You are on page 1of 9

ARTICLE IN PRESS

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com


JOURNAL OF
PSYCHIATRIC
Journal of Psychiatric Research xxx (2008) xxx–xxx
RESEARCH
www.elsevier.com/locate/jpsychires

Empathy and symptoms dimensions of patients


with obsessive–compulsive disorder
Leonardo F. Fontenelle a,b,*, Isabela D. Soares c, Flavia Miele a, Manuela C. Borges a,
Angélica M. Prazeres a, Bernard P. Rangé c, Jorge Moll d
a
Anxiety and Depression Research Program, Institute of Psychiatry, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
b
Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Institute of Community Health, Fluminense Federal University, Niterói, Brazil
c
Postgraduate Program in Psychology, Institute of Psychology, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
d
Brain Morphometry and Investigative Neuroimaging, LABS-D’Or Hospital Network, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil

Received 7 April 2008; received in revised form 24 May 2008; accepted 27 May 2008

Abstract

Patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) often display cognitions and/or behaviors that may well reflect the existence of
‘‘hyper-attachment” to different environmental elements, including their offspring, family members, divine entities, or even inanimate
objects. Based on the fact that both OCD symptoms and physiologic interpersonal attachment mechanisms involve overlapping ventral
fronto-limbic circuits, we hypothesized that there is a relationship between empathy, evaluated with the Interpersonal Reactivity Index
(IRI), and OCD symptom dimensions. We evaluated 53 patients with OCD and 53 age- and sex-matched individuals from the commu-
nity with the Structured Clinical Interview for the Diagnosis of DSM-IV axis I disorders, the Saving Inventory-Revised, the IRI (com-
posed of four sub-scales), the Obsessive–Compulsive Inventory – Revised, the Beck Depression Inventory, and the Beck Anxiety
Inventory. Patients with OCD displayed greater levels of affective empathy (i.e., empathic concern (p = 0.006) and personal discomfort
(p < 0.001)) than community controls. In bivariate analyses, the severity of hoarding symptoms of patients with OCD correlated with
empathic concern (r = 0.39; p < 0.001), fantasy (r = 0.36; p < 0.01), and personal discomfort (r = 0.39; p < 0.001). In partial correlation
analyses adjusting for comorbid depression and anxiety, only the association between hoarding and fantasy remained robust (r = 0.41;
p < 0.001). A model that included severity of hoarding, depression, and anxiety symptoms predicted 33% of the variance on the fantasy
scale. Our findings suggest that hoarding is linked to specific aspects of interpersonal reactivity. Comorbid depression and anxiety, how-
ever, explain a large proportion of the empathic profile exhibited by patients with OCD.
Ó 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Obsessive–compulsive disorder; Hoarding; Empathy

1. Introduction empathy is not a unitary system but rather a loose collec-


tion of partially dissociable neurocognitive systems. In par-
There is no dispute that empathy – the capacity to ticular, three main divisions have been made: cognitive
understand and share emotional states of others in refer- empathy (or Theory of Mind), emotional empathy, and
ence to oneself – plays a critical role in human interper- motor empathy (Blair, 2005). Several neuropsychiatric dis-
sonal engagement and social interaction (Decety and orders have been shown to present deficits in different types
Moriguchi, 2007). Nevertheless, it has been argued that of empathic abilities including schizophrenia (Montag
et al., 2007), Asperger syndrome (Rogers et al., 2007), psy-
*
chopathy (de Oliveira-Souza et al., 2008), brain injury
Corresponding author. Address: Rua Visconde de Pirajá 547, Sala 323,
(Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2004), and frontotemporal lobar
22410-003 Ipanema, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil. Tel./fax: +55 21 2239 4919.
E-mail address: Ifontenelle@gmail.com (L.F. Fontenelle). degeneration (Rankin et al., 2005).

0022-3956/$ - see front matter Ó 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.05.007

Please cite this article in press as: Fontenelle LF et al., Empathy and symptoms dimensions of patients ..., Journal of Psychiatric Re-
search (2008), doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.05.007
ARTICLE IN PRESS

