You are on page 1of 12

Res-h Report

Neuromuscular Coordination of Squat Lifting, 11:


Individual Differences

Backgmund and Purpose. 7;bisarticle reports individual drferences in the John P &hok
coordination (ie, the relative timing of joint movements and muscle activity) of Amy G McMillan
squat lrjling identzjkd by extended analysis of data reported in the authors'
companion article in this issue. Subjects. Two post hoc groups of 6 subjects
each were identijied from the original sample of 15 subjects based on qualita-
tive drferences in knee-lumbar spine relative motion plots during load acceler-
ation. Metbods. Subjects lifted a crate containing 15% to 75%of their maxi-
mum lzj2ing capacity using a symmetrical squat-lift technique. Movement
kinematic data were obtained with videography, and the electromyographic
(EMG) activity of the vastus lateralis and erector spinae muscles was recorded
with surface EMG. Measurements of coordination derived both kinematically
and via EMG and the kinematic data were examined for group dzferences.
Resu&s. Subjects in group 2 limited lumbar spine motion during load acceler-
ation for all loads lifted, whereas those in group 1 limited lumbar spine motion
more when lifting the heaviest loads. 7;bese dzyerences were obvious both quali-
tatiuely, via knee-lumbar spine relative motion plots, and quantitatively, via
measures of the relative timing of joint motions early in the lift. Z5e efect of
load on the coordination of these joints was the same for both post hoc groups
afier initial load acceleration. Signijicant dzyerences in other kinematic mea-
suremenrs were alsofound between these groups. Conclusion and Discus-
sion Despite specijic instructions about how to lzj2 the load, individual subjects
coordinated theirjoints d w e n t l y during the initial, accelerativephase of
squat lzfling. Individual dr$erences in coordination in response to load in-
creases could be categorized into two patterns, although the data of 2 subjects
were dtficult to categorize and thus not included in these analyses. Whether
the two dominant patterns have consequencesfor stress to the joints during
lzfling remains to be determined. [ScholzJP, McMillan AG. Neuromuscular
coordination of squat liJting,II: individual dzymnces. Phys mer.
1995;75:13.3- 144.1

Key Wotds: Coordination, Electmmyography, Lijling, Mataials handling, Motlement


kinematics, Spine.

An implicit assumption in much of the


literature on manual lifting appears to
JP Scholz, PhD, PT, is Associate Professor, Physical Therapy Department and Interdisciplinary be that the lifting "technique"used can
Neuroscience Program, University of Delaware. Newark, DE 19716 (USA). Address all correspon-
dence to Dr Scholz. be defined in terms of the body's
starting posture alone. Despite the fact
AG McMillan, PT, is a doctoral candidate in the Interdisciplinary Neuroscience Program, School of
Life and Health Sciences, University of Delaware.
that successful completion of this task
requires the coordination of a large
This study was approved by the Human Subjects Committee of the University of Delaware Re- number of movement components
search Office.
and the solution of a number of
This work was supported by a research grant to Dr Scholz from the Foundation for Physical Ther- potentially conflicting requirements
apy Inc. (eg, maintaining balance, producing a
7hb attick was submitted October 5, 1993, and was accepted September 8, 1994. precise load trajectory, minimizing

Physical Therapy/Volume 75, Number 2 /February 1995


musculoskeletal stress),l few studies to llft loads that were scaled to their appeared to be limited to a few pat-
have directly addressed the coordina- estimated maximum l i n g capacity terns used to accomplish this early
tion (ie, relative timing of joint move- (MLC). Specific measures of coordina- part of the Ilft. The purpose of the
ments and muscle activity) of this task. tion (eg, relative phasing between analyses presented in this article was
One may ask then whether lifts per- joint movements and the relative tim- to further explore the nature of these
formed from the same initial posture ing between electromyogmphic [EMG] apparent individual differences. We
can be assumed to evolve to the final events) were used. Prelimnary evi- were interested in knowing whether
posture in the same fashion under dence was provided that the coordina- individual differences observed quali-
different task conditions? It has been tion of this task can be adequately tatively could also be identified quanti-
suggested that our limited understand- described and studied by measuring a tatively and whether subjects who
ing of the coordination of lifting signif- small number of the many possible differed in their pattern of knee-
icantly limits our understanding of the coordination variables.1° lumbar spine coordination during load
mechanisms of back inj~ry.~23 acceleration also differed on other
To date, our studies have concentrated measures related to task performance.
In the earliest known report that ad- on the effect of changes in load mag-
dressed the coordination of manual nitude on the coordination of a spe- Although individual differences are a
I h g , Davis and colleagues4showed cific lifting technique (ie, squat l h g ) . common feature of all research, we
that subjects who were instructed to This "technique" is generally the one believe that exploring these dlfferences
llft using :3 squat lift tended to extend most recommended by clinicians for formally is important in clinically rele-
their legs faster than their back when minimizing stress to the back, al- vant studies such as this. In therapy,
the load to be lifted was made though controversy exists about we do not evaluate or treat the mean
heavier. Measures of coordination whether this is always the case.596 patient, but the individual. Thus, sensi-
were not used, however, and all sub- Despite the provision of specfic and tivity to individual differences in pre-
jects lifted the same loads despite consistent instructions to subjects, our sentation of symptoms and response
possible (runreported) dlfferences in results show that the coordination of to treatment is a hallmark of effective
individual strength or lifting capacity. squat lifting changes sigmficantly in therapeutic intervention. The fact that
Only one article3 could be found in the face of changes in the load to be most of our subjects exhibited patterns
the 20 years following this report that lifted (see our other article in this of coordination that could be catego-
addressed the coordination of t h ~ s i~sue).~JO The greatest change in coor- rized into two groups may also have
task, albeit indirectly. dination has been found to occur future significance for diagnostic clas-
between motion of the knee and sification of patients with back pain.
Several recent studies have considered motion of the lumbar spine. In an-
the coordination of lifting, although other article in this issue, we report
many of these studies were limited in the extension of our earlier findings
scope and used somewhat indirect on college students to industrial work- Methodological details about the ex-
methods for assessing coordination.58 ers. Changes in the timing of EMG periments are presented in our com-
Schipplein et a19 recently provided events of the vastus lateralis muscle panion article in this issue. Only the
indirect evidence that subjects convert (VL) and the erector spinae muscle most pertinent information is pre-
their lifting technique from one s i d a r (ES) were shown to be generally con- sented here.
to a squat lift to more of a back lift as sistent with changes in knee-lumbar
the load to be lifted is made heavier. spine coordination defined kinemati-
Interpretation of their results is compli- cally, using relative phase (&) vari-
cated, however, by the fact that sub- ables. Moreover, the body's coordina- Fifteen male subjects, ranging in age
jects were allowed to lift using a tech- tion during the lowering phase of the from 26 to 52 years (%?SDI=
nique of their choice (ie, freestyle), task was shown to be relatively unaf- 35.127.61, were recruited as the sam-
and information was not provided on fected by changes in the load. ple of convenience for the study.
the initial posture from which the lift Physical characteristics of the sample
was initiated. Despite the overall consistency of our are presented in the part I article.
results across subjects, we noted indi- Techmcal diaculties resulted in some
Thus, indications have appeared in the vidual differences in how subjects lost data for 1 subject. Based on quali-
literature that the coordination of lift- performed certain aspects of the 1A. tative analyses of knee-lumbar spine
ing is affected by manipulating task Specifically, a qualitative analysis of relative motion plots, an attempt was
variables such as the amount of load knee-lumbar spine coordination using made to categorize the coordination
being lifted. Our own experiments angle-angle or relative motion plots patterns of the 14 remaining subjects.
were designed to explore the coordi- (see Fig. 4 in our other article in this Two dominant patterns of coordina-
nation of manual I h g more system- issue) revealed individual differences tion during load acceleration were
atically than was the case in previous in the effect of load on the coordina- apparent (see the "Results" section).
work (see our companion article, part tion of the initial, accelerative phase of The data of 2 subjects could not be
I, in this issue).lJOSubjects were asked the lift. Moreover, these differences easily categorized into either pattern.

