You are on page 1of 22

The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

The Leadership Quarterly


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/leaqua

From classroom to conscription. Leadership emergence in childhood and T


early adulthood☆
Therese Reitana, , Sten-Åke Stenbergb

a
Academy of Public Administration, Södertörn University, S-141 89 Huddinge, Sweden
b
Swedish Institute for Social Research, Stockholm University, S-106 91 Stockholm, Sweden

ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT

Keywords: Research on leadership emergence has mainly focused on adulthood and relied on retrospective accounts of
Leadership assessment childhood factors. Based on a prospective cohort study of 7719 boys born in 1953, of which 5928 were later
Childhood drafted, we explored individual differences in leadership emergence in childhood and early adulthood. The data
Conscripts set consisted of register data from different time points and a survey of the cohort in the 6th grade. As expected,
Cohort study
cognitive ability, tallness and muscular power were decisive for assessment of military officer suitability.
Cragg-hurdle regression
However, we also found a moderate to strong impact on this assessment score from social class, leadership
aspirations in childhood, birth order, self-regulatory skills in school, parental support, and previous participation
in extracurricular activities. Similarly, social class, cognitive ability, developmental experiences, and birth order
were important for nominations as class party organizer in the 6th grade. Delay of gratification was not asso-
ciated with these nominations at all and was neutralized by cognitive ability in relation to officer suitability. The
results strongly support a life-cycle approach to leadership emergence.

Jack spoke. “We've got to decide about being rescued.” There was a Introduction
buzz. One of the small boys, Henry, said that he wanted to go home.
“Shut up,” said Ralph absently. He lifted the conch. “Seems to me William Golding's legendary novel Lord of the Flies from 1954 dra-
we ought to have a chief decide things.” matically portrays human behavior as it unfolds among an unspecified
“A chief! A chief.” number of boys approximately 6–12 years old who find themselves in
“I ought to be chief,” said Jack with simple arrogance, “because I'm an extreme, life-threatening situation after their plane has crashed.
chapter chorister and head boy. I can sing C sharp.” Another buzz. With no adults around, one of their first challenges is to identify a
“Well then,” said Jack, “I- -” He hesitated. leader and the novel becomes a story of group dynamics, leadership
The dark boy, Roger, stirred at last and spoke up. “Let's have a emergence and practice in a critical situation. Golding mercilessly re-
vote.” veals the truly dynamic character of leadership, where individual
“Yes!!” characteristics, power resources, and situational factors are put to play
“Vote for chief!” in complex interactions between potential leaders and followers
“Let's vote- -” (Zacharatos, Barling, & Kelloway, 2000). Furthermore, the novel de-
The toy of voting was almost as pleasing as the conch. Jack started monstrates how leadership is practiced already early in life (French &
to protest but the clamor changed from the general wish for a chief to Stright, 1991) and Golding thereby raises several fundamental issues
an election by acclaim of Ralph himself. None of the boys could have with relevance for leadership studies: What makes a leader? Do some
found good reason for this; what intelligence had been shown was individuals simply have the right attributes (being tall and attractive or
traceable to Piggy while the most obvious leader was Jack. But there able to sing C sharp) or is leadership something that comes from
was a stillness about Ralph as he sat that marked him out: there was his practice (as head of the choir)? Do leaders emerge through social in-
size, and attractive appearance, and most obscurely, yet most power- terplay whereby someone is nominated by peers? Are situational factors
fully, there was the conch. essential for leadership emergence (e.g. who is holding the conch – the
(William Golding: Lord of the Flies, chapter one) symbol of power – or what leadership qualifications are in demand on a


This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: therese.reitan@sh.se (T. Reitan), stenake@sofi.su.se (S.-Å. Stenberg).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2018.11.006
Received 3 December 2017; Received in revised form 15 November 2018; Accepted 22 November 2018
Available online 07 December 2018
1048-9843/ © 2018 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/BY-NC-ND/4.0/).
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

desert island)? Moreover, are leadership positions and attributes in locations, and 5) time. It is, then, not sufficient to ask who, what, and
childhood sustainable through to early adulthood? If Ralph and Jack which – but also where and when. The “when?” question may, in turn,
were to be drafted later in life, (they were actually saved by a naval refer to different aspects of time such as the magnitude, trends, dura-
officer) would they be considered “officer material”? tion, and periodicity of certain features/influences (Castillo & Trinh,
These questions originate from a) a deep-seated tradition of distin- 2018) – as well as when in history and when in a person's life course.
guishing between individual and environmental factors in relation to The aim of this study is, then, to analyze leadership development in
leadership emergence and development, b) a less prominent interest in childhood and young adulthood from an individual, inter-personal and
the influence of childhood patterns in adulthood, the continuity of contextual perspective. More specifically the study examines the impact
specific factors, and the interplay between them across the life course. of individual differences in middle childhood on assessments of leadership
The dichotomy between individual and environmental factors has capacity in early adulthood.
proven to be very persistent in leadership studies. Although the notion Both of the stated objectives concern assessments of leader capacity,
of a zero-sum battle is increasingly difficult to uphold, the distinction not formal leadership role occupancy or leadership performance. More
seems to be continuously useful in sorting causes “outside the body” specifically, the focus on developmental processes that promote, en-
from those “inside the body” (Duster, 2006). That factors, inside or courage, and assist in an individual's leadership potential in early life
outside the body, are mutually exclusive is, however, not a viable and that may lead to a recognition of leadership potential. We use
presumption (Jencks, 1980). Rather, we need to understand how per- leadership development and emergence interchangeably in order to
sonal, interpersonal and environmental factors interact under different describe a process whereby individuals develop into leadership, and not
temporal and contextual circumstances. the development of leadership.
Leadership studies have not denied or ignored the impact of child- The study is based on the Project Metropolitan which included all
hood and adolescence for leadership emergence in adulthood. children born in 1953 and who were residing in the Stockholm me-
Numerous references to the biographical history of individual leaders tropolitan area in 1963 (Stenberg & Vågerö, 2006). They were followed
are telling examples (Cunha, Lewis, Rego, & Smith, 2017; Shoup, 2005). from birth to their early 30s, but this study focuses on the period be-
Understanding leadership among children and youths has been central tween middle childhood and early adulthood and only on the boys
to psychology, sociology, and pedagogy albeit sometimes under other (N = 7719). The latter is because the main outcome measure is as-
headings, e.g. “social interaction”, “socio-metric status”, “deviant peer sessment of leadership capability in connection with conscription.
influences”, “interpersonal development” and so forth (Belsky, Apart from the study's longitudinal and prospective design, its major
Steinberg, & Draper, 1991; Dodge, Dishion, & Lansford, 2006; Gest, advantages are the large sample size and the richness of data and data
Graham-Bermann, & Hartup, 2002; Lerner & Steinberg, 2009). Still, sources – not least survey data from when the boys were aged 12–13.
life-cycle perspectives have not been nearly as prominent in leadership
studies as for example the relative importance of personal and inter- Leadership in childhood and adolescence
personal influences on leadership emergence, or the nature and con-
sequences of transformational/charismatic leadership styles. The em- The process in which a person becomes a leader, or exercises lea-
phasis in leadership studies has also concentrated on conditions and dership, is dynamic and iterative (Murphy & Johnson, 2011). How in-
events in adulthood (Murphy, 2011). dividuals develop a leader identity, seek arenas in which leader capa-
Despite the seemingly uncontroversial notion that childhood and cities can be developed, and are recognized and supported as leaders by
adolescence are crucial for adult leadership emergence and practice, peers and significant others is therefore fundamental. And, these pro-
there is a scarcity of empirical research validating this perception (Day, cesses start from a very early age (Fukada, Fukada, & Hicks, 1994;
2011; Gottfried et al., 2011). This shortage is not only an issue of Mawson, 2011). A life course approach to leadership also implies that
quantity but also of methodological weaknesses. Single case- or small events, learning experiences, peer recognition and so forth have varying
sample-studies dominate, often based on retrospective reports of importance and impact depending on when in life they occur.
childhood experiences, and selected populations such as students in An investigation of the “seeds of leadership” (Murphy & Johnson,
leadership programs or military cadets. Prospective studies of leader- 2011) from childhood on will unavoidably touch base with sociological,
ship development have similarly been oriented towards adults within epidemiological and demographic life-course studies or developmental
organizations, not on developmental roots from childhood (Hannah, psychology and the distinction between age, period, and cohort effects
Avolio, Luthans, & Harms, 2008; Strang & Kuhnert, 2009). (Glenn, 1976; Palmore, 1978; Yang & Land, 2016), although these are
The nature of leadership development is inherently multilevel and often difficult to disentangle empirically. Empirical studies of leader-
longitudinal. Whether we are dealing with formal or informal leader- ship in a life-course perspective, including childhood and adolescence,
ship, within formal or informal organizations, individuals are nested is no less challenging from a methodological point of view. In addition,
within teams, which in turn are nested within larger fields and struc- we would argue that there are two distinct approaches to “leadership in
tures (Day, 2011; Day & Harrison, 2007). Moreover, an individual's childhood and adolescence” that should be observed when tapping into
journey into leadership roles (whether formal or informal) and the how this field of research:
leadership practice develops is a matter of how intrapersonal and in- First, leadership in childhood and adolescence is often associated
terpersonal processes evolve over time and across the life cycle (Day, with issues of anti-sociality, destructive leader-follower interaction, and
Fleenor, Atwater, Sturm, & McKee, 2014; Murphy & Johnson, 2011). risk behavior. Fiction, theater and film are replete with illustrations of
Time is often neglected or poorly analyzed in leadership studies the harsh and sometimes fatal consequences of (negative) social inter-
(Castillo & Trinh, 2018). action among children and youths. Research studies in this area will
Adolescence and early adulthood are periods in which the socio- typically focus on deterring negative leadership and destructive group
economic and ideological environment is thought to be particularly processes, as well as studies of long-term “consequences of popularity”
important for foundations of adulthood (career choices, family life, (Allen, Porter, McFarland, Marsh, & McElhaney, 2005). Consequently,
lifestyle, political formation). The impact of various factors on leader- research has often been driven by an explicit aim to promote pro-social
ship development is therefore bound to vary depending on the timing in behavior, improve school environments, or enhance cognitive and so-
a person's life and the duration, magnitude, and periodicity of impact cial development among youngsters (Hoyt & Kennedy, 2008; Klepp,
(Castillo & Trinh, 2018). Rauthmann, Sherman, and Funder (2015) Halper, & Perry, 2018; McCullough, Ashbridge, & Pegg, 1994;
refer to five categories of “cues”, i.e. elements in the environmental Whitehead, 2009).
structures which need to be interpreted and acted upon: 1) persons, Second, leadership in pre-adulthood is linked to a search for the
relationships, and social interactions, 2) objects, 3) events/activities, 4) roots of leadership emergence and leadership practice as such. That is,

299
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

how to develop the right type of leaders and not primarily how to foster leader individual differences; foundational traits and leadership capa-
“better citizens”. These studies often have adolescence or early adult- cities. Foundational traits are relatively stable over time and situations,
hood as the starting point – typically departing from high-school, stu- i.e. “distal” traits. They are defined as individual attributes that orient a
dent, or military recruit populations – where the outcomes usually focus person towards seeking leadership roles and predispose him or her to-
on leadership role occupancy, leadership practice and development, wards behavioral strategies likely to be successful in that role (p 6).
occupational, participatory and career patterns in adulthood (Gayles & Some of these traits and leadership capacities have demonstrated sig-
Baker, 2015; Judge, Ilies, & Colbert, 2004; Kuhn & Weinberger, 2005; nificant heritability estimates in empirical studies. These traits are also
McFarland & Thomas, 2006). This approach is, nonetheless, no devoid causal precursors to the development and expression of more specific
of interventionistic ambitions. For example, Whitehead calls for an leadership capacities, including e.g. cognitive and problem-solving
improvement of leadership development programs in high school – skills, social skills, motivational orientation towards leadership and
because “society cannot rely solely on sports or club activities to de- leader self-efficacy. Leadership capacities are relatively mutable, or
velop young leaders” (Whitehead, 2009, p. 866). “proximal”, in that they can change and grow as a function of particular
Our study primarily falls into the latter category of approaches to developmental activities and experiences. It is important to note, then,
leadership in relation to childhood and adolescence. This study revolves that foundational traits not only predispose individuals towards lea-
around the “roots” of leadership in adulthood, i.e. the assessment of dership roles per se, but also orient them towards particular develop-
leadership capability in connection with conscription. However, we will mental activities and increase their readiness for such situations (p 7).
also apply a “within-childhood” approach to leadership by empirically As the authors point out, the suggested theoretical model can be
scrutinizing factors related to the choice of (informal) leaders among a deconstructed into simpler models for empirical testing without losing
group of children. its overall conceptual integrity (Zaccaro et al., 2018, p. 35). It is
therefore important to point out the following: First, we have marked
Longitudinal studies of leadership development from early life “Genetic predisposition” with a dotted line in Fig. 1. This is not to
suggest that genetic factors are not of interest or importance – the role
The shortage of investigations of leadership development from of genes in relation to leader individual differences and outcomes has
childhood to adulthood does not only concern the mere quantity of received increased attention in recent years (Ilies, Arvey, & Bouchard,
studies, but also problems related to insufficient data. Many studies 2006; Tuncdogan, Acar, & Stam, 2017) – but we do not have such data
with an attempted longitudinal approach include data referring to two and genetic factors are not included in this empirical study. Second, we
points in time with only a short period in between (Li, Arvey, & Song, do not study the actual execution of leadership in different situations.
2011). Studies with a time horizon of several years or longer are rare, So, leadership capacity in this study is reduced to a question of assess-
and cross-sectional designs dominate (Barling & Weatherhead, 2016; ment of leadership capacity by others. Third, time and context are not
Daly, Egan, & O'Reilly, 2015; Murphy, 2011). The lack of data does not specifically addressed in the original Zaccaro et al. framework but are
only concern limited points of observation. Given the interactional of course implicit in the distinction between mutable and stable traits,
character of leadership – there cannot be any leadership without fol- and the definition of foundational traits as being relatively stable over
lowers – a major weakness in leadership studies at large is the shortage time and situations (Zaccaro et al., 2018, p. 6).1 The framework also
of relational and subjective data. That is, the “who” issues as suggested incorporates biological maturation and age-normative socialization
by Rauthmann et al. (2015) and data pertaining and/or stemming from (early childhood factors) but it lacks a manifest link to life-course stu-
peers, followers, parents, colleagues, subordinates, and so forth. Some dies and life span models of leader development (Murphy & Johnson,
objective measures of conditions during childhood and adolescence can 2011). To highlight this issue we have added “historical and societal
be reconstructed retrospectively (e.g. socioeconomic status of the fa- context” to the theoretical framework. These are not variables in the
mily, educational attainment, participation in extracurricular activ- proper sense and are not directly connected to the other categories,2 but
ities), whereas subjective perceptions of relations and events cannot. including this dimension emphasizes the importance of addressing the
One of the main advantages of long-term studies starting from pre- “when?” question (Rauthmann et al., 2015) – or the cohort and period
adulthood is that they comprise data on a population unselected for effects, as it were (Palmore, 1978). Contextual knowledge is important
specific criteria such as participation in leadership programs, leadership in relation to interpretation and generalizability. This particular study
role occupancy and so forth (Gottfried et al., 2011). Such research is of leadership development, for example, concerns an interval of
invaluable to avoid depending on data solely from populations who are 5–8 years from childhood to early adulthood among a cohort growing
already selected into leadership contexts. The Fullerton Longitudinal up in an expanding welfare state at a time when in principle all males
Study is one of few examples of prospective, long-term studies. It were drafted, in a country not facing any immediate threat of war. Had
started with 130 children and their families who were followed from the country been at risk of war the escalating need for army officers
infancy to adulthood (Gottfried et al., 2011; Gottfried, Gottfried, & could potentially have influenced the assessment of leadership capacity.
Guerin, 2006; Reichard et al., 2011). The U.S. National Longitudinal
Survey of Youth, a large multi-wave and multisource data set, has also
been employed to study the both emergence of leaders and non‑leaders; 1
Whether an attribute is stable or mutable naturally depends on when and
“understanding who does not emerge as a leader might point to per- how it is measured. Socioeconomic status of a person's family of origin is a
sonal or contextual factors that limit the pool of future leaders (Barling foundational trait if measured retrospectively (Li et al., 2011), but may still
& Weatherhead, 2016, p. 1305). The Project Metropolitan was not change if measured prospectively. Even a seemingly given attribute such as
specifically designed to study leadership issues, but it includes a broad birth order may change throughout the life course, for example in connection
selection of data at the individual and inter-personal level with re- with family restructuring involving children from different relationships
levance to a life-cycle approach to leadership development. (Rosenberg & Hajal, 1985). Moreover, establishing degree of stability is not
only an issue of measurement but also determining how much change is ac-
ceptable before the attribute is no longer “stable” (Castillo & Trinh, 2018).
An integrated model of leader individual differences in a life- 2
It is, however, not hard to find evidence that historical context(s) may have
course perspective
a direct impact on other categories – even genetic predisposition (Stanner &
Yudkin, 2001). Societal expectations are also bound to differ depending on the
The underlying theoretical framework of our study is adopted from historical circumstances. Elder (1974) argues that children who grew up during
Zaccaro, Green, Dubrow, and Kolze (2018), emphasizing the additive the Great Depression did better in school, were more likely to enter higher
and pattern combinations of individual leader differences from child- education, and were generally more satisfied with life – largely because they
hood on. As shown in Fig. 1, the model includes two main categories of had to learn to handle challenges and difficulties at an early age.

