You are on page 1of 10

Psychology of Sport and Exercise 28 (2017) 68e77

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Psychology of Sport and Exercise


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychsport

How perceived autonomy support and controlling coach behaviors are


related to well- and ill-being in elite soccer players: A within-person
changes and between-person differences analysis
Boris Cheval a, b, Aïna Chalabaev a, Eleanor Quested c, Delphine S. Courvoisier d,
Philippe Sarrazin a, *
a
Univ. Grenoble Alpes, SENS, F-38041 Grenoble, France
b
Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
c
School of Psychology & Speech Pathology, Curtin University, GPO Box U1987, Perth, Western Australia, 6845, Australia
d
Division of Epidemiology, University Hospital of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Objectives: Grounded in Basic Psychological Needs Theory (Ryan & Deci, 2002), this study examined the
Received 27 September 2015 temporal relationships between perception of coaches' autonomy support and different facets of con-
Received in revised form trolling behaviors, the satisfaction-frustration of athletes' basic needs, and subjective vitality, self-esteem
11 October 2016
and burnout in elite sportsmen.
Accepted 25 October 2016
Methods: Participants (N ¼ 110 males) from three elite youth soccer academies in northwest France
Available online 26 October 2016
completed a questionnaire on three occasions during the last three months of the competitive season.
Results: Linear mixed models revealed that perceptions of coach-autonomy support and only two facets
Keywords:
Self-determination theory
of controlling coach behaviors (excessive personal control and negative conditional regard) were related
Multilevel to basic need satisfaction-frustration, which in turn were related to the indices of well- and ill-being. In
Intra-individual differences most cases, the relationships were observed both at the within- and between-person levels, but some
Inter-individual differences were observed only at one level.
Conclusions: The findings highlight the importance of considering the different facets of controlling
coach behaviors separately and disaggregating the between-person and within-person effects.
© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Sport participation is associated with emotional well-being Basic psychological needs theory (BPNT), a sub-theory of self-
(Adie, Duda, & Ntoumanis, 2008) and lower risk of depression determination theory (SDT; e.g., Ryan & Deci, 2002), appears to
(Jacka et al., 2011) among children and adolescents. However, be a relevant framework to account for both the “dark” and “bright”
participation in sport is not always health conducive; the physical side of people's functioning (Ryan & Deci, 2000). It specifies three
and mental demands placed on athletes can lead to negative psy- innate psychological needs, the satisfaction or frustration of which
chological experiences (Bartholomew, Ntoumanis, Ryan, Bosch, & affects psychological well- and ill-being: the needs for competence
Thogersen-Ntoumani, 2011). Because they are actively involved in (i.e., feelings of effectiveness in interacting with one's environ-
training and competition, coaches are assumed to play a critical role ment), autonomy (i.e., feelings of volition and the self-endorsement
in shaping the quality of players' sport experience, depending on of one's activity), and relatedness (i.e., feelings of love and care by
their motivational style (Duda, 2013). It is therefore crucial to significant others such as coaches and teammates). Whereas the
identify which coach behaviors result in psychological well- and ill- satisfaction of the psychological needs results in optimal human
being for their athletes, as well as the social-psychological pro- functioning and personal well-being, need frustration contributes
cesses that might explain these outcomes. to diminished personal functioning and ill-being (Deci & Ryan,
2000). Need frustration is experienced when athletes' basic psy-
chological needs are not just unsatisfied, but actively thwarted by
others within the sporting context, leading the athlete to feel
* Corresponding author. Laboratoire SENS, UFRAPS, Univ. Grenoble Alpes, BP 53,
incompetent, isolated, and controlled by others (Bartholomew
38041 Grenoble, France.
E-mail address: philippe.sarrazin@univ-grenoble-alpes.fr (P. Sarrazin). et al., 2011).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2016.10.006
1469-0292/© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
B. Cheval et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 28 (2017) 68e77 69

Importantly, SDT considers that the perceived social environ- questions such as whether over time variability in need
ment - notably autonomy supportive and controlling behaviors of satisfaction-frustration may be explained by over time fluctuations
an authority figure such as a coach - is an important determinant of in perceptions of coach behavior. For example, if an athlete feels
basic needs satisfaction and frustration. Autonomy support is evi- that in recent weeks the coach is less accepting of him or her than
denced when a coach involves others in decisions, provides real usual if he or she has disappointed him or her (i.e., the negative
choice, reduces pressures, and takes others' perspectives into ac- conditional regard facet of the controlling style) this may lead to
count (e.g., Mageau & Vallerand, 2003). In autonomy supportive feelings of rejection and isolation in relation to the coach (i.e., a
environments, athletes are more likely to experience higher need decrease in relatedness toward the coach) at that point in time. This
satisfaction and well-being (e.g., Adie et al., 2008; Amorose & type of within-person association between controlling behaviors
Anderson-Butcher, 2007; Felton & Jowett, 2013; Quested & Duda, and relatedness has been shown in parenting literature (e.g., Assor,
2011). In contrast, a controlling style of coaching is “a coercive, Roth, & Deci, 2004). However, these may be fleeting experiences of
pressuring, and authoritarian way to impose a specific and pre- need frustration that perhaps do not endure, or do not reflect how
conceived way of thinking and behaving upon athletes” the athlete typically feels. By contrast, between-person differences
(Bartholomew, Ntoumanis, & Thogersen-Ntoumani, 2010, p. 194). reflect a player's average levels over a specified period of time,
In such an environment, athletes are more likely to report lower contrasted with other players in the dataset. This facilitates the
needs satisfaction (e.g., Blanchard, Amiot, Perreault, Vallerand, & answering of questions such as whether variations in need
Provencher, 2009; Felton & Jowett, 2013; Isoard-Gautheur, Guil- satisfaction-frustration between players can be explained by dif-
let-Descas, & Lemyre, 2012; Kipp & Weiss, 2013), higher needs ferences in how controlling players perceive their coach to be. For
frustration (e.g., Balaguer et al., 2012; Bartholomew et al., 2011), example, sustained levels of negative conditional regard over a
maladaptive health outcomes (e.g., eating disorders, burnout, and longer period (i.e., when a player reports a higher average level of
physical symptoms), and perturbed physiological arousal prior to the coach's controlling behavior over a period than other players)
training (e.g., Bartholomew et al., 2011). may not necessarily be negatively related to a player's relatedness
A number of studies in sport have supported SDT's mediation toward the coach. This may be because an athlete who has exten-
hypothesis, which predicts that autonomy supportive and/or con- sive sport experience may have become accustomed to such be-
trolling coach behaviors are conducive to well- or ill-being through haviors (which are not uncommon in the domain of high-level
basic needs satisfaction or frustration (see Ntoumanis, 2012 for a sport). As such, the effects may be buffered. In summary, dis-
review). The present study aims to extend this line of research in aggregating within-person changes from between-person differ-
two important ways: Firstly, we will investigate the unique impact ences enables us to specify the respective contribution of each
of different facets of controlling coach behaviors and autonomy- aspect of coaching style on well- and ill-being through basic needs
supportive styles upon the satisfaction-frustration of psychologi- satisfaction-frustration. Thus, it allows us to know if the observed
cal needs.1 Secondly we will separate within-person changes from relationships between variables are present only at the within- (i.e.,
between-person differences. Past research has only considered the short term intra-individual variations) and/or between- (i.e., inter-
controlling coaching style in a global manner (e.g., Balaguer et al., individual idiosyncratic differences) person level.
2012; Bartholomew et al., 2011; Isoard-Gautheur et al., 2012). To the best of our knowledge, the study by Adie, Duda, and
However, this style includes specific types of behaviors likely to Ntoumanis (2012) is unique in the approach taken to test BPNT in
differentially impact athletes' satisfaction-frustration of needs and the area of sport. With a sample of 91 male players from an elite
well- or ill-being. In the sport context, Bartholomew et al. (2010) youth soccer academy, results showed that intra-individual
have identified four distinct ways that coaches can be controlling: changes and inter-individual differences in perceptions of coach-
controlling use of rewards (i.e., using extrinsic rewards and praise to autonomy support positively predicted intra-individual changes
induce engagement or persistence in certain behaviors and secure and inter-individual differences in basic needs satisfaction. In turn,
athlete compliance), negative conditional regard (i.e., ignoring the intra-individual changes, but not inter-individual differences, in the
athlete and withdrawing attention and affection when desired at- needs for competence and relatedness predicted intra-individual
tributes or behaviors are not displayed), intimidation (i.e., display- changes in vitality. These findings confirmed that the relation-
ing power-assertive strategies designed to humiliate and belittle), ships observed at one level of analysis may not generalize to the
and excessive personal control (i.e., using intrusive monitoring and other level. Despite these advances in our understanding of BPNT in
decreeing strict limits in areas of life that are not directly related to sport, the study by Adie et al. presented three limitations. First, the
the sport). As far as we know, no study has examined whether these study focused solely on coach autonomy support without exam-
distinct ways that coaches can control athletes are differentially ining the effects of controlling coach behaviors upon needs.
related to the satisfaction-frustration of basic needs and in turn, to Consequently, it was not possible to determine the extent to which
well- and ill-being. perceptions of coach interpersonal control and coach autonomy
Another shortcoming of extant research on BPNT is that it has support were independently linked to the players' basic needs
typically relied on cross-sectional (e.g., Adie et al., 2008; Felton & satisfaction and well- and ill-being. Second, this study only
Jowett, 2013) or 2 time-point (e.g., Balaguer et al., 2012; Isoard- assessed need satisfaction. As highlighted above, low need satis-
Gautheur et al., 2012) data. In past work, analyses have typically faction is not synonymous with high need frustration. As such, a
been based on between-person effects and this approach is poorly scale assessing both satisfaction and frustration associated with
suited to evaluating within-person processes (e.g., Curran & Bauer, basic needs would better capture psychological experience as a
2011). As pointed out by Curran and Bauer (2011), it is important to whole and should therefore be particularly useful to explain vari-
separate within-person from between-person associations. ance in athletes' well-and ill-being indices. Third, the need for
Within-person changes refer to short-term deviations from a per- relatedness was only assessed with regards to other members of
son's average levels. This approach enables researchers to answer the team. However, athletes also vary in the degree of relatedness
they feel towards their coach (e.g., Kipp & Weiss, 2013). Examining
whether relatedness towards peers and coaches have independent
1
Given that we considered and measured satisfaction and frustration of basic
effects on well- and ill-being is interesting from both a conceptual
needs as the two ends of a continuum (see measures section), we will use the and applied perspective (see Ntoumanis, 2012). Accordingly, the
expression ‘satisfaction-frustration’ throughout the text. goal of the present study is to advance understanding of BPNT in
70 B. Cheval et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 28 (2017) 68e77

