You are on page 1of 4

CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

Culture and Cognitive Development CHIMPANZEE AND


1 HUMAN CULTURE
Michael Tomasello
Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany It is widely agreed among be-
havioral biologists that the best ex-
amples of animal culture come
from chimpanzees. For example,
emphasize biology. These neo-
Abstract different chimpanzee communities
nativists believe that organic evo-
Human beings are biologi- have been documented to have dif-
lution has provided human beings ferent tool-use traditions, such as
cally adapted for culture in
with some specific domains of termite-fishing, ant-fishing, ant-
ways that other primates are
not. The difference can be knowledge of the world and its dipping, nut-cracking, and leaf-
clearly seen when the social workings and that this knowledge sponging (Tomasello & Call, 1997).
learning skills of humans and is best characterized as “innate.” Some of these community differ-
their nearest primate relatives Such domains include, for ex- ences are due to the different local
are systematically compared. ample, mathematics, language, bi- ecologies of different groups of
The human adaptation for cul- ology, and psychology. chimpanzees. The individuals of
ture begins to make itself In the other group are theorists each group learn to solve the prob-
manifest in human ontogeny at who have focused on the cultural lems presented by their local envi-
around 1 year of age as human dimension of human cognitive de- ronment using the resources avail-
infants come to understand velopment. These cultural psy- able in that environment.
other persons as intentional chologists begin with the fact that But experimental studies have
agents like the self and so en- human children grow into cogni- shown that there is more to it than
gage in joint attentional inter- tively competent adults in the con- this; chimpanzees can learn things
actions with them. This under- text of a structured social world from observing others using tools.
standing then enables young full of material and symbolic arti- What they learn, however, is less
children (a) to employ some facts such as tools and language, than might be expected. They learn
uniquely powerful forms of structured social interactions such the effects on the environment that
cultural learning to acquire the as rituals and games, and cultural can be produced with a particular
accumulated wisdom of their institutions such as families and re- tool; they do not actually learn to
cultures, especially as embod- ligions. The claim is that the cul- copy another chimpanzee’s behav-
ied in language, and also (b) to tural context is not just a facilitator ioral strategies. For example, in one
comprehend their worlds in or motivator for cognitive develop- study, chimpanzees were pre-
some uniquely powerful ways ment, but rather a unique “ontoge- sented with a rakelike tool and an
involving perspectivally based netic niche” (i.e., a unique context out-of-reach object. The tool could
symbolic representations. for development) that actually be used in either of two ways to
structures human cognition in fun- obtain the object. One group of
Keywords damental ways. chimpanzees observed one way of
culture; cognition; human evo- There are many thoughtful sci- using the tool, and another group
lution; language; joint attention entists in each of these theoretical observed the other way. However,
camps. This suggests the possibil- the demonstration observed had
ity that each has identified some no effect on which method or
Until fairly recently, the study of aspects of the overall theory that methods the chimpanzees used to
children’s cognitive development will be needed to go beyond Piaget obtain the object. This kind of
was dominated by the theory of and incorporate adequately both learning is called emulation learn-
Jean Piaget. Piaget’s theory was de- the cultural and the biological di- ing. In contrast, when human chil-
tailed, elaborate, comprehensive, mensions of human cognitive de- dren were given this same task,
and, in many important respects, velopment. What is needed to they much more often imitatively
wrong. In attempting to fill the achieve this aim, in my opinion, is learned the precise technique dem-
theoretical vacuum created by (a) an evolutionary approach to the onstrated for them (see Tomasello,
Piaget’s demise, developmental human capacity for culture and (b) 1996, for a review). Studies of
psychologists have sorted them- an ontogenetic approach to human chimpanzee gestural communica-
selves into two main groups. In the cognitive development in the con- tion have found similar results.
first group are those theorists who text of culture. Young chimpanzees ritualize sig-

