You are on page 1of 10

Early Childhood Research Quarterly 55 (2021) 242–251

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Early Childhood Research Quarterly

Preschool teachers’ self-efficacy, classroom process quality, and


children’s social skills: A multilevel mediation analysis
Bi Ying Hu a , Yuanhua Li a , Chuang Wang a , Huiping Wu b,∗ , Ginny Vitiello c
a
Faculty of Education, University of Macau, People’s Republic of China
b
Fujian Normal University, People’s Republic of China
c
University of Virginia, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This study examined the interconnections among preschool teachers’ self-efficacy, classroom process
Received 4 May 2020 quality and children’s social skill development based on a randomly selected sample of 5628 preschool
Received in revised form 5 December 2020 children in a Chinese preschool context. Given the nested data structure, multilevel mediation modeling
Accepted 9 December 2020
was utilized to investigate whether teachers’ self-efficacy, specifically teachers’ self-efficacy for instruc-
Available online 31 December 2020
tional strategies (EIS), classroom management (ECM) and student engagement (ESE), exerted indirect
effects on children’s social skills through the mediation of classroom process quality measured by Pianta,
Keywords:
Hamre and LaParo’s (2008) Classroom Assessment Scoring System (CLASS) in three domains—emotional
Teachers’ self-efficacy
Classroom process
support (ES), classroom organization (CO) and instructional support (IS). Findings showed that CO is the
Social skills only robust mediating mechanism between teachers’ self-efficacy and children’s social skills, indicating
Chinese preschools the importance of teachers’ CO in developing Chinese children’s social skills. Specifically, teachers’ CO
acts as a positive mediator between teachers’ ECM and children’s social skills, but a negative media-
tor between teachers’ ESE and children’s social skills. This interesting negative mediation of teachers’ CO
between teachers’ ESE and children’s social skills offers insights into Chinese preschool classroom dynam-
ics and child development. Findings and their implications are discussed in light of Chinese sociocultural
features.
© 2020 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction fostered in alignment with the mainstream principles of collec-


tivism, Confucianism, and group cohesiveness (Chen, Cen, Li, &
Social skills include a range of individuals’ competencies, He, 2005; Pearson & Rao, 2003). Preschoolers are expected to
including emotion regulation, the ability to construct and main- learn social skills that benefit whole-group harmony (Chen et al.,
tain relationships, effortful control, and executive functioning 2005). In classrooms, children are supposed to behave in confor-
(McClelland et al., 2007), which begin to appear in the second mity with prosocial values, act disciplined, be obedient to teachers,
year of children’s lives (Jeon, McCartney, Richard, & Johnson, 2020). and show good manners toward others (Chen & French, 2008; Zhou,
Many kindergarten teachers believe that social skills are more Eisenberg, Wang, & Reiser, 2004). Children who do not develop
important than academic skills for young children (Hustedt, Buell, these skills are viewed as less competent by teachers and peers,
Hallam, & Pinder, 2018). Long-term effects of children’s early displaying greater adjustment difficulties in later childhood (Chen,
social skills on later socioemotional functioning, academics, phys- Chen, Li, & Wang, 2009).
ical and mental health have been reported in previous studies With the spread of China’s two-child policy, coupled with high
(Berhenke, Miller, Brown, Seifer, & Dickstein, 2011; Djambazova- maternal employment, Chinese society is poised for an expanded
Popordanoska, 2016; Housman, 2017; Jeon et al., 2020). birth rate and more children entering the early childhood system
Children’s social skills are particularly important educational (Hu, Fan, Wu, LoCasale-Crouch, & Song, 2019). This is leading to
outcomes in Chinese society. In China, children’s social skills are higher expectations for Early Childhood Education (ECE) quality.
Accordingly, the central government has issued several national
plans to reform ECE by improving program quality (Ministry of
Education of People’s Republic of China [MOE], 2018). As China
∗ Corresponding author at: College of Mathematics and Informatics, Fujian Normal seeks to increase ECE quality, it is critical to understand how teacher
University, University Town, Fuzhou, Fujian Province, People’s Republic of China.
E-mail address: hpwu168@163.com (H. Wu).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2020.12.001
0885-2006/© 2020 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
B.Y. Hu et al. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 55 (2021) 242–251

and classroom characteristics are associated with the development preschool teachers tend to encourage children’s orderly but passive
of social skills among Chinese preschoolers. engagement in well-planned activities, with few considerations of
Extant research findings have demonstrated that quality ECE children’s personal interests, choices or ideas (Rao, Ng, & Pearson,
positively affects children’s cognitive (Barnett & Frede, 2017; 2010). These special Chinese contextual features may be relevant
Hatfield, Burchinal, Pianta, & Sideris, 2016) and social achievement to the current investigation into the relations among teachers’ self-
(Aguitar, Aguitar, Cadima, Correia, & Fialho, 2019; Broekhuizen, efficacy, classroom process, and children’s social skills.
Mokrova, Burchinal, Garrett-Peters, & Family Life Project Key
Investigators, 2016) from both short-term and long-term perspec- 2.1. Teachers’ self-efficacy
tives. It is widely accepted in the ECE field that quality can be
divided into two components, structural and process quality. Pro- Teachers’ self-efficacy is defined as “teachers’ beliefs of their
cess quality refers to the quality mainly reflected in teacher–child abilities of planning and practicing courses necessary for complet-
and peer interactions during academic and social exchanges (Hu ing specific teaching tasks in certain subjects” (Tschannen-Moran,
et al., 2017). Empirical studies in both Western and Chinese Hoy, & Hoy, 1998, p. 207). For years, teachers’ self-efficacy has been
preschool contexts similarly found that ECE process quality pos- regarded as one of the fundamental psychological resources for
itively associates with children’s social and academic performance teachers’ classroom practice (Klassen, Tze, Betts, & Gordon, 2011).
(Hatfield et al., 2016; Hu et al., 2017; Hu, Fan, Gu, & Yang, 2016; If teachers hold consistent high-level self-efficacy, they are more
Pianta, Downer, & Hamre, 2016;). likely to provide students with sensitive support, inspiring stu-
Teachers’ personal characteristics, such as self-efficacy, are dents’ cognitive processing and engagement in classroom activities
important determinants for ECE process quality reflected in class- (Zee & Koomen, 2016).
room interactions (Pianta, La Paro, Payne, Cox, & Bradley, 2002). In the current study, Zee and Koomen’s (2016) theoretical
Teachers’ self-efficacy “sets the tone” for high-quality classroom framework of the Heuristic model of teacher self-efficacy was adopted
processes (Zee & Koomen, 2016, p. 981). It has been suggested to guide construction of the hierarchical mediation model. This
that teachers’ self-efficacy might influence student-level outcomes, framework suggests that teachers’ self-efficacy predicts variances
including academic achievement, learning motivation and socio- in classroom processes (Woolfolk Hoy, Hoy, & Davis, 2009). Specif-
emotional competence (Caprara, Barbaranelli, Steca, & Malone, ically, according to the Heuristic model, teachers’ self-efficacy is
2006), although only a few studies have examined this to date (Guo, hypothesized to influence their classroom interactions with chil-
Piasta, Justice, & Kaderavek, 2010; Guo, Connor, Yang, Roehrig, & dren through three major domains, including emotional support,
Morrison, 2012). classroom organization, and instructional support (CLASS-PreK;
Despite the fact that researchers have proposed the poten- Pianta, La Paro, & Hamre, 2008). Teachers with higher self-efficacy
tial for teachers’ self-efficacy to explain student-level outcome feel more confident that they can support children’s development
variance, the mechanisms that bridge the association between through their interactions with children, leading them to engage
teachers’ self-efficacy and student outcomes has scarcely been in more positive, supportive, and sensitive interactions (Barni,
studied (Zee & Koomen, 2016). To explore this aspect of teach- Danioni, & Benevene, 2019). In turn, these classroom interactional
ers’ self-efficacy, the current study aims to investigate the features influence students’ classroom adjustment, including the
triadic relations among preschool teachers’ self-efficacy, classroom development of social skills (Woolfolk Hoy et al., 2009; Zee &
processes, and children’s social skills. In this way, we investi- Koomen, 2016).
gate a key mechanism—classroom processes (mainly reflected
in teacher-child interactions)—through which preschool teachers’
2.2. Teacher–child interactions
self-efficacy may be leveraged to improve children’s social skills.
Importantly, the current study makes contributions to both the-
Classroom processes are identified as interactional patterns
ory and practice. Theoretically, this work will help develop a fuller
between teachers and students that are influential for students’
understanding of significant classroom process mechanisms con-
outcomes (Mashburn et al., 2008). We adopted the Teaching through
necting teachers’ self-efficacy and children’s social outcomes. In
Interactions framework to guide our conceptualization of impor-
practice, we propose ways for teachers to enhance teacher–child
tant aspects of classroom process quality (Hamre et al., 2013).
interactions and to promote children’s social skill within diverse
This framework concentrates on teachers’ classroom interactional
sociocultural backgrounds.
behaviors in three domains: Emotional Support, Classroom Organi-
zation, and Instructional Support. Students who have experienced
high-quality interactions tend to show better social skills (Hu et al.,
2. Theoretical background
2017; Mashburn et al., 2008; Siekkinen et al., 2013).
In general, children’s social skills are reflected in their acceptable
and effective social interactions with others (Pearson & Rao, 2003). 2.3. Connections between teachers’ self-efficacy and classroom
Based on this socioecological perspective of child social develop- teacher–child interaction
ment, Chen and French (2008) proposed a theoretical framework
suggesting that children’s participation in social interactions, the A positive relation exists between teachers’ self-efficacy and
social responses and evaluations they receive, as well as their social their teaching practices. Teachers’ self-efficacy longitudinally pre-
development are guided by the customs and norms highly val- dicts classroom quality (Holzberger, Philipp, & Kunter, 2013;
ued within their society. This makes it important to explore how Künsting, Neuber, & Lipowsky, 2016). A number of studies have
preschool classroom experiences are associated with social devel- linked positive self-efficacy beliefs to teachers’ provision of warm
opment in diverse sociocultural contexts, like China, that display and safe classroom climate, decreased use of coercive discipline,
more collectivist values. greater reliance on positive behavior management techniques, and
China’s group-oriented social norms lead teachers to promote use of more innovative and effective instructional strategies (Guo
obedience to authority, group well-being, and polite manners et al., 2010; Hascher & Hagenauer, 2016; Künsting et al., 2016;
over individualism. Thus, teachers mostly organize highly regi- Shoulders & Krei, 2015). In sum, past findings support the idea
mented classroom activities, exhibiting comparatively high-level that teachers’ self-efficacy is related to teachers’ emotional support,
control and order (Zhou, Lam, & Chan, 2012). Traditional Chinese classroom organization, and instructional support.