sport, by addressing these gaps in the literature. well and ill-being would be mediated by the satisfaction-
To summarize, based on BPNT and past research, we conducted frustration of basic needs (Hypothesis 3 [H3]). Finally, because of
a three month longitudinal field study to examine the temporal the lack of previous literature, the specific associations at the inter-
relationships between perceived coach autonomy support and the individual and intra-individual level were explored but no a priori
different facets of controlling behaviors, the satisfaction-frustration hypothesis was made. It is likely that some relationships appear at
of athletes' basic needs, and indices of well- and ill-being, in the one particular level and not at another (e.g., Adie et al., 2012; Curran
context of elite soccer. Aligned with SDT traditions (see Ryan & & Bauer, 2011). For example, as indicated above, it is possible that a
Deci, 2001) we measured subjective vitality (Ryan & Frederick, negative relationship between negative conditional regard and
1997), a positive and phenomenally accessible state of having en- relatedness toward the coach will be evident only at the within-
ergy available to the self. We also assessed self-esteem, an index of person level.
self-worth and self-acceptance (see Guerin, Marsh, & Famose,
2003), because it is often considered as a basic feature of well- 1. Method
being and a protective factor of mental health (e.g., Mann,
Hosman, Schaalma, & de Vries, 2004). Finally, to capture both the 1.1. Participants
brighter and darker sides of athletes experiences, we measured
athlete burnout (Raedeke & Smith, 2001) as a specific indicator of Participants were 110 males (Mean age ¼ 16.54, SD ¼ 1.99, range
athlete ill-being. It has been noted in the literature that athletes are 13e21) from seven teams from three elite soccer clubs, in north-
at risk of burnout, but it is not an inevitable outcome of sport west France. Specifically, in each club athletes from the under-15
participation. Understanding the social-psychological de- and under-17 age categories were invited. In addition, the reserve
terminants of burnout is critical, if interventions are to be devel- team of one club was also invited to participate in the current study.
oped to prevent it. SDT has recently become a popular framework On average, athletes trained for 15 h per week and they all
for the study of determinants of burnout (e.g., Balaguer et al., 2012; competed in national leagues.
Isoard-Gautheur et al., 2012; Quested & Duda, 2011). However, to
our knowledge, consideration of which facets of controlling coach 1.2. Procedure
behaviors are relevant to the manifestation of burnout, remains
unknown to date. The design of our study has several characteris- Ethical approval to conduct this study was granted by the in-
tics that add value to the existing research on this topic: First, we vestigators' University School ethics committee. A letter was sent to
take into account several characteristics of the coaches' motiva- the managers of the centers, the head coaches, and the parents
tional style, namely perceived autonomy support and the four as- detailing the purpose and protocol of the study. Participants pro-
pects of controlling coach behaviors. Such an approach enables us vided informed consent to volunteer for the study. APA guidelines
to examine whether certain facets of the coaches' motivational regarding anonymity and confidentiality were followed. Data were
style were more strongly related to satisfaction-frustration of some collected once per month in March, April, and May 2010. The first
basic needs than others. Second, we examine both relatedness to- wave of data collection involved 104 athletes, the second wave
ward peers, and relatedness toward the coach. This design makes it involved 101 athletes, and the third wave involved 93 athletes. Data
possible to examine whether relatedness with the coach and with collection took place in the participants' elite training centers. In
other athletes has differential effects on athletes' well-and ill-being. order to maintain anonymity, players' responses at the different
Third, we disaggregate within-person changes and between- time points were matched using a coding system.
person differences over a three month period in order to deter-
mine whether the relationships between coach motivational style, 1.3. Measures
basic needs satisfaction-frustration, and well- and ill-being indexes
are consistent at a micro- (i.e., within-person) and/or a macro- (i.e., 1.3.1. Perceived autonomy support
between-person) level. Fourth, we recruited a sample of young elite Athletes' perception of the autonomy supportive behaviors
players as this enables us to examine the role of coach motivational exhibited by their coaches was assessed using the short version of
style in contexts where competition is particularly salient (Conroy the Sport Climate Questionnaire. This 6-item questionnaire is a
& Coatsworth, 2007). The environment created by the coach may sport-adaptation of the Learning Climate Questionnaire (Williams
hold differential significance for elite athletes because of their & Deci, 1996). Athletes were asked the extent to which they
extensive sport experience compared to their grassroots agreed with each item (e.g., “I feel that my coach provides me
counterparts. choices and options”) on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1
Based on the aforementioned literature, we expected a media- (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Previous research (e.g.,
tional sequence between athletes’ perceptions of the coaches’ be- Adie et al., 2008) has confirmed the reliability and validity of this
haviors, the satisfaction-frustration of athletes’ basic needs, and tool. The back-translation method (Brislin, 1986) was used to
well- and ill-being. More precisely, we hypothesized, first, that translate the original scale into French. A Confirmatory Factor
autonomy support and the different facets of controlling coach Analysis (CFA) of the data taken at the first wave of data collection
behaviors would be associated with the satisfaction-frustration of offered support for the factorial validity of the tool (c2 (9) ¼ 13.14,
basic needs, in particular with autonomy (Hypothesis 1 [H1]). p ¼ .11, CFI ¼ .97, IFI ¼ .97, RMSEA ¼ .08), following the traditional
Because the current study was to our knowledge the first to sepa- cutoff criteria (e.g., Marsh, 2007) according to which values close to
rate each of the dimensions of controlling coach behaviors, the .08 for RMSEA, and around .90 for CFI and IFI are indices of
specific links between each facet and basic needs, and well- and ill- acceptable fit. Moreover, with an omega composite reliability (u)
being indices were explored but no a priori hypothesis was made. index (McDonald, 1999) of .76, the internal consistency was good,
Second, we hypothesized that the satisfaction-frustration of basic following the recommendations of Reise, Bonifay, and Haviland
needs would be associated with well- and ill-being, after control- (2013) according to which values greater than .50 are a mini-
ling for autonomy support and the different facets of controlling mum, and values closer to .75 would be much preferred.
coach behaviors (Hypothesis 2 [H2]). Third, we expected that the
relationships between perceived autonomy supportive coach be- 1.3.2. Perceived controlling behaviors
haviors and the different facets of controlling coach behaviors and The Controlling Coach Behaviors Scale (CCBS; Bartholomew
B. Cheval et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 28 (2017) 68e77 71

