You are on page 1of 9

Psychiatry Research ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]]

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Psychiatry Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychres

Theory of mind and context processing in schizophrenia: The role


of cognitive flexibility
Maud Champagne-Lavau a,b,c,n, Anick Charest b,c, Karyne Anselmo c, Jean-Pierre Rodriguez c, Guy Blouin c
a
Laboratoire Parole et Langage, UMR 7309 CNRS, Aix-Marseille University, 5 Av. Pasteur, B.P. 80975, 13604 Aix-en-Provence, France
b
Department of Psychiatry, University of Montreal, Canada
c
Hôpital du Sacré-Cœur de Montréal, Pavillon Albert-Prévost, Canada

a r t i c l e i n f o abstract

Article history: The present study sought to identify whether cognitive flexibility and context processing may impact
Received 4 November 2011 theory of mind (ToM) ability in schizophrenia. Thirty two patients with schizophrenia and 29 matched
Received in revised form healthy participants were tested individually on their ToM ability using a task involving attribution and
8 June 2012
comprehension of a speaker’s ironic intent. This task made it possible to determine whether the degree
Accepted 10 June 2012
of incongruity between contextual information and a target sentence has an impact on the attribution
of ironic intent to the protagonists of a story. Participants were also assessed on their cognitive
Keywords: flexibility and working memory. The main results revealed that participants with schizophrenia
Mental state attribution correctly perceived contextual information cueing attribution of ironic intent to the protagonist of
Contextual information
the stimulus, but they showed difficulty to correctly integrate this information, performing significantly
Ironic intent
worse than healthy participants when they attributed mental states. However, some participants with
Schizophrenia
Cognitive flexibility schizophrenia performed like healthy control participants on the ToM task while others did not. A lack
of flexibility seems to differentiate the two schizophrenia subgroups thereby obtained, suggesting that
cognitive flexibility has an impact on ToM performances in schizophrenia. These difficulties were not
associated with clinical symptoms. Such results will have an impact on cognitive remediation.
& 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction schizophrenia may be more or less impaired in their ToM skills


(Pickup and Frith, 2001; Champagne-Lavau et al., 2009; Brüne and
Patients with schizophrenia are known to be impaired in their Schaub, 2011). Indeed, Brüne and Schaub (2011) mention that
theory of mind (ToM) ability (Frith, 2004; Brüne, 2005a; ‘‘there is evidence to suggest that about 20–50% of patients with
Harrington et al., 2005; Sprong et al., 2007; Bora et al., 2009). schizophrenia perform similar to control on a wide range of tasks
ToM is the ability to form representations of other people’s devised to examine mental state attribution skills’’. Cognitive
mental states (e.g. intention, thought, belief) and to use these flexibility – among other aspects of cognition-has been recently
representations to understand, predict and judge their statements proposed to account for such heterogeneity of performances in
and behaviors (Premack and Woodruff, 1978; Baron-Cohen et al., ToM tasks in schizophrenia (Bora et al., 2007; Woodwards et al.,
1985). ToM impairments have been found in schizophrenia in 2009; Brüne and Schaub, (2011). Such cognitive ability to shift
natural communication situations (McCabe et al., 2004; between two perspectives (or two beliefs about the world)
Champagne-Lavau et al., 2009) and also in tasks assessing first according to the demands of the social situation may be necessary
and second order false beliefs (Mazza et al., 2001; Brüne, 2005b; for the attribution of mental state to others. However, no
Bora et al., 2007; Champagne-Lavau and Stip, 2010), irony consensus emerges in the literature concerning the link between
comprehension (Leitman et al., 2006; Kosmidis et al., 2008; ToM impairments and a lack of flexibility in schizophrenia
Kerns et al., 2009; Sparks et al., 2010), hinting comprehension (Pickup, 2008). Although most of the patients with schizophrenia
(Corcoran et al., 1995; Janssen et al., 2003; Greig et al., 2004; Bora are impaired relative to control participants on flexibility assessed
et al., 2006) and in picture-sequencing tasks (Langdon et al., 2006; by the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) or the Trail-making
Brüne et al., 2007). Recent findings have shown that people with test, some studies have shown a relationship with flexibility
(Greig et al., 2004; Bora et al., 2006; Van Hooren et al., 2008;
n
Brüne et al., 2011), but others have not (Mazza et al., 2001; Brüne,
Corresponding author at: Laboratoire Parole et Langage, UMR 7309 CNRS,
Aix-Marseille University, 5 Av. Pasteur, B.P. 80975, 13604 Aix-en-Provence, France.
2005b; Pinkham and Penn (2006)). More consistent evidence for
Tel.: þ33 4 13 55 27 07; fax: þ 33 4 13 55 37 44. cognitive flexibility is needed to investigate the influence of
E-mail address: maud.champagne-lavau@lpl-aix.fr (M. Champagne-Lavau). cognitive flexibility on ToM ability in schizophrenia.

0165-1781/$ - see front matter & 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.011

Please cite this article as: Champagne-Lavau, M., et al., Theory of mind and context processing in schizophrenia: The role of
cognitive flexibility. Psychiatry Research (2012), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.011
2 M. Champagne-Lavau et al. / Psychiatry Research ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]]

