You are on page 1of 9

ASSESSMENT Bornstein

10.1177/1073191102238195
et al. / CONSTRUCT VALIDITY OF THE RPT

Construct Validity of the


Relationship Profile Test
Links With Attachment, Identity,
Relatedness, and Affect

Robert F. Bornstein
Gettysburg College

Kimberly J. Geiselman
Towson State University

Elizabeth A. Eisenhart
Bryn Mawr College

Mary A. Languirand
Gettysburg College

Studies suggest that overdependence and detachment have negative effects on psychological
adjustment, health, and therapy process and outcome. In contrast, healthy dependency (i.e.,
flexible, situation-appropriate help and support seeking) has beneficial effects in each of
these areas. In this investigation, 90 college students (50 women and 40 men) completed a
battery of personality scales including the Relationship Profile Test (RPT), a 30-item mea-
sure of destructive overdependence, dysfunctional detachment, and healthy dependency.
RPT scores showed the expected patterns of subscale intercorrelations, gender differences,
and links with measures of attachment style, identity, relatedness, and affect. Implications of
these results for the construct validity of the RPT are discussed in the context of theoretical
models of dependency-detachment.

Keywords: healthy dependency, overdependence, detachment, personality assessment, clini-


cal assessment

In recent years, researchers have delineated the under- ment (DD)—an inability to cultivate social ties and engage
lying structure of interpersonal dependency and examined in adaptive affiliative behaviors (Kantor, 1993; Millon,
links between dependency and a variety of other traits 1996). Moreover, researchers have found it useful, both
(Bornstein, 1993; Pincus & Gurtman, 1995). As a result of conceptually and empirically, to distinguish destructive
these efforts, a consensus has emerged among clinicians overdependence (DO) (characterized by rigid, inflexible
that the converse of dependency is dysfunctional detach- dependency) from healthy dependency (HD) (character-

Portions of this article were presented at the 73rd annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Boston, March 8, 2002.
Copies of the Relationship Profile Test, along with scoring information, can be obtained from the first author. Correspondence regarding
this article should be sent to Robert F. Bornstein, Ph.D., Department of Psychology, Gettysburg College, Gettysburg, PA 17325; e-mail:
bbornste@gettysburg.edu.
Assessment, Volume 9, No. 4, December 2002 373-381
DOI: 10.1177/1073191102238195
© 2002 Sage Publications