et al., 2010) was used to assess athletes' perceptions of controlling Frederick, 1997) was employed to measure athletes' feelings of
coach behaviors. The CCBS contains 15 items divided into four positive energy (e.g., “I feel alive and full of vitality”). Responses
subscales: controlling use of rewards (e.g., “My coach only uses were provided on a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (not true at all) to 7
rewards and praise so that I complete all the tasks he or she sets in (very true). In a previous study (Papaioannou et al., 2013) the scale
training”), negative conditional regard (e.g., “My coach is less demonstrated good factorial validity and reliability. In the current
accepting of me if I have disappointed him or her”), intimidation study a CFA was carried out on the first wave of data and offered
(e.g., “My coach threatens to punish me to keep me in line during support for the factorial validity of the scale (c2 (5) ¼ 9.00, p ¼ .11,
training”), and excessive personal control (e.g. “My coach tries to CFI ¼ .98, IFI ¼ .98, RMSEA ¼ .08). The u coefficient was .85. Burnout
interfere in aspects of my life outside of my sport”). Participants experienced by players was assessed using the French version of
responded to each item on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 the Athlete Burnout Questionnaire (ABQ; Isoard-Gautheur, Oger,
(strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The scale has demon- Guillet, & Martin-Krumm, 2010). The ABQ includes three 4-item
strated good content and factorial validity, as well as internal subscales that assess reduced sense of accomplishment (e.g., “It
consistency and invariance across gender and sport type seems that no matter what I do, I do not perform as well as I
(Bartholomew et al., 2010). The back-translation method was used should”), emotional and physical exhaustion (e.g., “I am exhausted
to translate the original scale into French. A CFA was carried out on by the mental and physical demands of soccer”), and devaluation
the first wave of data and offered support for the factorial validity of (e.g., “I am just not into soccer like I used to be”). Items are
a four-factor model (c2 (84) ¼ 134.99, p < .001, CFI ¼ .91, IFI ¼ .91, measured on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (almost never) to
RMSEA ¼ .09). The factor correlations were significant and ranged 5 (almost always). In the previous studies (e.g., Isoard-Gautheur
between .23 and .74. The u coefficients were good to minimally et al., 2010) the scale demonstrated good factorial validity and
acceptable (.88, .81, .77 and .57 for the controlling use of rewards, reliability. In the current study a CFA was carried out on the first
negative conditional regard, intimidation, and excessive personal wave of data and offered support for the factorial validity of a hi-
control subscales, respectively). erarchical model with three first-order latent factors and one
higher order latent factor (c2 (51) ¼ 75.72, p ¼ .014, CFI ¼ .92,
1.3.3. The satisfaction-frustration of basic needs IFI ¼ .92, RMSEA ¼ .07). The u coefficient was .83. The subscales
The satisfaction versus frustration of the three basic psycho- were combined to create a total burnout score. Athletes' self-
logical needs in sport was measured using the Needs Satisfaction- esteem was assessed using the general self-esteem subscale of
Thwarting Scale (NSTS; Cheval & Sarrazin, 2011). The NSTS uses a the French version of the Self-Description Questionnaire II (Guerin
7-point bipolar response format, each boundary representing the et al., 2003) (e.g., “Overall, most things I do turn out well”). Re-
frustration (3) versus the satisfaction (þ3) of a need and the sponses were provided on a 6-point Likert scale from 1 (False) to 6
median value (0) corresponding to a neutral sentiment. Following (True). In the previous studies (e.g., Guerin et al., 2003) the scale
the stem “In the past month, when you were playing soccer, you demonstrated good factorial validity and reliability. In the current
generally felt …”, participants were asked to respond to each of the study a CFA was carried out on the first wave of data and offered
16 items assessing autonomy (e.g., “I felt a lot of desire (vs. pres- support for the factorial validity of a hierarchical model with three
sure) to do things”), competence (e.g., “I felt capable (vs. incapable) first-order latent factors and one higher order latent factor (c2
of doing things correctly”), coach relatedness (e.g., “I felt supported (20) ¼ 27.20, p ¼ .13, CFI ¼ .95, IFI ¼ .93, RMSEA ¼ .06). The u co-
(vs. neglected) by my coach”), and peer relatedness (e.g., “I felt efficient was .77.
appreciated (vs. despised) by my teammates” satisfaction and
frustration. The use of such a bipolar format was motivated both by 1.4. Data analysis
theoretical and pragmatic reasons. From a theoretical point of view,
satisfaction and frustration scales cannot be totally independent. Data were analyzed using Linear Mixed Models (LMM) using the
Indeed, if low need satisfaction does not necessarily involve need R language lmerTest package, version 1.1e7. This package, like SAS
frustration, need frustration by definition involves low need satis- proc mixed, uses Satterthwaite's method to compute the degrees of
faction (Vansteenkiste & Ryan, 2013). The moderate to high cor- freedom of the t tests. LMM allows a correct estimation of param-
relations between need satisfaction and need frustration scales eters when data are hierarchically structured, i.e., with repeated
found in recent studies (e.g., Sheldon & Hilpert, 2012) provides measurements (i.e., Level 1) nested within each individual (i.e.,
support to this idea. Accordingly, we adopted a method of mea- Level 2), because LMM accounts for the shared variance due to
surement that reflects the perspective of a continuum (i.e., bipolar). multiple observations within the same participant (i.e., non-
From a pragmatic point of view, the use of NSTS allows a reduction independence). In addition, LMM does not require equal numbers
in the number of items to be completed. In previous studies (Cheval of responses from each participant. Therefore, players with missing
& Sarrazin, 2011) the scale demonstrated good factorial validity, values are not excluded from the analysis but contribute less to the
reliability and nomological validity (attested by expected correla- results (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). In the current study, the first
tions with motivation, athlete burnout and vitality). In the current wave of data collection involved 104 players (4.5% data missing),
study a CFA was carried out on the first wave of data and offered the second wave involved 101 players (8.2% data missing), and the
support for the factorial validity of a four-factor model (c2 third wave involved 93 players (15.5% data missing).
(98) ¼ 102.16, p ¼ .37, CFI ¼ .99, IFI ¼ .99, RMSEA ¼ .02). The factor In a first step, we separated each predictor into an individual
correlations were significant and ranged between .24 and .74. The u mean value over the three time points and an individual deviation
coefficients were good (.75, .78, .90 and .91 for the competence, score (from the individual mean value) for each time point. Each
autonomy, peer relatedness, and coach relatedness subscales, predictor was thus decomposed into two variables, the mean score
respectively). A constant of þ4 was added to each subscale score so capturing mean inter-individual differences and the deviation score
that the values ranged from one to seven. The higher the score, the capturing mean intra-individual change. Club membership was
more the need is satisfied; by contrast, the lower it is, the more the dummy-coded and introduced as a control variable in all models. To
need is frustrated. analyze the extent to which perceived coach behaviors (i.e., au-
tonomy support and each of the controlling behavior facets) and
1.3.4. Well-being and ill-being indicators the satisfaction-frustration of basic needs (i.e., autonomy, compe-
A French version of the Subjective Vitality Scale (SVS; Ryan & tence, coach relatedness, peer relatedness) predicted the well- and
72 B. Cheval et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 28 (2017) 68e77

