,
In sum, stressed participants decreased their CD56+ NK S. Ashton, D. Buchwald, Use of alternative treatments by chronic
cell percentages and their NK cell cytotoxic activity levels fatigue syndrome discordant twins, Integr. Med. 2 (1999) 97–
and increased their CD4+ T cell percentages. At the time of 103.
[4] A.C. Bakke, M.Z. Purtzer, P. Newton, The effect of hypnotic-guided
exams, participants who learned Johrei showed increases in imagery on psychological well-being and immune function in pa-
CD56+ NK cell percentages and decreases in their CD4+ T tients with prior breast cancer, J. Psychosom. Res. 53 (2002) 1131–
cell percentages, the opposite pattern shown here to be in- 1137.
252 A. Naito et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 62 (2003) 241–253

[5] S. Cohen, T. Kamarch, R. Mermelstein, A global measure of per- [26] J.K. Kiecolt-Glaser, R. Glaser, Psychoneuroimmunology: can psy-
ceived stress, J. Health Social Behav. 24 (1983) 385–396. chological interventions modulate immunity? J. Consult. Clin. Psy-
[6] R.J. Croft, J.D. Williams, C. Haenschel, J.H. Gruzelier, Pain per- chol. 60 (1992) 569–575.
ception, hypnosis and 40 Hz oscillations, Int. J. Psychophysiol. 46 [27] J.K. Kiecolt-Glaser, R. Glaser, E.C. Strain, J.C. Stout, K.L. Tarr, J.E.
(2002) 101–108. Holliday, C.E. Speicher, Modulation of cellular immunity in medical
[7] V. De Gucht, B. Fischler, C. Demanet, Immune dysfunction associ- students, J. Behav. Med. 9 (1986) 5–21.
ated with chronic professional stress in nurses, Psychiatry Res. 85 [28] J.K. Kiecolt-Glaser, P.T. Marucha, C. Atkinson, R. Glaser, Hypnosis
(1999) 105–111. as a modulator of cellular immune dysregulation during acute stress,
[8] R. Deinzer, N. Schuller, Dynamics of stress-related decrease of sali- J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 69 (2001) 674–682.
vary immunoglobulin A (sIgA): relationship to symptoms of the [29] T.M. Laidlaw, Designer testing: using subjects’ personal vocabulary
common cold and studying behavior, Behav. Med. 23 (1998) 161– to produce individualised tests, Pers. Individual Differences 27 (1999)
169. 1197–1207.
[9] F.S. Dhabhar, B.S. McEwen, Bidirectional effects of stress and glu- [30] T.M. Laidlaw, The interrupt distraction procedure: a brief hypnotic
cocorticoid hormones on immune function: possible explanations for intervention for belief change and diminishing distress, Am. J. Clin.
paradoxical observations, in: N. Cohen (Ed.), Psychoneuroimmunol- Hypn. 42 (1999) 22–34.
ogy, 3rd ed., Academic Press, San Diego, USA, 2001, pp. 483– [31] T.M. Laidlaw, R.G. Large, The Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic
497. Susceptibility and the Creative Imagination Scale: defining two sep-
[10] J.M. Dopp, G.E. Miller, H.F. Myers, J.L. Fahey, Increased natural arate but correlated abilities, Contemp. Hypn. 14 (1997) 26–37.
killer-cell mobilization and cytotoxicity during marital conflict, Brain [32] T.M. Laidlaw, A. Naito, P. Dwivedi, N.A. Enzo, C.E. Brincat, J.H.
Behav. Immun. 14 (2000) 10–26. Gruzelier, Mood changes after self-hypnosis and the Johrei healing
[11] T. Egner, E. Strawson, J.H. Gruzelier, EEG signature and phe- method prior to exams, Contemp. Hypn. 20 (2003) 25–40.
nomenology of alpha/theta neurofeedback training versus mock [33] R.S. Lazarus, Psychological stress and coping in adaptation and
feedback, Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 27 (2002) 261– illness, Int. J. Psychiatry Med. 5 (1974) 321–333.
270. [34] S.M. Levy, R.B. Herberman, A. Simons, T. Whiteside, J. Lee, R.
