You are on page 1of 23

Progress in Human Geography 33(6) (2009) pp.

766–788


Haptic geographies: ethnography, haptic
knowledges and sensuous dispositions
Mark Paterson*
School of Geography, Archaeology and Earth Resources, University of Exeter,
Rennes Drive, Exeter EX4 4RJ, UK

Abstract: This paper is the first overview of the treatment of haptic knowledges in geography,
responding to bodily sensations and responses that arise through the embodied researcher. After
Crang’s (2003) article on ‘touchy-feely’ methods identifies the dearth of actual touching and
embodied feeling in research methods, this article does three things. First, it clarifies the terminology,
which is derived from a number of disciplines. Second, it summarizes developments in sensuous
ethnographies within cultural geography and anthropology. Third, it suggests pathways to new
research on ‘sensuous dispositions’ and non-representational theory. We thereby see just how
‘touchy-feely’ qualitative methods have, or might, become.

Key words: ethnography, haptic, phenomenology, senses, touch.

I Introduction: ‘touchy-feely’ methods? experiences become barely articulated, or


When there has been much discussion about articulated barely. This problem is com-
the significance of the body, how do we pounded by the emphasis on visuality and
write meaningfully about those everyday visual metaphors in western culture, and the
embodied experiences of touching and feel- fragmented way that bodily touching and
ing, conjunctions of sensation and emotion feeling is discussed in different disciplines.
that cannot arise without the physicality of What are the implications for fieldwork
the body? And, further, what about forms if embodied experience is involved in the
of touching irreducible to mere skin contact, research process? My own research has en-
that involve feeling the body in movement countered this frustration twice, once involv-
and action? We effortlessly recognize sensa- ing a novel technological device (Paterson,
tions that seem to arise from within the body 2006a), and again while investigating experi-
during activities like dancing or riding a ences of Reiki massage (Paterson, 2005a).
rollercoaster, but one difficulty lies in com- In both cases, getting to grips with unusual
municating these bodily feelings and haptic bodily sensations and subsequently attempt-
sensations. Language is lacking, terms ing to articulate them revealed both epi-
desert us, and such instantly recognizable stemological and methodological difficulties.

*Email: m.w.d.paterson@exeter.ac.uk

© The Author(s), 2009. Reprints and permissions: DOI: 10.1177/0309132509103155


http://www.sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
Mark Paterson: Haptic geographies 767

While the idea of haptic geographies has about the role of haptic knowledges in em-
been mooted sporadically and unsystem- pirical research.
atically (eg, Rodaway, 1994), this paper pulls There are three main components of this
together some recent strands from across task. First, the section ‘Haptic knowledges’
the social sciences into a more coherent outlines the notion of the haptic as a prob-
characterization of the haptic, and provides lematic deriving from early psychology’s at-
the first sustained investigation of some tempt to characterize internal bodily sensa-
implications for research methods. Crang’s tions as distinct from the usual model of the
(2003) report on new qualitative methods five senses, and attempts to standardize the
in this journal, subtitled ‘Touchy, feely, look- terminology for future clarification. Second,
see?’, invokes research in contemporary dis- the section ‘Returning to our senses in field-
courses concerning the body and new ways work’ identifies a more general ‘return to
of feeling in fieldwork. Fundamentally he asks the senses’ within social research, most
‘whether methods often derided for being notably within anthropology, architecture,
somehow soft and ‘touchy-feely’ have in fact cultural history and sociology, rethinking the
been rather limited in touching and feeling’ positioned processes of research through
(p. 494). Surveying a series of textbook ap- the senses and in so doing problematizing
proaches, including the newly reaffirmed traditional Graeco-Roman hierarchies of the
visualistic bias of Rose’s visual methods, in senses that privilege vision and consider
a key section he turns to ‘performative and touch and taste as bestial and base. This sec-
haptic approaches’ (pp. 498–500). Regarding tion considers the significance of the under-
recent research on the cultures and history of examined somatic or internal bodily senses
fieldwork, he says, ‘we get glimpses of geog- in fieldwork raised in the previous section,
raphy as embodied work, as corporeal perfor- the main example here being walking. Third,
mances and cultures of doing geography in and equally pertinent to social and cultural
different places from research to fieldclass’ geography, the last section on ‘sensuous
(p. 499). But he also correctly identifies a dispositions’ traces the recent departure
major lacuna in terms of reflexively learning from discourses of embodiment towards
through the bodily sensations and responses more complexly layered investigations into
that occur inevitably as part of the embodied new ways of feeling that arise through the
experiences of the researcher within differ- technologies, disciplines and practices of
ent spatial contexts. It concerns, to use his late modernity, and which seek alternative
phraseology, ‘haptic knowledges’ (p. 499). ways of writing and expressing these, such
The purpose of this article is therefore to as non-representational theory (eg, Thrift,
attend to this lacuna, to identify research 1997; 2007). But, as Lorimer (2005: 84)
that addresses the generation of haptic identifies, there are also ‘sensuous disposi-
knowledges in the process of research and tions’ or new sensibilities at work here. The
fieldwork, to summarize some of the notable paper will therefore consider work on this
developments in this area since the appear- topic in geography and related disciplines,
ance of Crang’s article, and thereby to see pointing in conclusion to notable methodo-
just how ‘touchy-feely’ qualitative methods logical and epistemological implications for
have – or might – become. By clarifying these such work.
‘haptic knowledges’ and their part within
the reflexive processes of knowledge pro- II ‘Haptic knowledges’
duction we depart from any connotations Conducting ethnographic fieldwork in
of woolly-minded vagueness that ‘touchy- Scotland’s mountains, Lund makes the im-
feely’ might suggest, and offer a more clearly mediate yet accurate claim that ‘touch, as
defined and robust programme for thinking one of the five senses, has so far been under
768 Progress in Human Geography 33(6)