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on May 22, 2015


374 ASSESSMENT

ized by flexible, situation-appropriate help and support cation to strengthen long-term supportive relationships
seeking) (see Bornstein, 1998; Pincus & Wilson, 2001). (Bornstein, 1998).
Considerable effort has been devoted to elucidating the HD and secure attachment also differ with respect to
developmental antecedents of DD and DO. Studies indi- cross-situational stability. Secure attachment is associated
cate that detachment results from an array of underlying with greater behavioral consistency than is HD, with simi-
factors, including early socialization experiences that em- lar interaction patterns exhibited in different contexts (e.g.,
phasize independence and self-sufficiency at the expense friendships, romantic relationships) (see Heiss, Berman, &
of social connectedness (Clark & Ladd, 2000; Colgan, Sperling, 1996). In contrast, HD is expressed differently in
1987), intrapsychic conflicts regarding closeness and inti- different settings, with the healthy-dependent person ex-
macy (Birtchnell, 1996), and biologically based differ- hibiting an array of relationship-specific self-presentation
ences in temperament that elicit detachment-promoting styles (Pincus & Wilson, 2001) and modifying his or her
responses from parents and peers (Coolidge, Thede, & behavior in response to situational cues and external de-
Jang, 2001). In contrast, overdependence stems primarily mands (Bornstein & Languirand, in press).
from a sustained pattern of overprotective and/or authori- Perhaps most important from a psychometric stand-
tarian parenting (Head, Baker, & Williamson, 1991), al- point, studies confirm that HD and secure attachment are
though certain infantile temperament variables (e.g., low empirically as well as conceptually distinct (Klohnen &
soothability) may play a role in eliciting these parenting John, 1998). Healthy-dependency scores are only mod-
styles (Bornstein, 1993; Pincus & Gurtman, 1995). estly related to scores on measures of secure attachment,
Studies have also documented the early childhood fac- with correlations in the r = .30 to .40 range (Bornstein
tors that lead to HD later in life. HD is rooted in (a) a his- et al., 2001, 2002; Pincus & Wilson, 2001). Thus, secure
tory of authoritative parenting, which instills in the child a attachment scores account for—at most—approximately
sense of confidence and self-directedness (Cross & 16% of the variance in healthy-dependency scores.
Madson, 1997; Lee & Robbins, 1995), and (b) consistent Bornstein et al. (2001, 2002) obtained preliminary data
messages from parents and other authority figures that it is regarding the construct validity of a newly developed self-
acceptable to ask for support when needed (Clark & Ladd, report scale—the Relationship Profile Test (RPT)—that
2000; Lang-Takac & Osterweil, 1992). As a result, the was designed to assess detachment, overdependence, and
child learns that looking to others for help is not a sign of HD within the same instrument. The RPT yields three
weakness or failure. Studies suggest that only when au- subscale scores: DO, DD, and HD. In Bornstein et al.’s in-
thoritative parenting is coupled with active encourage- vestigations, all three RPT subscales showed the expected
ment of situation-appropriate help seeking is an individual patterns of intercorrelations and gender differences, ac-
likely to develop a healthy-dependent personality style ceptable internal consistency, and good retest reliability
(Kobayashi, 1989; Singelis, 1994; Wang, Bristol, Mowen, & over a 23-week intertest interval. In addition, the three
Chakraborty, 2000). RPT subscale scores showed theoretically predicted rela-
Although overdependence and detachment have been tionships with scores on measures of need for approval
studied for decades, the concept of HD—and its assess- (Crowne & Marlowe, 1964), life satisfaction (Diener,
ment in research and clinical settings—is relatively new. Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985), alexithymia (Taylor,
Although HD overlaps to some degree with connectedness Bagby, & Parker, 1992), psychological separateness/isola-
(Clark & Ladd, 2000), mature dependency (Baumeister & tion (Wang & Mowen, 1997), and relational-interdepen-
Leary, 1995), and mature object relations (Sundin, dent self-construal (Cross, Bacon, & Morris, 2000).
Armelius, & Nilsson, 1994), perusal of the empirical liter- Continued attention to the construct validity of the RPT
ature confirms that HD differs from these neighboring is important for several reasons. First, studies have shown
constructs in several ways (Bornstein, Languirand, West, that overdependence and detachment both have negative
Creighton, & Geiselman, 2001, 2002). For example, al- effects on psychotherapy process and outcome.
though HD is similar in certain respects to secure attach- Overdependent patients tend to overuse mental health ser-
ment, these constructs differ with respect to underlying vices, have difficulty terminating psychotherapy, and ob-
process: Whereas secure attachment is rooted in internal tain a significantly higher number of psychotropic
working models of self-other interactions that lead to posi- medication prescriptions than do nondependent patients
tive expectations regarding relationship outcomes (Main, with similar diagnostic profiles (Ekselius, Lindstrom, von
Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985), HD is rooted in a set of motives Knorring, Bodlund, & Kullgren, 1994; O’Neill &
and affective responses that enable the person to seek help Bornstein, 2001). Detached patients, on the other hand,
from others without feeling helpless, moderate affiliative delay seeking treatment after symptoms appear, terminate
urges through internal means, and delay short-term gratifi- therapy prematurely, and show only modest benefit in in-

