You are on page 1of 17

Journal of Business Ethics (2020) 163:347–363

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-018-4020-1

ORIGINAL PAPER

Moral Intensity, Issue Characteristics, and Ethical Issue Recognition


in Sales Situations
Evelyne Rousselet1 · Bérangère Brial1 · Romain Cadario2 · Amina Béji‑Bécheur1

Received: 7 August 2017 / Accepted: 12 September 2018 / Published online: 22 October 2018
© Springer Nature B.V. 2018

Abstract
Researchers have considered individual and organizational factors of ethical decision making. However, they have little
interest in situational factors (McClaren, Journal of Business Ethics 112(1):101–125, 2013) which is surprising given the
many situations sales persons face. We address this issue using two pilot qualitative studies successively and a 2 by 2 within-
subject experiment with sales scenarios. Qualitative and quantitative data are obtained from front-line employees of the main
French retail banks that serve low-income customers. We show that the recognition of an ethical issue differs depending on
the role behavior salespersons are engaged in and the nature of the conflict of interest they face. Moreover, the combined
effect of these two situational characteristics is mediated by moral intensity. This study not only adds evidence on situational
factors affecting ethical decision but also extends empirical research on sales ethics by revealing sales situations that are not
considered in the empirical literature. The research implications of the findings are discussed along with the study’s limita-
tions and suggestions for future research.

Keywords  Ethical issue recognition · Moral intensity · Salesperson · Conflict of interest · Role behavior

Introduction because of their poor service and high prices for the low-
income segment, which they consider as having “low eco-
The number of low-income customers is constantly grow- nomic value”. The situation has worsened so much that
ing in developed countries (Ekström and Hjort 2009). One the government created a national observatory of banking
related key issue is the lack of will or ability of firms to inclusion (2014) which monitors the sustainable access of
serve these customers in a manner consistent with their individuals to banking products and services that meet their
needs (Ekström and Hjort 2009; Hill 2002, 2008; Khavul needs1. Despite the development of e-banking, French low-
and Burton 2013; Taludkar 2008). In France, having a bank income consumers continue to visit branches. In this context,
account for receiving deposits, including social benefits, how do bank salespersons2, who are under great pressure to
is compulsory. However, retail banks are often criticized generate revenue (Poujol et al. 2016), behave when serving
customers with low incomes and high social needs?
Addressing this issue requires consideration of the ethical
* Evelyne Rousselet
evelyne.rousselet@u‑pem.fr decisions made by bank salespersons. Ethical decision mak-
ing (EDM) has been widely studied. The rationalist-based
Bérangère Brial
berangere.brial@u‑pem.fr approach considers that EDM is a multistage process (Fer-
rell and Gresham 1985; Hunt and Vitell 1986; Jones 1991;
Romain Cadario
r.cadario@ieseg.fr Rest 1986; Trevino 1986; Wotruba 1990). The first step
consists of the recognition by an individual that the issue
Amina Béji‑Bécheur
amina.becheur@u‑pem.fr being addressed has an ethical concern; this recognition is
1
1
  Source the website of the Ministry of Economic and Financial
University Paris Est, IRG, Institut de Recherche en Gestion, Affaires: https​://www.econo​mie.gouv.fr/insta​llati​on-de-l-obser​vatoi​
5 bd Descartes, Cité Descartes, Champs‑sur‑Marne, re-de-lincl​usion​-banca​ire.
77454 Marne‑la‑Vallée cedex 2, France 2
  Following Poujol et al. (2016), we use the term “bank salesperson”
2
IÉSEG School of Management (LEM-CNRS UMR 9221), 1 rather than “bank front-line employee,” although both are equivalent
Parvis de La Défense, 92044 Paris La Défense, France in the French context.

13
Vol.:(0123456789)

348 E. Rousselet et al.

essential since it triggers the entire decision process (Ferrell as an extreme case (Yin 1994). Indeed, retail banks offer
and Gresham 1985; Hunt and Vitell 1986; Jones 1991; Rest what these customers have a priority need for: money. This
1986). For instance, the ethical (unethical) behavior of banks may reveal ethical issues that could be difficult to observe
salespersons when serving low-income customers depends in other circumstances.
on whether or not they perceive that the conflict between This paper is organized as follows. The next section pro-
economic and social objectives or the conflict between their vides the theoretical foundations. Then, we successively
own system of values and the marketing strategy of their present the results of the qualitative analysis (study 1), the
employer has an ethical content. Therefore, the question is hypothesis development, the method, and the results of our
to determine whether bank salespersons have this ethical quantitative analysis (study 2). The final section is dedicated
sensitivity. to a general discussion including the discussion of our find-
The literature provides useful information regarding ings, the limitations of our research, and a research agenda.
this issue. The recognition of an ethical issue appears to
depend on individual and organizational factors (Craft 2013;
McClaren 2013; O’Fallon and Butterfield 2005). Jones Theoretical Foundations
(1991) also argues that it depends on the characteristics of
the moral issue itself. He introduces, in the so-called issue- Ethical Decision Models in Business and Sales
contingent model, the concept of moral intensity (MI), which
“capture[s] the extent of issue-related moral imperative[s] A number of models have been proposed to help explain eth-
in a situation” (p. 372). Empirical studies mostly confirm ical (or unethical) behaviors in business. Most of these are
the strong, positive, and direct effect of MI on ethical issue rationalist based (Schwartz 2016). These models consider
recognition (REC) (Craft 2013; O’Fallon and Buttterfield EDM as a multistage process (Ferrell and Gresham 1985;
2005). However, they rarely use samples of salespersons. Hunt and Vitell 1986; Jones 1991; Rest 1986; Trevino 1986;
“Future research might investigate whether previous find- Wotruba 1990). EDM consists of four successive steps: the
ings about MI from other populations are applicable to sales recognition of an ethical issue, the judgement of the ethical-
practitioners and selling” (McClaren 2013, p. 109). Indeed, ity of the issue, the formation of behavioral intentions, and
Bush and colleagues (2017) suggest that salespeople differ the subsequent behavior (Hunt and Vitell 1986; Rest 1986).
from other employees from an ethical perspective, which Each step of this moral decision structure (Wotruba 1990)
“could result in a different mindset when it comes to ethical depends on the previous one.
behavior” (p. 550). The “ethical decision/action process” framework Wotruba
Our goal is to help fill the gap in the literature by testing (1990) developed specifically for salespeople is built on
the effects of MI on REC in the situations that sales practi- this moral decision structure and includes three other major
tioners face. This effort is important because “maintaining components: characteristics of the decision-maker, situ-
an ethical workforce remains a challenging issue for organi- ational moderators, and outcomes. This framework enables
zations given the complexity of business operations” (Agni- researchers to direct research activity to specific components
hotri et al. 2012, p. 243). Moreover, the relation between of the ethical decision process in the context of sales (Ferrel
MI and REC may be contingent upon the context (May and et al. 2007; McClaren 2000).
Pauli 2002; Valentine and Hollingworth 2012). However, Empirical research in sales ethics has increased substan-
one reason why salespersons face many dilemmas is that tially since the end of the 1990s (McClaren 2013). Research-
they are responsible for multiple activities and have many ers have introduced sales-specific components in EDM
roles (Bush et al. 2017; Dubinsky et al. 1986). Therefore, we models such as sales activity (Ferrel et al. 2007; McClaren
also aim to add to the understanding of EDM in the context 2000) or sales subculture (Bush et al. 2017). They particu-
of sales (Agnihotri et al. 2012; Hansen and Riggle 2009; larly study organizational factors such as the sales ethical
Valentine 2009) by focusing on a variety of sales situations. climate, the supervisory style, and the use of rewards and
In this research, we first construct a set of theoretical punishments (see Ferrel et al. 2007; McClaren 2013) which
propositions about the characteristics of sales situations that are highly important for sales force management. Research-
could influence the recognition of an ethical problem. This ers also consider individual factors of EDM such as age,
step is based on both literature and an exploratory qualita- gender, education, and personal values (McClaren 2013).
tive analysis (study 1) that includes individual and focus However, they have little interest in MI (McClaren 2013)
group interviews. Then, we test these propositions in stage 2 which is the situational factor introduced by Jones (1991).
with an experimental design using sales scenarios (study 2).
Qualitative and quantitative data are obtained from front-line
employees of the main French retail banks that serve low-
income customers. This empirical setting can be considered