Physical Therapy /Volume 75, Number 2 /February 1995


Therefore, the analyses presented in qualitative analysis of normalized
this article are for the remaining 12 relative motion plots of diferent joint
subjects, whose patterns could be Subjects were instructed to l~ftthe pairs. The normalization procedure for
ready categorized. weighted crate to the height of their these plots is presented in detail in
waist from a starting posture with the part I. The amplitude of each joint was
knees bent, back relatively straight normalized to [- 1,1]in the process of
(squat lift), and feet positioned sym- calculating the relative phase of the
Reflective markers were attached to metrically. Subjects were carefully joint's motion. Because of intertrial
major landmarks of the right extremi- monitored to ensure that they began diferences in task period, all trials
ties and to the trunk using adhesive each trial in the generally defined were then normalized to 1000h using
Velcro@.*The markers were placed at position and were reminded verbally. linear interpolation. AU 12 normalized
the base of the fifth metatarsal, lateral The exact amount of initial knee flex- trials of a given load condition were
rnalleolus, lateral femoral condyle, ion and back extension at lift onset, then averaged, resulting in an average,
lateral humeral condyle, greater tro- however, was not controlled because normalized movement trajectory for
chanter, posterior superior iliac spine, of a desire to maintain some degree of each joint at each load. The averaged
and immediately inferior to the tip of external validity. Each subject's pre- knee trajectory for each load was then
the acrornial process laterally. A ferred foot position with respect to the plotted against that of the lumbar
marker was positioned on the crate to fixed location of the crate was deter- spine and examined for changes
be aligned with the styloid process of mined from a test trial at 60% MLC. across loads.
the ulna when the subject grasped the This position was then marked on the
crate's handles. For the purpose of floor and maintained as the starting Independent Variables
these experiments, the hand was con- position for all experimental trials.
sidered to be rigidly connected with The independent variables of interest
the forearm. Markers mounted on Data Reduction for these analyses were the percentage
balsa wood fins were placed at the of MLC lifted and the post hoc group,
midline of the spinous process of the The motion of the reflective markers defined based on knee-lumbar spine
1st sacral 6 - 0 , 3rd lumbar (L-3), 12th during each task repetition was digi- coordination during load acceleration
thoracic (T-12), and 7th cervical (C-7) tized, calibrated, and filtered (3 Hz), (see "Results" section).
vertebrae. The two-dimensional mo- yielding two-dimensional data for 12
tion of these markers during each task lifts at each percentage of MLC for Dependent Measures
repetition was videotaped and digi- each subject. A link-segment model
tized using a Peak Performance Tech- was used to calculate sagittal-plane (ie, Relative phase (a. The relative
nologies Inc (PPTI) motion analysis flexion-extension) angles for the an- phase of motion between the knee
~ystem.~ kle, knee, hip, lumbar spine, shoulder, and lumbar spine (&) and between
and elbow joints from the digitized, the knee and hip (&) were calcu-
Electromyographic records were taken calibrated coordinates of the reflective lated as continuous measures of coor-
from the left ES and from the left VL markers. dination.' Technical difficulties limited
using the Therapeutics Unlimited Inc the number of subjects for whom the
Model-544 EMG system.* Preamplifier Electromyographic signals were recti- relative phase of motion between the
electrode assemblies were used to fied using software. Cumulative sum knee and shoulder (&.J could be
detect the EMG signal at the skin histograms of each EMG signal then calculated to five for post hoc group 1
surface. The EMG signals were ampli- were obtained using the procedure of and three for post hoc group 2. This
fied (10K gain factor), high-pass fil- Ellaway. The histograms were then measure, therefore, was not examined
tered (75-Hz cutom, and digitized used to determine the onset and peak for group diferences. The within-lift
on-line at a rate of 360 Hz. of EMG activity on each lifting- mean relative phase (eg, &)across
lowering cycle. all samples was obtained for each lift,
Prior to each experiment, each sub- and the mean of this value across all
ject's MLC was determined by having Determination of Post Hoc trials of a load condition per subject
him perform a maximumeffort lifting Groups was calculated for statistical analyses.
motion against a load cell from a
squatting position, with the knees The basis for identifying individual Relative timing. To help distinguish
flexed and the back relatively straight. diierences in performance was the coordination daerences between the
period of initial load acceleration and
later in the lift cycle, discrete relative
- - - - - - - - -- -- - - -- timing measurements for knee-lumbar
'Velcro USA Inc, 406 Brown Ave, Manchester, NH 03108. spine and knee-hip coordination were
calculated at four diferent points in
+peak Performance Technologies Inc, 7388 Revere Pkwy, Suite 601, Englewood, CO 80112-9765.
the lift cycle.1° For purposes of this
%erapeutics Unlimited Inc, 2835 Fnendship St, Iowa City, IA 52245. calculation, the knee was defined as

