You are on page 1of 19

LSHSS

Tutorial

Using Language Sample Analysis to Assess


Spoken Language Production in
Adolescents
Jon F. Miller,a,b Karen Andriacchi,a,b and Ann Nockertsa,b

Purpose: This tutorial discusses the importance of language sample by grade and elicitation context, highlights language measures that are
analysis and how Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) higher or lower than the database mean values. Differences in values
software can be used to simplify the process and effectively assess the are measured in standard deviations.
spoken language production of adolescents. Conclusion: Language sample analysis remains a powerful method
Method: Over the past 30 years, thousands of language samples have of documenting language use in everyday speaking situations. A sample
been collected from typical speakers, aged 3–18 years, in conversational of talking reveals an individual’s ability to meet specific speaking
and narrative contexts. These samples have been formatted as reference demands.
databases included with SALT. Using the SALT software, individual These demands vary across contexts, and speakers can have difficulty
samples are compared with age- and grade-matched samples selected in any one or all of these communication tasks. Language use for
from these databases. spoken communication is a foundation for literacy attainment and
Results: Two case studies illustrate that comparison with database contributes to success in navigating relationships for school, work, and
samples of typical adolescents, matched community participation.

L
anguage sample analysis (LSA), as a data- language. Some discussion is also included relating to LSA
generating tool, is a powerful method of and written language.
documenting language use in everyday speaking
situations (Leadholm &
Miller, 1992; Paul, 2012). A sample of talking reveals an Why LSA?
individual’s ability to meet specific speaking demands.
These demands vary across contexts (e.g., conversation, LSA has been considered the gold standard for asses-
narration, exposition, and persuasion), and speakers may sing spoken language production by researchers and clini-
have dif- ficulty in any one or all of these communication cians for more than 90 years (Bloom & Lahey, 1978; Brown,
tasks. 1973; Donaldson, 1986; Hart & Risley, 1995; Miller, 1982;
Language use for spoken communication is a foundation Miller, Andriacchi, & Nockerts, 2011; K. Nelson, 1973;
for literacy attainment (Miller et al., 2006; Roskos, Tabors, Piaget, 1926; Slobin, 1985; Weir, 1962). It is a method for
& Lenhart, 2009) and contributes to success in navigating thoroughly describing language production and for
relationships for school, work, and community integra- monitor- ing change associated with linguistic development,
tion. This tutorial discusses the importance of LSA in effec- variation in linguistic contexts, and/or change from
tively assessing speakers’ spoken language. It describes how intervention. It can be used daily, weekly, monthly, or
Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) soft- yearly, providing ac- cess to a large range of word,
ware (Miller & Iglesias, 2012) can be used to simplify the morpheme, syntax, and dis- course measures. It allows for
process, overcoming the hurdles of transcription and analy- documentation of specific problems in a variety of contexts,
sis. The focus is on using LSA with adolescents and in- pinpointing strengths and weaknesses in spoken
cludes case studies to illustrate the effectiveness of using language. LSA evaluates natural, functional language use
language samples with comparison data to assess spoken within real-life contexts. Language samples, elicited
properly, often substantiate the reason for referral, and the
variety of analyses help to pinpoint which features of
University of Wisconsin–Madison
a
spoken language most and least impact effective
b
SALT Software LLC, Madison, WI communication. LSA outcomes can address the aims
Correspondence to Jon F. Miller: jfmille2@wisc.edu of assessment, answering questions such as “Does this
Editor: Marilyn Nippold
Received July 7, 2015
Disclosure: Jon F. Miller is CEO and co-owner of SALT Software, LLC, and has
Revision received September 2, 2015
a financial interest. Karen Andriacchi is an employee of SALT Software, LLC. Ann
Accepted November 30, 2015 Nockerts is COO and co-owner of SALT Software, LLC, and has a financial interest.
DOI: 10.1044/2015_LSHSS-15-0051

Downloaded From: http://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a New York University User on 09/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 47 • 99–112 • April 2016 • Copyright © 2016 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 99