13
Moral Intensity, Issue Characteristics, and Ethical Issue Recognition in Sales Situations 349

MI and REC sales ethical problems based on the individuals who gener-


ate them. They suggest that there are three situations that
The construct of MI refers to the individual’s evaluation of may pose ethical problems: customer-related situations that
the specific context of an ethical (unethical) decision. It is “involve customers and have [a] direct impact upon them”
seen as « a barometer for engaging in ethical behavior and (p. 48), work-related situations that are caused by the firm’s
intentions, with stronger moral intensity producing more directives or procedures and peer-related situations that
salient ethical intentions » (Lenhert et al. 2015, p. 205). It involve friends and relatives. This classification is contro-
is composed of six dimensions: the magnitude of the con- versial because it implicitly considers that the interaction
sequences, social consensus, proximity, the probability of between a salesperson and a customer is isolated from the
effect, the concentration of effect, and temporal immediacy firm. However, from the perspective of service-dominant
(Jones 1991). Jones (1991) argues that MI has a direct effect (S-D) Logic, the customer relationship should be consid-
on each of the four steps of EDM. ered as a result of the processes of serving; in other words,
Ethical issue recognition refers to “the degree to which the experience is co-created by the service supplier and the
an individual recognizes the aspects of a situation that service beneficiary (Vargo and Lusch 2004). Therefore, cus-
carry a reasonable likelihood of moral wrong or harm to tomer-related situations and work-related situations cannot
individuals, classes of people, or other entities” (VanSandt be systematically distinguished from an ethical perspective.
et al. 2006, p. 414). The relationship between MI and REC In addition, it is precisely because customer-related situ-
is confirmed in various studies (Barnett 2001; Barnett and ations involve three shareholders—the customer, the firm,
Valentine 2004; Butterfield et al. 2000; Deconinck 2005; and the salesperson—whose goals or interests often conflict
Leitsch 2004; May and Pauli 2002; Singhapakdi et al. 1996, (Agnihotri et al. 2012) that sales forces frequently face con-
1999; Valentine and Hollingworth 2012). However, with the flicts of interest (Agnihotri et al. 2012; Bellizzi and Hasty
exception of DeConinck (2005), these studies do not use 2003).
samples of salespersons (McClaren 2013). This is all the A conflict of interest occurs when one individual has sev-
more surprising as salespersons perform various activities eral goals or interests that, if mutually pursued, may result
(Bush et al. 2017; Dubinsky et al. 1986) and interact with in injury to the customers or to the firm (Beauchamp and
many stakeholders (Agnihotri et al. 2012). Therefore, there Bowie 1979). The literature most often describes customer-
may be variations in sales situations. related situations in which the interests of the sales force
Moreover, MI appears to have a different effect on REC conflict with those of their customers. Such conflicts arise,
depending on the various scenarios used within an empiri- for instance, when the desire of a salesperson to reach a sales
cal scenario-based study (Barnett and Valentine 2004; May quota may not produce a benefit to the customer (Abratt and
and Pauli 2002; Sweeney and Costello 2009). Valentine and Penman 2002; Boyle 2000; Honeycutt et al. 2001; Lee et al.
Holligworth (2012) suggest “explor[ing] the differences in 2009; Schepers et al. 2012; Valentine and Barnett 2007).
scenarios with respect to the different types of ethical per- Schepers et al. (2012) also refer to customer-related situa-
spectives that are embedded within them.” (Valentine and tions in which the interests of the customers contradict those
Hollingworth 2012, p. 519). Thus, understanding the rela- of the firm. However, salespersons might not give the same
tionship between MI and REC in a sales context means iden- importance to ethical transgressions depending on whether
tifying the characteristics that differentiate sales scenarios or their interests conflict with either their customers or their
sales situations from an ethical point of view. employers (Chonko and Hunt 1985; Dabholkar and Kella-
ris 1992; Kellaris and Dabholkar 1989). Therefore, from an
Sales Situations from an Ethical Perspective ethical perspective, customer-related situations may differ
according to the parties involved in the conflict of interest.
Not all sales contexts have an ethical concern (Singhapaki As mentioned above, the salesforce may also face many
1999). Nevertheless, it is well known that salespersons face ethical dilemmas because of their many roles and activi-
many ethical dilemmas (McClaren 2000). The literature ties (Bush et al. 2017). McClaren (2000) and Ferrell and
highlights several reasons among which the various inter- colleagues (2007) suggest that sales or selling activity
ests they have to balance (Agnihotri et al. 2012; Valentine impacts the MI of a situation (McClaren 2000, 2013). Efforts
2009) and their many activities and roles (Bush et al. 2017; have been made to study the extent to which certain sales
Dubinsky et al. 1986). We examine to what extent further activities impact EDM (McClaren 2013). However, despite
evaluating these reasons may help identify various types of McClaren’s claims (2000, 2013), there has been a very
situations that can differ from an ethical perspective. limited use of selling taxonomies in sales ethics research
Salespersons face ethical dilemmas because their own (McClaren 2013). Management control researchers have
interests and the interests of their customers and employ- long distinguished between in-role behaviors and extra-
ers may conflict. Levy and Dubinski (1983) differentiate role behaviors (McKenzie et al. 1991, 1998; Williams and

13

350 E. Rousselet et al.

Anderson 1991), which may offer one possible classification of his company whose reputation he may put at risk while
for sales situations from an ethical point of view. performing this extra-role behavior. However, the litera-
Role behavior refers to the nature of the behavior that ture does not describe the four situations resulting from the
employees adopt. In-role behaviors are required and are also combination of these two characteristics. Moreover, there
prescribed behaviors (Katz 1964; Van Dyne et al. 1998). are very few empirical studies on customer-directed extra-
In contrast, extra-role behaviors refer to activities that are role behaviors. Thus, we first conduct a qualitative study,
voluntarily engaged in outside of role prescriptions and are which has two goals: understanding how the salespersons
beyond the call of duty (Kim and Mauborgne 1996). This of banks interpret their in-role (extra) role behaviors and
distinction is particularly interesting for salespeople since understanding the nature of the possible conflicts of interest
their autonomy and boundary-spanning positions could facil- they must cope with in situations where they perform these
itate extra-role behaviors. When applied to salespersons in role behaviors. Then, we construct hypotheses and conduct
customer-related situations, in-role behaviors include activi- a quantitative experiment.
ties such as sales planning, sales adaptability, and after-sales
support, which are included in sales job descriptions. In con-
trast, extra-role behaviors are initiatives that improve service Qualitative Research Background
interactions with customers or respond to customer concerns
that go beyond formal role requirements (Bettencourt and Our qualitative research includes twenty-one individual and
Brown 1997). For instance, a salesperson recommends the two focus group interviews. Appendix A describes why and
business activity of one of his customers; he introduces cus- how we use these interviews and the methodological details.
tomers to people who can help them solve problems whether The main results are presented below.
those problems are work related or not (MacKenzie et al. There are six low customer-related situations mentioned
1998); or he gives personal advice to the customer (Brief by most of the interviewees. Only one situation is initiated
and Motowidlo 1986; MacKenzie et al. 1998). by bank salespersons: selling a banking product or service
An important issue is the boundary between in-role and to an existing low-income customer. Customers drive all
extra-role behaviors. It could vary depending on industry the other interactions. They visit branches frequently and
or organization (Morrison 1994). However, Van Dyne and have frequent requests, which result in five situations for
Lepine (1998) found that employees, peers, and supervisors the bank salespersons: reducing (or not) an overdraft fee for
distinguish extra- and in-role behaviors consistently over a customer who overdraws his account every time he meets
time. Moreover, focusing on one particular form of extra- an unexpected expense; reducing (or not) a debit rejection
role behavior (organizational citizenship behavior), Vey and charge for a customer in similar conditions; helping (or not)
Campbell (2004) found that in-role behaviors were more a customer manage his money and account; helping (or not)
frequently associated with conscientiousness and courtesy, a customer obtain an advance of money, even if his bank
whereas extra-role behaviors were more frequently associ- overdraft has reached the maximum allowed; and opening
ated with altruism and civic virtue items. This finding could (or not) a bank account for a new customer.
indicate that in-role and extra-role behaviors are not inspired The situation where bank salespersons sell a banking
by the same moral virtues. However, ethical conflict may product or service to an existing low-income customer as
arise when salespeople’s values differ from those perceived well as three of the five situations initiated by low-income
to be held by their immediate professional environment customers cause bank salespersons to engage in a behavior
(Schwepker et al. 1997). Thus, there could be a difference that fits with what they consider to be their job. There is
in ethics between customer-related situations where sales- no doubt that, for any of the interviewees, opening a bank
persons perform an in-role behavior, and situations where account, selling a product or service to an existing customer,
salespersons adopt an extra-role behavior. or negotiating an overdraft fee or debit rejection charge rep-
To conclude, from an ethical point of view, two char- resent a component of their job. These behaviors belong to
acteristics could distinguish sales situations: the nature of the general tasks they spontaneously list when they describe
the conflict of interest and the role behavior of salesper- their job during the personal interviews. Bank salespersons’
sons. Some examples suggest that these characteristics can role behavior in the last two situations that consist of help-
be combined. For instance, when a salesperson considers ing the customers is less obvious. However, the interpre-
whether to make false promises to close a sale (Abratt and tation of the data results in classifying them as extra-role
Penman 2002), the interest of his customer conflicts with behavior since these situations are either associated with
his self-interest while engaging in in-role behavior. When a environments that are different from the bank such as chari-
customer asks a salesperson to promote his business to other ties, or are contradictory to the business dimension of banks.
customers (Bettencourt et al. 1997), this salesperson must Selected quotes supporting our interpretation are provided
balance the interests of this one customer and the interests in Table 1.

