You are on page 1of 12

Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2011, 52, 329–340 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2011.00882.

Cognition and Neurosciences


Message sidedness in advertising: The moderating roles of need for
cognition and time pressure in persuasion
DANNY TENGTI KAO
National Hsinchu University of Education, Taiwan

Kao, D. T. (2011). Message sidedness in advertising: The moderating roles of need for cognition and time pressure in persuasion. Scandinavian Journal of
Psychology 52, 329–340.
Persuasion has been extensively researched for decades. Much of this research has focused on different message tactics and their effects on persuasion
(e.g., Chang & Chou, 2008; Lafferty, 1999). This research aims to assess whether the persuasion of a specific type of message is influenced by need for
cognition (NFC) and time pressure. The 336 undergraduates participated in a 2 (message sidedness: one-sided/two-sided) · 3 (time pressure: low/moder-
ate/high) between-subjects design. Results indicate that two-sided messages tend to elicit more favorable ad attitudes than one-sided messages. As com-
pared with low-NFC individuals, high-NFC individuals are likely to express more favorable ad attitudes, brand attitudes and purchase intention. Moderate
time pressure tends to lead to more favorable ad attitudes than low time pressure and high time pressure. In addition, moderate time pressure is likely to eli-
cit more favorable brand attitudes and purchase intentions than high time pressure, but does not elicit more favorable brand attitudes and purchase inten-
tions than low time pressure. Furthermore, when high-NFC individuals are under low or moderate time pressure, two-sided messages are more persuasive
than one-sided messages; however, message sidedness does not differentially affect the persuasion when high-NFC individuals are pressed for time. In con-
trast, one-sided messages are more persuasive than two-sided messages when low-NFC individuals are under low or high time pressure, and two-sided
messages are more persuasive than one-sided messages when low-NFC individuals are under moderate time pressure.
Key words: Message sidedness, need for cognition (NFC), time pressure, persuasion.
Danny Tengti Kao, National Hsinchu University of Education, Department of Educational Psychology and Counseling, 521, Nan-Da Road, Hsinchu,
Taiwan (300). E-mail: danny@mail.nhcue.edu.tw

INTRODUCTION labeled two-sided messages. The persuasive effect of either a one-


sided message or a two-sided message on attitude or belief change
A one-sided message is characterized by favorable claims on all is called the message sidedness effect (O’Keefe, 1999). To some
brand attributes mentioned in the message, whereas a two-sided extent, the message sidedness effect is somewhat counterintuitive;
message contains disclaimers or admissions of inferior performance common sense is that when an advertiser appeals for acceptance
on one or more relatively unimportant attributes (Hastak & Park, of any specific issue, no opposite or suspicious arguments should
1990). Advertisers and advertising researchers have long been inter- be proposed (Lim et al., 2008).
ested in the persuasive effects of one-sided versus two-sided ad Hastak and Park (1990) propose two major reasons why two-
message appeals (e.g., Becker-Olsen, 2006; Hastak & Park, 1990; sided messages may result in favorable effects on attitude and
Lim, Lin, Ferguson & Kiousis, 2008; Smith & Hunt, 1978). cognitive responses. First, as a two-sided message explicitly
Although the psychology and consumer behavior literature has admits inferior performance on disclaimed attributes (Smith &
examined message sidedness effects (e.g., Becker-Olsen, 2006; Hunt, 1978), it is likely to inhibit the audience from generating
Belch, 1981; Hastak & Park, 1990; Kamins & Assael, 1987; Lim counterarguments on those attributes. Second, these disclaimers
et al., 2008), previous research regarding whether a message is more may enhance the credibility of other claims expressed in the mes-
persuasive if it presents only the advertiser’s best arguments or if it sage, and thus suppress counterarguments and strengthen support-
discusses opposing ones in addition to the advertiser’s own has ing arguments.
failed to reach an agreement. Therefore, this research focuses on Some studies have indicated that, as compared with the one-
exploring whether, and under what conditions, a one-sided or a two- sided positive message, the inclusion of negative information
sided message can enhance the persuasiveness of communication. about a product enhances the persuasiveness of a message (e.g.,
This research applies the heuristic-systematic model (HSM) Allen, 1993; Pechmann, 1992). Kamins, Brand, Hoeke & Moe
and the resource matching theory to examine the effect of mes- (1989) argue that a two-sided communication elicits higher adver-
sage sidedness on persuasion. In addition, NFC and time pressure tising credibility, more favorable evaluations of perceived quality,
as the alternative moderators of message sidedness effects are as well as greater usage intentions than a one-sided ad appeal.
introduced to assess the differential persuasive effects of one-sided Moreover, some studies argue that two-sided messages effectively
and two-sided messages. enhance the perceived credibility of claims (Swinyard, 1981), the
perceived source credibility (Bohner, Einwiller, Erb & Siebler,
2003) and the perceived trustworthiness of the ad (Kamins, 1989),
The persuasive effect of message sidedness which lead to positive purchase intentions (Golden & Alpert,
Messages that contain only supporting arguments are labeled one- 1987; Kamins, 1989), and reduce counterarguments (Belch, 1981;
sided messages; those that also address opposing viewpoints are Kamins & Assael, 1987).

 2011 The Author.


Scandinavian Journal of Psychology  2011 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations. Published by Blackwell Publishing Ltd., 9600 Garsington
Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA. ISSN 0036-5564.
330 D. T. Kao Scand J Psychol 52 (2011)

