You are on page 1of 10

Cognitive Brain Research 24 (2005) 307 – 316

www.elsevier.com/locate/cogbrainres

Research report

Effects of dynamic rotation on event-related brain potentials


M.I. Núñez-Peñaa,b,T, J.A. Aznarb,c, D. Linaresc, M.J. Corralb, C. Escerab
a
Department of Behavioral Sciences Methods, Faculty of Psychology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
b
Cognitive Neuroscience Research Group, Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychobiology, Faculty of Psychology,
University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
c
Department of Basic Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain

Accepted 7 February 2005


Available online 23 March 2005

Abstract

Event-related potentials were recorded during a mental rotation task. Subjects were shown pairs of letter-like shapes and were asked to
make a parity judgment. The shape on the left was always in its canonical position and the shape on the right could either be in its canonical
position or be a mirror image. Two variables were manipulated for the shape on the right. First, it could appear at different orientations (508,
1008 or 1508); second, it could be presented in a stationary position, in a dynamic congruent direction (the shape slowly rotating toward its
normal upright position) or in a dynamic incongruent direction (the shape slowly rotating in the opposite direction to its normal upright
position). Orientation- and direction-dependent modulations of a negative slow wave were found. For orientation, the typical amplitude effect
over parietal sites was found, the amplitude becoming more negative as the rotational angle increased. For direction, the amplitude of the
negative slow wave was larger for stationary and dynamic incongruent trials than for dynamic congruent trials at 1008 and 1508. This result
suggests that presentation of a stimulus in a dynamic congruent direction facilitates the mental rotation process. At 508, differences between
dynamic incongruent trials and both stationary and dynamic congruent trials were found, suggesting that the incongruent movement elicits an
obstructing effect over the mental rotation process. In summary, the present experiment provides new evidence in support of the idea that the
amplitude modulation over the parietal cortex is a psychophysiological marker of the mental rotation process.
D 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Theme: Neural basis of behavior


Topic: Cognition

Keywords: Event-related brain potentials; Mental rotation; Cognition; Rotation-related negativity; Dynamic rotation; Perceived rotation

1. Introduction image of the other. Shepard and Metzler found that the time
taken to decide on the parity of a shape increased
Mental rotation is a process that has been extensively monotonically with the angular disparity of their orientation
investigated by cognitive psychologists. It was first in space. Since then, the same results have been repeatedly
described by Shepard and Metzler [20] in an experiment reported; the mental rotation effect has been demonstrated
where pairs of drawings of three-dimensional nonsense with alphanumeric characters, polygons, letter-like symbols,
shapes were presented, and subjects were asked to make a and common objects [2,5,12]. This increase in reaction time
parity judgment: that is, they were required to decide depending on angular disparity has been taken as evidence
whether drawings were the same or if one was a mirror that the system rotates mental representations in order to put
them in their normal position. Moreover, the subjects
themselves have reported from their own introspections
T Corresponding author. Department of Behavioral Science Methods,
Faculty of Psychology, University of Barcelona, Passeig Vall d’Hebron,
that they used mental rotation to perform tasks of this kind.
171, 08035 Barcelona, Spain. Fax: +34 93 402 13 59. Therefore, mental rotation has been considered a cognitive
E-mail address: inunez@ub.edu (M.I. Núñez-Peña). process that is isomorphic to physical rotation: it is
0926-6410/$ - see front matter D 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.02.004
308 M.I. Núñez-Peña et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 24 (2005) 307–316