Furthermore, some research has shown that impairments in To avoid such limitations, we used a task of irony under-
context processing might underlie dysfunctions in various cogni- standing manipulating the level of contextual incongruity. Indeed,
tive domains and may thus represent a core disturbance in several psycholinguistic studies have shown that irony can be
information processing in schizophrenia (Cohen and Servan- perceived as a result of incongruity between context and speaker
Schreiber, 1992; Stratta et al., 1999; Green et al., 2005). Some utterance (Ivanko and Pexman, 2003). It has been demonstrated
authors also suggested that insensitivity to context may contri- that several factors, such as the degree of incongruity between
bute to the clinical symptoms observed in schizophrenia (Stratta context and speaker utterance and the intonation of the speaker
et al., 1999), particularly disorganization (Hardy-Bayle et al., (prosody), influence the extent to which ironic intent is perceived
2003). However, despite increasing evidence for impaired context (Bryant and Fox Tree, 2002; Ivanko and Pexman, 2003). This
processing, there has been little research into the relevance of means that, among healthy adults, the more incongruent the
these deficits for an understanding of social cognition, particularly context is, the better irony is perceived. If a speaker says to me
ToM, in schizophrenia. The present study aimed to give insight ‘‘you are a wonderful singer,’’ the utterance could be interpreted
into the specific mechanisms underlying ToM impairments in as a literal compliment (i.e., you really are a good singer) or as an
schizophrenia by studying underlying cognitive processes, such as ironic insult (i.e., you are a horrible singer). Interpretation of the
context processing and cognitive flexibility. Regarding this issue, speaker’s intent in making this utterance could be easier if there
Abu-Akel and Shamay-Tsoory (2011) propose a distinction had been even stronger incongruity between the events and the
between the ability to represent mental states and the ability to literal meaning of the speaker’s utterance. For instance, if I got up
attribute mental states. Cognitive flexibility should be more and sang and people started to throw things at me, the event
relevant to the attribution (or deployment) process of currently could be perceived as even more negative than if I got up and sang
represented mental states, as when choosing amongst competing and a light laugh could be heard. Moreover, the first contextual
hypotheses. sentence would have contrasted more sharply with the positive
Authors suggested that patients’ apparent inability to attribute literal meaning of the utterance ‘‘You are a wonderful singer’’
intention to others results from their impairment in processing than the second contextual sentence would have. To summarize,
contextual information (Hardy-Bayle et al., 2003; Green et al., the contextual incongruity is weaker when the utterance appears
2005; Schenkel et al., 2005; Green et al., 2007). Following the after the second contextual sentence than after the first one.
distinction proposed by Abu-Akel and Shamay-Tsoory (2011), a This finding was used in the present study to determine the role
deficit in context processing might be the general determinant of of insensitivity to contextual information in the attribution of
the deficits in ToM processing particularly affecting processes intention by patients with schizophrenia. This paradigm, unlike
associated with representing mental states and/or updating of those used in other studies, has an advantage in that the role of
currently represented mental states. Indeed, of all the situations contextual information processing does not rely on correlation
encountered in everyday life, communication with others analyses, which are limited in scope.
requires the greatest ability to adapt to the context and represent Accordingly, the aim of the present study was twofold: to
mental states: a conversation is by definition a shifting and determine whether contextual information manipulated by level
uncertain context, and the data associated with it must be of incongruity-which has been demonstrated to cue speaker
consistently inferred and updated on the basis of peripheral ironic intent in healthy subjects-also cues speaker ironic intent
information (Sperber and Wilson, 1986). Interestingly, McCabe in patients with schizophrenia, and to determine whether a lack
et al. (2004) found that patients with schizophrenia showed of cognitive flexibility may be related to mentalizing skills in
specific ToM problems in case of disagreement with the psychia- those patients. As far as we know, no research investigating ToM
trist while they seemed to be able to represent mental states of ability via irony comprehension in schizophrenia has also
others during clinical encounters. In other words, patients with assessed cognitive flexibility. We hypothesized that difficulty in
schizophrenia seemed unable to respond to change and incorpo- using contextual data that cue speaker ironic intent will have an
rate newly introduced information to revise their mental states in impact on the ToM ability of schizophrenia participants. In
such a case. Some studies have found evidence for an impairment addition, ToM impairment was expected to be associated with a
of context processing and have indicated a potential association lack of cognitive flexibility in these participants.
between ToM deficits and impairment in context processing
(Schenkel et al., 2005; Uhlhaas et al., 2006). However, these
studies varied according to the sort of context processing they
tapped into (high- or low-level) and the sort of contextual
information they used (visual, verbal, emotional, etc.). For exam- 2. Methods
ple, Uhlhaas et al. (2006) suggested a link between ToM deficits
and impairment of perceptual context processing using the 2.1. Participants
Ebbinghaus illusion, while Schenkel et al. (2005) hypothesized
that ToM deficits assessed by the Hinting task were related to The sample consisted of 61 participants: thirty two individuals with schizo-
deficits in context processing, assessed by the first part of the phrenia (SZ) and twenty nine healthy control (HC) participants with no history of
psychiatric disorders. SZ patients were diagnosed by clinicians according to DSM-IV
Hayling test (Burgess and Shallice, 1996) – a verbal task – and by diagnostic criteria. All SZ participants were outpatients who had been recruited from
the Contour Integration task (Kovacs et al., 1999) – a visual task. the Pavillon Albert Prévost of the Hôpital du Sacré-Coeur of Montréal. They were stable
These studies consisted mainly in analyzing the correlation and on antipsychotic medication with a normal recommended range of dosage (the
between ToM and visual perception task (Schenkel et al., 2005; average chlorpromazine equivalent was 621 mg/day7393). Severity of symptoms
was measured using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS; Kay et al.,
Uhlhaas et al., 2006) or emotional perception task (e.g. the Eyes
1987). HC participants had been recruited in the local community. They were
Test) performances (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001; Uhlhaas et al., matched with the SZ patients for age and educational level (cf. Table 1 for the
2006). In no study was the context manipulated in a way that demographic data). The two groups did not significantly differ with regard to age
would make it possible to know whether context processing is (t(59)¼  0.39, P40.05), educational level (t(59)¼  1.37, P40.05) and IQ estimated
important for ToM ability. Correlation data can provide only using the NART (Nelson, 1982) (t(59)¼0.91, P40.05). All participants were native
French-speakers with no previous neurological history.
limited evidence on how context processing is involved in ToM Written consent forms were obtained from all participants, according to ethics
ability. Manipulating contextual information in a ToM task is guidelines set out by the University of Montreal and the Hôpital du Sacré-Coeur
needed. de Montréal.

Please cite this article as: Champagne-Lavau, M., et al., Theory of mind and context processing in schizophrenia: The role of
cognitive flexibility. Psychiatry Research (2012), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.011
M. Champagne-Lavau et al. / Psychiatry Research ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]] 3

Table 1
Demographic, clinical and neurpsychological data.

Schizophrenia Healthy control p-value

Mean S.D. Mean S.D.

Age 41.6 9.3 42.6 10.6 0.816


Educational level 12.8 2.1 13.7 2.9 0.153
Gender (male/female) (21/11) (14/15)
Duration of illness 15.8 9.4
PANSS (positive) 17.9 4.2
PANSS (negative) 20.3 4.6
PANSS (general) 38.4 6.3
NART 45.4 9.6 42.7 7.9 0.302
Trail B (completion time) 115.5 49.9 58.9 18.9 o 0.0001
Trail B–Trail A (time) 71.6 41.2 29.5 15.4 o 0.0001
WCST (categories) 3.7 1.9 4.8 1.5 0.017
WCST (% perseverative errors) 41.0 25.3 19.1 19.8 o 0.001
Digit span (forward) 9.7 2.2 11.0 1.9 0.017
Digit span (backward) 4.7 1.7 6.5 2.3 o 0.001