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on May 22, 2015


Bornstein et al. / CONSTRUCT VALIDITY OF THE RPT 375

sight-oriented treatment relative to nondetached patients style. Several sets of hypotheses were tested, all of which
(Kantor, 1993; Sperry, 1995). were derived from extant research on overdependence, de-
In contrast to overdependent and detached patients, evi- tachment, and HD (Baltes, 1996; Birtchnell, 1987;
dence suggests that healthy-dependent patients may be Bornstein, 1993; Colgan, 1987; Cross et al., 2000; Kantor,
particularly good candidates for insight-oriented therapy. 1993; Pincus & Wilson, 2001; Rude & Burnham, 1995).
Several qualities associated with HD (e.g., stable self-con- All statistically significant RPT-comparison measure cor-
cept, relationship flexibility, adaptive help seeking) have relations were expected to be moderate in magnitude (i.e.,
been shown to predict positive outcome in insight-oriented in the .25-.50 range) and comparable in women and men.
treatment (Blatt & Ford, 1994; Summers, 1999). More-
Attachment style (AAS). DO scores should be positively
over, successful insight-oriented psychotherapy is often
correlated with scores on the AAS Anxiety Scale and neg-
characterized by a shift from overdependent and/or de-
atively correlated with scores on the Close Scale. In con-
tached behavior to healthy-dependent functioning
trast, DD scores should be negatively correlated with
(Hoglend et al., 2000; Hoglend & Piper, 1997).
scores on both the Depend and Close subscales. HD scores
Parallel findings have emerged in the health arena.
should be positively correlated with scores on the Depend
Overdependence is associated with increased risk for ill-
and Close subscales but negatively correlated with scores
ness and disease (Bornstein, 1998) because over-
on the Anxiety subscale.
dependence acts as a diathesis that—when coupled with a
high level of interpersonal stress—compromises immune Identity (IASC Identity Impairment [II] and Suscepti-
function (Blatt, Cornell, & Eshkol, 1993). Detachment bility to Influence [SI] Scales). DO scores should be posi-
leads to increased illness risk as well because the tively correlated with scores on the IASC II and SI Scales.
alexithymic tendencies associated with detachment pre- Like DO scores, DD scores should be positively correlated
vent the person from disclosing negative emotions with II Scale scores. However, in contrast to DD scores,
(Pennebaker, 1990, 1995). HD has the opposite effect be- DO scores should be negatively correlated with SI Scale
cause the healthy-dependent person’s combination of au- scores. HD scores should be negatively correlated with
tonomy and connectedness facilitates disclosure of scores on both the II and SI Scales.
troubling feelings, leads to more effective coping with in-
Relatedness (IASC Interpersonal Conflicts [IC],
terpersonal conflict, and sets the stage for adaptive medi-
Idealization-Disillusionment [ID], and Abandonment
cal help seeking when symptoms appear (Bornstein &
Concerns [AC] Scales). DO scores should be positively
Languirand, in press; Cross & Madson, 1997).
correlated with scores on the AC Scale, whereas DD
The purpose of the present study was to obtain addi-
scores should be positively correlated with scores on the
tional information regarding the convergent and
ID Scale (a high ID score reflects a tendency to experience
discriminant validity of the RPT by assessing links be-
rapid shifts in opinions of other people). HD scores should
tween RPT scores and scores on measures of attachment
be negatively correlated with scores on all three related-
style, identity, relatedness, and affect regulation. As-
ness scales: IC, ID, and AC.
sessing the relationships between RPT scores and scores
on these constructs is important for several reasons. First, Affect regulation (IASC Affect Dysregulation [AD] and
exploring links between RPT scores and indices of attach- Tension Reduction Activities [TRA] Scales). DO and DD
ment style will help elucidate convergences and diver- scores should be positively correlated with scores on the
gences between these constructs and place HD within an AD Scale. However, HD scores should be negatively cor-
appropriate nomological net. Second, exploring links be- related with AD scores and with scores on the TRA Scale
tween RPT scores and indices of identity and relatedness (low TRA scores indicate an ability to cope effectively
will enhance understanding of the intrapersonal and inter- with negative affect through internal means).
personal dynamics of HD. Finally, examining the relation-
RPT subscale intercorrelations. These were expected
ships between RPT scores and affect regulation scores
to follow the patterns obtained by Bornstein et al. (2001,
may help specify the underlying factors that account for
2002), who found that HD scores were inversely related to
increased illness risk in overdependent and detached
DO and DD scores, whereas DO and DD scores were or-
persons and decreased illness risk in healthy-dependent
thogonal. Continued examination of the DO-DD relation-
individuals.
ship is particularly useful because extant theoretical
Briere’s (1998) Inventory of Altered Self-Capacities
models conceptualize DO and DD as opposing personality
(IASC) was used to assess dimensions of identity, related-
styles (Birtchnell, 1987, 1996), yet Ekselius et al. (1994)
ness, and affect, whereas Collins and Read’s (1990) Adult
found that psychiatric patients who obtained high scores
Attachment Scale (AAS) was used to assess attachment
on a measure of dependent personality disorder (PD) also