243
B.Y. Hu et al. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 55 (2021) 242–251

2.4. Classroom teacher–child interaction and children’s social nization, and instructional support. Specially, we attempt to answer
skills the following research question in present study: Do teacher–child
interactions mediate the relation between teachers’ self-efficacy
Evidence shows that classroom teacher-child interaction has and children’s social skills?
close associations with students’ social outcomes. Mashburn et al.
(2008) concluded from a study of 671 pre-K classrooms that teach-
3. Method
ers’ classroom emotional support was positively associated with
children’s social skills. Moreover, Curby, Brock, and Hamre (2013)
3.1. Participants
demonstrated that the consistency of teachers’ emotional support
across time significantly predicts children’s better gains in social
Participants included 5628 preschoolers (2929 girls and 2699
skills. In summary, teachers’ consistent warmth and sensitivity
boys) and their lead teachers recruited from 180 classrooms in
make children feel more confident in their capabilities of sensing
Guangdong, the most populous province in China and one with
others’ emotions, which, in turn, result in children’s better social
striking socioeconomic disparities. A stratified random sampling
skills (Curby et al., 2013).
approach was used to select participants. First, three municipali-
Teachers’ effective classroom organization and proactive disci-
ties were selected based on the levels of economic development:
pline can help students acquire a sense of responsibility and learn
advanced, average, and below average, as classified by the Guang-
how to foster self-control (Gordon, 2001). Based on 40 years of
dong provincial government. Second, in each municipality, 20
research, Skiba, Ormiston, Martinez, and Cummings (2016) con-
schools were randomly selected from a list of preschools pro-
cluded teachers’ strong classroom organization helps students
vided by the local education agency. Third, one class was randomly
focus on their tasks and decrease their disruptive behaviors. In
selected from each school and each grade level (K1, K2, and K3).
a meta-analysis, Korpershoek et al. (2016) found a significantly
Finally, all children in the classes investigated were invited to par-
positive but small association between teachers’ classroom man-
ticipate in the study, and 98% of children’s parents signed the
agement and children’s social development. Empirically, evidence
consent forms. The final sample included 1582 children in K1 (aged
supports the idea that students’ self-regulatory and social skills
3–4), 1894 in K2 (aged 4–5), and 2152 in K3 (aged 5–6).
are linked to teachers’ efforts to optimize classroom organizational
The number of participating children per class ranged from 7 to
climates (Yerdelen & Sungur, 2019).
58 (M = 31.27, SD = 8.34). Due to the shortage of teachers or low
Only a few studies have exclusively examined the power of
enrollment of students in some rural areas, the class size varied
teachers’ instructional support in predicting children’s social out-
substantially among the participating classrooms. All of the lead
comes. Past research suggested that children in classrooms led
teachers in the 180 participating classrooms signed consent forms
by teachers who display higher levels of instructional support
for video recording and filled out the questionnaire that focused on
showed greater empathy and less disruptive behaviors in class-
demographic information and teaching efficacy. Teachers were all
rooms (Siekkinen et al., 2013). Teachers’ instructional support, as
female, aged 18–58 years old (M = 31.62, SD = 7.33) with an average
an aspect of teacher–child interactions, may also have relevance to
of 10.32 years of teaching experience (SD = 6.92, Min = 1, Max = 30).
children’s social outcomes (Howes et al., 2008).
The majority of these teachers (77.2%) had obtained an associate’s
degree or higher through continuing education, majoring in ECE or
2.5. Teacher–child interactions as a mediator
related fields such as elementary education or art education.
Given evidence supporting the association between teachers’
self-efficacy and children’s social outcomes (Zee & Koomen, 2016), 3.2. Procedure
many researchers have proposed a more nuanced mechanism to
help understand through which paths teachers’ self-efficacy might The study was conducted in May and June of 2014 when chil-
be associate with variations in students’ outcomes. As conceptual- dren were at the end of the spring semester. Before the data were
ized in the Heuristic Model of Teachers’ Self-efficacy (Zee & Koomen, collected, the research project went through an ethics review at
2016), teacher–child interactions function as the bridge between the first author’s university. Sixteen graduate assistants majoring
teachers’ self-efficacy and classroom-level child outcomes. Empir- in educational psychology were trained to assist with data collec-
ical findings are limited in terms of this proposed cross-level tion. To become certified observers on the Classroom Assessment
mediation. Until now, only Guo et al. (2012) has affirmed that Scoring System (CLASS; Pianta et al., 2008), the study’s principal
highly efficacious teachers tended to create positive interactional observation tool, they underwent four days of training and passed
environments in classrooms, which were, in turn, associated with an online reliability test offered by Teachstone. These certified
children’s better performance in literacy. CLASS observers were subsequently dispatched to videotape the
Upon reviewing relevant literature, we conclude that several selected classrooms between May and June 2014. The assigned
gaps exist in the research concerning the mechanism through CLASS observer extracted five, randomly-selected, 20-min obser-
which teachers’ self-efficacy beliefs connect with children’s social vation cycles from each classroom video of approximately 3 hrs,
development. First, prior studies have not provided direct tests covering the major daily routines: whole-group teaching, outdoor
of whether teachers’ self-efficacy is associated with children’s play, center play activities, and routine care/activities. Following
social outcomes through teacher–child interactions. Second, a Curby, Grimm, and Pianta’s (2010) approach, we estimated the sta-
similar mediating mechanism has yet to be examined in ECE con- bility using autoregressive models and found that the quality of
texts and in a collectivism-based society, like China. Furthermore, classroom interaction was highly stable during the three hours of
multilevel methodologies have not been widely incorporated in a day.
similar analyses to explore whether the variance among differ- Two qualified CLASS raters independently observed and coded
ent classroom-level variables can explain student-level outcome each 20-min video of the observation cycle so that all cycles were
variability. Therefore, the current study is designed to fulfill the the- double-coded. After observing and scoring five cycles from one
oretical, contextual and methodological gaps in previous research classroom, the raters compared their ratings to see whether the
by investigating the indirect effect of teachers’ self-efficacy on ratings were within a 1-point difference on the scale of 1–7. If not,
children’s social outcomes, mediated by teacher–child interactions the raters had to repeat the cycle until they reached the accept-
measured in terms of teachers’ emotional support, classroom orga- able level of inter-rater consistency. As each classroom video was