2 L.F. Fontenelle et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research xxx (2008) xxx–xxx

An important emerging concept is that empathic abili- ent sub-types of OCD and at searching for additional
ties may strongly rely on the neurobiology of social attach- dimensions of neurobiological significance. It could be
ment or bonding, which has been extensively studied in the case, for example, that increased levels of empathy
animals (Insel, 1997; Young and Wang, 2004) and, more could lead to an overvaluation of certain thoughts (Obses-
recently, in humans (Bowlby, 1988; Moll et al., 2006, sive–Compulsive Cognitions Working Group – OCCWG,
2008; Krueger et al., 2007). Interestingly, patients with 1997) with aggressive (i.e., ‘‘I fear acting unwanted
obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) often show cogni- impulses (e.g., stabbing my children)”), sexual (i.e., ‘‘I am
tions and/or behaviors that may well reflect the existence afraid of having sexual behavior toward other people”),
of ‘‘hyper-attachment” or excessive bonding to different or even contamination themes (i.e., ‘‘I am concerned that
environmental elements, including their offspring or close will get others ill by spreading contaminants”), resulting
family members (e.g., in post-partum ‘‘maternal preoccu- in full-formed obsessions of the so-called ‘‘obsessions-
pations” – Leckman and Herman, 2002); divine entities checking” dimension of OCD.
(Birgegard and Granqvist, 2004) (e.g., in religious scrupu- One additional, perhaps more significant, role of
losity – Nelson et al., 2006) or even inanimate objects empathic abilities in OCD could be that played by a close
(e.g., in the compulsive hoarding syndrome – Frost et al., construct, i.e., emotional attachment, in certain hoarding
1995). In the present paper, we will address the possible behaviors. Although DSM-IV and Yale-Brown Obses-
links between OCD symptom dimensions and empathy. sive–Compulsive Scale (YBOCS) suggest that hoarding
Although it is unquestionable that OCD is a homoge- involves only non-sentimental saving, several studies con-
neous disorder in terms of form (i.e., the presence of obses- tradict this assumption (Frost et al., 1998). On the basis
sive thoughts and/or compulsive rituals), there is a large of some clinical observations, for example, it could be
variability in terms of symptoms content. For example, in argued that hoarding may be one way of expressing
a recent review paper of 12 principal-components and con- increased levels of empathic abilities or, more speculatively,
firmatory factor analytic studies involving over 2000 that empathy may represent some sort of cognitive or affec-
patients with OCD, at least four symptom dimensions were tive counterpart of some types of hoarding.
consistently extracted: symmetry/ordering, contamination/ For example, Frost and Gross found that hoarders
cleaning, obsessions/checking, and hoarding (Mataix-Cols reported more sentimental saving and greater emotional
et al., 2005). These dimensions were associated with dis- attachment to possessions than non-hoarders. In fact,
tinctive clinical, biological and outcome features. The evi- many hoarders see their possessions as extensions of their
dence supporting the hoarding dimension was bodies, i.e., when other people touch or move their belong-
particularly robust (Mataix-Cols et al., 2005). ings; the hoarders tend to feel violated (Frost et al., 1995).
In fact, the so-called ‘‘compulsive hoarding syndrome” For other individuals, some possessions may serve as
in OCD has been associated with several differential fea- meaningful reminders of important past social events and
tures, including earlier age at onset (Samuels et al., 2002; getting rid of them may sound like the loss of a close friend.
Fontenelle et al., 2004); worse social functioning (Saxena Some hoarders may even impart human qualities to their
et al., 2002; Frost et al., 2000; Lochner et al., 2005; Whea- possessions or ‘‘anthropomorphize” them (Greenberg,
ton et al., 2008); lower insight into symptoms (Damecour 1987). On the other hand, hoarders may extend their emo-
and Charron, 1998; Storch et al., 2007); higher rates of past tional attachment to another level, accumulating not only
traumatic events (Hartl et al., 2004; LaSalle-Ricci et al., objects, but also living beings, including animals and
2006); greater frequency of certain types of OCD symp- friends; a phenomenon that could mimic increased inter-
toms (e.g., symmetry/ordering) and comorbid axis I and personal empathy and social bonding.
II disorders (Frost et al., 2000; Samuels et al., 2002; Fon- Finally, there may be some biological commonalties
tenelle et al., 2004; Lochner et al., 2005; LaSalle-Ricci between empathy and OCD, particularly hoarding. For
et al., 2006); specific COMT polymorphisms and linkage example, oxytocin has been consistently implicated in the
to a region on chromosome 14 (Lochner et al., 2005; Sam- mechanisms underlying empathy in humans. For example,
uels et al., 2007a,b); higher frequency of hoarding among Domes et al. (2007) found that, compared to placebo, 24
first-degree members (Samuels et al., 2007a); distinguished UI of intranasal oxytocin improved the performance of
neuroanatomical (Saxena et al., 2004) and neuropsycholog- normal subjects on the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test.
ical (Lawrence et al., 2006) underpinnings; possible dopa- On the other hand, a handful of studies have found
minergic involvement (Stein et al., 1999), and poorer increased levels of oxytocin in the cerebrospinal fluid of
treatment outcome to serotonin-reuptake inhibitors (Black patients with OCD (Leckman et al., 1994). Neuroimaging
et al., 1998; Winsberg et al., 1999; Mataix-Cols et al., studies have also provided important evidence linking
1999), cognitive behavioral therapy (Mataix-Cols et al., brain systems involved with attachment responses and
2002), or their combination (Frost et al., 2000). pro-social behaviors. Decisions to donate to charitable
It is difficult to speculate on the role of social cognition organizations and decisions to cooperate based on trust
in OCD in general and obsessive–compulsive symptoms were associated with activation of the septal and sub-gen-
dimensions in particular, but empathic abilities could pro- ual regions (Moll et al., 2006; Krueger et al., 2007), which
vide an additional paradigm aimed at disentangling differ- are intimately linked to mechanisms of social bonding and

Please cite this article in press as: Fontenelle LF et al., Empathy and symptoms dimensions of patients ..., Journal of Psychiatric Re-
search (2008), doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.05.007
ARTICLE IN PRESS

L.F. Fontenelle et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research xxx (2008) xxx–xxx 3

oxytocin release by the anterior hypothalamus (Freedman participants signed an informed consent after a detailed
et al., 2000). explanation of the procedures involved in the study.
Based on the aforementioned findings, we predicted that Patients with OCD, but not controls, were evaluated with
patients with OCD and hoarding symptoms would exhibit the Brazilian version of the SCID-IV (Del-Ben et al.,
increased levels of affective and cognitive empathic abili- 2001). The SCID-IV is a semi-structured clinical interview
ties. If our hypothesis is correct, hoarding may be the ulti- to be administered by the clinicians in order to identify axis
mate result of a social neurocognitive system in overdrive I diagnosis according to the DSM-IV criteria. Since two
or, more speculatively, the gregariousness presented by different centers (IPUB/UFRJ and IP/UFRJ) were respon-
some patients with OCD may be a forme fruste of ‘‘inter- sible for the recruitment and administration of the SCID in
personal” hoarding. Speculations aside, we believe that the majority of cases, some questions regarding the reliabil-
the study of interpersonal aspects, especially empathic con- ity of our methods could be raised. Nevertheless, the simul-
cern, of patients with OCD, may be of significant interest in taneous psychiatric (Anxiety and Depression Research
order to clarify the cognitive and affective aspects underling Program) and psychological (Division of Applied Psychol-
pathological hoarding behavior in OCD. ogy) follow-up of a significant portion of patients allowed
The objectives of this study were (1) to compare patients us to discuss and reach a consensus on cases presenting dif-
with OCD and individuals from the community in terms of ficult diagnoses.
interpersonal reactivity according to scores obtained on the A socio-demographic questionnaire was administered to
IRI and (2) to evaluate the relationship between the sever- both patients and controls including the following informa-
ity of different OCD dimensions (according to the OCI and tion: age, gender, educational level, marital and employ-
the SI-R) with the patterns of interpersonal reactivity as ment status, and monthly income. A clinical
measured by the IRI. questionnaire was employed to gather patients’ data on
the age at onset of sub-clinical obsessions and/or compul-
2. Methods sions (i.e., the age in which the patient remember first pre-
sented obsessive–compulsive symptoms without distress or
Patients were consecutively selected among those under impairment), different psychiatric comorbidities according
treatment in (1) the Anxiety and Depression Research Pro- to the SCID, and current therapeutic strategies.
gram at the Institute of Psychiatry of the Federal Univer-
sity of Rio de Janeiro (IPUB/UFRJ), (2) the Division of 2.2. Instruments
Applied Psychology at the Institute of Psychology of the
same university (DPA/UFRJ), and (3) the first author’s 2.2.1. Saving inventory – revised (SI-R)
private practice. The inclusion criteria were (1) the diagno- The SI-R was developed by Frost et al. (2004) and val-
sis of OCD, with or without psychiatric comorbidity con- idated to Brazilian Portuguese by Soares (2007). The SI-R
firmed by means of the Structured Clinical Interview for is aimed to evaluate the severity of hoarding behaviors in
Axis I disorders (SCID), (2) age between 18 and 80 years, clinical and non-clinical populations. It is a self-adminis-
and (3) absence of any other neurological, endocrinologi- tered Likert Scale that includes 23 items. The items are
cal, or systemic disorder that could interfere in our results. divided in three sub-scales: the clutter sub-scale (9 items),
In the presence of any psychiatric comorbidity, only the difficult discarding sub-scale (7 items) and the acquisi-
patients who developed OCD as a primary disorder, either tion sub-scale (7 items). A detailed description on the psy-
in terms of chronology or in terms of severity of symptoms, chometric properties of the Brazilian version of the SI-R is
were included. available upon request.
The control group consisted of individuals recruited in
the community by means of local advertisements, and 2.2.2. Beck depression inventory (BDI)
included members of the medical and administrative staff The BDI is one of most used self-rated instruments to
of the Federal University of Rio de Janeiro. The inclusion evaluate the severity of depression in different populations.
criteria for volunteers selected to participate in the control The scale has 21 items, each consisting of four statements
group were (1) age between 18 and 80 years, and (2) describing increasing intensities of symptoms of depres-
absence of any other neurological, endocrinological, or sys- sion. Items are rated on a scale from 0 to 3, reflecting
temic disorder that could interfere in our results. Commu- how participants have felt over the past week. Possible
nity controls were not evaluated a priori for the presence of scores range from 0 to 48; higher scores reflect more severe
psychiatric disorders. With this strategy, we intended to anxiety symptoms. The BDI was validated to Brazilian
avoid the selection of a ‘‘supernormal” sample, which Portuguese by Cunha (2001).
would be not representative of the general population.
2.2.3. Beck anxiety inventory (BAI)
2.1. Procedures The BAI is a self-rated instrument to evaluate the sever-
ity of anxiety in different populations. The scale has 21
The local institutional review board approved this research items describing subjective, somatic, or panic-related symp-
protocol (SISNEP code = CAAE - 0002.0.249.000-07). All toms, each consisting of four statements about physiologi-