examined in the ethnographic context’, and what Boring et al. (1948) term ‘somesthesis’,
she seeks to rectify this by thinking how the refers to a set of inwardly felt bodily sensa-
sense of touch is involved with ‘how the body tions distinct from the culturally sedimented
moves in different contexts’ (Lund, 2005: 28). model of the five senses formulated since
In other words, like Ingold’s (2004) literal Aristotle (1984; 1986). Such sensations are
grounding of perception through the feet as difficult to resolve as distinct perceptions,
a muscular consciousness, Lund identifies evidenced by the fact that western medi-
that ‘touch’ is not reducible to tactility or cine, psychology and social science has
tactile sensation alone, and that immediate only relatively recently acknowledged them
bodily experience combines other sensations within the lexicon and there remains little
distributed throughout the body, felt as mus- consensus on the terminology. Recent cross-
cular tensions, movements and balance, along cultural anthropology (eg, Classen, 1993;
with sensitivity to temperature and pain. 1997; Geurts, 2002; Howes, 2003) certainly
All these sometimes uncomfortable tactile, explores this area, and analysis of linguistic
muscular and balance sensations are indub- constructions and everyday phraseology
itably present in a variety of embodied activ- reveals an awareness of these sensations
ities and contexts such as running, swimming in diverse pre-industrial cultures, discussed in
or walking within urban or rural settings, the following section. Rather than the
as readers will recognize. These sensations inward/outward distinction assumed by the
I collectively term ‘somatic sensations’ terms ‘interoception’ and ‘exteroception’
(Paterson, 2007), underexplored bodily sen- (preferred by Gibson, 1962; 1966), however,
sations that take place within what perceptual I prefer the collective term ‘somatic senses’
psychology terms the larger ‘haptic system’ as it acknowledges the multiplicity and
(eg, Gibson, 1966). As this paper unfolds, the interaction between different intern-
these and associated terms from a variety of ally felt and outwardly orientated senses.
disciplines will be clarified, to refine further Mountcastle (2005: 2) for example notes
what we mean by ‘haptic knowledges’. that ‘somatic sensibility’ and ‘somesthesis’
Although the word ‘haptic’ derives from are equivalent terms, and so the different
the Greek haptesthai meaning ‘of, or pertaining somatic senses collectively help constitute
to, touch’ (Oxford English Dictionary, second the underexplored background feelings of
edition), the kind of touch implied extends embodiment, the self-perception of inner
beyond straightforward skin contact, that is, bodily states. The somatic senses generally
cutaneous touch. Confusion persists in the work synergistically as part of the ‘haptic sys-
terminology as a result of the diversity of dis- tem’, as Gibson elsewhere (especially 1966:
ciplinary approaches, so an opportunity for 97ff) terms it, which includes kinaesthesia
clarification presents itself here. Even cuta- (the sense of movement), proprioception
neous skin sensations are irreducible simply to (felt muscular position) and the vestibular
pressure on the skin, as it includes returns from system (sense of balance; see also Reed and
various receptors in the skin that deal with Jones, 1979). More generally, and with apol-
pressure (mechanoreceptors), temperature ogies for the masculine pronoun:
(thermoreceptors) and pain (nociceptors).
Beyond immediate skin contact the term The haptic system … is an apparatus by
‘haptic’ is therefore applied more extensively which the individual gets information about
to include internally felt bodily sensations. both his [sic] environment and his body. He
feels an object relative to the body and the
This has been formalized most comprehen-
body relative to an object. It is the perceptual
sibly by Gibson (1966: 97ff) as the ‘haptic system by which animals and men are literally
system’. What Sherrington in 1947 termed in touch with the environment. (Gibson, 1966:
‘interoception’ (in Fowler, 2003: 1505), or 98, original emphasis)
Mark Paterson: Haptic geographies 769

Clearly there is a distinction between the ‘phantom limb’ phenomenon that Merleau-
notion of the ‘haptic’ and that of ‘touch’ for Ponty (1992) discusses, a form of phantom
Gibson, whereby the usual notion of touch sensation of cultural memory. This lies in-
as cutaneous (skin) contact is enlarged to terestingly at the interface between the
include a range of internally felt bodily states Bourdieu-type way in which practices be-
which functions as part of a larger haptic per- come ‘embodied’, which Geurts (2002) also
ceptual system. For the sake of clarification, employs, and the more phenomenological
a brief breakdown of the components of the analysis of interoception that Merleau-Ponty
haptic system follows. offers, especially in terms of ‘the spatiality of
one’s body and motility’ (Merleau-Ponty,
1 Kinaesthesia – the sense of movement 1992: 112ff).
As part of the haptic system, Gibson (1966:
111ff) writes of kinesthesis (kinaesthesia) as 2 Proprioception – the sense of bodily position
the perception of the body’s movement not The study of proprioception, like that of
as a distinct, individuated sense but as cutting kinaesthesia, is similarly historically dispersed
across several perceptual systems. Identified and intermittent, having been identified
originally in western medicine by Charles Bell originally by Sherrington in 1906 (1947).
in 1826, kinaesthesia was initially confused Proprioception is a perceptual system based
with proprioception as a kind of generalized on the sensory returns from nerve endings in
muscle sense, according to Boring et al. (1948: the muscles, so that as part of one’s embodi-
525ff). They are distinct, yet both involve ment the position of the body and limbs are
a sense of felt embodiment accomplished felt. Koffka’s (1935) Gestalt psychology led
through sense-returns from receptors dis- to empirically investigating a sense of the
tributed throughout the body, in physiology ‘vertical’, for example, and while what he
termed ‘re-afferent feedback’ (Gibson, called the ‘framework’ of space is a percep-
1966: 111). Kinaesthesia is a sense of movement tion, as Gibson (1968) details, the ‘accom-
that utilizes a range of nerve information, in- panying awareness of the axis of the body is
cluding that of muscular tension and balance. a proprioception’ (his emphasis). Compared
Husserl (1970) writes about kinaesthesia with a grounded spatial framework, then,
as a background to embodied experience the axis of the body is literally ‘felt’ as
(discussed further in Paterson, 2007: 27–35). upright or tilted, and limbs and their move-
Usually working in conjunction, kinaesthesia ment are distinguished in reference to this
and proprioception are invoked in areas fixed framework. Later this becomes sig-
such as dance research, performance studies nificant for Merleau-Ponty, as he acknow-
and anthropology where, for example, ledges the influence of empirical findings
Downey (2005) writes on martial arts and from Gestalt psychologists. The medical
Capoeira, and Ram (2005) on learning to writer Leder clarifies the phenomenon:
dance. For Downey (2005) kinaesthesia is
employed in anticipating, learning and per- proprioception traces out a completed sense of
forming complex dance or martial arts my surface body, allowing me to adjust every
limb, every muscle, in appropriate motoric
moves. If kinaesthesia does not straight-
response to tasks. Though visually this sense
forwardly correspond to particular sensors or is subliminal, I can close my eyes and pro-
receptors in the muscles, then it is no single prioceptively hone in on the position, the level
sense but works as a synergetic conjunction of tension and relaxation, in any region of the
or nexus of visceral sensation and exterior muscular body. (Leder, 1990: 42)
perception. For Ram (2005), embodying her
and her daughter’s ‘Indianness’ through tradi- Like other forms of interoception, proprio-
tional dance and tastes in film is likened to the ception as a perceptual subsystem relies not
770 Progress in Human Geography 33(6)