244
B.Y. Hu et al. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 55 (2021) 242–251

coded by two graduate students, we calculated intraclass corre- a 7-point Likert scale, defined to include low (1, 2), middle (3, 4,
lation coefficients (ICCs) on the 10 dimensions of the CLASS to 5), and high (6, 7) range levels. The cross-cultural validity of the
examine interrater consistency. More specifically, the ICCs were CLASS has been reported in many countries (Araujo, Carneiro, Cruz-
obtained from a two-way mixed-effect model for absolute agree- Aguayo, & Schady, 2016; Hamre, Hatfield, Pianta, & Jamil, 2014; La
ment (McGraw & Wong, 1996). The average interrater reliability Paro, Pianta, & Stuhlman, 2004; Leyva et al., 2015; Von Suchodoletz,
indexed by ICC was .89 and the standard deviation was .08 (Hu Fäsche, Gunzenhauser, & Hamre, 2014). A recent validation study
et al., 2016), suggesting a high degree of interrater consistency in conducted in China also supported the three-domain structure in
the CLASS ratings. the Chinese preschool context (Hu et al., 2016). The Cronbach’s ␣s
Consent forms were obtained from parents before delivering were .78, .84, and .92 for ES, CO and INS, respectively, suggesting a
questionnaires to families’ homes. The parent survey included good to excellent internal consistency reliability (Hu et al., 2016).
questions about demographic information and assessment of chil-
dren’s social skills and problem behaviors. After completing the 3.3.4. Covariates: Teacher and classroom demographic
surveys, parents were asked to return the completed packet to information
the classroom teachers who later returned the packets to research Each lead teacher of the 180 participating classes completed a
assistants on the day of classroom observation. Each child received questionnaire for their demographic information, including years
a small token (e.g., a picture book) for their participation. of teaching experience, educational background, number of chil-
dren and staff in the classroom. Years of teaching reflects the
3.3. Measures teacher’s experience in preschool service and was measured as a
continuous variable. Teacher’s education was defined as the high-
3.3.1. Outcomes: social skills est level of completed formal education and was dummy-coded (0
The Social Skills Improvement System-Rating Scales (SSIS-RS; = technical secondary school and below, 1 = above technical sec-
Gresham & Elliott, 2008; parental version) were used to assess chil- ondary school). Student-to-teacher ratio was computed by dividing
dren’s social skills. The social skills included 46 items, covering the number of children by the number of staff in the classroom
communication, cooperation, assertion, responsibility, empathy, at the time of the observation. Given that the participants were
engagement, and self-control. The items are on a four-point rat- recruited from three grade levels and three municipalities, both
ing scale (0 = never, 1 = seldom, 2 = often, and 3 = almost always), grade level and municipality were dummy coded and included as
on which parents indicate the behavior frequency that a child has control variables. K1 served as the reference group for grade level,
exhibited within the past two months, such as “makes eye contact and the municipality with a medium level of economic develop-
when talking” and “interacts well with other children”. The higher ment served as the reference group for municipality.
total score of the global scale is interpreted as better social skills.
A construct validity study reported the psychometric properties of 3.3.5. Covariates: parental demographic information
SSIS-RS in a Chinese sample (Hu et al., 2019). In the present study, Family socio-economic status (SES) is an economic and sociolog-
the internal consistency reliability indexed by Cronbach’s ˛ was .98 ical composite measure, including the quantification of father and
for social skills. mother’s education, occupation, and household income (Cohen,
Doyle, & Baum, 2006). These indicators were collected in the
3.3.2. Predictors: teacher self-efficacy parent survey, where educational level was assessed by seven
The short version of the Teacher Self-Efficacy Inventory (TSEI; response categories ranging from “primary school” to “doctoral
Tschannen-Moran & Woolfolk Hoy, 2001) was used to measure degree”; parental occupation was coded into five categories from
teachers’ perceptions of self-efficacy for student engagement, class- “unemployment, job-waiting, part-time job or farmer” to “senior
room management, and instructional strategies. The TSEI includes management personnel and senior professional”; and the annual
12 items rated using a 9-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = nothing income was divided into nine levels ranging from “less than RMB
to 9 = a great deal. The 12 items comprise three subscales, each with 2000” (equivalent to $280) to “more than RMB 100,000” (equiva-
four items: Efficacy in Student Engagement (ESE, e.g., “motivate lent to $14,000). The scores of these indicators were standardized
students who show low interest in schoolwork”), Efficacy in Class- and averaged to create the family SES index score (Cohen et al.,
room Management (ECM, e.g., “control disruptive behavior in the 2006).
classroom”), and Efficacy in Instructional Strategies (EIS, e.g., “use
a variety of assessment strategies”). The instrument was translated 3.4. Data analysis
into Chinese and then back-translated into English by two bilingual
translators. The results of confirmatory factor analysis supported Given the stratified nature of the sample, children were nested
the three-factor structure for our Chinese preschool sample (␹2 /df within classrooms and schools, which indicated non-independence
= 2.50, TLI = .92, CFI = .90, RMSEA = .09, SRMR = .05). In the present among observations. In the first stage, we examined intra-class
study, the Cronbach ˛ for Efficacy in Student Engagement (ESE) was correlations (ICCs) to assess the extent of nesting. To determine pos-
.83, Efficacy in Classroom Management (ECM) .80, and Efficacy in sible difference in children outcomes across classes and schools, we
Instructional Strategies (EIS) .77, with an overall reliability of the conducted an unconditional, three-level hierarchical linear model
scale equaling .90. (HLM): children at Level 1, classroom at Level 2, and school at
Level 3. The preliminary results show that between-class variance
3.3.3. Mediators: teacher–child interaction quality explained the majority of the variance for social skills (ICC = .634),
The CLASS (Pianta et al., 2008) is a widely validated observation whereas between-school variance could be ignored (ICC < .001).
tool for assessing teacher-child interaction quality through three Cohen (1988) suggested that the HLM method is more suitable than
domains: Emotional Support (ES), Classroom Organization (CO) general linear model when ICC is greater than .059. We thus decided
and Instructional Support (INS). ES consists of four dimensions: to control for classroom level but not for school by conducting a
positive climate, negative climate, teacher sensitivity, and regard two-level model with children at Level 1 and classrooms at Level 2
for student perspectives. CO consists of three dimensions: behav- in the following analysis.
ior management, productivity, and instructional learning formats. In the second stage, a conditional HLM with child-level and
INS consists of three dimensions: concept development, quality of classroom-level covariates was conducted. Child-level covariates
feedback, and language modeling. Each dimension was rated on contained child’s age, gender and family SES, and classroom-