Please cite this article in press as: Fontenelle LF et al., Empathy and symptoms dimensions of patients ..., Journal of Psychiatric Re-
search (2008), doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.05.007
ARTICLE IN PRESS

4 L.F. Fontenelle et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research xxx (2008) xxx–xxx

cal and cognitive components of anxiety. It was specifically gorical variables compared by means of the v2 test and
designed to reduce the overlap between depression and their continuous variables (including their scores on the
anxiety scales by measuring anxiety symptoms shared min- BDI, BAI, OCI-R, SI-R and IRI) compared by means of
imally with those of depression. Items are rated on a scale the Student’s t-test. We adopted standard deviation as
from 0 to 3, reflecting how participants have felt over the measure of variability. For the correlation analyses, only
past week. Possible scores range from 0 to 63; higher scores patients with OCD were selected. Bivariate Pearson’s cor-
reflect more severe anxiety symptoms. The BAI was vali- relation analyses were performed using IRI, OCI-R, and
dated to Brazilian Portuguese by Cunha (2001). the SI-R total and partial scores. Partial analyses including
these variables and controlling for the BDI and BAI scores
2.2.4. Obsessive–compulsive inventory – revised (OCI-R) were also performed. The level of statistical significance
The OCI-R, developed by Foa et al. (2002), is a revised was 5%.
and shorter version of the Obsessive–Compulsive Inventory
(OCI), an instrument created by the same group of investi- 3. Results
gators (Foa et al., 1998). The OCI-R evaluates the severity
of a series of obsessive–compulsive symptoms. The OCI-R 3.1. Description of the sample
is a Likert scale that contains 18 items and is self-adminis-
tered. It provides a total score and six sub-scores, including At first, a total of 135 participants fulfilled our inclusion
washing, checking, ordering, rumination, hoarding, and criteria: 65 patients with OCD and 70 community individ-
neutralization. Recently, the OCI-R was translated and uals. The volunteers were predominantly female individu-
adapted to Brazilian Portuguese by Souza et al. (2008). als (n = 82; 60.7%) with a current mean age of 37.3
(±13.5) years. Sixty-nine (51.1%) participants were single,
2.2.5. Interpersonal reactivity index (IRI) 48 (35.6%) were married, 7 (5.2%) were divorced, 6
The IRI, developed by Davis (1983), is the most used (4.4%) were separated and 5 (3.7%) were widowed. The vol-
self-administered instrument to assess the different compo- unteers had a mean of 13.2 ± 4.1 years of education and 78
nents of empathy. The IRI includes 28 items divided in four (57.8%) were employed. Their mean monthly income was
sub-scales (2 cognitive and 2 affective scales). Each answer 2.120 (±2.172) reais (about 1.100 American dollars).
score can vary from 0 (does not describe me well) to 4 A comparison between the socio-demographic charac-
(describes me very well). The scores of each sub-scale are teristics of our patients with OCD and volunteers from
calculated individually. The IRI does not provide a total the community is depicted in Table 1. Although the mean
score since each sub-scale evaluates an independent compo- monthly income of patients with OCD was lower than that
nent of empathy (D’Orazio, 2004). We employed a Brazil- of community controls, this difference did not reach statis-
ian Portuguese version of the IRI provided by Oliveira- tical significance.
Souza and Moll (unpublished version). In terms of OCD patients, 47 (72.3%) were selected from
The IRI cognitive sub-scales include the ‘‘perspective the sample being treated in the Anxiety and Depression
taking” and the ‘‘fantasy” sub-scales. The ‘‘perspective tak- Research Program from the IPUB/UFRJ, 10 (15.4%) from
ing” sub-scale evaluates the tendency that an individual has the Division of Applied Psychology of the IP/UFRJ, and 8
to adopt, spontaneously, the psychological point-of-view (12.3%) from the first authors’ private practice. Although it
of another person (e.g., I sometimes try to understand was tempting to compare patients recruited in different cen-
my friends better by imagining how things look from their
perspective). The ‘‘fantasy sub-scale” evaluates the ten-
dency that the individual has to identify him or herself with Table 1
fictitious personages, such as book, films, or plays charac- Comparison of socio-demographic features of patients with OCD and
community controls
ters (e.g., I really get involved with the feelings of the char-
acters in a novel). The IRI affective sub-scales include the Socio- Patients with Community Tests results
demographic OCD (SD) controls (SD)
‘‘empathic concern” and the ‘‘personal discomfort” sub-
features
scales. The empathic concern sub-scale refers to the feelings
Age (year) 39.3 (± 13.8) 35.5 (±13.0) t = 1.6;
of compassion, tenderness and concern for the others (e.g.,
df = 133; p = 0.1
I often have tender, concerned feelings for people less for- Gender v2 = 1.1, df = 1;
tunate than me). The ‘‘personal discomfort” sub-scale eval- Female 36 (55.4%) 46 (65.7%) p = 0.3
uates self-oriented anxiety and discomfort resulting from Male 29 (44.6%) 24 (34.3%)
tense personal situations (e.g., I sometimes feel helpless Marital status v2 = 1.7, df = 1;
Married 19 (29.2%) 28 (40.0%) p = 0.2
when I am in the middle of a very emotional situation).
Non-married 46 (70.8%) 42 (60.0%)
Educational level 13.0 (±3.9) 13.4 (±4.3) t = 0.5;
2.3. Statistical analyses (year) df = 132; p = 0.6
Monthly income 1.577 (±1121) 2.497 (±2615) Z = 1.08;
Fifty-three patients with OCD and 53 age- and gender- p = 0.3
matched individuals from the community had their cate- Note. SD: standard deviation.