2.2. Measures 3. Results

2.2.1. Neuropsychological measures 3.1. Group comparison on neuropsychological measures


Cognitive flexibility was assessed in all participants using the Trail-Making
Test (Reitan and Wolfson, 1993) and the WCST (Heaton, 1981). For the Trail-
Making test, the completion times in conditions B and A were recorded and a time
Significant differences between the SZ and HC groups were found
score (Trail B–Trail A) was calculated as a measure of flexibility. The number of on Trail-making test B (t(59)¼5.37, Po0.0001), Trail B–Trail
categories and the percentage of perseveration were recorded as a measure of A (t(59)¼5.39, Po0.0001), the WCST-category (t(59)¼  2.31,
flexibility for the WCST. Participants were also evaluated on their working Po0.025), the WCST-Perseverative errors (t(59)¼3.41, Po0.001),
memory to exclude any impairment that could have an impact on the irony task.
the Digit span forward (t(59)¼  2.21, Po0.031) and Digit span
The Digit Span subtest from the WAIS (Wechsler, 1981) was used to assess
working memory. backward (t(59)¼ 3.38, Po0.001). Thus, the SZ group performed
significantly worse than the HC group on tests that evaluate
flexibility and memory (cf. Table 1).
2.2.2. Theory of mind (ToM)
A task of irony understanding was used to test attribution of intention as the 3.2. Group comparison on ToM
comprehension of irony typically depends on the attribution and comprehension
of speaker intent. The 12 stories by Ivanko and Pexman (2003) controlled for The 2  3 ANOVA on the percentage of correct responses to the
familiarity and plausibility were used in the present study. To assess how context
question on speaker ironic intent revealed a main effect of context
manipulation influenced participants’ irony understanding, context was manipu-
lated according to the strength of context incongruity (strong incongruity, weak (F(2, 112) ¼55.69, Po0.0001) and a main effect of group
incongruity, no incongruity) between contextual information and target sentence (F(1,59)¼22.170, Po0.0001), showing that SZ patients made
(cf. Appendix for example). Thus, the 12 stories were distributed in three context more errors than HC participants. The group  context interaction
conditions intended to trigger either the literal meaning (no incongruity) or the was significant (F(2, 112) ¼7.60, Po0.001). This interaction was
ironic meaning (strong incongruity, weak incongruity) of the target sentence. Each
target sentence (e.g. Christine is a clever student) appeared in each context
decomposed according to group. The SZ group made more errors
condition across three versions of each stimulus (12  3 ¼36 stimuli). Contexts in answering questions on speaker intent in the weak incongruity
were presented in random order. To control for prosody and memory effect, context condition than in the no incongruity context condition
stimuli were presented on a sheet of paper. Participants were asked to read each of (P o0.0001) and in the strong incongruity context condition
these 36 stimuli, and then answer the following question: ‘‘What does X
(P o0.0001). They also made more errors in answering questions
(the speaker) really mean?’’ to assess their speaker intent understanding. Then a
control question was asked cueing on relevant contextual information needed to in the strong incongruity context condition than in the no
answer previous questions (e.g. Did Christine receive 100% on her mathematics incongruity context condition (P o0.0001). In the HC group, there
exam?). To prevent answers to the control question at chance level, half of the was no difference between the strong incongruity and no incon-
correct answers was ‘‘yes’’ while the other half was ‘‘no’’. Answers to each gruity contexts (P 40.05). However, like SZ patients, HC partici-
question were scored 1 if correct (for a maximum score of 12 per type of context
for each question).
pants made more errors in answering questions on speaker intent
All participants were tested individually over one session in a quiet room. in the weak incongruity context condition than in the no incon-
gruity context condition (Po0.0001) and the strong incongruity
context condition (Po0.0001) (cf. Table 2).
2.3. Data analysis The 2  3 repeated-measures ANOVA on the percentage of
correct responses to the control question on contextual informa-
Unpaired t-tests were used to explore group differences on the different tion revealed a main effect of the type of context (F(2,58)¼5.6,
neuropsychological variables. To determine group differences on the irony task a Po0.005) and a main effect of group (F(1,59)¼14.85, Po0.0001),
two group (SZ, HC)  3 context (no incongruity, strong incongruity, weak incon- showing that SZ patients made more errors than HC participants.
gruity) repeated-measures ANOVA was performed on the percentage of correct
However, the group  context interaction was not significant
responses to the different types of question: open question on speaker ironic
intent and control question. A Spearman’s correlation analysis was conducted on (F(2, 112)¼0.16, P40.05) (cf. Table 2).
the computed percentage of responses of the SZ group to examine the relationship To sum up, these results showed that participants with
between ToM and symptoms and between ToM and neuropsychological measures. schizophrenia made more errors in answering questions on
To determine the impact of flexibility on ToM ability, a final set of analyses was speaker ironic intent in the strong and weak incongruity context
performed by sub-dividing individuals in the SZ group according to their
performances on the WCST. A K-means cluster analysis was used to classify the
conditions while they performed like HC participants in the no
SZ data into relatively unimpaired (SZ-U) versus relatively impaired (SZ-I) on the incongruity context condition. As predicted, HC participants made
WCST. The alpha level was set at P o 0.05 for all the analyses. the same number of errors in the strong incongruity and no

Please cite this article as: Champagne-Lavau, M., et al., Theory of mind and context processing in schizophrenia: The role of
cognitive flexibility. Psychiatry Research (2012), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.011
4 M. Champagne-Lavau et al. / Psychiatry Research ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]]

Table 2
Percentage of correct responses to the question on ironic speaker intent and to the control question in schizophrenia and healthy control participants.

Schizophrenia Healthy control

Mean (raw score) SD Mean (%) SD Mean (raw score) SD Mean (%) SD

Question on ironic intent


Strong incongruity context condition 9.2 3.3 76.7 27.5 11.3 2.2 97.5 5.8
Weak incongruity context condition 7.2 3.2 60 26.7 9.8 2.4 84.2 13.3
No incongruity context condition 11.6 0.8 95.8 6.7 11.5 2.2 99.2 2.5
Control question
Strong incongruity context condition 10.8 1.3 89.2 10.8 11.1 2.2 95.8 5.8
Weak incongruity context condition 10.7 1.4 89.2 11.7 11.2 2.2 96.7 5.0
No incongruity context condition 11.2 1.3 92.5 10.0 11.5 2.2 99.2 1.7