245
B.Y. Hu et al. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 55 (2021) 242–251

beliefs to correlate in the following analyses, and did the same


for three CLASS domains. As is shown in Table 1, the three CLASS
domains were positively associated with teacher’s self-efficacy in
classroom management (ECM) and years of teaching experience,
but negatively correlated with student-teacher ratio.
Table 2 provides an overview of the results obtained from hier-
archical mediation models. ECM was significantly and positively
related to all domains of CLASS (ES: m 12 = .124, ˇ = .224, p = .027;
CO: m 22 = .165, ˇ = .257, p = .009; and INS: m 32 = .103, ˇ = .211,
p = .037). This suggests that teachers who were confident in their
ability to maintain good classroom management provided children
with more sensitive, well-organized, and stimulating interactions.
Interestingly, teachers’ self-efficacy in student engagement (ESE)
was significantly and negatively related to CO (m 21 = −.194,
Fig. 1. Hypothesized hierarchical mediation model.
Note. (a) Bold lines represent significant paths at p = .05 level; (b) the regression coef-
ˇ = −.245, p = .035) when other variables at the classroom level
ficients are standardized. All regressions were controlled by grade and municipality. were controlled, which suggests that teachers who felt efficacious
The three CLASS domains were also controlled by classroom-level demographic in maintaining students’ engagement had classrooms that looked
information (years of teaching, education, and student-to-teacher ratio). Children’s less organized overall. Teachers’ self-efficacy related to instruction
social skills were controlled by child-level demographic variables (age, gender, and
was not associated with any of the classroom process variables.
family SES); (c) *p < .05, **p < .01; and (d) the three domains of teacher’s self-efficacy
were included simultaneously and correlated in the data analysis, and the three In terms of covariates, teachers with more years of teaching
CLASS domains were also correlated. experience showed significantly higher CO (m 24 = .019, ˇ = .161,
p = .035), and higher teacher education level was associated with
significantly higher ES (m 15 = .367, ˇ = .268, p = .001), CO (m
level covariates contained teacher’s teaching experience, education
25 = .303, ˇ = .190, p = .012) and INS (m 35 = .182, ˇ = .151, p
background, student-teacher ratio, grade level, and municipality.
= .050). Larger student–teacher ratio was associated with signifi-
All equations are reported in the Appendix. In this study, we used
cantly lower ES (m 16 = −.033, ˇ = −.199, p = .018) and CO (m
the random-intercept model because this HLM focuses on the
26 = −.031, ˇ = −.150, p = .049). Teachers in K2 (m 37 = .230, ˇ =
relationships between child-level and teacher-level covariates and
.180, p = .034) and K3 (m 38 = .262, ˇ = .200, p = .027) performed
children’s social skills. The interaction and mediation effects were
higher INS than those in K1.
examined in the third stage.
In the third stage, the hypothesized hierarchical mediation
model depicted in Fig. 1 was performed for a 2-2-1 design. Here
4.2. Predicting children’s social skills
we treated the three subscales of teacher self-efficacy (ESE, ECM,
EIS) as independent variables, the three domains of teacher–child
The null model predicting children’s social skills yielded an ICC
interaction quality (ES, CO, INS) as mediators, and social skills as the
of .632, indicating that the variability in children’s social skills
dependent variable. Specifically, both independent variables and
was mainly attributed to between-classroom effects. For the con-
mediators were Level-2 (classroom) variables, while the dependent
ditional model with covariates (Table 2, Model 1, ICC = .056), the
variable was a Level-1 (children) variable.
variance of residuals at the classroom level substantially reduced
The estimate of the mediation effect, or indirect effect, was com-
compared to the null model. More precisely, the proportion of vari-
puted as the product of the slope of the independent variable (ESE,
ance explained by covariates was 2.3% ((309.48–302.23)/309.48 =
ECM, EIS) predicting the mediator (ES, CO, INS) and the slope of
.023) at the individual level and 96.6% ((532.14−17.90)/532.14 =
the mediator (ES, CO, INS) predicting the outcome variable (social
.966) at the classroom level. In terms of individual level, age ( y
skills) controlling for covariates. To assess the adequacy of the
10 = .672, ˇ = .041, p = .045), gender ( y 20 = 2.317, ˇ = .064, p <
hypothesized model, the following model fit indices were eval-
.001) and family SES ( y 30 = 4.877, ˇ = .236, p < .001) were posi-
uated: the ratio of Chi-square to degrees of freedom (2 /df), the
tively associated with social skills. In terms of classroom level, the
comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), the root
differences in children’s social skills between K1 and K2 were very
mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standard-
large, but the discrepancies between K2 and K3 were much smaller
ized root mean square residual (SRMR). An acceptable model fit
than those between K1 and K2 (K2 vs. K1:  y 04 = 45.495, ˇ = .969,
is expected to have a 2 /df with values below 3, CFI and TLI higher
p < .001; K3 vs. K1:  y 05 = 47.306, ˇ = .986, p < .001), indicating
than .90, RMSEA lower than .08, and SRMR below .06 (Hu & Bentler,
the non-linear growth trajectories across grades. Children in the
1999). The Sobel z statistic (Sobel, 1982) was used to test the sig-
municipality with a below-average level of economic development
nificance of the mediation effect. All HLM analyses were conducted
showed significantly lower social skills than the reference group (
using Mplus 7.4 (Muthén & Muthén, 2015).
y 06 = −5.077, ˇ = −.108, p < .001).
For the full model (Table 2, Model 2, ICC = .042), the results
4. Results indicated that the data fit the model reasonably well (2 /df = 2.90,
CFI = .936, TLI = .920, RMSEA = .023, SRMRwithin = .011, SRMRbetween =
4.1. Predicting teacher–child interactions .073). CO was significantly and positively related to children’s social
skills ( y 05 = 1.679, ˇ = .061, p = .029), but ES, INS, and teaching
The means, standard deviations, and correlations of indepen- efficacy were not (ps > .05), after all covariates at the individual level
dent variables, mediators and control variables at the classroom and classroom level were controlled. Compared to the conditional
level are reported in Table 1. Significant correlations were found model with covariates, adding teacher’s self-efficacy and CLASS as
between the three efficacy beliefs subscales and the three CLASS predictors of social skills reduced the between-classroom variance
domains (ps < .05). In the present study, we included all the three by .86% ((17.90−13.32)/532.14 = .0086). The ICC for the full model
subscales of efficacy beliefs in the analysis simultaneously to bet- was .042, indicating that the amount of variation unexplained by
ter examine the unique contribution of each subscale to children’s any predictors in the model that can be attributed to the classroom-
social skills. Therefore, we allowed the three subscales of efficacy level variation was trivial.

246
B.Y. Hu et al. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 55 (2021) 242–251

Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations of Variables at Level 2 (n = 180)

M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

1. ESE 6.644 1.045 —


2. ECM 6.995 1.239 .670** —
3. EIS 6.854 1.149 .774** .602** —
4. ES 5.025 .691 .070 .192* .038 —
5. CO 4.797 .808 −.066 .160* −.078 .773** —
6. INS 2.118 .612 .104 .240** .040 .522** .389** —
7. Teaching year 10.322 6.916 .158* .186* .153* .251** .293** .216** —
8. Teacher education 5.117 1.544 .060 .036 −.011 .362** .338** .243** .343** —
9. Student– teacher ratio 13.037 4.146 −.081 −.095 .064 −.223** −.231** −.185* −.140* −.214**

Note. (a) ESE = Efficacy in Student Engagement, ECM = Efficacy in Classroom Management, EIS = Efficacy in Instructional Strategies, ES = Emotional Support, CO = Classroom
Organization, INS = Instructional Support; (b) *p < .05, **p < .01.

Table 2
Hierarchical Mediation Results: Relations among TeacherSelf-efficacy, Teacher–child Interaction Quality, and Children’s Social Development

Mediators Outcome: Social skills

ES CO INS Model 0 Model 1 Model 2

Intercept 4.877** (.392) 5.300** (.444) 1.846** (.345) 102.181** (1.750) 66.042** (2.506) 63.989** (4.674)
Level 1 (child-level) variables
AGE .678* (.364) .699* (.380)
GENDER1 2.317** (.513) 2.244** (.533)
FAMILY SES 4.877** (.419) 4.888** (.439)
Level 2 (classroom-level) variables
ESE −.071 (.082) −.194* (.093) .002 (.072) .671 (.612)
ECM .124* (.057) .165** (.064) .103* (.050) −.570 (.429)
EIS −.026 (.071) −.044 (.080) −.075 (.062) −.380 (.514)
ES −.975 (.966)
CO 1.679* (.813)
INS .763 (.771)
TEACHING YEAR .011 (.008) .019* (.009) .009 (.007) .054 (.057) .016 (.059)
TEACHER EDU2 .367** (.107) .303* (.121) .182* (.093) .515 (.785) .405 (.825)
STRATIO −.033* (.014) −.031* (.016) −.022 (.013) −.129 (.104) −.139 (.109)
Grade K23 .142 (.123) .053 (.139) .230* (.108) 45.495** (.975) 45.310** (1.000)
Grade K33 .080 (.135) .018 (.152) .262* (.119) 47.306** (1.167) 47.327** (1.239)
MUNICIPALITY BELOW4 .011 (.125) −.277 (.142) −.108 (.110) −5.077** (.934) −4.763** (.970)
MUNICIPALITY ADVANCE4 −.017 (.132) −.016 (.149) −.077 (.116) .865 (.972) .754 (.994)
Random effects
Level 1 residual 309.478** (8.655) 302.230** (6.346) 301.398** (6.582)
Level 2 residual .377** (.042) .483** (.053) .292** (.032) 532.141** (26.088) 17.895** (2.245) 12.320** (2.135)
R2 Level 1 .062 .064
R2 Level 2 .193 .235 .200 .966 .974