60 / 135 Physical Therapy /Volume 75, Number 2 / February 1995


the reference joint. The four relative to index the body's posture at lift position existed between post hoc
timing measures per lift cycle are onset. groups.
characterized as the relative time in
the knee extension cycle (onset of 3. Task period. The period (in milli- 5. Horizontal velocity of the COM.
knee extension to maximum exten- seconds) of the lifting phase of the Indirect evidence has been provided
sion) during a lift at which the target task was assessed for each task cycle. that, when rising from a chair, subjects
joint (lumbar spine or hip) reaches convert initial horizontal momentum
I@?,30%, 50%, and 75% of full exten- 4. Location of crate and body center into vertical momentum to assist with
sion. That is, if T,, and T,, repre- of muss (COM) at lif2off The location rising.12If this represents a general
sent the times at which the knee be- of the crate and the body's COM with strategy, then one could expect it to
gins and c.:ompletes extending, respect to the head of the fifth meta- be used to assist with rising in the
respectively, and T,, is the time at tarsal (5MTH) was determined for lifting task as well. If so, greater poste-
which the hip joint reaches 50% of full each trial (1) immediately before liftoff rior horizontal momentum would be
extension during the lift, then and (2) at 15% of the period of vertical expected prior to lifting heavier loads.
crate movement after liftoff. At the The peak horizontal velocity of the
latter point in the lift cycle, the crate body's COM prior to liftoff of the crate
had achieved, on average, 5 cm of was examined to determine whether
vertical excursion, or less than 8%of peak velocity differed across loads and
where (TGtop- TKstart)is the period full excursion. The relative crate loca- between the two post hoc groups.
of knee extension. tion was with respect to the ctate
marker, which was centered on the Statistical Analyses
Electromyogaphic relative timing. upper edge of the side of the crate.
The relative time of ES and VL onsets The effects of load, post hoc group,
and peaks of EMG activity within a lift The starting crate location with respect and their interaction were tested by
cycle was determined from the cumu- to the feet was self-selected and fixed repeated-measures analyses of vari-
lative sum histograms as, for example, for each subject for all lifts. All subjects ance (ANOVAs) using the SYSTAT
chose a crate location that placed the statistical package.s Only the lifting
(2) RT, = (T-, - TJ/T near edge of the crate slightly between phase of the task was examined in
the distal phalanx of the great toes, these analyses. The test of subject
where RTn is the relative time of VL with the feet angled outward slightly. characteristics was done with a
onset of activity in a lifting cycle, T-, Thus, subjects had no room to slide between-groups ANOVA, with group
is absolute time of VL onset of EMG the crate further backward prior to as the only independent variable.
activity, T, is the time of initiation of liftoff and reduce the clockwise torque
vertical crate motion, and T is the produced by the load about the base
period of' the lifting phase of the task of support.
(ie, the time from initiation of vertical Determination of Post Hoc
crate motion to completion of the With a relatively fixed lever arm at Groups
l h g phase). All other EMG timing liftoff,this clockwise torque was deter-
measurements were determined mined largely by the magnitude of the An examination of averaged, normal-
similarly. load. As load became heavier, there- ized relative motion plots from the
fore, the challenge posed to a subject's data presented in part I led to the
Other measures. A number of addi- balance necessarily increased. One identification of individual dlaerences
tional variables were compared be- way to meet this challenge, and pre- in the way subjects coordinated knee-
tween the post hoc groups to deter- sumably reduce the required muscular lumbar spine motion during the accel-
mine whether physical characteristics effort, would be to move the body's erative portion of lifting. The patterns
and other kinematic factors related to COM further backward with respect to exhibited by 12 of the subjects tended
task pedbrmance also differed for the base of support prior to lifting to fall into two categories, whereas the
these groups. These variables included heavier loads. To determine whether patterns exhibited by the other 2 sub-
the following: this was done and the extent to which jects could not be easily categorized
this "strategy" d8ered among subjects, and are not examined further here.
1. Physical characteristics. Subject the body's COM location with respect Fortuitously, an equal number of sub-
height and weight. to the 5MlR was analyzed. The loca- jects fit into the two diferent patterns
tion of the crate with respect to the exhibited. These patterns are illus-
2. Initialposture. The angle of the 5MTH at liftoff was also analyzed to trated in Figure 1.The relative motion
knee and the angle of the lumbar determine whether differences in crate plots in Figure 1A show that as the
spine at the onset of the lift were used load was made heavy, lumbar spine
extension lagged further behind knee
extension. This was true at all stages
"YSTAT Inc, 1800 Sherman Ave, Evanston, IL 60201. of the lii, including the initial, acceler-

Physical Therapy / Volume 75, Number 2 / February 1995


accelerative portion of the lift, lumbar
spine motion was severely limited
dhile the knee extended when lifting
all loads. In many trials, the spine
A actually flexed slightly during this
initial period. The percentage of the
EXT 1.0 lift period over which lumbar spine
motion was limited varied somewhat
a across subjects in this group, although
d
all subjects clearly showed the effect.
f OS
Based on this finding, we sought to
2
.d
determine whether there were quanti-
,P 0.0 tative indicators of the differences
illustrated in Figure 1. We then asked
s
& whether these differences in knee-
lumbar spine coordination were re-
6 -03 lated to other differences in task per-
3 formance between these groups.