Copyright © 2000 American Psychological Society 37


38 VOLUME 9, NUMBER 2, APRIL 2000

nals with group mates over re- the context of something like the This social-cognitive revolution
peated encounters in which they accumulated wisdom of its entire at 1 year of age sets the stage for
essentially shape one another’s be- social group, past and present. the 2nd year of life, in which in-
havior. They do not learn the sig- fants begin to imitatively learn the
nals of group mates via imitation use of all kinds of tools, artifacts,
(Tomasello et al., 1997). and symbols. For example, in a
HUMAN CULTURAL
Chimpanzees and other nonhu- study by Meltzoff (1988), 14-
LEARNING
man animals may thus engage in month-old children observed an
some forms of cultural transmis- adult bend at the waist and touch
sion, defined very broadly as the The human adaptation for cul- his head to a panel, thus turning on
nongenetic transfer of information, tural learning is best seen ontoge- a light. They followed suit. Infants
but they do not do this by means of netically and in the context of in- engaged in this unusual and awk-
imitative learning if this is defined fants’ other social and cognitive ward behavior even though it
more narrowly as the reproduction activities. The key transition occurs would have been easier and more
of another individual’s actual be- at 9 to 12 months of age, as infants natural for them simply to push the
havioral strategy toward a goal. In begin to engage in interactions that panel with their hand. One inter-
contrast, human beings learn from are triadic in the sense that they in- pretation of this behavior is that
conspecifics by perceiving their volve the referential triangle of the infants understood that (a) the
goals and then attempting to repro- child, adult, and some outside en- adult had the goal of illuminating
duce the strategies the other per- tity to which they are both attend- the light and then chose one means
sons use in attempting to achieve ing. Thus, infants at this age begin for doing so, from among other
those goals—truly cultural learn- to flexibly and reliably look where possible means, and (b) if they had
ing, as opposed to merely social adults are looking (gaze following), the same goal, they could choose
learning (Tomasello, Kruger, & use adults as emotional reference the same means. Cultural learning
Ratner, 1993). points (social referencing), and act of this type thus relies fundamen-
This small difference in learning on objects in the way adults are act- tally on infants’ tendency to iden-
process leads to a huge difference ing on them (imitative learning)— tify with adults, and on their ability
in cultural evolution; specifically, in short, 1-year-olds begin to “tune to distinguish in the actions of oth-
only cultural learning leads to cu- in” to the attention and behavior of ers the underlying goal and the dif-
mulative cultural evolution in adults toward outside entities. At ferent means that might be used to
which the culture produces arti- this same age, infants also begin to achieve it. This interpretation is
facts—both material artifacts, such use communicative gestures to di- supported by Meltzoff’s (1995)
as tools, and symbolic artifacts, rect adult attention and behavior to more recent finding that 18-month-
such as language and Arabic nu- outside entities in which they are old children also imitatively learn
merals—that accumulate modifica- interested—in short, to get the actions that an adult intends to per-
tions over historical time. Thus, adult to “tune in” to them. Most form, even if she is unsuccessful in
one person invents something, often, the term joint attention has doing so. Similarly, my colleagues
other persons learn it and then been used to characterize this and I (Carpenter, Akhtar, & Toma-
modify and improve it, and then whole complex of triadic social sello, 1998) found that 16-month-
this new and improved version is skills and interactions, and it rep- old infants imitatively learned
learned by a new generation—and resents a revolution in the way in- from a complex behavioral se-
so on across generations. Imitative fants understand other persons. quence only those behaviors that
learning is a key to this process be- There is evidence that infants can appeared intentional, ignoring
cause it enables individuals to ac- begin to engage in joint attentional those that appeared accidental.
quire the uses of artifacts and other interactions only when they under- Young children do not just mimic
practices of their social groups rela- stand other persons as intentional the limb movements of other per-
tively faithfully, and this relatively agents like themselves, that is, as sons; rather, they attempt to repro-
exact learning then serves as a kind persons who have behavioral and duce other persons’ intended, goal-
of ratchet—keeping the practice in perceptual goals and make active directed actions in the world.
place in the social group (perhaps choices among the means for at- Although it is not obvious at
for many generations) until some taining those goals (Carpenter, first glance, something like this
creative innovation comes along. Nagell, & Tomasello, 1998). (I un- same imitative learning process
Each human child, in using these derstand attention to be intentional must happen if children are to
artifacts to mediate its interactions focusing on one aspect of experi- learn the symbolic conventions of
with the world, thus grows up in ence to the exclusion of others.) their native language. In some re-

Published by Blackwell Publishers Inc.


CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 39

cent experiments, we have found spective on the world that can be may be attentionally construed by
that children learn words in situa- entered into, directed, and shared. “us,” the users of the symbol. The
tions in which they must work Indeed, a strong argument can be perspectival nature of linguistic
fairly hard to discern the adult’s made that children can understand symbols thus represents a clear
communicative intentions. For ex- a symbolic convention in the first break with straightforward percep-
ample, one study involved an adult place only if they understand their tual or sensory-motor cognitive
playing a “finding game” with communicative partner as an inten- representations, and indeed this
children. The adult had each child tional agent with whom one may perspectivity is what gives linguis-
find four different objects in four share attention—because a linguis- tic symbols their awesome cogni-
different hiding places, one of tic symbol is nothing other than a tive power (Tomasello, 1999). It
which was a very distinctive toy marker for an intersubjectively even allows children to learn lin-
barn. Once the child had learned shared understanding of a situa- guistic means for conceptualizing
which objects went with which tion (Tomasello, in press). Thus, objects as actions (He porched the
places, the adult announced her in- children with autism do not under- newspaper), actions as objects (Ski-
tention to “find the gazzer.” She stand other persons as intentional ing is fun), and many other meta-
then went to the toy barn, but it agents, or they do so to only an im- phorical construals (Love is a jour-
turned out to be “locked.” She then perfect degree, and so (a) they are ney).
frowned at the barn and proceeded very poor at the imitative learning
to extract other objects from the of intentional actions in general, (b)
other hiding places. Later, the chil- only half of them ever learn any
CULTURAL COGNITION
dren demonstrated that they had language at all, and (c) those who
learned “gazzer” as the name of do learn some language are very
the object locked in the barn. What poor in word-learning situations The biological origin of human
is significant about this finding is such as those just described (Hob- culture is an adaptation that oc-
that the children knew which one son, 1993). curred at some point in human
was the gazzer even though they It is important to emphasize as evolution—probably quite re-
never saw the target object after well that when children learn lin- cently, in the past 150,000 years,
they heard the new word; they had guistic symbols, what they are with the rise of modern humans. It
to infer from the adult’s behavior learning is a whole panoply of was not an everyday adaptation,
(trying to get into the barn and ways to manipulate the attention of however, because it did not just
frowning when it was impossible) other persons, sometimes on a change one relatively isolated char-
which object she wanted, without single entity, on the basis of such acteristic, it changed the process of
even seeing the object (see Toma- things as human evolution. It did this most
sello, in press, for a review). immediately by changing the na-
This kind of learning can be re- ● generality (thing, furniture, chair, ture of human social cognition,
ferred to as cultural learning be- desk chair), which in turn changed the nature
cause the child is not just learning ● perspective (chase-flee, buy-sell, of human cultural transmission,
things from other persons but is come-go, borrow-lend), and which in turn led to a series of cas-
learning things through them—in ● function (father, lawyer, man, cading sociological and psycho-
the sense that he or she must know American; coast, shore, beach). logical events in historical and on-
something of the adult’s perspec- togenetic time. The new form of
tive on a situation in order to learn And there are many other perspec- social cognition that started the en-
the same intentionally communica- tives that arise in grammatical tire process was the understanding
tive act (Tomasello et al., 1993). The combinations of various sorts (She of other persons as intentional
adult in the study just described is smashed the vase vs. The vase was agents like the self, and the new
not just moving and picking up ob- smashed). Consequently, as chil- process of cultural transmission
jects randomly, she is searching for dren internalize a linguistic sym- was the various forms of cultural
an object, and the child must know bol—as they learn the human learning, the first and most impor-
this in order to make enough sense perspective embodied in that sym- tant of which was imitative learn-
of her behavior to connect the new bol—they not only cognitively rep- ing (the others are instructed learn-
word to its intended referent. An resent the perceptual or motoric ing and collaborative learning).
organism can engage in cultural aspects of a situation, but also cog- These new forms of cultural learn-
learning of this type only when it nitively represent one way, among ing created the possibility of a kind
understands others as intentional other ways of which they are also of ratchet effect in which human
agents like the self who have a per- aware, that the current situation beings not only pooled their cogni-

Copyright © 2000 American Psychological Society


40 VOLUME 9, NUMBER 2, APRIL 2000

tive resources contemporaneously, age. Monographs of the Society for Research in


Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Toma- Child Development, 63(4, Serial No. 255).
but also built on one another’s cog- sello, M. (1998). (See References)
Hobson, P. (1993). Autism and the development of
nitive inventions over time. This Tomasello, M. (1999). (See Refer- mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
new form of cultural evolution ences) Meltzoff, A. (1988). Infant imitation and memory:
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). (See Nine-month olds in immediate and deferred
thus created artifacts and social References) tests. Child Development, 59, 217–225.
practices with a “history.” The Tomasello, M., Kruger, A., & Ratner, Meltzoff, A. (1995). Understanding the intentions
most important artifact in this con- H. (1993). (See References) of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by
nection is language, the acquisition 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychol-
ogy, 31, 838–850.
of which leads to some new forms Tomasello, M. (1996). Do apes ape? In J. Galef & C.
of perspectivally based (i.e., sym- Note Heyes (Eds.), Social learning in animals: The
roots of culture (pp. 319–346). New York: Aca-
bolic) cognitive representation. demic Press.
Modern human cognition is thus a 1. Address correspondence to Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human
result not just of processes of bio- Michael Tomasello, Max Planck Insti- cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer-
sity Press.
logical evolution, but also of cul- tute for Evolutionary Anthropology,
Inselstrasse 22, D-04103 Leipzig, Ger- Tomasello, M. (in press). Perceiving intentions and
tural processes that human biologi- learning words in the second year of life. In M.
many; e-mail: tomas@eva.mpg.de.
cal evolution made possible in both Bowerman & S. Levinson (Eds.), Language ac-
quisition and conceptual development. New York:
cultural-historical time and ontoge- Cambridge University Press.
netic time. References Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Tomasello, M., Call, J., Warren, J., Frost, T., Car-
14- through 18-month-old infants differen- penter, M., & Nagell, K. (1997). The ontogeny
Recommended Reading tially imitate intentional and accidental ac- of chimpanzee gestural signals: A comparison
tions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 315– across groups and generations. Evolution of
Boesch, C., & Tomasello, M. (1998). 330. Communication, 1, 223–253.
Chimpanzee and human culture. Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Tomasello, M., Kruger, A., & Ratner, H. (1993).
Current Anthropology, 39, 591–604. Social cognition, joint attention, and commu- Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
nicative competence from 9 to 15 months of 16, 495–552.

Published by Blackwell Publishers Inc.

You might also like