Downloaded From: http://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a New York University User on 09/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
student qualify for services?”; “Which language features exposition for adolescents). The transcript provides the data
most impact communication?”; and “How much progress to compare with typical speakers and can be used to track
has been achieved over the past weeks?” These are ques- progress associated with intervention.
tions clinicians are frequently asked to address, yet many Transcribing a language sample greatly benefits the
of the assessment tools available today are limited by age examiner. Listening to the sample and typing it into a text
range, sensitivity, cultural bias, and/or frequency of deliv- file provides initial insights into language strengths and
ery. In contrast, language samples may be elicited from weaknesses. These observations can shape the approach to
speakers of any age, are sensitive to change, minimize cul- analysis, creating impressions of overall language
tural bias, and may be repeated frequently (Heilmann & production under specific speaking conditions. This is time
Westerveld, 2013; Rojas & Iglesias, 2010). well spent. Listening to a recorded language sample
The most common excuse cited for omitting language sometime after live elicitation offers the opportunity to
sampling from assessment is the lack of time available to attend to details possi- bly overlooked at the time of
complete the process in a clinic or school setting. The time elicitation. Transcribing the language sample offers the
constraint may have been accurate years ago, but the pro- experience of sharpening focus to features of the sample
cess is much more efficient today (Heilmann, 2010). Evalu- that will require further analysis.
ators are no longer bound by tape recorders, paper and SALT staff have worked with a number of school
pencil transcription, or by hand-counting units for analysis. dis- tricts and clinics who have reduced the time of the
Computer programs have helped to standardize the LSA LSA process for clinicians by setting up their own
process and have added many analyses that advance and transcription service using speech-language pathology
assist the interpretation process (Long, 2015; MacWhinney, assistants or other transcription staff. Typically, clinicians
2000; Miller & Iglesias, 2012). There are many compelling send their deiden- tified audio or video samples, often via a
reasons to use LSA, yet there are hurdles that must be secure website, to the transcription staff who transcribe the
overcome to comfortably implement this powerful assess- sample and return the coded text to the clinicians for
ment tool. analysis. Similarly, ex- ternal transcription services have
been used to eliminate the need to train and support on-site
transcribers. Whether this service is provided in-house or
Transcription: Overcoming the Hurdles externally, having one or more dedicated transcribers
Perhaps the biggest hurdle to overcome is transcribing improves efficiency. On the basis of data from SALT’s
the language sample. “Why can’t this be done automatically? transcription services with eight school districts across the
If my phone or tablet computer can understand my speech, United States, clinicians signifi- cantly increased their use
why can’t it create a text file from a student speaking? ” It of LSA when transcription was taken off their hands. A
can, with some accuracy, for well-constructed and articulated dedicated transcriber can reduce costs and free up the
sentences. But these speakers are typically not the students clinicians’ time for analysis, interpreta- tion, and therapy.
receiving services, and the resulting transcripts would not For clinicians who have transcription support, the first
include many of the features clinicians are interested in step in analyzing the sample is to go back and listen to the
documenting such as faithful utterance boundaries, bound recording while looking through the tran- script. This step
morphemes, pauses, repetitions, and revisions. There are will often highlight aspects of the recorded sample that were
a number of speech-to-text programs available. SALT staff not noticed during elicitation. It also pro- vides opportunity
tested many of them over the years and concluded that cor- to make changes to the transcript that reflect the clinician’s
recting the result takes as long, if not longer, than it takes interpretation of the communication interaction versus the
an experienced transcriber to complete the sample. SALT perception of the transcriber who was not present or who
transcription lab data, which are based on 550 audio sam- has little or no familiarity with the target speaker(s).
ples elicited from a variety of speakers in conversation and Transcribers strive for the most accurate repre- sentation of
narration, show it takes approximately 4–6 min per minute the communicative event. It is the responsibility of the
of audio to transcribe samples from typically developing clinician, however, to ensure that the transcript is
speakers. In contrast, it takes approximately 7–8 min to as valid as possible.
tran- scribe each minute of an audio sample produced by Since the early 1980s, SALT developers have worked
speakers being evaluated for services or receiving services. to implement consistent transcription formats (Miller, 1982).
This time difference is primarily the result of the The benefits are enormous. Measurement of the same units,
intelligibility of the speakers and the amount of mazing, such as clear identification of what constitutes a word, rules
pausing, errors, and overlapping speech that occurs in the for identifying bound morphemes, and rules for utterance
sample. There are many other factors that affect segmentation, are prominent examples of implementing
transcription time including consistency that have resulted in improved, valid, and reli-
(a) familiarity with the transcription conventions, (b) play- able evaluation outcomes (Heilmann et al., 2008; Miller,
back equipment, (c) typing skills, (d) quality of the recording, 1982; Miller et al., 2011). If samples are transcribed the
(e) length of the sample, (f) context of the sample, and (g) same way, then analyses will be calculated on the same
the number of nonstandard features coded. The time spent units: morphemes, words, and utterances. The consistent
on transcription pays off with a permanent record of transcrip- tion format was essential for the development of
language use in curriculum-relevant contexts (e.g., databases of typical speakers for specific speaking
persuasion or contexts across ages and grades. Using these databases,
measures from an
Downloaded From: http://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a New York University User on 09/04/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
al Samples
2010; Thordardottir,
100 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 47 • 99–112 •
and 2015; Tilstra & McMaster,
April 2016 Structured 2007). Heilmann et al.
Narrative (2008) looked at 241
individual speaker could establish rapport, narrative retell samples
be compared with samples introduce topics, ask open- Samples produced by English
of other age- and grade- ended questions, and As previously language learners in
matched speakers to follow the other speaker’s mentioned, much has grades K–2.
assess performance and to lead. The clinician’s role in changed with the LSA English and Spanish
document intervention direct- ing a conversation process over recent samples were collected
progress over time. can certainly alter the decades. Of significant using the word- less
Computers de- mand results and may cause importance is the picture book Frog, Where
consistency, and as some clinicians to avoid revelation that language Are You? (Mayer, 1969).
analysis programs were the conversational context samples do not have to Heilmann et al. (2008)
developed, consistent or to abandon LSA con- tain at least 100 documented that these
transcription formats altogether. Although utterances or be short samples (4 min on
became part of the reticence is understood, if approximately 15 min in average) generated
process. there are speakers being length in order to reliable estimates of
The transcript itself assessed with social accurately capture and spoken language skills.
is a very effective language concerns, assess natural lan- guage. The length of the
clinical tool. recording conversational Short conversational sample should be linked to