simply on the returns of particular receptors turning would make the visual perception
(in this case proprioceptors) in the muscles and of the world unclear, distorted or chaotic.
skin, but functions as a nexus of sensations In other words, without this reflex a fixed
from a variety of sensors throughout the body point in space could not be tracked. Ballet
that provide a sense of the body’s and limbs’ dancers in particular must track a fixed point
felt position in space as a series of subject- in space when they go ‘on point’, pirouetting
ively felt muscular tensions, and therefore on tiptoe without falling down. The tech-
feedback as to bodily posture and equilibrium nique is to visually locate a fixed object such
(Gibson, 1966: 34). In a completely darkened as a clock, and continually maintain eye
room, for example, one’s body is felt as up- contact with it while spinning (as the bal-
right, or one’s arms sensed as outstretched, lerina Deborah Bull explains in the BBC
as a result of proprioception. documentary ‘The dancer’s body’, 20021).
After this brief excursus into the physi-
3 The vestibular system – the sense of balance ology of the haptic system, the lesson here
Deriving from the ‘vestibule’ area of the inner is simple. While there are clearly specialized
ear, the vestibular system connects up infor- receptors for exteroceptive senses such as
mation picked up from weighted hair cells in sight and audition, there is no such one-to-one
the cochlea, triggered by movements of fluid correlation between receptors and organs
within three semicircular canals orientated for the somatic senses. This applies similarly
roughly along the three spatial axes. These to ‘touch’ which is itself a combination of
are the horizontal, anterior and posterior data primarily from receptors responding to
canals which pick up turning movements and pressure (mechanoreceptors), temperature
bodily orientation, and lateral movement is (thermoreceptors) and pain (nociceptors), as
picked up through the otoliths, which sense we have seen. The somatic senses provide
linear accelerations (Lackner and DiZio, re-afferent feedback from receptors distrib-
2005: 117). Rather than a distinct ‘sense’ uted not just cutaneously but throughout the
itself, bodily inertia or change of bodily orien- body, and the self-perception of inner bodily
tation or direction is picked up through this states is often indistinct or confused (see, for
system, and feeds directly into the other example, Leder, 1990; 2005: 336ff on vis-
somatic senses. To characterize it simply ceral perception). Indeed, Leder’s The absent
as balance neglects the complexity of its body (1990) still provides a useful template
functions, dealing with inertia and momen- for a phenomenologically informed investi-
tum and actively correlating with other dis- gation of the somatic senses. The spatial
tributed sense-returns. That is, information experience of easily discernible cutaneous
from the vestibular system of semicircular (skin-based) tactile sensation is contrasted
canals, cochlea and otoliths collectively helps with the imprecision of internally felt sensa-
constitute a sense of ‘bodily postural equi- tions. Yet cases of dysfunction of the somatic
librium’, as Gibson (1966: 67) puts it, sensitive senses are extremely rare. This is just as well,
to changes in orientation and self-produced for loss of proprioception when it does occur
movement, and is therefore indissociable as part of a progressive neurological condition
from the other somatic senses. Furthermore, can lead to surreal results whereby, if not
feedback from the vestibular system directly visually monitoring arms or legs, they go
influences the eye muscles and this is known ‘astray’, and the subject must learn to walk
as the vestibular-ocular reflex (Lackner and again from scratch by painstakingly and at-
DiZio, 2005: 119ff). Were this not the case tentively watching and coordinating each
then sudden head movements, movements movement, as Cole (1995) details in a fascina-
of the eyeballs or acts such as running or ting case study. Case studies of dysfunction
Mark Paterson: Haptic geographies 771

in these somatic senses may therefore reveal coining of the word ‘ocularcentrism’, as ‘the
insights into the background of generic scopic regime of modernity’, providing some
everyday, embodied spatial experience, and philosophical context in which to address
this trend of thinking is a mainstay of phe- this visualistic bias. On the other hand, especi-
nomenological psychology, particularly in ally since Classen’s Worlds of sense (1993),
Straus (1966) and Merleau-Ponty (1992). Van Paul Stoller’s Sensuous scholarship (1997) and
den Berg (1952) usefully summarizes treat- Ingold’s The perception of the environment
ments of movement and kinaesthesia within (2000), the increasing interest in anthropol-
the phenomenological tradition, placing ogy and cultural history has started to signify
lesser-known figures like Straus and Marcel something novel in the way the humanities
alongside the more prominent figures of and social sciences approach, categorize,
Husserl, Sartre and Merleau-Ponty. More describe and represent sensory experiences,
generally, while studies examine somatic and is attentive to cultural variations in the
phenomena predominantly from a medical, construction of sensory hierarchies (see
psychological and physiological perspective, for example Csordas, 1993; Classen, 1997;
the first-person experiential perspective Stoller, 1997). Much of this work, like Jay
that Leder (1990) proffers has periodically (1994), bemoans the emphasis on visuality in
been addressed ethnographically. But western industrialized cultures, but pursues
earlier encounters with bodily senses and this assumption empirically through a range
memory do occur within the tradition of hu- of ethnographic methods. A more recent
manistic geography, addressed in a section wave of sensuous scholarship, exemplified by
below. Law (2001), Geurts (2002), Howes (2003),
Bull and Back (2003), Drobnick (2006) and
III Returning to our senses in fieldwork: Paterson (2007; 2009), instead attempt to
the visual and the visceral reassert the validity of non-visual experiences
The recognition of the importance of haptics of space and place.2 Likewise, the journal
in everyday embodiment also signals an The Senses and Society, started in 2006, has
ability to use the body ‘as an instrument of an equally interdisciplinary take on the sen-
research’ (Crang, 2003) or as a ‘tool to gain sorium and sensuous scholarship.
insights into research subjects and their geo- The sensorium, ‘the subject’s way of co-
graphies’ (Longhurst et al., 2008). In geog- ordinating all the body’s perceptual and pro-
raphy this is beginning to be addressed as it prioceptive signals as well as the changing
coincides with an emergent body of work sensory envelope of the self’ (Jones, 2007: 8),
in the social sciences that engages directly is continually shifting and culturally variable.
with the senses. This is unsurprising because It varies according to a society’s rules or pro-
‘our first and foremost, most immediate and scriptions as well as technological mediation
intimately felt geography is the body, the and physical environment. This undoubtedly
site of emotional experience and expres- affects not only the reflexivity of the em-
sion par excellence’ (Davidson and Milligan in bodied ethnographer in their fieldwork, but
Longhurst et al., 2008: 210). On the one hand, also inflects the interpretation of particular
re-establishing interest in our senses and sen- linguistic constructions such as idioms or
sibilities is seemingly obvious, symptomatic proverbs, folk psychologies and the govern-
of the repatriation and revalidation of earlier ance of gait, posture or Mauss’ (1992) ‘tech-
prohibitions against sensual pleasures and niques of the body’. Therefore the regulation
delights in western cultural history. We are and disciplining of sensory hierarchies also
familiar with the persistence of the centrality potentially reveals power relations and
of vision, and Jay’s (1994) philosophical and Stoller (2004: 820) suggests that ‘sensuous
historical engagement with this topic and his descriptions improve not only the clarity and
772 Progress in Human Geography 33(6)

force of ethnographic representations but that we do not consume sorcery, history


also the analysis of power relations-in-the- or knowledge; rather it is history, sorcery and
knowledge that consume us. To accept sen-
world’. As such, the acknowledgement that
suousness is, like the Songhay spirit medium
sensuous ethnographies do indeed poten- or Sufi Saint, to lend one’s body the world and
tially signify something ‘new’ offers up in- accept its complexities, tastes, structures, and
novative possibilities of thinking, writing and smells. (Stoller, 1997: xvii)
reflecting on what has hitherto been ignored.
As ever, what is closest or most obvious to us So, while the immediacy of sensory experi-
is revealed to be most distant. Yet the poten- ence is one factor explored in this section,
tial reach for these new sensory scholar- a further intriguing implication is that ‘social
ships, ethnographies and engagements is knowledge’ can be derived by ‘openly and
far, and work in areas such as consumption modestly foregrounding local sensibilities’,
and retail psychology has similarly pursued as he claims elsewhere (Stoller, 2004: 822).
this trend (eg, Howes, 2005; Paterson, From the language of administrators in Niger,
2005b; Roe, 2006).3 Engaging with sensuous Stoller points towards a ‘sensuous politics
scholarship can therefore reveal insights both of postcolonial Africa’ (p. 824) through a
etic and emic, challenging the sensory as- somewhat poetic or idiosyncratic transposi-
sumptions of both researcher and researched, tion of sensory qualities and impressions onto
and in an academic climate that gestures the characteristics of the state, such as the
towards the ‘more-than-representational’ ‘corpulent’ wealth, or the ‘hardness’ and
(eg, Lorimer, 2005) must find innovative ‘softness’ of the occupying colonial forces, a
ways to evoke or transcribe those underrep- literal texture of power. A recurring phrase
resented, unproblematized realms of every- of the French postcolonial administrators
day, embodied sensory experience. While and commentators is tellingly that of the
such methods will directly be addressed guerre intestin, or ‘intestinal wars’ (p. 825),
towards the end of this paper, we must first for example, a phrase that expresses well the
ask: what exactly is sensuous scholarship? rumbling internal disquiet of the body politic
And what novel techniques in fieldwork does during a series of insurrections under French
it encourage? colonial rule between 1898 and 1915.
Remaining in Africa, Geurts’ (2002) mono-
1 Sensuous ethnography and social knowledge graph Culture and the senses: bodily ways
To consider the way the Songhay people of of knowing in an African community is an
Nigeria sense and structure their world, to example of sustained sensuous scholarship
use Stoller’s (1997) classic ethnography, is in the field, revealing evidence of long-term
not simply to map or translate series of sen- exposure to her chosen fieldwork with the
sations on individual bodies, but potentially Anlo-Ewe people of southeastern Ghana.
to open up the researcher’s body to accom- On the face of it, Geurts follows Stoller’s
modate a whole new sensory and cosmic work in writing an in-depth ethnography
semiology, and to make that body radically that accounts for the missing senses. If the
porous. Stoller himself defines sensuous criticism of a visual bias in western cultures
ethnography thus: is a common refrain, then Geurts’ prolonged
fieldwork and language acquisition allows
Sensuous ethnography, of course, creates a set her to interact with a number of characters
of instabilities for the ethnographer. To accept (not simply ‘respondents’, but a plethora of
sensuousness in scholarship is to eject the
character sketches, biographies, tales from a
conceit of control in which mind and body, self
and other are considered separate. It is indeed number of dwellers in the city or the villages,
a humbling experience to recognize, likewise drawn with vividness), and from this derive
Songhay sorcerers and griots [storytellers], great insights into the bodily or somatic senses.
Mark Paterson: Haptic geographies 773