Relative Phase (+)


-1.0
-1.0 -0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0
FLEX Mean & decreased (increased phase
Knee Angle EXT lag of lumbar spine motion) with
B increasing load for both post h a -
groups (F4,,,=56.0, P<.001), as de-
scribed in part I. Ovelall, the post hoc
groups did not differ on this measure
(P=.920). Load and group interacted
to affect & (F4,,=2.6, P<.05). This
effect was due to slight differences in
the order of the group means across
loads. For example, at 15% MLC, &
was -21.3 and -23.9 for groups 1
and 2, respectively, whereas at 75%
MLC, & was -41.6 and -38.5 (Fig.
2).

Increasing the load sigruficantly af-


fected & (F4,40=15.5, P<.001), lead-
ing to a slight increase in the lag of
hlp motion behind knee motion, as
FLEX reported in part I. Although relative
Knee Angle EXT motion plots suggested a possible
dgerence between many subjects in
Figure 1. Awrage, normalized relatiw motion plo& of knee-lumbar spine motion the two post hoc groups at the heavi-
during the l$ingphase of the squatting task for a rpesentatiw subect of ( A l p s t ha- est loads (Fig. 31, neither the group
group 1 (CF) and ( 2 ) p s t hoc gmup 2 UR). Normalized W m u mjointJ2exion and factor (P=,397) nor its interaction with
extension equals - 1.0and 1.O,e@ectitx?Iy. (EXT= extension, FLEX=$'exion, load (P=. 158) had a significant effect
MLC= &mum lifling capacity.) on &. None of the measures of
relative phase variability (eg, standard
ative phase. This pattern is similar to hocgroup 2. Note that during the last deviation of &) differed significantly
that exhibited by a different subject half of the Ilft, the effect of load on the between the groups.
(see Fig. 4 in part I) and constitutes coordination of the knee and spine
the pattern exhibited by all subjects in was similar to that observed for sub- Relative Timing
post hoc group 1. jects in post hoc group 1. That is, lum-
bar spine motion lagged behind knee The effect of load on all four relative
Figure 1B presents a pattern of results motion to a greater extent when llfting timing measures was highly sigruficant
for one representative subject of post heavier loads. In contrast, during the (F4,40>15.0,P<.001). This finding was

62 / 137 Physical Therapy / Volume 75, Number 2 / February 1995


the lag of hip extension behind knee
extension increased with increasing
load. For example, 58.5%, 59.4%,
60.8%,62.7%, and 67.3% of the knee
0- extension cycle had been completed
when the hip achieved 50% of full

-
-O
-10-
Group 1
extension (ie, KH,J, when lifting 15%
to 75% MLC, respectively. There was
no interaction between the group and
-
8 the load (all P>. 19).
2 -20- T

PI Electromyographk Relative
Timing
i? 30-
m
CP Other than the significant effect of
t -40-
load on the relative timing of onsets
(F4,40=6.0,P<.01) and peaks of ES
activity (F4,,=23.9, P<.001), as re-
ported in the companion article in this
-50 issue, there were no sigmiicant effects
15 30 45 60 75 of post hoc group (all D . 1 9 ) on any
of the EMG timing measures.
Percentage of MLC
The relative timing of VL peak activity
Figure 2. m e average relative phase of knee-lumbar spine motion (4& (2 1 sEM) was affected by an interaction of
for subjects in post hoc groups 1 and 2 for each load condition (pwcentage of maxi- group with load (F4,,=4.8, PC.01).
mum lifting capacity [MLCI). The mean relative times of occurrence
of VL peak activity were 36.5% and
expected and consistent with the rela- (all P<.05) except at 75% MLC 31% of the lift cycle at 15% and 75%
tive phase results (see part I). Of (P= .147). MLC, respectively, for group 1. The
greatest interest here was the effect of slight advance in VL peak activity with
group and its interaction with the load Both load and group interacted to increasing load in this group was not
on the relative timing measures at affect the relative time in the knee sigmiicant. For subjects in group 2,
different points in the lifting cycle. extension cycle where spinal exten- however, VL peak activity occurred
sion reached 30% of maximum s i m c a n t l y later in the lift cycle when
The relative time in the cycle of knee (F4 ,=3.1, P<.05). Planned compari- lifting heavy loads (52.8% at 75% MLC)
extension.at which the lumbar spine soh showed that the groups differed compared with light loads (46.2% at
achieved 10% of its motion (ie, KL,J only for the 15% MLC (group 1: 0.530, 15% MLC).
differed between the two post hoc group 2: 0.636) and 30% MLC (group
groups (F,,,,= 14.1, P<.01). The effect 1: 0.575, group 2: 0.674) conditions. Physic~lCharacteristics
is illustrated in Figure 4, which shows Again, lumbar spine motion lagged
that for group 2 the knee extension behind knee motion more at this point Although subjects were slightly taller,
cycle was, on average, more than 45% in the lift cycle for group 2 subjects. on average, in group 2 (1.803 m)
complete by the time the lumbar spine Neither the group (D.650) nor its compared with group 1 (1.789 m),
achieved 10% of full extension. Knee interaction with load ( D . 1 0 ) had a these differences did not approach
extension led lumbar spine extension s i m c a n t effect on relative timing sigmiicance (P= .725). Differences in
less for subjects in group 1. The differ- measures KL, and KL,,. weight between the two groups
ence between the groups appeared to (group 1: 920.3 N, group 2: 812.1 N)
decrease with increasing load, which The relative time in the knee exten- were also not significant (P=.W).
might be expected from the relative sion cycle at which the hip achieved The size of the mean weight difference
motion plots (Fig. 1). That is, the lo?? of full extension was unaffected was primarily due to one subject in
phase lag of lumbar spine motion by the load (P=.221) and did not group 1 who weighed 1,147.6 N.
during load acceleration was similar differ between groups (P=.301). The
between the two groups at the heavi- three measures of knee-hip timing Initial Posturn
est loads. Planned comparisons be- assessed at later points in the lift cycle
tween the group means for KL,, at (ie, KH,, KH,, and KH,,) were also The knee angle at lift onset was aF-
each load condition indicated signifi- unaffected by the group (all D.320), feaed significantly by an interaction
cant differences between the groups but they were affected by an increase between load and post hoc group
in the load (all P<.001). In all trials, (F4,40=7.4,P<.001). As shown in