Because it is readable, it samples can be critical to samples and structured the mea- sures one expects
provides a vehicle to an effective assessment. narratives take less time to to use and the type of
highlight areas of concern For example, students elicit and transcribe, and sample being elic- ited.
with parents, teachers, diagnosed with autism they produce con- sistent Conversations have no
and administrators. The spectrum disorder and the linguistic outcomes particular beginning or
clinician is able to point related diagnosis (Heilmann, Nockerts, & end.
out examples of specific of social communication Miller, Depending on the
areas of difficulty. The disorder, as they are speakers, a 5-min
parents understand: “Oh, defined in the fifth edition conversational sample
I see what you are of the Diagnostic and typically contains 40–50
concerned about. He does Statistical Manual of Mental utterances which, in most
that at home too.” A Disorders (American cases, is sufficient to yield
language sample captures Psychiatric Association, meaningful results
language use within real- 2013), have a basic (Heilmann, Nockerts, &
life contexts. The problem with social Miller, 2010). Unlike
transcript format provides reciprocity. These students conversations, narratives
direct access to enhance tend to perform better with do have an inherent
com- munication about narrative tasks than in structure, and the entire
specific language issues. conversa- tional contexts. sample should be tran-
Eliciting and analyzing scribed and analyzed.
both types of samples to Story retell narratives, for
Elicitation: The document strengths and example, require a
Clinician’s Role weaknesses can be beginning, a middle, and
essential to a thorough an end with specifics
When eliciting
assessment of spoken regarding characters,
narrative samples, such as
language performance that conflicts, and resolutions.
story retells, the clinician’s compares performance With these narratives,
role is primarily that of an with and without the analysis of the entire
attentive listener. demands of conversation. sample takes advantage
Narrative samples can be
In this case, the clinician’s of the speaker’s ability to
time saving, can highlight
active role in sam- ple organize language, to
many academically based
elicitation is necessary to cohesively retell a
linguistic features, and are
evaluate the student’s complete story, and to
familiar for, and
perfor- mance of the social produce as much
interesting to, the target
demands in a relevant lexicon as
speaker. The clinician
communication setting. possible. As students get
must be an active
older, they are able to
participant, however, when
retell more details of the
eliciting conversational
Using Short stories (Heilmann et al.,
samples. This sample type
Conversation 2010; Nippold, 2014).
requires the examiner to
Downloaded From: http://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a New York University User on 09/04/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Similarly, expository results. The results of ss
and persuasive sam- analyses should “make Sp
ok
ples have an inherent sense” (i.e., conform to en
structure with required observational analyses L
components (Heilmann and corrobo- rate with a an
& Malone, 2014). rereading of the gu
Depending upon a transcript). A high score ag
e
speaker’s age/grade, a on number of errors, for in
short narra- tive sample example, could be A
may indicate spoken misleading if the coder do
language deficits. It is holds the speaker to a le
sc
important to note, higher standard than is
en
however, whereas the rea- sonable, such as ts
length of a narra- tive coding a dialect speaker’s 1
language sample can be sample for Stan- dard 0
an important measure American English.
1
diagnos- tically, it is not
always a predictor of a contexts. It is important to
student’s ability to Table 1. Summary of Systematic
Analysis: reiterate the many ways Analysis of Language Transcripts
include necessary students express difficulty (SALT) reference databases:
structural elements. Comparin Grades 4–12.
with language use in
Concise stories with the g With everyday speaking
required structural Databases tasks. Clusters of variables profiles Grad A
features communicate from spoken language appear to
of Typical e in ge
elegantly, whereas longer assess- ment that be scho ran
stories emphasizing some Speakers ol ge (
characterize patterns of distinct
features and omit- ting Regardless of the 4th Narrative Story Retell 9;3–10;0 46 yea
disordered performance types of rs;
others may not hold streamlining of the LSA 5th Conversation 10;9–11;4 data 27
have been noted
Narrativein past
Story Retell deficits
10;7–11;9 base86 mo
listeners’ attention. The process over the years, research (Leadholm &
Narrative Student but can
10;9–11;4 27 nth
length interpretation of the data Miller, 1992; Selects
Miller, Story be s)
of a narrative language still remains dependent on 1991). ThisExposition
work 10;7–11;9
overlappi 86
sample can be an the clinician’s competence, 6th Narrative Story Retell 11;5–12;8 69
quantified Exposition
what clinicians ng11;5–12;8
as 69
important mea- sure, but skill, and expertise. report:
7th thatConversation
students well. Each
12;9–13;3 27
interpretation of results Clinicians have to make identified with language
Narrative Student is 12;9–13;3 27
requires clinical expertise sense of the data to Selects
disorders are not Story
alike. characteri
to evaluate the narrative recognize and resolve the Exposition 12;7–13;7 36
Miller’s
9th research,
Exposition zed by a
14;8–18;4 76
content. For any measure specific language summarizedPersuasion
most recently pattern
14;8–18;4 35
calcu- lated characteristics, behaviors, in
10thMiller etExposition
al. (2011), of 15;5–16;11
deficits 24
automatically, caution or commu- nication recognizedPersuasion
the following 15;5–16;11
resulting 24
needs to be exercised interaction styles that 11th Exposition 16;4–17;8 24
profiles of disordered
Persuasion from
16;4–17;8 24
when interpreting the emerge from the sampling performance: general analysis of
12th Exposition 17;4–18;9 29
developmental delay (low
Persuasion a series of
17;4–18;9 30
mean length of utterance, more than
M
ill
number of different words, 20
e total words, and words per different
r minute), word finding and measures,
e utterance formulation calculated
t problems (high number automatic
a of repetitions and ally from
l.
revisions at the word or a lan-
:
U phrase level), discourse guage
s deficits (trouble sample.
i maintaining topic, failure Each
n to respond to examiner deficit
g
questions), slow speaking category
L
S rate (low words per helps
A minute, high number of describe
t pauses), and fast speaking language
o rate with low semantic
A
content (high words per
s
s minute with
e circumlocution). These
Downloaded From: http://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a New York University User on 09/04/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
No. of samples Using LSA with sample from a sixth
production with the goals through 12th grade. Ages reference databases can grader.
of pinpointing areas of for each data set provide help docu- ment the
concern and guiding the a snapshot of grade and outcomes of practice. The
development of age relationship. Note databases provide access Case Study 1: Teresa
intervention targets. A that there are to several powerful tools Consider the
detailed analysis of 260 conversational samples in the quest to understand following scenario.
conversational samples of for fifth and seventh an indi- vidual speaker’s Teresa is 15 years old and
students 3–13 years of age graders, narrative language production. They is in the ninth grade. As
(Miller, 1991) found these samples where the provide norm- referenced part of a language assess-
profiles equally student selects the story data on multiple measures ment, Teresa was asked to
distributed across ages. (Narrative SSS) for fifth of language production explain how to play her
We find it surprising that and seventh graders, with which to compare a favorite game or sport, an
the pro- portion of narrative story retell student’s performance. elicitation task based on
delayed development samples for fourth to sixth They can substantiate the Nippold’s (2014) research.
profiles was virtually the graders, expository validity of the sampling The specific protocol
same for students aged samples (specifically how protocol for reveal- ing used, which includes a
3–5, 6–8, and 9–13 years. to play a game or sport) specific deficits and planning sheet listing eight