Indeed, as her research progresses and her and encapsulates perfectly the renewed in-
language acquisition solidifies, the particular terest in the somatic senses. Since it figures in
bodily senses of kinaesthesia (movement) the language and performative elaborations
and the vestibular sense (balance) figure of the Anlo, it functions as one of the keys to
prominently. What is most significant for her project of understanding an indigenous
Geurts is not the translation of sensory sensorium. On the one hand, seselelame help-
mappings onto Graeco-Roman traditions of fully redirects attention back into the bodily
the sensory hierarchy. Instead, her guiding interior and the set of somatic sensations
task is to ‘understand an indigenous Anlo that are more usually ignored in the Graeco-
sensorium’ (p. 38) from an etic perspective. Roman sensorium. On the other hand, it
Through a variety of methods, including is too conveniently deployed as a generic
the analysis of idioms and proverbs, innumer- conceptual ‘master key’ for unlocking long-
able fragments of conversation with the running philosophical problems that continue
people she encounters, and observations of to impact upon the social sciences, such
movement and gait, she observes a more as mind-body dualism, to elide the division
developed bodily vocabulary in Anlo-Ewe cul- between ‘perception’ and ‘sensation’ (p. 41)
ture. In other words, as performed through and, later, to reunite sensation, emotion and
language, posture, movement, infant devel- disposition (p. 164). But theoretically her
opment and education, the ‘internal’, bodily project is more evidently influenced by
or somatic senses of kinaesthesia, proprio- Bourdieu, employing a habitus that is ‘emi-
ception and the vestibular system are more nently sensuous’ (p. 243) and showing a two-
developed in the Anlo-Ewe sensorium. As fold process where, first, the sensory order is
Geurts herself acknowledges, so far this literally ‘embodied’ and, second, that sensory
addresses Csordas’ (1993: 138) call for an- order contains categories that are valuable
thropology to question ‘somatic modes of at- to a cultural group, thereby metaphorically
tention’. However, whereas some anthro- becoming a larger ‘body’ of thought (p. 231).
pologists like Downey (2002; 2005) achieve
this in the analysis of dance and movement 2 The social order and the sensory order
predominantly through the psychological Within anthropology, sensory studies of food
phenomenology of Merleau-Ponty, throw- and eating have long offered an accessible
ing up novel ways of approaching and writing invitation to broaden the discussion from the
movement and sensation, Geurts achieves supposed sensory immediacy of taste and
this through Bourdieu’s notion of habitus smell to more metaphorical notions of ‘taste’,
and works to see how sensory schemas are, the reproduction of a sensorium that is par-
appropriating Csordas’ phrase, ‘“perfor- ticular to place, and the performance of
matively elaborated” … in the habitus’ of the ethnic and gendered identity within that
Anlo, since ‘sensory experiences are pivotal place. Stoller’s (1989) classic conception of
in the formation of identity and cultural ‘bad sauce’ among the Songhay of Niger
difference’ (Geurts, 2002: 69). showed that taste, smell and the sensory
The great strength of Geurts’ work lies in aspects of food preparation and eating were
this connection between bodily senses and a valid way of expressing cultural experi-
routine cultural practices, such as the sense ence, being socially meaningful features
of movement and its connection with char- of everyday life. This sentiment is neatly
acter and moral sensibility. This finds its encapsulated within Law’s (2001) work on
clearest and most succinct expression in her the recreation of ‘home’ through cooking
discussion of seselelame. Briefly, seselelame is and smell by Filipino immigrant workers in
translated as ‘feeling in the body’, or more Hong Kong. Contrary to any assumption by
literally as ‘feel-feel-at-flesh-inside’ (p. 41), readers here that sensuous fieldwork leads
774 Progress in Human Geography 33(6)

only to a naïve, ahistoricized or depoliticized of place. Within geography, the work of


phenomenology, Law effectively explores Susan Smith and George Revill is particularly
ways to relate the smells, sounds and sights pertinent here (eg, Smith, 1994; 2000; Revill,
within specific regions of the city to larger 1998; 2005). However, the individuated origin
patterns of migration and the effects on of the actual experience of the senses that
a locale of globalized workforces. Rather manifests through the body is a familiar trope
than limit her fieldwork to the recording of within previous academic formulations of
sensuous experiences, then, Law makes ex- the connections between the senses and
plicit connections with place-making, global- place. The next section further discusses
local contexts and some of the psychosocial one such phenomenologically informed area
conditions that characterize experiences of within human geography.
migration and displacement in late modern-
ity. This view finds support in Howes’ essay 3 ‘Sensitive theory’: humanistic geography
‘Hyperesthesia’, subtitled ‘The sensual logic It is not coincidental that the textual turn so
of late capitalism’ (Howes, 2005: 281–303), prevalent within the humanities and social
which offers a broad historical critique of sciences occurs after our fascination with
the role of the senses in consumption. From embodiment fades. For the immediacy of
the disavowal of the senses due to the brute sensory experience, the new engagements
forces of industrialization, Howes charts the with the senses, and the new sensorium
increasing prominence of sensory stimulation arising within particular structures of cap-
in consumer capitalism, including a brief italism are being deliberately obscured.
detour into a business fad that has presum- David Howes (2005: 1) is perhaps correct
ably already run its course, that of Pine and to remark on the ‘sensorial poverty of con-
Gilmore’s ‘experience economy’ (p. 289ff). temporary theory’. Hence the title of his
Classen’s (2005) work on the alternative edited collection, Empire of the senses, being
sensory hierarchies of three geographically a necessary corrective to the ‘empire of
diverse peoples that respectively prioritize signs’ (Serres and Latour, 1995: 132). Rather
smell (the Ongee of the Andaman Islands), than being necessarily restrictive or overly
heat (the Tzotzil of Mexico) and colour specific to particular senses, charting the
(the Desana of the Amazon) shows how empire of the senses and its significance to
the sensory order is simultaneously a social geography may involve entwining multiple
order and a cosmological order. Steven strands within social theory, including the
Feld’s ‘Places sensed, senses placed’ (2005) aestheticization of everyday life, the cultural
invokes a ‘sensuous epistemology’ that fo- history of the formation of the sensorium
cuses more on sound, hence his neologism (eg, Corbin, 1986; 2005), or the ethnographic
of an ‘acoustemology’ (p. 184) that is always (eg, Geurts, 2002; 2005; Feld, 2005). Such
already synaesthetic. The relevance for texts do in fact acknowledge the social, eco-
geography is clear, invoking a sensuous eth- nomic and political issues around the con-
nography to achieve, in his words, ‘a social struction of the sensorium so that, as Classen
phenomenology and hermeneutics of senses (1998) elsewhere summarizes, the sensory
of place’ (p. 179). While Feld prioritizes the order is always simultaneously a moral order
ear and acoustemology in his fieldwork, per- and a social order. This is a corrective to
forming such a social phenomenology can the tendency to focus on the senses as in-
certainly occur through other modalities and dividualistic sensation, the temptation of
other means; utilizing experience of the senses much sensory theory and phenomenology.
as a gateway to the larger social order and to Furthermore, it solidifies Howes’ (2005)
other senses, orders or conceptualizations claim that this return to the senses is not
Mark Paterson: Haptic geographies 775