Physical Therapy /Volume 75, Number 2 / February 1995


than did those in group I (192.0" and
185.5O,respectively) at the onset of the
lift (195°=neutral)l Both groups were
slightly more flexed at the onset of
lifting the heaviest loads (Fig. 6).

Task M o d

There was no sigtllficant difference in


the period of llfting the load between
the two post hoc groups (P=.725), nor
was there an interaction of the group
with load (P=.N6). As noted in our
other article in this issue, the lift pe-
riod increased significantly with in-
creasing load after 45% MLC
(F4,,= 10.0, P<.OOl).

Location of Crate at Liftoff

There was no difference between post


hoc groups in the location of the crate
with respect to the 5MTH, either just
prior to liftoff (P=.580) or immediately
after liftoff (P=.986). The position of
the crate immediately after liftoff, how-
ever, did differ across loads
(F4,,,= 26.9, P<.001). All subjects
moved the crate closer to the 5MTH
when the load was heavier (eg, 15%
MLC=0.264 m, 75% MLC=0.228 m).
Load also interacted with group
(F4,40=8.4,P<.001), but this interac-
tion was due to a significant ddference
in relative crate locations only at 15%
MLC (F,,,,=8.5, PC.05; group
1=0.306 m, group 2=0.256 m).

Location of Body Center of Mass


at Liftoff

The COM was always slightly anterior


to the 5 m immediately prior to the
crate leaving the ground, and more so
for subjects in group 1 (F1,,,=7.2,
Figure 3. Average, normalized relative motion plots of knee-hip motion during the P<.05; group 1=0.052 m, group
lifting phase of the task for a representative subject of ( I ) post hoc group 1 (0and
) 2=0.022 m). Both groups moved the
(2) post hoc group 2 (m).Normalized maximum joint flexion and extension equals
- 1.0 and 1.0, respectively. ( m T = extension, FLEX=flexion, MLC= maximum lifting
COM hrther posterior prior to llftoff as
capacity.) the load was made heavier (F4,40=9.5,
P<.001; Fig. 7A).
Figure 5, knee flexion at lift onset was assumed a more extended knee p s i -
about 75 degrees (180°=full exten- tion (90° at 75% MLC). This same pattern of results held just
sion) for both groups when lifting 15% after Ilftoff (Fig. 7B), the body's COM
MLC. With heavier loads, particularly The initial lumbar spine angle still tending to be located anterior to
above 300h MLC, the knee was slightly changed with the load (F4,,,=11.0, the 5MTH. On average, the COM was
more flexed for subjects in group 2 at P<.001) and ddfered between the located 0.037 m and 0.007 m anterior
llft onset (70°, on average, at 75% groups (F1,,,=9.7, Pc.05). Subjects in to the 5MTH for subjects in groups 1
MLC), whereas subjects in group 1 group I1 had more extended spines and 2, respectively (F1,,,=6.8, Pc.05).
For subjects in group 2, however, the

64 / 139 Physical Therapy / Volume 75, Number 2 / February 1995


to liftoff increased as the magnitude of
the load was increased (F,,,= 13.4,
P<.Wl) and tended to be higher for
subjects in group 1 than for those in
0.75 group 2 for all but the heaviest loads
t (Table). The apparent group differ-
ences, however, failed to reach signifi-
Q)
I Group 2 cance (P= ,075).
U
h
u 0.45 Discussion
Q)
Our companion article in this issue
presents evidence that increasing the
cw load a subject lifts can lead to changes
0
1 in the coordination of squat lifting
gb 0.15- G"'P despite specific instructions and fre-
a
C, quent reminders about how to llft. The
3 data presented in this article show that
z 0.001
0 15 30 45 60 75
1

90 individual differences in performance


a" Percentage of MLC also exist under the same llfting
conditions.

Reanalysis of the relative phase of


Figure 4. 7he average relative time in the he extension cycle during lifing at which
the lumbar spine completes 10%offull atenqion (21 SEM) for subjec.5 in post hoc g m u p 1 knee-lumbar spine motion, including
and 2for each load condition ( p e n t a g e of mazimum lging capacit3, [MLCI). the group factor, yielded a quantitative
result apparently inconsistent with the
mean COM location was posterior to Horizontal Velocity of the Center qualitative analysis of relative motion
the 5MTH at 600h to 75% MLC (Fig. of Mass plots used to categorize the groups
7B). The body's COM was closer to (Fig. 1). It must be remembered, how-
the SMTH when lifting heavier loads Peak posterior horizontal velocity (in ever, that this continuous measure of
(F4,40=50.3,P<.Wl). meters per second) of the COM prior coordination (ie, &) is the average
across all samples of a lift. The identi-
fied categories of knee-lumbar spine
coordination were based on d8er-
ences in relative motion of these joints
during the initial, accelerative phase of
the lift only. Because the effect of load
on knee-lumbar spine coordination
did not appear to dfier substantially
between the groups after this early
phase (Fig. 11, & probably obscures
these dfierences early in the llft.