With improved for fifth to 12th graders, allow clinicians to expository components
automation of LSA, the and persuasive samples describe and validate how (object, preparations,
opportunity to analyze for ninth to 12th graders. specific linguistic contexts start, course of play,
large sample sets without These databases provide affect students’ spoken rules, scoring, dura- tion,
human error has pro- performance standards communication skills. and strategies) can be
duced a plethora of across advancing grades found on the SALT
comparison data and lan- guage sample website (SALT Software,
(MacWhinney, 2000; demands. Case Studies 2015a). Teresa chose
Miller & Iglesias, 2012). soccer and was given a
Examiners no longer have The following case
studies illustrate how few minutes to fill out the
to rely on summary charts planning sheet. She was
of language development databases of typically
developing speakers directed to explain how
(Miller, 1982), though they to play soccer while
can still be useful. SALT document individual
perfor- mance. The first having her completed
includes databases of planning sheet available
typical speakers in various case study examines an
expository sam- ple from for reference. The lan-
speaking conditions: guage sample was
conversation, narration, a ninth grader. The
second case study recorded and later
exposition, and persuasion transcribed following
for speakers 3–18 years of examines a narrative
age. These databases
contain thousands of
samples of English and
102 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 47 • 99–112 •
April 2016
Spanish monolingual and
bilingual speakers. the SALT transcription Once the comparison set of
They provide norm- conventions (SALT samples was identified,
referenced data to Software, 2015b). SALT produced a variety
highlight the strengths and Refer to Figure 1 for a of reports showing how
weaknesses of individual copy of her transcript. Teresa’s language sample
speakers (Heilmann, From listening to the compares with her peers.
Miller, & Nockerts, 2010). sample, several areas of The comparison values are
Analysis using SALT concern were identified by presented as means,
allows the user to access the clinician. The sample ranges, and standard
these databases to seemed very short. Teresa devia- tion values for each
compare the performance paused a lot, and her score. Values of more
of an individual student explanation of how to than 1 SD above or below
with age- or grade- play soccer was not very the database values are
matched peers under the detailed. Analysis in SALT highlighted.
same speaking allowed the clinician to Figure 2 compares
conditions. select a group of grade- the length of Teresa’s
Table 1 lists the matched language sam- sample with samples from
SALT reference databases ples from the SALT 41 typically developing
available for English- expository database ninth graders selected
fluent students in fourth (SALT Software, 2015a). from the SALT expository
Downloaded From: http://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a New York University User on 09/04/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
database. Results Software, 2015c). The SI
confirm Teresa produced is an assessment applied The Expository favorite game or sport.
a shorter expository to an existing transcript Scoring Scheme (ESS; The SALT
sample, using only 23 that produces a ratio of the Heilmann & Malone, expository database
utterances and 197 words, total number of clauses to 2014; SALT Software, offers direct com-
compared with her peers the total number of 2015d) was also parison data to
who used an average of 54 communication units. applied to the document Teresa’s
utterances and 709 Using the software transcript to assess the expository language,
words. This is tools to expedite the content, organization, confirming initial
approximately 2 SD analysis, each utterance and structure of concerns from eliciting
below the database was assigned a code Teresa’s sample. The and listening to the
mean. indicating the number ESS provides an overall sample and from reading
The next analysis of clauses produced in measure of a speaker’s the transcript. Explaining
(see Figure 3) looks at the utterance. To skill in explaining how how to do something is
specific lan- guage illustrate, Figure 4 to play a part of the Common Core
measures on the basis of contains several favorite game or sport. State Stan- dards (CCSS;
samples of the same utterances, coded for The ESS is scored for the CCSS Initiative, 2015) for
length. Teresa’s sample SI, taken from following 10 categories spoken as well
contained 179 number of Teresa’s expository using a scale of 1 as written language. This
total words (NTW) sample. (minimal ) to 5 ( example highlights
excluding words in All of the SALT proficient): preparations, Teresa’s per- formance on
mazes and in database samples have object of contest, start of an exposition task. As a
unintelligible or been coded for SI. Figure play, course of play, follow-up assessment, her
incomplete utterances. To 5 compares Teresa’s SI scoring, rules, strategy, written language skills
generate the report in scores with those of other duration, terminology, could be examined to
Figure 3, only the first 179 ninth graders in the and cohe- sion. The address her understanding
NTW of the database comparison set. Notice clinician or transcriber of explanation as a genre
samples are included for that none of Teresa’s reads the expository sam- and focus on the
the comparison. Cutting utterances contained more ple and inserts the scores reciprocity between
the database samples to than two clauses, on a template in the spoken and written
the same number of words resulting in a composite transcript. language chal- lenges
as Teresa’s sample ensures score that is 1.2 SD All the expository posed by the curriculum.
valid comparison of below that of her peers. samples in the SALT
measures, such as number Teresa’s scores confirm a database have been scored
for ESS. Figure 6 Case Study 2: Patrick
of different words (NDW), suspicion regarding a
number of pauses, and deficit with complex compares Teresa’s scores This case study
number of errors, which syntax. This leaves her at with those of her grade- examines the story
are directly affected by the a sub- stantial matched peers. ESS retelling skills of a sixth
length of the transcript. disadvantage in analysis documents grader, providing an
Notice that Teresa’s mean explaining a complex Teresa’s difficulties with opportunity to explore the
length of utterance game like soccer. expository organization SALT narrative story retell
(MLU) was 2 SD below and struc- ture. database (SALT Software,
the database mean. Knowledge of complex 2015e) to document
NDW, a measure of syntax and the ability narrative skills.
vocabulary diversity, was explain how to play a Patrick is almost 12
similar to that of her game or sport could be years old and is in the
peers. The observa- tion related skills. Without the sixth grade. As part of an
that her transcript ability to produce assessment, the clinician
contained a high number complex syntactic recorded Patrick re-
of pauses was confirmed, structures, it is im- telling the story Doctor
and, as a consequence, her possible to put together De Soto (Steig, 1982).
speaking rate, measured multiple propositions, From listen- ing to the
in words per minute, was expressing complex language sample
slow. relationships. Although recording while reading
Given her low MLU low ESS scores sometimes the transcript, the
value, it is suspected reflect this type of deficit, examiner noticed that
Teresa may have a further examination of the Patrick made several word
complex syntax deficit. To sample reveals that the errors and omissions not
verify, her transcript was ESS scores expose an expected of a student his
coded for the underlying problem with age. In addition, he
Subordination Index (SI; the structure of frequently repeated and
Loban, 1976; SALT explaining how to play a revised utter- ance
Downloaded From: http://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a New York University User on 09/04/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
segments. Figure 7 retell database. The
contains an excerpt from length of his sam- ple is
Patrick’s narrative. comparable with that of
Figure 8 his peers.
compares the length of The next analysis
Patrick’s sample with 69 (see Figure 9) looks at
typically developing sixth specific lan- guage
graders selected from the measures that are most
SALT narrative story valid when compared
with