suggested that subjects rotate images in their minds as they mental representations of objects are rotated through a
would rotate physical figures in the real world. trajectory in much the same manner that physical objects are
Studies with event-related potentials (ERPs) have rotated, it may be that the manipulation of perceived rotation
reported the existence of a psychophysiological marker of will affect the mental rotation process. Jolicoeur and
the mental rotation phenomenon. This marker is a slow Cavanagh [13] investigated the relationship between per-
parietal negativity with latency between 400 and 800 ms, ceived rotational motion and mental rotation by measuring
whose amplitude is modulated by the amount of mental reaction time. In a parity judgment task, they presented
rotation performed by the subject. The amplitude of this characters that moved1 either in the direction of mental
negative wave becomes larger as the angular disparity from rotation (congruent trials) or in the opposite direction
the upright increases. The rotation-related negativity was (incongruent trials). An effect of congruency was reported
first described by Peronnet and Farah [16], Wijers et al. [21], on reaction time. Whereas incongruent trials resulted in
and Rfsler et al. [18], who equated late negativity with the slower response times, congruent trials showed a faster
cognitive process of mental rotation. Although these authors response. It seemed that a character rotation in the opposite
used alphanumeric characters as stimuli, the same negativity direction interfered with the subject’s ability to perform
modulation has been reported in other experiments using mental rotation, while no interference but facilitation was
different shapes [11,15,19,22]. found in trials in which the direction of character rotation
A psychophysiological correlate of a cognitive process was the same as the postulated direction of mental rotation.
would be particularly useful for broadening our knowledge It is suggested that the congruency effect takes place
of human cognition. This ERP effect is a candidate, and throughout mental rotation rather than simply delaying its
several experiments have been performed to establish its onset. These results have been replicated by Heil et al. [9]
functional significance (for a review, see Ref. [6]). If a using characters as stimuli, by Jolicoeur et al. [14] using
functional significance is established between mental drawings of common objects, and by Corballis and Black-
rotation and the slow parietal negativity, the amplitude man [3] and Corballis and Corballis [4] using small bars.
modulation of this component could be used as an indicator In the present experiment, subjects were engaged in a
of the presence of mental rotation itself. The substantial parity task in which a pair of letter-like shapes was
body of research conducted by Heil et al. has provided presented simultaneously: the shape on the left of the
compelling evidence of the functional significance of the screen appeared in its normal version and the shape on the
rotation-related negativity. This evidence can be summar- right could be presented either in its normal version or as a
ized in two sets. One series of experiments manipulated the mirror reflection. Subjects had to compare the shapes and
presence or absence of the mental rotation process in order decide if they were identical in shape or if they were mirror
to perform a task [8,10]. Alphanumeric characters were images of each other. The shape on the right was shown in
presented in different orientations, and the type of task was different orientations (508, 1008, and 1508 from its normal
manipulated so that its performance did or did not involve upright position) and in different directions (stationary,
mental rotation. Obviously, if the amplitude modulation of dynamic congruent—the shape rotated slowly towards the
the slow negativity was a psychophysiological marker of normal upright—or dynamic incongruent—the shape
mental rotation, it would be absent if mental rotation was rotated slowly in the opposite direction to the normal
not used to solve the task. The results confirmed this upright). By manipulating the direction, we expected to
prediction. influence the mental rotation process. On the one hand, it is
On the other hand, the functional significance of the slow reasonable to assume that when a stimulus slowly rotates
negativity wave would also be supported if delaying the toward its canonical position, the mental rotation process
mental rotation process also delayed the onset of the will be facilitated—given that the process of mental rotation
amplitude modulation. Heil and Rolke’s experiment [7] is assumed to be isomorphic to the physical rotation, and so
confirmed this point. The mental rotation process was the rotation-related negativity will turn out to be less
delayed by manipulating the duration of perceptual encod- negative. On the other hand, when a stimulus slowly rotates
ing of the stimuli and by manipulating character identi- against its canonical position, the same argument may apply.
fication and discrimination. In compliance with the In this case, the mental rotation process will be impeded
sequential model of information processing, both processes because the shape rotates in the opposite direction to the one
were performed before the mental rotation process began. that shortens the distance to the upright, and therefore, the
Heil and Rolke [7] provided empirical evidence for the amplitude of the rotation-related negativity will become
temporal relationship between the cognitive process of more negative. In summary, if the amplitude of the slow
mental rotation and the onset of the amplitude modulation of negative potential is an index of the mental rotation process,
the slow negative wave.
The goal of the present study was to add new evidence to
support the idea that the amplitude modulation of the ERPs 1
Perceived motion was induced by means of the presentation of multiple
over the parietal cortex is a neurophysiological correlate of views of the character or by means of a moving background disk that was
the mental rotation process. As it has been proposed that presented below the character.
M.I. Núñez-Peña et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 24 (2005) 307–316 309