incongruity context conditions, in contrast to participants with Table 3


schizophrenia. In addition, while SZ participants made more Correlation analyses in schizophrenia group.
errors than HC participants in answering control questions on
Strong incongruity Weak incongruity No incongruity
contextual information, the same patterns of performance were
context condition context condition context
found for the two groups, according to the type of context (no condition
incongruity context, strong incongruity context, weak incongruity
context). Trail B–Trail A r¼  0.129 r¼ 0.160 r¼  0.102
(time) P¼ 0.482 P¼ 0.382 P¼ 0.580
WCST r¼ 0.433 r¼ 0.156 r¼ 0.191
3.3. Association of ToM performances with clinical symptoms
(categories) P¼ 0.017 P¼ 0.410 P¼ 0.312
WCST (% r¼  0.483 r¼  0.292 r¼  0.240
Examination of results from correlation analyses involving perseverative P¼ 0.008 P¼ 0.124 P¼ 0.210
ToM and patient symptoms measures revealed no significant errors)
Digit span r¼  0.028 r¼ 0.045 r¼ 0.208
relationship (cf. Table 3).
(forward) P¼ 0.880 P¼ 0.808 P¼ 0.253
Digit span r¼ 0.194 r¼  0.055 r¼ 0.096
3.4. Association of ToM performances with cognitive flexibility (backward) P¼ 0.288 P¼ 0.767 P¼ 0.600
in schizophrenia NART r¼ 0.240 r¼ 0.113 r¼ 0.30
P¼ 0.189 P¼ 0.535 P¼ 0.868
PANSS r¼  0.043 r¼  0.110 r¼ 0.067
Results from the analyses of the correlation between ToM
(positive) P¼ 0.820 P¼ 0.556 P¼ 0.719
performances and neuropsychological measures of cognitive PANSS r¼ 0.187 r¼ 0.094 r¼  0.138
flexibility, memory IQ and duration of illness in the SZ group (negative) P¼ 0.315 P¼ 0.616 P¼ 0.459
are reported in Table 3. They revealed that the percentage of PANSS r¼ 0.050 r¼  0.107 r¼ 0.062
correct responses to the question on speaker ironic intent in the (general) P¼ 0.791 P¼ 0.568 P¼ 0.742
Duration of r¼  0.136 r¼  0.195 r¼ 0.259
strong incongruity condition correlated significantly with perfor- illness P¼ 0.457 P¼ 0.286 P¼ 0.155
mance on the number of categories in the WCST, and the number
of perseverative errors in the WCST. No correlation was found
with memory, IQ and duration of illness. question on speaker ironic intent and to the control question.
The 3  3 ANOVA on the percentage of correct responses to
3.5. Influence of flexibility on ToM ability the question on speaker ironic intent revealed a main effect of
context (F(2, 116) ¼73.70, Po0.0001) and a main effect of group
As correlations were only found with the scores of WCST, (F(2,58)¼19.17, Po0.0001), showing that SZ-I patients made
a K-means cluster analysis was performed using the SZ partici- more errors than SZ-U patients (Po0.001) and HC participants
pants’ WCST-perseverative errors. Results of this analysis (Po 0.0001). SZ-U patients made also more errors than HC
revealed two subgroups (F(1, 30)¼85.9; Po0.0001): The SZ-I participants (Po0.018). The group x context interaction was
group contained 17 SZ participants relatively impaired on the significant (F(4, 116) ¼10.15, Po0.0001). This interaction was
WCST-perseverative errors while the SZ-U group contained 15 SZ decomposed according to group. The SZ-I group made more errors
participants relatively unimpaired on the WCST-perseverative in answering the question on speaker ironic intent in the weak
errors. incongruity context condition than in the no incongruity context
A series of t-tests showed that the two SZ subgroups were condition (Po0.0001) and in the strong incongruity context
comparable in age (t(30)¼ 0.57, P40.05), education level (t(30)¼ condition (Po0.001). They also made more errors in answering
1.23, P40.05), symptoms (PANSS (positive): (t(30)¼0.72, P40.05), questions in the strong incongruity context condition than in the
PANSS (negative): (t(30)¼0.57, P40.05), PANSS (general): (t(30)¼ no incongruity context condition (Po0.001). SZ-U and HC parti-
0.99, P40.05)); IQ (NART (t(30)¼0.20, P40.05)), Trail-Making (TMT cipants both made more errors in answering questions on speaker
B: (t(30)¼ 1.56, P40.05); TMT (B-A): (t(30)¼0. 1.47, P40.05)), intent in the weak incongruity context condition than in the no
and memory (Digit span forward (t(30)¼0.50, P40.05) and Digit incongruity context condition (respectively Po0.0001; Po0.0001)
span backward (t(27)¼0.84, P40.05)). However, they significantly and in the strong incongruity context condition (respectively
differed on the duration of illness (t(30)¼  2.36, Po0.05), and on Po0.0001; Po0.0001). For these two groups, unlike for the SZ-I
flexibility measured by the WCST (cf. Table 4 for demographic and group, no difference was found between the strong incongruity and
cognitive data). no incongruity context conditions (P40.05) (Fig. 1).
A 3  3 repeated measures ANOVA for group (SZ-U, SZ-I, As SZ-I participants had 49% of correct responses on the
HC)  context (no incongruity, strong incongruity, weak incongruity) question on speaker ironic intent in the weak incongruity context
was performed on the percentage of correct responses to the condition (cf. Fig. 1), an additional paired t-test was made to

Please cite this article as: Champagne-Lavau, M., et al., Theory of mind and context processing in schizophrenia: The role of
cognitive flexibility. Psychiatry Research (2012), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.011
M. Champagne-Lavau et al. / Psychiatry Research ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]] 5

Table 4
Demographic, clinical and neuropsychological data for the healthy control (HC) group compared to schizophrenia patients with unimpaired (SZ-U)
or impaired (SZ-I) flexibility.

SZ-I SZ-U Healthy control SZ-I vs SZ-U P-value

Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD

Age 42.6 9.7 40.7 9.1 42.6 10.6 0.595


Educational level 12.3 1.9 13.3 2.4 13.7 2.9 0.292
Gender (male/female) (11/6) (10/5) (14/15)
Duration of illness 19.9 10.3 12.4 7.3 0.025
PANSS (positive) 17.3 3.7 18.4 4.7 0.475
PANSS (negative) 19.8 4.2 20.7 5.1 0.575
PANSS (general) 37.2 6.6 39.4 6.0 0.330
NART 44.7 11.1 45.4 8.1 42.7 7.9 0.829
Trail B (completion time) 130.8 50.2 103.1 50.2 58.9 18.9 0.051
Trail B – Trail A (time) 84.5 32.6 63.5 46.2 29.5 15.4 0.060
WCST (categories) 2.5 1.8 4.7 1.8 4.8 1.5 o0.001
WCST (% perseverative errors) 64.0 14.4 20.3 11.8 19.1 19.8 o0.0001
Digit span (forward) 9.6 1.5 10.0 2.8 11.0 1.9 0.594
Digit span (backward) 4.5 1.8 5.1 1.7 6.5 2.3 0.465

100.0
Percentage of correct responses

90.0
80.0
70.0
60.0 Strong incongruity
50.0 Weak incongruity
40.0 No incongruity
30.0
20.0
10.0
0.0
SZ-U SZ-I HC
Type of group

Fig. 1. Percentage of correct responses to the question on ironic speaker intent for the healthy control (HC) group compared to schizophrenia patients with unimpaired
(SZ-U) and impaired (SZ-I) flexibility. SZ-U: SZ participants unimpaired on WCST assessment; SZ-I: SZ participants impaired on WCST assessment; HC: healthy control
participants.