In sharp contrast, some other studies have failed to reach simi- Chaiken, Giner-Sorolla, and Chen (1996) contend that the simi-
lar conclusions (e.g., Belch, 1981; Hastak & Park, 1990). Earl and larities are apparent between the HSM and the ELM. However,
Pride (1980) indicate that a two-side message is no better in Larson (2010) argues that systematic processing and heuristic pro-
increasing the respondent’s perception of the ad’s informativeness cessing in the HSM can operate independently and occur simulta-
than a one-sided message. Belch (1981) and Swanson (1987) con- neously, which are the main differences between the HSM and the
clude that there is no difference in purchase intentions between ELM. Larson (2010) further stresses that ‘‘in the ELM, there is an
one-sided and two-sided message appeals. Furthermore, some inverse relationship between the use of the central and the periph-
research argues that two-sided messages are more likely to eral routes – as one rises, the other falls. In the HSM, individuals
increase attitudinal resistance (Bohner et al., 2003; Kamins & could make systematic use of some evidence at the same time they
Assael, 1987) and reduce purchase intentions (Kanungo & Johar, are using heuristics for other information.’’ In this research, indi-
1975; Swinyard, 1981) than one-sided messages. viduals are assumed to decompose two-sided messages into posi-
Apparently, there is still disagreement regarding when one- tive and negative messages and process them at the same time.
sided messages or two-sided messages are more effective. Some Clearly, the HSM is better than the ELM in analyzing how individ-
moderators have been addressed to interpret the mixed conclu- uals process two-sided messages. Therefore, the HSM rather than
sions, such as the extent of the message recipient’s initial opposi- the ELM, together with the resource matching theory, are applied
tion to the message communicator and their education in this research to predict how consumers process messages.
background (Hovland, Lumsdaine & Sheffield, 1949), prior famil- Considering the HSM and the resource matching theory, mes-
iarity (Chu, 1967), correlational and correspondent inferences sage recipients tend to apply heuristic processing instead of sys-
(Pechmann, 1992), and perceived bias (Wilson & Brekke, 1994). tematic processing when fewer resources are allocated to the
In addition, Lang et al. (1999) contend that the conflicting results message than the message requires; however, they tend to apply
can be attributed to how the message persuasiveness is measured. systematic processing and commit excess resources to generate
Recent studies argue that message type, the time allowed for mes- either additional advocacy-consonant cognitions (Coulter & Punj,
sage recipients to process the ad content (Bohner et al., 2003) and 2004) or counterarguments in nature (Keller & Block, 1997) when
mood states (Becker-Olsen, 2006) can moderate the persuasion of the resources available to process the message exceed what the
message sidedness. message effectively requires. Therefore, it is assumed that individ-
uals may require more cognitive resources for systematically pro-
cessing two-sided messages than one-sided messages and thus
Resource matching theory, HSM and ELM may generate different extents of cognitive responses toward these
This research applies the resource matching theory and the heu- messages. In other words, message sidedness can elicit different
ristic-systematic model (HSM) rather than the elaboration likeli- levels of persuasion. More clearly, when the available cognitive
hood model (ELM) to examine the message sidedness effect. The resources are sufficient, individuals tend to systematically scruti-
cognitive resource matching theory states that the persuasive nize either one-sided or two-sided messages. The negative infor-
impact of a message is maximized when the resources allocated mation in two-sided messages is likely to suppress
to the elaboration of the communication match those required for counterarguments and strengthen supporting arguments (Bohner
the task. Stimuli that demand too much or too little of the et al., 2003; Kamins & Assael, 1987). Therefore, it is predicted
resources that an individual makes available should undermine that two-sided messages are more persuasive than one-sided
persuasion (Peracchio & Meyers-Levy, 1997). The HSM is a messages.
model of attitude change proposed by Chaiken (1980) and later
developed by Chaiken and her colleagues (see Eagly & Chaiken,
1993). It specifies two routes to persuasion: systematic process- Effects of NFC on information processing
ing, an analytic orientation to information processing; and heuris- Consumers are likely to vary substantially in their motivation in
tic processing, and a more restricted mode of information processing advertising messages (Megehee, 2009). Cacioppo and
processing that makes fewer demands on cognitive resources. Petty (1982) argue that need for cognition (NFC) affects individu-
While systematic processing is determined by the cognitive abil- als’ motivation to process information. NFC is defined as an indi-
ity and the motivation of respondents to process message argu- vidual’s propensity to engage in and enjoy cognitively demanding
ments, heuristic processing is triggered by features of the tasks (Cacioppo, Petty & Kao, 1984). NFC explores the cognitive
available information that enable the use of cognitive heuristics processes involved in information processing and contributes to
to form judgments and decisions. Alternatively, the ELM is a identify individual motivational differences (Kao, 2007). High-
model of how attitudes are formed and changed (Petty & Caciop- NFC individuals are intrinsically motivated towards thinking and
po, 1986). The ELM distinguishes between two routes to persua- thus exhibit a strong tendency to enjoy searching for more pro-
sion: the central route and the peripheral route. Central route cessing information (Verplanken, Hazenberg & Palenwen, 1992)
processes involve careful scrutiny of a persuasive communication and devote much cognitive effort (Batra & Stayman, 1990) to
to determine the merits of the arguments. Peripheral route pro- solve complex problems; in contrast, low-NFC individuals are
cesses, in contrast, do not involve elaboration of the message innately unmotivated or slightly motivated to elaborate on cogni-
through extensive cognitive processing of the merits of the actual tively demanding tasks. Similarly, Wegener and Petty (2001) con-
argument presented. Motivation and ability are two factors that tend that high-NFC individuals are more likely to favor a message
determine which route people will take in a persuasive situation strategy characterized by extensive and elaborate processing than
(Petty & Cacioppo, 1981). low-NFC individuals.

 2011 The Author.


Scandinavian Journal of Psychology  2011 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
Scand J Psychol 52 (2011) Message sidedness in advertising 331

Prior literature has explored how to use need for cognition 1992). Suri and Monroe (2003) also argue that moderate time
(NFC) as a basis for designing message contents (Martin, Lang & constraints may increase people’s motivation and intentions to
Wong, 2003; Williams-Piehota, Schneider, Pizarro, Mowad & perform a task.
Salovey, 2003). Brennan and Bahn (2006) suggest that audiences’ In more detail, individuals under the high time pressure condi-
responses to the messages may vary as a function of their NFC. tion may lack the cognitive resources to systematically scrutinize
Also, low-NFC individuals tend to engender favorable ad attitudes information; however, individuals under the low time pressure
and brand attitudes toward an ad in a literal style, whereas high- condition may lack motivation to systematically analyze informa-
NFC individuals tend to favorably evaluate an ad in a figurative tion. Therefore, persuasion will not occur in the conditions of
style. Williams-Piehota et al. (2003) also contend that high-NFC high time pressure and low time pressure. However, in the condi-
individuals change their health behavior after receiving messages tion of moderate time pressure, individuals can be motivated to
with more detailed information than messages with information apply the available cognitive resources to systematically process
presented in a simplistic manner. information and thus are likely to engender positive cognitive
Reinhard and Messner (2009) contend that any variable that responses.
can determine the extent of elaboration, will lead individuals to
invest differential cognitive effort in information processing and
bring about differential persuasion. NFC, as an individual’s ten- Effects of NFC, time pressure and message sidedness on
dency to engage in cognitive endeavors, is regarded as an impor- persuasion
tant factor to influence how individuals are motivated and Meyerowitz and Chaiken (1987) argue that negative messages are
persuaded. Considering the HSM and the resource matching the- more likely to violate people’s expectancies than positive mes-
ory, high-NFC individuals are more willing to apply the available sages, as people are more frequently exposed to arguments pre-
cognitive resources to systematically process messages, and thus sented in positive terms than in negative terms. Moreover, the
have more chances to favorably evaluate these messages than negativity bias concept addressed by Kanouse (1984) contends
low-NFC individuals, who are unmotivated or have limited moti- that negative information may be more salient in an inherently
vation to process these messages. positive world and thus has a greater impact than positive infor-
mation. Similarly, Fiske (1980) argues that negative information
is usually regarded as highly informative and thus may receive
Effects of time pressure on information processing extra weights on the message assessment. Specifically, as com-
Besides the intrinsic motivation variables (such as NFC), some pared with positive information, negative information is more
external situational variables (such as time pressure) can affect likely to motivate individuals and receive greater scrutiny when it
how information is processed. The HSM proposed by Chaiken violates expectations (Baker & Petty, 1994). These premises
(1980) provides a basis for predicting how time pressure affects imply that one-sided ad messages (incorporating positive argu-
information processing. When individuals are motivated and have ments only) may not motivate individuals to carefully assess
sufficient time to process information, they are likely to process information and require a small amount of cognitive resources for
information systematically. However, when the motivation to pro- elaboration; in contrast, two-sided ad messages (incorporating
cess information is low or if the time allowed for processing infor- both positive and negative arguments) may highly motivate peo-
mation is constrained, individuals are expected to apply heuristic ple to scrutinize and require a considerable amount of cognitive
processing (Suri & Monroe, 2003). Specifically, time pressure can resources for further processing. As the HSM and the resource
affect the route that individuals choose to process information matching theory implied, individuals tend to systematically pro-
(heuristically vs. systematically) and the reached level of persua- cess information when they have sufficient cognitive resources; in
sion. contrast, they appear to heuristically process information when
Kao (2006) indicates that time can be regarded as a type of their cognitive resources are insufficient. Larson (2010) argues
external cognitive resource and other types of cognitive resources that systematic processing and heuristic processing in the HSM
may interact with time pressure. Considering the HSM and the can operate independently and occur simultaneously. Hence, it is
resource matching theory, individuals under differential levels of predicted that one-sided messages will be heuristically processed,
time pressure are likely to apply differential cognitive resources whereas the positive information in two-sided messages will be
to either systematically or heuristically process information. Even heuristically processed and the negative information in two-sided
though other cognitive resources are sufficient, individuals under messages will be systematically processed at the same time.
high time pressure are less likely to apply those cognitive This research posits that the inconsistent conclusions regarding
resources to scrutinize the positive information advocated by the persuasiveness of message sidedness in previous studies can
advertisers than those who are under low time pressure. Namely, be ascribed to a missing consideration for personality traits. As
those supporting arguments have fewer chances to be favorably Petty, Unnava and Strathman (1991) indicated, NFC acts as an
evaluated by individuals under high time pressure than under individual factor affecting an individual’s motivation to process
low time pressure. Therefore, persuasion is less likely to occur information. Martin et al. (2003) contend that high-NFC individ-
when individuals are under high time pressure than those who uals prefer a message strategy characterized by extensive and
are under low time pressure. However, as Liebert and Morris elaborate processing. Martin, Sherrard, and Wentzel (2005) also
(1967) indicated, adequate time pressure may activate a state of indicate that high-NFC people should show positive attitudes
alertness, vigor or peppiness, which leads to an increased effort toward a product or a service with the presentation of the mes-
and motivation to perform a given task (Eysenck & Calvo, sage that matches their processing style. Two-sided messages,