Please cite this article in press as: Fontenelle LF et al., Empathy and symptoms dimensions of patients ..., Journal of Psychiatric Re-
search (2008), doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.05.007
ARTICLE IN PRESS

L.F. Fontenelle et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research xxx (2008) xxx–xxx 5

ters, the low numbers precluded valid analyses. In general ing for comorbid depression and anxiety, only the associa-
lines, however, they were similar from the socio-demo- tion between hoarding and fantasy remained robust
graphic point-of-view. (r = 0.41; p < 0.001). Female patients with OCD displayed
Patients with OCD reported the onset of obsessions and significantly greater scores on the personal distress scale
compulsions at 14.5 ± 10.5 and 15.9 ± 12.7 years, respec- than male patients (t = 3.21, df = 50, p = 0.002).
tively. In terms of comorbid disorders, 35.9% of the Since only the IRI fantasy scores were independently
patients with OCD exhibited a current major depressive associated with a particular obsessive–compulsive symp-
episode; 6.3% had social phobia and 6.3% presented a dys- tom dimension, we decided to perform a specific stepwise
thimic disorder. The low prevalence of comorbid condi- regression analysis using fantasy as the dependent variable
tions may be attributed by transversal nature of the study and the BAI, BDI, OCI-R dimensions, and the SI-R scores
and by the fact that patients were under psychological as independent ones. We found that a model that included
and/or pharmacological treatment. For example, 79.7% severity of depression, anxiety, and hoarding predicted
of patients were under a serotonin reuptake inhibitor, 33% of the variance on the fantasy scale (Table 5).
40% were using a benzodiazepine, and 32.3% were under
an antipsychotic. Further, 32.3% of the patients were being 4. Discussion
treated with cognitive behavioral therapy.
We found that patients with OCD presented signifi-
3.2. Main findings cantly higher levels of affective empathy (i.e., empathic con-
cern and personal discomfort) than community individuals.
Patients with OCD and controls were matched accord- Although, as predicted, statistically significant correlations
ing to age and gender, generating two groups of 53 partic- were found between the two forms of affective empathy and
ipants each, for the comparison of the profiles of response the severity of hoarding behaviors, similar correlations
on the IRI, as seen in Table 2. All patients with OCD were found between empathic concern and personal dis-
(n = 65) were included in the bivariate Pearson’s correla- comfort with other obsessive–compulsive symptoms, such
tion analyses (Table 3) and the partial analyses controlling as ordering, checking and washing. Further, a subsequent
for the BDI and BAI scores (Table 4). analysis revealed that the severity of comorbid symptoms
Patients with OCD displayed greater levels of affective of depression and anxiety largely explained the correlations
empathy (i.e., empathic concern (p = 0.006) and personal between affective empathy and obsessive–compulsive
discomfort (p < 0.001)) than community controls. In bivar- symptoms in patients with OCD.
iate analyses, the severity of hoarding symptoms of patients Leckman and Cohen (1999) suggested that patients with
with OCD correlated with empathic concern (r = 0.39; Tourette syndrome (TS), a condition biologically linked to
p < 0.001), fantasy (r = 0.36; p < 0.01), and personal some forms of OCD, are ‘‘sensitive to the feelings and
(r = 0.39; p < 0.001). In partial correlation analyses adjust- experiences of others, and have a thinner barrier to stimu-

Table 2
Comparison between the Interpersonal Reactivity Index of patients with OCD and community controls
IRI Patients with OCD (SD) Community individuals (SD) Results
Perspective taking 16.26 (±4.96) 16.81 (±3.51) t = 0.65; df = 93.6; p = 0.51
Fantasy scale 14.15 (±6.54) 12.43 (±5.04) t = 1.51; df = 97.6; p = 0.13
Empathic concern 20.83 (±4.63) 18.32 (±4.66) t = 2.77; df = 104; p = 0.006
Personal discomfort 17.32 (±6.75) 10.45 (±5.13) t = 5.90; df = 97.0; p < 0.001
Note. IRI: Interpersonal Reactivity Index; DF: degree of freedom; SD: standard deviation. As can be seen, some DF were slightly lower than 104 due to
few missing values.

Table 3
Bivariate correlation matrix of IRI’s sub-scores and psychopathological scores of patients with OCD (n = 65)
OCI-R SI-R
Checking Neutralization Obsession Ordering Washing Hoarding Total Clutter Dif. dis. Acquisition
IRI – PT 0.009 0.080 0.043 0.038 0.018 0.010 0.021 0.010 0.020 0.104
IRI – FS 0.228 0.289* 0.141 0.120 0.132 0.319** 0.392*** 0.415*** 0.364** 0.207
IRI – EC 0.247* 0.151 0.197 0.384** 0.384** 0.295* 0.364** 0.379** 0.301* 0.250*
IRI – PD 0.289* 0.251* 0.427*** 0.280* 0.329** 0.387*** 0.395*** 0.337** 0.419** 0.287*
Note. IRI: interpersonal reactivity index; PT: perspective taking; FS: fantasy scale; EC: empathic concern; PD: personal discomfort; OCI-R: obsessive–
compulsive inventory – revised; SI-R: saving Inventory – revised.
*
p < 0.05.
**
p < 0.01.
***
p < 0.001.