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on May 22, 2015


376 ASSESSMENT

scored high on schizoid PD (r = .30) and avoidant PD (r = Birtchnell (1987, 1996), Bornstein (1993), Gurtman
.66), even though these latter disorders are thought to be (1992), Kantor (1993), Livesley (1995), Millon (1996),
detachment based (Millon, 1996). If evidence continues to Overholser (1996), Paris (1998), Pincus and Gurtman
show that DO and DD scores are orthogonal (Bornstein (1995), and Rude and Burnham (1995). In addition, the
et al., 2001, 2002) or positively correlated (Ekselius et al., Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
1994), theoretical frameworks that conceptualize depend- (4th ed.) (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) de-
ency and detachment as opposing personality styles may pendent PD, avoidant PD, and schizoid PD symptom crite-
need to be revised. ria were used to develop certain DO and DD items. Initial
validation data confirmed that all three RPT subscales
RPT subscale gender differences. Consistent with re-
showed good internal consistency and retest reliability and
sults from initial validity studies (Bornstein et al., 2001,
theoretically predicted relationships with measures of
2002), women should score higher than men on the DO
need for approval, life satisfaction, alexithymia, psycho-
and HD Scales; there should be no gender differences on
logical separateness, and relational-interdependent self-
the DD Scale. Previous investigations of clinical and col-
construal (Bornstein et al., 2001, 2002).
lege student samples have also found that women obtain
higher scores than men on measures of overdependence
Comparison Measures
and HD (Pincus & Wilson, 2001), with minimal gender
differences in detachment (Birtchnell, 1996).
Two measures were used to assess the convergent and
discriminant validity of the three RPT subscales.
The AAS (Collins & Read, 1990) is a 21-item measure
METHOD
of attachment style in adolescents and adults. Each AAS
item is a self-statement, which is rated by the participant
Participants on a 5-point scale anchored with the terms not at all char-
acteristic (1) and very characteristic (5). The AAS yields
Participants were 90 undergraduates (50 women and 40
three 7-item subscale scores: Depend (i.e., confidence in
men) enrolled in general psychology classes at Gettysburg
the dependability of others), Close (i.e., comfort with
College, who took part in the study to fulfill a course re-
closeness and intimacy), and Anxiety (i.e., concerns re-
quirement. Participants ranged in age from 17 to 21, with a
garding rejection and abandonment).
mean of 18.35 years (SD = 0.82). No participants from the
Information regarding the construct validity of the
present study had taken part in earlier RPT construct valid-
AAS is provided by Collins and Read (1990) and Brennan,
ity studies.
Clark, and Shaver (1998). AAS scores show good retest re-
liability over 8 weeks and yield theoretically predicted re-
Measures lationships with scores on measures of self-concept, self-
esteem, trust, and love style. Recent investigations confirm
The RPT that AAS scores also show the expected relationships with
scores on measures of depression and anaclitic personality
The RPT is a rationally derived questionnaire that asks traits (Whiffen, Aube, Thompson, & Campbell, 2000). An
the participant to respond to 30 self-statements, each of additional advantage of the AAS over other adult attach-
which is rated on a 7-point scale anchored by the terms not ment measures is that AAS items are worded to tap broad
at all true of me (1) and very true of me (7). The RPT yields relationship patterns rather than assessing behavior in ro-
three 10-item subscale scores: (a) DO, (b) DD, and (c) HD. mantic relationships alone (Brennan et al., 1998).
Sample items from the RPT subscales include “Being re- Despite its many strengths, the AAS also has two disad-
sponsible for things makes me nervous” (DO), “Other vantages relative to other adult attachment scales. First,
people want too much from me” (DD), and “It is easy for AAS subscales do not map well onto current four-cell
me to trust people” (HD). models of attachment and may not assess certain attach-
RPT statements were derived from the theoretical and ment patterns exhibited by adolescents and adults
empirical literature on overdependence, detachment, and (Brennan et al., 1998). Second, AAS subscale scores show
HD. Statements were written to tap the four components of relatively modest correlations with theoretically related
each personality style (i.e., cognitive, emotional, motiva- subscale scores from other adult attachment measures
tional, behavioral), as well as other core features of the di- (e.g., West & Sheldon-Keller, 1994).
mension in question. Sources used to derive RPT items The IASC (Briere, 1998) is a 63-item scale assessing
included Baille and Lampe (1998), Benjamin (1996), various dimensions of identity, relatedness, and affect reg-

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on May 22, 2015


Bornstein et al. / CONSTRUCT VALIDITY OF THE RPT 377

TABLE 1 TABLE 2
Gender Differences in RPT and Intercorrelations of RPT Subscale Scores
Comparison Measure Scores
RPT-DO RPT-DD RPT-HD
Women Men
RPT-DO — .09 –.35**
Scale Score SD Score SD t RPT-DD .25 — –.52**
RPT-HD –.24 –.18 —
RPT
DO 27.14 5.90 23.75 6.62 3.54** NOTE: N = 50 women and 40 men. Subscale intercorrelations for women
DD 29.74 6.33 30.86 5.68 1.18 are above the diagonal; those for men are below the diagonal. RPT = Re-
HD 37.77 6.02 34.52 6.04 2.27* lationship Profile Test; DO = Destructive Overdependence; DD = Dys-
AAS functional Detachment; HD = Healthy Dependency.
Depend 21.42 4.64 20.13 3.59 1.57 **p < .01.
Anxiety 13.95 4.50 14.78 4.38 0.96
Close 22.91 4.40 23.00 3.62 0.10
IASC
Procedure
II 18.31 6.64 17.10 7.21 0.92
SI 16.64 5.37 13.98 4.46 2.72**
IC 20.49 5.27 20.22 5.13 0.26 Participants completed batteries of measures in two 30-
ID 17.81 6.27 16.83 5.53 0.84 to 40-minute sessions separated by 7 to 14 days. Within
AC 18.05 6.81 16.75 6.52 1.00 each session, measures were presented in random order.
AD 18.27 6.14 17.65 6.59 0.52
TRA 13.29 3.56 13.43 4.04 0.20

