You are on page 1of 8

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/331224631

Self-Knowledge

Chapter · February 2019


DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_2004-1

CITATIONS READS

0 785

1 author:

Henryk Bukowski
University of Vienna
35 PUBLICATIONS   358 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Workshop (7th November 2019, Brussels) "From Self-knowledge to knowing others: Insights from clinical populations with socio-cognitive disorders" View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Henryk Bukowski on 26 September 2019.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


S How to cite (APA style):
Bukowski, H.(2019). Self-Knowledge.
In V. Zeigler-Hill & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.),
Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences
(pp. 1–5). Cham: Springer International Publishing.
doi:10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_2004-1

Self-Knowledge Introduction

Henryk Bukowski The scattered current state of the study of Self-


Social Perception Reasoning Interaction knowledge can be attributed to the fact that vari-
Neuroscience Group, Psychological Sciences ous domains of psychology have focused on
Research Institute, Université catholique de specific aspects of Self-knowledge, such as its
Louvain, Louvain-La-Neuve, Belgium domains of knowledge, the processes contributing
to or influencing Self-knowledge (e.g., self-
awareness), and the qualities of Self-knowledge
(e.g., accuracy and structure). Self-knowledge can
Synonyms
be about any aspect of the Self; this explains why
no consensus has as yet been reached about the
Self-appraisals; Self-insight; Self-perceptions;
organization of these aspects into domains and
Self-views
their terminology. Neisser (1988), for instance,
distinguishes five kinds of Self-knowledge,
pertaining to the ecological, interpersonal,
Definition extended, private, and conceptual selves. In prac-
tice, however, research on Self-knowledge seems
Self-knowledge refers to the collection of repre- organized according to the following main
sentations believed to truly and accurately depict domains: episodic and sematic autobiographical
the Self. Like classic knowledge, Self-knowledge memories, personality traits, attitudes, social iden-
is acquired, stored, retrieved, and organized, and it tity, emotions, physical attributes, reputational
conveys meaning and guidance on how to interact attributes (e.g., public image, likeability), partner
with the environment, in particular with other or relationship knowledge, goals and motives,
social beings. Unlike classic knowledge, Self- physical and mental health, preferences and
knowledge is not learnt from any textbooks or values, metacognitive knowledge, and future
media but essentially from introspection and inter- actions and performance. An emerging pattern
actions. Importantly, Self-knowledge exists in across these domains is the separation of Self-
many forms, such as knowledge of our past, our knowledge between controlled, explicit, con-
personality, or our life goals, and its accuracy is scious, goal-driven processing of self-relevant
often difficult if not impossible to evaluate information and automatic, implicit, unconscious
objectively. processes influencing the processing of Self-
knowledge. Unconscious processes typically
© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019
V. Zeigler-Hill, T. K. Shackelford (eds.), Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_2004-1
2 Self-Knowledge