[12] I.J. Elenkov, R.L. Wilder, G.P. Chrousos, E.S. Vizi, The sympathetic McDonald, M. Beadle, Persistently low natural killer cell activity in
nerve—an integrative interface between two super systems: the brain normal adults: immunological, hormonal and mood correlates, Nat.
and the immune system, Pharmacol. Rev. 52 (2000) 595–638. Immun. Cell Growth Regul. 8 (1989) 173–186.
[13] P. Evans, F. Huckelebridge, A. Clow, Mind, Immunity and Health: [35] M. Maes, D.R. Van Bockstaele, A. Gastel, C. Song, C. Schotte, H.
The Science of Psychoneuroimmunology, Free Association Books, Neels, I. DeMeester, S. Scharpe, A. Janca, The effects of psycho-
London, UK, 2000. logical stress on leukocyte subset distribution in humans: evidence
[14] A. Field, Discovering Statistics Using SPSS for Windows, Sage of immune activation, Neuropsychobiology 39 (1999) 1–9.
Publications, London, UK, 2003. [36] S.F. Maier, L.R. Watkins, Cytokines for psychologists: implications
[15] P.A. Fox, D.C. Henderson, S.E. Barton, A.J. Champion, M.S. Rollin, of bidirectional immune-to-brain communication for understanding
J. Catalan, S.M. McCormack, J. Gruzelier, Immunological markers of behaviour, mood, and cognition, Psychol. Rev. 105 (1998) 83–107.
frequently recurrent genital herpes simplex virus and their response [37] B.S. McEwen, C.A. Biron, K.W. Brunson, K. Bulloch, W.H. Cham-
to hypnotherapy: a pilot study, Int. J. STD AIDS 10 (1999) 730–734. bers, F.S. Dhabhar, R.H. Goldfarb, R.P. Kitson, A.H. Miller, R.L.
Spencer, J.M. Weiss, The role of adrenocorticoids as modulators
[16] R. Glaser, J.K. Kiecolt-Glaser, J.C. Stout, K.L. Tarr, C.E. Speicher,
of immune function in health and disease: neural, endocrine and
J.E. Holliday, Stress-related impairments in cellular immunity, Psy-
immune interactions, Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 23 (1997) 79–
chiatry Res. 16 (1985) 233–239.
133.
[17] K. Godoy-Ramirez, K. Franck, H. Gainers, A novel method for the
[38] J.J. McGlone, E.A. Lumpkin, R.L. Norman, Adrenocorticotropin
simultaneous assessment of natural killer cell conjugate formation
stimulates natural killer cell activity, Endocrinology 129 (1991) 1653–
and cytotoxicity at the single-cell level by multi-parameter flow
1658.
cytometry, J. Immunol. Methods 239 (2000) 35–44.
[39] J.A. Moynihan, S.Y. Stevens, Mechanisms of stress-induced mod-
[18] J.H. Gruzelier, A review of the impact of hypnosis, relaxation, guided
ulation of immunity in animals, in: R. Ader, D.L. Felten, N. Co-
imagery and individual differences on aspects of immunity and health,
hen (Eds.), Psychoneuroimmunology, 3rd ed., Academic Press, San
Stress 5 (2002) 147–163.
Diego, USA, 2001, pp. 227–250.
[19] J.H. Gruzelier, The role of psychological intervention in modulating
[40] Y. Omura, Impression on observing psychic surgery and healing in
aspects of immune function in relation to health and well-being, Int.
Brazil which appear to incorporate (+)qi gong energy & the use of
Rev. Neurobiol. 52 (2002) 383–417.
acupuncture points, Acupunct. Electrother. Res. 22 (1997) 17–33.
[20] J.H. Gruzelier, J. Levy, J. Williams, D. Henderson, Self-hypnosis
[41] S. Reichlin, Neuroendocrine–immune interactions, N. Engl. J. Med.
and exam stress: comparing immune and relaxation-related imagery
329 (1993) 1246–1253.
for influences on immunity, Contemp. Hypn. 18 (2001) 73–86.
[42] C. Rivier, The hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenal axis response to im-
[21] J. Gruzelier, F. Smith, A. Nagy, D. Henderson, Cellular and humoral mune signals, in: R. Ader, D.L. Felten, N. Cohen (Eds.), Psychoneu-
immunity, mood and exam stress: the influences of self-hypnosis and roimmunology, 3rd ed., Academic Press, San Diego, USA, 2001, pp.
personality predictors, Int. J. Psychophysiol. 42 (2001) 55–71. 633–648.