300
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

Leadership Learning Capacities and


Skills Developmental Experiences
Early Life Experiences
Membership in sports
Family Academic intrinsic motivation club/scouts
socioeconomic status
Frequency of
Birth order extracurricular
Parental support activities

Foundational traits
Leadership Capacities
Cognitive ability
Peer ascription of
Self-regulatory skills
Genetic leadership in childhood
Motivation to lead
Predisposition Assessment of
Physical characteristics
leadership capability at
conscription

Historical and societal context

Fig. 1. A framework for leadership emergence in childhood and early adulthood, adapted from Zaccaro et al. (2018).

In the following, the main categories and variables in the model are the world (Andeweg & Van den Berg, 2003). The mechanisms behind
presented in further detail. this pattern have not been fully explored, but the main arguments
usually revolve around child-parental relations or sibling relations.
Some argue that first-born receive more attention, resources and are
Early life experiences subject to higher expectations from their parents. Others maintain the
Family socioeconomic status. Family socioeconomic status has not been a importance of power struggles between siblings and first-born
prominent factor in studies of leadership emergence (Martin, Innis, & children's experience as both leaders and followers (Andeweg & Van
Ward, 2017), but already 70 years ago, Stogdill identified > 100 data- den Berg, 2003). Birth order also figures as a control variable in
driven articles in which the most consistent correlates of leadership leadership studies (McCullough et al., 1994).
emergence were SES, various indicators of intelligence, in addition to
personality traits, social skills and emotional control (Stogdill, 1948).
Parental support.
Contrary to Elder's (1974) aforementioned presumption about the
potential benefits of hardships in childhood on leadership emergence Ralph paddled backwards down the slope, immersed his mouth and
later in life, early exposure to poverty is generally assumed to hinder blew a jet of water into the air. Then he lifted his chin and spoke.
later leadership role occupancy by e.g. reducing access to relevant ‘I could swim when I was five. Daddy taught me. He's a commander
education and development of skills and personal mastery (Barling & in the Navy. When he gets leave he'll come and rescue us. What's
Weatherhead, 2016). The economic and social environments that your father?’
individuals experience during their formative years has also been Piggy flushed suddenly.
found to have an impact on the magnitude of genetic influences on ‘My dad's dead,’ he said quickly, ‘and my mum – ’
leadership later in life (Zhang, Ilies, & Arvey, 2009). Based on a study of For children and adolescents, the family environment is critical for
twins they found that heritable traits were more strongly associated their development and well-being on many levels. According to Zhang
with leadership role occupations for individuals from lower social et al. (2009), family environments such as socioeconomic resources and
classes compared to those from higher classes. In short, individuals with parental support are more mutable and influential than other aspects of
lower access to privileges and opportunities provided by a higher SES adolescents' social environments, such as school, peers, neighborhoods.
background have to “rely” on heritable traits to a larger degree. The However, from the adolescent's point of view they are still relatively
association is not simplistic though; the impact of childhood non-mutable and factors that the individual are subject to more than
deprivation on leadership emergence has been found to differ being active participants in. Parents may support their offspring emo-
between men and women (Barling & Weatherhead, 2016). In fact, Li tionally by promoting self-confidence and achievement orientation, by
et al. (2011) found that (higher) family socioeconomic status exerted an acting as role models, by encouraging the engagement in certain ac-
adverse effect on leadership advancement for women. tivities (Brungardt, 1997). Parents may of course also play a role in
introducing their children to networks and arenas that are otherwise
Birth order. Birth order studies have attempted to empirically test the not equally accessible.
common notion that the first born often takes on a leadership role, not
only in relation to his/her siblings but also among peers and in later Leadership learning capacities and skills
organizational and work life. There is substantial empirical evidence Academic intrinsic motivation. Intrinsic motivation includes enjoyment
that birth order is related to leadership in adulthood (Andeweg & Van of an activity as such without the receipt or promise of external
den Berg, 2003; Eckstein et al., 2010). First-born children, including rewards. Academic intrinsic motivation refers to the motivation for
singletons, are overrepresented among political leaders in all parts of learning or doing schoolwork out of curiosity or the pleasure of

301
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

learning, not primarily because one has to (Gottfried et al., 2011). analyses by Zaccaro et al. (2018) – or “simply” a trait-like capacity the
Academic intrinsic motivation is, in turn, important to consider in ability to exert self-control not only varies between individuals, but
relation to leadership. It includes attributes that are likely to contribute seems to do so already at an early age (Tuncdogan et al., 2017). More
to leadership, such as an orientation towards mastery, curiosity, per- importantly, this capacity has been associated with a number of
sistence, and engaging in new and challenging tasks. Based on data outcomes later in life. The large body of research spurred by the
from the Fullerton Longitudinal Study, Gottfried et al. found that aca- “marshmallow tests” of Walter Mischel has aimed to establish a link
demic intrinsic motivation in childhood and adolescence provided a between the ability to delay gratification in childhood with e.g.
foundation for motivation to lead in adulthood. From childhood to educational outcomes, health, criminality, substance abuse and so
adolescence, individuals with higher academic intrinsic motivation forth later in life (Tuncdogan et al., 2017).
were more likely to want to lead because of enjoyment and without Individual differences in e.g. cognitive capacities, personality and
regard of rewards or costs and are therefore less likely to avoid lea- temperament, and ability to regulate emotional responses have been
dership roles based on such calculations (Gottfried et al., 2011). identified as crucial in the acquisition and improvement of leadership
skills (Lord & Hall, 2005), not least in a lifespan approach to leader
Foundational traits development (Murphy, 2011). Measures of self-regulation have also
Cognitive ability. Childhood intelligence or cognitive ability have been been found to vary between age groups, partly related to the fact that
shown to predict a wide array of short- and long-term outcomes. The cognitive abilities develop in stages (Gollner, Ballhausen, Kliegel, &
more obvious are related to school performance, educational transition, Forstmeier, 2017).
in addition to occupational and income status (Fergusson, Horwood, & Self-control, defined as “impulsive acting out without regard for
Ridder, 2005). But, even long-term effects of childhood IQ on survival consequences” and rated by teachers when the child, was included as a
to old age have been uncovered (Whalley & Deary, 2001). covariate in a British longitudinal cohort study of leadership role oc-
Leadership tasks are generally cognitively demanding in terms of cupancy (Daly et al., 2015). Overall, rule-breaking or norm-challenging
performance requirements and higher intelligence increases the like- behavior in childhood and adolescence are strong warning signs of
lihood of engaging in such tasks (Zaccaro et al., 2018). In the same way continued social deviancy and poor outcomes in many areas (Zhang,
as cognitive ability in adulthood is likely to influence leadership Welte, & Wieczorek, 2002). However, others have argued that not all
emergence and leadership development, results from a large study of problem behaviors are detrimental to educational achievement or life
two representative British cohorts suggest that childhood cognitive prospects. Firstly, a large majority of adolescents engage in some form
ability may also profoundly shape leadership trajectories in later life of rule-breaking behavior at some point although very few continue
(Daly et al., 2015). into an antisocial life-path (Bandura, 1986; Moffitt, 2015). Secondly, if
Questions have, nonetheless, been raised about the validity of IQ in one accepts that leadership and entrepreneurship share many simila-
relation to both leadership and other developmental or social outcomes. rities (Reid, Anglin, Baur, Short, & Buckley, 2018) and that en-
Based on the Fullerton Longitudinal Study, Guerin et al. (2011) found trepreneurs almost by definition are rule-breakers (Zhang et al., 2009),
that adolescent IQ was not associated with either social skill or lea- modestly rebellious behavior could even be beneficial if it increases
dership potential in adulthood. Instead, they propose the inclusion of self-knowledge and helps the individual explore the limits and con-
early individual differences in temperament and/or personality in sequences of norm-breaking behavior. Obschonka (2016), for example,
conceptual frameworks of leader development, or alternative con- distinguishes between breaking rules and breaking the law, pointing
ceptualizations of cognitive ability. Moreover, the impact of cognitive out how adolescent rule breaking can be a valuable asset for future
abilities is highly related to situational factors (Fiedler, 2002). entrepreneurship.
Be that as it may, there are good reasons for including cognitive
ability in childhood in this model. It is associated with leader role oc- Motivation to lead. Motivation is a broad category of individual traits
cupancy in adulthood, not least through educational attainment (Daly that are more or less mutable while also susceptible to environmental
et al., 2015). Cognitive ability may also contribute positively to mental influence. Motivation is partly a general ability that children develop as
and physical health, which could facilitate leader role emergence for they mature (Baumeister & Vohs, 2003) or specifically directed towards
example through access to developmental experiences conducive to becoming the person they want to be (Banaji & Prentice, 1994). Mastery
leadership emergence (Daly et al., 2015; Deary, Weiss, & Batty, 2010). motivation in young children has been defined as a psychological force
Research on military cadets showed that cognitive ability, physical that stimulates an individual to attempt to independently in a focused
fitness, self-esteem, and prior experience associated with leader emer- and persistent manner, to solve a problem or master a skill or task
gence (Day & Sin 2011). Although cognitive ability in itself cannot “which is at least moderately challenging for him or her” (Morgan,
predict leadership role occupancy (Li et al., 2011), there are good Harmon, & Maslin-Cole, 1990, p. 319). The latter qualifying term is an
reasons to believe cognitive ability contributes to leadership emergence attempt to separate motivation from general cognitive ability.
through different pathways in childhood and between childhood and When it comes to leadership motivation specifically, Lord and Hall
early adulthood. The association between cognitive ability and lea- (2005) argue that leader identity is essential for leader skill develop-
dership is, nonetheless, complicated by the fact that individuals higher ment. To emerge as a leader in the first place, though, is often the result
in intelligence are more likely to be perceived as leaders by followers of a long and dynamic process by which an individual becomes aware
(Lord, De Vader, & Alliger, 1986). However, the value of intelligence in that leaders exist and his/her own aspirations of becoming a leader.
terms of social status among peers could be an intricate act of balance in Already in early childhood we learn to recognize that certain people
childhood when group identification and ordinal rank can be highly have a special authority (parents, teachers, and so on), without perhaps
important for expectations and behavior (Elsner & Isphording, 2018). being defined as leaders. As children grow older, some of them also
There is evidence suggesting that follower perceptions of leadership start experimenting or engaging in leadership activities through extra-
qualities follow a curvilinear function of intelligence. Too wide dis- curricular activities (Day, Harrison, & Halpin, 2008) and learning self-
crepancies between leaders and followers may for example jeopardize regulation (Murphy, 2002). Developing preferences for certain occu-
the perception of unity and common interests in a group. There are pations starts at an early age and advances in stages as children acquire
therefore good reasons to test non-linear functions of IQ (Antonakis, a better understanding of themselves and the world of work. Children's
House, & Simonton, 2017; Simonton, 1985). occupational preferences are, then, likely to reflect the awareness of
specific occupations and their content, but also a perception of occu-
Self-regulatory skills. Whether a form of cognitive capacity – as pational characteristics such as degree of leadership (Stockard &
suggested in the overview of major leadership trait reviews and meta- McGee, 1990).

302
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

Considerable research has supported the relative permanence of predispositions (such experiences can also alter genetic structures), and
childhood occupational preferences and the concordance between ca- facilitate the contribution of foundational traits to particular leadership
reer aspirations of youngsters and their actual careers (Wahl & capacities. Little empirical work has, however, been done on specific
Blackhurst, 2000). A follow-up study of two British cohorts revealed developmental experiences in relation to different leadership capacities
that teenage aspirations were good predictors of adult occupational (Murphy & Johnson, 2011; Zaccaro et al., 2018). Parental encourage-
attainment. Teenagers with high aspirations were more likely to enter a ment and socioeconomic status are bound to influence the extent to
professional or managerial career compared to their less ambitious which children participate in sports and other activities, and not least
peers. The socio-historical context is, nonetheless, extremely important: which activities (McNeal, 1998) – which is another reason to include
Due to changes in social, economic, and education policies, the two both in the theoretical framework.
cohorts – born only 12 years apart – grew up under very different cir-
cumstances. Increasing unemployment, loss of manufacturing jobs, and Leadership capacity. As mentioned at the outset this study does not
technological transformation meant that the younger cohort were under analyze the actual performance of leadership, but rather the assessment
much greater pressure to continue full-time education and to acquire of leader capacity by others. Formal assessment and ascription of
formal qualifications and steer away from certain occupations (Schoon leadership capability is a widely studied topic, not least in relation to
& Parsons, 2002). gender and leadership emergence (Rosette, Leonardelli, & Phillips,
2008; Sczesny & Kühnen, 2004; Solansky, 2010).
Physical characteristics. In his seminal overview of leadership research The main aim of this paper is to analyze the impact of factors in
Stogdill (1948) noted that athletic ability and “physical prowess” childhood and early adulthood on the formal assessment of officer
appeared to be associated with leadership status in boys' gangs and suitability in connection with conscription. However, informal leader-
groups. Piggy in The Lord of the Flies is an intelligent and rational boy ship ascription is essential in understanding group behavior and the
who in that respect would be a potential leadership candidate. self-reinforcing processes leading towards leadership role occupancy
However, he has serious flaws in other areas; apart from being (Paunova, 2015). At no time in life is this more important than in
cowardly, immature, and sensitive, he is overweight, has poor childhood and adolescence. Children in middle childhood spend a great
eyesight and asthma. In contrast, Ralph's somewhat inferior cognitive deal of time trying to learn the social rules and self-presentational
ability is compensated for by his tall and athletic approach. He is strategies necessary for inclusion – and are acutely aware of pecking
described as a potential boxer “as far as width and heaviness of orders and their precariousness (Lease, Musgrove, & Axelrod, 2002).
shoulders [goes]” – his brawns are clearly an important element in Observational studies of group dynamics and socio-metric measures
his leadership emergence. The question has continued to intrigue have been used for decades to investigate social isolation, status and
researchers and there is some support for the notion that tall men are general acceptance among children (Y. Almquist, 2009, 2011; Horrocks
more likely to attain leadership positions compared to shorter men & Thompson, 1946; Lease et al., 2002; V. Östberg, 2003; V. Östberg &
(Judge & Cable, 2004; Lindqvist, 2011), although there is a probable Modin, 2008). The school survey also includes a socio-metric measure
bias in reporting (Elgar, 2016). The possible association has been linked whereby pupils were asked to anonymously nominate classmates as
to psychological research suggesting that tall people are seen as more potential class party organizers, i.e. as a form of informal ascription of
persuasive, impressive and capable (Stulp, Buunk, Verhulst, & Pollet, leadership. Because of an endogeneity problem this measure cannot be
2013). Interestingly enough there also seems to be evidence of a included in the main analytical model, but it is analyzed as a dependent
reciprocal connection in the sense that people in prestigious positions variable using most of the other variables in the main model.
are perceived to be taller than they are (Lindqvist, 2011). Lundborg,
Nystedt, and Rooth (2009) found that the well-established “height Method
premium” in wages in fact may be explained by physical capacity,
cognitive, and non-cognitive skills. However, Blaker et al. (2013) found Participants, design, and procedures
a gender difference in this respect; although part of the height premium
may be attributed to increased cognitive capacity tall males still seem to The sample and data for this study stem from the Project
have an added advantage of being perceived as more dominant and Metropolitan, which was primarily a sociological study of social mo-
healthy compared to women. bility, conformity and deviance in a life-course perspective. It was
The role of physical characteristics as foundational leader traits has originally planned as a joint effort between Stockholm, Oslo,
seen a resurgence in recent years, linking “new” scientific disciplines Copenhagen, and Helsinki, but ended up including only Stockholm and
such as neuroscience, behavioral genetics, and human biology to lea- Copenhagen (Stenberg, 2018). The Swedish project covered all children
dership studies (Antonakis, Day, & Schyns, 2012; Arvey, Rotundo, who were born in 1953 and who were residing in the Stockholm Me-
Johnson, Zhang, & McGue, 2006; Chaturvedi, Zyphur, Arvey, Avolio, & tropolitan area in 1963 (totaling 7398 girls and 7719 boys). The data
Larsson, 2012; Dinh & Lord, 2012). Studies have for example shown were collected from the cohort members themselves, their guardians,
significant associations between facial appearance and different aspects and from registries and covers the time period between 1953 and 1985
of leadership (Little, 2014). (Janson, 1966; Stenberg & Vågerö, 2006). One of the central sources of
information was the school survey performed in 1966, when the cohort
Developmental experiences members were 12–13 years old. The survey was distributed by teachers
In the campaign between leadership candidates in The Lord of the in the classroom and completed by the children themselves. The school
Flies Jack invokes his experience as “chapter chorister and head boy”. It survey included questions about attitudes towards school and educa-
might not have been the most impressive merit for his potential fol- tion, plans for the future, occupational aspirations, hobbies and leisure
lowers, but Jack was clearly on to something. As children start school activities. It also included three different cognitive tests, and questions
and grow a bit older, they begin to observe and recognize effective and about relations to their classmates and parental support and expecta-
ineffective adult leaders (Ruhm, 1992). Participation in organized ac- tions. Even children who were not included in the study sample, but
tivities such as choirs, musical bands, theater groups, scouts, and sports who were present in the classroom were given a survey form. Their
clubs, may provide invaluable opportunities for such observations, ex- responses were included in the project if these responses had bearing on
posure to role models, learning and practicing leadership skills (Dobosz the study cohort members (e.g. nominations for class party organizer)
& Beaty, 1999; Karagianni & Jude Montgomery, 2018; Murphy, 2011; and they were also included in the information about class size. In-
Murphy & Johnson, 2011; Ruhm, 1992). The suggested framework formation was also collected from registers and censuses of population
suggests that early development experiences can actualize genetic and housing – e.g. family income and social class – and conscription