bias the access to and formation of Self- explicitly encoded as such, and explicit memory
knowledge, they are generally referred to as of the Self develops (Hart and Matsuba 2012;
the blind spots. Accuracy is therefore a central Ross et al. 2011).
measure in Self-knowledge despite the lack of a
perfect criterion to ascertain the accuracy of Self-
knowledge. A common criterion is self-other Storing and Organizing the Self
overlap, which is the extent to which other peo-
ple’s impressions match self-views. These impres- The central storing unit of the Self is autobio-
sions can be provided by a single person but are graphical memory, which is an explicit memory
generally constructed from the average of several system allowing to access episodic Self-
people; it is known as the social consensus crite- knowledge by recollecting episodes of our life
rion. These people can be strangers (giving and the semantic Self-knowledge by retrieving
their first impressions), acquaintances, or judges facts about ourselves that have been abstracted
(experts trained to judge particular attributes of a or inferred from commonalities across episodes
person). Other criteria are the pragmatic crite- (D’Argembeau 2015). Autobiographical memory
rion, which assesses the extent to which self- also indexes self-views resulting from future
views predict future actual behavior, and the thinking, which refers to imagining oneself in
objective criterion, which is provided by existing the future, either as a first-person-experienced
standardized measures, such as academic perfor- episode, commonly referred to as episodic future
mance and IQ, and experience-sampling devices. thinking, or as an abstract thinking about future
Finally, more domain-specific criteria could be the personal goals and self-schemas (D’Argembeau
statistical plausibility of a self-view (e.g., better 2015). In addition to accessing already acquired
than average) or the consistency with previously Self-knowledge, autobiographical memory allows
reported self-views (Schriber and Robins 2012). to form new Self-knowledge by recollecting our
The present review synthesizes Self- past experiences and inferring what they say about
knowledge research into sections addressing us. Autobiographical memory also provides a
how Self-knowledge develops and is stored, orga- sense of continuity of the Self over time and
nized, examined, influenced, and socialized. ensures the maintenance of a coherent Self-
identity, including established social connections.
Critically, memory recollection is often a recon-
The Developing Self struction exercise that is easily biased by the
selective retrieval of features that are, for instance,
To build up Self-knowledge, one needs a minimal most accessible to mind (accessibility bias) and
form of self-recognition capacities to recognize most congruent with current mood (mood con-
and dissociate self- from non-self-information. gruency bias), by filling in the voids (especially
Infant behaviors indicate they represent their when recollection is based on semantic memories)
own body as distinct from other entities, over or by appropriating non-lived memories as our
which they can exert control and thus experience own (due to failed source monitoring). These
a sense of body ownership, a sense of agency, biased and false memories are eventually stored
and, more generally, a subjective self (Gergely as if they were authentic memories (Kelley and
2002). By year 2 they explicitly recognize their Jacoby 2012).
body and their face as their own. By year 3, they Aggregations of episodic and semantic Self-
can tell whether an object was remembered in a knowledge form distinct Self-schema, which are
self-referent context or not, and they show a mne- sets of representations thematically organized
monic advantage for material referring to self, around key features of our life (e.g., my life goal
which is commonly referred to as the self- to win an athletic competition, my insomnia, or
reference effect. From that point, the Self as my high school years). Different organizations of
agent and experiencer becomes implicitly and the Self-schema have been proposed. Cognitive
Self-Knowledge 3

neuroscience distinguishes between the concep- to think about one’s own thoughts
tual Self containing most abstract representations (as self-awareness) but is often assimilated to the
such as life goals and Self-identities, the general formation of and access to knowledge about one’s
knowledge containing generic information about skills and to the real-time monitoring of perfor-
the Self and the past, and the episodic memory mance and learning. These cognitive processes or
system containing the detailed memories as they states may be considered as prerequisites to access
were lived (Conway et al. 2019). Another and form Self-knowledge via introspection, but
approach, known as Self-structure research, is they don’t necessarily lead to accurate Self-
to consider that the Self can be multiple, since knowledge, which actually depends on the type
we have different identities or roles in different of introspection/self-awareness achieved (Hixon
contexts (e.g., as a mother versus as a police and Swann 1993). The main distinctions are
agent). Two main measures of the multiplicity of (i) private/public self-awareness, whether the
the Self are termed Self-complexity or Self- self-information is private (e.g., my secret
Concept differentiation. Self-complexity high- motives) or public (e.g., how others see me);
lights the richness of the Self and its adaptive (ii) ruminative/reflective self-awareness,
functions (e.g., adapt to different contexts), whether the motives are negative feelings or an
whereas Self-Concept differentiation emphasizes intellectual curiosity for the self; and (iii) whether
the fragmentation or compartmentalization of the introspection aims to address why one experi-
the Self and its maladaptive outcomes (e.g., frag- ences a specific mental state versus what the
ile and incoherent identity). Relatedly, Self- mental state is; ruminative and why-driven intro-
concept clarity (or Self-clarity) inspects the spection are less likely to result in accurate
extent to which our Self-identity is clearly Self-knowledge (Hixon and Swann 1993;
and confidently defined, coherent, and stable Morin 2011).
(Pilarska and Sucha 2015; Showers et al. 2015). A characteristic feature and function of intro-
spective reasoning and autobiographical memory
reconstruction is the narrative Self. Narrative
Examining the Self psychology distinguishes between two modes of
thinking, the paradigmatic mode, which uses
The most important activity by which we reach logical explanations to build a rational account
Self-knowledge is Introspection, which refers to of reality, and the narrative mode, which uses
the action of examining our inner world (e.g., subjectively meaningful interpretations to build a
feelings, goals, memories) in contrast to the exter- coherent account of our identity. Critically, the
nal world. Introspection is often further character- narratives serve to bind the past, present, and
ized by the following key concepts: Self-focus future episodic events of our lives into a coherent
refers to the mental activity of focusing attention temporal sequence. The narratives are also
on the Self. Self-awareness refers either to the marked by causal coherence (the narratives con-
capacity for introspection or the state of awareness tain explanations to link different sets of actions),
of one’s own thoughts (Silvia and Gendolla 2001). thematic coherence (the characteristic features of
Self-insight is generally considered as a synonym the identity (e.g., personality trait, life goal) are
to Self-knowledge, but its more technical defini- recurring themes of the narratives), and cultural
tion is an extended state of awareness which coherence (the narratives tend to espouse cultural
requires awareness of the (i) content of the expe- templates of how lives unfold). The narrative
rienced mental state (or its association with an mode forms Self-knowledge that is accurate only
object, such as “being mad at my neighbor”), to the extent that the story is believable by an
(ii) the cause that triggered the mental state, and external audience, while the paradigmatic mode
(iii) the past or future consequences of the mental forms accurate Self-knowledge to the extent that
state on behavior (Gawronski and Bodenhausen the explanation is verifiable despite the fact that
2012). Metacognition refers to the capacity these explanations may challenge the coherence
4 Self-Knowledge