M
ill
e
r
e
t
a
l.
:
U
s
i
n
g
L
S
A
t
o
A
s
s
e
s
s
S
p
o
k
e
n
L
a
n
g
u
a
g
e
i
n
A
d
o
l
e
s
c
e
n
t
s
1
0
3

Downloaded From: http://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a New York University User on 09/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Figure 1. Case study 1–Expository transcript in Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) format.

104 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 47 • 99–112 • April 2016
Figure 2. Case study 1–Taken from Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) Transcript Length and Intelligibility report.

Figure 3. Case study 1–Taken from Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) Standard Measures report.

Miller et al.: Using LSA to Assess Spoken Language in Adolescents 105


Downloaded From: http://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a New York University User on 09/04/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Figure 4. Case study 1–Utterances coded for Subordination Index.

samples of the same length. Patrick’s sample contains


report also includes the utterances, allowing the examiner
513 NTW. To generate this report, the database samples
to review the context of each omission and coded error.
were cut at 513 NTW, creating a set of length-matched
It is interesting that he made each omission and error only
samples for comparison.
once. Although one can gain some preliminary insight by
Figure 9 shows that Patrick’s MLU was more than reviewing the utterances in this report, it may be more pro-
1 SD below the database mean, though his NDW, a mea- ductive to review the transcript again as a way of processing
sure of vocabulary diversity, was higher than his peers. the context for the omissions and errors.
His false starts, repetitions, and revisions (mazes) were very
Patrick’s language sample revealed a number of
high, with more than 20% of his words in mazes. His omis-
strengths as well as deficits. His vocabulary use was above
sions and errors were very frequent for his age. These
that of his peers for this task, though his average utterance
analyses reveal a short average utterance length, perhaps
length was below. Follow-up analyses of complex syntax
due to a complex syntax deficit.
revealed an SI composite score at the low end of the typical
To explore Patrick’s use of complex syntax, SI coding group. His very frequent mazes indicate he may have been
(SALT Software, 2015c) was applied to each utterance in struggling with utterance formulation, perhaps related to
his transcript. Figure 10 reveals how his scores compared facility with complex sentence forms.
with his grade-matched peers. His SI composite score was
Although this case study illustrates how Patrick per-
slightly lower than the database samples and may warrant
formed on a text-based narrative task, it may be prudent
further investigation of the sample to explore the possibility
to assess his language skills using the content of the class-
of a deficit in complex syntax.
room curriculum. The results can offer detailed information
To learn more about the content in the increased to share with classroom teachers and provide a platform
number of mazes, the Maze Summary report (see Figure 11) from which to build an intervention plan.
was run. This report revealed that Patrick’s mazes are pre-
dominantly at the phrase level, indicating difficulties with
utterance formulation. Phrase-level mazes can point to Written Language
problems with attempting to incorporate more propositions
The correlation between spoken language and written
into utterances without the necessary syntactic ability.
language is well established (Gillam & Johnston, 1992;
The Omissions and Error Codes report in Figure 12
Scott & Windsor, 2000). Hall-Mills and Apel (2015) inves-
lists four words and one bound morpheme that Patrick
tigated written language development in elementary grades
omitted as well as nine word-level errors he produced. The
across narrative and expository genres. They argue that