simply another fashionable stream of theory while attempts to evoke, describe and engage
to add to our academic repertoire of colonial- with such experience allowed increasingly
ism, gender, embodiment or material culture, experimental textual investigations.
for example. Instead, the senses in their sup- In particular, the experiential fieldwork
posed immediacy are the medium through of Rowles on gerontology and geography
which such aspects are accessed or experi- provides moving and affective ethnographic
enced. As Stoller indicates, the implications descriptions of the relations between elderly
for fieldwork extend far outside the individual people’s fading senses and bodily abilities,
body in its academic isolation: memory and their environmental context
(Rowles, 1978; 1980). He describes his meth-
Bearing witness through sensuous ethno- odology as a form of creative dialogue, a
graphy, however, is more than an arcane
epistemological practice that scholars pursue
process of mutual discovery, an ‘immersion
in academic isolation. Using sensuous ethno- in participants’ lifeworlds’ or ‘intersubjective
graphy to bear witness to the forms of social encounter’ (Rowles, 1980: 57). For his elderly
trauma, abuse, and repression … has the subjects, geographical experience involved
potential to shock readers into newfound more than ‘mere behavioristic locomotion
awareness, enabling them, following the
through timeless Cartesian space’; instead, by
insights of Antonin Artaud (1958), to think
new thoughts or feel new feelings. (Stoller, drawing into proximity with them, Rowles
2004: 832) discovered ‘a fusion of implicit awareness,
thought, and action, entailing holistic involve-
However, some readers may be forgiven for ment within a ‘lived space’ – a lifeworld
assuming geographers have visited cognate with temporal depth and meaning as well as
territory before, in terms of humanistic spatial extent’ (p. 61). After spending several
geography (eg, Tuan, 1974; 1977; Porteous, months getting to know his elderly subjects,
1985; 1986; Pocock, 1993). It is important becoming increasingly immersed in their
to acknowledge that many of the issues lifeworlds through casual conversation
and debates discussed so far were explicitly and listening and sharing their stories and
addressed within that tradition. Rather than observations, glimpses not only of how his
retread such arguments in detail, its position respondents see the world but also how their
in the geographical intervention between the bodies feel the world tentatively begin to
senses and qualitative methods is inestim- occur. Particularly pertinent here are obser-
able and there are a wealth of productive vations relating some of the physiological and
connections between the humanistic tradi- psychological effects of aging and the impact
tion and more recent ethnographic methods upon their experience of the environment. In
(see Cloke et al. 2004: 169ff, for a discussion somewhat dispassionate terms, Rowles sum-
of this), and even relations between the hu- marizes these effects: ‘a propensity for col-
manistic tradition and non-representational lapse of the spinal column, reduced lung
theory (see Lorimer, 2007: 92–93). Human- capacity, calcification of ligaments, reduced
istic geography’s broad significance lay in circulatory system capability, and sensory
approaches and methods that were critical decrements in sight, hearing, taste, touch
of the prevalent positivist, behaviourist and and smell. Each of these changes modifies the
quantitative axis of human geography at that individual’s experience of the physical envir-
time, and central to this is the return to ex- onment’ (p. 67). With such physiological
perience as an object of inquiry. Reliance more effects, combined with information of the
on experiences of the senses, movement, pain and limitations of scope communicated
rhythm and the genius loci (eg, the essays in through snippets of conversation, asides,
Buttimer and Seamon, 1980) show a direct demonstrations of physical breathlessness
lineage from phenomenological methods, and so on, the generic notion of ‘lifeworld’
776 Progress in Human Geography 33(6)

becomes modifiable, more specific to each the researcher must become translator and
respondent’s muscular and mental particul- seek ‘to project an aura of authenticity’; the
arities. Since physiological effects of aging researcher ‘is successful only insofar as [they]
obviously vary between bodies, as research- can effect a translation imparting the essence
ers we must similarly countermand the of the participant’s subjective knowing as
tendency to generalize. Rowles suggests revealed in the interpersonal exchanges that
that the task is not to generalize, then, but to transpired’ (p. 187). The translation most
understand: ‘Understanding, it can be said, is suitable for the presentation of such ‘sens-
the deeper level of awareness which arises itive theory’ points towards a development
from drawing close enough to a person to of writing skills and style of expression, ‘a
become a sympathetic participant within her subtle sensitivity of expression allowing
lifeworld and to have her integrally involved the reader to identify with the experience
in one’s own’ (p. 68). and a detailed explication of the process
While this intersubjective understanding through which conclusions were reached’
of the physiological particularities of a re- (p. 188). Documenting this process between
spondent’s lifeworld shows promise in con- researcher and researched entails autobio-
sidering the haptic knowledges of another, graphical presentation, using the first-person
and how they alter over time, there are voice to ‘directly communicate the research-
methodological limitations. Discussing one er’s role within the research experience’
of his favourite elderly respondents, ‘Marie’, (p. 188). Certainly, when thinking of and writ-
Rowles’ faith in the ‘authenticity’ of rep- ing about the haptic experience of others, it
resenting such lifeworlds is naïve and mis- is inescapably mediated through the haptic
placed when arguing that ‘Descriptive experience of the researcher. Sensuously
understanding of Marie’s involvement within reflexive autobiographical presentation,
the spaces and places of her life provides an especially given our earlier clarification of
authentic representation of the reality of terminology, would acknowledge this.
growing old as experienced by one person The presence of such literature, along with
dwelling within a specific spatio-temporal the reappraisal of the haptic in Rodaway’s
setting’ (p. 68). Now, in constructing such (1994) work entitled Sensuous geographies,
‘sensitive theory’ (p. 68), we would agree and more recently Hetherington’s (2003)
when Rowles argues it should be specific, placing of touch in the constitution of visually
tied to particular respondents and their ex- impaired people’s access to museums, indi-
periences. But any claim to ‘authenticity’ in cates that within a larger timeframe some
observations and descriptions of subjectively geographies have already been broadly, if
felt bodily impairments and sensory decre- unsystematically, ‘haptic’. Correspondingly,
ments is doomed to failure. Elsewhere Rowles a multiplicity of strands dealing with touch,
acknowledges something like this when sensuous ethnographies and sensuous geo-
discussing the nature of the inductive pro- graphies have been steadily percolating
cess that participants and researcher pursue through work in anthropology, cultural his-
together, seeking ‘consensual expression tory, and humanistic geography. Longhurst
of their interpersonal knowing’ that is sub- et al.’s (2008) attempt at ‘using the body
sequently edited by the researcher to produce as a research tool’ also draws attention to
‘descriptive vignettes’ (Rowles, 1978: 187) more prosaic and messy embodiment in
that can be agreed upon. A further stage tasting and eating, such that unwanted or
of inductive editing takes place when undesirable bodily responses like involuntary
writing up results for academic presentation gagging should not be left out of the written
and publication, thinks Rowles; while the research account. In other words, the
conclusions remain those of the researcher, messy unpredictability of the body remains
Mark Paterson: Haptic geographies 777