Note. (a) Model 0 = null model, Model 1 = conditional model with covariates, Model 2 = full model with independent variables, mediators and covariates. (b) ESE = Efficacy
in Student Engagement, ECM = Efficacy in Classroom Management, EIS = Efficacy in Instructional Strategies, ES = Emotional Support, CO = Classroom Organization, INS =
Instructional Support. (c) *p < .05, **p < .01. (d) 1 Gender was dichotomous (0 = boy, 1 = girl); 2 teacher’s education was dichotomous (0 = technical secondary school graduated
and below, 1 = above technical secondary school graduated); 3 dummy coded with Grade K1 as the reference group; 4 dummy coded with the municipality with average
level of economic development as the reference group.

Using the product-of-coefficient method, the indirect effect of Specifically, we examined whether teachers’ self-efficacy (Level-2
ESE on children’s social skill through CO was −.326 with 95% con- predictor) relates to children’s social skills (Level-1 outcome) as
fidence interval (CI) [−.631, −.021], while the indirect effect of mediated by teacher–child interactions (Level-2 mediators). The
ECM on social skills through CO was .278 (95% CI = [.034, .522]). most important takeaway is that Classroom Organization (CO) was
For ESE, teachers with higher self-efficacy for student engage- confirmed to be a positive mediator between Efficacy in Classroom
ment provided less organized classroom environments, and these Management (ECM) and children’s social skills; however, it was a
less organized environments were associated with lower parent- negative mediator between Efficacy in Student Engagement (ESE)
reported social skills. For ECM, teachers with greater self-efficacy and children’s social outcomes. Below, we discuss the findings and
related to classroom management provided students with more their implications in detail.
organized classroom environments, which in turn was associated
with higher parent-reported social skills.
5.1. Teachers’ self-efficacy and teacher–child interactions

5. Discussion The current finding of an association between teachers’ self-


efficacy and teacher–child interactions supports the basic tenets
The development of positive social skills is a key element of child of the Heuristic Model of Teacher Self-efficacy that teachers’ self-
well-being during the preschool years. As the Chinese government efficacy constitutes a major psychological foundation for effective
seeks to expand access to high quality preschool, it is important classroom teaching (Zee & Koomen, 2016). These results are in
to understand the processes that support positive development keeping with prior research, which has found that efficacious teach-
in this domain. In this study, we utilized a multilevel mediation ers are more likely to have high-quality interactions with students
model to investigate the interconnections among teachers’ self- (Holzberger et al., 2013; Künsting et al., 2016), as they are con-
efficacy, teacher–child interactions, and preschoolers’ social skills. fident in their teaching abilities and effectiveness, demonstrating

247
B.Y. Hu et al. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 55 (2021) 242–251

persistence in helping children master concepts and utilize diverse 5.4. Well-organized classrooms facilitate children’s social skills
feedback strategies.
In the current mediation analysis, we identified a direct cross-
level effect of teachers’ classroom organization quality on children’s
social skills, which echoed previous findings (Korpershoek et al.,
5.2. Positive association between ECM and teacher–child
2016; Skiba et al., 2016). Children in well-organized classrooms
interactions
are more likely to exhibit better task engagement (Ponitz, Rimm-
Kaufman, Grimm, & Curby, 2009), more prosocial behavior, and less
The current findings indicate that teachers with higher ECM
aggression toward others (Simonsen, Fairbanks, Briesch, Myers,
tended to exhibit better emotional, behavioral, and instructional
& Sugai, 2008). It may be that well-organized classrooms create
support — all three domains of teacher–child interactions. ECM
more opportunities for children to engage in positive and meaning-
captures teachers’ beliefs about their competencies in execut-
ful social interactions because time is well-managed, children are
ing effective control over classroom order and student behaviors
engaged in purposeful activities, and disruptive, negative behaviors
(Browers & Tomic, 2000). Classroom management is a significant
are kept to a minimum (Pianta et al., 2008). In this way, children
challenge for Chinese preschool teachers, who often lead large
tend to acquire better social skills in well-organized classrooms
classes averaging 30–40 children each (Hu et al., 2017). Many teach-
(McCormick, Cappella, O’Connor, & McClowry, 2015).
ers find it difficult to manage behavior in such large classes, because
children show various patterns of behavioral and learning chal-
5.5. The mediating role of classroom organization
lenges (Blatchford & Russell, 2019). Based on the current findings,
Chinese teachers who feel confident in their classroom manage-
In the current study, teachers’ emotional and instructional sup-
ment skills are more likely to provide higher quality emotional
port were not statistically significant mediators between teachers’
support, classroom organization, and instructional support to their
self-efficacy and children’s social skills. Classroom organization
students.
was the only classroom-level mediator bridging the associa-
tion between teachers’ self-efficacy and children’s social skills.
Teachers’ self-efficacy in classroom management was higher in
5.3. The negative association between ESE and classroom classrooms that were better organized, had fewer discipline prob-
organization lems, and made better use of class time, and parents of students in
those classrooms perceived that their children had stronger social
To our surprise, we found that as teachers’ efficacy of student skills. However, teachers’ self-efficacy in student engagement was
engagement (ESE) increased, their ratings of classroom organi- negatively associated with classroom organization rated by the par-
zation decreased. After careful consideration, we speculate that ticipating parents.
this association may be related to teachers’ tendency to organize It is reasonable that teachers’ instructional support showed no
classroom activities that elicit children’s meaningful and active significant mediation of the path from teachers’ self-efficacy to chil-
engagement (Bandura, 1997). Teachers with high ESE may seek dren’s social skills. Previous research has indicated that children’s
to facilitate children’s active involvement in classroom processes social skill development is more likely to be influenced by teach-
rather than making children work exclusively on tasks arranged by ers’ emotional and behavioral support rather than instructional
teachers, which may result in less productive and organized use of support (Broekhuizen et al., 2016). What is surprising though is
class time. On the other hand, teachers with low ESE might more that the current research finding does not replicate the mediating
rigidly hold children’s attention to classroom tasks (Alford, Rollins, role of teachers’ emotional support between teachers’ self-efficacy
Padrón, & Waxman, 2016). and children’s social outcomes. This is inconsistent with some ear-
Discipline and order are highly valued by teachers in Chinese lier findings that teachers’ emotional support showed significant
preschool classrooms due to cultural influences of Collectivism (Rao associations with children’s socio-emotional development (Curby
& Sun, 2017). Previous observational studies suggest that Chinese et al., 2013; Mashburn et al., 2008). It may be, given that emotional
teachers tend to receive high ratings for well-disciplined class- support and behavioral support are highly correlated, that the influ-
rooms because children are obedient even when their involvement ence of classroom organization overshadows emotional support in
might be passive (Lan et al., 2009). Teachers with high ESE may Chinese preschool classrooms (Hu et al., 2017), especially because
place more value on children’s free choice and active involvement, Chinese class sizes tend to be large.
which may have the unintended effect of reducing classroom orga- Consistent with our hypothesis, we found that teachers’ CO
nization. The decrease in classroom organization may be associated positively mediated the relation between teachers’ ECM and chil-
with teachers’ loosening of control over classroom conformity and dren’s social skills. This finding is also in line with the extant
discipline, which suggests that the participating teachers lack effec- literature that teachers’ self-efficacy is positively associated with
tive behavioral management skills. Further research is needed to classroom processes, which will in turn positively affect chil-
test this hypothesis. dren’s outcomes (e.g., Guo et al., 2012; Zee & Koomen, 2016).
Moreover, well-organized classrooms “at the high-end of behav- Unexpectedly, teachers’ CO was a negative mediator between
ior management may not be evident “in teachers’ behaviors such teachers’ ESE and children’s social skills. This seemingly awk-
as proactive strategies and effective redirection because children’s ward finding may reflect the dilemma Chinese preschool teachers
behavior is so well managed.” (Pianta et al., 2008, p. 44). When face as they struggle to maintain the balance of implementing
teachers’ proactivity and misbehavior redirection are not observed child-centered initiatives and training children to behave socially
in the classroom organization process when no students show in accordance with norms and expectations of Chinese soci-
misbehaviors, the raters may suppose that “effective behavioral ety.
strategies are in place and a classroom may score in the high range” Social skills are socially acceptable, learned behaviors enabling
(Pianta et al., 2008, p.4). Given the abovementioned rating rules, individuals’ effective and appropriate interactions with others in
Chinese teachers tend to get high scores on Classroom Organiza- society (Pearson & Rao, 2003). According to Chen and French (2008),
tion domain because their children are trained to follow teachers’ sociocultural values are reflected in the realization of children’s
rules, classroom disciplines, and comply with commands (Hu et al., classroom socialization goals. Strongly influenced by the sociocul-
2017), which are prosocial behaviors valued by the Chinese society. tural value of Confucianism, Chinese parents and teachers train