 2011 The Author.


Scandinavian Journal of Psychology  2011 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
332 D. T. Kao Scand J Psychol 52 (2011)

which contain the negative arguments that require a considerable messages can be systematically processed at the same time. As
amount of cognitive resources for scrutiny, are consistent with the HSM and the resource matching theory implied, two-sided
high-NFC individuals’ personality traits of enjoying elaboration. messages (incorporating positive and negative arguments) require
Therefore, two-sided messages are perceived as more persuasive more cognitive resources for systematic processing than one-sided
than one-sided messages. Considering the HSM and the resource messages (incorporating positive arguments only). In the moderate
matching theory, high-NFC individuals under moderate or low time pressure condition, low-NFC individuals can be motivated to
time pressure may apply sufficient cognitive resources to system- systematically process information. The resource matching theory
atically scrutinize information. Also, the negative information in indicates that persuasion can occur when the cognitive resources
two-sided messages is likely to suppress counterarguments and allocated to process the communication match those required for
strengthen supporting arguments (Bohner et al., 2003; Kamins & the task. Therefore, low-NFC individuals under moderate time
Assael, 1987). Hence, high-NFC individuals under moderate or pressure are likely to be motivated to apply sufficient cognitive
low time pressure are more likely to favorably evaluate two-sided resources to systematically process information. As Bohner et al.,
messages than one-sided messages. (2003) and Kamins and Assael (1987) implied, the negative infor-
In contrast, individuals under high time pressure lack the mation in two-sided messages is likely to suppress counterargu-
required cognitive resources and may heuristically apply a less ments and strengthen supporting arguments. Therefore, it is
effortful thought rather than systematic processing (Dhar & Now- predicted that low-NFC individuals under the moderate time pres-
lis, 1999). Considering the HSM and the resource matching the- sure condition may evaluate two-sided messages more favorably
ory, high-NFC individuals under high time pressure are unable to than one-sided messages.
invest sufficient cognitive resources in systematically evaluating Given the theoretical background presented above, the author
the message sidedness. The cognitive resource matching theory proposes the following hypotheses:
argues that persuasion will not occur when the cognitive
resources available are insufficient (Coulter & Punj, 2004). H1: Message sidedness will have an impact on persuasion. Spe-
Therefore, it is assumed that when individuals are pressed for cifically, two-sided messages will elicit more favorable ad atti-
time, two-sided messages will not yield better persuasion than tudes, brand attitudes and purchase intentions than one-sided
one-sided messages. Specifically, when high-NFC individuals are messages.
under high time pressure, the message sidedness effect will not H2: NFC will have an impact on persuasion. Specifically, high-
activate. NFC individuals will engender more favorable ad attitudes, brand
Low-NFC individuals intrinsically lack motivation to scrutinize attitudes and purchase intentions than low-NFC individuals.
information, and their evaluations of a message are probably H3: Time pressure will have an impact on persuasion. Specifi-
based on simple inferences. Considering the HSM and the cally, moderate time pressure will elicit more favorable ad atti-
resource matching theory, low-NFC individuals under low time tudes, brand attitudes and purchase intentions than low time
pressure are usually unmotivated and tend to heuristically process pressure and high time pressure.
information, even though the cognitive resources available are H4: The message sidedness effect will not activate when high-
sufficient. However, low-NFC individuals under high time pres- NFC individuals are under high time pressure; in contrast, two-
sure lack motivation and cognitive resources to carefully assess sided messages will lead to more favorable ad attitudes, brand atti-
information, and hence are likely to heuristically evaluate infor- tudes and purchase intentions when high-NFC individuals are
mation (Biswas & Sherrell, 1993). One-sided messages incorpo- under low or moderate time pressure.
rating positive arguments only require a small amount of H5: One-sided messages will elicit more favorable ad attitudes,
cognitive resources for scrutiny and can be more heuristically pro- brand attitudes and purchase intentions than two-sided messages
cessed than two-sided messages. Specifically, low-NFC individu- when low-NFC individuals are under low or high time pressure;
als under low time pressure lack motivation to process in sharp contrast, two-sided messages will elicit more favorable
information, and thus are likely to more favorably evaluate one- ad attitudes, brand attitudes and purchase intentions than one-
sided messages than two-sided messages. Likewise, low-NFC sided messages when low-NFC individuals are under moderate
individuals under high time pressure lack both motivation and time pressure.
cognitive resources for processing effortful information and thus
tend to more favorably evaluate one-sided messages than two-
sided messages. METHODOLOGY
However, as Liebert and Morris (1967) indicated, adequate time
pressure may activate a state of alertness, vigor or peppiness, Pretests
which leads to an increased effort and motivation to perform a
given task (Eysenck & Calvo, 1992). Suri and Monroe (2003) The first pretest was conducted to determine the attribute impor-
also argue that moderate time constraints may increase people’s tance for an MP3 player. Thirty undergraduates participated in an
motivation and intentions to perform a task. Baker and Petty open-ended question to list up to six priorities they set for pur-
(1994) contend that negative information requires more systematic chasing an MP3 player (see Pham & Avnet, 2004 and Nam, 2008
processing than positive information. As indicated above, one- for a similar method). Results indicate that brand, price and sound
sided positive messages tend to be heuristically processed, quality are the top three concerned attributes; in contrast, colors,
whereas the positive information in two-sided messages can be styling and warranty are relatively unimportant attributes.
heuristically processed and the negative information in two-sided

 2011 The Author.


Scandinavian Journal of Psychology  2011 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
Scand J Psychol 52 (2011) Message sidedness in advertising 333