303
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

data were collected for the boys.3 exemption (e.g. mental and physical illness or handicap). Even pre-
The focus of our study is the assessment of leadership capacity mature deaths and emigration are most probably biased. We have
measured as the assessment of military officer suitability in connection therefore, for our purposes, based our analyses on the individuals who
with conscription. At that time (early 1970s) practically all Swedish were drafted and represented in the Conscription Board's registry.
males were drafted for military service. The draft usually took place at Informal ascription of leadership status among children is analyzed
the age of 18 or 19, and consisted of interviews, psychological tests, and socio-metrically with the use of a question from the school survey
medical examinations over a three-day period. The draft process served asking them to nominate classmates for the role as class party orga-
several purposes. First, it was to ensure that all male citizens were nizer. The question is assumed to reflect both social status in the class
called in and fulfilled their societal duties. Only those who were already and general popularity, but also a perceived ability to lead a group and
serving in the military forces, seamen, and the severely handicapped get things done. Peer status ratings based on the school survey have
(physically or mentally) were exempt from appearing before the draft previously been used in studies of e.g. educational attainment and
board. Second, this was an arena for assembling and producing in- health-related outcomes in adulthood (Y. Almquist, 2011; Y. B.
formation. The draft boards collected data from other registers, and Almquist & Östberg, 2013; Almquist, Modin, & Östberg, 2010).
produced new data through the examinations and tests performed An overview of the variables in this study, the sources of informa-
during the three-day assessment period. Apart from serving the needs of tion, the year the variables refer to, and non-response rates is presented
the army, these data played an important role in policy documents and in Table 1. The variables are presented in further detail in the following:
the production of official statistics (National Board of Health and
Welfare, 2009; Public Committee for the Education of Conscripts, Dependent variables
1986). Furthermore, the availability and structure of this comprehen-
sive and comparable information for whole cohorts over a long time In connection with the draft leadership capacity, or more precisely
period, came to provide an invaluable asset for research on for example military officer suitability, was assessed on a scale from 1 to 9, with 1
health, social mobility and labor market participation (Karnehed, representing the lowest and 9 the highest values. As mentioned in the
Rasmussen, Hemmingsson, & Tynelius, 2012; Lindqvist & Vestman, last section the evaluators were advised not to let intellectual capacity
2011; Lundin, Lundberg, Hallsten, Ottosson, & Hemmingsson, 2010; influence the judgment of other factors too greatly. In practice, how-
Sidorchuk, Hemmingsson, Romelsjö, & Allebeck, 2012). The third ever, inductees with an IQ of 4 or less were generally not selected for
purpose was to process and use information for screening and selection assessment. Overall, 31% of the males in our study did not pass this first
of military recruits. Recruits were deemed either fit or unfit for service,4 hurdle, and only 15 individuals (of 5928) with an IQ of 4 or less were in
and those who were fit for service had to be matched with military fact assessed for military officer suitability. The non-assessed in-
requirements and needs. dividuals were assigned the value 0 on military officer suitability. In
The first selection of recruits for assessment of potential officer Fig. 2 the left bar represents non-selected recruits, whereas the selectees
suitability was very cursory and primarily based on test results from the show a near normal distribution on the 1–9 scale.
draft (IQ, body height, muscular capacity, medical data) and/or In the analysis of factors related to informal ascription of leadership
average school mark in the 9th grade, without a prolonged personal status among children we use the question in the school survey the
encounter. Inductees who were selected to be assessed for military of- children were asked to name up to three classmates who they would
ficer suitability5 were interviewed according to the guidelines from the like to see as a class party organizer. The distribution of number of peer
Central Conscription Authority. At the outset, the draft board had ac- nominations is shown in Fig. 3. The largest category consisted of those
cess to basic demographic information, test data from the draft as well who got no votes, while the maximum number of nominations was 26.
as average school mark at the end of the 9th grade. The evaluation was
to be based on intellectual capacity (measured in various tests, school
marks, and other previous accomplishments), attitudes towards the Independent variables
military, emotional stability, ability to be accepted by others, social
interaction skills, occupational ambitions, as well as physical fitness Family socioeconomic status was measured by social class based on
(Central Conscription Authority, 1952). According to the guidelines, the father's occupation in 1963.7 The information was originally col-
intellectual capacity generally accounted for approximately 40% of the lected from population register data, and coded into five categories by
variation in officer suitability. The recruit's motivation and ambition the original project group: Upper and upper middle class, Lower middle
was also mentioned as decisive factors, and evaluators were advised not class, officials, and non-agricultural employees, Lower middle class or
to let intellectual capacity influence the judgment of other factors too entrepreneurs, Working class (skilled workers), Working class (un-
greatly (personality, stability, social skills, etc.). Evaluating the recruit's skilled workers).
emotional stability was part of the purpose of the interview and was an The population registry was also the source of information about
element in the general assessment of officer suitability It should be birth order and was calculated on the base of data on older and younger
mentioned that the average time allotted for each recruit was 12 min, so siblings. Sibling status refers to the situation in 1963, when the cohort
the breadth and depth of topics was limited. The Central Conscription members were 10–11 years old, and may of course have changed later if
Authority was responsible for filing draft data. Data for most of the for example an only child got a younger sibling after that age.
variables were provided to the Project Metropolitan. Parental support is an index constructed by the original project staff
This study is based on the 6928 draftees from the original cohort of and based on 10 questions in the school survey about the family's at-
7719 boys.6 We are not able to specify the various grounds for attrition titudes towards higher education (e.g. “Do your parents think that
but the loss of observations was not random given the strict criteria for having a higher education will give a more secure future?”).8
Academic intrinsic motivation was measured by an index based on 10
questions in the school survey about interest in schoolwork (e.g. whe-
ther they read other books than textbooks about things they studied at
3
Codebooks and information about the Project are available at http://www.
stockholmbirthcohort.su.se/about-the-project/original-data-1953-1983.
4 7
In addition, the board could exempt others temporarily or indefinitely, and If information about the father was missing, data concerning the mother was
allocate yet others to limited service or to serve as conscientious objectors. used instead. This was also the case if the mother had an occupation in the
5
Befälsskattning in Swedish. upper-middle class or higher and the father's occupation was working class. For
6
The study has been approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board in more information on how these groups were constructed, see Codebook II.
8
Stockholm. See variable SCHOL16 in Codebook I for a full list of questions.

304
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

Table 1
Overview of variables, data sources, year information refers to and age of cohort members, and non-response rates.
Variable Database Source of information Year information refers to Non-response (%) (N = 6928)
(age of cohort members)

Outcomes
Military officer suitability National Conscription Board register Draft officer 1971–73 (18–20) 0
Nominations as class party organizer School survey Class mates 1966 (12−13) 16
Independent variables
Family socioeconomic status Population register Coded by original project 1963 (9–10) 15
Birth order Census 1964 Household members 1964 (10−11) 0
Parental support School survey Child 1966 (12–13) 10
Academic intrinsic motivation School survey Child 1966 (12–13) 10
IQ School survey Child 1966 (12–13) 13
Leader occupation aspiration School survey Child 1966 (12–13) 13
Delay of gratification School survey Child 1966 (12–13) 13
Expulsion from classroom School survey Child 1966 (12–13) 10
Membership in sports club/scouts School survey Child 1966 (12–13) 13
Frequency of extracurricular activity School survey Child 1966 (12–13) 11
Muscular capacity National Conscription Board register Draft officer 1971–73 (18–20) 5
Body height National Conscription Board register Draft officer 1971–73 (18–20) 5

Recruits with value 0 were not assessed


30
20
Percent
10
0

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
Assessment scores

Fig. 2. Assessment scores for military officer suitability (percent).

school, and whether they found homework dull).9 manager11 and (military) officer. Leader occupation aspiration was coded
Lastly, we included the children's IQ in the 6th grade. The measure into three categories depending on how many of these occupations the
is based on a spatial, verbal, and numerical sub-test as part of the school boy had chosen (both, one, or none).
survey. Each sub-test rendered a maximum score of 40 and, so, the Self-regulatory skills were measured in two ways: The first item is a
highest possible score overall was 120.10 The IQ scores in this study measure of capacity to suppress immediate gratification for the benefit
group varied between 12 and 116, with a mean of 70. In order to test of long-term reward. In the school survey the children were presented
the non-linear effect of IQ we also included the squared IQ score with a hypothetical question asking whether they would choose if they
(Simonton, 1985). had the prospect of receiving SEK 100 now or SEK 1000 in five years,
Motivation to be a leader was based on the pupils' answers to comparable to the test of self-control and delayed gratification in the
questions about what they want to be when they grow up. The children classical “Stanford Marshmallow experiment” (Mischel & Ebbesen,
were presented with a list of 56 different occupations and for each 1970). The second measure is based on a question in the school survey,
occupation the child was to indicate whether he/she “would like to which may reflect an ability to regulate emotions and curb dis-
become” (1), “do not know” (2), or “would not like to become” (3) this obedience, namely whether the pupil was ever told to leave the classroom
occupation. As a measure of motivation to lead we included two oc- during a lesson because of something he did.
cupations which are closely associated with leadership functions;

11
“Direktör” in the original Swedish version. This has been translated to
9
See variable SCHOL18 in the codebook for full list of questions. “manager” in Codebook I, but “director” is actually more appropriate because it
10
See Codebook I. has a clearer connotation to an executive or leadership role.

305
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

30
20
Percent
10
0

0 5 10 15 20 25
Number of votes

Fig. 3. Nominations for class party organizer (percent).

The foundational traits in the theoretical framework also consist of the first equation. However, we did not include the IQ score from the
physical characteristics. Two measures from the draft procedures were draft, but rather IQ as measured in the school survey. The reasons are
included as indicators of physical stature; body height (in centimeters) threefold: First, there was – not unexpectedly – a substantial overlap
and muscular power (in Newton, grouped on a scale 1–9). between IQ in the 6th grade and at the draft (r = 0.64) and as such
Developmental experiences are important for the practice of lea- these measures are congruent but not fully overlapping. Second – and
dership skills and exposure to arenas and situations that may be helpful more importantly – neither the draft board nor the inductee had any
for this purpose. In the school survey, the children were asked whether knowledge of the IQ score from 6th grade and as such it was not
they were members of different types of associations and clubs, and the “contaminated” by the draft situation where inductees might have
frequency of activity in such associations. They were allowed to provide wanted to manipulate results if they did not wish to be assessed for
up to three different types of memberships and for our purpose, we leadership capacity or to serve in the military at all. Third, the IQ score
focused on membership in a sports clubs and/or the scouts. Both alter- from the 6th grade was more widely dispersed, with scores varying
natives emphasize physical and mental training, teamwork, goal at- between 12 and 116, compared to a grouped score of 0–9 for IQ at the
tainment and competitiveness. The pupils were also asked about how draft. In the first equation, we also included body height and muscular
often they were busy with club or association activities, based on a capacity, which were directly available from the draft board. Muscular
presumption that the “dosage” of developmental experiences also power was easier to manipulate than body height but are overall highly
matters. Frequency of extracurricular activities was coded into four ca- correlated and were easily accessible and interpretable measures that
tegories; not a member of an organized activity, never, sometimes, may have influenced the initial categorization of candidates for as-
weekly. sessment albeit not as clearly as in the case of IQ.
Means, standard deviations, and correlations are presented in The second equation provides an estimate for the scores on the 1–9
Table 2a and the distribution between categories for categorical vari- scale among those who were selected for assessment. Because the
ables are presented in Table 2b. children in the original Project Metropolitan were grouped in 612
school classes in 1966 (the year the school survey was performed),
standard errors are probably not unbiased. Therefore, we also calcu-
Procedure lated clustered standard errors. Contrary to the first equation, all
variables were included in the second equation. Hence, the model in-
When it comes to the assessment of military officer suitability, we cludes information that was or might have been known by the draft
are dealing with a bounded distribution. Assessment of military officer board and/or inductee at the outset (birth order, family socioeconomic
suitability is bounded by zero, i.e. the value assigned to draftees who status), information which none of the parties had knowledge about or
were not assessed at all. When the dependent variable is bounded a remembered (e.g. parental support, leader occupation aspiration). The
hurdle regression is an appropriate statistical technique. model also includes information that may have emerged during the
Hurdle models were first proposed by Cragg (1971). A hurdle model assessment procedure (such as participation in sports).
resembles a tobit model, but the main difference is that a hurdle model In order to study the effect of factors relating to early life experi-
provides separate equations for the bounded and unbounded outcomes ences, leadership learning capacities and skills, foundational traits, and
whereas a tobit model uses the same equation for both. Hurdle models developmental experiences in childhood on leadership emergence “in
assume that the unbounded outcomes are the result of passing an initial the classroom”, we performed a negative binominal regression. This is
“hurdle”. The model consists of two equations. In the first equation, we an appropriate method for over-dispersed count data, without an ex-
estimate the chance of proceeding to the unbounded selection, i.e. of cessive number of zeros, where the conditional variance exceeds the
passing the “hurdle” (values 0 or 1).12 conditional mean (see Fig. 3). The dependent variable is number of
Because IQ was a central criterion at this stage, we included IQ in nominations the cohort member received from his classmates (anon-
ymously) for the potential role as class party organizer. The in-
12
Estimated with the churdle (Cragg-Hurdle) command in Stata 15.1. dependent variables are all those related to childhood, i.e. all variables

306
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

Table 2a
Descriptive statistics: Means, standard deviations, and (Pearson r) correlations (N = 6928).
Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13

1. Military officer 3.72 3.05


suitability
2. Nominations for class 2.47 3.09 0.18⁎⁎⁎
party organizer

Family socio-economic status


3. Unskilled worker 0.17 0.37 −0.18⁎⁎⁎ −0.07⁎⁎⁎
4. Working class, skilled 0.23 0.42 −0.12⁎⁎⁎ −0.07⁎⁎⁎ −0.24⁎⁎⁎
workers
5. Lower middle class, 0.08 0.26 0.00 0.02 −0.13⁎⁎⁎ −0.15⁎⁎⁎
entre-preneurs
6. Lower middle class 0.36 0.48 0.07⁎⁎⁎ 0.04⁎⁎ −0.34⁎⁎⁎ −0.41⁎⁎⁎ −0.21⁎⁎⁎
7. Upper, upper-middle 0.17 0.37 0.22⁎⁎⁎ 0.09⁎⁎⁎ −0.20⁎⁎⁎ −0.24⁎⁎⁎ −0.13⁎⁎⁎ −0.34⁎⁎⁎
class