248
B.Y. Hu et al. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 55 (2021) 242–251

children not to reveal their inner feelings and to maintain solemnity skills to satisfy parental and societal expectations. These teachers
and self-control as important early childhood socialization goals have a strong desire to foster children’s good manners, obedience
(Bond & Wang, 1983). In Chinese schools, children are expected to to authoritarian figures, and respect for others in their classroom
develop social skills that can enhance harmony and collectivity in organizational processes.
groups (Cheah & Rubin, 2003). Thus, Chinese teachers mostly use On the other hand, teachers practicing child-centeredness in
classroom organization strategies to train children’s social skills in classrooms focus more on children’s active participations and indi-
line with social norms, such as good manners, sense of responsibil- vidualized experiences (Lerkkanen et al., 2016). Children taught by
ity, tolerance/respect for others, and obedience to authority figures high-ESE teachers may pay more attention to their own thoughts
(Cheah & Rubin, 2003). These teachers might have a strong sense and feelings, rather than classroom rules and discipline. These
of classroom management, but they lack a child-centered approach children are more likely to get lower evaluations from parents
to teaching and learning. and society on social skills. Therefore, Chinese preschool teachers
On the other hand, teachers who take a more child-centered should strike a better balance between eliciting children’s active
approach to teaching might have a stronger sense of engage- participations in classroom processes and their effective learn-
ment, as they might prefer authentically incorporating children’s ing of good social skills advocated within Chinese social systems.
ideas into their teaching, eliciting children’s active participation. Researchers and ECE professionals are charged with the respon-
Teachers with strong ESE might value students’ behaviors such sibility to define the goals for social skills training for preschool
as questioning authority and even being argumentative, even if children in Chinese sociocultural contexts.
that means ignoring other on-going group activities. These learn-
ing and social behaviors, however, might be perceived as selfish, CRediT authorship contribution statement
disobedient or lacking in Chinese sociocultural context. In such
a case, teachers with high ESE may hinder the positive effects of Bi Ying Hu: Conceptualization, Investigation, Supervision.
classroom organization on children’s social skills in Chinese soci- Yuanhua Li: Data curation, Writing - original draft. Chuang Wang:
ety. Methodology, Writing - review & editing. Huiping Wu: Method-
ology, Formal analysis, Writing - original draft. Ginny Vitiello:
5.6. Limitation, directions for future research and implications for Writing - review & editing.
practice
Acknowledgements
This study has several limitations. First, this study only pro-
vides evidence for the correlations among teacher self-efficacy, The research was supported by the project “The Effects of
classroom organization, and child social skills. Cross-lagged or Preschool Program Quality on Children’s Mid- to Long-term
experimental designs should be applied in future research to Learning and Development Outcomes: A Follow-up Three-year
determine cause-and-effect relations among teachers’ efficacy Longitudinal Study” (University of Macau Multi-Year Research
beliefs, classroom practices, and child social outcomes. Second, Grant; MYRG20l8-00024-FED). Also, this work was partially sup-
we were primarily interested in children’s social outcomes in ported by the Program for Probability and Statistics: Theory and
the current study. Future relevant studies should expand this Application (No. IRTL1704) to the fourth author.
work to look at children’s academic and cognitive outcomes in
order to investigate how the mediation mechanism of classroom Appendix A
processes works to connect teachers’ personal efficacy beliefs
to other aspects of children’s performance. Third, this study The conditional model with child-level and classroom-level covari-
was conducted in the Chinese preschool context, so the results ates but without mediators:
may not generalize to children in other social or cultural back-
Level1 :Y ij =ˇy0 j +ˇy1 j (AGE)+ˇy2 j (GENDER)+ˇy3 j (FAMILYSES)+εyij
grounds.
Despite the limitations, this research indicates that there
are indirect effects of ECM and ESE on children’s social skills
through the mediation of classroom organization. These impor- Level2 :ˇy0 j = y00 + y01 (TEACHINGYEAR)+ y02 (TEACHEREDU)
tant mediation mechanisms may have implications for both theory + y03 (STRATIO)+ y04 (GRADEK2)+ y05 (GRADEK3)
and practice. First, these mediation mechanisms extend previous
conceptual models regarding associations among teachers’ self- + y06 (MUNICIPALITYBELOW)+ y07 (MUNICIPALITYADVANCE)
efficacy, classroom processes, and child performance into Chinese +uy0 j
preschool contexts. By exploring these mechanisms, we developed
a deeper understanding that classroom organization is a major
classroom-level element explaining how teachers’ efficacy exerts
ˇyqj = yqj (+uyqj ), forq= 1, 2, 3,
an influence on children’s social skills. Thus, future curricula aim-
ing at improving teachers’ self-efficacy classroom organization skill where Yij is the outcome variable (social skills) for the ith child
should be added to both pre- and in-service teachers’ professional in the jth class; εyij and uy0 j are Level-1 (child-level) and Level-2
development programs. (classroom-level) residuals for Y, respectively, with assumptions
The negative mediation effect of CO for the relationship between of εyij ∼ N(0, ␴2 ), uy0 j ∼ N(0,  00 ) and Cov(εyij , uy0 j ) = 0; inter-
ESE and children’s social skills suggests a possible gap between cept  y00 represents the estimated grand mean value of Y across
Chinese preschool teachers’ beliefs and practices in terms of effi- all children; slopes  y1 j ,  y2 j ,  y3 j represent the fixed effects of
ciently training children to meet parents’ expectations. Modern child-level covariates (age, gender, family SES) on Y; and  y0q (q
Chinese children’s social development is a complicated issue influ- = 1, 2, . . ., 7) represents the fixed effects of classroom-level covari-
enced jointly by Chinese Confucianism and Western child-centered ates (teaching year, teacher’s education, student-teacher ratio, two
ideologies (e.g., Hu et al., 2017; Pearson & Rao, 2003). On one dummy-coded grade variables and two dummy-coded municipal-
hand, traditional Chinese teachers tend to train children’s social ity variables) on Y.