To avoid the potential weakness that many researchers selected Beach (1996), high time pressure can be primed by subtracting
time allowance arbitrarily (Ordonez & Benson, 1997), 30 subjects one standard deviation from the mean. Hence, the high/moderate/
participated in the second pretest and were asked to self-record low time pressure conditions were manipulated by asking subjects
the starting and ending time to determine the average time to complete the questionnaire within 51, 61, 71 seconds respec-
required to comprehend and complete the questionnaire. The tively.
average time was 61 seconds. Then, 61 seconds was used to
determine the time pressure for testing the effectiveness of the ad.
Dependent variable
Subjects, research design and procedure Zhang and Zinkhan (2006) argue that ad attitudes, brand attitudes
A total of 336 undergraduates were randomly assigned to a 2 and purchase intentions can be regarded as different facets of ad
(message sidedness: one-sided/two-sided) · 3 (time pressure: persuasion. In this research, ad attitudes were measured with 4
low/moderate/high) between-subjects design, where message sid- seven-point scales anchored by bad/good, dislike/like, not irritat-
edness is a manipulated independent variable, time pressure is a ing/irritating, not interesting/interesting (a = 0.88) (Martin et al.,
manipulated moderator, and NFC acts as a measured moderator. 2003). Brand attitudes were measured with 3 seven-point scales
To avoid the pre-existed brand preference or prejudice, this anchored by bad/good, not nice/nice, unlikable/likable (a = 0.89)
questionnaire used a fictitious brand name. Subjects were (Zhang & Zinkhan, 2006). Purchase intentions were measured
informed of the time allowed for them to evaluate the one-sided with 2 seven-point scales anchored by strongly consider not buy-
or two-sided messages. ing/would strongly consider buying, and definitely would not buy/
definitely would buy (a = 0.82) (Zhang & Zinkhan, 2006; Teng,
2009).
Manipulation of message sidedness
The ad was clearly stated in a one-sided or a two-sided appeal.
The manipulation of message sidedness in this research was RESULTS
adapted from the study of Hastak and Park (1990). Subjects were
asked to read a statement on key features of a fictitious MP3 Manipulation checks
brand (iMusic). The following excerpt shows the main ad copy of
the one-sided appeals: The message sidedness manipulation was assessed by having
respondents rate whether the ad message contains both positive
– Frankly, the iMusic MP3 player makes a great impression from and negative information on a seven-point scale anchored by
the start – a distinctive design that reflects a unique styling, and a 1 = extremely agree and 7 = extremely disagree. As expected, the
wide assortment of colors to choose from. And this impression is one-sided message and the two-sided message were regarded as
even strengthened when one starts enjoying the music. The pat- being different (Mone-sided = 5.08, Mtwo-sided = 2.13, t(334) =
ented technique ‘‘advanced digital filters (ADF)’’ designed by 23.28, p < 0.001). Therefore, the manipulation of message sided-
iMusic helps remove unwanted digital distortion and thus offer ness was effective.
excellent sound quality. The time pressure manipulation was assessed on a seven-point
scale (‘‘no time pressure’’, ‘‘too much time’’, ‘‘more than ade-
In contrast, the main ad copy of the two-sided appeals is stated quate time available’’, ‘‘adequate time’’, ‘‘not adequate time avail-
below: able’’, ‘‘need a lot more time to do’’, and ‘‘too high time
– Frankly, the iMusic MP3 player does not make a particularly pressure’’) (Suri & Monroe, 2003). A one-way ANOVA revealed
good first impression – a very ordinary design that is not uniquely the mean values of low, moderate and high time pressure
styled, and a limited assortment of colors to choose from. This (MLTP = 2.04, MMTP = 3.37, MHTP = 5.23). The Scheffe post hoc
impression changes, however, when one starts enjoying the music. analysis further revealed that these mean values differed signifi-
The patented technique ‘‘advanced digital filters (ADF)’’ designed cantly from each other (MLTP-MTP = )1.33, p < 0.001;
by iMusic helps remove unwanted digital distortion and thus offer MLTP-HTP = )3.19, p < 0.001; MMTP-HTP = )1.86, p < 0.001).
excellent sound quality. Hence, the manipulation of time pressure was successful.

Measurement for NFC


Hypothesis testing and results
The NFC level for each subject was measured by the 18 five-point
NFC scales devised by Cacioppo et al. (1984). Subjects were The results from the MANOVA analysis indicated that a three-
dichotomized into high and low NFC groups based on a median way interaction effect, three two-way interaction effects and three
split (median = 2.97, a = 0.76) (Chang 1996). High-NFC and main effects were all significant. In more detail, the interaction
low-NFC individuals exhibited significantly different scores effects of message sidedness · NFC · time pressure (Wilks’K =
(MhNFC = 3.75, MlNFC = 2.20, t(334) = 31.96, p < 0.001). 0.76, p = 0.000, gp2 = 0.130), NFC · time pressure (Wil-
ks’K = 0.80, p = 0.000, gp2 = 0.106), message sidedness · time
pressure (Wilks’K = 0.65, p = 0.000, gp2 = 0.195) and message
Manipulation of time pressure sidedness · NFC (Wilks’K = 0.84, p = 0.000, gp2 = 0.159) on
The pretest indicates that the average time to complete the ques- persuasion were significant (see Table 1). These interactions pro-
tionnaire is 61 seconds (s.d. = 10). According to Benson and vide initial evidence of differentiations across the experimental

 2011 The Author.


Scandinavian Journal of Psychology  2011 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
334 D. T. Kao Scand J Psychol 52 (2011)

Table 1. Multivariate analysis for the 3-way, 2-way interaction effects further t-test revealed that high-NFC individuals tended to engen-
and main effects der more favorable ad attitudes (MLow NFC = 3.74, MHigh
NFC = 4.92, t(334) = )16.27, p = 0.000), brand attitudes (MLow
Source Wilks’K F p gp2
NFC = 3.30, MHigh NFC = 4.60, t(334) = )19.49, p = 0.000) and
Message sidedness 0.96 4.39 0.005 0.039 purchase intentions (MLow NFC = 3.42, MHigh NFC = 3.99,
NFC 0.32 226.53 0.000 0.679 t(334) = )5.86, p = 0.000, see Table 3) than low-NFC individu-
Time pressure 0.69 21.52 0.000 0.167 als. Hence, H2 was supported.
Message sidedness · NFC 0.84 20.30 0.000 0.159
Message sidedness · Time 0.65 25.98 0.000 0.195 H3: Effects of time pressure on persuasion. Likewise, the ANOVA
pressure
analysis indicated that time pressure had a significant main effect on
NFC · Time pressure 0.80 12.76 0.000 0.106
Message sidedness · NFC 0.76 16.09 0.000 0.130 ad attitudes (F(2, 333) = 7.58, p = 0.001, gp2 = 0.044), brand atti-
· Time pressure tudes (F(2, 333) = 5.59, p = 0.004, gp2 = 0.032) and purchase
intentions (F(2, 333) = 12.58, p = 0.000, gp2 = 0.070, see
Table 2). The ANOVA Scheffe post hoc analysis revealed that mod-
conditions. The individual main effects of NFC, time pressure and erate time pressure tended to elicit more favorable ad attitudes
message sidedness on ad attitudes, brand attitudes and purchase (MMTP = 4.59, MLTP = 4.25, MHTP = 4.16; MMTP-LTP = 0.34,
intentions are examined in the following hypotheses. p = 0.015; MMTP-HTP = 0.43, p = 0.001; MLTP-HTP = 0.09,
p = 0.732) than low time pressure and high time pressure. In addi-
H1: Effects of message sidedness on persuasion. The ANOVA tion, moderate time pressure tended to elicit more favorable brand
analysis indicated that message sidedness had a marginally signifi- attitudes than high time pressure, but did not elicit more favorable
cant main effect on ad attitudes (F(1, 334) = 3.63, p = 0.058, brand attitudes than low time pressure (MMTP = 4.13, MLTP = 3.98,
gp2 = 0.011), but had no statistically significant main effect on MHTP = 3.74; MMTP-LTP = 0.15, p = 0.44; MMTP-HTP = 0.39,
brand attitudes (F(1, 334) = 1.24, p = 0.267, gp2 = 0.004) and p = 0.004; MLTP-HTP = 0.24, p = 0.129). Likewise, moderate time
purchase intentions (F(1, 334) = 0.49, p = 0.486, gp2 = 0.001, pressure tended to elicit more favorable purchase intentions than
see Table 2). The further t-test revealed that message sidedness high time pressure, but did not elicit more favorable purchase inten-
marginally affected ad attitudes (Mone-sided = 4.24, Mtwo-sided = tions than low time pressure (MMTP = 3.95, MLTP = 3.81,
4.42, t(334) = 1.91, p = 0.058), but did not differentially affect MHTP = 3.37; MMTP-LTP = 0.13, p = 0.543; MMTP-HTP = 0.58,
brand attitudes (Mone-sided = 3.89, Mtwo-sided = 4.00, t(334) = 1.11, p = 0.000; MLTP-HTP = 0.45, p = 0.001, see Table 3). Therefore, H3
p = 0.267) and purchase intentions (Mone-sided = 3.67, Mtwo-sided = was partially supported.
3.74, t(334) = 0.70, p = 0.49, see Table 3). Therefore, H1 was
partially supported. H4: Effects of high NFC, time pressure and message sidedness
on persuasion. Two-way ANOVAs were administered to examine
H2: Effects of NFC on persuasion. The ANOVA analysis indi- the interaction effect of message sidedness · time pressure on ad
cated that NFC had a significant main effect on ad attitudes (F(1, attitudes, brand attitudes and purchase intentions.
334) = 264.66, p = 0.000, gp2 = 0.442), brand attitudes (F(1,
334) = 379.68, p = 0.000, gp2 = 0.532) and purchase intentions (1) Ad attitudes (Aad). Under the high-NFC condition, the simple
(F(1, 334) = 34.31, p = 0.000, gp2 = 0.093, see Table 2). The interaction effects of message sidedness · time pressure on ad