NOTE: N = 50 women and 40 men. RPT = Relationship Profile Test RESULTS


(Bornstein, Languirand, West, Creighton, & Geiselman, 2001, 2002); DO
= Destructive Overdependence; DD = Dysfunctional Detachment; HD =
Healthy Dependency; AAS = Adult Attachment Scale (Collins & Read, The results of this study are summarized in Tables 1
1990); Depend = confidence in the dependability of others; Anxiety = fear through 3.
of abandonment/rejection; Close = comfort with closeness and intimacy;
IASC = Inventory of Altered Self-Capacities (Briere, 1998); II = Identity
Impairment; SI = Susceptibility to Influence; IC = Interpersonal Con- RPT and Comparison
flicts; ID = Idealization-Disillusionment; AC = Abandonment Concerns; Measure Gender Differences
AD = Affect Dysregulation; TRA = Tension Reduction Activities.
*p < .05. **p < .01.
As Table 1 shows, women obtained significantly higher
scores than men on the RPT DO and HD Scales. Although
men scored slightly higher than women on the DD Scale,
ulation. IASC items are preceded by the question, “In the this difference was not significant. When the gender dif-
last 6 months, how often have you experienced the follow- ference effect sizes obtained in this study were compared
ing?” Items describing intrapersonal and interpersonal ex- to those of Bornstein et al. (2001, 2002), using focused
periences follow (e.g., “suddenly hating someone you comparisons (Rosenthal, 1984), no significant differences
liked a lot,” “feeling like you don’t really have an identity,” between the present effect sizes and the earlier ones were
“feeling empty when people went away from you”). Each found (Zs were 0.72, 0.35, and 0.79, respectively, for the
item is rated on a 5-point scale anchored by the terms never DO, DD, and HD effect size contrasts).
(1) and very often (5). Analyses of comparison measure (AAS and IASC)
The IASC yields seven subscale scores: II, SI, IC, ID, scores revealed no significant gender differences on any
AC, AD, and TRA. Two of these subscales (II and SI) tap AAS subscale and only one gender difference on the
facets of identity, three (IC, ID, and AC) assess related- IASC: Women obtained significantly higher scores than
ness, and the remaining two (AD and TRA) assess aspects men on the SI Scale.
of affect control.
Initial construct validity studies confirmed that the RPT Subscale Intercorrelations
IASC subscales show good internal consistency in com-
munity and clinical samples and good convergent and As Table 2 shows, similar RPT subscale correlations
discriminant validity with respect to (a) archival measures were obtained in women and men. As expected, HD scores
of suicidality and child abuse history and (b) self-reports were inversely related to DO and DD scores in participants
of Axis II pathology, interpersonal problems, and defense of both genders. Although these correlations were statisti-
style (see Briere, 1998, for a detailed discussion of these cally significant in women, the HD-DO and HD-DD cor-
data). relations in men—although in the predicted direction—

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on May 22, 2015


378 ASSESSMENT

TABLE 3 All three RPT scales also showed predictable correla-


Convergent and Discriminant tions with IASC identity, relatedness, and affect regulation
Validity of the RPT Subscales indices. DO scores were associated with high levels of AC,
Comparison Measure–Subscale Correlations II, SI, and AD. DD scores were associated with high levels
RPT-DO RPT-DD RPT-HD
of ID and AD. HD scores were associated with low levels
Comparison of ID, AC, II, SI, AD, and TRA. Including predicted
Measure Predict Obtain Predict Obtain Predict Obtain nonrelationships, 18 of 21 RPT-IASC correlations (86%)
were as expected.
AAS
Depend o .07 – –.41** + .39**
Anxiety + .24* o .09 – –.29**
Close – –.25* – –.38** + .60** DISCUSSION
IASC
II + .55** + .14 – –.37** The present results illuminate the intrapersonal and in-
SI + .55** – .03 – –.26*
terpersonal dynamics of DO, DD, and HD. Our findings
IC o .02 o .17 – –.18
ID o .07 + .37** – –.31** indicate that the overdependent person experiences con-