13
Moral Intensity, Issue Characteristics, and Ethical Issue Recognition in Sales Situations 351

Table 1  Bank sales persons’ extra-role behavior: additional evidence as direct when the seller considers that his personal inter-
Situations Representative quotes
est contradicts with the interest of the buyer. The interest
divergence is indirect when the seller does not perceive that
Helping a customer Customers think we manage their banking his interests conflict but considers that those of his company
manage his money account. They are wrong. We are not here
may conflict with those of the buyer.
and account for that
Bank is a commercial firm. We are in branch Combining data from the individual interviews and focus
to do business groups, we classify situations according to the nature of the
I am not a social worker conflict of interest and the role behavior. Next, we focus
Helping a customer That is not in our hands on four of the six situations, excluding circumstances with
with an advance of ­ oeura
I tell them to go to les Restos du C
legal or technical issues that may create ambiguity about
money
ethics. The four scenarios include the following: selling a
a
 Charities banking product or service to a low-income customer (SAL);
reducing an overdraft fee for a low-income customer who
overdraws his account every time he meets an unexpected
Regarding role behavior, the interviewees classify the expense (OVE); helping a low-income customer manage his
situations in the same way; however, their behaviors vary. money and account, while he has various types of expenses
Some of them respond favorably to low-income customers’ and credit card debt that he cannot afford (MAN); helping a
requests for help, whereas others do not. It could perhaps low-income customer whose bank overdraft has reached the
depend on their sensitivity to the distress of their custom- maximum allowed, although he struggles to meet his basic
ers. Our qualitative research does not allow for conclusive needs and lacks money (HEL). Table 2 shows the charac-
statements on this issue. However, the analysis reveals that teristics of these situations regarding conflict of interest and
most of the focus group interviewees view the six situations role behavior.
as conflicts of interest, which differ according to the parties
involved.
Bank salespersons do not feel personally involved in situ-
ations where the interest of their company is at stake, for
Hypothesis Development
instance when low-income customers apply for a reduction
As shown in Fig. 1, we posit that the interplay of role behav-
in overdraft charges. One interviewee explains that his inter-
ior and interest divergence with REC is mediated by moral
est is not at stake, and he emphasizes that “bank employees
intensity. In this section, we develop separate hypotheses to
do not receive any commission on banking fees”. This is also
disentangle the associated mechanisms.
the case when a salesperson reduces (or not) an overdraft
fee for a customer who overdraws his account every time he
The Effect of Salesperson’s In‑role Versus Extra‑role
meets an unexpected expense. Moreover, some employees
Behaviors
consider that this situation is unbalanced: the advance that
low-income customers ask for is a substantial amount of
REC is an interpretative process (Rest 1986), and situations
money for them, whereas the cost for the bank in case of
are not always easy to interpret. Individuals use cognitive
non-repayment is very tiny compared to its profit.
frameworks to process information when evaluating a cir-
In contrast, bank salespersons appear to consider their
cumstance they encounter (Gioia and Poole 1984). These
own interests in any situation where they may either lose
frameworks are partly shaped by the working social context
an opportunity to reach their quota—in a sales situation—
that people operate in (Butterfield et al. 2000; Morris and
or lose time. All the interviewees consider low-income
McDonald 1995; Valentine and Barnett 2007; Valentine and
customers to be time-consuming. For a bank salesperson,
Bateman 2011). A context with increased commercial pres-
helping (or not) a customer manage his money and account
sure on sales and aggressive competition results in lower
takes time without an opportunity to reach his or her quota
REC (Butterfield et al. 2000; Valentine and Bateman 2011).
because of the poor economic value of the customer; for
Situations where salespersons adopt in-role behaviors are
most of the interviewees, opening a bank account for a low-
contexts where people are focused on sales, whereas situ-
income consumer means losing time with time-consuming
ations where they engage in extra-role behaviors are not
people because of the many payment incidents and without
(MacKenzie et al. 1998). Extra-role behaviors refer to the
the opportunity to sell banking products or services.
completion of extra duties (Hoffman et al. 2007). Conse-
Therefore, in what follows, we shall use the term “inter-
quently, we could expect REC to be higher in situations
est divergence”, which represents the conflict or tension
in which salespeople engage in extra-role behaviors than
between the interests of two parties involved in a sale:
in situations in which they engage in in-role behaviors.
buyer, seller, and employer. “Interest divergence” is defined

13

352 E. Rousselet et al.

Table 2  Typical salesperson/low-income customer situations


Salesperson role behavior
In-role Extra-role

Interest divergence
 Direct: between the customer Sales (SAL) A customer has incomes so low that his account Help with account and money management
and the seller is frequently in the red at the end of month. However, (MAN) A low-income customer does not
selling him a banking product or service can help a bank manage his money. He has various types of
salesperson to meet his sales targets. The bank salesperson expenses and consumer credits whereas he
has to decide whether to make a sale or not cannot afford them. The bank salesperson
with whom he is in contact has to decide
whether to help him manage his money and
account or not
 Indirect: between the customer Cutting on overdraft fees (OVE) A low-income customer Help when necessity of money for meeting
and the bank has his overdraft accounts in excess of the authorized limit basic needs (HEL) A low-income customer
every time he meets an unexpected expense. This results meets financial difficulties from a personal or
in bank charges for him. The salesperson with whom he is family event. His bank overdraft has reached
in contact has to decide whether to reduce overdraft fees the maximum allowed. However, he struggles
or not to meet his basic needs including food and
hygiene. He needs money. The bank salesper-
son with whom he is in contact has to decide
whether to help him or not

Direct vs. H1  REC will be stronger in situations in which salespeo-


indirect interest
divergence ple engage in extra-role behaviors than those in which they
H2
engage in in-role behaviors.
Salesperson in- Moral Intensity Ethical
- Magnitude of consequences, issue
role vs. extra-role
- Probability of effect recognition
behavior H3
- Temporal immediacy H3 The Interplay Between Salesperson Role Behaviors
and Interest Divergence
H1
We first consider situations in which salespeople adopt in-
role behaviors. A careful review of empirical findings reveals
Fig. 1  Theoretical framework
that the literature focuses on in-role behaviors. Earlier
research suggests that salespersons judge ethical transgres-
In addition, firms provide training programs to improve sions affecting customers as less ethical than those affecting
the performance and effectiveness of their salesforce’s in- their employers (Chonko and Hunt 1985). We can try and
role behaviors. During these trainings, salespersons learn extrapolate this assumption about moral judgement on REC.
about customer categories, sales strategies, and practices However, empirical studies report conflicting results (Abratt
(Sharma et al. 2007). Valentine and Hollingworth (2012) and Penman 2002; Dabholkar and Kellaris 1992).
suggest that the use of cognitive scripts and quantitative Nevertheless, when salespeople perform in-role behaviors
decision tools could reduce the ability of employees to “per- and face situations in which interest divergence is direct,
ceive and interpret the ethical nuances or moral implications they can choose to favor their self-interest, whereas they
of a particular situation” (Valentine and Hollingworth 2012, are unlikely to do so when interest divergence is indirect.
p. 512) because they contribute to over-simplified decision This occurs because salespersons may not promote their
making. Following Valentine and Hollingworth (2012), self-interest at the customers’ cost since they risk punish-
guiding sales forces to engage in the most appropriate in- ment (Trevino 1992). Therefore, salespersons may perceive
role behavior (Sharma et al. 2007) could reduce their ability the situations where interest divergence is direct as riskier
to identify ethical nuances in situations where they adopt than situations where interest divergence is indirect. How-
in-role behaviors. In contrast, extra-role behaviors are spon- ever, sales managers who perceive greater risk in an ethical
taneous behaviors (Hoffman et al. 2007). Therefore, we sug- dilemma are more likely to consider that the dilemma is an
gest that when adopting them, salespersons deliberate and ethical issue (Cherry and Fraedrich 2002). Extending this
therefore pay attention to ethical nuances. These converging result beyond sales managers to any salesperson, we present
arguments lead to the following hypothesis: the following hypothesis:

13
Moral Intensity, Issue Characteristics, and Ethical Issue Recognition in Sales Situations 353

H2a  For situations in which salespeople engage in in-role Moreover, we suggest that, in situations where salesper-
behaviors, REC will be stronger when interest divergence is sons perform in-role behaviors, MI is stronger when interest
direct than when it is indirect. divergence is direct than when it is indirect. In these situa-
tions, salespeople focus on sales (MacKenzie et al. 1998),
In contexts where salespersons can adopt extra-role which partly shapes the cognitive frameworks they use
behaviors, our qualitative study reveals that these are help- when evaluating situations (Gioia and Poole 1984; Morris
ing behaviors. To our knowledge, no research has previously and McDonald 1995). It is possible that salespersons evalu-
studied the helping behaviors of salespeople that are directed ate the MI of situations by primarily considering their sales
at customers. There has been a large amount of research quotas and incentives because these would be in their self-
on helping behaviors that are directed at other employees interest. However, salespersons’ interests are more obvious
within organizations (Karriker and Williams 2009; Moss- in situations where interest divergence is direct than when
holder et al. 2011; Podsakoff et al. 2000; Tang et al. 2008). it is indirect.
These behaviors appear to be significantly associated with Let us use the following as an example: a salesperson
fairness or justice perceptions (Karriker and Williams 2009). favors his customer in a situation where interest divergence
We can assume that the cognitive frameworks salespersons is direct. His prejudice is related to his variable compensa-
use when evaluating situations that require them to engage tion since salespersons often have high performance-related
in extra-role behaviors are partly shaped by equity concerns. bonuses. In a situation where interest divergence is indirect,
According to equity theory (Adams 1963, 1965), people if a salesperson favors his customer to the detriment of his
evaluate the fairness of a situation by comparing the ratio of employer, the negative consequence for his employer may
their own inputs and outcomes with the ratio of the inputs be offset by the positive results of other salespersons. Thus,
and outcomes of others. In our case, it is difficult to support the effect of his behavior on himself is more likely in the
this principle because salespeople are not directly involved former situation than in the latter one. This effect is also
in situations where interest divergence is indirect. How- more immediate. Indeed, sales quotas generally apply to
ever, our qualitative study reveals that several bank sales- shorter periods than the sales or profit targets of companies.
persons spontaneously noted that situations where interest Therefore, for situations in which employees engage in in-
divergence is indirect present a great imbalance: the finan- role behaviors, MI should be stronger when interest diver-
cial issue of the situation is much more important for the gence is direct than when it is indirect. This hypothesized
low-income customer than for the bank. None of the bank relationship combined with our assumption regarding the
salespersons stressed such an imbalance in situations where positive impact of MI on REC implies that when salespeople
interest divergence was direct. Therefore, the salespersons perform in-role behaviors, interest divergence affects REC
may perceive that situations for which interest divergence via a mediator, MI:
is indirect present greater financial inequity than situations
for which interest divergence is direct. Consequently, the H3a  For situations in which salespersons engage in in-role
former situations should be more salient than the latter one. behaviors, MI will mediate the positive impact of direct
However, salient issues are more likely to catch the attention interest divergence on REC.
of any decision-maker (Jones 1991). These elements lead us
to suggest the following hypothesis: In contrast, in situations where salespersons are supposed
to adopt an extra-role behavior, they no longer focus on sales
H2b  For situations in which salespeople engage in extra- issues. The qualitative results reveal that low-income cus-
role behaviors, REC will be stronger when interest diver- tomers ask the bank salespersons for help. We can expect
gence is indirect than when it is direct. that salespersons assess the MI of the situation first con-
sidering the interests of their customers. However, custom-
The Mediating Role of MI ers’ requests for help are not the same in both situations
in which salespeople may engage in extra-role behaviors.
As mentioned before, MI appears to have a direct influence Low-income customers ask for advances for essential needs
on REC in several studies (Barnett 2001; Barnett and Valen- such as food, hygiene, or housing in situations where interest
tine 2004; Butterfield et al. 2000; Deconinck 2005; Leitsch divergence is indirect, whereas they ask for assistance with
2004; May and Pauli 2002; Singhapakdi et al. 1996, 1999; managing their bank account in situations where interest
Valentine and Hollingworth 2012). With the exception of divergence is direct. We suggest that salespersons perceive
Deconinck (2005), these studies do not use samples of sales- that the financial difficulties of low-income customers are
people (McClaren 2013); however, we can expect that MI more important in the former situation than in the latter one.
has a positive impact on REC. Salespersons may also consider that if they choose to help