Table 2. Individual main effects of NFC, time pressure and message sidedness on ad attitudes, brand attitudes and purchase intentions (comparison of
F and p values)

NFC Time pressure Message sidedness

Brand Purchase Brand Purchase Brand Purchase


Ad attitudes attitudes intentions Ad attitudes attitudes intentions Ad attitudes attitudes intentions

F 264.66 379.68 34.31 7.58 5.59 12.58 3.63 1.24 0.49


p 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.001 0.004 0.000 0.058 0.267 0.486
gp2 0.442 0.532 0.093 0.044 0.032 0.070 0.011 0.004 0.001

Table 3. Individual main effects of NFC, message sidedness and time pressure on ad attitudes, brand attitudes and purchase intentions (comparison of
mean values)

NFC Time pressure Message sidedness

Mean Low High Low Moderate High One-sided Two-sided

Ad attitudes 3.74 4.92 4.25 4.59 4.16 4.24 4.42


Brand attitudes 3.30 4.60 3.98 4.13 3.74 3.89 4.00
Purchase intentions 3.42 3.99 3.81 3.95 3.37 3.67 3.74

 2011 The Author.


Scandinavian Journal of Psychology  2011 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
Scand J Psychol 52 (2011) Message sidedness in advertising 335

Table 4. Ad attitudes across NFC · Time pressure · Message sidedness conditions

High-NFC Low-NFC

LTP MTP HTP LTP MTP HTP

Ad attitudes 2-S 1-S 2-S 1-S 2-S 1-S 2-S 1-S 2-S 1-S 2-S 1-S

Mean 5.64 4.40 5.30 5.02 4.65 4.74 3.32 3.69 4.47 3.75 3.17 3.79
S.D. 0.24 1.27 0.25 0.44 0.25 0.21 0.33 0.51 0.37 0.42 0.29 0.34
t 4.81 2.71 )1.59 )3.02 7.31 )6.72
p 0.000 0.01 0.118 0.004 0.000 0.000

attitudes (F(2, 162) = 18.59, p = 0.000), brand attitudes (F(2, (Mtwo-sided = 3.17, Mone-sided = 3.79, t(46) = )6.72, p = 0.000)
162) = 12.24, p = 0.000) and purchase intentions (F(2, and under low time pressure (Mtwo-sided = 3.32, Mone-sided = 3.69,
162) = 13.46, p = 0.000) were significant. The further t-test t(53) = )3.02, p = 0.004). In sharp contrast, under the moderate
for Aad revealed that message sidedness did not differentially time pressure context, low-NFC individuals exhibited more favor-
affect the ad attitudes (Mtwo-sided = 4.65, Mone-sided = 4.74, able ad attitudes toward two-sided messages than one-sided mes-
t (62) = )1.59, p = 0.118) of high-NFC individuals, who were sages (Mtwo-sided = 4.47, Mone-sided = 3.75, t(63) = 7.31,
under high time pressure. However, high-NFC individuals exhib- p = 0.000, see Table 4 and Fig. 1).
ited more favorable ad attitudes toward two-sided messages than (2) Brand attitudes. A similar pattern was evident for brand atti-
one-sided messages when they were under moderate time pressure tudes. The t-test for AB revealed that low-NFC individuals engen-
(Mtwo-sided = 5.30, Mone-sided = 5.02, t(45) = 2.71, p = 0.01) and dered more favorable brand attitudes toward one-sided messages
under low time pressure (Mtwo-sided = 5.64, Mone-sided = 4.40, than two-sided messages when they were under high time pres-
t(55) = 4.81, p = 0.000, see Table 4 and Fig. 1). sure (Mtwo-sided = 2.89, Mone-sided = 3.13, t(46) = )2.31,
(2) Brand attitudes (AB). Similarly, the t-test for AB revealed that p = 0.025) and under low time pressure (Mtwo-sided = 2.83, Mone-
message sidedness did not differentially affect the brand attitudes sided = 3.53, t(53) = )5.65, p = 0.000). In contrast, low-NFC indi-
of high-NFC individuals, who were under high time pressure viduals under moderate time pressure expressed more favorable
(Mtwo-sided = 4.32, Mone-sided = 4.28, t(62) = 0.52, p = 0.604). In brand attitudes toward two-sided messages than one-sided mes-
addition, high-NFC individuals engendered more favorable brand sages (Mtwo-sided = 3.77, Mone-sided = 3.33, t(63) = 5.00,
attitudes toward two-sided messages than one-sided messages p = 0.000, see Table 5 and Fig. 1).
when they were under moderate time pressure (Mtwo-sided = 5.03, (3) Purchase intentions. Similarly, the t-test for PI revealed that
Mone-sided = 4.73, t(45) = 2.60, p = 0.012) and under low time low-NFC individuals expressed more favorable purchase inten-
pressure (Mtwo-sided = 5.31, Mone-sided = 4.24, t(55) = 4.61, tions toward one-sided messages than two-sided messages when
p = 0.000, see Table 5 and Fig. 1). they were under low time pressure (Mone-sided = 3.45, Mtwo-sided =
(3) Purchase intentions (PI). Likewise, the t-test for PI revealed 2.71, t(53) = )4.09, p = 0.000) and under high time pressure
that message sidedness did not differentially affect the purchase (Mone-sided = 3.85, Mtwo-sided = 2.57, t(46) = )7.10, p = 0.000). In
intentions of high-NFC individuals, who were under high time sharp contrast, low-NFC individuals under moderate time pressure
pressure (Mtwo-sided = 3.43, Mone-sided = 3.67, t(62) = )1.56, engendered more favorable purchase intentions toward two-sided
p = 0.125). However, high-NFC individuals expressed more messages than one-sided messages (Mone-sided = 3.50, Mtwo-sided =
favorable purchase intentions toward two-sided messages than 4.22, t(63) = 4.43, p = 0.000, see Table 6 and Fig. 1). Therefore,
one-sided messages when they were under moderate time pressure H5 was supported.
(Mtwo-sided = 4.31, Mone-sided = 3.62, t(45) = 3.18, p = 0.003) and
under low time pressure (Mtwo-sided = 5.12, Mone-sided = 3.97,
t(55) = 4.90, p = 0.000, see Table 6 and Fig. 1). Hence, H4 was
DISCUSSION
supported.
Theoretical implications of hypothesis testing
H5: Effects of low NFC, time pressure and message sidedness on
persuasion. Two-way ANOVAs were administered to examine the The results for H1 indicate that message sidedness has a margin-
interaction effect of message sidedness · time pressure on ad atti- ally significant main effect on ad attitudes, but has no statistically
tudes, brand attitudes and purchase intentions. significant impact on brand attitudes and purchase intentions.
Nonetheless, the ANOVA results reveal that brand attitudes and
(1) Ad attitudes. Under the low-NFC condition, the simple inter-
purchase intentions may be affected by message sidedness to
action effects of message sidedness · time pressure on ad atti-
some extent, which implies that message sidedness can still elicit
tudes (F(2, 162) = 47.61, p = 0.000), brand attitudes (F(2,
different levels of persuasion (even the mean values of two-sided
162) = 31.83, p = 0.000) and purchase intentions (F(2,162) =
messages are marginally or no significantly greater than those of
36.41, p = 0.000) were significant. The further t-test for Aad
one-sided messages). Considering the HSM and the resource
revealed that one-sided messages were more effective than two-
matching theory, two-sided messages (incorporating both positive
sided messages for low-NFC individuals under high time pressure

 2011 The Author.


Scandinavian Journal of Psychology  2011 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
336 D. T. Kao Scand J Psychol 52 (2011)

Fig. 1. Interactions of Message sidedness · NFC · Time pressure on dependent variables.