ill-being outcomes (i.e., subjective vitality, burnout, and self- variance in the dependent variables was explained at the between-
esteem), a series of LMMs were built for each outcome variable. person level. They suggest that a considerable proportion of the
Time was coded as 0, 1, and 2 for the three time points, in order to total variance (between 69.6 and 39.1%) was attributable to the
estimate main effects at the beginning of the study. The random within-person level, confirming the utility of disaggregating be-
effects of the intercept, the linear slope, and the covariance slope- tween- and within-person effects.
intercept were included in each of the models, and the best
random structure was determined based on Bayesian information 2.2. Model A: perceptions of coach-autonomy support and
criterion (BIC) and on a likelihood ratio test. To explore whether the controlling behaviors as predictors of mean levels and change in the
relationships between predictors and outcome variables differed satisfaction-frustration of basic needs (Table 2)
across occasions, interaction terms (time  predictor) were also
entered into the level 1 and level 2 equations in all conditional At level 1, athletes' time-varying perceptions of autonomy
models. The model with the best fit to the data was again deter- support positively predicted the satisfaction-frustration of the need
mined based on BIC and on a likelihood ratio test (see Raudenbush for relatedness towards the coach (b ¼ 0.49, p < .001) and towards
& Bryk, 2002). peers (b ¼ 0.24, p < .001). Negative conditional regard negatively
In Model A, we regressed each basic needs satisfaction- predicted the satisfaction-frustration of the need for competence
frustration variable onto both athletes' perceptions of coach au- (b ¼ 0.12, p < .001) and of the need for relatedness towards the
tonomy support and each facet of the coach controlling behaviors. coach (b ¼ 0.23, p < .001). Excessive personal control negatively
In Model B, we regressed each well- and ill-being index onto the predicted the satisfaction-frustration of the need for autonomy
four basic needs satisfaction-frustration variables, controlling for (b ¼ 0.14, p ¼ .008). At level 2, athletes' average perceptions of
perceived coach autonomy support and each facet of the coach autonomy support positively predicted the satisfaction-frustration
controlling behaviors. Finally, the potential mediational effects of of the needs for relatedness towards the coach (b ¼ 0.57,
the satisfaction-frustration of basic needs were examined using the p < .001) and towards peers (b ¼ 0.28, p ¼ .022). Moreover, athletes'
a and b joint significance test (Cohen & Cohen, 1983). As indicated average perceptions of excessive personal control negatively pre-
by Judd, Yzerbyt, and Muller (2014), this test is as powerful as other dicted the satisfaction-frustration of the need for autonomy
tests of indirect effects (percentile bootstrap and numerical inte- (b ¼ 0.33, p ¼ .001).
gration tests), and it is the only test that does not suffer from Type I
error. It is also relevant in the case of multilevel models. Specifically, 2.2.1. Range of effect sizes at each level of analysis
this test has two steps. The first step is to establish a relationship At the within-person level the R21 values were .06 for compe-
between the predictors (i.e., perceived coach autonomy support tence, .02 for autonomy, .05 for peer relatedness, and .20 for coach
and controlling behaviors) and the mediators (i.e., the satisfaction- relatedness indicating that adding coach motivational style as
frustration of basic needs). The second step aims to determine predictors reduced the level 1 unexplained variance estimated in
whether there is a relationship between the mediators and the the model without such predictors by 6%, 2%, 5% and 20% respec-
outcomes (i.e., well- and ill-being indicators), after controlling for tively. At the between-person level the R22 values were .17 for
the effects of the predictors on the outcomes. These steps were competence, .33 for autonomy, .39 for peer relatedness, and .75 for
tested in Models A and B respectively. Then, we also calculated a coach relatedness.
95% confidence interval (CI) for the mediated effects using nu-
merical integration tests (e.g., Fritz, Taylor, & Mackinnon, 2012). 2.3. Model B: the satisfaction-frustration of basic needs predicting
Pseudo R21 and R22 values were calculated to estimate the pro- well- and ill-being, controlling for perceived coach autonomy
portional amount by which errors of prediction have been reduced support and controlling coach behaviors (Table 3)
from the unconditional model to the conditional model. These
values are an estimate of effect size, similar to the R2 value in 2.3.1. Subjective vitality
traditional ordinary least squares regression analyses (Singer & At level 1, athletes' time-varying satisfaction-frustration of the
Willett, 2003). need for competence (b ¼ 0.48, p < .001) and autonomy (b ¼ 0.24,
p < .001) positively predicted subjective vitality. At level 2, athletes'
2. Results average satisfaction-frustration of the need for competence
(b ¼ 0.51, p ¼ .010), and autonomy (b ¼ 0.26, p ¼ .018) positively
2.1. Descriptive statistics, Cronbach's alpha coefficients, and predicted subjective vitality.
intraclass correlations (ICC)
2.3.2. Burnout
We first checked the statistical assumptions associated with At level 1, athletes' time-varying perceptions of coach autonomy
linear mixed modeling by exploring the residuals in the full con- support (b ¼ 0.06, p ¼ .022), and satisfaction-frustration of the
ditional models. Plots of the standardized Level 1 residuals against need for competence (b ¼ 0.12, p ¼ .001), and autonomy
their normal scores showed a reasonably linear relationship, indi- (b ¼ 0.13, p < .001) negatively predicted burnout. At level 2,
cating relative normality and no extreme outliers. Furthermore, the athletes' average satisfaction-frustration of the need for compe-
residuals against the predicted scores of the outcome variables tence (b ¼ 0.15, p ¼ .022) and autonomy (b ¼ 0.11, p < .001)
showed no major signs of heteroscedasticity. Table 1 presents the negatively predicted burnout. By contrast, athletes' average per-
means, standard deviations and Cronbach's alpha coefficients for ceptions of excessive personal control positively predicted burnout
each variable at each time point, as well as the ICCs. All subscales at (b ¼ 0.12, p ¼ .005).
all time points (except excessive personal control at time 1) showed
acceptable internal consistency. Over the three-month period, 2.3.3. Self-esteem
athletes reported average levels of coach-controlling behaviors and At level 1, athletes' time-varying satisfaction-frustration of the
burnout under the scales' midpoints. Conversely, players' reported need for competence (b ¼ 0.25, p < .001), and autonomy (b ¼ 0.20,
subjective vitality, self-esteem, perception of autonomy support, p < .001) positively predicted self-esteem. At level 2, athletes'
and basic need satisfaction-frustration were above the scale mid- average satisfaction-frustration of the need for competence
points. The ICCs indicated that between 30.4 and 60.9% of the (b ¼ 0.25, p ¼ .002), autonomy (b ¼ 0.16, p < .001) and peer
B. Cheval et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 28 (2017) 68e77 73

Table 1
Means, standard deviations, Cronbach's alpha coefficients, and intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) of all study variables.