we expect to find an effect of direction on the ERP negativity, the movement effect upon the ERP might be
amplitude: the amplitude will be more negative for the obscured.
dynamic incongruent stimuli, medium for the stationary Stimuli were shown in green on a white background
stimuli, and less negative for the dynamic congruent stimuli. (luminance 110 cd/m2), and subtended a vertical visual
angle of 1.728 and a horizontal visual angle of 1.338. The
programme language used to run the experiment was C++/
2. Materials and methods Open GL, a software interface for 3D graphics with
hardware developed by Silicon Graphics, Inc.
2.1. Participants
2.3. Procedure
Ten healthy, right-handed volunteers (seven women)
took part in the experiment. Age ranged from 18 to 26 years, Participants were seated in an electrically-shielded,
(mean F SD 19.9 F 2.3). Handedness of the subjects was sound-attenuating room at a distance of 150 cm from the
evaluated by self-report. All participants were university display screen, whose center was at eye level. The session
students and had normal or corrected-to-normal visual began with a training period consisting of the presentation
acuity. Written informed consent was obtained from all of trials similar to those used in the recording period. To be
participants prior to the start of the experiment. sure that subjects clearly understood the task to be
performed, the training period finished when the participant
2.2. Stimulus materials had correctly answered eight consecutive trials. Participants
achieved this criterion in a few trials.
Pairs of three-dimensional letter-like shapes were used as When the training period was over, the recording period
stimuli (see Fig. 1), and were presented using a Pentium IV started. Subjects were instructed to relax and to keep their
computer and a 17 in. SVGA monitor. Each pair consisted eyes on the screen. They were also told that it was important
of a shape presented in its normal version on the left of the to avoid blinking, and that if they needed to blink, they
computer screen and another shape presented in its normal should try to wait until a question mark or a rest message
or mirror-image version on the right of the screen. The appeared on the screen. Each trial began with the
shapes were presented simultaneously. In the shape on the presentation of a red fixation point in the center of the
right, two variables were manipulated: first, this shape could screen for 500 ms. A stimulus was then presented for 2 s.
appear at 508, 1008 or 1508 clockwise from the vertical After that, a question symbol was displayed in the center of
upright and second, it could be presented in a stationary the monitor. The inter-trial interval was 1000 ms. Subjects
position, in a dynamic congruent direction (the shape was were required to determine whether the shape on the right
moved smoothly rotating counter-clockwise) or in a was a normal or a mirror-reversed version of the shape on
dynamic incongruent direction (the shape was moved the left. They had to indicate their decisions after the
smoothly rotating clockwise). Both dynamic congruent question symbol by pressing the corresponding mouse keys,
and dynamic incongruent shapes were presented at a 258/s with responding fingers being counterbalanced across
angular velocity on the screen. Although a zero degrees subjects.
condition is usually used in mental rotation experiments, Twelve blocks of 72 trials were presented to each
this condition was not included in the present experiment participant. A message indicating a 1-min rest appeared on
because it is not possible to combine it with the movement the screen after each block, and there was a 15-min rest in
conditions (dynamic congruent and dynamic incongruent the middle of the recording session. The type of trials was
direction). The inclusion of a zero degrees condition would controlled within each block in such a way that a block
increase the number of trials in the stationary conditions by included trials resulting from the combination of direction
25% as compared to the dynamic conditions. Hence, (stationary, dynamic congruent, and dynamic incongruent),
dynamic conditions would occur at a comparatively low orientation (508, 1008, and 1508), version (normal and
frequency and, therefore, might give rise to a P3b mirror-reversed), and type of stimulus (four letter-like
component (for a review see [17]). Because the P3b shapes). Each type of trial was randomly presented in every
component has similar latency as the rotation-related block. All participants were tested on 864 trials, 48 for each
experimental condition.

2.4. Electrophysiological recording

EEG was recorded with the SynAmps/SCAN 3.0


hardware and software (NeuroScan, Herndon, VA) from
31 tin electrodes mounted in a commercial electro-cap
(Electro-Cap International, Eaton, OH). Nineteen electrodes
Fig. 1. Normal upright version of the stimuli used in the present experiment. were positioned according to the 10–20 International
310 M.I. Núñez-Peña et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 24 (2005) 307–316

System: three electrodes were placed over midline sites at carried out with the Greenhouse–Geisser correction for
Fz, Cz, and Pz locations, along with 8 lateral pairs of sphericity departures, which was applied when appropriate.
electrodes over standard sites on frontal (FP1/FP2, F7/F8, The F value, the probability level following correction, and
F3/F4), central (C3/C4), temporal (T3/T4, T5/T6), parietal the q value are reported. Whenever a main effect reached
(P3/P4), and occipital (O1/O2) positions. Two electrodes significance, t tests contrasts were calculated and the
were placed at Fpz and Oz, and ten electrodes were placed Hochberg approach was used to control for the increase in
halfway between the following additional locations: fronto- type I error. Simple effects were calculated in order to
central (FC1/FC2), fronto-temporal (FT3/FT4), centro-pari- analyze significant interactions. Finally, trend analyses were
etal (CP1/CP2), temporo-parietal (TP3/TP4), and mastoids performed to try to fit a linear trend between amplitude and
(M1/M2). The common reference electrode for EEG orientation.
measurements was placed on the tip of the nose. EEG
channels were continuously digitized at a rate of 500 Hz by
a SynAmpTM amplifier (5083 model, NeuroScan, Herndon, 3. Results
VA). A band pass filter was set from 0.05 to 30 Hz, and
electrode impedance was always kept below 5 kV. For 3.1. Behavioral data
monitoring eye movement and blinks, FP1, FP2, FPz, and
an electrode placed at the external canthi of the right eye Error rate analysis showed that there were not differences
were used. in accuracy between conditions; no main effect or interaction
reached significance (all P values N 0.05). The absence of
2.5. Data analysis differences might be due to the fact that subjects were not
asked to answer as quickly as possible, but after a question
Error rate was analyzed by means of repeated measure- symbol appeared on the screen. As a consequence, subjects
ments ANOVA taking as factors direction (stationary, might have had enough time to think about their answer and
dynamic congruent and dynamic incongruent), orientation therefore, subjects had a few errors. Table 1 shows means of
(508, 1008, and 1508) and version (normal and mirror- error rate and standard deviations for the three orientations in
reversed). Reaction times were not recorded in the present stationary, dynamic congruent, and dynamic incongruent
experiment because subjects were to respond only after the stimuli.
question mark appeared on the screen in order to avoid a
contamination of the ERP by response related activity. 3.2. Electrophysiological data
As for the electrophysiological data, epochs for each
subject in each experimental condition were averaged Fig. 2 shows the grand average ERPs for the different
relative to a pre-stimulus baseline that was made up of the orientations in the stationary position, the dynamic con-
100 ms of activity preceding the epoch of interest. Trials gruent direction, and the dynamic incongruent direction at
with artifacts (voltage exceeding F75 AV in any channel) P3, Pz, and P4. The usual pattern of voltage for the
and trials with incorrect behavioral responses were excluded orientation effect in the stationary position and in the
from the ERP average. The mean number of epochs dynamic congruent direction can be seen: the greater the
included in each ERP average varied between 72.8 and degree to be rotated, the more negative the amplitude of the
79.9 for the various types of stimuli used (as the version slow wave. However, this pattern changed in the dynamic
factor did not produce any significant difference, the data incongruent direction, where these amplitude differences
were averaged ignoring this factor), and the minimum were not present. In Fig. 3, the same ERPs as Fig. 2 are
number of epochs included in any individual average was rearranged so as to show the slow wave modulation as a
57. ERP data were analyzed by computing the mean function of the stimulus direction—grand average ERPs are
amplitude in 50 ms windows from 500 to 1000 ms post- shown at the different directions for 508, 1008 and 1508. A
stimuli. The purpose of working with this large range of direction-dependent modulation of the amplitude of the
latency windows was to identify the time interval where the slow wave can be distinguished in Fig. 3. As for 508 and
electrophysiological differences between experimental con- 1008, the voltage of this slow wave was more positive for
ditions began, where these differences finished, and where the dynamic congruent direction, medium for the stationary
they reached their maximum amplitude.
A 3  3  2  3  5 repeated-measures analysis of Table 1
variance (ANOVA) was performed on the ERP amplitude at Means of error rate and standard deviations (in brackets) for the three
15 electrodes (F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8, T3, C3, Cz, C4, T4, T5, orientations in stationary, dynamic congruent, and dynamic incongruent
P3, Pz, P4, T6) taking as factors direction (stationary, stimuli
dynamic congruent, and dynamic incongruent), orientation 508 1008 1508
(508, 1008, and 1508), version (normal and mirror-reversed), Stationary 0.01 (0.02) 0.02 (0.02) 0.04 (0.04)
frontality (frontal, central, and parietal) and laterality (five Dynamic congruent 0.02 (0.02) 0.02 (0.03) 0.02 (0.01)
Dynamic incongruent 0.03 (0.04) 0.05 (0.04) 0.02 (0.01)
levels from left to right). Repeated-measures ANOVAs were
M.I. Núñez-Peña et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 24 (2005) 307–316 311