and negative information) require more cognitive resources for usually scrutinize information systematically, whereas low-NFC
systematic scrutiny than one-sided messages (incorporating posi- individuals tend to process information heuristically. When high-
tive information only). Namely, when individuals are exposed to NFC individuals are exposed to message arguments, they tend to
one-sided messages, they tend to engage in limited cognitive systematically scrutinize these arguments. If these arguments are
endeavors and heuristically process the positive information. acceptable, persuasion will occur. Alternatively, when low-NFC
However, when individuals are exposed to two-sided messages, individuals are exposed to message arguments, they tend to ignore
they are likely to apply comparatively fewer cognitive resources or heuristically process these arguments. In other words, high-
to heuristically process the positive information and apply com- NFC individuals are more likely to generate positive cognitive
paratively more cognitive resources to systematically process the responses toward message arguments than low-NFC individuals.
negative information. Moreover, the negative information in two- The main effect of NFC on persuasion also reveals that high-NFC
sided messages is expected to mitigate individuals’ intrinsic cog- individuals tend to express more favorable ad attitudes, brand atti-
nitive resistance and increase the probability of accepting these tudes and purchase intentions than low-NFC individuals.
messages. However, this research demonstrates that, except for ad The results for H3 indicate that time pressure has a significant
attitudes, individuals tend to generate no differentially favorable impact on persuasion. Considering the HSM and the resource
brand attitudes and purchase intentions toward either one-sided or matching theory, individuals under the moderate time pressure tend
two-sided messages, which imply that the message sidedness to be motivated to apply available cognitive resources to systemati-
effect can be moderated when it interacts with other factors. This cally process information; on the contrary, when the time pressure
implication is consistent with the assumption in the introduction is low or high, individuals are usually unmotivated or unable to
section of this research. apply available cognitive resources to systematically process infor-
The results for H2 support the notion that NFC has a significant mation. In other words, persuasion is more likely to occur when
impact on persuasion. As the HSM implied, high-NFC individuals individuals are under moderate time pressure than under low or

 2011 The Author.


Scandinavian Journal of Psychology  2011 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
Scand J Psychol 52 (2011) Message sidedness in advertising 337

Table 5. Brand attitudes across NFC · Time pressure · Message sidedness conditions

High-NFC Low-NFC

LTP MTP HTP LTP MTP HTP


Brand
attitudes 2-S 1-S 2-S 1-S 2-S 1-S 2-S 1-S 2-S 1-S 2-S 1-S

Mean 5.31 4.24 5.03 4.73 4.32 4.28 2.83 3.53 3.77 3.33 2.89 3.13
S.D. 0.24 1.14 0.28 0.49 0.25 0.37 0.34 0.52 0.38 0.29 0.36 0.35
t 4.61 2.60 0.52 )5.65 5.00 )2.31
p 0.000 0.012 0.604 0.000 0.000 0.025

Table 6. Purchase intentions across NFC · Time pressure · Message sidedness conditions

High-NFC Low-NFC

LTP MTP HTP LTP MTP HTP


Purchase
intentions 2-S 1-S 2-S 1-S 2-S 1-S 2-S 1-S 2-S 1-S 2-S 1-S

Mean 5.12 3.97 4.31 3.62 3.43 3.67 2.71 3.45 4.22 3.50 2.57 3.85
S.D. 0.93 0.84 0.79 0.67 0.63 0.59 0.66 0.68 0.67 0.61 0.63 0.59
t 4.90 3.18 )1.56 )4.09 4.43 )7.10
p 0.000 0.003 0.125 0.000 0.000 0.000

high time pressure. The main effect of time pressure on persuasion the time pressure is low, and lack both motivation and cognitive
indicates that, as compared with low time pressure and high time resources to systematically assess the cognitively demanding
pressure, moderate time pressure tends to lead to more favorable information when the time pressure is high. For low-NFC individ-
ad attitudes. However, moderate time pressure tends to elicit more uals, one-sided messages are easier to process and thus are likely
favorable brand attitudes and purchase intentions than high time to have a higher acceptance probability than two-sided messages.
pressure, but does not elicit more favorable brand attitudes and Interestingly, this research indicates that low-NFC individuals can
purchase intentions than low time pressure. be motivated to process information by moderate time pressure.
The results for H4 support the notion that the message sided- The further analysis concludes that low-NFC individuals under
ness effect does not activate when high-NFC individuals are under moderate time pressure tend to express more favorable ad atti-
high time pressure, whereas two-sided messages are more persua- tudes, brand attitudes and purchase intentions toward two-sided
sive than one-sided messages when high-NFC individuals are messages than one-sided messages.
under low or moderate time pressure. As the HSM and the
resource matching theory implied, when high-NFC individuals are
under high time pressure, they lack the required cognitive Theoretical contributions
resources to systematically assess information and thus tend to From an academic perspective, theoretical understanding of mes-
engender no differentially favorable ad attitudes, brand attitudes sage sidedness effects is promising but underdeveloped. Some
and purchase intentions toward either two-sided messages or one- empirical studies in the literature of consumer behavior have
sided messages. Alternatively, when high-NFC individuals are examined the effect of message sidedness (e.g., Becker-Olsen,
under low or moderate time pressure, they are motivated to apply 2006; Belch, 1981; Czuchry & Gray, 2009; Hastak & Park, 1990;
the available cognitive resources to heuristically process the Kamins & Assael, 1987); however, there is substantial disagree-
positive information and systematically scrutinize the negative ment regarding whether one-sided or two-sided messages are
information in two-sided messages simultaneously. However, more persuasive. This disagreement can be attributed to some
one-sided messages containing positive information only cannot internal (such as personal traits) and external (such as time con-
attract high-NFC individuals for further elaboration. Therefore, straints) factors, which moderate the persuasiveness of message
high-NFC individuals under low or moderate time pressure may sidedness. This research successfully links the internal and exter-
express more favorable ad attitudes, brand attitudes and purchase nal factors and explores their interaction effects on persuasion.
intentions toward two-sided messages than one-sided messages. This research differs from the prior research in three main
The results for H5 support the notion that low-NFC individuals respects, which contribute to the persuasion literature. First, this
under low or high time pressure prefer one-sided messages to research is the first to provide empirical evidence that NFC mod-
two-sided messages; however, low-NFC individuals under moder- erates the persuasive impact of message sidedness. Although NFC
ate time pressure may more favorably evaluate two-sided mes- has been identified as a potential moderating factor in previous
sages than one-sided messages. Considering the HSM and the research (e.g., Martin et al., 2003; Sawyer & Howard, 1991), its
resource matching theory, low-NFC individuals intrinsically lack influence on the persuasive impact of message sidedness has not
motivation to systematically process complex information when been examined yet. This research supports the belief that NFC has

 2011 The Author.


Scandinavian Journal of Psychology  2011 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
338 D. T. Kao Scand J Psychol 52 (2011)