100.0
90.0
Percentage of correct responses

80.0
70.0
60.0
50.0 Strong incongruity
40.0 Weak incongruity
30.0 No incongruity
20.0
10.0
0.0
SZ-U SZ-I HC
Type of group

Fig. 2. Percentage of correct responses to the control question for the healthy control (HC) group compared to schizophrenia patients with unimpaired (SZ-U) and
impaired (SZ-I) flexibility. SZ-U: SZ participants unimpaired on WCST assessment; SZ-I: SZ participants impaired on WCST assessment; HC: healthy control participants.

assess whether their performance in this condition differed from 5.92, Po0.004) and a main effect of group (F(2,58)¼7.76, Po0.001),
random choice. The results pointed out that SZ-I participants showing that HC participants made fewer errors than SZ-I patients
answered at random (t(14)¼ 0.161, P ¼0.875) when the target (Po0.001) and SZ-U patients (Po0.02). The group  context inter-
sentence was preceded by context with a weak incongruity. action was not significant (F(4, 116)¼1.64, P¼0.17) (Fig. 2).
The 3  3 ANOVA on the percentage of correct responses to the To examine the relative contribution of flexibility on ToM
control question revealed a main effect of context (F(2, 116) ¼ ability in schizophrenia, a linear regression analysis was also

Please cite this article as: Champagne-Lavau, M., et al., Theory of mind and context processing in schizophrenia: The role of
cognitive flexibility. Psychiatry Research (2012), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.011
6 M. Champagne-Lavau et al. / Psychiatry Research ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]]

conducted with the score obtained in the strong incongruity others. These expected results concurred with previous findings
context condition as the dependant variable and the SZ subgroup in healthy individuals, showing that the greater the incongruity
variable (SZ-I versus SZ-U) as the independent variable. The score between context and target utterance, the easier it was to
obtained in the strong incongruity context condition was used as understand ironic intent (Ivanko and Pexman, 2003). In contrast
ToM performance since correlation was only found between this to these groups, SZ participants with a lack of flexibility (SZ-I)
condition and the WCST data in the participants with schizo- showed a difference in performance between the strong incon-
phrenia (cf. Table 3). The results showed that the subgroup gruity and no incongruity conditions, meaning that they are not
variable was a significant predictor of the patients’ performance able to infer ironic intent about the speaker of the story. Although
on the ToM task (F(1, 31)¼17.595; Po0.0001). The R2 was 0.378 SZ-I participants had difficulties attributing ironic intent to the
(with an adjusted R2 ¼ 0.356). This indicated that the subgroup speaker, they seemed to be sensitive to changes in contextual
variable can account for 37.8% of the variance of the score information, as evident by their performance in answering the
obtained in the strong incongruity condition. control questions. They generally made more errors than HC
To sum up, these last results showed that the SZ-U group participants in answering control questions on contextual infor-
exhibited the same pattern of performances as the HC group mation but this percentage of errors was actually low (less than
across the different context conditions for the question on 9%). They also made more errors in the weak incongruity context
speaker ironic intent. In other words, these two groups made condition than in the strong incongruity context condition.
the same number of errors in the strong incongruity context In other words, the weaker the degree of incongruity between
condition as in the no incongruity context condition. By contrast, context and target utterance, the more errors they made, as HC
SZ-I patients made more errors in answering questions on speaker and SZ-U participants did.
ironic intent in the strong and weak incongruity context condi- Such sensitivity to contextual information agreed with the
tions while they performed like HC participants and SZ-U patients results of Titone et al. (2002) showing that SZ participants are
in the no incongruity context condition. In addition, concerning able to decode contextual information and to use context to
demographic and cognitive data, the difference between the SZ-U facilitate contextually relevant representation. However, this
group and the SZ-I group concerned flexibility and duration of sensitivity to the degree of incongruity between context and
illness. However, no correlation was found between ToM perfor- target utterance of the story did not help SZ-I participants to
mances and duration of illness in the SZ group. understand the ironic intent of the protagonist of the story. The
degree of incongruity did not seem to cue SZ-I participants’
understanding of speaker intent since they made more errors in
4. Discussion understanding irony in the strong incongruity context condition
than in the no incongruity context condition. Thus, even if they
The present study sought to identify whether cognitive flex- perceived and understood that there was an incongruity between
ibility and context processing may impact ToM ability in patients contextual information and speaker utterance, SZ-I participants
with schizophrenia. We used a psycholinguistic paradigm show- wrongly inferred intention of the speaker in the story. They
ing that the degree of contextual incongruity influences the tended to interpret ironic utterances as errors or lies, attributing
extent to which ironic intent is understood. a wrong intention to the speaker. These data are consistent with
In general, we noted that SZ participants performed worse those of Uhlhaas et al. (2006) and Green et al. (2007), showing
than HC participants on the task assessing their ToM ability, inefficient integration of social contextual information in schizo-
committing more errors than HC participants in the strong phrenia (Green et al., 2008). In the framework of the model
incongruity and weak incongruity context conditions. In contrast proposed by Abu-Akel and Shamay-Tsoory (2011), the results
to HC participants, they also made more errors in the strong would be interpreted as SZ-I participants having difficulty to
incongruity context condition than in the no incongruity context update their mental states when new information is added, and
condition. Nevertheless, SZ participants were able to correctly SZ-U participants being able to represent and update adequately
infer the ironic intent in 76.6% of the cases. This ToM difficulty is their mental states.
consistent with previous research studies using paradigms invol-
ving irony or sarcasm understanding (Leitman et al., 2006; 4.2. Influence of flexibility on ToM ability
Kosmidis et al., 2008; Kern et al., 2009; Sparks et al., 2010).
Interestingly, extended analyses revealed that only a subgroup of In the present study, the association between ToM impairment
SZ participants (SZ-I) with a lack of cognitive flexibility differed and lack of flexibility assessed by the WCST seemed to differ-
significantly from HC participants in their ToM abilities. Such entiate the two SZ subgroups, SZ-I patients showing a lack of
result is consistent with recent findings showing the existence of flexibility being those who exhibited a ToM impairment. This
SZ subgroups comprising patients with either ‘‘poor’’ or ‘‘fair’’ result does not support those of Brüne et al. (2011), who used a
mentalizing ability (McCabe et al., 2004; Champagne-Lavau et al., linear regression analysis to find that the number of WCST
2009; Brüne and Schaub, 2011). In our study, neither age, perseveration errors could not predict mental state attribution
education, IQ, memory deficits or symptoms of SZ participants in schizophrenia in contrast to verbal IQ and executive planning
made it possible to account for the difference in patterns of skills. But, interestingly, they found that both ToM impairment
performance between the SZ-I and SZ-U subgroups. However, the measured by the MSAT-T (Brüne’s mental state sequencing task)
association between ToM impairments and lack of flexibility and lack of flexibility measured by the number of WCST perse-
seemed to differentiate these two SZ subgroups. verative errors were significant predictors of the odds of being a
patient with schizophrenia. Like Mo et al. (2008), we found that
4.1. Influence of contextual information processing on ToM ability comprehension of irony is relatively independent of IQ.
The present research also agrees with a body of studies
In both HC and SZ participants without a lack of flexibility showing a correlation between ToM performances and either
(SZ-U), strong incongruity condition cues speaker ironic intent in WCST or Trail-making task performances in schizophrenia (Greig
the sense that no difference was found between the strong et al., 2004; Bora et al., 2006; Van Hooren et al., 2008; Cham-
incongruity and the no incongruity conditions. This means that pagne-Lavau et al., 2010; Brüne et al., 2011). In the study by Van
some SZ patients are able to use contextual information to infer Hooren et al. (2008) for example, the results from the factor