AC + .27** o .15 – –.33** cerns about abandonment, anxiety regarding the continued
AD + .27** + .33** – –.32** availability of caregivers, problems with closeness and in-
TRA o .14 o .17 – –.23* timacy, difficulty resisting external pressure and influ-
NOTE: N = 50 women and 40 men. RPT = Relationship Profile Test; DO ence, and impairments in identity and affect regulation.
= Destructive Overdependence; DD = Dysfunctional Detachment; HD = When coupled with Bornstein et al.’s (2001, 2002) find-
Healthy Dependency; Predict = predicted RPT–comparison measure re- ings regarding the overdependent person’s lack of self-
lationship; Obtain = obtained RPT–comparison measure relationship;
AAS = Adult Attachment Scale; Depend = confidence in the dependabil- confidence and dissatisfaction with life, the picture that
ity of others; Anxiety = fear of abandonment/rejection; Close = comfort emerges is that of an anxious, insecure individual with a
with closeness and intimacy; IASC = Inventory of Altered Self-Capac- tenuous self-concept who may at times be overwhelmed
ities; II = Identity Impairment; SI = Susceptibility to Influence; IC = In-
terpersonal Conflicts; ID = Idealization-Disillusionment; AC = by negative affect.
Abandonment Concerns; AD = Affect Dysregulation; TRA = Tension Like the overdependent individual, the detached person
Reduction Activities. Predicted relationships are as follows: + = positive experiences difficulties in affect regulation and problems
correlation predicted, – = negative correlation predicted, and o = scores
expected to be uncorrelated. with closeness and intimacy. However, in contrast to the
*p < .05. **p < .01. overdependent person, the detached person sees others as
undependable and has a tendency to respond to conflict
with wholesale shifts in attitude toward the other individ-
ual. When coupled with Bornstein et al.’s (2001, 2002)
were not significant. However, focused comparisons of findings regarding the detached person’s sense of isola-
RPT interscale intercorrelation coefficients obtained in the tion, difficulty expressing feelings, and dissatisfaction
present study with those obtained by Bornstein et al. with life, the picture that emerges is of a withdrawn, almost
(2001, 2002) revealed no significant effect size differences schizoid individual with a generalized negative outlook,
between investigations (cross-study effect size compari- low expectations regarding interpersonal relationships,
son Zs ranged from 0.29 to 0.65 in women and from 0.72 to and difficulty managing troubling emotions.
1.51 in men). In many ways, the most intriguing results to emerge
from this investigation involve HD. The healthy-depend-
Convergent and Discriminant ent person sees others as dependable, is capable of experi-
Validity of RPT Subscale Scores encing genuine intimacy and closeness, has few concerns
regarding abandonment by a valued other, and is able to
As Table 3 shows, DO, DD, and HD scores exhibited maintain a consistent view of significant figures, even in
the expected patterns of intercorrelations with AAS the face of interpersonal conflict. In addition, the healthy-
subscale scores. DO scores were positively correlated with dependent person has little difficulty with affect regula-
Anxiety scores and negatively correlated with Close tion, few identity concerns, and the ability to evaluate ex-
scores. DD scores were negatively correlated with Depend ternal influence critically and resist this influence if he or
and Close scores. HD scores were positively correlated she so desires. When combined with Bornstein et al.’s
with Depend and Close scores and negatively correlated (2001, 2002) findings regarding the healthy-dependent
with Anxiety scores. Including predicted nonrelationships person’s interdependent self-construal, need for approval,
(i.e., DO-Depend and DD-Anxiety), nine of nine RPT- ease in verbalizing feelings, and satisfaction with life, the
AAS comparisons (100%) were as expected. picture that emerges is that of a secure, confident individ-