Variable Scale range Time 1 Time 2 Time 3 ICC

M SD Alpha M SD Alpha M SD Alpha

Perceived autonomy support 1e7 4.96 1.05 .79 4.79 1.05 .84 4.78 1.05 .85 .59
Controlling use of rewards 1e7 2.09 1.22 .85 1.97 1.15 .85 1.87 1.07 .88 .61
Negative conditional regard 1e7 3.58 1.40 .79 3.34 1.48 .82 3.29 1.36 .80 .65
Intimidation 1e7 2.71 1.27 .74 2.67 1.30 .80 2.68 1.20 .74 .70
Excessive personal control 1e7 2.27 1.28 .55 2.46 1.27 .62 2.21 1.15 .64 .55
Satisfaction-frustration of need for
Autonomy 1e7 5.71 1.05 .67 5.72 1.12 .76 5.80 1.05 .79 .59
Competence 1e7 4.72 0.69 .78 4.79 0.72 .76 4.93 0.75 .83 .58
Coach relatedness 1e7 5.00 1.20 .89 4.98 1.08 .76 5.04 1.17 .91 .42
Peer relatedness 1e7 5.64 1.04 .87 5.47 1.08 .90 5.42 1.25 .93 .46
Burnout 1e5 2.27 0.47 .74 2.16 0.46 .71 2.18 0.50 .79 .61
Self-esteem 1e6 4.70 0.62 .81 4.65 0.70 .75 4.74 0.72 .80 .51
Subjective vitality 1e7 4.38 1.13 .74 4.65 0.99 .78 4.53 1.16 .85 .30

Table 2
(Model A). Linear mixed models exploring within- and between- person variability in perception of coach autonomy support and controlling coach behaviors as predictors of
mean levels and change in psychological needs satisfaction-frustration.

Predictors Competence Autonomy Coach relatedness Peer relatedness

b SE b SE b SE b SE

Fixed effects
Intercept 4.45 0.12*** 5.35 0.17*** 4.46 0.15*** 5.09 0.17***
Time 0.07 0.04 0.02 0.05 0.02 0.06 0.09 0.06
Team membership (ref ¼ Club 1)
Club 2 0.52 0.14*** 0.62 0.20** 0.78 0.15*** 0.97 0.19***
Club 3 0.12 0.15 0.40 0.21 0.59 0.16*** 0.80 0.20***
Within-person level
Perceived autonomy support 0.03 0.04 0.12 0.07 0.49 0.06*** 0.24 0.07***
Controlling use of rewards 0.03 0.04 0.08 0.06 0.03 0.05 0.01 0.06
Negative conditional regards 0.12 0.04*** 0.09 0.05 0.23 0.05*** 0.01 0.06
Intimidation 0.02 0.04 0.07 0.06 0.02 0.06 0.12 0.07
Excessive personal control 0.02 0.03 0.14 0.05** 0.02 0.05 0.04 0.06
Between-person level
Perceived autonomy support 0.02 0.08 0.15 0.12 0.57 0.10*** 0.28 0.12*
Controlling use of rewards 0.10 0.07 0.01 0.10 0.06 0.08 0.08 0.10
Negative conditional regards 0.09 0.07 0.01 0.10 0.14 0.08 0.11 0.10
Intimidation 0.10 0.09 0.15 0.12 0.01 0.10 0.03 0.12
Excessive personal control 0.07 0.07 0.33 0.10** 0.04 0.08 0.12 0.10
Random effects
Random intercept 0.25 0.45 0.14 0.36
Error 0.19 0.46 0.60 0.63
R21 .06 .02 .20 .05
R22 .17 .33 .75 .39

Note. R12 and R22 values indicated the proportional amount of variance by which errors of prediction have been reduced from the unconditional model to the conditional model
at the within- and between- person level, respectively.*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

relatedness (b ¼ 0.16, p ¼ .019) positively predicted self-esteem. autonomy support and self-esteem. Finally, at both levels, the
satisfaction-frustration of the need for autonomy is likely to
2.3.4. Range of effect sizes at each level of analysis mediate the relationships between excessive personal control and
At the within-person level the R21 values were .15 for subjective the three dependent variables. The joint significance test revealed
vitality, .14 for burnout, and .17 for self-esteem, indicating that that, at level 1, time-varying satisfaction-frustration of the need for
adding the coach motivational style and basic needs satisfaction- competence mediated the relationships between negative condi-
frustration reduced the level 1 unexplained variance estimated in tional regard and vitality (indirect effect ¼ 0.058, 95% CI
the model without such predictors by 15%, 14% and 17% respec- [0.106, 0.018]), burnout (indirect effect ¼ 0.014, 95% CI [0.003,
tively. At the between-person level the R22 values were .76 for 0.03]), and self-esteem (indirect effect ¼ 0.030, 95% CI
subjective vitality, .51 for burnout and .67 for self-esteem. [0.056, 0.009]). At level 2, athletes' average satisfaction-
frustration of the need for relatedness towards peers mediated
2.4. Mediational analyses the relationship between perception of autonomy support and self-
esteem (indirect effect ¼ 0.033, 95% CI [0.001, 0.109]). Finally, at
Results from the A and B models revealed that, at level 1, ath- both levels, the satisfaction-frustration of the need for autonomy
letes' time-varying satisfaction-frustration of the need for compe- mediated the relationships between excessive personal control and
tence is likely to mediate the relationships between negative subjective vitality (indirect effects ¼ 0.034, 95% CI
conditional regard and the three dependent variables. At level 2, [0.067, 0.007] and 0.086, 95% CI [0.189, 0.011], at level 1 and
athletes' average satisfaction-frustration of the need for relatedness 2 respectively), and burnout (indirect effect ¼ 0.018, 95% CI [0.004,
toward peers is likely to mediate the relationship between 0.035] and 0.036, 95% CI [0.003, 0.082], at level 1 and 2
74 B. Cheval et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 28 (2017) 68e77

Table 3
(Model B). Linear mixed models exploring within- and between- person variability in basic needs satisfaction-frustration as predictors of mean levels and change in subjective
vitality, burnout and self-esteem, controlling for perceived coach autonomy support and controlling coach behaviors.

Predictors Subjective Vitality Burnout Self-esteem

b SE b SE b SE

Fixed effects
Intercept 4.51 0.16*** 2.34 0.07*** 4.61 0.09***
Time 0.01 0.06 0.02 0.02 0.02 0.03
Team membership (ref ¼ Club 1)
Club 2 0.10 0.18 0.21 0.08* 0.21 0.11
Club 3 0.08 0.17 0.01 0.09 0.14 0.11
Within-person level
Perceived autonomy support 0.12 0.07 0.06 0.03* 0.01 0.04
Controlling use of rewards 0.01 0.06 0.03 0.02 0.01 0.03
Negative conditional regards 0.06 0.05 0.03 0.02 0.04 0.03
Intimidation 0.01 0.06 0.03 0.03 0.01 0.04
Excessive personal control 0.04 0.05 0.01 0.02 0.02 0.03
Autonomy 0.24 0.07*** 0.13 0.03*** 0.20 0.04***
Competence 0.48 0.09*** 0.12 0.04** 0.25 0.05***
Coach relatedness 0.01 0.12 0.05 0.03 0.02 0.04
Peer relatedness 0.03 0.06 0.04 0.03 0.06 0.04
Between-person level
Perceived autonomy support 0.13 0.12 0.04 0.05 0.02 0.07
Controlling use of rewards 0.10 0.09 0.01 0.04 0.01 0.04
Negative conditional regards 0.08 0.09 0.01 0.04 0.06 0.05
Intimidation 0.04 0.10 0.03 0.05 0.01 0.06
Excessive personal control 0.13 0.86 0.12 0.04** 0.01 0.05
Autonomy 0.26 0.11* 0.11 0.05* 0.16 0.07*
Competence 0.51 0.13*** 0.15 0.06* 0.25 0.08**
Coach relatedness 0.01 0.12 0.05 0.03 0.04 0.08
Peer relatedness 0.07 0.11 0.03 0.05 0.16 0.07*
Random effects
Random Intercept 0.09 0.07 0.08
Error 0.71 0.08 0.19
R21 .15 .14 .17
R22 .76 .51 .67