significant if the totality of the body is used as epistemological, aesthetic and ontological
a research tool. Similar concerns are directly positions among its proponents. Let us now
addressed in a later section on pain. We now consider the significance of kinaesthesia
turn to detailed sensuous ethnographies and and motor intentionality for thinking about
the evolution of appropriate methodologies ‘grounded perception’ through the example
potentially of relevance to geographers. of walking.
Rather than the smells and sounds of
4 Pedestrian touch, grounded perception places, the kinaesthetic performance of
What Merleau-Ponty (1992: 253) identifies as walking is simultaneously mundane, yet
‘a tactile perception of space’ is a good start- something that binds internally felt and
ing point for an embodied ethnography of externally orientated senses and which has
walking, thinking of our embodied everyday consequently been of interest to anthropol-
stance not as the separation of mind from ogy and recent geography. Following on from
body, head from feet, but as diverse strands Rowles’ work on elderly bodies and mobility,
of sense returns from limbs, viscera, sense discussed above, Edensor (2000) updates
organs and muscular movement that variously these themes, identifying the romantic-era
combine as an almost elastic sensory-spatial desire to escape the city, which actively dulls
envelope, a sensorium in action. We can the senses, through the activity of walking
therefore posit something like a corporeal in the countryside so that, in walking, ‘the
context based on what Merleau-Ponty walker returns to his senses’ (Thoreau, in
(p. 110) terms a ‘motor intentionality’, that is, Edensor, 2000: 86). Ingold’s earlier work
the reaching out towards other things. In largely concentrated on sight in The percep-
conjunction with the classification of the tion of the environment (Ingold, 2000), but
haptic system earlier, phenomenology does subsequently he acknowledges that ‘a more
provide some background for thinking through literally grounded approach to perception
the haptic and the somatic: should help to restore touch to its proper
place in the balance of the senses’, since ‘it is
The very fact that the way is paved to true vi- surely through our feet, in contact with the
sion through a phase of transition, and through ground (albeit mediated by footwear), that
a sort of touch effected by the eyes, would be
we are most fundamentally and continually
incomprehensible unless there were a quasi-
spatial tactile field, into which the first visual “in touch” with our surroundings’ (Ingold,
perceptions may be inserted. (Merleau-Ponty, 2004: 330, original emphasis). By differen-
1992: 259) tiating the most usual form of touching
through the hands, ‘manual touch’, from the
Here Merleau-Ponty does not simplistically sensations that occur through the feet, he
equate vision with touch, since ‘touching is attempts to find the distinct properties of a
not seeing’ (p. 260). But there seems to be ‘pedestrian touch’. In line with our earlier
an appreciation of the multiple interrelations discussion of the haptic system, Ingold not
and correspondences between the senses only asks of the role of the ear in maintaining
such that ‘one can still touch with one’s eyes’, balance, which we identified as the vestibular
as Lund (2005: 30) puts it, an immediately system, but also the intriguing possibility of
graspable proposition that she deploys in hearing through the feet (p. 331).
her fieldwork. There remains a tendency Ingold’s work, along with Lund (2005)
among social scientists to take an early stage and Macpherson (2009) in particular, shows
of one particular figure such as Merleau- an increasing interest in performing ethno-
Ponty as shorthand for ‘phenomenology’ in graphies of the somatic sensations within
general, which simply fails to acknowledge walking, Lund with mountaineers in Scotland
the historical diversity and wildly differing and Macpherson with visually impaired
778 Progress in Human Geography 33(6)

walkers in northern England. There is an auto- muscular feedback with their perception of
ethnographic component for Lund, as when the landscape. ‘Bethan’, for example, said:
she describes approaching a narrow path:
[T]o gather my own impression of what the
The act of balancing my body and all my terrain is like, I get that through my own
various muscle tensions was no longer the feet more than anything else, you know? …
centre of my attention; the tension between Actually you can pick up a lot of information
my body and the ground had evened out. My from your feet, and maybe because I used to
eyes still focused on the ground but with more see it helps piece together a picture of what
ease. I did not need to stop to look around the whole thing is like just from walking over it.
anymore. The shape of the ground allowed me (In Macpherson, 2009)
to move with a more flexible posture. (Lund,
2005: 28) While the visual is indisputably prioritized
historically in aesthetic conceptions of
Lund’s description exhibits an increasing ease landscape, the ‘picture’ which Bethan herself
and fluidity through movement, no longer invokes is decidedly irreducible to a visual
separating the cognitive precision of the ‘image’ or map. Instead, we can use such
head and visual acuity of the eyes from non-visual ‘pictures’ or somatic conceptions
the clunky remainder of the body, the of space through the feet and hands in
classic separation which Ingold (2004: 323) activities like walking as examples of haptic
observes as characteristic of modernity, the knowledges, and note their ability to expand
tendency of sedentary perception. Instead, the repertoire of ethnographies by employing
Lund wishes to reunite vision and touch a wider understanding and representation
through the kinaesthetic act of walking, of somatic sensations and sensibilities. Now
and her ethnography, interviews and own we consider further examples of sensuous,
autoethnographic descriptions aim to answer ethnographic investigations that exhibit
exactly how these modalities interact through potentially post-phenomenological ap-
the process of moving: proaches to embodied activities, approaches
that are potentially conducive to think-ing
Walking is a bodily movement that not only about non-representational theory.
connects the body to the ground but also
includes different postures, speeds, rhythms.
These shape the tactile interactions between IV Sensuous dispositions and
the moving body and the ground, and play a non-representational theory
fundamental part in how the surroundings are With an expanded and clarified sensory ter-
sensually experienced. (Lund, 2005: 28) minology discussed at the beginning of this
paper, researchers may start to articulate
Just as Rowles managed in his research to and describe the raft of somatic sensations
extol the possibilities of ‘intersubjective as they relate to the expanded sensorium.
encounter’ (Rowles, 1980: 57) through en- In this spirit, reminding ourselves of Stoller’s
gaging with another person’s lifeworld, earlier position on sensuous ethnography as
comparable research has been conducted being outside the sphere of individualistic or
by walking and talking with congenitally academic isolation, where bearing witness to
blind respondents (born blind) as well as ethnographic events is ‘to think new thoughts
those with adventitious blindness (becoming or feel new feelings’ (Stoller, 2004: 832),
blind after a period of sight). Macpherson something of the difficulty of articulating
found both blind groups equally forthcoming and representing previously unthought
about their experiences of touching with feelings and novel somatic sensations is now
the feet rather than the hands, correlating addressed. The following three examples
their haptic knowledge through the feet and are framed explicitly within the mode of the
Mark Paterson: Haptic geographies 779