Birth order
8.Youngest child 0.30 0.46 −0.02 0.05⁎⁎⁎ −0.03⁎ −0.04⁎⁎ 0.01 0.01 0.05⁎⁎⁎
9. Only child 0.21 0.41 0.02 −0.01 0.01 0.03⁎ −0.01 0.04⁎⁎ −0.08⁎⁎⁎ −0.34⁎⁎⁎
10. First born 0.32 0.47 0.05⁎⁎⁎ −0.01 −0.00 0.00 −0.02 0.02 −0.02 −0.45⁎⁎⁎ −0.35⁎⁎⁎
11. Middle child 0.17 0.38 −0.05⁎⁎⁎ −0.04⁎⁎ 0.03⁎ 0.01 0.03⁎ −0.07⁎⁎⁎ 0.04⁎⁎ −0.30⁎⁎⁎ −0.23⁎⁎⁎ −0.31⁎⁎⁎
12. Parental support 6.10 2.26 0.36⁎⁎⁎ 0.17⁎⁎⁎ −0.19⁎⁎⁎ −0.13⁎⁎⁎ −0.00 0.10⁎⁎⁎ 0.21⁎⁎⁎ −0.00 0.04⁎⁎ 0.03⁎ −0.07⁎⁎⁎
13. Academic intrinsic 4.87 2.46 0.13⁎⁎⁎ 0.06⁎⁎⁎ −0.06⁎⁎⁎ −0.02 −0.03⁎ 0.03⁎ 0.06⁎⁎⁎ −0.06⁎⁎⁎ 0.05⁎⁎⁎ 0.05⁎⁎⁎ −0.04⁎⁎ 0.25⁎⁎⁎
motivation
14. IQ 69.47 18.09 0.54⁎⁎⁎ 0.20⁎⁎⁎ −0.19⁎⁎⁎ −0.13⁎⁎⁎ 0.01 0.09⁎⁎⁎ 0.21⁎⁎⁎ −0.02 0.03⁎ 0.04⁎⁎ −0.06⁎⁎⁎ 0.43⁎⁎⁎ 0.07⁎⁎⁎

Delay of gratification
15. Certainly SEK 100 0.06 0.24 −0.07⁎⁎⁎ −0.01 0.05⁎⁎⁎ 0.02 −0.04⁎⁎ 0.01 −0.05⁎⁎⁎ −0.00 −0.01 −0.01 0.03⁎ −0.08⁎⁎⁎ −0.05⁎⁎⁎
now
16. Probably SEK 100 0.05 0.22 −0.03⁎ −0.04⁎ 0.02 0.02 −0.02 −0.01 −0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 −0.01 −0.03⁎⁎ −0.01
now
17. Cannot choose 0.06 0.25 −0.06 ⁎⁎⁎
−0.03 ⁎
−0.00 0.01 0.01 0.01 −0.03 ⁎
0.02 −0.01 −0.01 −0.01 −0.05⁎⁎⁎
0.00
18. Probably SEK 1000 0.33 0.47 0.02 0.00 0.00 0.00 −0.02 −0.00 0.01 0.01 0.00 −0.00 −0.02 0.01 −0.01
in five years
19. Certainly SEK 1000 0.49 0.50 0.06⁎⁎⁎ 0.03⁎ −0.03⁎ −0.03⁎ 0.04⁎⁎ 0.00 0.03⁎ −0.02 0.00 0.01 0.01 0.07⁎⁎⁎ 0.03⁎
in five years

Expulsion from classroom


20. Never expelled 0.26 0.44 0.03⁎ −0.02 −0.01 0.02 −0.01 −0.01 0.01 −0.05⁎⁎⁎ 0.03⁎ 0.04⁎⁎ −0.02 0.04⁎⁎ 0.18⁎⁎⁎
21. Once 0.54 0.50 0.03⁎ 0.02 −0.00 −0.02 −0.00 0.01 0.01 0.03⁎ −0.02 −0.00 −0.02 0.03⁎ 0.05⁎⁎⁎
22. Twice or more 0.21 0.41 −0.06⁎⁎⁎ 0.00 0.01 0.00 0.02 −0.00 −0.02 0.01 −0.01 −0.04⁎⁎⁎ 0.04⁎⁎⁎ −0.08⁎⁎⁎ −0.25⁎⁎⁎

Leader occupation aspiration


23. None 0.41 0.49 −0.14⁎⁎⁎ −0.02 0.05⁎⁎⁎ 0.04⁎⁎ 0.02 −0.04⁎⁎ −0.06⁎⁎⁎ −0.01 −0.02 −0.00 0.04⁎⁎ −0.16⁎⁎⁎ −0.03⁎
24. One 0.43 0.50 0.11⁎⁎⁎ 0.05⁎⁎⁎ −0.07⁎⁎⁎ −0.02 −0.02 0.04⁎⁎ 0.05⁎⁎⁎ −0.01 0.02 0.00 −0.02 0.13⁎⁎⁎ 0.02
25. Both 0.16 0.37 0.03⁎ −0.04⁎⁎ 0.02 −0.02 −0.01 −0.00 0.01 0.02 −0.01 0.00 −0.03 0.04⁎⁎ 0.01
26. Body height 179.66 6.51 0.17⁎⁎⁎ 0.01 −0.05⁎⁎⁎ −0.03⁎ −0.01 0.03⁎⁎ 0.05⁎⁎⁎ 0.02 0.02 0.00 −0.05 ⁎⁎⁎
0.10⁎⁎⁎
0.03⁎
27. Muscular capacity 5.18 1.69 0.11 ⁎⁎⁎
0.01 0.02 −0.01 0.02 −0.02 −0.00 −0.01 −0.01 −0.01 0.03 ⁎⁎
−0.01 0.00

Membership in scouts and/or sports club


28. Neither 0.60 0.49 −0.13⁎⁎⁎ −0.11⁎⁎⁎ 0.01 0.02 −0.00 −0.02 −0.00 −0.01 0.02 −0.01 0.01 −0.07⁎⁎⁎ −0.03⁎⁎
29. Scouts 0.06 0.23 0.12⁎⁎⁎ 0.06⁎⁎⁎ −0.06⁎⁎⁎ −0.04⁎⁎ −0.01 0.04⁎⁎ 0.06⁎⁎⁎ −0.03⁎⁎ 0.01 0.04⁎⁎⁎ −0.03⁎ 0.09⁎⁎⁎ 0.04⁎⁎
30. Sport 0.34 0.47 0.07⁎⁎⁎ 0.07⁎⁎⁎ 0.03⁎ 0.00 0.00 −0.00 −0.03⁎ 0.03⁎ −0.02 −0.01 0.01 0.02 0.01
31. Both 0.01 0.09 0.06⁎⁎⁎ 0.04⁎⁎ −0.03⁎ −0.02 0.00 0.03⁎ 0.01 −0.02 −0.01 0.02 0.01 0.05⁎⁎⁎ 0.04⁎⁎⁎

Frequency extracurricular activity


32. Never 0.11 0.32 −0.12⁎⁎⁎ −0.07⁎⁎⁎ 0.05⁎⁎⁎ 0.01 0.03 −0.04⁎⁎ −0.03⁎ 0.03⁎ 0.02 −0.05⁎⁎⁎ 0.02 −0.08⁎⁎⁎ −0.04⁎⁎
33. Sometimes 0.14 0.35 −0.01 0.01 −0.04⁎⁎ −0.01 0.01 −0.00 0.04⁎⁎ 0.01 0.01 −0.01 −0.00 0.01 0.02
34. Weekly 0.46 0.50 0.10⁎⁎⁎ 0.12⁎⁎⁎ 0.00 −0.02 −0.01 0.02 −0.01 −0.01 −0.02 0.03⁎ −0.00 0.06⁎⁎⁎ 0.04⁎⁎⁎
35. Not member in a club 0.28 0.45 −0.03 −0.08⁎⁎⁎ −0.01 0.01 −0.02 0.01 −0.01 −0.02 0.01 0.02 −0.01 −0.01 −0.03⁎
or association

Variable 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26

14. IQ

Delay of gratification
15. Certainly SEK 100 now −0.10⁎⁎⁎
16. Probably SEK 100 now −0.04⁎⁎ −0.06⁎⁎⁎
17. Cannot choose −0.06⁎⁎⁎ −0.07⁎⁎⁎ −0.06⁎⁎⁎
18. Probably SEK 1000 in five years 0.06⁎⁎⁎ −0.18⁎⁎⁎ −0.17⁎⁎⁎ −0.19⁎⁎⁎
19. Certainly SEK 1000 in five years 0.04⁎⁎ −0.25⁎⁎⁎ −0.23⁎⁎⁎ −0.26⁎⁎⁎ −0.69⁎⁎⁎

Expulsion from classroom


20. Never expelled 0.07⁎⁎⁎ −0.02 0.03⁎⁎ 0.02 −0.01 −0.01
21. Once −0.03⁎ −0.01 −0.01 −0.01 0.00 0.01 −0.63⁎⁎⁎
22. Twice or more −0.05⁎⁎⁎ 0.03⁎ −0.02 −0.01 0.00 −0.00 −0.30⁎⁎⁎ −0.55⁎⁎⁎

(continued on next page)

307
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

Table 2a (continued)

Variable 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26

Leader occupation aspiration


23. None −0.13⁎⁎⁎ 0.04⁎⁎ 0.03⁎ 0.05⁎⁎⁎ −0.03 −0.03⁎⁎ 0.02 −0.01 −0.00
24. One 0.12⁎⁎⁎ −0.04⁎⁎ −0.02 −0.02 0.02 0.02 0.01 0.00 −0.01 −0.72⁎⁎⁎
25. Both 0.02 −0.00 −0.01 −0.04⁎⁎ 0.00 0.02 −0.03⁎ 0.01 0.02 −0.36⁎⁎⁎ −0.38⁎⁎⁎
26. Body height 0.14⁎⁎⁎ −0.02 −0.00 −0.02 −0.01 0.03 0.01 0.00 −0.01 −0.04⁎⁎ 0.02 0.03⁎

Variable 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35

14. IQ −0.00 −0.07 ⁎⁎⁎


0.10 ⁎⁎⁎
0.01 0.06 ⁎⁎⁎
−0.13 ⁎⁎⁎
0.00 0.04 ⁎⁎
0.05⁎⁎⁎

Delay of gratification
15. Certainly SEK 100 now −0.03⁎ 0.03⁎ −0.02 −0.02 −0.01 0.03⁎ −0.01 −0.03⁎ 0.01
16. Probably SEK 100 now −0.00 0.01 −0.01 −0.01 −0.01 0.00 0.01 −0.02 0.02
17. Cannot choose −0.02 0.01 0.00 −0.01 −0.02 0.02 0.01 −0.02 0.00
18. Probably SEK 1000 in five years −0.01 0.01 −0.03⁎ −0.00 0.01 −0.02 0.02 −0.02 0.03⁎
19. Certainly SEK 1000 in five years 0.03⁎ −0.04⁎⁎ 0.04⁎⁎ 0.02 0.01 −0.01 −0.02 0.05⁎⁎⁎ −0.04⁎⁎

Expulsion from classroom


20. Never expelled −0.06⁎⁎⁎ 0.08⁎⁎⁎ 0.01 −0.08⁎⁎⁎ −0.02 0.02 −0.00 −0.07⁎⁎⁎ 0.07⁎⁎⁎
21. Once 0.01 −0.03⁎ 0.00 0.03⁎ 0.01 −0.02 −0.00 0.03⁎⁎ −0.02
22. Twice or more 0.05⁎⁎⁎ −0.05⁎⁎⁎ −0.01 0.06⁎⁎⁎ 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.04⁎⁎ −0.05⁎⁎⁎

Leader occupation aspiration


23. None −0.02 0.06⁎⁎⁎ −0.04⁎⁎ −0.04⁎⁎ −0.01 0.04⁎⁎ 0.00 −0.05⁎⁎⁎ 0.02
24. One 0.02 −0.05⁎⁎⁎ 0.02 0.04⁎⁎ 0.02 −0.06⁎⁎⁎ −0.00 0.04⁎⁎ 0.00
25. Both 0.01 −0.01 0.02 0.00 −0.02 0.02 0.00 0.02 −0.04⁎⁎
26. Body height 0.23⁎⁎⁎ 0.00 0.02 −0.01 0.00 −0.04⁎⁎ 0.02 −0.02 0.02

Variable 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35

27. Muscular capacity

Membership in scouts and/or sports club


28. Neither −0.07⁎⁎⁎
29. Scouts −0.01 −0.30⁎⁎⁎
30. Sport 0.08⁎⁎⁎ −0.87⁎⁎⁎ −0.18⁎⁎⁎
31. Both −0.01 −0.11⁎⁎⁎ −0.02 −0.07⁎⁎⁎

Frequency extracurricular activity


32. Never −0.02 0.19⁎⁎⁎ −0.08⁎⁎⁎ −0.15⁎⁎⁎ −0.03⁎
33. Sometimes −0.01 0.02 −0.05⁎⁎⁎ 0.01 −0.03⁎ −0.15⁎⁎⁎
34. Weekly 0.05⁎⁎⁎ −0.59⁎⁎⁎ 0.23⁎⁎⁎ 0.48⁎⁎⁎ 0.09⁎⁎⁎ −0.33⁎⁎⁎ −0.38⁎⁎⁎
35. Not member in a club or association −0.03⁎ 0.51⁎⁎⁎ −0.15⁎⁎⁎ −0.44⁎⁎⁎ −0.06⁎⁎⁎ −0.22⁎⁎⁎ −0.26⁎⁎⁎ −0.58⁎⁎⁎

apart from those pertaining to the draft. in the National Conscription Board register (N = 6928).
In both the analysis of military officer suitability and nominations In addition to missing observations, there was item non-response on
for class party organizer, the models were tested with and without fixed most variables (see Table 1).13 There are several ways of handling in-
effects of school classes by use of a Chow test. In the first case there complete data with different gains and losses. For item non-response we
were no significant differences between the models performed multiple imputations.14
(prob > Chi2 = 0.149) and therefore we did not use fixed effects in
this case. In the latter case, however, the Chow test did show a sig- Post-estimations
nificant difference between the models with and without fixed effects.
Therefore, the analyses of nominations for class party organizer include All post-estimations were based on listwise deletion of missing data.
fixed effects for school classes (Halaby, 2004). For the Hurdle model we calculated the average marginal effects (dy/
dx), i.e. how each additional change in x is associated with a change in
Missing data y.15 In order to post-estimate effects in the negative binominal regres-
sion analysis we calculated the incidence ratio relationship (IRR).
Although the data set is very comprehensive and has a high cov-
erage rate, there are of course missing data. First, the Conscription
13
Board register included information on 6928 individuals from the ori- Among the draftees 390 individuals (5.3%) had not participated in the
ginal sample of 7719. The loss of observations between these points is school survey at all, presumably mainly due to absence from school when the
caused by death, emigration, or exemption from draft due to religious survey was carried out.
14
Multiple imputation is not supported by Stata for Cragg-Hurdle or negative
faith, severe handicap, already serving in the military, working as a
binominal regressions, but the cmdok command allows unsupported estima-
seaman or other valid grounds for exemption. It is, however, not pos-
tions with multiple imputations estimates. Because the data set is large and
sible to break down the loss of observations by different causes. Because consists of both categorical and continuous variables imputations were per-
the vast majority of male cohorts were drafted at the time, the no-show formed with fully conditional specification (FCS). The estimated model has a
group is likely to be quite select and it is therefore not reasonable to minimum degree of freedom > 100. We used the Stata command “how_ma-
impute values for missing observations. The point of departure in this ny_imputations” to determine the number of imputations (von Hippel, 2018).
study is, then, males in the Stockholm Birth Cohort who were registered 15
Using the “margins, dydx (*)” command in Stata.