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on May 22, 2015


Bornstein et al. / CONSTRUCT VALIDITY OF THE RPT 379

ual who feels connected to other people and desires to this limitation is less problematic than it might otherwise
please others without being overly concerned regarding be because focused comparisons of effect size demon-
the possibility of relationship conflict or disruption. strated no differences in gender or subscale inter-
The positive relationships between AD scores and correlations across independent studies and samples,
overdependence and detachment scores may help explain future construct validity investigations should include
the increased illness risk associated with these two person- larger participant groups whenever possible. This will be
ality styles. Previous investigators have attributed the in- particularly important in deriving clinical and nonclinical
creased illness risk associated with overdependence to the norms for the RPT DO, DD, and HD Scales.
negative effects of relationship conflict (Bornstein, 1998) If the RPT continues to yield promising results, it may
and attributed the increased illness risk associated with de- prove to be a useful assessment tool for assessing depend-
tachment to alexithymic tendencies and an inability to ver- ency- and detachment-related traits in research and clini-
balize troubling feelings (Kantor, 1993). Although these cal settings. Studies now under way are assessing the long-
factors almost certainly play a role in observed over- term retest reliability of RPT scores and the construct va-
dependence-illness and detachment-illness links, the pres- lidity of the RPT in psychiatric inpatients and outpatients.
ent findings suggest that AD (i.e., affective lability Future investigations should also examine factors that alter
coupled with difficulty modulating dysphoric states) may overdependent, detached, and healthy-dependent behav-
also underlie increases in illness rates in overdependent iors and emotional reactions. Such studies will help re-
and detached individuals. searchers make more accurate predictions regarding trait-
Given the marked differences in affect regulation ca- related variability in responding across different situations
pacity across these three personality styles, continued ex- and settings and may also lead to improved psycho-
ploration of contrasting affective experiences in DO, DD, therapeutic interventions for effecting long-term change.
and HD is warranted. Although studies have documented
links between DD and depression (Kantor, 1993) and be-
tween DO and both depression and anxiety (Bornstein, REFERENCES
1993), no investigations have assessed milder daily affect
patterns associated with these traits. Similarly, although American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical
the present results—along with those of Bornstein et al. manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
(2001, 2002)—indicate that HD is associated with effec- Baille, A. J., & Lampe, L. A. (1998). Avoidant personality disorder: Em-
pirical support for DSM-IV revision. Journal of Personality Disor-
tive coping and overall life satisfaction, no studies have ex- ders, 12, 23-30.
plored the range of affective experiences characteristic of Baltes, M. M. (1996). The many faces of dependency in old age. Cam-
HD. bridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
In addition to providing new information regarding the Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for
interpersonal attachment as a fundamental human motivation. Psy-
intrapersonal and interpersonal dynamics of overdepen- chological Bulletin, 117, 497-529.
dence, detachment, and HD, the present findings confirm Benjamin, L. S. (1996). Interpersonal diagnosis and treatment of person-
and extend the initial RPT construct validity findings of ality disorders. New York: Guilford.
Bornstein et al. (2001, 2002). Most important, these re- Birtchnell, J. (1987). Attachment-detachment, directiveness-receptive-
ness: A system for classifying interpersonal attitudes and behavior.
sults offer additional support for the convergent and dis- British Journal of Medical Psychology, 60, 217-227.
criminant validity of the three RPT subscales. They also Birtchnell, J. (1996). Detachment. In C. G. Costello (Ed.), Personality
replicate earlier findings regarding RPT gender differ- characteristics of the personality disordered (pp. 173-205). New
ences and intersubscale relationships. DO and DD scores York: John Wiley.
Blatt, S. J., Cornell, C. E., & Eshkol, E. (1993). Personality style, differ-
were again uncorrelated, a finding that dovetails with ear- ential vulnerability, and clinical course in immunologic and cardio-
lier RPT construct validity data. In light of these results— vascular disease. Clinical Psychology Review, 13, 421-450.
and other findings documenting positive relationships be- Blatt, S. J., & Ford, R. Q. (1994). Therapeutic change. New York: Plenum.
tween dependency- and detachment-related PD scores in Bornstein, R. F. (1993). The dependent personality. New York: Guilford.
Bornstein, R. F. (1998). Depathologizing dependency. Journal of Ner-
clinical and nonclinical participants (Ekselius et al., vous and Mental Disease, 186, 67-73.
1994)—it may be time to revise extant theoretical frame- Bornstein, R. F., & Languirand, M. A. (in press). Healthy dependency.
works that conceptualize dependency and detachment as New York: Newmarket.
opposing personality styles (Birtchnell, 1987; Millon, Bornstein, R. F., Languirand, M. A., West, M. A., Creighton, J. A., &
Geiselman, K. J. (2001, April 20). Construct validity of the Relation-
1996). ship Profile Test. Paper presented at the 72nd meeting of the Eastern
One limitation of the present investigation involves its Psychological Association, Washington, D.C.
relatively modest sample size. It is possible that stronger Bornstein, R. F., Languirand, M. A., West, M. A., Creighton, J. A., &
Geiselman, K. J. (2002). Construct validity of the Relationship Pro-
results in certain domains would have been obtained if a file Test: A self-report measure of dependency-detachment. Manu-
greater number of participants were assessed. Although script submitted for publication.