Based on the patterns exhibited in


Figure 1, we expected that group
diferences on knee-lumbar spine
relative timing would be greatest at
early points in the lift, especially when
llfting light loads, and that they would
disappear at later points in the lift
cycle. The results of the relative timing
I I Group 2
I I analysis confinned this prediction.

For KL,, , knee extension led spine

I Percentage of MLC
I extension by a greater amount for
subjects in group 2 (Fig. 41, consistent
with dfierences in the relative motion
Figure 5. Average fnitiul knee angle( 2 1 SEMI for both post hoc groups plots (Fig. 1). Although the interaction
(EXT= extension, FLFX=Pexion. MLC= maximum lifiing capacity.) between group and load was not

Physical Therapy / Volume 75, Number 2 / February 1995


group 1). It is difficult to imagine that
this result was due to the inability of
the knee extensors to generate suffi-
cient force in this subject.
EXT 200-
Group 1's knee-hip coordination ap-
n
e peared to be affected more by in-
creases in the load as compared with
Q)
group 2 (Fig. 3). Neither & nor the
relevant relative timing measures,
Group 2 however, differed sigmficantly be-
Q)
tween the groups. It is certainly appar-
a ent that knee-hip coordination is much
less affected by the magnitude of the
h load lifted than is knee-lumbar spine
sEl 185-
coordination (Figs. 1 and 3). This has
been a consistent finding in all of our
3 work to date (see part I).lJO

Although we controlled the starting


FLEX 180 ij 30
I
45 60 75 posture for the lift by specrFyig loose
boundaries (ie, providing general
Percentage of MLC instructions for starting position, such
as with knees bent, rather than spe-
cific angles of flexion), this posture
Figure 6. Average initial lumbar spine angle (2 I SEM) for both post hoc groups. was not fixed. Significant group differ-
Neutml spine is 195 degrees. (EXT= extension, FLEX=Jexion, MLC= maximum lifting
capacity.) ences in the starting posture of the
body were associated with group
sighcant, planned comparisons diaerences in knee-lumbar spine coor-
Schipplein et a19 reported results sirni-
showed that the group diierence was dination measured during the acceler-
lar to those exhibited by group 1, that
s i m c a n t at all loads except 75% is, an increasing lag between knee
ative phase of lifting. Although both
MLC. This finding also appears consis- and lumbar spine extension as the groups assumed, on average, the same
tent with the relative motion plots. At initial knee flexion position when
load became heavy. Their subjects,
a slightly later point in the lift cycle, however, were allowed to lift freestyle. lifting light loads, this position became
load and group interacted to affect The authors concluded that the ob- more divergent when lifting heavy
knee-spine timing (KL3J. Here, the lag served change in lifting pattern was loads (Fig. 5). Subjects in group 1
of lumbar spine extension behind due to the inability of knee extensors showed the greatest change, starting
knee extension was greater for group to generate sufficient torque when more extended when lifting all but the
2 only at 15% to 30% MLC. Relative lifting the heaviest loads. Thus, the lightest load. Moreover, these subjects
timing measurements calculated later burden was assumed to be shifted to had more initial lumbar spine flexion
in the lift cycle, after load acceleration at lift onset at all loads than did sub-
the back after the knee had already
(ie, KLWand KL,,), did not differ be- extended. The data suppolting this jects in group 2 (Fig. 6). Both groups
tween the two post hoc groups. conclusion, however, were weak. If assumed a more flexed spine to start
this interpretation were accurate, it the lift when the load became heavy.
The results of these analyses provide would suggest that our group 2 sub- The exact starting position has not
quantitative support for conclusions jects had weaker knee extensors be- been reported for most studies of
drawn from the relative motion plots cause they showed a substantial lum- manual liftiig techniques, even when
(Fig. 1). During the accelerative phase bar spine extension lag behind knee the subjects were allowed to lift free-
of lifting, the inertia of the load has to extension at all loads during load style.9 Thus, the findings of those
be overcome, leading to the greatest studies for which the investigators
acceleration. This conclusion seems
likelihood of joint stress." Subjects in unlikely. For example, our earlier failed to report the starting position
group 2 restricted lumbar spine mo- experiments on squat liftinglJOin- are especially diff~cultto interpret.
tion regardless of the load magnitude, cluded one subject who was a triple-
possibly to minimize stress to the back Despite these significant effects of
jumper. This athlete was very strong
during this period of high stress. In and yet exhibited an increased phase group and load on the initial posture
contrast, subjects in group 1 appeared lag between knee and back motion of the body at lift onset, the actual
to use this "strategy" primarily when even across only moderately heavy values (Figs. 5 and 6) indicate that the
lifting the heaviest loads. loads (similar to pattern exhibited by subjects in both groups began the lift
in a squatting posture. That is, the

66 / 141 Physical Therapy / Volume 75, Number 2 / February 1995


-
Table. Peak Horizot~talPosterior
Velocity (5 Standard Deviation) of the
Body's Center of Mass Pn'or to Crate
Liftoff

Peak Velocity (rnls)


Group 1 Percentage
of MLCe Group 1 Group 2
0.08

p p p p p

%LC =maximum lifting capacity.

cance. Moreover, at loads at which the


starting posture of the knee was most
-0.02 different between the groups (eg, 75%
0 15 30 45 60 75 90
MLC), differences in knee-lumbar
Percentage of MLC spine coordination were smallest (Fig.
1).

We could have mechanically fixed the


subjects' starting posture, as has been
done in other recent studies.14J5Al-
though such a procedure would help
eliminate the starting posture as a
possible intervening variable, it is
inconsistent with how squat llfting is
taught clinically and implemented in
the real world. In our experience,
clinicians do not necessarily speclfy
precisely the initial posture of clients
when teaching them how to lift, al-
though control of lumbar spine pos-

2
-0.05 15 30 45

Percentage of MLC
60
Group 2
75
ture has generated much recent inter-
est.'6z17 If future work shows that small
differences in lifting posture make a
meaningful daerence, then more
precise training procedures than are
currently used will be essential.