Figure 5. Case study 1–Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) Subordination Index report.

106 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 47 • 99–112 • April 2016
Downloaded From: http://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a New York University User on 09/04/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Figure 6. Case study 1–Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) Expository Scoring Scheme report.

speech-language pathologists are well trained to work with


developing rules for dealing with issues specific to written
teachers on language-based analyses, particularly for those
samples (e.g., misspellings, capitalization, and incorrect or
students receiving speech and language services. Funda-
missing punctuation). SALT has been used by written lan-
mental to their study is the need for more research on writ-
guage researchers to facilitate analyses (Hall-Mills & Apel,
ten language skill development. Written language analysis
2015; N. W. Nelson, 2014a, 2014b). It can expedite written
has used similar methods as spoken LSA without having
language analysis, but the interpretation of the results rela-
to transcribe the acoustic signal into standard orthography. tive to typical students is left to curriculum standards.
Both share similar issues for consistency: identifying
Price and Jackson (2015) detail the analysis of written
words, morphemes, and utterance segmentation as well as
language using SALT as the methodology to work through

Figure 7. Case study 2–Excerpt of story retell transcript in Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) format.

Miller et al.: Using LSA to Assess Spoken Language in Adolescents 107


Downloaded From: http://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a New York University User on 09/04/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Figure 8. Case study 2–Taken from Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) Transcript Length and Intelligibility report.

Figure 9. Case study 2–Taken from Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) Standard Measures report.

108 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 47 • 99–112 • April 2016

Downloaded From: http://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a New York University User on 09/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Figure 10. Case study 2–Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) Subordination Index report.

multiple levels of analysis. They provide an overview of important as students pursue specialized fields of study in
analysis options and explain why the analysis of written high school and beyond. The CCSS for literacy in science,
language is important for developing a complete under- social studies, history, and the technical subjects guide edu-
standing of the challenges students face within the curricu- cators to help students meet the literacy challenges within
lum. The authors are not aware of any available written each particular field of study. This national effort is referred
language databases but believe they would be very useful in to as disciplinary literacy (Wisconsin Department of Public
documenting academic progress. These could be organized Instruction, 2015). In a recent e-mail communication,
around the CCSS (CCSS Initiative, 2015) by grade and M. Nippold suggested,
genre, with students being asked to produce written narra-
Once a language sample has been elicited,
tives and expository work across subjects (e.g., history, so-
transcribed, and analyzed using SALT normative
cial studies, and the sciences). Written language databases
data, and the results indicate significant deficits in
would allow teachers and speech-language pathologists to
spoken language production, it is important to use
match performance with expectations for students with this information
typical literacy skills and those with language challenges. to improve the student’s language skills in [other] real
world contexts, such as the classroom. For a middle
school student, short samples of spoken language
Disciplinary Literacy could be taken in the classroom to plan individualized
The ability to read, write, listen, speak, and think crit- intervention and to monitor the student’s progress
ically begins to develop early and becomes increasingly over time. Appropriate contexts might include re-
cording the student while giving an oral report in

Figure 11. Case study 2–Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) Maze Summary report.

Downloaded From: http://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a New York University User on 09/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Figure 10. Case study 2–Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) Subordination Index report.

Miller et al.: Using LSA to Assess Spoken Language in Adolescents 109

Downloaded From: http://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a New York University User on 09/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Figure 12. Case study 2–Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) Omissions and Error Codes report.

history class (expository discourse); retelling a folk content by creating lists of words reflecting the curriculum
tale in English class (narrative discourse); debating a content. SALT can count the frequency of use for each
classmate in civics class ( persuasive discourse); or chat- word and pull up the utterances containing them. These
ting with a peer during lunch break (conversational data provide access to the student’s vocabulary in explaining
discourse). (personal communication as part of the the subject content. Advanced syntax and text-level analy-
review process, September 22, 2015). ses can be evaluated in a similar manner by coding features
Although these sampling contexts are beyond the relevant to the genre. Although no database comparisons
scope of the current SALT databases, the software can be can be made, the results support evidence-based practice.
used to examine literacy across the curriculum. Samples of Users can code any language feature of interest at the word,
spoken or written language can be examined for vocabulary morpheme, utterance, or text level. Using SALT’s advanced

110 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 47 • 99–112 • April 2016