308
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

Table 2b effects (dy/dx) which represent the conditional average change in y


Descriptive statistics: distribution among categorical variables from a one-unit change in x. The confidence intervals also provide a
(N = 6928). useful hint of the level of uncertainty for each variable.
Variable Percent Among the variables measuring early life experiences, Table 4
suggests that family socioeconomic status in childhood strongly influ-
Early life experiences ences leadership assessment, and the association is positive and linear.
Family socioeconomic status
It is worth pointing out that we have controlled for other factors such as
Unskilled workers 3
Working class, skilled workers 16 IQ, which almost consistently varies between social groups in favor of
Lower middle class, entrepreneurs 22 higher social classes (Fischbein, 1980).
Lower middle class 7 The effect is not great – 0.12 units on the 1–9 scale – but Table 4
Upper and upper-middle class 35
shows that being firstborn does have an effect on suitability assessment
Missing 16
Birth order
compared to being the youngest among siblings. Parental support also
Youngest child 30 showed a positive but weak effect on leadership assessment. The same
Only child 21 applies to academic intrinsic motivation.
First born 32 Most notable in Table 4 is the effect of IQ also on the actual as-
Middle child 17
sessment of officer suitability. For every increase of one standard de-
Foundational traits
Delay of gratification viation on IQ, we see a rise of 1.33 points on the 1–9 assessment scale.
Certainly SEK 100 now 5 Delay of gratification (“marshmallow” measure) showed no effect
Probably SEK 100 now 5 on leadership assessment, but the other measure of self-regulatory skills
Cannot choose 6
showed a slight impact: Being expelled from the classroom was slightly
Probably SEK 1000 in five years 30
Certainly SEK 1000 in five years 44
“beneficial” for leadership assessment if the draftee had been expelled
Missing 10 on one occasion.
Expulsion from classroom Draftees who aspired to become leadership occupations when they
Never expelled 23 were in the 6th grade (an army officer or director) did receive a higher
Once 48
assessment score compared to peers who did not have such ambitions.
Twice or more 19
Missing 10 The average increase in assessment ratings was around 0.17 regardless
Leader occupation aspiration of whether they had wished to become one, or both, of these occupa-
None 36 tions.
One 38 Among other foundational traits, Table 4 shows that a one-cen-
Both (director and manager) 14
Missing 13
timeter increase in body height had an average impact on suitability
Developmental experiences assessment of 0.03 points. This may not seem much at first glance, but
Membership in scouts and/or sport club equals an average increase of 0.3 on the leadership assessment score
Neither 52 between a body height of 1.65 and 1.75 m. Muscular power seems to
Scouts 5
have approximately the same impact as body height, but they cannot be
Sport 30
Both 1 compared directly because they are measured on different scales.
Missing 13 When it comes to developmental experiences being a member of a
Frequency extracurricular activity sports club or the scouts is clearly influential for the assessment of
Never 10 leadership capability. As shown in Table 4 the association is particu-
Sometimes 13
Weekly 41
larly strong for scouting, with an average increase of 0.53 on the officer
Not member in a club or association 25 suitability score, compared to a 0.31 increase for sports club member-
Missing 11 ship. The difference between these two is statistically significant at the
0.001 level. Moreover, the “dosage” of developmental experiences was
important in the sense that frequency of extracurricular activities meant
Stata does not calculate elasticities for fractional variables (ey/ex) an increase in officer suitability score. There was, however, no sig-
so we have presented marginal effects in Figs. 4 and 5 with dy/dx. nificant difference between the intensity of activities (sometimes or
weekly) – the important thing is to participate.
Results In order to visualize marginal effects we present these graphically
for IQ and family socioeconomic status in Figs. 4 and 5. IQ is a foun-
Two Cragg-Hurdle regression models are presented in Table 3. One dational trait of the individual and family socioeconomic status is an
presents coefficients and standard errors using listwise deletion and the environmental factor describing early life experiences. Table 4 portrays
other using multiple imputation. Within each model the first two col- the average marginal effects, while Figs. 4 and 5 illustrate the effects for
umns present coefficients and standard errors for being chosen for as- each value of the respective variables thereby also visualizing the
sessment at all, i.e. passing the “hurdle”, and the next two columns curvilinear pattern for IQ.
presenting ditto for the actual assessment on the 1–9 scale for those
who did pass the “hurdle”. Informal ascription of leadership: Nominations as class party organizer
First, we note that the results do not differ much between the two
models based on listwise deletion and multiple imputation respectively. As a secondary analysis we studied the informal ascription of lea-
Second, IQ had a significant, positive and curvilinear effect on passing dership status in childhood through a negative binominal regression on
the “hurdle”. Moreover, body height and muscular power also showed a nominations as class party organizer (in the 6th grade). The results are
significant impact in the first step – as could be expected. Third, all the presented in Table 5. In the same way as for the Cragg-Hurdle regres-
included factors also showed a significant impact on the actual assess- sion, Table 5 presents results for both listwise deletion and multiple
ment of military officer suitability, but the curvilinear effect of IQ was imputation. Again, we see very slight differences between these two
not significant in this stage. models.
The coefficients in Table 3 are not directly interpretable. We cannot Interpretable post-estimations of regression effects (incidence rate
conclude that a certain change in x produces a given change in y. To ratios, IRR) are presented in Table 6. Keeping all other covariates at a
obtain the effect of a covariate on the model, we calculated marginal constant level (means), IRR measures ratio of certain events for

309
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

8
Predictive margins with 95 % CIs
6
4
2
0

-4 -2 0 2
IQ (standardized)

Fig. 4. Conditional mean estimates of military officer suitability.


5
Predictive margins with 95 % CIs
4
3

1 2 3 4 5
Social class 1963 (1 highest, 2 lowest)

Fig. 5. Conditional mean estimates of military officer suitability.

categorical variables. That is, the number of events (in this case no- nominations. In contrast to the model for assessment of military officer
minations as class party organizer) per unit of time (school lesson), suitability, there was no curvilinear pattern between IQ and nomina-
divided by the groups we are studying – analogous to percent for tions for class party organizer.
continuous variables. The interpretation of IRR is similar to that of dy/ As in the case of military officer assessment, delay of gratification
dx, but a more familiar comparison would be odds ratios (OR). If the did not have any impact on nominations whatsoever. Expulsion from
confidence interval includes 1, the ratio is not significant. the classroom, however, was influential – and there seemed to be a
For family socioeconomic status there was a significant impact for all difference between being expelled once compared to several times
classes except the two lowest (unskilled and skilled workers). If the boy (p = 0.08). Wanting to become a leader, i.e. aspiring to become a di-
came from the highest social class, the incidence rate increased 0.46 times rector or manager, did not increase the chances of receiving many
compared to the lowest social class, controlling for the other variables in nominations as class party organizer. In fact, wanting to become both a
the model. As far as birth order is concerned, the youngest siblings were director and a manager significantly decreased the likelihood of being
the only group to show a significant impact on nominations. Boys who nominated for class party organizer.
perceived to have strong parental support were also more likely to receive Developmental experiences are more tangible and known to other
more nominations. Academic intrinsic motivation did not, however, show pupils, which may be one reason that participation in sports and in
any effect on nominations for class party organizer. extracurricular activities was clearly positively associated with nomi-
Among the foundational traits, IQ showed a strong effect on nations. For those who were most active (weekly) the impact on

310
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

Table 3
Cragg hurdle regression estimates for score on assessment of military officer suitability (0–9, truncated at 0). Clustered standard errors by 612 school classes.
Listwise deletionb (N = 5069) Multiple imputationc (N = 6928)

Estimates for being assessed Estimates for assessment Estimates for being assessed Estimates for assessment

Independent variables Coefficient S.E. Coefficient S.E. Coefficient S.E. Coefficient S.E.

Constant −2.12⁎⁎⁎ 0.58 −0.17 0.94 −2.15⁎⁎⁎ 0.56 −0.66 0.92

Early life experiences


Family socioeconomic status
Unskilled workersa
Working class, skilled workers 0.33⁎ 0.14 0.33⁎⁎ 0.13
Lower middle class, entrepreneurs 0.53⁎⁎ 0.18 0.41⁎⁎ 0.15
Lower middle class 0.66⁎⁎⁎ 0.11 0.65⁎⁎⁎ 0.10
Upper & upper-middle class 1.01⁎⁎⁎ 0.12 1.03⁎⁎⁎ 0.12
Birth order
Youngest childa
Only child 0.01 0.09 0.05 0.09
First born 0.16⁎ 0.08 0.15⁎ 0.08
Middle child −0.02 0.10 −0.01 0.09
Parental support 0.11⁎⁎⁎ 0.02 0.11⁎⁎⁎ 0.02

Leadership learning capacities and skills


Academic intrinsic motivation 0.07⁎⁎⁎ 0.01 0.07⁎⁎⁎ 0.01

Foundational traits
IQ in 6th grade (standardized) 0.87⁎⁎⁎ 0.03 0.45⁎⁎⁎ 0.05 0.79⁎⁎⁎ 0.02 0.52⁎⁎⁎ 0.06
IQ squared −0.11⁎⁎⁎ 0.02 0.03 0.03 −0.11⁎⁎⁎ 0.02 0.01 0.03
Delay of gratification
Certainly SEK 100 nowa
Probably SEK 100 now 0.09 0.22 −0.01 0.21
Cannot choose −0.04 0.20 −0.08 0.19
Probably SEK 1000 in five years 0.13 0.16 0.08 0.15
Certainly SEK 1000 in five years 0.12 0.16 0.07 0.15
Expulsion from classroom
Never expelleda
Once 0.15 0.08 0.12 0.07
Twice or more −0.10 0.10 −0.12 0.10
Both
Leader occupation aspiration
Nonea
One 0.22⁎⁎ 0.07 0.21⁎⁎ 0.07
Both 0.23⁎ 0.10 0.21⁎ 0.09
Body height 0.02⁎⁎⁎ 0.00 0.01⁎ 0.01 0.02⁎⁎⁎ 0.00 0.01⁎⁎ 0.01
Muscular capacity 0.03⁎⁎ 0.01 0.19⁎⁎⁎ 0.02 0.04⁎⁎ 0.01 0.18⁎⁎⁎ 0.02

Developmental experiences
Membership in scouts and/or sports club
Neithera
Scouts 0.72⁎⁎⁎ 0.14 0.74⁎⁎⁎ 0.13
Sport 0.42⁎⁎⁎ 0.08 0.36⁎⁎⁎ 0.08
Both 0.69⁎⁎ 0.26 0.75⁎⁎ 0.27
Frequency extracurricular activity
Nevera
Sometimes 0.29⁎ 0.13 0.24⁎ 0.13
Weekly 0.34⁎⁎ 0.13 0.34⁎⁎ 0.13
Not member in a club or association 0.23 0.14 0.18 0.12

a
Reference category.
b
Pseudo R2 = 0.11.
c
20 imputations.

p < 0.05.
⁎⁎
p < 0.01.
⁎⁎⁎
p < 0.001.

nominations was almost twice as strong compared to those who were while the air rang. Ralph looked at him, eager to offer something.
less active (sometimes). It should be noted, though, that scouting was ‘The choir belongs to you, of course.’
not favorable vis-à-vis nominations as class party organizer. ‘They could be the army – ’.
‘Or hunters – ’.
Discussion ‘They could be – ’.
The suffusion drained away from Jack's face. Ralph waved again for
Ralph counted. silence.
‘I'm chief then.’ ‘Jack's in charge of the choir. They can be – what do you want them
The circle of boys broke into applause. Even the choir applauded; to be?’
and the freckles on Jack's face disappeared under a blush of morti- ‘Hunters.’
fication. He started up, then changed his mind and sat down again

311
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

Table 4 the 6th grade. All of these factors turned out to be important for lea-
Conditional mean estimates of assessment of military officer suitability. Cragg dership emergence although in varying ways.
hurdle regression (N = 5069). Social class was an influential factor for leadership ascription both
dy/dxa 95% CI in childhood and for assessment of leadership capability at the draft. As
noted in so much social research, the imprint of social class is evident at
Independent variables an early age and seems to be maintained and perhaps emphasized in
young adulthood. Position among siblings also turned out to be influ-
Early life experiences
Family socioeconomic status ential in this study, but in diverging directions. As could be expected,
Unskilled workersb the eldest sibling was more likely to be considered officer material. The
Working class, skilled workers 0.24 0.04 0.43 pattern was reverse for arranging a class party, with the younger sibling
Lower middle class, entrepreneurs 0.39 0.12 0.64
being the favored choice. This is perhaps surprising but maybe rea-
Lower middle class 0.48 0.32 0.64
Upper and upper-middle class 0.74 0.58 0.90
sonable given the different tasks at hand. Studies have shown that later-
Birth order born children are more popular among peers compared to first-borns. A
Youngest childb suggested explanation is that younger siblings are more likely to de-
Only child 0.01 −0.11 0.13 velop interpersonal or social skills in order to compensate for their
First born 0.12 0.00 0.23
power deficit vis-à-vis older siblings (Miller & Maruyama, 1976). Social
Middle child −0.01 −0.15 0.12
Parental support 0.08 0.06 0.10 skills are definitely relevant for party organizers, while the military are
Leadership learning capacities and skills probably searching for that extra sense of responsibility and authority
Academic intrinsic motivation 0.05 0.03 0.07 often associated with first-born.
Foundational traits
Parental support also showed a positive effect on leadership emer-
IQ in sixth grade (standardized) 1.33 1.27 1.41
Delay of gratification
gence in both instances, although not as notably in early adulthood. It is
Certainly SEK 100 nowb plausible that the “influence” of parent support has worn off by the time
Probably SEK 100 now 0.07 −0.23 0.37 the cohort has reached early adulthood or rather transported through
Cannot choose −0.03 −0.31 0.26 educational and career choices, attitudes, emotional and physical
Probably SEK 1000 in five years 0.09 −0.14 0.32
status, etc. Parental support may provide the offspring with a general
Certainly SEK 1000 in five years 0.09 −0.14 0.32
Expulsion from classroom self-confidence, but it may also have materialized in terms of en-
Never expelledb couragement of leadership role occupancy or even specific attitudes
Once 0.11 −0.00 0.22 towards the military as such (Legree et al., 2000). In contrast, academic
Twice or more −0.08 −0.22 0.07
intrinsic motivation did not have a particularly strong impact on lea-
Leader occupation aspiration
Noneb
dership emergence in any instance.
One 0.16 0.06 0.26 Among the foundational traits, IQ stands out as particularly im-
Both 0.17 0.03 0.31 portant for leadership emergence, i.e. ascription or assessment of lea-
Body height 0.03 0.02 0.04 dership qualities. As such the results are compatible with those from a
Muscular capacity 0.17 0.13 0.21
large-scale, long-term prospective study of two British cohorts in which
Developmental experiences
Membership in scouts and/or sports club childhood cognitive ability was found to be highly influential for actual
Neitherb leadership role occupation at various stages in adulthood (Daly et al.,
Scouts 0.53 0.34 0.73 2015). In our study, the patterns of association did differ somewhat
Sports 0.31 0.19 0.43 between childhood and early adulthood though. In the former case the
Both 0.50 0.12 0.89
Frequency extracurricular activity
impact of IQ showed a linear patterns, while the effect of IQ wore off at
Neverb the highest levels in relation to military officer suitability. These results
Sometimes 0.21 0.02 0.40 suggest that there is a limit to the value of IQ, so to speak, in the sense
Weekly 0.25 0.08 0.42 that too much of a good thing can be problematic (Antonakis et al.,
Not member in a club or association 0.17 −0.02 0.36
2017).
a
dy/dx for factor levels is the discrete change from the base level. The well-known “marshmallow” test has been used in different
b
Reference category. forms to measure a component of self-control thought to be associated
with various outcomes. Our analyses showed that this factor had no
effect whatsoever in any of the models. Recent studies have shown that
the ability to delay gratification is an aspect of self-regulation that is
Commanding a troop at war is one thing – organizing a class party closely associated with cognitive ability and sensitive to inclusion of
in the 6th grade something quite different. Having said that, deciding control variables (Burks, Carpenter, Goette, & Rustichini, 2009;
on which qualities are relevant for the task and identifying individuals Dohmen, Falk, Huffman, & Sunde, 2010; Watts, Duncan, & Quan,
who have these qualities can certainly be challenging in both cases. For 2018). A closer look at this relationship showed that delay of gratifi-
Jack and Ralph, two youngsters struggling to survive on the desert is- cation had no connection to choice of class party organizer at all (re-
land, leadership is more similar to commanding troops than directing a gardless of inclusion of control variables), while its effect was neu-
choir – or organizing a party. tralized by IQ in relation to officer suitability. Other self-regulatory
Leadership ascription in childhood and leadership assessment in skills were of some interest though. Being expelled from the classroom
early adulthood takes place in different contexts at different points in (even on several occasions) was positively linked to nominations a class
the life cycle, and is steered by diverging considerations. Still, this study party organizer, but the effect had diminished by the time the boys
has shown that the process of identifying leadership qualities share were drafted. A certain degree of rebelliousness can be advantageous
many similarities despite varying contexts and phases in life. Also, the vis-à-vis leadership emergence, but maybe less so in a military context
study provides ample support for the application of a longitudinal and where discipline and order are fundamental. As discussed in the lim-
life-cycle approach to leadership emergence as represented by the itations section, peer nominations may simply reflect perceived popu-
theoretical framework in Fig. 1. larity rather than leadership qualities and popularity in childhood and
The early life experiences analyzed in this study focused on the adolescence may certainly be a double-edged sword. Allen et al. (2005)
socioeconomic situation in the family of origin, the pupil's rank among found that popularity in adolescence was associated with positive
siblings, and parental support as perceived by the pupil when he was in markers of social development and functioning, but also increased

312
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

Table 5
Negative binominal regression estimates for nominations as class party organizer.a
Listwise deletion. Clustered standard errors by 607 school Multiple imputation.c Clustered standard errors by 612 school
classes.b N = 4654 classes. N = 6928

Independent variables Coefficient S.E. Coefficient S.E.