non-representational, they testify to a certain technologies places different emphases on


ineffability and attempt to ‘grasp’ ongoing, the practice of conducting fieldwork. Un-
embodied experiences in different ways. doubtedly, in-depth ethnographic fieldwork
Most pertinently, what Lorimer (2005: 84) has – and does – address the taken-for-
identified as ‘sensuous dispositions’, or new granted, the felt unsaid, and sometimes the
sensibilities within non-representational ineffable or tacit knowledges that emerge
theory (see, for example, Thrift, 1997; 2000; through encounters with people, as we saw
2007). Lorimer’s (2005; 2007) series of with Rowles (1978; 1980).
papers in this journal offer an effective syn- In contrast to our earlier exposition of
optic introduction to this emerging area. Gibson’s haptic system and the phenomeno-
So I concentrate here on some concerns logical interest in kinaesthetic embodiment as
that cut across non-representational theory a form of anticipative motor intentionality,
(NRT), renewed interest in the somatic at this juncture we can firmly differentiate
senses within dance and performance, and sensations (that is, information routed via dis-
the shifting of the sensorium through new tributed nerves and sense-system clusters)
techniques and technologies. Ethnographic from sensuous dispositions (the sociohistoric-
methods have constantly been straining al construction of the sensorium, its repro-
towards the rethinking and rewriting of duction over time and its alteration through
embodiment, interaction, performance and contexts and technologies). This distinction
affect, and consequently address the sen- highlights the importance not only of the
sorium. As Thrift has written elsewhere, immediacy of conscious sensation and cuta-
the alteration of the human sensorium neous contact, then, but also the historically
through new technologies and techniques sedimented bodily dispositions and patterns
offers ‘changes in the way in which the body of haptic experience that become habituated
“talks” and is addressed’ (Thrift, 2004: 584). over time. What follows then is a sample of
Such changes entail a shifting register of geographical work that directly addresses
experience that is manifested through new the shifting, multiple, mutable sensorium
techniques of the body and performance within different fieldwork settings, variously
(for example, pilates, massage, the Five dealing with spaces of immediacy, proximity
Rhythms™ – see, for example, McCormack, and distance: first, the feeling of nudity on
2002) and new technologies (the human- the beach with concomitant sensations of
computer interface, ‘haptics’ – see, for ex- warmth on the skin, feeling at ease with an
ample, Paterson 2006a; 2007; 2008) that alternative bodily comportment; second,
require new means of interrogation that are to rethink the notion of touch as pertaining
sympathetic to the alterability of sensuous not only to presence and proximity, through
dispositions and somatic sensations. Since the experiences of some visually impaired
non-representational theory does not proffer visitors to a museum; and, third, we revisit
any single methodology, it propagates in- the embodied practice of walking in order to
stead a raft of creative approaches and tech- reflect on a more affective phenomenology
niques, rethinking the usefulness and purpose of bodies and landscape. These three cases
of empirical investigations through what decentre previous notions of haptic know-
Dewsberry et al. (2002: 440) term a ‘resolute ledge from the earlier discussion of the psy-
experimentalism’ in the ‘taking-place of chology of perception, allowing far greater
the empirical’, the folding of theory into the affective bearing upon such knowledges,
practices of fieldwork (p. 239). Certainly, shifting the focus from individualistic intro-
following Thrift’s phraseology, listening to spection to more current social and cultural
how bodies ‘talk’ and ‘are addressed’ with- concerns, and widening the scope of previous
in different contexts and through various discussions of grounded perception.
780 Progress in Human Geography 33(6)

1 The sensuous envelope of skin through traditional interview methods. In fact


Obrador-Pons (2007) uses the term ‘haptic this underlines the poverty not only of the
geographies’ to explore sensations and feel- somatosensory imagination but also of arti-
ings of nudity on the beach, limiting the culating sensuous dispositions. So how bet-
experience of the naked body neither to skin ter to ‘grasp’ such feelings and dispositions?
contact, the visualistic bias of the ‘gaze’, nor As McCormack writes in reference to the
to mere representation. Following Jay (1994) experience of kinaesthesia, the difficulty
he suggests not only an alternative ‘scopic lies in capturing the intensities of what is
regime’ in fact, but ‘a different order of the ‘often below the cognitive threshold of rep-
sensual’ so that he is interested instead ‘in resentational awareness that defines what is
the production of feelings and sensual dis- admitted into serious research’ (McCormack,
positions’ (Obrador-Pons, 2007: 124–25). 2002: 470) – the seemingly irresolvable
Following Radley’s (1995) work on the ‘elusory paradox of finding a means to articulate such
body’, he wishes to engage in empirical work experience without limiting it to mere repre-
that contests the usual discursive construc- sentation, without establishing yet another
tions of the body, attempting to re-engage lexicon, nor returning to the embodied sub-
with ‘an elusory and affectual body open to jectivity that is the starting point for the
the world that feels and senses’ (Obrador- phenomenological project.
Pons, 2007: 125). Nudity as an inevitably Our common conception of a cutaneous
embodied yet deeply countercultural prac- subject conveniently enveloped (limited)
tice (in the context of late modernity in by skin has no neuropsychological basis
the west, at least) is therefore concerned whatsoever, as we have argued. In terms of
not only with sensations but with ‘the cul- sensation, there is no simple inside and out-
tivation of sensibilities’ and, following Thrift, side. The distribution of nerves throughout
in being naked on the beach the ‘capacity of the body elides any neat distinction between
the body-subject to create non-denotative interoception and exteroception in the on-
meaning through its senses, movements and going nature of somatic experiences, and
tasks, as well as its ability to dwell in particu- consequently troubles the notion of the haptic
lar spaces and things’ (p. 128) is of interest. as clearly delimited within an individuated
Seen optimistically, nudity allows a height- body. The ongoing and multifarious nature
ened series of sensations, for example the of tangible interactions with the world open
gentle caress of warm sun on skin. But what up the haptic beyond cutaneous sensation
of that bane of British people abroad, the ex- (prosaic tactility), beyond the subjectively
cruciating pain of sunburn? Or the sense of coherent felt interaction of dispersed sensory
shame that goes below the blush of the skin, systems (Gibson’s haptic system), to include
the embarrassment that can be described sensuous dispositions that exceed anything
as making one’s skin crawl? Obrador-Pons’ we might posit as a subjectively felt body-
interviewees attempted to refer outside the space with a distinct interiority and exter-
realm of immediate cutaneous experience, iority. This must be borne in mind when we
but had difficulty articulating these feelings consider the remaining examples of this
with any precision (p. 130). Thus, while his section: first, acts of touching material ob-
research is a valiant attempt to refine the no- jects that collapse subject-object and inside-
tion of ‘haptic geographies’ after Rodaway’s outside distinctions, and then perceiving and
(1994: 41ff) initial coinage, his own research interacting with landscape as an aesthetic
is hampered by conceptual and methodo- (feeling) body. In both cases, we can posit, as
logical issues as he tries to grasp towards Merleau-Ponty (2000: 133) puts it, ‘becoming
understanding the co-implication of skin, a tangible being’ as a form of ‘muscular con-
touch, feeling and sensuous dispositions sciousness’ (Bachelard, in Ingold, 2004: 333)
Mark Paterson: Haptic geographies 781