Please cite this article in press as: Fontenelle LF et al., Empathy and symptoms dimensions of patients ..., Journal of Psychiatric Re-
search (2008), doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.05.007
ARTICLE IN PRESS

6 L.F. Fontenelle et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research xxx (2008) xxx–xxx

Table 4
Partial correlation matrix of the IRI sub-scores and the psychopathological scores among patients with OCD (n = 65), controlling for BDI and BAI scores
OCI-R SI-R
Checking Neutralization Obsession Ordering Washing Hoarding Total Clutter Dif. dis. Acquisition
IRI – PT 0.005 0.073 0.050 0.022 0.033 0.015 0.056 0.014 0.014 0.146
IRI – FS 0.185 0.241 0.092 0.071 0.078 0.307* 0.417*** 0.464*** 0.352** 0.170
IRI – EC 0.003 0.063 0.019 0.079 0.219 0.053 0.076 0.133 0.027 0.009
IRI – PD 0.034 0.034 0.226 0.100 0.059 0.137 0.046 0.002 0.126 0.005
Note. IRI: interpersonal reactivity index; PT: perspective taking; FS: fantasy scale; EC: empathic concern; PD: personal discomfort; OCI-R: obsessive–
compulsive inventory – revised; SI-R: saving inventory – revised.
*
p < 0.05.
***
p < 0.001.
**
p < 0.01.

Table 5 opmental disorders were preliminarily excluded (n = 2).


Multiple stepwise regression analyses with the fantasy scale of the IRI as These conceptual and methodological aspects may have
the dependent variable (R2 = 0.33) contributed to a sample free of severe pathologies of the
B EP B t Significance central nervous system whose repetitive behaviors may be
BDI .332 .094 .570 3.522 0.001 best understood as stereotypes, and not obsessive–compul-
BAI .207 .083 .413 2.505 0.01 sive behaviors.
SI-R .126 .049 .455 2.568 0.01 We found that correlations between empathic concern
Note. BAI: Beck anxiety inventory; BDI: Beck depression inventory; SI-R: and obsessive–compulsive symptoms were no longer signif-
saving inventory – revised. icant when the effects of comorbid anxiety and depression
were controlled for. Since correlation analyses do not allow
lation” (also termed ‘‘somatosensory hyper-awareness”). inferences about causalities, these findings can be inter-
Although this was an untested clinical observation, find- preted in at least two not mutually excludable ways: (1)
ings from other controlled studies on TS are interesting empathic concern is a symptom of anxious or depressed
in this regard. Harrison et al. (2007) compared the profile patients with OCD and/or (2) increased levels of empathic
of response on the IRI of patients with TS with (n = 30) concern among patients with OCD is a risk-factor for
and without (n = 40) echopraxia. In this study, patients greater severity of comorbid anxiety and/or depression.
with TS and echopraxia exhibited higher scores on the In support for the first assertion, Griens et al. (2002)
IRI’s empathic concern, personal discomfort, and fantasy found that changes in the Neuroticism and Extraversion
scores than patients with TS without echopraxia. Although dimensions of the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (which
this evaluation was somewhat restricted, it would also be assesses the five major domains of the five-factor model
interesting to test whether patients with TS and obses- of personality (neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agree-
sive–compulsive symptoms describe increased levels of ableness, and conscientiousness)) and, most importantly,
interpersonal reactivity. Interestingly, echophenomena in the ‘‘interpersonal awareness” scores of the Interper-
were already associated with the presence of increased sonal Sensitivity Measure (IPSM), related strongly to the
obsessionality in TS (Robertson et al., 1988). decrease in depression severity of 80 patients with depres-
The fact that patients with OCD displayed increased lev- sive disorders after 12 weeks of treatment with psychother-
els of empathic concern than community controls is appar- apy and/or drugs. Therefore, we cannot discard the
ently in contrast to some recent findings. For example, possibility that empathic concern represents a ‘‘state”, as
Bejerot et al. (2001) described autistic traits in up to 20% opposed to a ‘‘trait” feature attributable to greater levels
of their OCD patients. This same group has insisted on of depression (and maybe anxiety) symptoms.
the existence of an autistic sub-type of patients with If increased levels of empathic concern predisposes to
OCD that would be characterized, among other features, depression, guilt feelings may mediate this relationship
by increased severity of hoarding behaviors (Bejerot, (O’Connor et al., 2007). For example, a study with daugh-
2007). Since pervasive developmental disorders are associ- ters of depressed mothers found that increased levels of
ated with decreased levels of empathy (Rogers et al., empathy related to the maternal suffering were associated
2007), we believe that the inconsistence between our study to inadequate problem-solving strategies and feelings of
findings and those described by these authors (Bejerot being guilty for the mother’s psychiatric disorder (Kil-
et al., 2001) can be ascribed to the different psychopatho- mes-Dougan and Bolger, 1998). Since OCD is associated
logical concepts of pathological hoarding behaviors with increased levels of pathological responsibility and
(Maier, 2004) and to our inclusion criteria: Firstly, patho- guilt (Rachman, 1993; Salkovskis et al., 1999), being more
logical hoarding may include diverse forms of symptoms, empathic can lead those individuals to be more distressed
such as compulsive, impulsive, or stereotypic behaviors and to present greater severity of depression and anxiety
(Maier, 2004). Secondly, in our study, patients with devel- symptoms. For example, obsessive–compulsive symptoms

Please cite this article in press as: Fontenelle LF et al., Empathy and symptoms dimensions of patients ..., Journal of Psychiatric Re-
search (2008), doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.05.007
ARTICLE IN PRESS

L.F. Fontenelle et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research xxx (2008) xxx–xxx 7