13

354 E. Rousselet et al.

Table 3  Summary of hypotheses
Hypotheses

H1 REC will be stronger in situations in which salespeople engage in extra-role behaviors than those in which they engage in in-role behaviors
H2a For situations in which salespeople engage in in-role behaviors, REC will be stronger when interest divergence is direct than when it is
indirect
H2b For situations in which salespeople engage in extra-role behaviors, REC will be stronger when interest divergence is indirect than when it
is direct
H3a For situations in which salespersons engage in in-role behaviors, MI will mediate the positive impact of direct interest divergence on REC
H3b For situations in which salespeople engage in extra-role behaviors, the moral intensity will mediate the negative impact of direct interest
divergence on REC

low-income customers, the positive consequence of their helps reduce social desirability bias (Robertson and
behavior for their customers will be more immediate when Anderson 1993). The first pair is salesperson behavior:
interest divergence is indirect than when it is direct. Indeed, extra-role vs. in-role, and the second pair is interest diver-
assistance with managing a bank account, which can be gence: direct vs. indirect.
viewed as improving financial literacy, can help customers Ninety-nine French bank apprentices participated in
improve their financial situation in a progressive manner. In the study. In France, an apprenticeship is similar to the
contrast, providing an advance to a low-income customer definition proposed by Lewis (2014): « a contract between
helps him immediately. an employer and a person that combines a structured pro-
Therefore, salespersons should perceive greater and more gramme of on-the-job training and productive work with
immediate consequences of their behavior related to custom- part-time, formal technical education » (Lewis 2014,
ers in the former situation (indirect interest divergence) than p. 498). Apprentices spend about 60 percent of their time
in the latter one (direct interest divergence). Therefore, for in bank branches where they are considered full members
extra-role behaviors, MI should be stronger when interest and placed in front-line positions of increasing responsibil-
divergence is indirect than when it is direct. This proposed ity. The participants, of whom 54 percent are women, are
relationship combined with our assumption regarding the on average 22 years old and have seniority of 15 months
impact of MI on REC implies that when salespeople perform in front-line positions in retail banking. Thus, they can
extra-role behaviors, interest divergence affects REC via a be considered as junior bank salespersons. It is unlikely
mediator, moral intensity. that they consider themselves as students, as they are not
mixed with students who are not apprentices. Participants
H3b  For situations in which salespeople engage in extra- were recruited through two leading French universities and
role behaviors, the moral intensity will mediate the negative have not received any ethics courses, which could sub-
impact of direct interest divergence on REC. ject the study to bias. Apprentices interviewed in the two
focus groups of study 1 were excluded from this study. We
Table 3 summarizes the hypotheses tested in study 2. conducted all experimental sessions in classrooms during
teaching hours to minimize the risk that students would
rush while answering the questionnaire.
Study 2 We developed four scenarios from our qualitative study,
following the vignette model used by Valentine and Bate-
Method man (2011), in the sense that each scenario describes a
situation but does not report the action the salespersons
Design, Participants, and Scenarios have decided to pursue. This is not the most common type
of scenario in the business ethics empirical literature, but
We use a 2 (salesperson’s role behavior: extra-role vs. in- it is well suited to our study since our qualitative findings
role) by 2 (interest divergence: direct vs. indirect) within- reveal great variations in the behaviors bank employees
subject experiment with sales scenarios. In the ethics lit- adopt when facing the same circumstances. We deliber-
erature, researchers often use quantitative scenario-based ately wrote short texts to determine the test length and
methodologies (O’Fallon and Butterfield 2005; Lenhert ran a pretest involving four bank employees who had
2015). The use of profession-specific settings ensures over 20 years of experience working in retail banks. The
that participants are completely involved (Greenberg and vignettes appear to be entirely plausible, although the texts
Eskew 1993) and avoids candid or ready-made responses are brief. We made a few semantic adjustments for two
(Robertson and Anderson 1993). Projective questioning of the vignettes to ensure the participants would be able

13
Moral Intensity, Issue Characteristics, and Ethical Issue Recognition in Sales Situations 355

to understand them. The four scenarios are presented in was measured on a three-item scale scored on a 7-point Lik-
Table 2. In the questionnaire, the sequence of vignettes ert format that included “influential”, “result-oriented”, and
was randomized to avoid position bias (Converse and “ambitious”. In alignment with Reynolds (2006), formalism
Presser 1986). and utilitarian were not correlated (r = 0.05, p = 0.58).
Ethical climate can be broadly defined as an employee’s
Dependent and Mediating Measures perception of the ethical content of the practices, proce-
dures, and actions of his company (Victor and Cullen 1988;
REC was measured with a single item that has been used Wimbush and Shepard 1994). We included this as a con-
in previous research (Barnett and Valentine 2004; Bateman trol variable for two reasons: ethical climate may influence
et al. 2013; Hollingworth and Valentine 2015; Singhapakdi the ethical decision-making process (Valentine and Barnett
et al. 1996, 1999; Valentine and Barnett 2007; Valentine 2007) and the participants in our survey worked in different
and Hollingworth 2012). MI was measured by the probable French retail banks that may have different ethical climates.
magnitude of the consequences. Magnitude of the conse- Ethical climate was measured using the scale developed by
quences is the first factor of the 3-factor structure of MI that Lavorata (2007), which includes commercial components.
McMahon and Harvey (2006) found. It includes three of the Poujol and colleagues (Poujol et al. 2016) used this scale
six dimensions of MI: magnitude of consequences, prob- to measure ethical climates in retail banks (Poujol et al.
ability of effect, and temporal immediacy. Each dimension 2016). The first dimension, ethical procedures within the
was measured with two items that are also used in McMahon company, was measured with three items (α = 0.86): “The
and Harvey (2006). All the responses used a seven-point general management of your company has made it clear that
Likert format anchored by “completely agree/completely non-ethical behavior will not be tolerated”; “Salespersons
disagree”. These items were developed in English and had acting unethically in the company’s interest are sanctioned”;
never been translated into French. Two professional transla- and “Salespersons acting unethically in their own interest
tors translated them into French, and we ran a pilot test on 20 are sanctioned”. The second dimension, ethical peer behav-
students. At the end of the test, participants were encouraged iors, was also measured with three items (α = 0.72): “In your
to ask questions, and there did not appear to be any problems company, customers are sometimes encouraged to buy prod-
understanding them. All the items are presented in Appen- ucts they don’t really need”; “In your company, sales person-
dix B. Separate reliability analyses for each dimension of nel are primarily evaluated on their results rather than on the
MI by experimental conditions yielded acceptable results quality of their work”; and “In your company, sales competi-
(Cronbach’s alpha was higher than 0.74 for MI). Reliability tions push salespeople to sell products that are not always
analyses yielded Spearman–Brown statistics ranging from suited to their customers’ needs”. This scale was originally
0.33 to 0.72. developed in a French-speaking context. We controlled for
We performed Harman’s single factor tests for all four translation as with the previous variables.
within-subject experimental conditions. The total variance
explained by a single factor varied from 27 to 36%. Common Results
method bias was not considered an issue since these val-
ues were below the threshold of 50% (Podsakoff and Organ Repeated Measures ANOVAs
1986).
First, we ran a repeated measures ANOVA on MI with
Control Variables the within-subject independent variables salesperson role
behavior and interest divergence as well as their interaction.
Ethical predisposition and ethical climate are included as The results for the main effect of salesperson role behavior
control variables based on previous research findings (see show that MI is stronger for extra-role than in-role behaviors
Appendix C). Ethical predisposition refers to the cogni- (M = 5.45 vs. M = 4.65, F(1.94) = 42.78, p < 0.001). There
tive framework individuals preferentially use in the ethical was no main effect for interest divergence. Of greater impor-
decision-making process (Brady and Wheeler 1996), which tance, we find a significant interaction between salesper-
influences REC (Reynolds 2006). Researchers usually con- son role behavior and interest divergence (F(1.94) = 26.51,
sider that ethical predisposition has two main forms: utilitar- p < 0.001) (see Fig. 2). Planned contrast tests showed that
ian and formalist. These forms were measured using items for the extra-role behavior condition, MI is lower in the
from Brady and Wheeler (1996). These items have previ- case of direct interest divergence (M = 5.27 vs. M = 5.63,
ously been used in ethics research (Reynolds 2006). Formal- F(1.94) = 8.41, p < 0.01). However, for the in-role behavior
ism (α = 0.73) was measured using a three-item scale scored condition, MI is higher in the case of direct interest diver-
using a 7-point Likert format that included “honest”, “trust- gence (M = 4.99 vs. M = 4.31, F(1.94) = 22.65, p < 0.001).
worthy”, and “good-intentioned”. Utilitarianism (α = 0.57)