of the identity (Adler 2012). Individuals may shift to the tendency to consider oneself to be the cause
from one mode to another, depending on their of positive outcomes and external and/or uncon-
motives. trollable factors to be the cause of negative out-
comes. The Self-verification motive, on the other
hand, is fuelling the confirmation bias, which
Influencing the Self translates into selecting information that confirms
our beliefs, including beliefs about our self-
Our construction of Self-knowledge is guided identity. In contrast to self-enhancement, self-
by four main motives. The Self-enhancement depreciation (or self-diminishing) consists in
motive (or positivity striving) refers to striving producing negatively biased self-views, including
to put the Self in a positive light and away from seeing oneself as lower than average or expecting
threats. The Self-assessment motive refers to the pessimistic future outcomes; self-depreciation
need to achieve accurate Self-knowledge in order characterizes psychological disorders such as
to reduce uncertainty about the Self. The Self- depression and anxiety. An underlying illusion
verification (or self-coherence) motive refers to partly explaining these biases and illusions is the
striving for maintaining self-perceptions coherent introspection illusion, which refers to over-
with the established Self-identity or self-views weighting self-information originating from intro-
held by important others. The Self-improvement spection (in opposition to external sources) when
(or self-expansion) motive refers to striving to forming Self-knowledge. Additional biases in
develop new facets of the Self (Strube 2012). Self-knowledge may also originate from other
The preponderance of one motive over others known unconscious tendencies or attitudes, such
varies across contexts (see below) but also as social conformity, prejudices, and egocentrism
depending on individuals’ self-esteem, clarity (Hansen and Pronin 2012; Leary and Toner 2012;
of the self, or personality types (Schriber and Schultheiss and Strasser 2012). Finally, the most
Robins 2012). drastic case of inaccuracy in Self-knowledge is
These motives can lead to various forms of self-deception, which refers to inaccurate beliefs
bias and illusion in our Self-knowledge. Fuelled that are so deeply motivated that they resist aware-
by the Self-enhancement motive, the most perva- ness of contradictory evidence (Paulhus and
sive bias is the tendency to hold or produce pos- Buckels 2012).
itively biased self-views, which goes along with a Generally speaking, the probability for Self-
series of related illusions. The “better-than-aver- knowledge to be biased depends on which motive
age” illusion consists in believing that we are best fits the context and the verifiability of Self-
better than the average other on many aspects knowledge. The benefits of expressing positively
despite statistical unlikelihood. The unrealistic biased Self-knowledge, for instance, such as
optimism illusion translates into expecting unre- building a positive public self-image, often out-
alistically positive outcomes, especially if they weigh the associated risks, such as social rejection
result from our actions. The illusion of control (due to overestimation of social status). However,
consists in over-confidence that an expected out- contexts in which accuracy is valued and external
come is dependent on our own actions. Relatedly, standards are available will prompt Self-
magical thinking translates into thinking we are assessment. Verifiability depends on whether the
the cause of an outcome when there is no scientific content of Self-knowledge has low observability
account of this causation. The prediction illusion (or external visibility) or no clear standards (e.g.,
consists in being overconfident in accurately pre- being creative); low verifiability Self-knowledge
dicting our future behaviors or mental states. The tend to be biased (Strube 2012). Congruently,
illusion of objectivity (or naïve realism) consists self-observation of behavior is consistently
in erroneously believing that our decisions and found to be less biased than other ways to
perceptions are objective and thus devoid of form Self-knowledge. However, uncertainty
biases. The self-serving attributional bias refers (or mutability) about a trait (e.g., generous)
Self-Knowledge 5