(e.g., contamination obsessions and avoidant behaviors) educational levels than regular patients with OCD. From
may inhibit the initiative and execution of altruistic actions the neurocognitive point-of-view, Hartl et al. (2004)
that would otherwise be performed by empathic individu- observed that patients with pathological hoarding did not
als, increasing guilt and depressive feelings. The recent evi- exhibit the impaired verbal learning strategies exhibited
dence pointing to the involvement of the sub-genual–septal by patients with OCD in general. In terms of social aspects,
area in major depression (Drevets et al., 1998; Mayberg difficulties exhibited by patients with pathological hoarding
et al., 2005) and in normal social attachment responses were reported in a related study (Soares et al., unpublished
(Bartels and Zeki, 2004; Aron et al., 2005; Moll et al., thesis) where 62% of the scores on the SF-36 sub-scores
2006; Krueger et al., 2007) strongly support this view. ‘‘social aspects” of patients with OCD were explained by
The levels of personal discomfort, i.e., the emotional a model that included the total scores of the IC-R.
response that involves fear of discomfort from witnessing It could be argued that shyness, loneliness, and social
stressful circumstances or a negative emotional state in anxiety in hoarders can be hardly consistent with increased
other people, were significantly greater among patients affective empathic levels, and, as initially predicted, exces-
with OCD than among individuals from the community. sive attachment or increased social bonding. We suggest,
Although the intensity of personal discomfort correlated however, that the negative social effects of severe hoarding
with all the OCI-R dimensions, analyses of covariance behaviors may hamper the full expression of increased
showed that the BDI and BAI scores explained the signif- empathic abilities that these patients may present.
icance of these findings. Indeed, although elevated levels of Admittedly, our study has a number of drawbacks:
IRI’s personal discomfort were seen in different psychiatric Firstly, it enrolled a small number of research subjects. Sec-
disorders, including schizophrenia (Montag et al., 2007); ondly, the comparison group included medical and admin-
Asperger syndrome (Rogers et al., 2007); and frontotempo- istrative staff at our research institution, hardly a random
ral dementia (Rankin et al., 2005), analyses of covariance control group. Thirdly, our patients with OCD were under
aimed at excluding the role of affective symptoms were drug and/or cognitive behavioral treatments and it would
not performed in these studies. Since personal discomfort have been interesting to compare empathic abilities
belongs to the affective dimension of empathy, we believe between those groups. Nevertheless, since there was a great
that this sort of analyses should be an essential part of overlap between patients being treated with drugs (almost
these types of studies. This approach may unveil whether 80%) and patients being treated with cognitive behavior
personal discomfort is a primary condition or secondary therapy (32%), the small number of patients being treated
to comorbid anxiety and depression. exclusively with one or another therapeutic strategy would
According to Davis (1983), the IRI fantasy scale taps compromise valid statistical analyses. It would also be
respondents’ tendencies to transpose themselves imagina- interesting to study OCD patients’ before and after treat-
tively into the feelings and actions of fictitious characters ment to check whether their distinctive empathic abilities
in books, movies, and plays. Unexpectedly, we found a cor- are ‘‘state” or ‘‘trait” features. Finally, it must also be sta-
relation between the fantasy scale of the IRI and the sever- ted that the fantasy scale of the IRI have been criticized for
ity of hoarding behaviors that remained significant after being overly broad (Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright, 2004),
excluding the effects of the severity of anxiety and depres- including not only aspects related to cognitive empathy,
sion symptoms. A subsequent stepwise regression analysis but also imagination (‘‘I daydream and fantasize, with
revealed that a model that included severity of depression, some regularity, about things that might happen to me”)
anxiety, and hoarding symptoms predicted 33% of the var- and emotional self-control (‘‘In emergency situations, I feel
iance on the fantasy scale (Table 5). apprehensive and ill at ease”).
In his original study, Davis (1983) found that the fan- In sum, while depression and anxiety explained
tasy scale was associated with shyness, loneliness, and increased levels of empathic concern and personal discom-
social anxiety in males, and with increased ‘‘emotionality” fort of patients with OCD, increased levels of fantasy were
and verbal fluency in general. Similar neurocognitive especially associated with hoarding behaviors. These find-
results were found by Rankin et al. (2005), who observed ings are consistent with the view that hoarding constitutes
that 25% of the fantasy scale scores where explained by a dimension of OCD that is associated with increased levels
the phonemic fluency. Davis (1983) suggested that individ- of psychopathology. Given the ever-rising literature on
uals with high scores on the fantasy scale were uncomfort- social cognition and the fact that these new paradigms
able in social settings, isolated, emotional, and intelligent. can help to identify neglected behavioral phenotypes, an
Therefore, it would be predictable that these patients approach based on social neuroscience may provide a fruit-
devote their time and intellectual involvement to the non- ful avenue of research to redraw the diagnostic frontiers of
social worlds of books, movies, and television – as the con- different neuropsychiatric disorders.
tent of the fantasy scale items implies. Indeed, this profile is
very similar to the one found in patients with pathological Conflicts of interest
hoarding in several studies.
For example, Fontenelle et al. (2004) found that patients All authors of the manuscript entitled ‘‘Empathy and
with OCD and pathological hoarding presented increased symptoms dimensions of patients with obsessive–compul-

Please cite this article in press as: Fontenelle LF et al., Empathy and symptoms dimensions of patients ..., Journal of Psychiatric Re-
search (2008), doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.05.007
ARTICLE IN PRESS