Note. R21 and R22 values indicated the proportional amount of variance by which errors of prediction have been reduced from the unconditional model to the conditional model
at the within- and between- person level, respectively. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

respectively). players' need for autonomy. This relationship was observed both
when players perceived their coach as more controlling than usual
3. Discussion (i.e., within-person level), and more controlling than others did (i.e.,
between-person level), suggesting that such behavior could have a
The purpose of this three month longitudinal field study was to particularly strong impact on the satisfaction-frustration of the
evaluate both at a within-person and between-person level the need for autonomy for this sample. Moreover, within-person
BPNT-grounded hypotheses that the relationships between changes in negative conditional regard negatively predicted the
perceived autonomy support and controlling coach behaviors (i.e., satisfaction-frustration of the needs for competence and related-
controlling use of rewards, negative conditional regard, intimida- ness towards the coach. These findings reflect what has been
tion, and excessive personal control) on players' vitality, self- shown in the parenting literature (e.g., Assor et al., 2004); parents
esteem, and burnout would be sequentially mediated through the who provide less attention and affection than usual when their
satisfaction-frustration of athletes' basic needs, among elite soccer children do not enact desired behaviors experienced resentment
players. The results provided substantial but not complete support from the children (i.e., a reduction in their children's closeness
for our hypotheses. toward them). Our result suggests that in the coaching context, the
use of such controlling behavior by a coach could also convey a lack
3.1. Perceptions of autonomy support and controlling coach of trust in the athletes' ability to succeed and undermine or even
behaviors, and the satisfaction-frustration of basic psychological frustrate the players' need for competence and need for relatedness
needs towards the coach. Interestingly, these relationships were observed
only at the within-person level. At the between-person level,
Extending past studies (e.g., Adie et al., 2012) the analysis of the players who reported experiencing lower sustained negative con-
negative side of coach behaviors, revealed specific relationships ditional regard than others over the three-month period did not
between two facets of perceived controlling coach behaviors and report lower basic needs satisfaction-frustration. It may be that
the satisfaction-frustration of certain basic needs. Specifically, we players with extensive sport experience and greater maturity are
found excessive personal control to negatively predict within- better able to buffer some negative effects of controlling coach
person changes and between-person mean differences in behavior. That is, they may be able to accommodate some con-
satisfaction-frustration of the psychological need for autonomy. In trolling behaviors because they consider these to be part of an elite
other words, when coaches attempted to interfere in aspects of the sport culture. As such, sport experience and/or level may be what
players' lives that were not directly associated with their partici- acts as a buffer to the potentially harmful impact of controlling
pation in sport or expected the players' entire lives to center on coach behaviors. This proposal deserves to be investigated in future
sport participation, they undermined or even frustrated the studies.
B. Cheval et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 28 (2017) 68e77 75

The two remaining facets of controlling behaviors (i.e., con- without the coach controlling dimensions), results revealed that
trolling use of rewards and intimidation) were not related to the within-person changes in coach autonomy support positively pre-
satisfaction-frustration of any basic need. This unexpected result dicted the satisfaction-frustration of the need for autonomy
could be explained theoretically and statistically. From a statistical (b ¼ 0.15, p ¼ 0.015). These results highlight the need to take into
point of view, the two subscales are moderately to strongly (be- account both autonomy supportive and controlling coach behaviors
tween .31 and .74) correlated with the two other controlling coach to better understand the role of coach motivational style.
behaviors which exhibited relationships with basic needs (see Ultimately the five coach behaviors have predicted between 2
above). Because the estimates of the four controlling coach be- and 20 percent of the variance in the basic needs at the within-
haviors on basic needs represent the unique effect of each predic- person level and between 17 and 75 percent at the between-
tor, the lack of significant relationships for intimidation and for the person level. Other coach behaviors and other elements of the so-
controlling use of rewards could simply mean that these two be- cial environment (e.g., family or academic pressures) could be
haviors did not have sufficient remaining unique variance for sig- added to increase the percentage of variance explained in need
nificant associations to be observed in the analyses. From a satisfaction-frustration, particularly for the needs for autonomy
theoretical point of view, according to cognitive evaluation theory, and competence at the within-person level.
another SDT mini-theory (see Ryan & Deci, 2002), the functional
significance (i.e., attributed meaning) of the situation may differ 3.2. The satisfaction-frustration of basic psychological needs and
from one individual to another. In other words, the same social well- and ill-being
event can be perceived as having a higher or lower informational or
controlling salience. It is possible that the use of extrinsic rewards The present results support the BPNT proposition that the
or intimidation strategies can be so integrated into the elite sport satisfaction-frustration of basic psychological needs can substan-
culture that their influence on the satisfaction-frustration of basic tially account for both the “dark” and “bright” side of people's
needs becomes negligible. It is also possible that athletes become psychological health (Ryan & Deci, 2002). They highlight in
accustomed to viewing rewards positively, and are not fully cogent particular the fundamental role of the needs for autonomy and
of when rewards are used in a controlling manner, hence there competence in the domain of elite sport. Indeed, the more players
appears to be no evident negative impact upon the basic needs. felt they were at ‘the origin’ of their behavior and ‘competent’ in
However, without additional investigation it would be inappro- soccer, the higher subjective vitality and self-esteem they reported,
priate to interpret this finding to mean that such controlling be- and the lower their burnout. Supporting the contention that these
haviors can be regarded as harmless. needs are key nutriments for optimal psychological functioning
Regarding the positive side of coach behavior, in agreement with among the elite adolescent soccer players (e.g., Adie et al., 2012),
past studies (e.g., Adie et al., 2012) results revealed that within- such relationships were found both at the intrapersonal and inter-
person changes and between-person mean differences in auton- individual levels and explained between 14 and 17 percent of the
omy supportive style positively predicted the satisfaction- well- and ill-being index variance at the within-person level and
frustration of the need for relatedness towards peers. In other between 51 and 76 percent at the between-person level. It is of
words, when a coach provides real choice, gives rationale, takes interest to note some differences between the present study and
others' perspectives into account and displays other behaviors that of Adie et al. (2012). First, they did not find any relationship
characteristic of autonomy support (e.g., Mageau & Vallerand, between needs satisfaction and the emotional and physical
2003) he or she facilitates a feeling of closeness between team- exhaustion facets of burnout; second, the positive relationships
mates, probably because the climate is relaxed and everyone can between needs satisfaction and subjective vitality were observed
express themselves. Extending the results of Adie et al. (2012), our only at the within-person level. We believe these differences are
study showed that an autonomy supportive style also positively related to the tool we used to measure psychological needs. While
predicted the satisfaction-frustration of the need for relatedness the tool used by Adie et al. allowed only satisfaction of the need (i.e.,
towards the coach at a micro- (i.e., within-person) and a macro- high vs. low) to be captured, our tool used a bipolar format and
(i.e., between-person) level. Unlike the negative conditional regard allowed the capture of both satisfaction (when the score is high)
that seems to cause a decrease in the satisfaction-frustration of the and frustration (when the score is low) of a basic need experience.
need for relatedness toward the coach, autonomy support appears Interestingly, the analyses underlined that relatedness when
to facilitate the players' closeness toward him or her. measured in connection with two different significant others (i.e.,
By contrast, but in line with some past research (e.g., Kipp & the coach and teammates), is not related in the same way with
Weiss, 2013), results revealed no significant relationships be- well- and ill-being. Specifically, in line with the cross-sectional
tween perceptions of coach autonomy support and the two other study by Kipp and Weiss (2013), results showed that between-
basic needs. Regarding the need for competence, a possible expla- person differences in the satisfaction-frustration of the need for
nation for this result may be related to the social environmental relatedness towards teammates positively predicted self-esteem. In
features targeted in this study (i.e., autonomy support and con- other words, when a player felt higher levels of closeness towards
trolling coaching). Other dimensions of the perceived social envi- teammates than other players, they reported a higher self-esteem,
ronment (see Mageau & Vallerand, 2003) such as the degree to after controlling for the relationships between the two other needs
which the coach creates a well-structured environment with clear and self-esteem. By contrast, results revealed that the satisfaction-
tasks and feedback guidelines or encourages self-referenced rather frustration of the need for relatedness towards the coach did not
than other-referenced perceptions of competence (i.e., a task- predict any indicators of well- and ill-being. There are several
involving climate) may be more directly related to the need for possible explanations. First, the need for relatedness has overall a
competence and could explain more variance in the satisfaction- more subordinate role in the players' well- and ill-being, compared
frustration of the need for competence. Regarding the need for to the needs for autonomy and competence (Deci & Ryan, 2000);
autonomy, the insignificant finding could have been the result of a second, given the correlations between the needs, there is a similar
shared variance issue; the controlling coach behaviors may have shared variance issue to the ones highlighted above; third, peers,
taken up much of the shared variance in the prediction of the need more than adults could be particularly influential in the formation
for autonomy. When autonomy support was modeled by itself (i.e., of self-concept during adolescence (e.g., Amorose, 2002); finally, as
76 B. Cheval et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 28 (2017) 68e77