Constant 0.62⁎⁎⁎ 0.15 0.52⁎⁎⁎ 0.13


Early life experiences
Family socioeconomic status
Unskilled workersd
Working class, skilled workers −0.00 0.07 −0.00 0.07
Lower middle class, entrepreneurs 0.21⁎ 0.09 0.17⁎ 0.09
Lower middle class 0.20⁎⁎ 0.06 0.17⁎⁎⁎ 0.06
Upper and upper-middle class 0.38⁎⁎⁎ 0.08 0.33⁎⁎⁎ 0.08
Birth order
Youngest childd
Only child −0.14⁎ 0.06 −0.10⁎ 0.05
First born −0.19⁎⁎⁎ 0.05 −0.12⁎⁎ 0.05
Middle child −0.19⁎⁎ 0.07 −0.17⁎⁎ 0.05
Parental support 0.07⁎⁎⁎ 0.01 0.06⁎⁎⁎ 0.01
Leadership learning capacities and skills
Academic intrinsic motivation 0.00 0.00 0.01 0.01
Foundational traits
IQ in 6th grade (standardized) 0.31⁎⁎⁎ 0.023 0.26⁎⁎⁎ 0.03
IQ squared 0.02 0.01 0.02 0.02
Delay of gratification
Certainly SEK 100 nowd
Probably SEK 100 now −0.22 0.13 −0.17 0.11
Cannot choose −0.24 0.12 −0.14 0.09
Probably SEK 1000 in five years −0.09 0.10 −0.04 0.07
Certainly SEK 1000 in five years −0.13 0.10 −0.05 0.07
Expulsion from classroom
Never expelledd
Once 0.12⁎ 0.05 0.12⁎⁎ 0.04
Twice or more 0.22⁎⁎ 0.07 0.19⁎⁎ 0.06
Leader occupation aspiration
Noned
One −0.00 0.05 −0.00 0.04
Both −0.11 0.06 −0.12⁎ 0.05
Developmental experiences
Membership in scouts and/or sport club
Neitherd
Scouts −0.01 0.11 0.06 0.09
Sport 0.15⁎⁎ 0.05 0.13⁎⁎ 0.05
Both 0.31 0.19 0.23 0.17
Frequency extracurricular activity
Neverd
Sometimes 0.18⁎ 0.08 0.13⁎ 0.07
Weekly 0.33⁎⁎⁎ 0.08 0.31⁎⁎⁎ 0.06
Not member in a club or association 0.10 0.07 0.06 0.06

a
Fixed effects of school classes included.
b
Pseudo R2 = 0.05.
c
20 imputations.
d
Reference category.

p < 0.05.
⁎⁎
p < 0.01.
⁎⁎⁎
p < 0.001.

likelihood of behavioral changes consistent with peer norms but not strongly associated with perceptions of leadership (Stulp et al., 2013).
necessarily compatible with norms of the adult society. The body height bonus was noteworthy, with a 0.3 increase on the
Aspirations for leadership occupations in childhood showed a re- assessment scale for every 10-cm increase in height. The impact of
verse pattern. Wanting to become a director or a manager in the 6th muscular power was also significant. Although these results confirm
grade did not lead to more nominations as class party organizer – previous research on the importance of body height. We should not
wanting to become both even had a negative impact – but these as- draw too hasty conclusions about the magnitude of this association. The
pirations made a greater impression on military officer evaluators. measures of body height and muscular power were a manifest part of
Physical characteristics are as foundational traits obviously closely this assessment process and almost certainly emphasized more in this
linked to genetic predisposition (see Fig. 1), but also influenced by early particular context compared to other situations involving formal or
childhood experiences (e.g. family economic status) and also by the informal assessment of leadership qualifications.
historical period and individual life-events (Elder, 1974; Stanner & Apart from the foundational traits and early childhood experiences,
Yudkin, 2001). We were only able to include measures of physical the importance of developmental experiences was evident in both in-
characteristics in the analysis of leadership assessment in connection stances in the cohort's lives. Scouting was not related to peer nomina-
with the draft. As has been noted in other studies body height is tions in childhood, but was definitely a long-term investment in clearly

313
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

Table 6 socialization, shared experiences of certain cohorts, and specific life-


Incidence rate ratios (IRR) of nominations for class party organizer. events with significant impact on individual lives (Baltes, Reese, &
Negative binominal regression (N = 4654)a. Lipsitt, 1980) has not been the aim of this study. However, these issues
IRR 95% CI are relevant when discussing generalizability and the specific empirical
context.
Independent variables We have argued that time (and place) must be taken into account in
a life-cycle approach to leadership development. In practice, historical
Early life experiences
Family socioeconomic status 1963 period cannot be a variable in the traditional sense. Many aspects of
Unskilled workersb human life can be manipulated, e.g. for scientific purposes, but human
Working class, skilled workers 1.00 0.87 1.15 behavior always takes place in a certain historical and geographic
Lower middle class, entrepreneurs 1.23 1.03 1.47
context which is usually fixed. It does matter whether potential leaders
Lower middle class 1.22 1.07 1.38
Upper and upper-middle class 1.46 1.25 1.70
are born in a period of high levels of living and low mortality rates or in
Birth order a society characterized by deprivation and social unrest – perhaps even
Youngest childb with a high risk of not surviving childhood and adolescence at all.
Only child 0.87 0.78 0.98 Leadership is developed and practiced in settings that are not
First born 0.83 0.74 0.92
wholly controllable and it is therefore problematic to apply the golden
Middle child 0.83 0.75 0.94
Parental support 1.07 1.05 1.09 standard in intervention research, randomized controlled trials (RCT),
Leadership learning capacities and skills in leadership research. Moreover, the obvious advantages of long-
Academic intrinsic motivation 1.01 0.99 1.03 itudinal study designs in comparison with cross-sectional approaches
Foundational traits
are also “polluted” by environmental factors and constant change. The
IQ in 6th grade (standardized) 1.37 1.30 1.44
IQ squared 1.02 0.98 1.06
longer the longitudinal approach, the greater the loss of control over
Delay of gratification historical and social change. Generalizability may always be questioned
Certainly SEK 100 nowb in longitudinal studies such as this one.
Probably SEK 100 now 0.80 0.62 1.03 A longitudinal approach is not only a question of repeated mea-
Cannot choose 0.79 0.62 1.00
surements over time, but also about conducting prospective research –
Probably SEK 1000 in five years 0.91 0.75 1.06
Certainly SEK 1000 in five years 0.93 0.80 1.07 studying groups not chosen based on the outcome variable (i.e. those
Expulsion from classroom who are unemployed, have cancer, have become grandparents, − or are
Never expelledb leaders). It is very easy to forget about all the promising computer
Once 1.13 1.03 1.24
programmers who did not become Bill Gates, overlooking one of the
Twice or more 1.25 1.09 1.43
Leader occupation aspiration
most important determinants, namely their date – and we would also
Noneb add place – of birth (Dixon, Horton, & Weir, 2011). This approach
One 1.00 0.92 1.09 obviously entails “an early start”, and schools are extremely suitable
Both 0.90 0.80 1.01 contexts in that respect. They are relatively standardized institutions,
Developmental Experiences
universal in their aim to target everyone within a certain age group.
Membership in scouts and/or sport club
Neitherb This was also the case for the children born in 1953 and residing in the
Scouts 0.99 0.80 1.22 Stockholm metropolitan area in 1963. Moreover, universalism was also
Sport 1.16 1.05 1.29 a key factor at the other end of our study – the draft. At this time, in
Both 1.37 0.95 1.97 principle all males were subject to a drafting procedure after their 18th
Frequency extracurricular activity
Neverb
birthday. Our study would, then, have been less valuable in today's
Sometimes 1.20 1.02 1.41 situation, where obligatory military service was abolished in 2010 and
Weekly 1.39 1.20 1.62 reintroduced on a small scale for both men and women in 2017 (J.
Not member in a club or association 1.10 0.96 1.27 Östberg, 2018; Strand & Berndtsson, 2015). Sweden was also at the
a time a very homogeneous society socially, economically, and ethni-
Fixed effects of school classes included.
b cally. Most mothers were homemakers or working part-time. Income
Reference category.
differences were generally low, and it was an optimistic era in terms of
low unemployment and substantial economic growth. The welfare state
was in full development with large advances in education, social wel-
influencing leadership assessment in early adulthood. Being active in a fare and health care. These children could generally dream about – and
sports club, or generally engaged in extracurricular activities, was de- expect – a better future compared to their parents.
finitely positive in relation to leadership emergence in both childhood The geographical context is easier to reproduce in the sense that the
and early adulthood. If being active in a sports club can be translated Stockholm metropolitan area is still an area with a different social
into “athleticism”, these findings clearly correspond to those presented composition compared to other parts of the country. For example, re-
by Vannatta, Gartstein, Zeller, and Noll (2009). In their study of peer latively fewer fathers were farmers or industrial workers then and this
perceptions of attributes and various behavioral qualities, athleticism is bound to have influenced the children's occupational choices. This
was found to be associated with leadership, particularly for boys. would probably also be the case today. Another common factor is the
age-normative issue related to biological maturation: The cohort con-
Generalizability and relevance for leadership research sists of boys during a crucial formative period in their lives. In grade 6
they are on the verge of puberty and adolescence – an age when all
Although time is an obvious aspect of all human development, it is youngsters are seeking their identity, male norms, and working on peer
often overlooked and poorly studied – in leadership studies and other relations. The next point of measurement is at the other end of the
disciplines alike (Bartolini, 1993; Castillo & Trinh, 2018). Foundational adolescence tunnel when the now young men are confronted with ex-
traits are characteristics that are more stable and with relatively higher tensive demands on them as adult citizens in a setting reeking with
heritability (or intergenerational transmission as may be a more male norms.
common term in some social sciences). Such a proposition cannot be It is possible, though, that the importance of peer relations was not
made without an underlying temporal assumption. Unveiling the re- as crucial for this sample as it might have been in a group of boys
lative importance of biological maturation and age-graded without adult male role models. As children most of the cohort

314
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

members lived in two-parent families where fathers – and many mo- events which may have influenced an individual to a great degree (e.g.
thers – were in employment. Birth order, family socioeconomic status serious illness, loss of a family member, or so). There are abundant
and cognitive ability are likely to be relevant in most historical and examples of people's lives changing overnight following events that
cultural contexts – but probably to varying degrees and in varying could not easily be foreseen (Hogan & Kaiser, 2005) and exposure to the
ways. same influences may have very different impact depending on personal
Besides the unsolvable issue of how to generalize results from and individual resilience or whether this experience is shared by most
(historical) longitudinal research, our study also begs for some com- of one's “contemporaries”.
ments on the choice of setting vis-à-vis leadership studies. In their de- Closely linked to this issue is the fact that not all boys from the
fense and encouragement of unconventional contexts, Bamberger and cohort presented at the draft. Around 10% were not present in the
Pratt (2010) particularly mention primary and secondary schools as an Conscription Board register. It is conceivable that some of the cohort
ideal, multilevel setting for exploring boundaries of extant theories and members were even “non-selected” at an earlier stage, before being
for generating new theory (p. 667). Another advantage of this study is called to the draft. The youths with the least academic motivation,
that data collection took place in controlled environments; in the cognitive ability, developmental experiences, parental support and so
classroom under the supervision of teachers, through use of register forth might already have gone off to sea, emigrated, been incarcerated,
data which could not be manipulated, and through a highly controlled become seriously ill, or even passed away. Including these individuals
and standardized draft procedure. might have produced even more distinct results. It should also be kept
in mind that the cohort was selected in 1963, which means that some of
Limitations the potential cohort members (born in 1953) had already passed away
or moved from the area during their first childhood years. This cir-
Despite the uniqueness of data and many advantages of the pro- cumstance is of course important to recognize in studies focusing on
spective design, there are several limitations to this study which need to premature mortality or early childhood outcomes.
be taken into consideration. We have already mentioned that the Another limitation is the lack of data on physical characteristics in
Project Metropolitan was not specifically designed for the purpose of the 6th grade and the limited ditto at the draft. For example, we did not
studying leadership. The questions and variables in the dataset are have access to any data on physical or facial appearance, which could
therefore not directly applicable for this purpose, which means that a) have been relevant in several different ways in this study. Physical
the variables are not very valid measures of the theoretical categories appearance has for example, been shown to effect academic outcomes
we are trying to explore, and b) we lack data on other closely related in the sense that students with attractive peers received higher grades
concepts and important confounders. compared to those in a course with average peers. This may simply
Neither of the dependent variables were specifically designed to reflect a form of discrimination, but may also be explained by increased
measure leadership qualities in a broad sense. The draft board's primary motivation and effort when classmates are more attractive (Hernández-
goal is of course to find suitable candidates to serve as military officers. Julián & Peters, 2018). Appearance and leadership ascription is also
That is, to practice leadership in a very specific context under very rigid relevant in childhood (Vannatta et al., 2009). Facial appearance has
organizational circumstances. Having said that, the universal con- also been linked to inferences about competence, for example among
scription and training of officers has in practice also supplied the labor political leaders (Todorov, Mandisodza, Goren, & Hall, 2005), and fa-
market and society at large with carefully selected and well-trained cial appearance was found to influence promotions to top positions in
leaders (Sundevall, 2017). As such, the assessment of military officer the army (Mueller & Mazur, 1996).
suitability is a reasonable measure of leadership qualities. Access to
He was old enough, twelve years and a few months, to have lost the
socio-metric data from when the cohort was aged 12–13 is of course
prominent tummy of childhood; and not yet old enough for ado-
unique and invaluable, but nominations for class party organizer is
lescence to have made him awkward. You could see now that he
probably more remotely related to leadership per se and may simply
might make a boxer, as far as width and heaviness of shoulders
reflect (perceived) popularity. Popularity can, of course, also be ex-
went, but there was a mildness about his mouth and eyes that
tremely volatile and be associated with factors that are impossible to
proclaimed no devil.
encompass in a survey such as this (e.g. a big brother on the local
(Description of Ralph in chapter 1 of Lord of the Flies)
football team, newborn kittens at home, owning the latest Beatles re-
cord, or having a father who is an airline pilot). Moreover, physical Obviously, a whole range of verbal and non-verbal traits and ges-
appearance, athletic ability and academic competence were found to be tures are possible predictors of leadership ascription in a group
moderately to strongly related to peer ratings of social acceptance (Ridgeway, 2002; Sanchez-Cortes, Aran, Jayagopi, Schmid Mast, &
among pupils in elementary school (Vannatta et al., 2009). The relative Gatica-Perez, 2013).
importance varied, however, been grades and gender, which reminds us Another important limitation relates to the use of IQ in the Cragg-
that the peer nominations in this study came from both boys and girls in Hurdle analysis. The draft officer had knowledge of the draftees' IQ
the classroom and it is possible that there were different factors ef- scores when deciding on a) who to refer for assessment, and b) the
fecting the likelihood of a boy being nominated compared to a girl actual assessment. Including the IQ score from the draft would there-
(Chiao, Bowman, & Gill, 2008). Almost half of the boys had been ex- fore have been problematic, particularly for the first “hurdle”, given
pelled from the classroom on at least one occasion. This may have been that the data set provides evidence of an informal cut-off score of 5 or
both expected and accepted for a boy, but a sign of severe deviance and more for being referred for assessment in the first place. Therefore, we
a source of mistrust for a girl. One mitigating factor in this respect is included the IQ score from the 6th grade instead. Nevertheless, there is
that the nominations were made anonymously, in writing, and the a strong correlation between these two measures of IQ and it is likely
pupils did not know how many nominations they received or from that the “cursory” sorting mechanism continued on to the second part
whom. of the process, i.e. the actual assessment of military officer suitability.
There are of course a number of potential weaknesses as far as lack Lastly, it should be pointed out that we were not able to correct for
of information is concerned. Although the dataset is very comprehen- the effects of measurement error, for example regarding cognitive
sive and consists of a variety of information from different sources, we ability. This was due to software limitations in combining errors-in-
were not able to include subjective data from the parents, compre- variables in non-linear or complex models such as negative binominal
hensive data on physical appearance, and knowledge about specific regressions or Cragg-Hurdle models.