8 L.F. Fontenelle et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research xxx (2008) xxx–xxx

sive disorder” declare that they have no conflicts of Bowlby J. Developmental psychiatry comes of age. American Journal of
interest. Psychiatry 1988;145(1):1–10.
Cunha JA. Manual da versão em português das Escalas Beck. São Paulo:
Casa do Psicólogo; 2001.
Contributors Damecour CL, Charron M. Hoarding: a symptom, not a syndrome.
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 1998;59:267–72.
Find below the list of authors of the manuscript entitled Davis MH. Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a
multidimensional approach. Journal of Personal and Social Psychol-
‘‘Empathy and symptoms dimensions of patients with
ogy 1983;44:113–26.
obsessive–compulsive disorder” and how they have con- de Oliveira-Souza R, Hare RD, Bramati IE, Garrido GJ, Azevedo Ignácio
tributed to the study. F, Tovar-Moll F, et al. Psychopathy as a disorder of the moral brain:
Drs. Leonardo F. Fontenelle, Isabela D. Soares, Ber- fronto-temporo-limbic grey matter reductions demonstrated by voxel-
nard P Rangé, and Jorge Moll designed the study and based morphometry. Neuroimage 2008;40(3):1202–13.
Decety J, Moriguchi Y. The empathic brain and its dysfunction in
wrote the protocol. Drs. Isabela D. Soares, Flavia Miele,
psychiatric populations: implications for intervention across different
and Manuela C. Borges managed the literature searches clinical conditions. Biopsychosocial Medicine 2007;1:22.
and analyses. Dr. Angélica M. Prazeres undertook the sta- Del-Ben CM, Vilela JAA, Crippa JAS. Confiabilidade da ‘‘Entrevista
tistical analysis, and Drs. Leonardo F. Fontenelle and Jor- Clı́nica Estruturada para o DSM-IV – Versão Clı́nica” traduzida para
ge Moll wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors o português. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria 2001;23:156–9.
Domes G, Heinrichs M, Michel A, Berger C, Herpertz SC. Oxytocin
contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.
improves ‘‘mind-reading” in humans. Biological Psychiatry
2007;61:731–3.
Funding D’Orazio DM. The journal’s publication of research that incorrectly
employs Davis’ Interpersonal Reactivity Index. Sexual Abuse
2004;16:173–4.
Funding for the manuscript entitled ‘‘Empathy and
Drevets WC, Ongür D, Price JL. Neuroimaging abnormalities in the
symptoms dimensions of patients with obsessive–compul- subgenual prefrontal cortex: implications for the pathophysiology of
sive disorder” study was provided by the Conselho Nacion- familial mood disorders. Molecular Psychiatry 1998;3(3):220–6, 190–1.
al de Desenvolvimento Cientfico e Tecnolgico (CNPq); the Foa EB, Kozak MJ, Salkovskis PM, Coles ME, Amir N. The validation of
CNPq had no further role in study design; in the collection, a new obsessive–compulsive disorder scale: the obsessive–compulsive
inventory. Psychological Assessment 1998;10:206–14.
analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the
Foa EB, Huppert JD, Leiberg S, Langner R, Kichic R, Hajcak G, et al.
report; and in the decision to submit the paper for The Obsessive–Compulsive Inventory: development and validation of
publication. a short version. Psychological Assessment 2002;14:485–96.
Fontenelle LF, Mendlowicz MV, Soares ID, Versiani M. Patients with
Acknowledgement obsessive–compulsive disorder and hoarding symptoms: a distinctive
clinical subtype? Comprehensive Psychiatry 2004;45:375–83.
Freedman LJ, Insel TR, Smith Y. Subcortical projections of area 25
We thank Prof. Ricardo de Oliveira-Souza, who assisted (subgenual cortex) of the macaque monkey. Journal of Comparative
with the discussion of the manuscript ‘‘Empathy and symp- Neurology 2000;421(2):172–88.
toms dimensions of patients with obsessive–compulsive Frost RO, Hartl TL, Christian R, Williams N. The value of possessions in
disorder”. compulsive hoarding: patterns of use and attachment. Behavior
Research and Therapy 1995;33:897–902.
Frost RO, Steketee G. Hoarding: clinical aspects and treatment strategies.
References In: Jenike MA, Baer L, Minichiello WE, editors. Obsessive–compul-
sive disorders practical management. 3rd ed. St. Louis (MO); 1998. p.
Aron A, Fisher H, Mashek DJ, Strong G, Li H, Brown LL. Reward, 533–54.
motivation, and emotion systems associated with early-stage intense Frost RO, Steketee G, Grisham J. Measurement of compulsive hoarding:
romantic love. Journal of Neurophysiology 2005;94(1):327–37. saving inventory – revised. Behavior Research and Therapy
Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S. The empathy quotient: an investigation 2004;42:1163–82.
of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and Frost RO, Steketee G, Williams LF, Warren R. Mood, personality
normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- disorder symptoms and disability in obsessive compulsive hoarders: a
ders 2004;34:163–75. comparison with clinical and nonclinical controls. Behavior Research
Bartels A, Zeki S. The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. and Therapy 2000;38:1071–81.
Neuroimage 2004;21(3):1155–66. Greenberg D. Compulsive hoarding. American Journal of Psychotherapy
Bejerot S. An autistic dimension: a proposed subtype of obsessive– 1987;41:409–16.
compulsive disorder. Autism 2007;11:101–10. Griens AM, Jonker K, Spinhoven P, Blom MB. The influence of
Bejerot S, Nylander L, Lindström E. Autistic traits in obsessive– depressive state features on trait measurement. Journal of Affective
compulsive disorder. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry 2001;55:169–76. Disorders 2002;70:95–9.
Birgegard A, Granqvist P. The correspondence between attachment to Harrison NA, Cavanna AE, Draganski B, Robertson MM, Frackowiack
parents and God: three experiments using subliminal separation cues. RS, Critchely HD. Echophenomena in Tourette’s disorder predict
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 2004;30:1122–35. emotional empathy and are associated with increased grey matter
Black DW, Monahan P, Gable J, Blum N, Clancy G, Baker P. Hoarding volume in frontal mirror and parietal default mode neural systems.
and treatment response in 38 nondepressed subjects with obsessive– Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience
compulsive disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 1998;59:420–5. 2007;19:209–12.
Blair RJ. Responding to the emotions of others: dissociating forms of Hartl TL, Frost RO, Allen GJ, Deckersbach T, Steketee G, Duffany SR,
empathy through the study of typical and psychiatric populations. et al. Actual and perceived memory deficits in individuals with
Consciousness and Cognition 2005;14:698–718. compulsive hoarding. Depression and Anxiety 2004;20:59–69.

Please cite this article in press as: Fontenelle LF et al., Empathy and symptoms dimensions of patients ..., Journal of Psychiatric Re-
search (2008), doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.05.007
ARTICLE IN PRESS

L.F. Fontenelle et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research xxx (2008) xxx–xxx 9