249
B.Y. Hu et al. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 55 (2021) 242–251

The final model for the individual outcome: Skopek, & M. Triventi (Eds.), Childcare, early education and social inequality.
Norththampton, MA: Edward Elgar Publishing.
Level1 :Y ij =ˇy0 j +ˇy1 j (AGE)+ˇy2 j (GENDER)+ˇy3 j (FAMILYSES)+εyij Barni, D., Danioni, F., & Benevene, P. (2019). Teachers’ self-efficacy: The role of
personal values and motivations for teaching. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1–7.
Berhenke, A., Miller, A. L., Brown, E., Seifer, R., & Dickstein, S. (2011). Observed
emotional and behavioral indicators of motivation predict school readiness in
Level2 :ˇy0 j = y00 + y01 (ESE)+ y02 (ECM)+ y03 (EIS)+ y04 (ES) Head Start graduates. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26(4), 430–441.
Blatchford, P., & Russell, A. (2019). Class size, grouping practices and classroom
+ y05 (CO)+ y06 (INS)+ y07 (TEACHINGYEAR) management. International Journal of Educational Research, 96, 154–163.
Bond, M. H., & Wang, S. (1983). China: Aggressive behavior and the problems of
+ y08 (TEACHEREDU)+ y09 (STRATIO)+ y0,10 (GRADEK2) maintaining order and harmony. In A. P. Coldstein, & M. H. Segall (Eds.),
Aggression in global perspective (pp. 58–74). New York: Pergamon.
+ y0,11 (GRADEK3)+ y0,12 (MUNICIPALITYBELOW) Broekhuizen, M. L., Mokrova, I. L., Burchinal, M. R., Garrett-Peters, P. T., & Family
Life Project Key Investigators. (2016). Classroom quality at pre-kindergarten
and kindergarten and children’s social skills and behavior problems. Early
+ y0,13 (MUNICIPALITYADVANCE)+uy 0j
Childhood Research Quarterly, 36, 212–222.
Browers, A., & Tomic, W. (2000). A longitudinal study of teacher burnout and
perceived self-efficacy in classroom management. Teaching and Teacher
Education, 16, 239–253.
ˇy qj = yq0 , forq= 1, 2, 3, Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Steca, P., & Malone, P. S. (2006). Teachers’
self-efficacy beliefs as determinants of job satisfaction and students’ academic
where  y 0q (q = 1, 2, . . ., 13) represents the fixed effects of achievement: A study at the school level. Journal of School Psychology, 44(6),
classroom-level variable (three subscales of teaching self-efficacy, 473–490.
three domains of CLASS, teacher and classroom demographic infor- Cheah, C. S., & Rubin, K. H. (2003). European American and mainland Chinese
mothers’ socialization beliefs regarding preschoolers’ social skills. Parenting:
mation) on Y. Science and Practice, 3(1), 1–21.
The equations for the mediators: Chen, X., & French, D. C. (2008). Children’s social competence in cultural context.
Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 591–616.
Level2 : ES = m10 + m11 (ESE)+ m12 (ECM)+ m13 (EIS) Chen, X., Cen, G., Li, D., & He, Y. (2005). Social functioning and adjustment in
Chinese children: The imprint of historical time. Child Development, 76,
+ m14 (TEACHINGYEAR)+ m15 (TEACHEREDU)+ m16 (STRATIO) 182–195.
Chen, X., Chen, H., Li, D., & Wang, L. (2009). Early childhood behavioral inhibition
+ m17 (GRADEK2)+ m18 (GRADEK3) and social and school adjustment in Chinese children: A 5-year longitudinal
study. Child Development, 80(6), 1692–1704.
+ m19 (MUNICIPALITYBELOW) Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.).
Hillsdale, NJ: Eribaum.
+ m1,10 (MUNICIPALITYADVANCE)+um1 j Cohen, S., Doyle, W. J., & Baum, A. (2006). Socioeconomic status is associated with
stress hormones. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68(3), 414–420.
Curby, T. W., Brock, L. L., & Hamre, B. K. (2013). Teachers’ emotional support
CO = m20 + m21 (ESE)+ m22 (ECM)+ m23 (EIS) consistency predicts children’s achievement gains and social skills. Early
Education & Development, 24(3), 292–309.
+ m24 (TEACHINGYEAR)+ m25 (TEACHEREDU)+ m26 (STRATIO) Curby, T. W., Grimm, K. J., & Pianta, R. C. (2010). Stability and change in early
childhood classroom interactions during the first two hours of a day. Early
+ m27 (GRADEK2)+ m28 (GRADEK3) Childhood Research Quarterly, 25(3), 373–384.
Djambazova-Popordanoska, S. (2016). Implications of emotion regulation on
+ m29 (MUNICIPALITYBELOW) young children’s emotional wellbeing and educational achievement.
Educational Review, 68(4), 497–515.
+ m2,10 (MUNICIPALITYADVANCE)+um2 j Gordon, D. G. (2001). Classroom management problems and solutions: A few basic
guidelines for classroom management can improve student behavior and
reduce stress on the music educator. Music Educators Journal, 88(2), 17–23.
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (2008). Social skills improvement system rating scales
INS = m30 + m31 (ESE)+ m32 (ECM)+ m33 (EIS) manual. Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson.
Guo, Y., Connor, C. M., Yang, Y., Roehrig, A. D., & Morrison, F. J. (2012). The effects of
+ m34 (TEACHINGYEAR)+ m35 (TEACHEREDU)+ m36 (STRATIO) teacher qualification, teacher self-efficacy, and classroom practices on fifth
graders’ literacy outcomes. The Elementary School Journal, 113(1), 3–24.
+ m37 (GRADEK2)+ m38 (GRADEK3) Guo, Y., Piasta, S. B., Justice, L. M., & Kaderavek, J. N. (2010). Relations among
preschool teachers’ self-efficacy, classroom quality, and children’s language
+ m39 (MUNICIPALITYBELOW) and literacy gains. Teaching and Teacher Education, 26(4), 1094–1103.
Hamre, Hatfield, Pianta, & Jamil. (2014). Evidence for general and domain-specific
+ m3,10 ((MUNICIPALITYADVANCE)+um3 j , elements of teacher–child interactions: Associations with preschool children’s
development. Child Development, 85(3), 1257–1274.
where intercepts  m10 ,  m20 ,  m30 represent the averaged values Hamre, B. K., Pianta, R. C., Downer, J. T., DeCoster, J., Mashburn, A. J., Jones, S. M., . . .
of ES, CO and INS across all classrooms, slopes  m1q ,  m2q ,  m3q (q = & Brackett, M. A. (2013). Teaching through interactions: Testing a
developmental framework of teacher effectiveness in over 4000 classrooms.
1, 2, . . ., 10) represent the fixed effects of classroom-level variables The Elementary School Journal, 113(4), 461–487.
on ES, CO and INS, respectively; and um1 j , um2 j , um3 j are Level-2 Hascher, T., & Hagenauer, G. (2016). Openness to theory and its importance for
residuals for mediators (ES, CO and INS), respectively. pre-service teachers’ self-efficacy, emotions, and classroom behaviour in the
teaching practicum. International Journal of Educational Research, 77, 15–25.
Hatfield, B. E., Burchinal, M. R., Pianta, R. C., & Sideris, J. (2016). Thresholds in the
References association between quality of teacher–child interactions and preschool
children’s school readiness skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 36,
Aguitar, A., Aguitar, C., Cadima, J., Correia, N., & Fialho, M. (2019). Classroom quality 561–571.
and children’s social skills and problem behaviors: Dosage and disability status Holzberger, D., Philipp, A., & Kunter, M. (2013). How teachers’ self-efficacy is
as moderators. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 49(4), 81–92. https://doi. related to instructional quality: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Educational
org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.05.005 Psychology, 105(3), 774–786.
Alford, B. L., Rollins, K. B., Padrón, Y. N., & Waxman, H. C. (2016). Using systematic Housman, D. K. (2017). The importance of emotional competence and
classroom observation to explore student engagement as a function of self-regulation from birth: A case for the evidence-based emotional cognitive
teachers’ developmentally appropriate instructional practices (DAIP) in social early learning approach. International Journal of Child Care and Education
ethnically diverse pre-kindergarten through second-grade classrooms. Early Policy, 11(1), 13.
Childhood Education Journal, 44(6), 623–635. Howes, C., Burchinal, M., Pianta, R., Bryant, D., Early, D., Clifford, R., . . . & Barbarin,
Araujo, M., Carneiro, P., Cruz-Aguayo, Y., & Schady, N. (2016). Teacher quality and O. (2008). Ready to learn? Children’s pre-academic achievement in
learning outcomes in kindergarten. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 131(3), pre-kindergarten programs. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23(1), 27–50.
1415–1453. Hu, B. Y., Fan, X., Gu, C., & Yang, N. (2016). Applicability of the classroom
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. London: Macmillan. assessment scoring system in Chinese preschools based on psychometric
Barnett, W. S., & Frede, E. C. (2017). Long-term effects of a system of high-quality evidence. Early Education and Development, 27(5), 714–734. https://doi.org/10.
universal preschool education in the United States. In H. Blossfeld, N. Kulic, J. 1080/10409289.2016.1113069