suggested by Kipp and Weiss's study, the satisfaction-frustration of conceptualization of needs that considers need satisfaction and
the need for relatedness toward the coach could be related to short- frustration as lying on a continuum. In recent years there has been
term hedonic well-being (e.g., positive affect), that we have not debate in the literature concerning whether needs should be
assessed in this study, whereas the satisfaction-frustration of the considered as lying on a continuum or modeled as distinct con-
need for relatedness toward teammates could be more important structs with unique effects (e.g., Bartholomew et al., 2011; Sheldon
for players' overall sense of worthiness as a person. Accordingly, & Hilpert, 2012; Vansteenkiste & Ryan, 2013). Since our study was
disaggregating the relatedness construct, to capture relatedness completed, recent research findings (e.g., Cordeiro, Paix~ ao, Lens,
towards the coach seperately from relatedness towards teammates Lacante, & Sheldon, 2016) have strengthened the case that need
in order to assess their relationships with several other indices of satisfaction and frustration should be conceptually and statistically
well-being, constitutes an interesting avenue of research. differentiated. Thus, in the future, researchers could use different
scales to assess and explore the singular consequences of each of
3.3. The mediational role of the satisfaction-frustration of basic these experiences. Sixth, this study focused on perceived coach
psychological needs autonomy support and different facets of controlling behaviors.
Other coach behaviors more directly related to the dimensions of
According to SDT (Ryan & Deci, 2002), the effects of autonomy competence or relatedness, and more generally other dimensions
support and controlling coach behaviors on athletes' ill- and well- of the social environment may be useful to explain more variance in
being, are accounted for via the satisfaction-frustration of the these respective needs. Finally, in the current study the reliability of
athletes' basic psychological needs. Extending previous longitudi- the excessive personal control subscale was minimally acceptable
nal work conducted in the sport domain (Adie et al., 2008, 2012), (u coefficients  .57). This low reliability reduces the ability to
the general pattern of results revealed some evidence of mediation detect a significant association and requires cautious interpretation
between two facets of the controlling coach behaviors (i.e., negative of non-significant associations. Future research should examine
conditional regard and excessive personal control) on well- and ill- whether the reliability of this subscale is improved in other samples
being indices via the needs for competence and autonomy. Spe- and whether the pattern of results is similar to those obtained in
cifically, at the within-person level, the satisfaction-frustration of the current study.
the need for competence mediated the relationships between Despite these limitations, this longitudinal study provides in-
negative conditional regard and all the well- and ill-being out- sights into the determinants of within-person change and
comes. At both levels, the satisfaction-frustration of the need for between-person differences in indices of well- and ill-being in elite
autonomy mediated the relationship between excessive personal soccer players. The information provided can inform coach edu-
control and subjective vitality and burnout. cation as this study shows that reducing controlling coach behav-
Finally, unlike some previous work (Adie et al., 2012), evidence iors alongside promoting autonomy support may lead to positive
of mediating processes between coach autonomy support and well- changes in indices of well- and ill-being. Accordingly, coach edu-
or ill-being was weak, with only one significant indirect effect with cation interventions are required that not only support coaches in
self-esteem through peer relatedness. The inclusion of the ‘dark promoting autonomy support, but also highlight how controlling
side’ of the coach-created environment enabled us to reveal that behaviors can be reduced. Interventions programs such as the
this relationship may not actually be evident when a more rounded, Empowering Coaching™ training program developed through the
multi-dimensional picture of the coaching climate is assessed. This PAPA project (Duda, 2013) could be implemented in an elite sport
could suggest that controlling coach behaviors are important to context in order to (a) help coaches to understand how they can
consider in predicting elite athletes’ well- and ill-being. foster athletes' well-being and reduce their ill-being and to (b)
provide them with strategies for promoting a more autonomous
3.4. Limitations and future directions and less controlling environment.
To conclude, the present study revealed that perceptions of
Although this study goes beyond cross-sectional investigations negative conditional regard at the within-person level and of
by examining patterns of relationships over time both at the excessive personal control both at the between- and within-person
within- and between-person level, the present findings stem from levels, were the most influential facets of controlling coach be-
correlational data and as such, causality cannot be determined. haviors on well- and ill-being indices, via their impact on the need
Second, although our results offer an insight into the processes by for competence and autonomy, respectively. It also revealed that
which athletes' well-being and ill-being may be enhanced, we while relatedness toward teammates was related to one index of
focused only on psychological functioning. Markers of metabolic well-being (i.e., self-esteem), relatedness toward the coach was not
and immunological functioning (e.g., Quested et al., 2011), along- related to any index. In sum, this study highlights the need (1) to
side self-reported measures, could extend our understanding of the consider each facet of the perceived coach-controlling environment
processes involved in maintaining healthy sport participation at the separately, (2) to assess the construct of relatedness both towards
elite level. Third, our findings may be limited to elite adolescent the coach and towards teammates and (3) to conduct longitudinal
soccer players. Even if Ryan and Deci (2000, 2002) claimed the studies that allow the disaggregating of between-person and
three needs are universal and that all people would benefit from within-person effects.
basic need satisfaction regardless of their age or cultural values, this
does not imply that their relative salience are the same in all cul-
References
tures. Accordingly, it could be interesting to study whether the
relationships observed in this study are the same with other pop- Adie, J., Duda, J., & Ntoumanis, N. (2008). Autonomy support, basic need satisfaction
ulations (e.g., grassroots' players, sportswomen, adult players). and the optimal functioning of adult male and female sport participants: A test
of basic needs theory. Motivation and Emotion, 32(3), 189e199. http://
Fourth, in the same vein, it is possible that previous levels of sport
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11031-008-9095-z.
experience may moderate some of the relationships assessed in the Adie, J. W., Duda, J. L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2012). Perceived coach-autonomy support,
present study. For instance, players with little sport experience may basic need satisfaction and the well- and ill-being of elite youth soccer players:
be more strongly influenced by the perceived social environment A longitudinal investigation. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13(1), 51e59.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.07.008.
than players with greater experience. This possibility should be Amorose, A. J. (2002). The influence of reflected appraisals on middle school and
investigated in future research. Fifth, in this study we adopted the high school athletes' self-perceptions of sport competence. Pediatric Exercise
B. Cheval et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 28 (2017) 68e77 77