Insel TR. A neurobiological basis of social attachment. American Journal O’Connor LE, Berry JW, Lewis T, Mulherin K, Crisostomo PS. Empathy
of Psychiatry 1997;154(6):726–35. and depression: the moral system on overdrive. In: Farrow TFD,
Kilmes-Dougan B, Bolger AK. Coping with maternal depressed affect and Woodruff PWR, editors. Empathy in mental illness. Cambridge: Cam-
depression: adolescent children of depressed and well mothers. Journal bridge University Press; 2007. p. 49–75.
of Youth and Adolescence 1998;27:1–15. Rachman S. Obsessions, responsibility and guilt. Behavior Research and
Krueger F, McCabe K, Moll J, Kriegeskorte N, Zahn R, Strenziok M, Therapy 1993;31:149–54.
et al. Neural correlates of trust. Proceedings of the National Academic Rankin KP, Kramer JH, Miller BL. Patterns of cognitive and emotional
Science, USA 2007;104(50):20084–9. empathy in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Cognitive and Behav-
LaSalle-Ricci VH, Arnkoff DB, Glass CR, Crawley SA, Ronquillo JG, ioral Neurology 2005;18:28–36.
Murphy DL. The hoarding dimension of OCD: psychological comor- Robertson MM, Trimble MR, Lees AJ. The psychopathology of the Gilles
bidity and the five-factor personality model. Behavior Research and de la Tourette syndrome. A phenomenological analysis. British
Therapy 2006;44:1503–12. Journal of Psychiatry 1988;152:383–90.
Lawrence NS, Wooderson S, Mataix-Cols D, David R, Speckens A, Rogers K, Dziobek I, Hassenstab J, Wolf OT, Convit A. Who cares?
Phillips ML. Decision making and set shifting impairments are Revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and
associated with distinct symptom dimensions in obsessive–compulsive Developmental Disorders 2007;37:709–15.
disorder. Neuropsychology 2006;20:409–19. Salkovskis P, Shafran R, Rachman S, Freeston MH. Multiple pathways to
Leckman JF, Cohen DJ. Beyond the diagnosis – Darwinian perspectives inflated responsibility beliefs in obsessional problems: possible origins
on pathways to successful adaptation. In: Leckman JF, Cohen DJ, and implications for therapy and research. Behavior Research and
editors. Tourette’s syndrome: tics, obsessions, compulsions – develop- Therapy 1999;37:1055–72.
mental psychopathology and clinical care. New York: John Wiley and Samuels J, Bienvenu 3rd OJ, Riddle MA, Cullen BA, Grados MA, Liang
Sons; 1999. p. 140–52. KY, et al. Hoarding in obsessive compulsive disorder: results from a
Leckman JF, Goodman WK, North WG, Chappell PB, Price LH, Pauls case-control study. Behavior Research and Therapy 2002;40:517–28.
DL, et al. Elevated cerebrospinal fluid levels of oxytocin in obsessive– Samuels JF, Bienvenu Jr OJ, Pinto A, Fyer AJ, McCracken JT, Rauch SL,
compulsive disorder. Comparison with Tourette’s syndrome and et al. Hoarding in obsessive–compulsive disorder: results from the
healthy controls. Archives of Genetic Psychiatry 1994;51:782–92. OCD Collaborative Genetics Study. Behavior Research and Therapy
Leckman JF, Herman AE. Maternal behavior and developmental 2007a;45:673–86.
psychopathology. Biological Psychiatry 2002;51:27–43. Samuels J, Shugart YY, Grados MA, Willour VL, Bienvenu OJ,
Lochner C, Kinnear CJ, Hemmings SM, Seller C, Niehaus DJ, Knowles Greenberg BD, et al. Significant linkage to compulsive hoarding on
JA, et al. Hoarding in obsessive–compulsive disorder: clinical and chromosome 14 in families with obsessive–compulsive disorder: results
genetic correlates. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 2005;66:1155–60. from the OCD Collaborative Genetics Study. American Journal of
Maier T. On phenomenology and classification of hoarding: a review. Psychiatry 2007b;164:493–9.
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 2004;110:323–37. Saxena S, Brody AL, Maidment KM, Smith EC, Zohrabi N, Katz E, et al.
Mayberg HS, Lozano AM, Voon V, McNeely HE, Seminowicz D, Cerebral glucose metabolism in obsessive–compulsive hoarding.
Hamani C, et al. Deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant American Journal of Psychiatry 2004;161:1038–48.
depression. Neuron 2005;45(5):651–60. Saxena S, Maidment KM, Vapnik T, Golden G, Rishwain T, Rosen RM,
Mataix-Cols D, Marks IM, Greist JH, Kobak KA, Baer L. Obsessive– et al. Obsessive–compulsive hoarding: symptom severity and response
compulsive symptom dimensions as predictors of compliance with and to multimodal treatment. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 2002;63:21–7.
response to behaviour therapy: results from a controlled trial. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tomer R, Goldsher D, Berger BD, Aharon-Peretz J.
Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 2002;71:255–62. Impairment in cognitive and affective empathy in patients with brain
Mataix-Cols D, Rauch SL, Manzo PA, Jenike MA, Baer L. Use of factor- lesions: anatomical and cognitive correlates. Journal of Clinical and
analyzed symptom dimensions to predict outcome with serotonin Experimental Neuropsychology 2004;26:1113–27.
reuptake inhibitors and placebo in the treatment of obsessive– Soares ID. Colecionismo patológico: avaliacßão clı́nica e psicométrica. Tese
compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry (doutorado). Rio de Janeiro: Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro,
1999;156:1409–16. Instituto de Psicologia; 2007.
Mataix-Cols D, Rosario-Campos MC, Leckman JF. A multidimensional Souza FP, Foa EB, Meyer E, Niederauer KG, Raffin AL, Cordioli AV.
model of obsessive–compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychi- Obsessive–compulsive inventory and obsessive–compulsive inventory-
atry 2005;162:228–38. revised scales: translation into Brazilian Portuguese and cross-cultural
Moll J, De Oliveira-Souza R, Zahn R. The neural basis of moral adaptation. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria 2008;30(1):42–6.
cognition: sentiments, concepts, and values. Annals of New York Stein DJ, Seedat S, Potocnik F. Hoarding: a review. Israel Journal of
Academic Science 2008;1124:161–80. Psychiatry and Related Sciences 1999;36:35–46.
Moll J, Krueger F, Zahn R, Pardini M, de Oliveira-Souza R, Grafman J. Storch EA, Lack CW, Merlo LJ, Geffken GR, Jacob ML, Murphy TK,
Human fronto-mesolimbic networks guide decisions about charitable et al. Clinical features of children and adolescents with obsessive–
donation. Proceedings of the National Academic Science, USA compulsive disorder and hoarding symptoms. Comprehensive Psychi-
2006;103(42):15623–8. atry 2007;48:313–8.
Montag C, Heinz A, Kunz D, Gallinat J. Self reported empathic abilities Wheaton M, Timpano KR, Lasalle-Ricci VH, Murphy D. Characterizing
in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 2007;92:85–9. the hoarding phenotype in individuals with OCD: associations with
Nelson EA, Abramowitz JS, Whiteside SP, Deacon BJ. Scrupulosity in comorbidity, severity and gender. Journal of Anxiety and Disorders
patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder: relationship to clinical 2008;22:243–52.
and cognitive phenomena. Journal of Anxiety and Disorders Winsberg ME, Cassic KS, Koran LM. Hoarding in obsessive–compulsive
2006;20:1071–86. disorder: a report of 20 cases. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry
Obsessive–Compulsive Cognitions Work Group (OCCWG). Cognitive 1999;60:591–7.
assessment of obsessive–compulsive disorder. Behavior Research and Young LJ, Wang Z. The neurobiology of pair bonding. Nature Neuro-
Therapy 1997;35:667–81. science 2004;7(10):1048–54.

Please cite this article in press as: Fontenelle LF et al., Empathy and symptoms dimensions of patients ..., Journal of Psychiatric Re-
search (2008), doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.05.007

You might also like