13

356 E. Rousselet et al.

6 vs. M = 4.62, F(1.94) = 8.05, p < 0.01). However, for the in-


5.63
role behavior condition, REC is higher in the case of direct
Moral intensity*

5.27

4.99 interest divergence (M = 5.21 vs. M = 3.57, F(1.94) = 47.49,


5
p < 0.001).
4.31 Although the significance of the main effects is not con-
4
sistent for the two dependent variables, the pattern of the
Extra-role In-role interaction effect is stable for both dependent variables:
Salesperson role behavior there is a decrease for the extra-role behavior condition and
* captured by the three dimensions of the probable magnitude of the consequences an increase for the in-role behavior condition (see Fig. 2).
Hence, MI appears to be a mediator in the relationship
between the interplay of salesperson role behavior and
6
interest divergence on REC. In the next section, we describe
Ethical issue

5.21
mediation analyses to test this assumption.
recognition

5
4.62
We also ran similar repeated measures ANOVAs using
4
4.03
the different dimensions of MI as dependent variables (see
3.57
Table 4). Consistent with the previous results, we find that
3 the interaction effect is significant for the three components
Extra-role In-role
we selected for measuring MI: the magnitude of the conse-
Salesperson role behavior
quences, the probability of effect, and temporal immediacy.
Indirect Direct
Interest divergence Mediation Analyses

Fig. 2  The interplay between salesperson role behavior and the nature We conducted mediation analyses using Montoya and
of interest divergence Hayes’s (2017) mediation and moderation in repeated meas-
ures (MEMORE) macro. Given the absence of the moder-
ated mediation macro in the within-subject design, we ran
Second, we ran a repeated measures ANOVA on REC
separate mediation analyses for each condition of salesper-
with the within-subject independent variables salesperson
son role behavior. We ran two mediation analyses (1) for the
role behavior and interest divergence, as well as their inter-
extra-role behavior condition and (2) for the in-role behavior
action. The results show a main effect of interest divergence,
condition and for both the indirect and direct interest diver-
such that REC is stronger in the case of direct interest diver-
gence. The macro calculates the difference between the two
gence (M = 4.62 vs. M = 4.07, F(1.94) = 10.55, p < 0.01).
mediator measurements (MI for direct interest divergence
There was no main effect for salesperson role behavior. Of
minus MI for indirect interest divergence) and the differ-
greater importance, we find a significant interaction between
ence between the two dependent variable measurements (MI
role behavior and interest divergence (F(1.94) = 51.06,
for direct interest divergence minus MI for indirect inter-
p < 0.001), which is illustrated in Fig. 2. Planned contrast
est divergence). These are modeled in accordance with the
tests showed that for the extra-role behavior condition, REC
is lower in the case of direct interest divergence (M = 4.03

Table 4  Direct (H1) and Dependent variables Independent variables


interaction (H2) effects from the
repeated measures ANOVAs Salesperson role Interest divergence Role behav-
behavior (H1) ior × interest diver-
gence (H2)

Ethical issue recognition (REC) 0.15 10.55** 51.06***


Moral intensity (MI) 42.78*** 3.23 26.51***
Magnitude of consequences (MC) 51.41*** 8.25** 26.36***
Probability of effect (PE) 29.27*** 3.87 8.97**
Temporal immediacy (TI) 15.70*** 0.28 20.32***

Note Results from repeated measures ANOVAs. Each line represents a specific depended variable, and the
three columns correspond to the two within-subject factors, as well as their interaction. Value reported is
the F-Statistic
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001

13
Moral Intensity, Issue Characteristics, and Ethical Issue Recognition in Sales Situations 357

Table 5  Results from the mediation analyses (H3)


Mediators M Within the salesperson’s in-role behavior Within the salesperson’s extra-role behavior condi-
condition (H3a) tion (H3b)
X on M M on Y X on Y X on M M on Y X on Y through M
through M

Moral intensity (MI) 0.66** 0.40* 0.27* − 0.36* 0.60** − 0.35**


 Magnitude of consequences (MC) 0.99** 0.29* 0.29* − 0.40* 0.44** − 0.18*
 Probability of effect (PE) 0.62** 0.26* 0.16* − 0.17 0.31* − 0.06
 Temporal immediacy (TI) 0.38* 0.20 0.07 − 0.50** 0.42** − 0.20**

Note The independent variable X is the nature of interest divergence. The dependent variable Y is ethical issue recognition. We used the macro
MEMORE (Montoya and Hayes 2017) for within-subject design mediation analysis
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01

procedure described in Montoya and Hayes (2017) and Judd First, the results confirm that REC is issue-contingent
et al. (2001). Table 5 summarizes the mediation analyses. (Jones 1991) and more precisely, that it depends on MI,
First, for the extra-role behavior condition, we find as observed in past research (Barnett and Valentine 2004;
that direct interest divergence negatively influences MI Butterfield et al. 2000; Deconinck 2005; Leitsch 2004;
(a = − 0.36, t = − 2.83, p < 0.01). Second, controlling for May and Pauli 2002; Singhapakdi et al. 1996, 1999; Val-
interest divergence, we find that MI significantly influences entine and Bateman 2011; Valentine and Hollingworth
REC (b = 0.61, t = 4.04, p < 0.001). Third, controlling for 2012). Second, the impact of MI on REC differs depend-
MI, we find that interest divergence marginally influences ing on the situation. This result supports the assumption
REC (c = − 0.35, t = − 1.83, p = 0.07). Finally, the indirect of May and Pauli (2002) when interpreting their findings:
path of the effect of interest divergence on REC through the “It may be that the moral intensity—moral recognition
MI (a × b = − 0.22) was significant with a 99 percent confi- relation is contingent on the context involved.” (p. 109).
dence interval excluding zero (− 0.5141 to − 0.0262). We study the characteristics of the context involved. Our
Second, for the in-role behavior condition, we find results suggest that the nature of conflict of interest sales-
that direct interest divergence positively influences MI persons face does not elicit ethical sensitivity in the same
(a = 0.66, t = 4.58, p < 0.001). Second, controlling for inter- way depending on the role behavior they are engaged in. For
est divergence, we find that MI significantly influences REC situations in which salespersons engage in in-role behaviors,
(b = 0.41, t = 2.44, p < 0.05). Third, controlling for MI, we REC is stronger when interest divergence is direct than when
find that interest divergence significantly influences REC it is indirect. When salespeople engage in in-role behaviors
(c = 1.38, t = 5.31, p < 0.001). Finally, the indirect path of and face circumstances where interest divergence is direct,
the effect of interest divergence on REC through the MI they can choose to favor their self-interest whereas they
(a × b = 0.27) was significant with a 95 percent confidence are not likely to do so when interest divergence is indirect.
interval excluding zero (0.0371 to 0.5604). Thus, salespersons may perceive the situations where inter-
est divergence is direct as riskier than situations where inter-
est divergence is indirect. Indeed, the ethics codes of banks
and the ethical training banks provide to their salesforces
General Discussion probably emphasize and draw attention on in-role behav-
iors and situations where interest divergence is direct rather
In this section, we discuss the results, the contributions of than indirect because banks aim at reducing situations where
the research, its limitations, and conclude by offering sug- salespersons behave in a self-interested manner. This can
gestions for future studies. lead to a higher ethical sensitivity of salespersons in the
former situation.
Discussion of the Results Regarding situations in which salespeople engage in
extra-role behaviors, REC is stronger when interest diver-
Our goal is to enhance the understanding of sales EDM by gence is indirect than when it is direct. One explanation lies
testing the contingent nature of REC in sales situations. in the gap perceived by sales persons between the conse-
We find that REC depends both on MI and a combina- quences of their behavior for parties involved. Indeed, in
tion of two issue characteristics: role behavior and interest the qualitative study, the bank salespersons highlight that a
divergence. Our results are consistent with the literature. small amount of money does not impact the bank, whereas