Downloaded From: http://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a New York University User on 09/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
search and reporting features, these codes can be summa- analyzed using similar methods. However, at this time
rized for interpretation. Utterances containing coded SALT does not include reference databases for comparison.
features can be examined, providing context for further Middle and high school students must meet
analyses. challenges in the curriculum, some of which stem from
the CCSS, which mentions the need for students to
integrate skills in speaking, listening, reading, and writing
Current Research in specific con- tent areas. These curricular challenges
require clinicians to find ways to elucidate their specific
SALT Software’s newest project is the creation of a difficulties. Spoken and written language samples can
reference database of persuasive language samples. This capture these expectations for student learning (Miller,
database project targets high school students in ninth to Andriacchi, & Nockerts, 2013; SALT Software, 2015f).
12th grades. The choice of a persuasive task emanated from The case studies demonstrate how databases of typi-
the CCSS in which students are expected to be competent cally developing speakers can be used to confirm clinical
in spoken and written argumentation across the curriculum. impressions and direct follow-up with additional analyses
The persuasion protocol used to elicit the samples includes to more precisely document individual challenges. The
providing the student with a list of suggested topics, such databases capture typical language use in the contexts that
as convincing school officials to start school later in the these students meet every day in the classroom. Similar
day, or persuading government officials to increase the min- databases could be constructed for spoken and written liter-
imum wage. The protocol also includes a planning sheet acy contexts to document expectations for vocabulary, syn-
to provide the student with a scaffold for components that tax, and text-level organizational skills across subjects and
are expected in this task. Preliminary analyses of 113 stu- grades. Computer solutions provide the tools to explore lan-
dents from Wisconsin, grades 9–12, compared their per- guage use in detail. The challenge is to find ways to use
formance in exposition and persuasion. Students produced these powerful tools within the demands of clinical
shorter samples with more complex syntax in persuasion practice.
than exposition (see Table 2). Additional samples are being
collected in California and Australia, which will then be
analyzed in detail to determine whether they can be com-
bined with the Wisconsin samples.
References
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and
statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington,
Summary DC: Author.
LSA is a versatile tool to assess language production Bloom, L., & Lahey, M. (1978). Language development and
in everyday speaking situations, providing evidence to vali- lan- guage disorders. New York, NY: Wiley.
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge,
date clinical practice. A language sample is easily
MA: Harvard University Press.
understood by parents, teachers, administrators, and other Common Core State Standards (CCSS) Initiative. (2015). English
professionals, offering the opportunity to explain language arts standards. Retrieved from http://www.corestandards.
assessment outcomes and intervention decisions. Using org/ELA-Literacy/
databases of samples from typically developing students Donaldson, M. (1986). Children’s explanations: A psycholinguistic
provides detailed maps of study. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University
age- and grade-level performance expectations. These data Press.
will confirm that typical students’ language production Gillam, R., & Johnston, J. (1992). Spoken and written language
relationships in language/learning-impaired and normally
is not perfect and includes omissions, repetitions, and revi-
achieving school-age children. Journal of Speech and Hearing
sions as well as word choice and grammatical errors. Pro- Research, 35, 1303–1315.
duction errors of typical students, however, occur far less Hall-Mills, S., & Apel, K. (2015). Linguistic feature development
frequently or in different combinations than in students across grades and genre in elementary writing. Language,
requiring therapy. Samples of written language can be Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 46, 242–255.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the
Table 2. Comparison of sample length (NTW) and sentence complexity (SI) by
every- day experiences of young American children. Baltimore,
grade: Expository and persuasive samples. MD: Brookes.
Heilmann, J. (2010). Myths and realities of language sample analy-
sis. Perspectives on Language Learning and Education, 17, 4–8.
Grade in No. of
Heilmann, J., & Malone, T. (2014). The rules of the game: Proper-
school samples Protocol Mean NTW Mean SI
ties of a database of expository language samples. Language,
9th 35 Persuasion 457.03 2.04 Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 45, 277–290.
35 Exposition 829.26 1.69 Heilmann, J., Miller, J., Iglesias, A., Fabiano-Smith, L., Nockerts, A.,
10th 24 Persuasion 584.58 2.02 & Digney Andriacchi, K. (2008). Narrative transcription accuracy
24 Exposition 789.13 1.66 and reliability in two languages. Topics in Language Disorders,
11th 23 Persuasion 534.09 2.19 28, 178–188.
23 Exposition 874.52 1.70
Heilmann, J., Miller, J., & Nockerts, A. (2010). Using language
12th 29 Persuasion 535.66 2.15
sample databases. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services
29 Exposition 896.93 1.69
in Schools, 41, 84–95.