Fig. 2. Grand average ERPs (n = 10) for the different orientations in the stationary position and in both dynamic directions at P3, Pz, and P4.

Fig. 3. Grand average ERPs (n = 10) for the different directions in 508, 1008 and 1508 at P3, Pz, and P4.
312 M.I. Núñez-Peña et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 24 (2005) 307–316

position, and more negative for the dynamic incongruent regarding the 1508 orientation, only differences between the
direction. For the 1508 orientation, the pattern of voltage stationary position and the dynamic congruent direction
was similar to that observed in the other orientations, except were found at P3, Pz and P4 in the 850–1000 ms window.
for the dynamic incongruent direction; when the stimulus In summary, statistical analysis showed that the direction
was presented in a dynamic incongruent direction, the effect differed depending on the orientation of the stimuli.
amplitude of the negative slow wave was between those of When the rotational angle was short (508), amplitude was
the stationary position and the dynamic congruent direction. more negative for the shapes presented in a dynamic
The ANOVA results for the 50 ms windows from 500 to incongruent direction than for the shapes presented in a
1000 ms post-stimulus are shown in Table 2. The main stationary position or in a dynamic congruent direction. It
effect of direction reached statistical significance in the 500– seemed as if at 508, the movement of the shape towards its
950 interval ( P values less than 0.05). t tests contrasts normal upright did not facilitate the mental rotation process,
showed that there were amplitude differences between the whereas the opposite movement impeded it. When the
stationary position and the dynamic congruent direction in rotational angle was 1008, the ERP pattern was different:
the 500–750 ms windows ( P values less than 0.007), and amplitude was more positive for dynamic congruent stimuli
differences between the two dynamic directions in the 500– than for both stationary and dynamic incongruent stimuli.
950 ms windows ( P values less than 0.017). As for t tests This result suggests a facilitating effect of the congruent
contrasts between the stationary position and the dynamic movement on the mental rotation process at 1008 orienta-
incongruent direction, all failed to reach statistical signifi- tion. Finally, when the orientation was 1508, amplitude was
cance, although the amplitude average for dynamic incon- more negative for stationary stimuli than for dynamic
gruent stimuli was more negative than the amplitude congruent stimuli, suggesting again the presence of a
average for stationary stimuli. facilitating effect over the mental rotation process. However,
For a more detailed analysis of the direction effect, it must be highlighted that this effect occurred later than the
complementary analyses were performed in every orienta- facilitating effect described for 1008 orientation.
tion at P3, Pz, and P4 (Table 3 shows a summary of these As for the effect of the orientation on the ERPs, the
results). In the 508 orientation, the following results were ANOVA showed that the orientation  frontality interaction
found: there were statistically significant differences reached statistical significance in the 500–950 ms windows,
between the incongruent and congruent directions in the and the direction  orientation  frontality interaction
500–800 ms interval at P3, in the 550–800 ms interval at Pz, reached statistical significance in the 500–550 and the 650–
and in the 600–700 ms interval at P4; moreover, there were 950 ms windows (see Table 2). Simple effects analysis
also differences between the stationary position and the showed that the orientation effect was only present at central
dynamic incongruent position in the 500–700 ms interval at locations in the 600–800 ms post-stimulus, and at parietal
P3 and in the 500–600 ms interval at Pz. As for the 1008 locations in the 550–900 ms interval (see results in Table 4).
orientation, differences between the incongruent and con- A detailed analysis of the orientation effect was
gruent directions were found in the 650–950 ms interval at performed at P3, Pz, and P4, locations where the orientation
P3, in the 600–1000 ms interval at Pz and in the 600–1000 effect is usually found. This analysis consisted of a trend
ms interval at P4; differences were also found between the analysis for the orientation in the stationary position, the
stationary position and the dynamic congruent direction in dynamic congruent direction, and the dynamic incongruent
the 650–800 ms interval at P3, and in the 600–850 ms direction. The following results were found. A linear trend
interval at Pz and in the 600–800 ms interval P4. Finally, could be adjusted between amplitude and orientation in the