The measured differences in coordina-


Figure 7. Location of the body's center of mass (COM) with respect to theJfth tion also do not appear to be related
metatarsal head (A) immediateL$prior to crate [@off and (B) at 15% of the period of to differences in the physical character-
vertical crate motion. Positive values indicate that the COM is anterior to tbeJflh istics of the subjec'ts. There was no
metatarsal head. (MLC=maximum ltJti7g capacity.) si@cant difference in either the
height or the weight of the subjects in
knee was flexed 90 degrees or more posture directly determined the ob- the two post hoc groups. Weight dif-
when llfting all loads and the lumbar served group diferences in coordina- ferences came close to si@cance, as
spine was flexed, on average, no tion. Correlation matrices (unreported) the mean weight of group 1 was
more than 10 degrees from neutral. revealed that starting angles accounted higher than that of group 2. As noted
for no more than 15% of the variance earlier this mean difference was pri-
Although clearly related, it does not in any coordination measure, and only marily due to one subject in group 1
appear that the dltferences in starting a few correlations reached signifi- who was extremely heavy.

Physical Therapy /Volume 75, Number 2 /February 1995 142 / 67


Based on the kinematic results (Fig. tion force vector close to the knee heaviest loads, they presumably re-
11, we expected onset and peak of ES axis, and that this phenomenon re- quired less muscular effort to maintain
activity to occur later for subjects in flects a general principle of antagonis- balance. If so, this "strategy" might
group 2 than for those in group 1, and tic muscle coordination. also reduce joint stress, although a
especially so when lifting the lighter kinetic analysis will be required to
loads. The results of our reanalysis of Likewise, many spinal extensors are determine this for certain. Certainly,
EMG timing data, however, were likely to be involved in controlling the limited spine extension during
generally inconsistent with the group spinal motion during lifting. The role load acceleration in group 2 is consis-
differences in knee-lumbar spine coor- of the abdominal muscles also cannot tent with lower stress to the back.l3
dination, defined hematically, during be ruled out, although recent data Nonetheless, the results represent a
the initial period of the lift. The only suggest that these muscles are rela- difference in degree, as both groups
significant group effect on EMG timing tively quiescent during load accelera- positioned the COM more posteriorly
was that peaks of VL activity occurred tion.ls Moreover, understanding kine- when lifting heavier loads (Fig. 7). It is
later in the lift cycle for subjects in matically defined changes in not clear why the group differences
group 2, especially when lifting the coordmation is complicated by contri- did not disappear for the heaviest
heaviest loads. Moreover, there was a butions to joint motion from reactive loads lifted, because coordination
tendency, although nonsigmficant, for forces from adjacent segments. Analy- during load acceleration was more
ES peaks of activity to occur later in ses of the task's kinetics, which are similar between the groups at these
the lift cycle for group 1 compared currently under way in our laboratory, loads (Fig. 1).
with group 2. as well as future experiments that
record from multiple muscles will be Increasing the horizontal velocity of
The different-than-expected timing of necessary to resolve this issue. the COM prior to liftoff would gener-
peak EMG activity may be because VL ate greater horizontal momentum. If
and ES peaks of activity both occur Differences between the post hoc this momentum could be converted to
relatively late in the lift. Therefore, groups on other variables suggest that vertical momentum, as has been sug-
group diierences on these measures the measured differences in knee- gested for a sit-to-stand task,l2 this
are likely to be unrelated to coordina- lumbar spine coordination may be conversion could make lifting the load
tion differences occurring early in the related to individual differences in easier. Horizontal velocity increased
lift cycle (ie, during load acceleration). meeting task requirements such as the significantly for both groups as the
The lack of a group effect for timing of control of balance. Although the posi- load was made heavier, suggesting
onset of ES activity is somewhat more tion of the crate with respect to the that all subjects used this "strategy"
puzzling. This result as well as the foot did not differ between the groups (Table). The greater mean horizontal
smaller, albeit significant, effect of load either prior to or shortly after liftoff, velocities generated by group 1 at all
on EMG timing compared with kine- the position of the body's COM did MLC conditions except the 75% MLC
matic relative timing measures (see differ. The COM was positioned more condition would appear to reflect a
part I) may be due to our limited anterior to the foot by subjects in greater use of this mechanism com-
sampling of EMG activity. For exam- group 1 compared with those in pared with subjects in group 2. The
ple, ES activity was recorded at only group 2 for all loads lifted. group difference approached but did
one spinal segment. Similarly, VL was not reach sigruficance. Nevertheless,
the only knee extensor recorded Surprisingly, the COM was in mast the results of this analysis and the
directly. trials still anterior to the 5MTH just results reported in our companion
after the load left the ground. A stan- article in this issue hint that individual
Many additional muscles are likely to dard method was used to calculate the diierences in coordination strategy are
participate in coordinating knee- body's COM.lWalculations of the related to differences in the primacy of
lumbar spine motion during perfor- overall COM, however, are affected by different task constraints among
mance of this task. The data of Ek- errors in estimating the location of the subjects.
holm et a17 suggest that although COM of individual segments. Thus, it
quadriceps femoris muscle activity is possible that the COM was not More research is needed to confirm
initiates squat lifting, hamstring muscle actually t h s far forward. If the COM the existence of these individual differ-
activity occurs as early as during the had been this far forward, more dy- ences in performance before their
first 1W/o to 20% of the lift cycle. Their namic muscular action would have clinical significance can be fully as-
results are consistent with those of a been required to counteract the clock- sessed. Nonetheless, our results sug-
recent analysis of a more constrained wise torque generated by the crate gest that minimizing spinal motion
squat lift.14Toussaint et all4 suggested and to maintain balance than if the during the initial accelerative phase of
that coordination of the monoarticular COM had been located more posteri- a lift may minimize threats to balance.
quadriceps fernoris muscle and the orly. Because subjects in group 2 As noted, if individual differences in
biarticular hamstring muscle helps to positioned their COM more posteriorly how the neuromuscular system is
preseme balance during squat ldting at this point in the lift cycle and, on coordinated during squat b g have
by keeping the vertical ground reac- average, well behind the 5MTH for the consequences for the magnitude of