Please cite this article as: Champagne-Lavau, M., et al., Theory of mind and context processing in schizophrenia: The role of
cognitive flexibility. Psychiatry Research (2012), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.011
M. Champagne-Lavau et al. / Psychiatry Research ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]] 7

analyses indicate a strong relationship between the Hinting task tapping on their ability to shift back and forth from two different
and the neurocognition measures including cognitive flexibility, viewpoints of the same event (Frith and Vignemont, 2005). While
while other measures of social cognition (i.e. jumping to conclu- some studies did not report a ToM impairment in Asperger’s
sions, source monitoring and attribution style) loaded on others syndrome (Bowler, 1992 for example), Frith and Vignemont
factors. Taken together, these results support the hypothesis (2005) underlined that the presence or not of ToM impairment
according to which adopting someone else’s point of view, taking in this population depends on the task used to assess this ability.
into account his/her perspective requires cognitive flexibility to For instance, individuals with Asperger’s syndrome have been
understand the state of mind of another person (Ruby and Decety, shown to success in Sally-Anne task and Strange Stories test
2003). According to Russell (1996), thinking flexibly about alter- while they were unable to attribute mental states spontaneously
native perspectives is at the very heart of one’s ability to attribute (Senju et al., 2009).
mental states to others. Indeed, mentalizing would require a Following the distinction proposed by Abu-Akel and Shamay-
distinct and effortful representation of others’ knowledge, beliefs, Tsoory (2011), between representation/updating and attribution
and intentions. Research by Keysar et al. (2000) and Epley et al. of mental states, ToM impairments in patients with schizophrenia
(2004) suggests that people adopt an egocentric perspective might reflect either difficulty to represent and/or update mental
relying on their privileged ground (set of information that is states according to the context, or problem to flexibly adopt
known to the self but that is not known to the speaker) to make perspective of the others to attribute mental states. According to
judgments about others. They adjust away from this egocentric this hypothesis, SZ patients tested by McCabe et al. (2004) would
starting point, and this adjustment – cognitively effortful – not have difficulty to attribute mental states to others, but rather
requires executive functions. This is consistent with the growing problems to update their mental states in some situations such as
body of evidence and arguments from developmental studies situation of disagreement, for example. More appropriate studies
suggesting that executive functions are an integral part of are required to assess whether patients with schizophrenia are
children’s mindreading competences (Apperly, 2011). impaired in representing/updating mental states and/or attribut-
ing mental states to others.
4.3. Symptoms An important consequence of our results concerns cognitive
remediation since ToM ability could have an impact on social
Brüne and Schaub (2011), who did not find any difference on functioning in schizophrenia (Nuechterlein et al., 2004; McGurk
WCST performances between poor and fair mentalizers in their et al., 2007). Integration of contextual information and/or cogni-
sample study, suggested that conceptual disorganization rather tive flexibility seems to be a good target for cognitive remediation
than cognitive flexibility or planification may differentiate ‘‘poor’’ aiming to increase social cognition ability, and as a consequence,
from ‘‘fair’’ mentalizers. However, the difficulties encountered by social functioning (Lecardeur et al., 2009, 2010).
the patients in our sample (SZ-I) were not linked with sympto- The present study contains potential shortcomings that need
matology since no correlation was found between performance to be mentioned. The ToM task we used assessed the attribution
on the irony understanding task and clinical symptoms in SZ of mental states in an off-line way since participants were not
participants. Data from the present study did not show that taking part in the interaction but made explicit use of mentalizing
ToM deficits assessed by irony comprehension were associated to answer questions on the speaker ironic intent (in the story).
with disorganized symptoms, as suggested by various authors This way to assess ToM puts more weight on meta-cognitive
(Sarfati and Hardy-Bayle, 1999; Schenkel et al., 2005; Uhlhaas processes (Frith, 2004). Therefore, the relationship found with a
et al., 2006). Schenkel et al. (2005), for example, demonstrated lack of flexibility might be due to such cognitive overload.
correlations between disorganization and ToM performances, Unfortunately, except for our study, none of the studies investi-
but their regression analysis disclosed that disorganization did gating ToM ability with a task assessing irony or sarcasm under-
not contribute to ToM performances. The present study was standing has tested for cognitive flexibility to confirm such
concordant with some experiments that found no correlation hypothesis. To overcome such possible cognitive overload due
between ToM deficits and symptoms (Marjoram et al., 2005; to the task itself and to confirm the relationship between
Leitman et al., 2006; Sprong et al., 2007). Actually, the association cognitive flexibility and ToM ability in schizophrenia, it would
between ToM deficits and clinical symptoms is far from clear be necessary to find such result with a task assessing ToM ability
(Woodward et al., 2009). Given the inherent heterogeneity of in a natural situation of conversation like in Champagne-Lavau
schizophrenia and the fact that the patients included were more et al. (2009).
or less stabilized, the literature reported that there were as many
studies showing an association between ToM ability and clinical
symptoms as investigations discerning no association (Harrington Acknowledgments
et al., 2005; Lee et al., 2004).
To sum up, the present study provides a meaningful argument This study was supported by the Fonds de la Recherche en
that the ability to attribute intention to others varies as a function Santé du Québec (New investigator and operating grant #5793)
of cognitive flexibility in schizophrenia. First, results of the and the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of
present study point out that some participants with schizophre- Canada (Operating grant #327313-06) to MCL. Further, we would
nia performed like HC participants on the ToM task while others like to thank anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on
did not. Second, participants with schizophrenia correctly per- an earlier version of this manuscript.
ceived contextual information cueing attribution of ironic intent
to the protagonist of the stimulus, but they did not always
correctly integrate this information to attribute mental states. Appendix A. Sample stimuli
Third, a lack of flexibility seems to have an impact on ToM
performances in schizophrenia. Such pattern of impairments has A.1. Stimuli with strong contextual incongruity
also been found in patients with Asperger’s syndrome who
exhibit an inability to appreciate the thoughts and feelings of Christine and Marie wrote the same math exam. Christine
others. Indeed, this inability has been linked to difficulties in both hardly studied and received 20% on the exam. The next day Marie
integrating contextual information, and to executive dysfunctions said to Chantal, ‘‘Christine is a clever student’’.