Science, 14, 377e390. and physical education context. European Journal of Psychological Assessment,
Amorose, A. J., & Anderson-Butcher, D. (2007). Autonomy-supportive coaching and 26(3), 203e211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000027.
self-determined motivation in high school and college athletes: A test of self- Jacka, F. N., Pasco, J. A., Williams, L. J., Leslie, E. R., Dodd, S.,
determination theory. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 8(5), 654e670. http:// Nicholson, G. C., … Berk, M. (2011). Lower levels of physical activity in childhood
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2006.11.003. associated with adult depression. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 14(3),
Assor, A., Roth, G., & Deci, E. L. (2004). The emotional costs of parents' conditional 222e226. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2010.10.458.
regard: A self-determination theory analysis. Journal of Personality, 72(1), Judd, C. M., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Muller, D. (2014). Mediation and moderation. In
47e88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/J.0022-3506.2004.00256.X. H. T. Reis, & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and per-
Balaguer, I., Gonzalez, L., Fabra, P., Castillo, I., Merce, J., & Duda, J. L. (2012). Coaches' sonality psychology (2nd ed., pp. 653e676). New York: Cambridge University
interpersonal style, basic psychological needs and the well- and ill-being of Press.
young soccer players: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Sports Sciences, 30(15), Kipp, L. E., & Weiss, M. R. (2013). Social influences, psychological need satisfaction,
1619e1629. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2012.731517. and well-being among female adolescent gymnasts. Sport Exercise and Perfor-
Bartholomew, K. J., Ntoumanis, N., Ryan, R. M., Bosch, J. A., & Thogersen- mance Psychology, 2(1), 62e75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0030236.
Ntoumani, C. (2011). Self-determination theory and diminished functioning: Mageau, G. A., & Vallerand, R. J. (2003). The coach-athlete relationship: A motiva-
The role of interpersonal control and psychological need frustration. Personality tional model. Journal of Sports Sciences, 21(11), 883e904. http://dx.doi.org/
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(11), 1459e1473. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ 10.1080/0264041031000140374.
0146167211413125. Mann, M., Hosman, C. M., Schaalma, H. P., & de Vries, N. K. (2004). Self-esteem in a
Bartholomew, K. J., Ntoumanis, N., & Thogersen-Ntoumani, C. (2010). The control- broad-spectrum approach for mental health promotion. Health Education
ling interpersonal style in a coaching context: Development and initial vali- Research, 19, 357e372. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/her/cyg041.
dation of a psychometric scale. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 32(2), Marsh, H. W. (2007). Application of confirmatory factor analysis and structural
193e216. equation modeling in sport/exercise psychology. In G. Tenenbaum, &
Blanchard, C. M., Amiot, C. E., Perreault, S., Vallerand, R. J., & Provencher, P. (2009). R. C. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (3rd ed., pp. 774e798).
Cohesiveness, coach's interpersonal style and psychological needs: Their effects Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9781118270011.ch35.
on self-determination and athletes' subjective well-being. Psychology of Sport McDonald, R. P. (1999). Test theory: A unified treatment. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
and Exercise, 10(5), 545e551. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ Erlbaum Associates.
j.psychsport.2009.02.005. Ntoumanis, N. (2012). A self-determination theory perspective on motivation in
Brislin, R. W. (1986). The wording and translation of research instruments. In sport and physical education: Current trends and possible future research di-
W. J. Lonner, & J. W. Berry (Eds.), Field methods in educational research (pp. rections. In G. C. Roberts, & D. C. Treasure (Eds.), Advances in motivation in sport
137e164). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. and exercise (pp. 91e128). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Cheval, B., & Sarrazin, P. (2011). Vers la construction d’un outil de mesure de la Papaioannou, A. G., Appleton, P. R., Torregrosa, M., Jowett, G. E., Bosselut, G.,
satisfaction et de la menace des besoins psychologiques fondamentaux dans le Gonzalez, L., … Zourbanos, N. (2013). Moderate-to-vigorous physical activity
domaine du sport. In H. Zouhal, G. Cabagno, S. Vincent, F. Bekono, & R. Kulpa and personal well-being in European youth soccer players: Invariance of
(Eds.), Actes du XIV eme Congr es International de l’Association des Chercheurs en physical activity, global self-esteem and vitality across five countries. Interna-
Activites Physiques et Sportives (ACAPS) (pp. 216e217) [Toward the construction tional Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11(4), 351e364. http://dx.doi.org/
of a questionnaire measuring satisfaction and threat of basic psychological 10.1080/1612197x.2013.830429.
needs in the sport domain. In H. Zouhal, G. Cabagno, S. Vincent, F. Bekono, & R. Quested, E., Bosch, J. A., Burns, V. E., Cumming, J., Ntoumanis, N., & Duda, J. L. (2011).
Kulpa (Eds.) Proceedings of the XIV congress of the French Association of Basic psychological need satisfaction, stress-related appraisals, and dancers'
Researcher in Physical Activity and Sport]. cortisol and anxiety responses. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 33(6),
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analyses for the 828e846.
behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Quested, E., & Duda, J. L. (2011). Antecedents of burnout among elite dancers: A
Conroy, D. E., & Coatsworth, J. D. (2007). Assessing autonomy-supportive coaching longitudinal test of basic needs theory. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 12(2),
strategies in youth sport. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 8, 671e684. http:// 159e167. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2010.09.003.
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2006.12.001. Raedeke, T. D., & Smith, A. L. (2001). Development and preliminary validation of an
Cordeiro, P., Paix~ ao, P., Lens, W., Lacante, M., & Sheldon, K. (2016). Factor structure athlete burnout measure. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 23(4), 281e306.
and dimensionality of the balanced measure of psychological needs among Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models. Thousand Oaks:
Portuguese high school students. Relations to well-being and ill-being. Learning Sage Publications.
and Individual Differences, 47, 51e60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ Reise, S. P., Bonifay, W. E., & Haviland, M. G. (2013). Scoring and modeling psy-
j.lindif.2015.12.010. chological measures in the presence of multidimensionality. Journal of Person-
Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2011). The disaggregation of within-person and between- ality Assessment, 95, 129e140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2012.725437.
person effects in longitudinal models of change. Annual review of psychology, 62, Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). The darker and brighter sides of human existence:
583e619. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100356. Basic psychological needs as a unifying concept. Psychological Inquiry, 11(4),
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human 319e338. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327965pli1104_03.
needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11(4), Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of
227e268. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327965pli1104_01. research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology,
Duda, J. L. (2013). The conceptual and empirical foundations of Empowering 52(1), 141e166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141.
Coaching™: Setting the stage for the PAPA project. International Journal of Sport Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2002). Overview of self-determination theory: An
and Exercise Psychology, 11(4), 311e318. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ organismic dialectical perspective. In E. L. Deci, & R. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of
1612197x.2013.839414. self-determination (pp. 3e37). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
Felton, L., & Jowett, S. (2013). “What do coaches do” and “how do they relate”: Their Ryan, R. M., & Frederick, C. M. (1997). On energy, personality, and health: Subjective
effects on athletes' psychological needs and functioning. Scandinavian Journal of vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-being. Journal of Personality, 65(3),
Medicine & Science in Sports, 23(2), e130ee139. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ 529e565. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1997.tb00326.x.
sms.12029. Sheldon, K. M., & Hilpert, J. C. (2012). The balanced measure of psychological needs
Fritz, M. S., Taylor, A. B., & Mackinnon, D. P. (2012). Explanation of two anomalous (BMPN) scale: An alternative domain general measure of need satisfaction.
results in statistical mediation analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 47(1), Motivation and Emotion, 36(4), 439e451. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11031-012-
61e87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2012.640596. 9279-4.
Guerin, F., Marsh, H. W., & Famose, J. P. (2003). Construct validation of the self- Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling
description questionnaire II with a French sample. European Journal of Psycho- change and event occurrence. Oxford University Press.
logical Assessment, 19(2), 142e150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027//1015- Vansteenkiste, M., & Ryan, R. M. (2013). On psychological growth and vulnerability:
5759.19.2.142. Basic psychological need satisfaction and need frustration as a unifying prin-
Isoard-Gautheur, S., Guillet-Descas, E., & Lemyre, P. (2012). A prospective study of ciple. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 23(3), 263e280. http://dx.doi.org/
the influence of perceived coaching style on burnout propensity in high level 10.1037/a0032359.
young athletes: Using a self-determination theory perspective. The Sport Psy- Williams, G. C., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Internalization of biopsychosocial values by
chologist, 26(2), 282e298. medical students: A test of self-determination theory. Journal of Personality and
Isoard-Gautheur, S., Oger, M., Guillet, E., & Martin-Krumm, C. (2010). Validation of a Social Psychology, 70(4), 767e779. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
French version of the athlete burnout questionnaire (ABQ) in competitive sport 3514.70.4.767.

You might also like