Physical Therapy / Volume 75, Number 2 / Februarv 1995


joint stress or maintaining balance ing balance or generating the vertical 5 Bejjani FJ, Gross CM, Pugh JW. Model for
static lifting: relationship of loads on the spine
during lifting, then it will be important load trajectory. A full understanding of and the knee. J Biomech. 1984;17:281-286.
to consider these subtle differences the neuromuscular basis of the kine- 6 Pamianpour M, Bejjani FJ, Pavlidis L.
when training workers to lift properly. matically measured coordination dif- Worker training: the fallacy of a single, correct
ferences is complicated by the possi- lifting technique. Ergonomics. 1987:30:331-
334.
Certamly, the best way to prevent ble action of multijoint muscles and
7 Ekholm J, Arborelius UP, Nemeth G. The
injury to the back is to prevent work- the contribution of reactive forces load on the lumbosacral joint and trunk mus-
ers fmm lifting in situations in which fmm adjacent motion segments. More cle activity during lifting. Ergonomics. 1982;25:
the risk of injury is increased substan- detailed studies of muscular contribu- 145-161.
tially, including minimizing the load tions as well as kinetic analyses are 8 Patterson P, Congleton J, Koppa R, Huch-
ingson RD. The effects of load knowledge on
that they are required to lift and its needed to better understand the influ- stresses at the lower back during lifting. Eqo-
height with respect to the floor. Given ence of changing task variables on the nomics. 1987;30:539-549.
that workers are often required to llft coordination of this task and the clini- 9 Schipplein OD, Trafimow JH, Andersson
GBJ, Andriacchi TP. Relationship between
heavy loads from the floor, deterrnin- cal sigruficance of individual differ- moments at the L5/S1 level, hip and knee
ing and teaching the optimum coordi- ences in performance. Nonetheless, joint when lifting. J Biomech. 1%0;23:307-912.
nation strategy would be beneficial. the measured coordination differences 10 Scholz JP. The effect of load scaling in the
Our results suggest that we may not suggest that clinicians may need to coordination of manual lifting. Human Move-
ment Science. 19!33;12:427-459.
have an adequate understanding at pay more attention to subtle differ-
11 Ellaway PH. Cumulative sum technique
present of what is an optimal lifting ences in coordination when training and its application to the analysis of peris-
technique. The results presented in clients to lift objects. timulus time histograms. Flectmcephalogr
this article and in the companion CIin Natrophysiol. 1978;45:302-304.
12 Riley PO, Shenkrnan ML, Mann RW,
article in this issue indicate that a Acknowledgments Hodge WA. Mechanics of a constrained chair-
lifting "technique" such as squat lifting rise. JBiomech. 1991;24:77-85.
cannot be defined in terms of the We are grateful to the Foundation for 13 Lindh M. Biomechanics of the lumbar
body's starting posture alone. The Physical Therapy Inc for its support of spine. In: Nordin M, Frank1 VH, eds. Basic
Biomechanics of the Musculoskeletal System.
magnitude of load lifted and perhaps this project and to Jeffrey P Millford, Philadelphia, Pa: Lea & Febiger; 1989:195-202.
other factors such as the speed of PT, for his assistance with data pro- 14 Toussaint HM, van Baar CE, van Langen
lifting can have significant effects on cessing. We give special thanks to Dr PP, et al. Coordination of the leg muscles in
how the body is coordinated when Julie Moyer and Dr Irene McClay for leglift and backlift. J Biomech. 1992;25:1279-
1289.
performing this task. their many helpful suggestions. 15 Toussaint HM, Commissaris DACM, Dieen
JH van, et al. Controlling the ground reaction
force during lifting. journal of Motor Behav-
References ior. In press.
16 Delitto RS, Rose SJ, Apts DW. Electromyo
Individual differences occur in how 1 Scholz JP. Organizational principles for the graphic analysis of two techniques of squat
subjects lift a load from an initial coordination of lifting. Human Movemat Sci- lifting. Phys 7her. 1987;67:1329-1334.
squatting posture despite identical and a c e . 1993;12:537-576. 17 Holmes JK, Damaser MS, Lehman SL. Erec-
consistent instructions. Differences in 2 Grieve DW. The dynamics of lifting. k e n tor spinae activation and movement dynamics
Sport Sci Rw.1977;5:157-179. about the lumbar spine in lordotic and ky-
coordination among subjects appear to 3 Grieve DW. Dynamic characteristics of man photic squat-lifting. Spine. 1992;17:327-334.
be limited to the initial, accelerative during crouch- and stoop-lifting. In: Nelson 18 McGill SM, Norman RW. Reassessment of
phase of lifting, and the coordinative RC, Morehouse CA, eds. Biomechanics N. the role of intra-abdominal pressure in spinal
Baltimore, Md: University Park Press; 1974:9- compression. Ergonomics. 1987;30:15651588.
patterns are limited in number. More-
29. 19 Winter DA. Biomechanics and Motor Con-
over, these differences appear to be 4 Davis PR, Troup JDG, Burnard JH. Move- tml of Human Movement. New York, NY:
related to how subjec- manage differ- ments of the thoracic and lumbar spine when Wiley-Interscience; 1990.
ent task constraints such as maintain- lifting: a chronocyclo photographic study.
J Anat. 1965;991526.

Physical Therapy / Volume 75, Number 2 / February 1995

You might also like