315
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

Conclusions Choosing a leader on a desert island is a question of life and death.


The exit option is not available and the children must act upon limited
Individual and environmental conditions in childhood and adoles- knowledge. The choice is not free either (no anonymous votes) but part
cence are not absent in the discourse on leadership emergence, be it in of an intricate group formation. It is important to question and discuss
early life or later in adult life. For empirical studies in this area we claims to leadership in different situations. Is singing a high C a valid
mostly have to make do with retrospective accounts from those already competence on a desert island? Is physical strength more valuable than
in a leader position, cross-sectional data, small (and selected) samples, intellectual reasoning? For Piggy, cognitive ability was not a sufficient
secondary data, or data collected at a later stage and from other sources asset for leadership – he had too many deficits in other areas – but he
than our primary study objects. was an important source of knowledge and advanced solutions to dif-
With access from the Project Metropolitan we were able to remedy ferent problems. He kept count of the children and tried to uphold order
some of these shortcomings in studies of leadership emergence. The of speech and social rules. Until he dies in an accident. When the res-
main assets of the data set from the project was its comprehensiveness cuing officer asks how many they are on the island, Ralph is not able to
in terms of incorporating a whole cohort in a large urban area and in answer.
terms of the breadth of data sources and topics covered. Of particular
‘I should have thought,’ said the officer as he visualized the search
importance is the school survey performed when the respondents were
before him. ‘I should have though that a pack of British boys – you're
in the 6th grade. The cohort was followed through early adulthood into
all British aren't you? – would have been able to put up a better
their thirties. After the dataset was anonymized in 1986, no more
show than that – I mean – ’
surveys could be performed but it has been possible to follow the cohort
‘It was like that at first,’ said Ralph, ‘before things – ’
through registers as they now move into the age of retirement
He stopped.
(Stenberg, 2018).
(Lord of the Flies, chapter 12)
Our study only includes the male members of the cohort and only
those who were drafted (N = 5928). With data on birth order and fa- A true life cycle perspective should also take on the practice of
mily socioeconomic status when they were 10, through an extensive leadership outside work hours and even after working age. We do not
survey when they were in the 6th grade, and on to circumstances re- know how many of the Project Metropolitan boys actually ended up as
lated to the draft, we cover the cohort members' first 20 or so years. military officers or in other leadership roles. A true life-cycle perspec-
This is a fairly long period as such but is also a particularly formative tive on leadership emergence should of course include the entirety of
period in life. By following the cohort prospectively, we were also able adulthood. For Ralph and Jack the events on the desert island might
to avoid departing from study populations already selected for the have put them off any sort of leadership position in the future.
outcome we are actually interested in. Having said that, this was not an However, in our view, it is highly likely that they do emerge as leaders
investigation of formal leadership or the practice of leadership, but the in some form or another – possibly as naval officers. Apart from the
assessment or ascription of leadership at two points in the cohort's lives. assets that made them emerge as leaders in the first place, they can now
Overall, we found that early life experiences in terms of family so- also point to previous leadership experience.
cioeconomic status, parental support, and birth order were all im-
portant for leadership emergence at both points in life, although References
somewhat differently. Interestingly family socioeconomic status did not
affect leadership ascription in childhood, but was all the more sig- Allen, J. P., Porter, M. R., McFarland, F. C., Marsh, P., & McElhaney, K. B. (2005). The two
nificant in early adulthood – even when controlling for a number of faces of adolescents' success with peers: Adolescent popularity, social adaptation, and
deviant behavior. Child Development, 76(3), 747–760. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
other factors, including IQ. 1467-8624.2005.00875.x.
Cognitive ability has been identified as a crucial factor in leadership Almquist, Y. (2009). Peer status in school and adult disease risk: A 30-year follow-up
emergence, and this study is no exception. Still, cognitive ability is but study of disease-specific morbidity in a Stockholm cohort. Journal of Epidemiology and
Community Health, 63(12), 1028–1034. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech.2009.088377.
one important factor related to leadership emergence and it might even Almquist, Y. (2011). Social isolation in the classroom and adult health: A longitudinal
have its limits. The positive effect of IQ on leadership assessment at the study of a 1953 cohort. Advances in Life Course Research, 16(1), 1–12. https://doi.org/
draft did show a curvilinear pattern. Leadership ascription is contextual 10.1016/j.alcr.2010.11.001.
Almquist, Y., Modin, B., & Östberg, V. (2010). Childhood social status in society and
and the children seem to appreciate the younger sibling for organizing school: Implications for the transition to higher levels of education. British Journal of
class parties while the draft board is looking for the responsible elder Sociology of Education, 31(1), 31–45. https://doi.org/10.1080/01425690903385352.
brother. Almquist, Y. B., & Östberg, V. (2013). Social relationships and subsequent health-related
behaviours: Linkages between adolescent peer status and levels of adult smoking in a
As so much social and behavioral research has reported, cognitive
Stockholm cohort. Addiction, 108(3), 629–637. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-
ability and social class showed very strong influences on outcomes both 0443.2012.04097.x.
in childhood and later in early adulthood. And this was also the case in Andeweg, R. B., & Van den Berg, S. B. (2003). Linking birth order to political leadership:
a society with relatively small class differences. The lesson for leader- The impact of parents or sibling interaction? Political Psychology, 24(3), 605–623.
https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00343.
ship studies may be that including childhood factors is relevant not only Antonakis, J., Day, D. V., & Schyns, B. (2012). Leadership and individual differences: At
for fostering (good) leaders, but also for understanding followers (Elgar, the cusp of a renaissance. The Leadership Quarterly, 23(4), 643–650. https://doi.org/
2016). That is, to observe and highlight not only how some children are 10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.05.002.
Antonakis, J., House, R. J., & Simonton, D. K. (2017). Can super smart leaders suffer from
chosen, or choose themselves, as leaders – but also to encourage re- too much of a good thing? The curvilinear effect of intelligence on perceived lea-
flection about what qualifications are made relevant and which in fact dership behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 102(7), 1003. https://doi.org/10.
are relevant. 1037/apl0000221.
Arvey, R. D., Rotundo, M., Johnson, W., Zhang, Z., & McGue, M. (2006). The determi-
For those interested in leadership emergence, the results offer sup- nants of leadership role occupancy: Genetic and personality factors. The Leadership
port for including childhood factors in models attempting to understand Quarterly, 17(1), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2005.10.009.
how leaders are formed. For those engaged in fostering leaders they will Baltes, P. B., Reese, H. W., & Lipsitt, L. P. (1980). Life-span developmental psychology.
Annual Review of Psychology, 31(1), 65–110. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.31.
find support for encouraging youngsters to engage in sports, scouts, and 020180.000433.
other extracurricular activities. Based on the Project Metropolitan data Bamberger, P. A., & Pratt, M. G. (2010). Moving forward by looking back: Reclaiming
Kassman (2017) found that taking part in youth organizations and unconventional research contexts and samples in organizational scholarship.
Academy of Management Journal, 53(4), 665–671. https://doi.org/10.5465/AMJ.
specifically the scouts did had a positive impact on school marks and
2010.52814357.
future income. Scouts showed signs of more positive trajectories even Banaji, M. R., & Prentice, D. A. (1994). The self in social contexts. Annual Review of
when controlling for differences in their social background and per- Psychology, 45(1), 297–332. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.45.020194.
sonal cognitive abilities. 001501.

316
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

Bandura, A. (1986). The explanatory and predictive scope of self-efficacy theory. Journal Elsner, B., & Isphording, I. E. (2018). Rank, sex, drugs, and crime. Journal of Human
of Social and Clinical Psychology, 4(3), 359–373. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.1986. Resources, 53(2), 356–381.
4.3.359. Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., & Ridder, E. M. (2005). Show me the child at seven II:
Barling, J., & Weatherhead, J. G. (2016). Persistent exposure to poverty during childhood Childhood intelligence and later outcomes in adolescence and young adulthood.
limits later leader emergence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101(9), 1305–1318. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(8), 850–858. https://doi.org/10.1111/
https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0000129. j.1469-7610.2005.01472.x.
Bartolini, S. (1993). On time and comparative research. Journal of Theoretical Politics, Fiedler, F. E. (2002). The curious role of cognitive resources in leadership. In R. E. Riggio,
5(2), 131–167. https://doi.org/10.1177/0951692893005002001. S. E. Muprhy, & F. J. Pirozzolo (Eds.). Multiple intelligences and leadership (pp. 91–
Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). Self-regulation and the executive function of the 104). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
self. Handbook of Self and Identity, 1, 197–217. Fischbein, S. (1980). IQ and social class. Intelligence, 4(1), 51–63.
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal de- French, D. C., & Stright, A. L. (1991). Emergent leadership in Children's small groups.
velopment, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Small Group Research, 22(2), 187–199. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Development, 62(4), 647–670. 1046496491222003.
Blaker, N. M., Rompa, I., Dessing, I. H., Vriend, A. F., Herschberg, C., & van Vugt, M. Fukada, S., Fukada, H., & Hicks, J. (1994). Structure of leadership among preschool
(2013). The height leadership advantage in men and women: Testing evolutionary children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 155(4), 389–395. https://doi.org/10.
psychology predictions about the perceptions of tall leaders. Group Processes & 1080/00221325.1994.9914789.
Intergroup Relations, 16(1), 17–27. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430212437211. Gayles, J. G., & Baker, A. R. (2015). Opportunities and challenges for first-year student-
Brungardt, C. (1997). The making of leaders: A review of the research in leadership de- athletes transitioning from high school to college. New Directions for Student
velopment and education. Journal of Leadership Studies, 3(3), 81–95. https://doi.org/ Leadership, 2015(147), 43–51. https://doi.org/10.1002/yd.20142.
10.1177/107179199700300309. Gest, S. D., Graham-Bermann, S. A., & Hartup, W. W. (2002). Peer experience: Common
Burks, S. V., Carpenter, J. P., Goette, L., & Rustichini, A. (2009). Cognitive skills affect and unique features of number of friendships, social network centrality, and socio-
economic preferences, strategic behavior, and job attachment. Proceedings of the metric status. Social Development, 10(1), 23–40. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.
National Academy of Sciences, 106(19), 7745. 00146.
Castillo, E. A., & Trinh, M. P. (2018). In search of missing time: A review of the study of Glenn, N. D. (1976). Cohort analysts' futile quest: Statistical attempts to separate age,
time in leadership research. The Leadership Quarterly, 29, 165–178. https://doi.org/ period and cohort effects. American Sociological Review, 41(5), 900–904. https://doi.
10.1016/j.leaqua.2017.12.001. org/10.2307/2094738.
Central Conscription Authority (1952). Instruktion för exploratörer vid Gollner, L. M., Ballhausen, N., Kliegel, M., & Forstmeier, S. (2017). Delay of gratification,
inskrivningsförrätningar [instructions for evaluators in the assessment of military con- delay discounting and their associations with age, episodic future thinking, and fu-
scripts]. Sweden: Lund. ture time perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2304. https://doi.org/10.3389/
Chaturvedi, S., Zyphur, M. J., Arvey, R. D., Avolio, B. J., & Larsson, G. (2012). The fpsyg.2017.02304.
heritability of emergent leadership: Age and gender as moderating factors. The Gottfried, A. E., Gottfried, A. W., Reichard, R. J., Guerin, D. W., Oliver, P. H., & Riggio, R.
Leadership Quarterly, 23(2), 219–232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.08.004. E. (2011). Motivational roots of leadership: A longitudinal study from childhood
Chiao, J. Y., Bowman, N. E., & Gill, H. (2008). The political gender gap: Gender bias in through adulthood. The Leadership Quarterly, 22(3), 510–519. https://doi.org/10.
facial inferences that predict voting behavior. PLoS One, 3(10), e3666. https://doi. 1016/j.leaqua.2011.04.008.
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0003666. Gottfried, A. W., Gottfried, A. E., & Guerin, D. W. (2006). The Fullerton longitudinal
Cragg, J. D. (1971). Some statistical models for limited dependent variables with appli- study: A long-term investigation of intellectual and motivational giftedness. Journal
cation to the demand for durable goods. Econometrica, 39(5), 829–844. for the Education of the Gifted, 29(4), 430–450.
Cunha, M. P.e., Lewis, M., Rego, A., & Smith, W. K. (2017). Biographical methods in Guerin, D. W., Oliver, P. H., Gottfried, A. W., Gottfried, A. E., Reichard, R. J., & Riggio, R.
leadership research. In B. Schyns, R. J. Hall, & P. Neves (Eds.). Handbook of methods in E. (2011). Childhood and adolescent antecedents of social skills and leadership po-
leadership research (pp. 372–402). Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar Publishing. tential in adulthood: Temperamental approach/withdrawal and extraversion. The
Daly, M., Egan, M., & O'Reilly, F. (2015). Childhood general cognitive ability predicts Leadership Quarterly, 22(3), 482–494. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.04.006.
leadership role occupancy across life: Evidence from 17,000 cohort study partici- Halaby, C. N. (2004). Panel models in sociological research: Theory into practice. Annual
pants. The Leadership Quarterly, 26(3), 323–341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua. Review of Sociology, 30, 507–544.
2015.03.006. Hannah, S. T., Avolio, B. J., Luthans, F., & Harms, P. D. (2008). Leadership efficacy:
Day, D. V. (2011). Integrative perspectives on longitudinal investigations of leader de- Review and future directions. The Leadership Quarterly, 19(6), 669–692. https://doi.
velopment: From childhood through adulthood. The Leadership Quarterly, 22(3), org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2008.09.007.
561–571. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.04.012. Hernández-Julián, R., & Peters, C. (2018). Physical appearance and peer effects in aca-
Day, D. V., & Sin, H.-P. (2011). Longitudinal tests of an integrative model of leader de- demic performance. Applied Economics Letters, 25(13), 887–890. https://doi.org/10.
velopment: Charting and understanding developmental trajectories. The Leadership 1080/13504851.2017.1380282.
Quarterly, 22(3), 545–560. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.04.011. Hogan, R., & Kaiser, R. B. (2005). What we know about leadership. Review of General
Day, D. V., Fleenor, J. W., Atwater, L. E., Sturm, R. E., & McKee, R. A. (2014). Advances in Psychology, 9(2), 169–180. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.9.2.169.
leader and leadership development: A review of 25 years of research and theory. The Horrocks, J. E., & Thompson, G. G. (1946). A study of the friendship fluctuations of rural
Leadership Quarterly, 25(1), 63–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2013.11.004. boys and girls. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 69(2), 189–198. https://doi.org/10.
Day, D. V., & Harrison, M. M. (2007). A multilevel, identity-based approach to leadership 1080/08856559.1946.10533388.
development. Human Resource Management Review, 17(4), 360–373. Hoyt, M. A., & Kennedy, C. L. (2008). Leadership and adolescent girls: A qualitative study
Day, D. V., Harrison, M. M., & Halpin, S. M. (2008). An integrative approach to leader of leadership development. American Journal of Community Psychology, 42(3),
development: Connecting adult development, identity, and expertise. New York: 203–219. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10464-008-9206-8.
Psychology Press. Ilies, R., Arvey, R. D., & Bouchard, T. J. (2006). Darwinism, behavioral genetics, and
Deary, I. J., Weiss, A., & Batty, G. D. (2010). Intelligence and personality as predictors of organizational behavior: A review and agenda for future research. Journal of
illness and death: How researchers in differential psychology and chronic disease Organizational Behavior, 27(2), 121–141. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.351.
epidemiology are collaborating to understand and address health inequalities. Janson, C.-G. (1966). Project Metropolitan. Acta Sociologica, 9(1–2), 110–115.
Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 11(2), 53–79. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Jencks, C. (1980). Heredity, environment, and public policy reconsidered. American
1529100610387081. Sociological Review, 45(5), 723–736. https://doi.org/10.2307/2094892.
Dinh, J. E., & Lord, R. G. (2012). Implications of dispositional and process views of traits Judge, T. A., & Cable, D. M. (2004). The effect of physical height on workplace success
for individual difference research in leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 23(4), and income: Preliminary test of a theoretical model. Journal of Applied Psychology,
651–669. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.03.003. 89(3), 428.
Dixon, J., Horton, S., & Weir, P. (2011). Relative age effects: Implications for leadership Judge, T. A., Ilies, R., & Colbert, A. E. (2004). Intelligence and leadership: A quantitative
development. The International Journal of Sport and Society, 2(2)https://scholar. review and test of theoretical propositions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(3),
uwindsor.ca/humankineticspub/6. 542–552.
Dobosz, R. P., & Beaty, L. A. (1999). The relationship between athletic participation and Karagianni, D., & Jude Montgomery, A. (2018). Developing leadership skills among
high school student's leadership ability. Adolescence, 34(133), 215–220. adolescents and young adults: A review of leadership programmes. International
Dodge, K. A., Dishion, T. J., & Lansford, J. E. (2006). Deviant peer influences in inter- Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 23(1), 86–98. https://doi.org/10.1080/02673843.
vention and public policy for youth. Social Policy Report, 20(1). 2017.1292928.
Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., & Sunde, U. (2010). Are risk aversion and impatience Karnehed, N., Rasmussen, F., Hemmingsson, T., & Tynelius, P. (2012). Obesity and at-
related to cognitive ability? American Economic Review, 100(3), 1238–1260. https:// tained education: Cohort study of more than 700,000 Swedish men. Obesity, 14(8),
doi.org/10.1257/aer.100.3.1238. 1421–1428. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2006.161.
Duster, T. (2006). Comparative perspectives and competing explanations: Taking on the Kassman, A. (2017). One of all the others – A life course study of scouts, social capital,
newly configured reductionist challenge to sociology. American Sociological Review, and stratification in the Swedish ‘Folkhem’. Journal of Civil Society, 13(1), 71–89.
71(1), 1–15 (10.1177%2F000312240607100101). https://doi.org/10.1080/17448689.2017.1287843.
Eckstein, D., Aycock, K. J., Sperber, M. A., McDonald, J., Van Wiesner, V., III, Watts, R. E., Klepp, K., Halper, A., & Perry, C. L. (2018). The efficacy of peer leaders in drug abuse
& Ginsburg, P. (2010). A review of 200 birth-order studies: Lifestyle characteristics. prevention. Journal of School Health, 56(9), 407–411. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
Journal of Individual Psychology, 66(4), 408–434. 1746-1561.1986.tb05783.x.
Elder, G. H. (1974). Children of the great depression. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Kuhn, P., & Weinberger, C. (2005). Leadership skills and wages. Journal of Labor
Elgar, M. A. (2016). Leader selection and leadership outcomes: Height and age in a Economics, 23(3), 395–436. https://doi.org/10.1086/430282.
sporting model. The Leadership Quarterly, 27(4), 588–601. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Lease, A. M., Musgrove, K. T., & Axelrod, J. L. (2002). Dimensions of social status in
leaqua.2015.12.005. preadolescent peer groups: Likability, perceived popularity, and social dominance.