13

358 E. Rousselet et al.

Table 6  The strength of the mediating effect of MI on the relation- customer-related situations where salespersons engage in-
ship between the combination of the two issue characteristics and role behaviors: they develop sales arguments (Honeycutt
REC
et al. 2001) and charge prices (Bellizzi and Hasty 2003).
In-role Extra-role
Direct Interest Divergence REC + REC - Therefore, our research draws attention to one type of behav-
Indirect Interest Divergence REC - REC+
ior that has not been previously considered.
Second, our research contributes to the sales ethics lit-
erature by exploring the relationship between MI and REC,
it is of great importance for poor people. Moreover, they do which has rarely been studied (McClaren 2013). More
not compare the consequences of their behavior for their importantly, by revealing two characteristics of sales situ-
customers and the consequences for themselves in situations ations whose combined effect on REC is mediated by MI,
with extra-role behaviors. Therefore, the gap between the our research contributes to the ethics literature on MI by
consequences for the parties involved is probably stronger confirming that the relationship between MI and REC is
in situations where interest divergence is indirect than in sit- contingent, as assumed by May and Pauli (2002) and Val-
uations where it is direct. Therefore, salespersons could pay entine and Hollingworth (2012).
more attention to the ethical issue in the former situations. Finally, our research identifies a way to differentiate sales
MI mediates the relationship between the combination activities from an ethical perspective, while sales activity is
of the two issue characteristics (role behavior and interest considered as a specific component of sales EDM models
divergence) and REC. This result attests that the two issue (Ferrell et al 2007; McClaren 2000). Therefore, it comple-
characteristics are not sufficient to model ethical issue recog- ments previous research on sales ethics. Moreover, from a
nition. MI has also an impact. Moreover, as shown in Table 6, practical point of view, it provides descriptions of the cir-
the strength of the mediating effect of MI (indicated by the cumstances where ethical issue recognition is important or
intensity of the gray shading) differs according the situation. not and therefore guidance to sales organizations. Despite
For situations with extra-role behaviors, the mediating effect these contributions, our research has some limitations, some
of MI is less important probably because it is not discussed of which present opportunities for future research.
in the code of conduct, whereas for situations with in-role
behaviors, the sellers have increased awareness, and MI is Limitations and Future Research
more important.
The limitations of this study are mainly methodological.
Contributions First, we did not include an actual measure of social desir-
ability as a control in the experimental conditions in study
First, our research extends empirical research on sales eth- 2. Because of our within-subject design, between-subject
ics (McClaren 2000, 2013) by revealing and using sales differences such as social desirability “cannot contribute
situations that are not considered in the empirical litera- to the within-subject error variance because these indi-
ture. Although researchers frequently use sales scenarios vidual differences affect all the repeated measures of a
including conflicts of interest, they primarily rely on cus- subject in the same way” (Csikszentmihalyi 2011, p. 288).
tomer-related situations where the interests of the customer However, future research may further explore the influence
conflict with the interests of the salespersons (Abratt and of within-subject social desirability to eliminate the risk
Penman 2002; Bellizzi and Hasty 2003; Barnett and Val- of social desirability biases. Second, given the cross-sec-
entine 2004; Boyle 2000; Deconinck 2005; Lee et al. 2009; tional research design of study 2, our research cannot dem-
Valentine and Barnett 2007; Schweper and Good 2007). Our onstrate causality among the variables. Thus, the results
research describes situations where the conflict of interest must be interpreted with caution. Third, we use the prob-
lies between the customer and the employer. Revealing and able magnitude of the consequences as a proxy of MI. It
emphasizing this type of conflict of interest in customer- is the first of the three factors that McMahon and Harvey
related situations is essential because how a firm reduces (2006) identify when studying the factor structure of MI.
it does not depend on the salespersons but on the capacity However, McMahon and Harvey also find two other factors
of the firm to better meet customers’ needs and ensure the and each consists of items related to one component that
long-term performance of its brand equity. Jones proposes: proximity (factor 2) and social consensus
Likewise, our research describes customer-related extra- (factor 3). We do not include these in our measure of MI.
role behaviors. Although role behavior is a valuable con- Not every empirical study includes all six components of
cept in the sales management literature (MacKenzie et al. MI (Tsalikis et al. 2008). However, future research should
1998), it has thus far been excluded from the ethics litera- test our hypotheses that measure MI based on these two
ture. By carefully reviewing the vignettes used in empirical other factors.
research on sales ethics, we note that they describe only

13
Moral Intensity, Issue Characteristics, and Ethical Issue Recognition in Sales Situations 359

Finally, the generalizability of our findings is limited for adopting a grounded research approach because qualitative
two reasons. The focus groups and the quantitative study research should better help understand the perceptions sales
were carried out using junior salespersons at banks. Our persons have of their role behaviors and the nature of the
sample had a seniority of 15 months when interviewed. conflict of interest they have to cope with. This is our second
Therefore, we can assume that they understand the situations recommendation for future research.
described in the study and have already encountered ethical MI is the first situational factor that was identified in the
dilemmas at work. Nevertheless, they are young. REC could ethics literature. Our study explores the relationship between
be influenced by age (Craft 2013; McClaren 2013; Lenhert MI and REC. Future research is also needed to explore the
et  al. 2015) and work experience (Lenhert et  al. 2015). relationship between MI and each of the other steps of
Therefore, our results may not be generalized to career bank EDM, including ethical judgement, ethical intent, and ethi-
salespersons. We encourage future empirical research to use cal behavior.
samples that include both senior and junior salespersons that
work in retail banks. More importantly, we chose to carry
out an extreme case (Yin 1994): data were obtained on bank Appendix A: Qualitative Research
salespersons who serve low-income customers. This context Methodology
allowed the study to consider some issues that could not
be observed in other circumstances. However, consistently Research Design
with study 1, this led us to consider only one type of cus-
tomer-related extra-role behavior, namely, helping custom- Our qualitative study combines both individual and group
ers. However, the literature mentions other such behaviors, interviews with grounded-theory building. The individual
including taking the initiative to improve service (Betten- interviews mainly focus on bank salespersons’ interactions
court et al. 1997, 2001) and recommending a customer’s with low-income customers and related situations. This
business to other customers (MacKenzie et al. 1998). We type of interview is appropriate for exploring a theme for
recommend that future research considers other sales situ- which there have been few empirical studies. We use focus
ations to strengthen the external validity of our findings: group interviews to explore role behavior and conflict of
circumstances where bank salespersons interact with mid- interest because these are sensitive issues. The focus group
dle- and high-income customers and circumstances in other method allows participants to feel more comfortable than
business activities where extra-role behaviors may be dif- they would in individual interviews because the questions
ferent from helping. This is the first suggestion for future are not specifically directed to a single person (Carey
research. 1994). This general scheme does not accurately reflect
This cannot be done without strengthening the under- the comprehensive goals and uses of individual and group
standing of the constructs of situational characteristics we interviews; these are detailed in the following Table 7.
identify in our research: role behavior and interest diver-
gence within seller–buyer interactions. We recommend

Table 7  Goals of the study, data obtained, and use of the data from the individual and group interviews
Individual interviews (21) Group interviews (2)

Goals Identify the main objectives and tasks of the inter- Understand how bank salespersons interpret their role
viewee behaviors and the possible conflicts of interest they
Identify and describe any circumstances where bank face when helping low-income customers
salespersons meet low-income customers and the
behaviors associated with these situation
Identify possible tensions within these circumstances
and their causes
Data
 Interview duration 54 h and 38 min 1 h and 40 min
 Transcribed verbatim material 937 pages of text 123 pages of text
Use of data Produce a list and description of low-income Identify the type of conflict of interest possibly
customer-related situations involved in situations identified in individual inter-
Improve our understanding of the organizational views
context of bank salespersons and their perceptions Identify the type of role behavior associated with situ-
of in-role behavior ations identified in individual interviews
Triangulate evidence from group interviews regarding Triangulate evidence from personal interviews regard-
role behavior and conflict of interest ing low-income customer-related situations

13

360 E. Rousselet et al.

Data Collection used for each of the six situations identified in the individual
interviews. The interviewer did not note domination by any-
Individual Interviews one who could have biased the discussion.
Both the individual and focus group interviews were
Twenty-one face-to-face semi-structured interviews were audiotaped, with the consent of the participants, and fully
conducted by the authors. All the participants were front- transcribed.
line employees of retail banks who were in direct contact
with low-income customers. The participants were cho-
sen to maximize structural variation (Glaser and Strauss Data Analysis
1967). We aimed to collect data from employees who rep-
resent the diversity of salespersons who work in French The data analysis was conducted separately by two of the
retail banks and their various positions within branches, four authors following a manual procedure.
which include the following: receptionists, customer advi-
sors, and branch managers. In their positions, the partic- Step 1: Coding Individual Interviews
ipants meet low-income customers when they have too
many incidents of non-payment or have credit problems. The analysis focused on two subjects: the low-income cus-
Data collection continued until redundancy was reached, tomer-related situations bank salespersons face and the per-
implying theoretical saturation (Glaser and Strauss 1967). ceptions they have of their goals and tasks.
The respondents were 24–50 years old, with an average The situations were defined as circumstances involving
age of 34 years. Nine participants were male and 12 were both a bank salesperson and a low-income customer regard-
female. At the beginning of the study, the participants less of who was at the origin of the interaction. While read-
were asked general questions about their bank, their pro- ing the selected data, the coders created a separate list of
fessional careers, and their present position. Then, the situations and reread them to complete the list. In addition,
interview guide reviewed the following topics: (1) the they noted a careful description of the situations, the parties
major missions and goals the interviewee had regarding directly or indirectly involved and the behavior of the inter-
low-income customers; (2) the day-to-day relationships viewees. Second, the authors had a discussion about the situ-
with low-income customers; (3) the perceptions of these ations. Third, they read the selected data again, combined
customers and their banking needs and attitudes; and (4) some of the situations to reduce their number, and assessed
the products and services the bank provides to these cus- whether the situations recurred in all the interviews. Finally,
tomers. The interviews were conducted by two of the four they discussed the results and produced a list of the situa-
authors of this study. tions, their descriptive details, and the various behaviors the
bank salespersons adopted.
Group Interviews Regarding the perceptions bank sales persons have of
their role and tasks, the coders created a list of the tasks each
Data were collected by means of two group interviews; there bank salespersons mentioned when they described their job
were four retail banking apprentices in each group. Appren- during the personal interviews. They compared this list from
tices are part-time employees. All of the participants have on interviewee to one another. They isolated any comment
been in direct contact with low-income customers from 12 regarding the representation interviewees had of their role
to 24 months in the main French retail banks. They can be and tasks. This material was used to triangulate data from
considered as junior employees and have an average age of focus group interviews.
22 years.
One of the authors conducted the interviews for both Focus Groups Interviews
groups following the same semi-structured framework. The
session began by asking the apprentices to describe their Data were examined by two of the four authors and ana-
relationships with low-income customers and the various lysed using the following research questions as a template:
circumstances in which they serve those customers. Then, Were the situations interpreted by interviewees as a con-
the interviewer presented one of the situations mentioned flict of interest? When this was the case, which parties had
in the individual interviews. The participants were asked conflicting interests? Finally, were the behaviors exhibited
which behavior their colleagues would adopt and the nature during these situations viewed as either in-role or extra-role
of these behaviors, referring to in-role behavior and extra- behaviors? What were the reasons for these perceptions? The
role behavior. They were also asked to reflect on the possible authors paid attention to both manifest and latent content
interpretation of the situation as a conflict of interest and (Bergh 2004).
to clarify which parties were in conflict. This pattern was