Table 2
ANOVA results for the effects that reached statistical significance
Time (ms) Direction Orientation  Frontality Direction  Orientation  Frontality
F P e F P e F P e
500–550 12.815 0.001TTT 0.896 4.376 0.023TT 0.561 3.399 0.027T 0.412
550–600 7.099 0.005TT 0.822 7.994 0.004TT 0.486 3.056 0.057 0.314
600–650 9.912 0.001TTT 0.923 8.263 0.003TT 0.504 2.669 0.064 0.392
650–700 16.806 0.001TTT 0.826 17.170 0.001TTT 0.523 3.301 0.003TT 0.366
700–750 15.381 0.001TTT 0.973 18.091 0.001TTT 0.393 1.451 0.260 0.256
750–800 8.352 0.003TT 0.981 24.130 0.001TTT 0.405 3.130 0.004TT 0.366
800–850 5.538 0.013T 0.974 9.484 0.003TT 0.425 2.855 0.008TT 0.398
850–900 4.157 0.033T 0.944 7.270 0.005TT 0.508 3.229 0.003TT 0.418
900–950 3.721 0.044T 0.922 5.244 0.030T 0.355 4.639 0.001TTT 0.424
950–1000 1.175 0.331 0.973 2.800 0.086 0.513 2.650 0.130 0.492
T 0.05 z P N 0.01.
TT 0.01 z P N 0.001.
TTT P V 0.001.
M.I. Núñez-Peña et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 24 (2005) 307–316 313

Table 3
Time epochs where the paired contrasts for dynamic incongruent, dynamic congruent, and stationary stimuli reached statistical significance
Contrast 508 1008 1508
P3 Pz P4 P3 Pz P4 P3 Pz P4
Inc-Cong 500–800 550–800 600–700 650–950 600–1000 600–1000 – – –
Inc-Sta 500–700 500–600 – – – – – – –
Cong-Sta – – – 650–800 600–850 600–800 850–1000 850–1000 850–1000
Results are shown for every orientation at P3, Pz and P4.
Inc: dynamic incongruent; Cong: dynamic congruent; Sta: stationary.