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on May 22, 2015


380 ASSESSMENT

Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measure- O’Neill, R. M., & Bornstein, R. F. (2001). The dependent patient in a psy-
ment of adult attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson chiatric inpatient setting: Relationship of interpersonal dependency
& W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships to consultation and medication frequencies. Journal of Clinical Psy-
(pp. 46-76). New York: Guilford. chology, 57, 289-298.
Briere, J. (1998). Inventory of Altered Self-Capacities manual. Odessa, Overholser, J. C. (1996). The dependent personality and interpersonal
FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. problems. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 184, 8-16.
Clark, K. E., & Ladd, G. W. (2000). Connectedness and autonomy sup- Paris, J. (1998). Working with traits: Psychotherapy of personality disor-
port in parent-child relationships. Developmental Psychology, 36, ders. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson.
485-498. Pennebaker, J. (1990). Opening up: The healing power of confiding in
Colgan, P. (1987). Treatment of dependency disorders in men: Toward a others. New York: William Morrow.
balance of identity and intimacy. Journal of Chemical Dependency Pennebaker, J. (Ed.). (1995). Emotion, disclosure, and health. Washing-
Treatment, 1, 205-227. ton, DC: American Psychological Association.
Collins, N. J., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models, Pincus, A. L., & Gurtman, M. B. (1995). The three faces of interpersonal
and relationship quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and dependency: Structural analysis of self-report dependency measures.
Social Psychology, 58, 644-663. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 744-758.
Coolidge, F. L., Thede, L. L., & Jang, K. J. (2001). Heritability of person- Pincus, A. L., & Wilson, K. R. (2001). Interpersonal variability in de-
ality disorders in childhood: A preliminary investigation. Journal of pendent personality. Journal of Personality, 69, 223-251.
Personality Disorders, 15, 33-40. Rosenthal, R. (1984). Meta-analytic procedures in social research.
Cross, S. E., Bacon, P. L., & Morris, M. L. (2000). The relational-interde- Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
pendent self-construal and relationships. Journal of Personality and Rude, S. S., & Burnham, B. L. (1995). Connectedness and neediness:
Social Psychology, 78, 791-808. Factors of the DEQ and SAS dependency scales. Cognitive Therapy
Cross, S. E., & Madson, L. (1997). Models of the self: Self-construals and and Research, 19, 323-340.
gender. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 5-37. Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdepen-
Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1964). The approval motive. New York: dent self-construals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20,
John Wiley. 580-591.
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satis- Sperry, L. (1995). Handbook of diagnosis and treatment of the DSM-IV
faction With Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71- personality disorders. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
75. Summers, F. (1999). Transcending the self: An object relations model of
Ekselius, L., Lindstrom, E., von Knorring, L., Bodlund, O., & Kullgren, psychoanalytic therapy. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.
G. (1994). Comorbidity among the personality disorders in DSM-III- Sundin, E., Armelius, B. C., & Nilsson, T. (1994). Reliability studies of
R. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 155-160. scales of psychological capacities. Psychoanalysis and Contempo-
Gurtman, M. B. (1992). Construct validity of interpersonal personality rary Thought, 17, 591-615.
measures: The interpersonal circumplex as a nomological net. Jour- Taylor, G. J., Bagby, M. R., & Parker, J. D. (1992). The Revised Toronto
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 105-118. Alexithymia Scale: Some reliability, validity, and normative data.
Head, S. B., Baker, J. D., & Williamson, D. A. (1991). Family environ- Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 57, 34-41.
ment characteristics and dependent personality disorder. Journal of Wang, C. L., Bristol, T., Mowen, J. C., & Chakraborty, G. (2000). Alter-
Personality Disorders, 5, 256-263. native modes of self-construal: Dimensions of connectedness-sepa-
Heiss, G. E., Berman, W. H., & Sperling, M. B. (1996). Five scales in rateness and advertising appeals to the cultural and gender-specific
search of a construct: Exploring continued attachment to parents in self. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 9, 107-115.
college students. Journal of Personality Assessment, 67, 102-115. Wang, C. L., & Mowen, J. C. (1997). The separateness-connectedness
Hoglend, P., Bogwald, K. P., Amlo, S., Heyerdahl, O., Sorbye, O., Mar- self-schema. Psychology and Marketing, 14, 185-207.
ble, A., et al. (2000). Assessment of change in dynamic psychother- West, M. L., & Sheldon-Keller, A. E. (1994). Patterns of relating: An
apy. Journal of Psychotherapy Practice and Research, 9, 190-199. adult attachment perspective. New York: Guilford.
Hoglend, P., & Piper, W. E. (1997). Treatment length and termination Whiffen, V. E., Aube, J. A., Thompson, J. M., & Campbell, T. L. (2000).
contracts in dynamic psychotherapy. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, Attachment beliefs and interpersonal contexts associated with de-
51, 37-42. pendency and self-criticism. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychol-
Kantor, M. (1993). Distancing. Westport, CT: Praeger. ogy, 19, 184-205.
Klohnen, E. C., & John, O. P. (1998). Working models of attachment: A
theory-based prototype approach. In J. A Simpson & W. S. Rholes
(Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 115-140). Robert F. Bornstein, Ph.D., is a professor of psychology at Get-
New York: Guilford. tysburg College. He is author of The Dependent Personality
Kobayashi, J. S. (1989). Depathologizing dependency: Two perspec- (Guilford, 1993), coeditor (with Joseph Masling) of six volumes
tives. Psychiatric Annals, 19, 653-658. in the Empirical Studies of Psychoanalytic Theories series (APA
Lang-Takac, E., & Osterweil, Z. (1992). Separateness and
Books, 1993, 1994, 1996, 1998, 2002), and coauthor (with Mary
connectedness: Differences between the genders. Sex Roles, 27, 277-
289. A. Languirand) of When Someone You Love Needs Nursing
Lee, R. M., & Robbins, S. B. (1995). Measuring belongingness: The So- Home Care (Newmarket, 2001) and Healthy Dependency
cial Connectedness and Social Assurance Scales. Journal of Coun- (Newmarket, 2003).
seling Psychology, 42, 232-241.
Livesley, W. J. (Ed.). (1995). The DSM-IV personality disorders. New Kimberly J. Geiselman, B.A., is enrolled in the Clinical Mas-
York: Guilford. ter’s Program at Towson State University in Towson, Maryland.
Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, child-
hood, and adulthood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Elizabeth A. Eisenhart, B.A., is enrolled in the Clinical-
Child Development, 50, 66-104. Developmental Doctoral Program at Bryn Mawr College in Bryn
Millon, T. (1996). Disorders of personality: DSM-IV and beyond. New
Mawr, Pennsylvania.
York: John Wiley.

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on May 22, 2015


Bornstein et al. / CONSTRUCT VALIDITY OF THE RPT 381

Mary A. Languirand, Ph.D., is in private practice in Gettys- coauthor (with Robert F. Bornstein) of When Someone You Love
burg, Pennsylvania. She is coauthor (with Lynn Tondat Carter) of Needs Nursing Home Care (Newmarket, 2001) and Healthy De-
The Thinking Skills Workbook: A Cognitive Skills Remediation pendency (Newmarket, 2003).
Manual for Adults (Charles C Thomas, 1980, 1984, 2000) and

Downloaded from asm.sagepub.com at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on May 22, 2015

You might also like