has been shown to foster associated behaviors particularly reputational attributes (likeability,
(e.g., give to a charity) that signals to the Self a attractiveness, social status) (Srivastava 2012).
positive self-view about this trait; a phenomenon Other people’s perceptions are generally more
known as self-signalling (Bodner and Prelec accurate than individuals’ perceptions of them-
2003). Finally, ignorance and misinformation selves for attributes with a high motivational rel-
are obvious contributors to inaccurate Self- evance because motives are likely to bias self-
knowledge. perceptions. Other people are however less accu-
rate for low observability attributes (Vazire 2010).
Reliance on other people as a source of Self-
The Socializing Self knowledge varies across individuals and contexts.
For instance, younger people are more likely to
The second main source of information to form assimilate others’ inputs than adults because their
Self-knowledge is other people, by comparing to self-concept is not yet well-established. Self-
them, by reflecting on the impressions of us they enhancement and self-verification motives might
express, or by inferring their impressions of lead individuals to disregard, respectively, nega-
us. Social comparison is a pervasive mental tive appraisals from others (via self-serving attri-
activity leading to Self-knowledge through either bution bias) and appraisals conflicting established
assimilation (“I am like my best friend”) or con- self-views (via the objectivity illusion). In addi-
trast (“I am smarter than my neighbor”). Accuracy tion, individuals are more likely to weight the
can be compromised by self-enhancement opinions of people they wish to affiliate with,
motives as we tend to intentionally perform according to the social tuning hypothesis
assimilative comparisons against similar people (Srivastava 2012).
and contrastive upward or downward compari-
sons with superior or inferior others, respectively
(Suls et al. 2002). Reflected appraisals are the Conclusion
appraisals of the Self expressed by others, which
have been converted into self-appraisal to form Self-knowledge covers any type of information
Self-knowledge. Before being converted into relevant to the Self, from knowledge of physical
Self-knowledge, the appraisals that others have appearances to knowledge of the limited Self-
of us are also referred to as metaperceptions, knowledge we possess. We all seek Self-
and accuracy about how accurately we guess knowledge, not necessarily for its accuracy but
how people in general or a specific person also for giving a meaningful narrative to our
sees us is termed generalized or dyadic past, present, and future actions, a sense of conti-
(or differential) meta-accuracy, respectively. nuity over time, a sense of being both unique
Although highly correlated with Self-knowledge, and similar to others, a sense of being tied
metaperceptions are not believed to be true depic- to other people, and a sense of having one coher-
tions of the Self but simply external subjective ent and stable identity. Is accuracy necessary to
views of the Self; knowledge of this distinction well-being? The answer is a matter of debate.
is called meta-insight. Metaperceptions have Overestimating your capabilities leads to system-
their own biases such as the tendency to assume atic failures, frustrations, and risky choices, but it
that other people see us as we see them, known as also prompts confidence and motivation. Self-
assumed reciprocity, and that they share our enhancing illusions seem helpful to cope with
mental attributes (e.g., personality traits), known stressful, challenging, and traumatic events but
as assumed similarity. The knowledge typically not to bond lasting relationships. Maladjustment
inferred in these social contexts pertains to the and psychopathology are associated with inaccu-
social (or interpersonal) Self, which, namely, rate Self-knowledge, but one can ponder whether
includes knowledge of our social identities (e.g., mental health is simply a prerequisite to accept
group membership), family ties, social skills, and accurate self-views. Most importantly, believing
6 Self-Knowledge