Downloaded From: http://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a New York University User on 09/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
M MacWhinney, B. (2000). The Ehren, & G. Wallach
ill CHILDES project: Tools (Eds.), Handbook of
e for analyz- ing talk (3rd language and literacy
r ed.). Mahwah, NJ: development and disorders
e Erlbaum. (2nd ed., pp. 524–543).
t Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where New York, NY: Guilford.
a are you? New York, NY: Dial Nelson, N. W. (2014b).
l. Press. Integrating language
: Miller, J. (1982). Assessing assessment, instruc- tion,
U language production in and intervention in an
s children. Baltimore, MD: inclusive writing lab
i University Park Press. approach. In B. Arfé, J.
n
Miller, J. (1991). Quantifying Dockrell, & V.
g
L
productive language Berninger (Eds.),
S disorders. In Writing devel-
A J. Miller (Ed.), Research opment in children with
on child language hearing loss, dyslexia, or
t disorders: A decade of oral language problems (pp.
o progress (pp. 211–231). 273–300). New York, NY:
A Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Oxford University Press.
s Miller, J., Andriacchi, K., & Nippold, M. (2014). Language
s Nockerts, A. (Eds.) sampling with adolescents:
e (2011). Assessing Im- plications for
s language production intervention (2nd ed.). San
s using SALT software: A Diego, CA: Plural
S clinician’s guide to
p Publishing.
language sample analysis.
o
Middleton, WI: SALT
k
e
Software LLC.
n Miller, J., Andriacchi, K., &
L Nockerts, A. (2013, July
a 1). Analysis assistance:
n SLPs turn to language
g sample analysis software
u to document progress on
a the Common Core State
g Standards. Advance
e Healthcare Network for
i Speech and Hearing.
n
Retrieved from
A
d
http://speech-language-
o pathology-
l audiology.advanceweb.
e com/Features/Articles/A
s nalysis-Assistance.aspx
c Miller, J., Heilmann, J.,
e Nockerts, A., Iglesias, A.,
n Fabiano, L., & Francis, D.
t (2006). Oral language and
s reading in bilingual chil-
1 dren. Learning Disabilities
1 Research & Practice, 21,
30–43.
1
Miller, J., & Iglesias, A.
Pathology, 15, 87–93.
Heilmann, J., Nockerts, A., & (2012). Systematic
Leadholm, B., & Miller, J.
Miller, J. (2010). Analysis of Language
(1992). Language sample
Language sam- pling: Transcripts (SALT)
analysis: The Wisconsin
Does the length of the [Computer software].
guide. Madison, WI:
transcript matter? Middleton, WI: SALT
Wisconsin Department
Language, Speech, and Software LLC.
of Public Instruction.
Hearing Services in Nelson, K. (1973). Structure
Loban, W. (1976). Language and strategy in learning to
Schools, 41, 393–404. development: Kindergarten
Heilmann, J., & Westerveld, talk.
through grade twelve. Monographs of the Society
M. (2013). Bilingual Urbana, IL: National
language sample for Research in Child
Council of Teachers.
analysis: Considerations Development, 38, 1–135.
Long, S. (2015). Computerized
and technological Nelson, N. W. (2014a).
Profiling [Computer
advances. Journal of Classroom-based writing
software].
Clinical Practice in assessment. In
Milwaukee, WI: Marquette
Speech-Language A. Stone, E. Silliman, B.
University.
Downloaded From: http://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a New York University User on 09/04/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
retell reference da-
Paul, R. (2012). Language tabase. Retrieved from
disorders from infancy http://saltsoftware.com/m
through adoles- cence (4th edia/wysiwyg/
ed.). St. Louis, MO: reference_database/Nar
Elsevier Mosby. StoryRetellRDBDoc.pdf
Piaget, J. (1926). SALT Software. (2015f).
Language and thought Using SALT to access
of the child. The the Common Core. State
Hague: Mouton. standards for speaking
Price, J., & Jackson, S. and listening: Grades K–
(2015). Procedures for 12. Retrieved from
obtaining and ana- http://saltsoftware.com/m
112 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 47 • 99–112 •
lyzing writing samples of April 2016
edia/wysiwyg/CCSS_
school-age children and grid.pdf
adolescents. Language, Scott, C., & Windsor, J.
Speech, and Hearing (2000). General language
Services in Schools, 46, performance measures in
277–293. spoken and written
Rojas, R., & Iglesias, A. narrative and expository
(2010). Using language discourse of school-age
sampling to mea- sure children with language
language growth. learning dis- abilities.
Perspectives on Journal of Speech,
Language Learning and Language, and Hearing
Education, 17, 24–31. Research, 43, 324–339.
Roskos, K., Tabors, P., & Slobin, D. (Ed.). (1985). The
Lenhart, L. (2009). cross-linguistic study of
Oral language and language acquisition.
early literacy in Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
preschool: Talking, Steig, W. (1982). Doctor De
reading, and writing Soto. New York, NY:
(2nd ed.). Newark, Farrar, Straus, &
DE: International Giroux.
Reading Association. Thordardottir, E. (2015,
SALT Software. (2015a). June). French
SALT expository language samples:
database. Retrieved Does length matter?
from Poster session
http://saltsoftware.com/m presented at the
edia/wysiwyg/reference_d meeting of the
atabase/ Symposium on
ExpoRDBDoc.pdf Research in Child
SALT Software. (2015b). Language Disorders,
Summary of SALT Madison, WI.
transcription conven- Tilstra, J., & McMaster, K.
tions. Retrieved from (2007). Productivity,
http://saltsoftware.com/m fluency, and
edia/wysiwyg/ grammaticality
tranaids/TranConvSum measures from
mary.pdf narratives: Potential
SALT Software. (2015c). indicators of language
Subordination Index. proficiency?
Retrieved from Communication
http://saltsoftware.com/me Disorders Quarterly, 29,
dia/wysiwyg/codeaids/SI_S 43–53.
coring_ Guide.pdf Weir, R. (1962). Language in
SALT Software. (2015d). the crib. The Hague: Mouton.
Expository Scoring Scheme Wisconsin Department of
(ESS) guide. Public Instruction.
Retrieved from (2015). CCSS for
http://saltsoftware. literacy in all subjects.
com/media/wysiwy Retrieved from
g/ http://dpi.wi.gov/sites/
codeaids/ESS_Sc default/files/imce/sta
oring_Guide.pdf ndards/pdf/las-
SALT Software. (2015e). stds.pdf
SALT narrative story

Downloaded From: http://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a New York University User on 09/04/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx

You might also like