an influence on the message sidedness effect. In addition, this ers are advised to adopt a two-sided message strategy with a
research speculates that the message sidedness effect on persua- reasonable period of promotion duration.
sion is not only driven by changes in internal mental states (Celsi
& Olson, 1988), but also other external factors may drive the per-
suasion changes. Limitations and future research
Second, an important methodological issue is that most previ- First, while message sidedness lies well within the control of
ous research regarding time pressure has used two levels of time advertisers as an ad strategy, advertisers may wonder how to
pressure: either no time pressure vs. time pressure or low time make use of findings relating to NFC. Advertisers may face a
pressure vs. high time pressure. Such previous research (e.g., Dhar tough task to precisely measure the NFC levels of message recip-
& Nowlis, 1999; Svenson, Edland & Slovic, 1990) could not test ients before their ad strategies are devised. Luna and Peracchio
the curvilinear effects of time pressure; however, using three lev- (2002) contend that NFC offers additional information for market
els of time pressure can effectively fill the gap (Suri & Monroe, segmentation, where segments can be classified as high-NFC or
2003). This research applies three levels of time pressure to low-NFC. Martin et al. (2003) suggest that one approach to dis-
empirically examine the curvilinear relationship between message tinguish high-NFC from low-NFC is to study the nature of the
sidedness and persuasion. media vehicle, in which the ad is to be placed. A judgment can
At last, previous literature has indicated that low-NFC individu- be made regarding the NFC levels of the readers in the target
als are usually unmotivated or slightly motivated to spend cogni- markets, based on preferred content and featured articles. For
tive effort on processing complex problems (e.g., Cacioppo & example, investment or business magazines that offer corporate
Petty, 1982; Kivetz & Simonson, 2000). However, this research case studies tend to be cognitively demanding (Meyers-Levy &
empirically supports that low-NFC individuals under moderate Peracchio, 1995) and thus the readership profile for these maga-
time pressure can be motivated to invest cognitive effort in zines may be closer to high-NFC than low-NFC. In contrast, the
processing the complex information and favorably evaluate the reader profile for magazines, which are less text-based or high-
cognitively demanding tasks (such as two-sided messages). light the news of movie celebrities, may be closer to low-NFC
than high-NFC.
Second, the use of a student sample may be said to limit the
Practical implications generalizability of the results and caution must be mentioned
This research suggests that the message sidedness effect can be when giving advice to advertisers in real business contexts. None-
moderated by other factors, which implies that advertisers are theless, it is predicted that advertisers may benefit from these find-
advised to adopt an ad message sidedness strategy together with ings in this research. Future research is expected to extend these
other marketing stimuli or market segmentation. In addition, as findings to other populations.
compared with low-NFC individuals, high-NFC individuals are Chang and Chou (2008) indicate that involvement may affect
more likely to generate positive cognitive responses toward the persuasion effects; however, their research does not include
advertisements and become potential consumers. Moreover, a rea- involvement as a covariate or a moderator to affect persuasion.
sonable period of promotion duration can be more effective than a McQuarrie and Phillips (2005) also do not presuppose a high
long period or a short period of promotion duration. degree of consumer involvement, instead relying on more sponta-
Furthermore, in the experiments of this research, time pressure neous generation of implicatures. Nonetheless, an important task
manipulation was designed for simulating the scenarios in the real for future research is to study whether involvement can moderate
world and for examining how subjects reacted to the ad stimulus persuasion.
under different levels of time pressure. In the real world, the time At last, some more moderators pertaining to personality traits,
length that individuals are exposed to a commercial film or a print such as regulatory focus (prevention-focused vs. promotion-focused
ad is usually no longer than 1 minute. In addition, it is common individuals) and risk tendency (risk-averse vs. risk-seeking), can be
that time pressure is manipulated by seconds or minutes in TV investigated in the effect of message sidedness on persuasion in
shopping channels, such as ‘‘the unprecedented special price for the future research. In addition, the future studies on message sid-
the last 60 seconds’’. In other words, different lengths of promo- edness are encouraged to extend the persuasion effect from adver-
tion duration can affect how strongly consumers are motivated to tising to more social science fields, such as laws and politics.
process the ad messages. The time pressure manipulation by sec-
onds in this research does not mean that advertisers can have their The author wishes to thank the editor Dr Magnus Lindgren and the anony-
promotion durations counted in seconds only. Instead, advertisers mous reviewers for their insightful comments, as well as the National
Science Council of Taiwan for their financial supports (NSC 95-2416 -H-
can manipulate their promotion durations counted in minutes,
158 -014 and NSC 97-2410-H-158-007-MY2).
hours, days, weeks, even months, depending on their marketing
goals.
This research implies that, when the promotional offers for REFERENCES
high-NFC individuals span over a very short period, there are no
Allen, M. (1993). Determining the persuasiveness of message sidedness:
differences in persuasion between a one-sided and a two-sided A prudent note about utilizing research summaries. Western Journal
message strategy. However, advertisers are advised to choose a of Communication, 57, 98–103.
two-sided ad message strategy with a reasonable period or a long Baker, S. M. & Petty, R. E. (1994). Majority and minority influence:
period of promotion to persuade high-NFC individuals. Alterna- Source-position imbalance as a determinant of message scrutiny.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 5–19.
tively, for the potential consumers who are low in NFC, advertis-

 2011 The Author.


Scandinavian Journal of Psychology  2011 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
Scand J Psychol 52 (2011) Message sidedness in advertising 339