Please cite this article as: Champagne-Lavau, M., et al., Theory of mind and context processing in schizophrenia: The role of
cognitive flexibility. Psychiatry Research (2012), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.011
8 M. Champagne-Lavau et al. / Psychiatry Research ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]]

A.2. Stimuli with weak contextual incongruity Green, M.J., Uhlhaas, P.J, Coltheart, M., 2005. Context processing and social
cognition in schizophrenia. Current Psychiatry Reviews 1, 11–22.
Green, M.J., Waldron, J.H, Coltheart, M., 2007. Emotional context processing is
Christine and Marie wrote the same math exam. Christine impaired in schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 12, 259–280.
studied for 2 day and received 60% on the exam. The next day Marie Green, M.J., Waldron, J.H., Simpson, I, Coltheart, M., 2008. Visual processing of
said to Chantal, ‘‘Christine is a clever student’’. social context during mental state perception in schizophrenia. Journal of
Psychiatry & Neuroscience 33, 34–42.
Greig, T.C., Bryson, G.J, Bell, M.D., 2004. Theory of mind performance in schizo-
A.3. Stimuli with no contextual incongruity phrenia: diagnostic, symptom, and neuropsychological correlates. The Journal
of Nervous and Mental Disease 192, 12–18.
Hardy-Bayle, M.C., Sarfati, Y, Passerieux, C., 2003. The cognitive basis of disorganiza-
Christine and Marie wrote the same math exam. Christine
tion symptomatology in schizophrenia and its clinical correlates: toward a
studied for 1 day and received 90% on the exam. The next day Marie pathogenetic approach to disorganization. Schizophrenia Bulletin 29, 459–471.
says to Chantal: ‘‘Christine is a clever student’’. Harrington, L., Siegert, R.J, McClure, J., 2005. Theory of mind in schizophrenia:
a critical review. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 10, 249–286.
Heaton, R.K., 1981. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: Manual. Neuropsychological
Questions Assessment Resources, Odessa, FL.
Ivanko, S.L, Pexman, P.M., 2003. Context incongruity and irony processin.
Discourse Processes 35, 241–279.
 Open question on speaker ironic intent: What did Marie Janssen, I., Krabbendam, L., Jolles, J, van Os, J., 2003. Alterations in theory of mind
really mean? in patients with schizophrenia and non-psychotic relatives. Acta Psychiatrica
 Control question on contextual information: Did Christine Scandinavica 108, 110–117.
Kay, S.R., Fiszbein, A., Opler, L.A., 1987. The positive and negative syndrome scale
receive 100% on her mathematics exam?
(PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 13, 261–276.
Kern, R.S., Green, M.F., Fiske, A.P., Kee, K.S., Lee, J., Sergi, M.J., Horan, W.P., Subotnik,
K.L., Sugar, C.A, Nuechterlein, K.H., 2009. Theory of mind deficits for processing
counterfactual information in persons with chronic schizophrenia. Psycholo-
References gical Medicine 39, 645–654.
Keysar, B., Barr, D.J., Balin, J.A, Brauner, J.S., 2000. Taking perspective in conversa-
Abu-Akel, A., Shamay-Tsoory, S., 2011. Neuroanatomical and neurochemical bases tion: the role of mutual knowledge in comprehension. Psychological Science
of theory of mind. Neuropsychologia 49, 2971–2984. 11, 32–38.
Apperly, I., 2011. Mindreaders: the Cognitive Basis of ‘‘Theory of Mind’’. Psychology Kosmidis, M.H., Aretouli, E., Bozikas, V.P., Giannakou, M, Ioannidis, P., 2008.
Press, Sussex. Studying social cognition in patients with schizophrenia and patients with
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A.M, Frith, U., 1985. Does the autistic child have a ‘‘theory frontotemporal dementia: theory of mind and the perception of sarcasm.
of mind’’? Cognition 21, 37–46. Behavioural Neurology 19, 65–69.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y, Plumb, I., 2001. The ‘‘Reading the Kovacs, I., Kozma, P., Feher, A.Benedek, G., 1999. Late maturation of visual spatial
Mind in the Eyes’’ test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults integration in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science 96,
with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology 12204-12209.
and Psychiatry, and allied disciplines 42, 241–251. Langdon, R., Corner, T., McLaren, J., Ward, P.B, Coltheart, M., 2006. Externalizing
Bora, E., Eryavuz, A., Kayahan, B., Sungu, G, Veznedaroglu, B., 2006. Social and personalizing biases in persecutory delusions: the relationship with poor
functioning, theory of mind and neurocognition in outpatients with schizo- insight and theory-of-mind. Behaviour Research and Therapy 44, 699–713.
phrenia; mental state decoding may be a better predictor of social functioning Lecardeur, L., Stip, E, Champagne-Lavau, M., 2010. Cognitive remediation therapy
than mental state reasoning. Psychiatry Research 145, 95–103. of social cognition in schizophrenia: a critical review. Current Psychiatry
Bora, E., Sehitoglu, G., Aslier, M., Atabay, I, Veznedaroglu, B., 2007. Theory of mind Reviews 6, 280–287.
and unawareness of illness in schizophrenia: is poor insight a mentalizing Lecardeur, L., Stip, E., Giguere, M., Blouin, G., Rodriguez, J.P, Champagne-Lavau, M.,
deficit? European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 257, 2009. Effects of cognitive remediation therapies on psychotic symptoms and
104–111. cognitive complaints in patients with schizophrenia and related disorders: a
Bora, E., Murat, Y., Pantelis, C., 2009. Theory of mind impairment in schizophrenia: randomized study. Schizophrenia Research 111, 153–158.
meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research 109, 1–9. Lee, K.H., Farrow, T.F., Spence, S.A, Woodruff, P.W., 2004. Social cognition, brain
Bowler, D.M., 1992. Theory of mind in Aspeger’s syndrome. Journal of Child networks and schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 34, 391–400.
Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 33, 877–893. Leitman, D.I., Ziwich, R., Pasternak, R, Javitt, D.C., 2006. Theory of Mind (ToM) and
Brüne, M., 2005b. Emotion recognition, ‘theory of mind,’ and social behavior in counterfactuality deficits in schizophrenia: misperception or misinterpreta-
schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 133, 135–147. tion? Psychological Medicine 36, 1075–1083.
Brüne, M., 2005a. ‘‘Theory of mind’’ in schizophrenia: a review of the literature. Marjoram, D., Tansley, H., Miller, P., MacIntyre, D., Owens, D.G., Johnstone, E.C.,
Schizophrenia Bulletin 31, 21–42. Lawrie, S., 2005. A theory of mind investigation into the appreciation of visual
Brüne, M., Abdel-Hamid, M., Lehmkamper, C, Sonntag, C., 2007. Mental state jokes in schizophrenia. BMC Psychiatry 5, 12.
attribution, neurocognitive functioning, and psychopathology: what predicts Mazza, M., De Risio, A., Surian, L., Roncone, R, Casacchia, M., 2001. Selective
poor social competence in schizophrenia best? Schizophrenia Research 92, impairments of theory of mind in people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia
151–159. Research 47, 299–308.
Brüne, M., Schaub, D., 2011. Mental state attribution in schizophrenia: what McCabe, R., Leudar, I, Antaki, C., 2004. Do people with schizophrenia display
distinguishes patients with ‘‘poor’’ from patients with ‘‘fair’’ mentalising theory of mind deficits in clinical interactions? Psychological Medicine 34,
skills? European Psychiatry: The Journal of the Association of European 401–412.
Psychiatrists. McGurk, S.R., Twamley, E.W., Sitzer, D.I., McHugo, G.J, Mueser, K.T., 2007. A meta-
Brüne, M., Schaub, D., Juckel, G, Langdon, R., 2011. Social skills and behavioral analysis of cognitive remediation in schizophrenia. American Journal of
problems in schizophrenia: the role of mental state attribution, neurocogni- Psychiatry 164, 1791–1802.
tion and clinical symptomatology. Psychiatry Research 190, 9–17. Mo, S., Su, Y., Chan, R.C.K., Liu, J., 2008. Comprehension of metaphor and irony in
Bryant, G.A, Fox Tree, J.E., 2002. Recognizing verbal irony in spontaneous speech. schizophrenia during remission: The role of theory of mind and IQ. Psychiatry
Metaphor and Symbol 17, 99–117. Research 157, 21–29.
Burgess, P.W, Shallice, T., 1996. Bizarre responses, rule detection and frontal lobe Nelson, H.E., 1982. National Adult Reading Test (NART). Test Manuel. National
lesions. Cortex 32, 241–259. Foundation for Educational Research-Nelson, Windsor.
Champagne-Lavau, M., Fossard, M., Martel, G., Chapdelaine, C., Blouin, G., Nuechterlein, K.H., Barch, D.M., Gold, J.M., Goldberg, T.E., Green, M.F, Heaton, R.K.,
Rodriguez, J.P, Stip, E., 2009. Do patients with schizophrenia attribute mental 2004. Identification of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia. Schizo-
states in a referential communication task? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 14, phrenia Research 72, 29–39.
217–239. Pickup, G.J., 2008. Relationship between Theory of Mind and executive function in
Champagne-Lavau, M, Stip, E., 2010. Pragmatic and executive dysfunction in schizophrenia: a systematic review. Psychopathology 41, 206–213.
schizophrenia. Journal of Neurolinguistics 23, 285–296. Pickup, G.J, Frith, C.D., 2001. Theory of mind impairments in schizophrenia: sympto-
Cohen, J.D., Servan-Schreiber, D., 1992. Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connec- matology, severity and specificity. Psychological Medicine 31, 207–220.
tionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. Psychological Pinkham, A.E., Penn, D.L., 2006. Neurocognitive and social cognitive predictors of
Review 99, 45–77. interpersonal skill in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 143, 167–178.
Corcoran, R., Mercer, G, Frith, C.D., 1995. Schizophrenia, symptomatology and Premack, D, Woodruff, G., 1978. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? The
social inference: investigating ‘‘theory of mind’’ in people with schizophrenia. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4, 515–526.
Schizophrenia Research 17, 5–13. Reitan, R.M., Wolfson, D., 1993. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test
Frith, C.D., 2004. Schizophrenia and theory of mind. Psychological Medicine 34, Battery. Neuropsychology Press, Tucson.
385–389. Ruby, P, Decety, J., 2003. What you believe versus what you think they believe:
Frith, U., Vignemont, F., 2005. Egocentrism, allocentrism, and Asperger syndrome. a neuroimaging study of conceptual perspective-taking. European Journal of
Consciousness and Cognition 14, 719–738. Neuroscience 17, 2475–2480.