[22] R. Halvorsen, O. Vassend, Effects of examination stress on some [43] B. Scales, CAMPing in the PACU: using complementary and alter-
cellular immunity functions, J. Psychosom. Res. 31 (1987) 693–701. native medical practices in the PACU, J. Perianesth. Nurs. 16 (2001)
[23] J. Henning, P. Netter, K.H. Voigt, Cortisol mediates redistribution 325–334.
of CD8+ but not of CD56+ cells after the psychological stress of [44] M. Schedlowski, R. Jacobs, G. Stratmann, S. Richter, A. Hadicke,
public speaking, Psychoneuroendocrinology 26 (2001) 673–687. U. Tewes, T.O. Wagner, R.E. Schmidt, Changes of natural killer
[24] C. Inoue-Sakurai, S. Maruyama, K. Morimoto, Posttraumatic stress cells during acute psychological stress, J. Clin. Immunol. 13 (1993)
and lifestyles are associated with natural killer cell activity in victims 119–126.
of the Hanshin-Awaji Earthquake in Japan, Prev. Med. 31 (2000) [45] H. Selye, A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents, Nature
467–473. 138 (1936) 32.
[25] G.A. Jamieson, The Structure and Meaning of Absorption, Unpub- [46] H. Selye, Confusion and controversy in the stress field, J. Human
lished Master’s Dissertation, University of Queensland, 1987. Stress 1 (1975) 37–44.
A. Naito et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 62 (2003) 241–253 253

[47] H. Selye, Implications of stress concept, N.Y. State J. Med. 75 (1975) [54] A.V. Turnbull, C.L. Rivier, Regulation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–
2139–2145. adrenal axis by cytokines: actions and mechanisms of action, Physiol.
[48] C.L. Sheridan, S.A. Ramacher, Stress and health, in: C.L. Sheridan, Rev. 79 (1999) 1–71.
S.A. Ramacher (Eds.), Health Psychology: Challenging the Biomed- [55] P.N. Uchakin, B. Tobin, M. Cubbage, G. Marshall Jr., C. Sams, Im-
ical Model, Wiley, New York, USA, 1992, pp. 148–151. mune responsiveness following academic stress in first-year medical
[49] R.E. Shor, E.C. Orne, Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Suscep- students, J. Interferon Cytokine Res. 21 (2001) 687–694.
tibility, Form A, Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, CA, [56] B.N. Uchino, J.T. Cacioppo, W. Malarkey, R. Glaser, Individual
1962. differences in cardiac sympathetic control predict endocrine and
[50] B.G. Tabachnick, L.S. Fidell, Using Multivariate Statistics, Harper immune responses to acute psychological stress, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.
Collins College Publishers, New York, 1996. 69 (1995) 736–743.
[51] E. Targ, Research methodology for studies of prayer and distant [57] W.D. Wardell, J. Engebretson, Biological correlates of reiki touch
healing, Complement. Ther. Nurs. Midwifery 8 (2002) 29–41. healing, J. Adv. Nurs. 33 (2001) 439–445.
[52] A. Tellegen, G. Atkinson, Openness to absorbing and self-altering [58] W.G. Whitehouse, D.F. Dinges, E.C. Orne, S.E. Keller, B.L. Bates,
experiences (“absorption”), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility, N.K. Bauer, P. Morahan, B.A. Haupt, M.M. Carlin, P.B. Bloom, L.
J. Abnorm. Psychol. 83 (1974) 268–277. Zaugg, M.T. Orne, Psychosocial and immune effects of self-hypnosis
[53] H. Tryphonas, M. Fournier, F. Lacroix, P. McGuire, S. Hayward, F. training for stress management throughout the first semester of med-
Bryce, D. Flipo, D.L. Arnold, Effects of great lakes fish consumption ical school, Psychosom. Med. 58 (1996) 249–263.
on the immune system of Sprague–Dawley rats investigated during [59] G. Willemsen, D. Carroll, C. Ring, M. Drayson, Cellular and mucosal
a two-generation reproductive study. II. Quantitative and functional immune reactions to mental and cold stress: associations with gender
aspects, Regul. Toxicol. Pharmacol. 27 (1998) S40–S54. and cardiovascular reactivity, Psychophysiology 39 (2002) 222–228.

View publication stats

You might also like