13
Moral Intensity, Issue Characteristics, and Ethical Issue Recognition in Sales Situations 361

Appendix B Cronbach’s alpha

Ethical climate: ethical peer behaviors (Lavorata 0.72


Scales, items, and reliability for moral intensity* by experimental con- 2007)
dition  In your company, customers are sometimes
encouraged to buy products they do not really
need
MAN SAL HEL OVE  In your company, sales personnel are primarily
evaluated on their results rather than on the
Magnitude of consequence** (MC) 0.58 0.72 0.58 0.67 quality of their work
 The negative consequences (if any) of  In your company, sales competitions push
the decision will be very serious (R) salespeople to sell products that are not always
 The overall harm (if any) as a result of suited to their customers’ needs
the decision will be very small
Probability of effect** (PE) 0.33 0.72 0.59 0.68
 There is a very small likelihood that the
decision will actually cause any harm
 The decision is likely to cause harm (R)
References
Temporal immediacy** (TI) 0.47 0.68 0.51 0.50
Abratt, R., & Penman, N. (2002). Understanding factors affecting sales-
 The decision will not cause any harm in
people’s perceptions of ethical behavior in South Africa. Journal
the immediate future
of Business Ethics, 35(4), 269–280.
 The negative effects (if any) of the deci-
Adams, J. S. (1963). Towards an understanding of inequity. The Jour-
sion will be felt very quickly (R)
nal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67(5), 422–463.
Moral intensity*** 0.74 0.86 0.74 0.78 Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
(MI* = MC + PE + TI) Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 267–299). New
York: Academic Press.
*Captured by the three dimensions of the probable magnitude of the Agnihotri, R., Rapp, A., Kothandaraman, P., & Singh, R. K. (2012). An
consequences emotion-based model of salesperson ethical behaviors. Journal of
**Two items: Spearman–Brown statistic Business Ethics, 109(2), 243–257.
Barnett, T. (2001). Dimensions of moral intensity and ethical decision
***More than two items: Cronbach’s alpha
making: An empirical study. Journal of Applied Social Psychol-
ogy, 31(5), 1038–1057.
Barnett, T., & Valentine, S. (2004). Issue contingencies and marketers’
recognition of ethical issues, ethical judgments and behavioral
Appendix C intentions. Journal of Business Research, 57, 338–346.
Bateman, C. R., Valentine, S., & Rittenburg, T. (2013). Ethical deci-
sion making in a peer-to-peer file sharing situation: The role
Scales, items, and reliability for control variables of moral absolutes and social consensus. Journal of Business
Ethics, 115(2), 229–240.
Beauchamp, T. L., & Bowie, N. E. (1979). Ethical theory and busi-
Cronbach’s alpha ness. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.
Bellizzi, J. A., & Hasty, R. W. (2003). Supervising unethical sales
Ethical predispositions: Formalism (Brady and 0.73 force behavior: How strong is the tendency to treat top sales per-
Wheeler 1996) formers leniently? Journal of Business Ethics, 43(4), 337–351.
 Trustworthy Berg, B. L. (2004). Qualitative research methods for the social sci-
 Honest ences (5th edn.). Boston: Pearson Education Inc.
 Good-intentioned Bettencourt, L. A., & Brown, S. W. (1997). Contact employees:
Ethical predispositions: Utilitarism (Brady and 0.57 Relationships among workplace fairness, job satisfaction and
Wheeler 1996) prosocial service behaviors. Journal of Retailing, 73(1), 39–61.
 Influential Bettencourt, L. A., Gwinner, K. P., & Meuter, M. L. (2001). A com-
 Result-oriented parison of attitude, personality, and knowledge predictors of
 Ambitious service-oriented organizational citizenship behaviors. Journal
Ethical climate: procedures within the company 0.86 of Applied Psychology, 86(1), 29–41.
(Lavorata 2007) Blocker, C. P., Ruth, J. A., Sridharan, S., Beckwith, C., Ekici, A.,
 The general management of your company has Goudie-Hutton, M., & Varman, R. (2013). Understanding pov-
made it clear that non-ethical behavior will not erty and promoting poverty alleviation through transforma-
be tolerated tive consumer research. Journal of Business Research, 66(8),
 Salespersons acting unethically in the com- 1195–1202.
pany’s interest are sanctioned
 Salespersons acting unethically in their own
interest are sanctioned

13

362 E. Rousselet et al.

Boyle, B. A. (2000). The Impact of customer characteristics and Hill, R. P. (2008). Disadvantaged consumers: An ethical approach
moral philosophies on ethical judgments of salespeople. Journal to consumption by the poor. Journal of Business Ethics, 80(1),
of Business Ethics, 23(3), 249–267. 77–83.
Brady, F. N., & Wheeler, G. E. (1996). An empirical study of ethical Hoffman, B. J., Blair, C. A., Meriac, J. P., & Woehr, D. J. (2007).
predispositions. Journal of Business Ethics, 15(9), 927–940. Expanding the criterion domain? A quantitative review of the
Brenkert, G. G. (1998). Marketing and the vulnerable. Business Eth- OCB literature. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(2), 555–566.
ics Quarterly, 1, 7–20. Hollingworth, D., & Valentine, S. (2015). The moderating effect of per-
Brief, A. P., & Motowidlo, S. J. (1986). Prosocial organizational ceived organizational ethical context on employees’ ethical issue
behaviors. Academy of Management Review, 11(4), 710–725. recognition and ethical judgments. Journal of Business Ethics,
Bush, V., Bush, A. J., Oakley, J., & Cicala, J. E. (2017). The sales 128(2), 457–466.
profession as a subculture: Implications for ethical decision Honeycutt, E. D., Glassman, M., Zugelder, M. T., & Karande, K.
making. Journal of Business Ethics, 142(3), 549–565. (2001). Determinants of ethical behavior: A study of autosales-
Butterfield, K. D., Trevin, L. K., & Weaver, G. R. (2000). Moral people. Journal of Business Ethics, 32(1), 69–79.
awareness in business organizations: Influences of issue-related Hunt, S. D., & Vitell, S. (1986). A general theory of marketing ethics.
and social context factors. Human Relations, 53(7), 981–1018. Journal of Macromarketing, 6(1), 5–16.
Carey, M. A. (1994). The group effect in focus groups: Planning, Jones, T. M. (1991). Ethical decision making by individuals in organi-
implementing, and interpreting focus group research. In J. zations: An issue-contingent model. Academy of Management
Morse (Ed.), Critical issues in qualitative research methods Review, 16, 366–395.
(pp. 225–241). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Judd, C. M., Kenny, D. A., & McClelland, G. H. (2001). Estimating
Cherry, J., & Fraedrich, J. (2002). Perceived risk, moral philosophy and testing mediation and moderation in within-subjects designs.
and marketing ethics: mediating influences on sales managers’ Psychological Methods, 6, 115–134.
ethical decision-making. Journal of Business Research, 55(12), Karriker, J. H., & Williams, M. L. (2009). Organizational justice and
951–962. organizational citizenship behavior: A mediated multifoci model.
Chonko, L. B., & Hunt, S. D. (1985). Ethics and marketing manage- Journal of Management, 35(1), 112–135.
ment: An empirical examination. Journal of Business Research, Katz, D. (1964). The motivational basis of organizational behavior.
13(4), 339–359. Systems Research and Behavioral Science, 9(2), 131–146.
Converse, J. M., & Presser, S. (1986). Survey questions: Handcraft- Kellaris, J. J., & Dabholkar, P. A. (1989). The PSE scale: A scenario-
ing the standardized questionnaire (Vol. 63). Thousands Oaks, based approach to assessing the ethical sensitivity of sales stu-
CA: Sage. dents and professionals. Journal of Personal Selling and Sales
Craft, J. L. (2013). A review of the empirical ethical decision-mak- Management, 9, 60–70.
ing literature: 2004–2011. Journal of Business Ethics, 117(2), Khavul, S., & Bruton, G. D. (2013). Harnessing innovation for change:
221–259. Sustainability and poverty in developing countries. Journal of
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2011). Handbook of research methods for study- Management Studies, 50(2), 285–306.
ing daily life. New York: Guilford Press. Kim, W. C., & Mauborgne, R. A. (1996). Procedural justice and man-
Dabholkar, P. A., & Kellaris, J. J. (1992). Toward understanding mar- agers’ in-role and extra-role behavior: The case of the multina-
keting students’ ethical judgment of controversial personal selling tional. Management Science, 42(4), 499–515.
practices. Journal of Business Research, 24(4), 313–329. Lavorata, L. (2007). Proposal for a measurement scale of the ethical cli-
Deconinck, J. B. (2005). The influence of ethical control systems and mate in business: A study in the area of B-to-B selling. Recherche
moral intensity on sales managers’ethical perceptions and behav- et Applications en Marketing (English Edition), 22(1), 1–23.
ioral intentions. Marketing Management Journal, 15(2), 123–131. Lee, N., Beatson, A., Garrett, T. C., Lings, I., & Zhang, X. (2009). A
Dubinsky, A. J., Howell, R. D., Ingram, T. N., & Bellenger, D. N. study of the attitudes towards unethical selling amongst Chinese
(1986). Salesforce socialization. The Journal of Marketing, 50, salespeople. Journal of Business Ethics, 88, 497–515.
192–207. Lehnert, K., Park, Y. H., & Singh, N. (2015). Research note and
Ekström, K. M., & Hjort, T. (2009). Hidden consumers in marketing: review of the empirical ethical decision-making literature:
The neglect of consumers with scarce resources in affluent socie- Boundary conditions and extensions. Journal of Business Eth-
ties. Journal of Marketing Management, 25(7–8), 697–712. ics, 129(1), 195–219.
Ferrell, O. C., & Gresham, L. G. (1985). A contingency framework for Leitsch, D. L. (2004). Differences in the perceptions of moral inten-
understanding ethical decision making in marketing. The Journal sity in the moral decision process: An empirical examination
of Marketing, 49, 87–96. of accounting students. Journal of Business Ethics, 53(3),
Ferrell, O. C., Johnston, M. W., & Ferrell, L. (2007). A framework 313–323.
for personal selling and sales management ethical decision mak- Levy, M., & Dubinsky, A. J. (1983). Identifying and addressing retail
ing. Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management, 27(4), salespeople’s ethical problems: A method and application. Journal
291–299. of Retailing, 59(1), 46–66.
Gioia, D. A., & Poole, P. P. (1984). Scripts in organizational behavior. Lewis, P. (2014). The over-training of apprentices by employers in
Academy of Management Review, 9(3), 449–459. advanced manufacturing: A theoretical and policy analysis.
Glaser, B., & Strauss, A. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory. Human Resource Management Journal, 24(4), 496–513.
Hawthorne, NY: Aldine Publishing Company. MacKenzie, S. B., Podsakoff, P. M., & Ahearne, M. (1998). Some
Greenberg, J., & Eskew, D. E. (1993). The role of role playing in possible antecedents and consequences of in-role and extra-role
organizational research. Journal of Management, 19(2), 221–241. salesperson performance. The Journal of Marketing, 87–98.
Hansen, J. D., & Riggle, R. J. (2009). Ethical salesperson behavior in MacKenzie, S. B., Podsakoff, P. M., & Fetter, R. (1991). Organizational
sales relationships. Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Manage- citizenship behavior and objective productivity as determinants of
ment, 29(2), 151–166. managerial evaluations of salespersons’ performance. Organiza-
Hill, R. P. (2002). Stalking the poverty consumer a retrospective exami- tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50(1), 123–150.
nation of modern ethical dilemmas. Journal of Business Ethics, May, D. R., & Pauli, K. P. (2002). The role of moral intensity in ethical
37(2), 209–219. decision making: A review and investigation of moral recognition,
evaluation, and intention. Business & Society, 41(1), 84–117.