Batra, R. & Stayman, D. (1990). The role of mood in advertising effec- Hovland, C. I., Lumsdaine, A. A. & Sheffield, F. D. (1949). Experiments
tiveness. Journal of Consumer Research, 17, 203–214. on mass communication. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Becker-Olsen, K. (2006). Music-visual congruency and its impact Kamins, M. A. (1989). Celebrity and noncelebrity advertising in a two-
on two-sided message recall. Advances in Consumer Research, 33, sided context. Journal of Advertising Research, 29, 34–42.
578–580. Kamins, M. A. & Assael, H. (1987). Moderating disconfirmation of
Belch, G. E. (1981). An examination of comparative and noncomparative expectations through the use of two-sided appeals: A longitudinal
television commercials: The effects of claim variation and repetition approach. Journal of Economic Psychology, 8, 237–253.
on cognitive response and message acceptance. Journal of Marketing Kamins, M. A., Brand, M. J., Hoeke, S. A. & Moe, J. C. (1989). Two-
Research, 18, 333–349. sided versus one-sided celebrity endorsements: The impact on
Benson, L. & Beach, L. (1996). The effects of time constraints on the advertising effectiveness and credibility. Journal of Advertising, 18,
pre-choice screening of decision options. Organizational Behavior 4–10.
and Human Decision Processes, 67, 222–228. Kanouse, D. E. (1984). Explaining negativity biases in evaluation and
Biswas, A. & Sherrell, D. (1993). The influence of product knowledge choice behavior: Theory and research. In T. C. Kinnear (Ed.),
and brand name on internal price standards and confidence. Psychol- Advances in Consumer Research (vol. 11, pp. 703—708). Provo, UT:
ogy and Marketing, 10, 31–46. Association for Consumer Research.
Bohner, G., Einwiller, S., Erb, H. P. & Siebler, F. (2003). When small Kanungo, R. N. & Johar, J. S. (1975). Effects of slogan and human
means comfortable: Relations between product attributes in two-sided model characteristics in product advertisements. Canadian Journal of
advertising. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13, 454–463. Behavioral Science, 7, 127–138.
Brennan, I. & Bahn, K. D. (2006). Literal versus extended symbolic Kao, D. T. (2006). The impact of ad message framing on consumer atti-
messages and advertising effectiveness: The moderating role of need tudes: The moderating roles of MOA. PhD dissertation, National Cen-
for cognition. Psychology and Marketing, 23, 273–295. tral University, Taiwan.
Cacioppo, J. & Petty, R. (1982). The need for cognition. Personality and Kao, D. T. (2007). Conclusion explicitness in message communication:
Social Psychology, 42, 116–131. The roles of NFC and knowledge in attitude formation. Social Behav-
Cacioppo, J., Petty, R. & Kao, C. F. (1984). The efficient assessment of ior and Personality, 35, 819–826.
NFC. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 306–307. Keller, P. A. & Block, L. G. (1997). Vividness effects: A resource match-
Celsi, R. L. & Olson, J. C. (1988). The role of involvement in attention ing perspective. Journal of Consumer Research, 24, 295–304.
and comprehension processes. Journal of Consumer Research, 15, Kivetz, R. & Simonson, I. (2000). The effects of incomplete information
210–224. on consumer choice. Journal of Marketing Research, 37, 427–449.
Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic processing and the use of Lafferty, B. A. (1999). The influence of consumer persuasion knowledge
source vs. message cues in persuasion. Journal of Personality and on advertising persuasiveness. American Marketing Association, Con-
Social Psychology, 39, 752–766. ference Proceedings, 10, 18.
Chaiken, S., Giner-Sorolla, R. & Chen, S. (1996). Beyond accuracy: Lang, B., Lee, C. K. & Zwick, R. (1999). Message sidedness at the
Defense and impression motives in heuristic and systematic informa- brand and product form levels: Overcoming the shortcomings of two-
tion processing. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (eds.), The psy- sided messages?. Advances in Consumer Research, 26, 485–491.
chology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior. Larson, C. U. (2010). Persuasion: reception and responsibility (12th
New York: Guilford Press. edn). Boston: Wadsworth.
Chang, C. C. & Chou, Y. J. (2008). Goal orientation and comparative Liebert, R. M. & Morris, L. W. (1967). Cognitive and emotional compo-
valence in persuasion. Journal of Advertising, 37, 73–87. nents of test anxiety: A distinction and some initial data. Psychologi-
Chang, Y. (1996). The effect of humor in advertising: An individual-dif- cal Reports, 20, 975–978.
ference perspective. Psychology and Marketing, 13, 531–545. Lim, J. S., Lin, S. Y., Ferguson, M. & Kiousis, S. (2008). The effect of
Chu, G. C. (1967). Prior familiarity, perceived bias, and one-sided versus message sidedness on CSR perceptions and company evaluations.
two-sided communications. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol- International Communication Association, Conference Proceedings,
ogy, 3, 243–254. Dresden International Congress Centre, Dresden, Germany.
Coulter, K. S. & Punj, G. N. (2004). The effects of cognitive resource Luna, D. & Peracchio, L. A. (2002). ‘‘Where there is a will ...’’: Motiva-
requirements, availability and argument quality on brand attitudes: A tion as a moderator of language processing by bilingual consumers.
melding of elaboration likelihood and cognitive resource matching Psychology and Marketing, 19, 573–593.
theories. Journal of Advertising, 33, 53–64. Martin, B., Lang, B. & Wong, S. (2003). Conclusion explicitness in
Czuchry, M. & Gray, B. (2009). Influence of message sidedness, pic- advertising: The moderating role of need for cognition (NFC) and
tures, and need for cognition on beliefs and behavior: The Terri Schi- argument quality (AQ) on persuasion. Journal of Advertising, 32,
avo case. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 39, 762–789. 57–65.
Dhar, R. & Nowlis, S. (1999). The effect of time pressure on consumer Martin, B., Sherrard, M. J. & Wentzel, D. (2005). The role of sensation
choice deferral. Journal of Consumer Research, 25, 369–384. seeking and need for cognition on web-site evaluations: A resource-
Earl, R. L. & Pride, W. M. (1980). The effects of advertisement struc- matching perspective. Psychology & Marketing, 22, 109–126.
ture, message sidedness, and performance test results on print adver- McQuarrie, E. F. & Phillips, B. J. (2005). Indirect persuasion in advertis-
tisement informativeness. Journal of Advertising, 9, 36–45. ing: How consumers process metaphors presented in pictures and
Eagly, A. H. & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Orlando, words. Journal of Advertising, 34, 7–20.
FL: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College. Megehee, C. M. (2009). Advertising time expansion, compression, and
Eysenck, M. W. & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The cognitive processing influences on consumer acceptance of message
processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409–434. and brand. Journal of Business Research, 62, 420–431.
Fiske, S. T. (1980). Attention and weight in person perception: The Meyerowitz, B. E. & Chaiken, S. (1987). The effect of message framing
impact of negative and extreme behavior. Journal of Personality and on breast self-examination attitudes, intentions, and behavior. Journal
Social Psychology, 38, 889–906. of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 500–510.
Golden, L. L. & Alpert, M. I. (1987). Comparative analysis of the rela- Meyers-Levy, J. & Peracchio, L. A. (1995). Understanding the effects of
tive effectiveness of one- and two-sided communication for contrast- color: How the correspondence between available and required
ing products. Journal of Advertising, 16, 18–25. resources affects attitudes. Journal of Consumer Research, 22, 121–
Hastak, M. & Park, J. W. (1990). Mediators of message sidedness effects 138.
on cognitive structure for involved and uninvolved audiences. In G. Nam, X. (2008). The pursuit of self-regulatory goals: How counterfactual
Marvin & R. Pollay (Eds.), Advances in consumer research (pp. thinking influences advertising persuasiveness. Journal of Advertis-
329–336). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research. ing, 37, 17–27.

 2011 The Author.


Scandinavian Journal of Psychology  2011 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
340 D. T. Kao Scand J Psychol 52 (2011)

O’Keefe, D. J. (1999). How to handle opposing arguments in persuasive Suri, R. & Monroe, K. B. (2003). The effects of time constraints on con-
messages: A meta-analytic review of the effects of one-sided and sumers’ judgments of prices and products. Journal of Consumer
two-sided messages. In M. E. Roloff (ed.), Communication yearbook, Research, 30, 92–104.
(vol. 22, pp. 209–249). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Svenson, O., Edland, A. & Slovic, P. (1990). Choices and judgments of
Ordonez, L. & Benson, L. (1997). Decisions under time pressure: How incompletely described decision alternatives under time pressure. Acta
time constraint affects risky decision making. Organizational Behav- Psychologica, 75, 153–169.
ior and Human Decision Processes, 71, 121–140. Swanson, L. A. (1987). The persuasive effect of volunteering negative
Pechmann, C. (1992). Predicting when two-sided ads will be more effec- information in advertising. International Journal of Advertising, 6,
tive than one-sided ads: The role of correlational and correspondent 237–248.
inferences. Journal of Marketing Research, 29, 441–453. Swinyard, W. R. (1981). The interaction between comparative advertising
Peracchio, L. A. & Meyers-Levy, J. (1997). Evaluating persuasion- and copy claim variation. Journal of Marketing Research, 18, 175–
enhancing techniques from a resource-matching perspective. Journal 186.
of Consumer Research, 24, 178–191. Teng, L. (2009). A comparison of two types of price discounts in shifting
Petty, R. E. & Cacioppo, J. T. (1981). Attitudes and persuasion: Classic consumers’ attitudes and purchase intentions. Journal of Business
and contemporary approaches. Dubuque, IA: Wm. C. Brown. Research, 62, 14–21.
Petty, R. E. & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model Verplanken, P. T., Hazenberg, P. T. & Palenwen, G. R. (1992). Need for
of persuasion. In Leonard Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental cognition and external information search effort. Journal of Research
social psychology (vol. 19, pp. 123–205). New York: Academic in Personality, 26, 128–136.
Press. Wegener, D. T. & Petty, R. E. (2001). Understanding effects of mood
Petty, R. E., Unnava, R. H. & Strathman, A. J. (1991). Theories of atti- through the elaboration likelihood and flexible correction models. In
tude change. In T. S. Robertson & H. H. Kassarjian (Eds.), Hand- L. L. Martin & G. L. Clore (Eds.), Theories of mood and cognition:
book of consumer behavior (pp. 241–280). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: A users’ guidebook (pp. 177–210). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Prentice Hall. Associates.
Pham, M. T. & Avnet, T. (2004). Ideals and oughts and the reliance on Williams-Piehota, P., Schneider, T. R., Pizarro, J., Mowad, L. & Salovey,
affect vs. substance in persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, P. (2003). Matching health messages to information processing styles:
30, 503–518. Need for cognition and mammography utilization. Health Communi-
Reinhard, M. A. & Messner, M. (2009). The effects of source likeability cation, 15, 375–392.
and need for cognition on advertising effectiveness under explicit per- Wilson, T. D. & Brekke, N. (1994). Mental contamination and mental
suasion. Journal of Consumer Behavior, 8, 179–191. correction: Unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations. Psy-
Sawyer, A. & Howard, D. (1991). Effects of omitting conclusions in chological Bulletin, 116, 117–142.
advertisements to involved and involved audiences. Journal of Mar- Zhang, Y. & Zinkhan, G. M. (2006). Responses to humorous ads: Does
keting Research, 28, 467–474. audience involvement matter? Journal of Advertising, 35, 113–127.
Smith, R. E. & Hunt, S. D. (1978). Attributional processes and effects in
promotional situations. Journal of Consumer Research, 5, 149–158. Received 24 April 2010, accepted 3 February 2011

 2011 The Author.


Scandinavian Journal of Psychology  2011 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.

You might also like