250
B.Y. Hu et al. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 55 (2021) 242–251

Hu, B. Y., Fan, X., Wu, Y., LoCasale-Crouch, J., & Song, Z. (2019). Contributions of Pearson, E., & Rao, N. (2003). Socialization goals, parenting practices, and peer
Teacher–Child Interaction Quality to Chinese Children’s Development in the competence in Chinese and English preschoolers. Early Child Development and
Early Childhood Years. Early Education and Development, 30(2), 159–177. Care, 173(1), 131–146.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2018.1544809 Pianta, R., Downer, J., & Hamre, B. (2016). Quality in early education classrooms:
Hu, B. Y., Fan, X., Wu, Z., LoCasale-Crouch, J., Yang, N., & Zhang, J. (2017). Definitions, gaps, and systems. The Future of Children, 26(2), 119–137.
Teacher-child interactions and children’s cognitive and social skills in Chinese Pianta, R. C., La Paro, K. M., & Hamre, B. K. (2008). The classroom assessment scoring
preschool classrooms. Children and Youth Services Review, 79, 78–86. https:// system manual (Pre-K). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2017.05.028 Pianta, R. C., La Paro, K. M., Payne, C., Cox, M. J., & Bradley, R. (2002). The relation of
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance kindergarten classroom environment to teacher, family and school
structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural characteristics and child outcomes. Elementary School Journal, 102, 225–238.
Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. Ponitz, C. C., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Grimm, K. J., & Curby, T. W. (2009). Kindergarten
Hustedt, J. T., Buell, M. J., Hallam, R. A., & Pinder, W. M. (2018). While kindergarten classroom quality, behavioral engagement, and reading achievement. School
has changed, some beliefs stay the same: Kindergarten teachers’ beliefs about Psychology Review, 38(1), 102–120.
readiness. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 32(1), 52–66. Rao, N., & Sun, J. (2017). Conclusions: Similarities and distinctions in early
Jeon, H. J., McCartney, M. C., Richard, V., & Johnson, S. J. (2020). Associations childhood education across Chinese societies. In Early childhood education in
between parent–teacher and teacher–child relationships and children’s Chinese societies. pp. 251–257. Dordrecht: Springer.
socioemotional functioning. Early Child Development and Care, 1–15. Rao, N., Ng, S. S., & Pearson, E. (2010). Preschool pedagogy: A fusion of traditional
Klassen, R. M., Tze, V. M., Betts, S. M., & Gordon, K. A. (2011). Teacher efficacy Chinese beliefs and contemporary notions of appropriate practice. In Revisiting
research 1998–2009: Signs of progress or unfulfilled promise? Educational the Chinese learner. pp. 255–279. Dordrecht: Springer.
Psychology Review, 23(1), 21–43. Shoulders, T. L., & Krei, M. S. (2015). Rural high school teachers’ self-efficacy in
Korpershoek, H., Harms, T., De Boer, H., Van Kuijk, M., Doolaard, S., & Research student engagement, instructional strategies, and classroom management.
Evaluation of Educational Effectiveness. (2016). A meta-analysis of the effects American Secondary Education, 44(1), 50–61.
of classroom management strategies and classroom management programs on Siekkinen, M., Pakarinen, E., Lerkkanen, M. K., Poikkeus, A. M., Salminen, J.,
students’ academic, behavioral, emotional, and motivational outcomes. Review Poskiparta, E., . . . & Nurmi, J. E. (2013). Social competence among 6-year-old
of Educational Research, 86(3), 643–680. children and classroom instructional support and teacher stress. Early
Künsting, J., Neuber, V., & Lipowsky, F. (2016). Teacher self-efficacy as a long-term Education & Development, 24(6), 877–897.
predictor of instructional quality in the classroom. European Journal of Simonsen, B., Fairbanks, S., Briesch, A., Myers, D., & Sugai, G. (2008).
Psychology of Education, 31(3), 299–322. Evidence-based practices in classroom management: Considerations for
La Paro, K. M., Pianta, R. C., & Stuhlman, M. (2004). The classroom assessment research to practice. Education and Treatment of Children, 31(3), 351–380.
scoring system: Findings from the prekindergarten year. The Elementary School Skiba, R., Ormiston, H., Martinez, S., & Cummings, J. (2016). Teaching the social
Journal, 104(5), 409–426. curriculum: Classroom management as behavioral instruction. Theory into
Lan, X., Ponitz, C. C., Miller, K. F., Li, S., Cortina, K., Perry, M., . . . & Fang, G. (2009). Practice, 55(2), 120–128.
Keeping their attention: Classroom practices associated with behavioral Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in
engagement in first grade mathematics classes in China and the United States. structural equation models. In S. Leinhardt (Ed.), Sociological methodology 1982
Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24, 198–211. (pp. 290–312). Washington, DC: American Sociological Association.
Lerkkanen, M. K., Kiuru, N., Pakarinen, E., Poikkeus, A. M., Rasku-Puttonen, H., Tschannen-Moran, M., & Woolfolk Hoy, A. (2001). Teacher efficacy: Capturing
Siekkinen, M., . . . & Nurmi, J. E. (2016). Child-centered versus teacher-directed elusive construct. Teaching and Teacher Education, 17, 783–805.
teaching practices: Associations with the development of academic skills in Tschannen-Moran, M., Hoy, A. W., & Hoy, W. K. (1998). Teacher efficacy: Its
the first grade at school. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 36, 145–156. meaning and measure. Review of Educational Research, 68(2), 202–248.
Leyva, D., Weiland, C., Barata, M. C., Yoshikawa, H., Snow, C., Treviño, E., . . . & Rolla, Von Suchodoletz, A., Fäsche, A., Gunzenhauser, C., & Hamre, B. (2014). A typical
A. (2015). Teacher–child interactions in Chile and their associations with morning in preschool: Observations of teacher–child interactions in German
prekindergarten outcomes. Child Development, 86(3), 781–799. preschools. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 509–519.
Mashburn, A. J., Pianta, R. C., Hamre, B. K., Downer, J. T., Barbarin, O. A., Bryant, D., Woolfolk Hoy, A., Hoy, W. K., & Davis, H. A. (2009). Teachers’ self-efficacy beliefs. In
. . . & Howes, C. (2008). Measures of classroom quality in prekindergarten and K. Wentzel, & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp.
children’s development of academic, language, and social skills. Child 627–653). New York, NY: Routledge.
Development, 79(3), 732–749. Yerdelen, S., & Sungur, S. (2019). Multilevel investigation of students’
McClelland, M. M., Cameron, C. E., Wanless, S. B., Murray, A., Saracho, O., & Spodek, self-regulation processes in learning science: Classroom learning environment
B. (2007). Executive function, behavioral self-regulation, and social-emotional and teacher effectiveness. International Journal of Science and Mathematics
competence. Contemporary Perspectives on Social Learning in Early Childhood Education, 17(1), 89–110.
Education, 1, 113–137. Zee, M., & Koomen, H. M. (2016). Teacher self-efficacy and its effects on classroom
McCormick, M. P., Cappella, E., O’Connor, E. E., & McClowry, S. G. (2015). processes, student academic adjustment, and teacher well-being: A synthesis
Social-emotional learning and academic achievement: Using causal methods of 40 years of research. Review of Educational Research, 86(4), 981–1015.
to explore classroom-level mechanisms. AERA Open, 1(3), 1–26. Zhou, Q., Eisenberg, N., Wang, Y., & Reiser, M. (2004). Chinese children’s effortful
McGraw, K. O., & Wong, S. P. (1996). Forming inferences about some intraclass control and dispositional anger/ frustration: Relations to parenting styles and
correlation coefficients. Psychological Methods, 1(1), 30–46. children’s social functioning. Developmental Psychology, 40, 352–366.
Ministry of Education of People’s Republic of China [MOE]. (2018). Opinions of the Zhou, N., Lam, S. F., & Chan, K. C. (2012). The Chinese classroom paradox: A
CPC Central Committee and State Council on Deepening the Reform and Regulating cross-cultural comparison of teacher controlling behaviors. Journal of
of Preschool Education [ ] Educational Psychology, 104(4), 1162–1212.
Retrieved from. http://www.moe.gov.cn/jyb xwfb/xw fbh/moe 2069/xwfbh
2018n/xwfb 20181128/sfcl/201811/t20181128 361643.html

251

You might also like