Please cite this article as: Champagne-Lavau, M., et al., Theory of mind and context processing in schizophrenia: The role of
cognitive flexibility. Psychiatry Research (2012), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.011
M. Champagne-Lavau et al. / Psychiatry Research ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]] 9

Russell, J., 1996. Agency: Its Role in Mental Development. Erlbaum, Hove. Stratta, P., Daneluzzo, E., Bustini, M., Prosperini, P.L., Rossi, A., 1999. Schizophrenic
Sarfati, Y, Hardy-Bayle, M.C., 1999. How do people with schizophrenia explain the patients use context-independent reasoning more often than context-depen-
behaviour of others? A study of theory of mind and its relationship to thought dent reasoning as measured by the cognitive bias task (CBT): a controlled
and speech disorganization in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 29, study. Schizophrenia Research 37, 45–51.
613–620. Titone, D., Holzman, P.S, Levy, D.L., 2002. Idiom processing in schizophrenia: literal
Schenkel, L.S., Spaulding, W.D, Silverstein, S.M., 2005. Poor premorbid social implausibility saves the day for idiom priming. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
functioning and theory of mind deficit in schizophrenia: evidence of reduced ogy 111, 313–320.
context processing? Journal of Psychiatry Research 39, 499–508. Uhlhaas, P.J., Phillips, W.A., Schenkel, L.S, Silverstein, S.M., 2006. Theory of mind
Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., Frith, U., 2009. Mindblind eyes: an absence of and perceptual context-processing in schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry
spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science 325, 883–885. 11, 416–436.
Sparks, A., McDonald, S., Lino, B., O’Donnelle, M, Green, M.J., 2010. Social cognition, Van Hooren, S., Vermissen, D., Janssen, I., Myin-Germeys, I., Campo, J., Mengelers,
empathy and functional outcome in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research R., Vans Os, J., Krabbendam, L., 2008. Social cognition and neurocognition as
122, 172–178. independant domains in psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 103, 257–265.
Sperber, D., Wilson, D., 1986. Relevance: Communication and Cognition. Blackwell, Wechsler, D., 1981. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised. The Psychological
Oxford. Corporation, San Antonio, TX.
Sprong, M., Schothorst, P., Vos, E., Hox, J, van Engeland, H., 2007. Theory of mind in Woodward, T.S., Mizrahi, R., Menon, M, Christensen, B.K., 2009. Correspondences
schizophrenia: meta-analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry: the journal of between theory of mind, jumping to conclusions, neuropsychological mea-
mental science 191, 5–13. sures and the symptoms of schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 170, 119–123.

Please cite this article as: Champagne-Lavau, M., et al., Theory of mind and context processing in schizophrenia: The role of
cognitive flexibility. Psychiatry Research (2012), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.011

You might also like