to be more intelligent, important, and rightful cognitive development (pp. 26–46). Malden: Blackwell
than others is the starting point, not for compro- Publishers.
Hansen, K. E., & Pronin, E. (2012). Illusions of self-
mise but for acts of violence such as world wars, knowledge. In S. Vazire & T. D. Wilson (Eds.), Hand-
genocides, and terrorism (Leary and Toner 2012). book of self-knowledge (pp. 345–362). New York:
Unfortunately, on the path to accurate Self- Guilford Press.
knowledge, we all walk blind to the very illusions Hart, D., & Matsuba, M. K. (2012). The development of
self-knowledge. In S. Vazire & T. D. Wilson (Eds.),
that bias our knowledge of ourselves. Handbook of self-knowledge (pp. 7–21). New York:
Guilford Press.
Hixon, J. G., & Swann, W. B. (1993). When does intro-
spection bear fruit? Self-reflection, self-insight, and
Cross-References interpersonal choices. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 64(1), 35–43.
Kelley, C. M., & Jacoby, L. L. (2012). Past selves and
▶ Agency
autobiographical memory. In S. Vazire & T. D. Wilson
▶ Metacognition (Eds.), Handbook of self-knowledge (pp. 293–309).
▶ Self-Appraisals New York: Guilford Press.
▶ Self-Awareness Leary, M. R., & Toner, K. (2012). Reducing egoistic biases
in self-beliefs. In S. Vazire & T. D. Wilson (Eds.),
▶ Self-Complexity
Handbook of self-knowledge (pp. 413–428).
▶ Self-Concept New York: Guilford Press.
▶ Self-Concept Clarity Morin, A. (2011). Self-awareness part 1: Definition, mea-
▶ Self-Concept Structure sures, effects, functions, and antecedents. Social and
Personality Psychology Compass, 10, 807–823.
▶ Self-Enhancement Bias
Neisser, U. (1988). Five kinds of self-knowledge.
▶ Self-Enhancement Motives Philosophical Psychology, 1, 35–59.
▶ Self-Reflection Paulhus, D. L., & Buckels, E. (2012). Classic self-
▶ Self-Schema deception revisited. In S. Vazire & T. D. Wilson
(Eds.), Handbook of self-knowledge (pp. 363–378).
New York: Guilford Press.
Pilarska, A., Sucha, A. (2015). Self-complexity and self-
concept differentiation – what have we been measuring
References for the past 30 years?. Current Psychology 34 723–743.
Ross, J., Anderson, J. R., Campbell, R. N., &
Adler, J. M. (2012). Sitting at the nexus of epistemological Collins, W. A. (2011). I remember me: Mnemonic
traditions: Narrative psychological perspectives on self-reference effects in preschool children
self-knowledge. In S. Vazire & T. D. Wilson (Eds.), (Monographs of the society for research in child devel-
Handbook of self-knowledge (pp. 327–344). opment, Vol. 76, No. 3, pp. 1–102). Boston: Wiley-
New York: Guilford Press. Blackwell.
Bodner, R., & Prelec, D. (2003). Self-signaling and Schriber, R. A., & Robins, R. W. (2012). Self-knowledge:
diagnostic utility in everyday decision making. The An individual-differences perspective. In S. Vazire &
Psychology of Economic Decisions, 1, 105–126. T. D. Wilson (Eds.), Handbook of self-knowledge
Conway, M. A., Justice, L. V., & D’Argembeau, A. (2019). (pp. 105–130). New York: Guilford Press.
The self-memory system revisited: Past, present, and Schultheiss, O. C., & Strasser, A. (2012). Referential pro-
future. In J. H. Mace (Ed.), The organization and struc- cessing and competence as determinants of congruence
ture of autobiographical memory (pp. x–x). New York: between implicit and explicit motives. In S. Vazire &
Oxford University Press. T. D. Wilson (Eds.), Handbook of self-knowledge
D’Argembeau, A. (2015). Self-knowledge. In Arthur W. (pp. 39–62). New York: Guilford Press.
Toga (Ed.), Brain mapping (Vol. 3, pp. 35–39). Showers, C. J., Ditzfeld, C. P., & Zeigler-Hill, V. (2015).
Academic Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA. Self-concept structure and the quality of self-
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2012). Self-insight knowledge. Journal of Personality, 83(5), 535–551.
from a dual-process perspective. In S. Vazire & Silvia, P. J., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2001). On introspection
T. D. Wilson (Eds.), Handbook of self-knowledge and self-perception: Does self-focused attention
(pp. 22–38). New York: Guilford Press. enable accurate self-knowledge? Review of General
Gergely, G. (2002). The development of understanding self Psychology, 5(3), 241–269.
and agency. In Blackwell handbook of childhood
Self-Knowledge 7

Srivastava, S. (2012). Other people as a source of self- Suls, J., Martin, R., & Wheeler, L. (2002). Social compar-
knowledge. In S. Vazire & T. D. Wilson (Eds.), ison: Why, with whom, and with what effect? Current
Handbook of self-knowledge (pp. 90–104). New York: Directions in Psychological Science, 11(5), 159–163.
Guilford Press. Vazire, S. (2010). Who knows what about a person? The
Strube, M. J. (2012). From “out there” to “in here”: Impli- self-other knowledge asymmetry (SOKA) model. Jour-
cations of self-evaluation motives for self-knowledge. nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(2),
In S. Vazire & T. D. Wilson (Eds.), Handbook of self- 281–300.
knowledge (pp. 397–412). New York: Guilford Press.

View publication stats

You might also like