317
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

Social Development, 11(4), 508–533. 282–295. https://doi.org/10.1093/sf/57.1.282.


Legree, P. J., Gade, P. A., Martin, D. E., Fischl, M. A., Wilson, M. J., Nieva, V. F., ... Paunova, M. (2015). The emergence of individual and collective leadership in task
Laurence, J. (2000). Military enlistment and family dynamics: Youth and parental groups: A matter of achievement and ascription. The Leadership Quarterly, 26(6),
perspectives. Military Psychology, 12(1), 31–49. https://doi.org/10.1207/ 935–957. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2015.10.002.
S15327876MP1201_2. Public Committee for the Education of Conscripts (1986). SOU 1986:43 Befrielse från
Lerner, R. M., & Steinberg, L. (2009). Handbook of adolescent psychology, volume 1: värnpliktstjänstgöring [Exemption from military service, Swedish Public Investigation].
Individual bases of adolescent development. vol. 1. Hoboken, N.J.: John Wiley & Sons. Stockholm: Ministry of Defence.
Li, W.-D., Arvey, R. D., & Song, Z. (2011). The influence of general mental ability, self- Rauthmann, J. F., Sherman, R. A., & Funder, D. C. (2015). Principles of situation research:
esteem and family socioeconomic status on leadership role occupancy and leader Towards a better understanding of psychological situations. European Journal of
advancement: The moderating role of gender. The Leadership Quarterly, 22(3), Personality, 29(3), 363–381. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1994.
520–534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.04.009. Reichard, R. J., Riggio, R. E., Guerin, D. W., Oliver, P. H., Gottfried, A. W., & Gottfried, A.
Lindqvist, E. (2011). Height and leadership. The Review of Economics and Statistics, 94(4), E. (2011). A longitudinal analysis of relationships between adolescent personality
1191–1196. https://doi.org/10.1162/REST_a_00239. and intelligence with adult leader emergence and transformational leadership. The
Lindqvist, E., & Vestman, R. (2011). The labor market returns to cognitive and non- Leadership Quarterly, 22(3), 471–481. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.04.005.
cognitive ability: Evidence from the Swedish enlistment. American Economic Journal: Reid, S. W., Anglin, A. H., Baur, J. E., Short, J. C., & Buckley, M. R. (2018). Blazing new
Applied Economics, 3(1), 101–128. trails or opportunity lost? Evaluating research at the intersection of leadership and
Little, A. C. (2014). Facial appearance and leader choice in different contexts: Evidence entrepreneurship. The Leadership Quarterly, 29(1), 150–164. https://doi.org/10.
for task contingent selection based on implicit and learned face-behaviour/face- 1016/j.leaqua.2017.11.005.
ability associations. The Leadership Quarterly, 25(5), 865–874. https://doi.org/10. Ridgeway, C. L. (2002). Gender, status, and leadership. Journal of Social Issues, 57(4),
1016/j.leaqua.2014.04.002. 637–655. https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00233.
Lord, R. G., De Vader, C. L., & Alliger, G. M. (1986). A meta-analysis of the relation Rosenberg, E. B., & Hajal, F. (1985). Stepsibling relationships in remarried families. Social
between personality traits and leadership perceptions: An application of validity Casework, 66(5), 287–292. https://doi.org/10.1177/104438948506600504.
generalization procedures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71(3), 402–410. Rosette, A. S., Leonardelli, G. J., & Phillips, K. W. (2008). The white standard: Racial bias
Lord, R. G., & Hall, R. J. (2005). Identity, deep structure and the development of lea- in leader categorization. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(4), 758.
dership skill. The Leadership Quarterly, 16(4), 591–615. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Ruhm, C. (1992). Youth leadership training in curricular, cocurricular, and community pro-
leaqua.2005.06.003. grams. Doctoral dissertationThe University of Wisconsin-Madison.
Lundborg, P., Nystedt, P., & Rooth, D.-O. (2009). The height premium in earnings: The role of Sanchez-Cortes, D., Aran, O., Jayagopi, D. B., Schmid Mast, M., & Gatica-Perez, D. (2013).
physical capacity and cognitive and non-cognitive skills. IZA discussion papers 4266. Emergent leaders through looking and speaking: From audio-visual data to multi-
Berlin: Institute for the Study of Labor. modal recognition. Journal on Multimodal User Interfaces, 7(1), 39–53. https://doi.
Lundin, A., Lundberg, I., Hallsten, L., Ottosson, J., & Hemmingsson, T. (2010). org/10.1007/s12193-012-0101-0.
Unemployment and mortality. A longitudinal prospective study on selection and Schoon, I., & Parsons, S. (2002). Teenage aspirations for future careers and occupational
causation in 49321 Swedish middle-aged men. Journal of Epidemiology & Community outcomes. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 60(2), 262–288. https://doi.org/10.1006/
Health, 64(01), 22–28. jvbe.2001.1867.
Martin, S. R., Innis, B. D., & Ward, R. G. (2017). Social class, leaders and leadership: A Sczesny, S., & Kühnen, U. (2004). Meta-cognition about biological sex and gender-ste-
critical review and suggestions for development. Current Opinion in Psychology, 18, reotypic physical appearance: Consequences for the assessment of leadership com-
49–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.001. petence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(1), 13–21. https://doi.org/10.
Mawson, B. (2011). Children's leadership strategies in early childhood. Journal of 1177/0146167203258831.
Research in Childhood Education, 25(4), 327–338. https://doi.org/10.1080/ Shoup, J. R. (2005). A collective biography of twelve world-class leaders: A study on devel-
02568543.2011.605207. oping exemplary leaders. Lanham, Maryland: University Press of America.
McCullough, P. M., Ashbridge, D., & Pegg, R. (1994). The effect of self-esteem, family Sidorchuk, A., Hemmingsson, T., Romelsjö, A., & Allebeck, P. (2012). Alcohol use in
structure, locus of control, and career goals on adolescent leadership behavior. adolescence and risk of disability pension: A 39 year follow-up of a population-based
Adolescence, 29(115), 605–611. conscription survey. PLoS One, 7(8), e42083. https://doi.org/10.16993/bav.
McFarland, D. A., & Thomas, R. J. (2006). Bowling young: How youth voluntary asso- Simonton, D. K. (1985). Intelligence and personal influence in groups: Four nonlinear
ciations influence adult political participation. American Sociological Review, 71(3), models. Psychological Review, 92(4), 532–547.
401–425. https://doi.org/10.1177/000312240607100303. Solansky, S. T. (2010). The evaluation of two key leadership development program
McNeal, R. B. (1998). High school extracurricular activities: Closed structures and stra- components: Leadership skills assessment and leadership mentoring. The Leadership
tifying patterns of participation. The Journal of Educational Research, 91(3), 183–191. Quarterly, 21(4), 675–681. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2010.06.009.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00220679809597539. Stanner, S. A., & Yudkin, J. S. (2001). Fetal programming and the Leningrad siege study.
Miller, N., & Maruyama, G. (1976). Ordinal position and peer popularity. Journal of Twin Research, 4(5), 287–292. https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.4.5.287.
Personality and Social Psychology, 33(2), 123–131. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022- Stenberg, S.-Å. (2018). Born in 1953. The story about a post-war Swedish cohort, and a
3514.33.2.123. longitudinal research project. Stockholm: Stockholm University Press.
Mischel, W., & Ebbesen, E. B. (1970). Attention in delay of gratification. Journal of Stenberg, S.-Å., & Vågerö, D. (2006). Cohort profile: The Stockholm birth cohort of 1953.
Personality and Social Psychology, 16(2), 329–337. https://doi.org/10.1037/ International Journal of Epidemiology, 35(3), 546–548. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/
h0029815. dyi310.
Moffitt, T. E. (2015). Life-course-persistent versus adolescence-limited antisocial beha- Stockard, J., & McGee, J. (1990). Children's occupational preferences: The influence of
vior. In D. Cicchetti, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.). Developmental psychopathology (pp. 570– sex and perceptions of occupational characteristics. Journal of Vocational Behavior,
598). Hoboken, N.J.: John Wiley & Sons. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470939406. 36(3), 287–303. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-8791(90)90033-X.
ch15. Stogdill, R. M. (1948). Personal factors associated with leadership: A survey of the lit-
Morgan, G. A., Harmon, R. J., & Maslin-Cole, C. A. (1990). Mastery motivation: Definition erature. The Journal of Psychology, 25(1), 35–71. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.
and measurement. Early Education and Development, 1(5), 318–339. 1948.9917362.
Mueller, U., & Mazur, A. (1996). Facial dominance of West Point cadets as a predictor of Strand, S., & Berndtsson, J. (2015). Recruiting the “enterprising soldier”: Military re-
later military rank. Social Forces, 74(3), 823–850. https://doi.org/10.2307/2580383. cruitment discourses in Sweden and the United Kingdom. Critical Military Studies,
Murphy, S. E. (2002). Leader self-regulation: The role of self-efficacy and multiple in- 1(3), 233–248. https://doi.org/10.1080/23337486.2015.1090676.
telligences. In R. E. Riggio, S. E. Murphy, & F. J. Pirozzolo (Eds.). LEA's organization Strang, S. E., & Kuhnert, K. W. (2009). Personality and leadership developmental levels as
and management series. Multiple intelligences and leadership (pp. 163–186). Mahwah, predictors of leader performance. The Leadership Quarterly, 20(3), 421–433. https://
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2009.03.009.
Murphy, S. E. (2011). Providing a foundation for leadership development. In S. E. Stulp, G., Buunk, A. P., Verhulst, S., & Pollet, T. V. (2013). Tall claims? Sense and non-
Murphy, & R. J. Reichard (Eds.). Early development and leadership: Building the next sense about the importance of height of US presidents. The Leadership Quarterly,
generation of leaders (pp. 3–37). New York: Psychology Press/Routledge. 24(1), 159–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.09.002.
Murphy, S. E., & Johnson, S. K. (2011). The benefits of a long-lens approach to leader Sundevall, F. (2017). Military education for non-military purposes. History of Education
development: Understanding the seeds of leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 22(3), Review, 46(1), 58–71. https://doi.org/10.1108/HER-05-2016-0024.
459–470. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.04.004. Todorov, A., Mandisodza, A. N., Goren, A., & Hall, C. C. (2005). Inferences of competence
National Board of Health and Welfare (2009). Folkhälsorapport 2009 [Public health report from faces predict election outcomes. Science, 308(5728), 1623–1626.
2009]. Stockholm: Socialstyrelsen. Tuncdogan, A., Acar, O. A., & Stam, D. (2017). Individual differences as antecedents of
Obschonka, M. (2016). Adolescent pathways to entrepreneurship. Child Development leader behavior: Towards an understanding of multi-level outcomes. The Leadership
Perspectives, 10(3), 196–201. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12185. Quarterly, 28(1), 40–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2016.10.011.
Östberg, J. (2018). We want you as our new recruit. Prerequisites for recruitment to and Vannatta, K., Gartstein, M. A., Zeller, M., & Noll, R. B. (2009). Peer acceptance and social
retention in the Swedish Armed Forces. PhD thesisSweden: University of Karlstad. behavior during childhood and adolescence: How important are appearance, athle-
Östberg, V. (2003). Children in classrooms: Peer status, status distribution and mental ticism, and academic competence? International Journal of Behavioral Development,
well-being. Social Science & Medicine, 56(1), 17–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277- 33(4), 303–311. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025408101275.
9536(02)00006-0. von Hippel, P. T. (2018). How many imputations do you need? A two-stage calculation
Östberg, V., & Modin, B. (2008). Status relations in school and their relevance for health using a quadratic rule. Sociological Methods & Research, 1–20. https://doi.org/10.
in a life course perspective: Findings from the Aberdeen children of the 1950's cohort 1177/0049124117747303.
study. Social Science & Medicine, 66(4), 835–848. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Wahl, K. H., & Blackhurst, A. (2000). Factors affecting the occupational and educational
socscimed.2007.10.018. aspirations of children and adolescents. Professional School Counseling, 3(5), 367–374.
Palmore, E. (1978). When can age, period, and cohort be separated? Social Forces, 57(1), Watts, T. W., Duncan, G. J., & Quan, H. (2018). Revisiting the marshmallow test: A

318
T. Reitan, S.-Å. Stenberg The Leadership Quarterly 30 (2019) 298–319

conceptual replication investigating links between early delay of gratification and integration. The Leadership Quarterly, 29(1), 2–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.
later outcomes. Psychological Science, 29(7), 1159–1177. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 2017.10.003.
0956797618761661. Zacharatos, A., Barling, J., & Kelloway, E. K. (2000). Development and effects of trans-
Whalley, L. J., & Deary, I. J. (2001). Longitudinal cohort study of childhood IQ and formational leadership in adolescents. The Leadership Quarterly, 11(2), 211–226.
survival up to age 76. BMJ, 322(7290), 819. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1048-9843(00)00041-2.
Whitehead, G. (2009). Adolescent leadership development: Building a case for an au- Zhang, L., Welte, J. W., & Wieczorek, W. F. (2002). Underlying common factors of ado-
thenticity framework. Educational Management Administration & Leadership, 37(6), lescent problem behaviors. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 29(2), 161–182. https://doi.
847–872. https://doi.org/10.1177/1741143209345441. org/10.1177/0093854802029002003.
Yang, Y., & Land, K. C. (2016). Age-period-cohort analysis: New models, methods, and em- Zhang, Z., Ilies, R., & Arvey, R. D. (2009). Beyond genetic explanations for leadership: The
pirical applications. New York: Chapman and Hall/CRC. moderating role of the social environment. Organizational Behavior and Human
Zaccaro, S. J., Green, J. P., Dubrow, S., & Kolze, M. (2018). Leader individual differences, Decision Processes, 110(2), 118–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2009.06.004.
situational parameters, and leadership outcomes: A comprehensive review and

319

You might also like