500–950 interval for the stationary stimuli, in the 600–800 reduced it. In the present experiment, we studied whether the
interval for the dynamic congruent stimuli, and in the 700– same phenomenon can be described by means of ERPs. In
800 ms in the dynamic incongruent stimuli (see Results in other words, we aimed to examine whether the amplitude of
Table 5). When the stimuli were presented in a stationary the rotation-related negativity would be affected by the
position or in a dynamic congruent direction, the results are congruent or incongruent movement of the stimulus. If a
the same as those described in the literature: the greater the movement effect could be demonstrated, this would provide
degree to be rotated, the more negative the amplitude of the further support for the idea that the amplitude modulation of
slow wave. However, when the direction was dynamic the parietal negative wave is a psychophysiological marker of
incongruent, the main effect of orientation could only be mental rotation.
described by a linear trend in the 700–800 interval. In order Our results agree at least to some extent with those
to analyze the effect of the orientation in the dynamic reported in reaction time studies for the movement effect
incongruent direction, t test contrasts were performed and over the mental rotation process. A direction effect over the
differences were only found between 508 and 1508 (all P ERPs amplitude was found. However, this effect depended
values less than or equal to 0.02). As a consequence, it can on the orientation of the stimuli. When the stimulus was
be concluded that the typical orientation effect was not presented at 508 orientation, only the obstructing effect
elicited when the shape was presented in a dynamic could be described. The amplitude of the slow negative
incongruent direction. wave was larger for the dynamic incongruent stimuli than
for both the stationary and the dynamic congruent stimuli.
This negativity increase may have been due to an
4. Discussion obstruction of the mental rotation process because the
perceived rotation was divergent from mental rotation.
The present study examined the effect of mental rotation Although this result agrees with those reported in reaction
on event-related potentials. Previous studies with ERPs, time studies (a longer reaction time for incongruent stimuli),
where objects and characters were shown in different the expected facilitation effect over mental rotation for the
orientations, have reported the presence of a slow negative dynamic congruent stimuli was not found. ERPs results
potential related to the mental rotation process (for a review, showed no differences between the dynamic congruent and
see Ref. [6]). The increased negativity of the amplitude of the stationary stimuli, suggesting that there was no
ERPs over the parietal cortex depending on the rotational facilitation of the mental rotation process when the stimuli
angle necessary to place the object in its normal upright moved toward their canonical position. This unexpected
position gave support to the idea that this modulation of the result may be due to the fact that when the angle to be
amplitude is an index of mental rotation itself. To date, mentally rotated is small (508), the task would be too easy to
mental rotation using ERPs has been studied exclusively be facilitated by the congruent movement. In fact, it is often
with stationary stimuli. In the present experiment, the study found that for short rotations, there is no difference from the
of mental rotation was tested with dynamic stimuli. upright condition either behaviorally or in the ERPs (for
Our results confirmed those reported in previous studies example, Bajric et al. [1] did not find differences in the ERP
with stationary stimuli. When the stimuli were presented in a amplitude between 08 and 458 in the ERP slow wave in a
stationary position the slow negativity increases linearly as a parity judgment task; Cooper and Shepard [2] reported
function of the angular disparity of the stimuli from the mental rotation for mirror/normal letter discriminations and
upright. Moreover, this effect was larger at parietal locations, also found this lack of linearity for reaction time near the
as usually reported in mental rotation research. However, the upright). As a consequence, it has been suggested that
most important aspect of the present experiment is the effect mental rotation might not be necessary to make a parity
of dynamic rotation on the ERPs. Reaction time studies judgment at short angular disparities. This might explain the
[3,4,9,13,14] have reported both facilitation and obstruction absence of facilitation effect in the present experiment.
of the mental rotation process associated with motion of the Regarding the 1008 orientation, the results closely
characters and objects. Trials with incongruent motion matched the predictions except for the lack of statistical
increased reaction time, whereas trials with congruent motion significance in the dynamic incongruent vs. stationary
314 M.I. Núñez-Peña et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 24 (2005) 307–316

Table 4
Simple effects analysis for the interaction orientation  frontality
Time (ms) Frontal Central Parietal
F P e F P e F P e
500–550 1.29 0.299 0.971 0.27 0.769 0.938 1.48 0.253 0.801
550–600 0.91 0.556 0.943 1.13 0.345 0.766 3.72 0.044TT 0.681
600–650 0.40 0.674 0.949 4.47 0.027T 0.850 9.14 0.002TT 0.641
650–700 0.54 0.592 0.996 7.93 0.003TT 0.915 17.85 0.001TTT 0.732
700–750 0.91 0.592 0.540 9.36 0.002TT 0.819 22.47 0.001TTT 0.911
750–800 0.17 0.846 0.903 4.39 0.028T 0.742 18.60 0.001TTT 0.814
800–850 0.48 0.629 0.921 0.59 0.567 0.817 9.50 0.002TT 0.921
850–900 0.94 0.408 0.991 0.08 0.920 0.905 4.97 0.019T 0.871
900–950 0.77 0.477 0.972 0.11 0.896 0.890 2.38 0.121 0.908
950–1000 0.61 0.556 0.850 0.41 0.669 0.790 0.29 0.753 0.840
T 0.05 z P N 0.01.
TT 0.01 z P N 0.001.
TTT P V 0.001.

comparison. As predicted, the amplitude of the slow wave but is a consequence of the orientation effect since, by the
was less negative for the dynamic congruent stimuli than for time of onset of the mental rotation process, congruent
the stationary and the dynamic incongruent stimuli. These motion will have reduced the angular distance from to
results indicate again that the rotating movement modulates upright whereas incongruent motion will have increased it.
the amplitude of the rotation-related negativity, although the This possibility can be discarded because of the following
ERP pattern is different from that described for the 508 reason. It is generally assumed that mental rotation starts at
orientation—where only the obstructing effect was found in about 400 ms after the stimulus presentation. Dynamic
the statistical analysis. In summary, with 1008, the facilitat- congruent and dynamic incongruent stimuli in the present
ing effect of the dynamic congruent movement was experiment were presented at a 258/s angular velocity.
confirmed, but the obstructing effect of the incongruent Therefore, 400 ms after the stimulus presentation, the
movement was not. stimulus has moved about 108. An orientation shift of 108
Finally, when the angle to be rotated was 1508, again a is too small to account for the differences in voltages.
facilitating effect over the mental rotation process was When the orientation effect was analyzed in the sta-
found, since congruent stimuli were associated with less tionary position and in both dynamic directions at parietal
negativity than stationary ones; however, the predicted sites, the typical amplitude modulation was evident when
opposite effect for the dynamic incongruent stimuli did not the stimulus was presented in a stationary position—
occur. As has been mentioned above, there were no replicating the results previously described in the ERPs
differences between dynamic incongruent and both sta- literature—and in a dynamic congruent direction. In both
tionary and dynamic congruent stimuli. cases, the amplitude of the rotation-related negativity
Concerning these results, one could speculate that the became larger as the rotational angle from the upright
movement effect is not due to the stimulus movement per se, increased. However, contrary to expectations, the rotation-