13
Moral Intensity, Issue Characteristics, and Ethical Issue Recognition in Sales Situations 363

McClaren, N. (2000). Ethics in personal selling and sales management: Singhapakdi, A., Vitell, S. J., & Kraft, K. L. (1996). Moral intensity
A review of the literature focusing on empirical findings and con- and ethical decision-making of marketing professionals. Journal
ceptual foundations. Journal of Business Ethics, 27(3), 285–303. of Business Research, 36(3), 245–255.
McClaren, N. (2013). The personal selling and sales management eth- Sweeney, B., & Costello, F. (2009). Moral intensity and ethical
ics research: Managerial implications and research directions from decision-making: An empirical examination of undergraduate
a comprehensive review of the empirical literature. Journal of accounting and business students. Accounting Education, 18(1),
Business Ethics, 112(1), 101–125. 75–97.
McMahon, J. M., & Harvey, R. J. (2006). An analysis of the factor Talukdar, D. (2008). Cost of being poor: Retail price and consumer
structure of Jones’ moral intensity construct. Journal of Business price search differences across inner-city and suburban neighbor-
Ethics, 64(4), 381–404. hoods. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(3), 457–471.
Montoya, A. K., & Hayes, A. F. (2017). Two condition within-partic- Tang, T. L. P., Sutarso, T., Davis, G. M. T. W., Dolinski, D., Ibrahim,
ipant statistical mediation analysis: A path-analytic framework. A. H. S., & Wagner, S. L. (2008). To help or not to help? The
Psychological Methods, 22(1), 6–27. Good Samaritan Effect and the love of money on helping behavior.
Morris, S. A., & McDonald, R. A. (1995). The role of moral intensity Journal of Business Ethics, 82(4), 865–887.
in moral judgments: An empirical investigation. Journal of Busi- Trevino, L. K. (1986). Ethical decision making in organizations: A
ness Ethics, 49(11), 1367–1393. person-situation interactionist model. Academy of Management
Morrison, E. W. (1994). Role definitions and organizational citizenship Review, 11(3), 601–617.
behavior: The importance of the employee’s perspective. Academy Trevino, L. K. (1992). The social effects of punishment in organiza-
of Management Journal, 37(6), 1543–1567. tions: A justice perspective. Academy of Management Review,
Mossholder, K. W., Richardson, H. A., & Settoon, R. P. (2011). Human 17(4), 647–676.
resource systems and helping in organizations: A relational per- Tsalikis, J., Seaton, B., & Shepherd, P. (2008). Relative importance
spective. Academy of Management Review, 36(1), 33. measurement of the moral intensity dimensions. Journal of Busi-
O’Fallon, M. J., & Butterfield, K. D. (2005). A review of the empirical ness Ethics, 80(3), 613–626.
ethical decision-making literature: 1996–2003. Journal of Busi- Valentine, S. (2009). Ethics training, ethical context, and sales and mar-
ness Ethics, 59(4), 375–413. keting professionals’ satisfaction with supervisors and coworkers.
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Paine, J. B., & Bachrach, D. G. Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management, 29(3), 227–242.
(2000). Organizational citizenship behaviors: A critical review of Valentine, S., & Barnett, T. (2007). Perceived organizational ethics and
the theoretical and empirical literature and suggestions for future the ethical decisions of sales and marketing personnel. Journal of
research. Journal of Management, 26(3), 513–563. Personal Selling & Sales Management, 27(4), 373–388.
Podsakoff, P. M., & Organ, D. W. (1986). Self-reports in organizational Valentine, S., & Hollingworth, D. (2012). Moral intensity, issue impor-
research: Problems and prospects. Journal of Management, 12(4), tance, and ethical reasoning in operations situations. Journal of
531–544. Business Ethics, 108(4), 509–523.
Poujol, F. J., Harfouche, A., & Pezet, E. (2016). Salespeople’s unethi- Valentine, S. R., & Bateman, C. R. (2011). The impact of ethical ide-
cal behavior during a sales contest: The mediation effect of the ologies, moral intensity, and social context on sales-based ethical
perceived ethical climate of the game. Recherche et Applications reasoning. Journal of Business Ethics, 102(1), 155–168.
en Marketing (English Edition), 31(2), 21–39. Van Dyne, L., & LePine, J. A. (1998). Helping and voice extra-role
Rest, J. R. (1986). Moral development: Advances in research and behaviors: Evidence of construct and predictive validity. Academy
theory. New York: Praeger. of Management journal, 41(1), 108–119.
Reynolds, S. J. (2006). Moral awareness and ethical predispositions: VanSandt, C. V., Shepard, J. M., & Zappe, S. M. (2006). An examina-
Investigating the role of individual differences in the recognition tion of the relationship between ethical work climate and moral
of moral issues. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(1), 233–243. awareness. Journal of Business Ethics, 68(4), 409–432.
Robertson, D. C., & Anderson, E. (1993). Control system and task Vargo, S. L., & Lusch, R. F. (2004). Evolving to a new dominant logic
environment effects on ethical judgment: An exploratory study for marketing. Journal of Marketing, 68(1), 1–17.
of industrial salespeople. Organization Science, 4(4), 617–644. Vey, M. A., & Campbell, J. P. (2004). In-role or extra-role organi-
Schepers, J., Falk, T., Ruyter, K. D., Jong, A. D., & Hammerschmidt, zational citizenship behavior: Which are we measuring? Human
M. (2012). Principles and principals: Do customer stewardship Performance, 17(1), 119–135.
and agency control compete or complement when shaping front- Victor, B., & Cullen, J. B. (1988). The organizational bases of ethical
line employee behavior? Journal of Marketing, 76(6), 1–20. work climates. Administrative Science Quarterly, 33, 101–125.
Schwartz, M. S. (2016). Ethical decision-making theory: An integrated Williams, L. J., & Anderson, S. E. (1991). Job satisfaction and organi-
approach. Journal of Business Ethics, 139(4), 755–776. zational commitment as predictors of organizational citizenship
Schwepker, C. H., Ferrell, O. C., & Ingram, T. N. (1997). The influence and in-role behaviors. Journal of Management, 17(3), 601–617.
of ethical climate and ethical conflict on role stress in the sales Wimbush, J. C., & Shepard, J. M. (1994). Toward an understanding of
force. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 25(2), 99. ethical climate: Its relationship to ethical behavior and supervi-
Sharma, A., Levy, M., & Evanschitzky, H. (2007). The variance in sory influence. Journal of Business Ethics, 13(8), 637–647.
sales performance explained by the knowledge structures of sales- Wotruba, T. R. (1990). A comprehensive framework for the analysis of
people. Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management, 27(2), ethical behavior, with a focus on sales organizations. Journal of
169–181. Personal Selling & Sales Management, 10(2), 29–42.
Singhapakdi, A., Vitell, S. J., & Franke, G. R. (1999). Antecedents, Yin, R. K. (1994). Case study research: Design and methods, applied
consequences, and mediating effects of perceived moral intensity social research methods series. Biography, 5. London: Sage
and personal moral philosophies. Journal of the Academy of Mar- Publications.
keting Science, 27(1), 19–36.

13

You might also like