Table 5
Trend analysis results ( P values for linear trend inside the cell) for the orientation in the stationary position, the dynamic congruent direction, and the dynamic
congruent direction at P3, Pz, and P4
Time (ms) P3 Pz P4
Stationary Dynamic Dynamic Stationary Dynamic Dynamic Stationary Dynamic Dynamic
congruent incongruent congruent incongruent congruent incongruent
500–550 0.005TT 0.125 0.307 0.001TTT 0.185 0.381 0.001TTT 0.210 0.506
550–600 0.002TT 0.058 0.587 0.001TTT 0.077 0.587 0.001TTT 0.070 0.718
600–650 0.001TTT 0.023T 0.966 0.001TTT 0.079 0.841 0.001TTT 0.035T 0.788
650–700 0.001TTT 0.007TT 0.056 0.001TTT 0.043T 0.088 0.001TTT 0.040T 0.107
700–750 0.001TTT 0.007TT 0.001TTT 0.002TT 0.053 0.002TT 0.006TT 0.039T 0.002TT
750–800 0.001TTT 0.002TT 0.012T 0.002TT 0.038T 0.028T 0.005TT 0.042T 0.027T
800–850 0.005TT 0.032T 0.185 0.007TT 0.163 0.417 0.018T 0.348 0.175
850–900 0.015T 0.977 0.095 0.017T 0.528 0.368 0.021T 0.528 0.278
900–950 0.018T 0.490 0.090 0.026T 0.363 0.506 0.026T 0.303 0.536
950–1000 0.108 0.322 0.614 0.122 0.144 0.823 0.071 0.099 0.841
T 0.05 z P N 0.01.
TT 0.01 z P N 0.001.
TTT P V 0.001.
M.I. Núñez-Peña et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 24 (2005) 307–316 315

related negativity modulation was not evident when the on ERPs. New support to the existence of a rotation-related
stimulus slowly rotated in the opposite direction to its negativity as a psychophysiological marker of a cognitive
normal upright. process has been added. The present experiment differs
Some explanations for this unexpected result can be from previous research in that dynamic stimuli were
drawn as follows. According to Jolicoeur and Cavanagh presented in order to facilitate or impede the mental rotation
[13] and Jolicoeur et al. [14], the fact that the reaction time process. The study makes two important contributions. First,
became longer for incongruent stimuli could be caused by a it confirms the notion that, with large angular disparities, if a
tendency of subjects to mentally rotate incongruent stimuli shape is presented slowly rotating to its normal upright, the
in the direction of their physical motion and, hence, along mental rotation process is facilitated: the typical orientation
the longer of the two possible paths to upright. If subjects effect over the amplitude of the ERPs was present, but the
acted always in this way, when a stimulus was presented amplitude was systematically more positive than those of
with an incongruent movement, the amplitude of the mental- either stationary or dynamic incongruent stimuli. Second,
rotation negativity to 508, 1008, and 1508 would be expected when a stimulus is presented in a short angular disparity and
to decrease as the angular departure from the upright moved slowly in the opposite direction to its normal upright,
increases—because 508 would transform to 3108, 1008 the mental rotation process is obstructed. These findings
would transform to 2608, and 1508 would transform to 2108. provide empirical evidence that the rotation-related neg-
However, the present experiment showed neither this pattern ativity is a psychophysiological correlate of the mental
of amplitude modulation of the mental-rotation negativity rotation process. In other words, the functional relationship
nor the pattern expected if the subjects preferred the shorter between the process of mental rotation and the rotation-
rotation. Therefore, there is no evidence that subjects chose related negativity is validated.
always the longer way to make the parity judgment.
Although the present experiment allows us to discard the
possibility that subjects rotate always the stimulus along the Acknowledgments
longer way when the stimulus moved against its normal
upright, it is plausible to consider the following: subjects This research was supported by grants BS02003-02440
might tend to act this way with longer angles—when there is and BS022001-3639 from the Spanish Ministry of Science
not a large difference between the distance going clockwise and Technology. The authors thank to Dr. Jeff P. Hamm and
or going counterclockwise—and not with shorter angles— one anonymous reviewer for their helpful suggestions on an
when the distance is much longer if subject opted to go earlier version of the manuscript.
clockwise. In the present experiment, differences in the
rotation-related negativity were found only between 508 and
1508 when a stimulus was presented in an incongruent
References
direction—the amplitude was more negative for 1508.
Because no differences were found between 1008 and [1] J. Bajric, F. Rfsler, M. Heil, E. Hennighausen, On separating
1508, it might be that participants opted to rotate some processes of event categorization, task preparation, and mental
stimuli using the longer way and other using the shorter rotation proper in a handedness recognition task, Psychophysiology
way. If this happened, the expected orientation effect over 36 (1999) 399 – 408.
the ERPs, with these angles, might have been cancelled. [2] L.A. Cooper, R.N. Shepard, Chronometric studies of the rotation of
mental images, in: W.G. Chase (Ed.), Visual Information Processing,
Therefore, the orientation effect would not be present in Academic Press, New York, 1973.
incongruent stimuli. Although this might be a possible [3] M.C. Corballis, R. Blackman, The effect of apparent movement on
explanation, the issue merits further investigation. It is mental rotation, Mem. Cogn. 18 (1990) 551 – 555.
important that in future experiments, subjects are asked to [4] P.M. Corballis, M.C. Corballis, How apparent motion affects mental
report the direction in which they rotate the items. Another rotation: push or pull? Mem. Cogn. 21 (1993) 458 – 466.
[5] M.G. Eley, Identifying rotated letter-like symbols, Mem. Cogn. 10
way of investigating this fact is to analyze the time course of (1982) 25 – 32.
the voltage standard deviations rather than the time course [6] M. Heil, The functional significance of ERP effects during mental
of the mean voltages because more variation is expected to rotation, Psychophysiology 39 (2002) 535 – 545.
be found if subjects use the long way rotations in some trials [7] M. Heil, B. Rolke, Towards a chronopsychophysiology of mental
rotation, Psychophysiology 39 (2002) 414 – 422.
and the short way rotations in other trials. Unfortunately,
[8] M. Heil, J. Bajric, F. Rfsler, E. Hennighausen, Event-related
raw ERP traces show so much background noise that the potentials during mental rotation: disentangling the contributions of
increase in variation due to occasional long routes may not character classification and image transformation, J. Psychophysiol.
be detectable. Overall, ERPs are a useful measure for 10 (1996) 326 – 335.
clarifying the kind of strategy that subjects use to make their [9] M. Heil, J. Bajric, F. Rfsler, E. Hennighausen, A rotation aftereffect
decision when there is a discrepancy between perceived and changes both the speed and the preferred direction of mental rotation,
J. Exp. Phychol. Human 23 (1997) 681 – 692.
mental rotation. [10] M. Heil, M. Rauch, E. Henninghausen, Response preparation begins
In conclusion, this study extends previous work on before mental rotation is finished: evidence from event-related brain
mental rotation and reiterates the effects of angular disparity potentials, Acta Psychol. 99 (1998) 217 – 232.
316 M.I. Núñez-Peña et al. / Cognitive Brain Research 24 (2005) 307–316

[11] M. Inoue, A. Yoshino, A. Suzuki, T. Ogasawara, S. Nomura, [17] J. Polich, A. Kok, Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: an
Topographic study of human event-related potentials using a task integrative review, Biol. Psychol. 41 (1995) 103 – 146.
requiring mental rotation, Neurosci. Lett. 253 (1998) 107 – 110. [18] F. Rfsler, G. Schumacher, B. Sojka, What the brain reveals when it
[12] P. Jolicoeur, The time to name disoriented objects, Mem. Cogn. 13 thinks. Event-related potentials during mental rotation and mental
(1985) 289 – 303. arithmetic, Ger. J. Psychol. 14 (1990) 185 – 203.
[13] P. Jolicoeur, P. Cavanagh, Mental rotation, physical rotation, and [19] F. Rfsler, M. Heil, J. Bajric, A.C. Pauls, E. Hennighausen, Patterns of
surface media, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. 18 (1992) 371 – 384. cerebral activation while mental images are rotated and changed in
[14] P. Jolicoeur, M.C. Corballis, R. Lawson, The influence of perceived size, Psychophysiology 32 (1995) 135 – 149.
rotary motion on the recognition of rotated objects, Psychon. Bull. 5 [20] R.N. Shepard, J. Metzler, Mental rotation of three-dimensional
(1998) 140 – 146. objects, Science 191 (1971) 701 – 703.
[15] S.D. Muthukumaraswamy, B.W. Johnson, J.P. Hamm, A high density [21] A.A. Wijers, L.J. Otten, S. Feenstra, G. Mulder, L.J.M. Mulder, Brain
ERP comparison of mental rotation and mental size transformation, 52 potentials during selective attention, memory search, and mental
(2003) 271 – 280. rotation, Psychophysiology 26 (1989) 452 – 467.
[16] F. Peronnet, M.J. Farah, Mental rotation: an event-related potential study [22] A. Yoshino, M. Inoue, A. Suzuki, A topographic electrophysiologic
with a validated mental rotation task, Brain Cogn. 9 (1989) 279 – 288. study of mental rotation, Cogn. Brain Res. 9 (2000) 121 – 124.

You might also like