that problematizes our intuitive or prephe- and the understanding of the interaction be-
nomenological conception of a felt bodily tween remaining senses alters the available
interior and exterior. perceptual feedback, significantly modifying
our experiences of place. To de-emphasize
2 Proximal and distal touch the optic and the centrality of sight in favour
Haptic knowledges are particularly signific- of reasserting the haptic is to validate other
ant in thinking about spatial experiences of forms of knowledge, and while we remember
the blind and visually impaired. Obviously the the popular expression ‘seeing is believing’, a
non-visual mode of routine wayfinding and forgotten corollary is ‘but feeling’s the truth’,
navigation involves spatializing the non- as Thomas Fuller reminds us in Gnomologia
visual senses. This is explored in some psy- (Fuller, 1732: 174; see also Bronner, 1982:
chology and geography research (eg, Butler 352, on haptic culture). For sighted and non-
and Bowlby, 1997; Paterson, 2006b; 2006c). sighted alike, this opening out of tactility
Yet the types of tactility assumed are often effectively asks ‘how geographical know-
underdeveloped, and insufficient attention is ledge might understand place as a proximal
paid to detailed somatic processes or haptic construction of touch’ (Hetherington, 2003:
knowledges in discussions concerning spatial 1936). Something of the experience of mingl-
encounters. One exception was Macpherson ing, of collapsing self and other, interiority
(2009), as we saw, bringing her body almost and exteriority, making the distanced more
empathetically to accompany her fellow proximate (and vice versa) is present in his
walkers, then to reflect on her own and her interviewing of a blind respondent who
respondents’ kinaesthetic experiences. From undergoes touch tours of museums:
Hetherington’s (2002; 2003) research on
visually impaired visitors to museums, one When I am touching something there is no
aspect in particular that emerges from inter- ‘me’ and the object I am touching. It is just
the object. So the me disappears … for me it’s
view data includes the contention that place
just touching, identifying with the actual thing
and touch can be understood as ‘proximal there … The way I touch is an identification
nonrepresentational forms of knowledge’ with something somewhere inside of you,
(Hetherington, 2003: 1938), acknowledging you have got a relationship with it. (‘Sarah’, in
the difficulty of theorizing tactile experience in Hetherington, 2003: 1934)
the blind and visually impaired. Hetherington
is discontent with Gibson’s explanations From such observations on touch, Hetherington
either in terms of haptic subsystems (Gibson, considers the set of ‘performative repertoires’
1968, as discussed above) or his familiar that his blind and partially sighted respondents
notion of ‘affordances’ (Gibson, 1986), since exhibit, including moving, touching, hearing,
both remain centred on individuated em- listening and speaking to others (p. 1935).
bodied experience and are therefore ‘socio- The experience of space can thereby be
logically weak’ (Hetherington, 2003: 1938). understood as ‘a decentred and partially con-
Instead, Hetherington sees tactility as a nected experience of the performing (and
‘performative’ rather than ‘representational’ performed) body’ (p. 1935). As we have seen,
form of knowledge since it is more proximal, haptic knowledges involve multiple relation-
opening out tactility from this ‘centred (hu- ships between the visual, the non-visual and
manist) subject’ (p. 1934). The historical em- the somatic senses. For Hetherington, touch
phasis on sight and the optic solidifies is only one possible focus for proximal and
perceptual ‘self’/’other’ boundaries between performative forms of knowledge in the
‘my’ body and others based on visual feed- making of place (p. 1936), and he similarly ac-
back and clearly identifiable visual representa- knowledges the importance of kinaesthesia.
tions, and entails ‘distal’ rather than ‘proximal’ In addition to performative repertoires,
knowledges. Therefore the troubling of sight haptic knowledges are usually indissociable
782 Progress in Human Geography 33(6)

from acts of memory, and the manifold asso- distance concerns more than mingling, then,
ciations between non-visual sensory experi- indicating a larger tactile-spatial imaginary
ences of objects, places and movements are that accommodates poetic insights of memory
outside the scope of this paper (although see and material proximity, further extending the
Seremetakis, 1994). For Hetherington, initial repertoire of haptic knowledges as a result.
interviews with visually impaired respondents
is the departure point for considerations of the 3 Pain and pleasure, sensuous depths and folds
non-representational role of tactility and It is important to recognize that not all haptic
the emergent non-visual productions of place feelings or somatic sensations are pleasant.
and, like Obrador-Pons (2007), he utilizes Other landscapes and bodies that deal with
empirical data in order to retheorize, in this more deranged sensations and environmental
case, place, touch and memory. Therefore sensitivities, damaged sensoria, or bodies of
relationships between touch and memory pain tell very distinct stories (eg, Scarry, 1985),
are played out through ideas of presence and and involve very different haptic knowledges
absence such that the footfalls and hand- as a result; and diseased, diseasing or simply
holds of an unfamiliar house, or the evocative aging bodies (as ethnographically investi-
and familiar sight and feel of his grand- gated by Rowles, 1978; 1980) remove the
father’s mug, is a touch that triggers involun- historically recognizable geographical trope
tary memories. In this there is an immediate of able body within accessible landscape.
and empathic sense of recognition in the Returning to cross-cultural anthropology,
reader. This tactile element of absent pres- Lynne Hume (2007: 82ff) describes the
ence he terms ‘praesentia’ (after Josipovici, effects of self-flagellation, body modification
1996, originally concerning medieval relics) and dance rituals involving bodily pain, scari-
where ‘something absent … can attain fication and mutilation that collectively
presence through the materiality of a thing’ imagines pain as a portal to spiritual dis-
(Hetherington, 2003: 1941). The tactile ap- covery. Furthermore, not all touch is ‘good’
propriation of familiar objects, calling ab- touch. While Obrador-Pons (2007) explored
sences into presences through praesentia the sensations of sun on the skin in his haptic
(his grandfather’s mug, an old teddy bear) is, geographies of the beach, especially for
like the blind respondents in the museum, more light-skinned western Europeans, a
‘about mingling: distance and proximity; pres- negative corollary is the uncomfortably hot
ence and absence; human and nonhuman; and unbearably tender sensation of sun-
subject and object; time and space; vision and burn. Likewise, in the previous discussion
touch’ (p. 1940). It consequently becomes of the resolutely middle-class ideal of the
somewhat theoretically overburdened. His leisurely landscapes of walking we should
insistence that touch is usually conceptualized also recognize the more unpleasant haptic
as ‘proximal’ – up close, specific, local to the sensations that accompany hard work and
body – seen as an exemplum of the perfor- back-breaking labour, especially when itiner-
mative blurring of boundaries between ant labour necessitated walking in inhospit-
objects, becomes nevertheless spatially com- able territories solely for economic survival
plexified by the ability to perform touch and (eg, Edensor, 2000). Such recognition of
presence at a distance, either through the ab- the bodily pain of movement, even within an
sent presence of praesentia itself, or through activity commonly recognized as worthy and
various haptic technologies, for example influenced by historical romanticism, is arti-
(eg, Paterson, 2006a; 2008), technologies culated in some recent cultural geography
which arose originally to aid blindness and that situates the body, pain and memory
visual impairment, appropriately enough. within the landscape. Yusoff, for example,
The ability to touch and be touched at a examines archival accounts of Mawson’s
Mark Paterson: Haptic geographies 783

polar expedition of 1912 which explicitly ad- immediacy of feeling folded into the landscape,
dress the tactile sensations of walking on blurring the distinctness of the boundaries
uneven snow in reindeer-skin boots, where between the romantic ideal of the solitary
‘you get a sense of touch which nothing else walker (the optic, the distanced) and the more
except bare feet would give you’ (Yusoff, quotidian ‘ache, ennui and enervation’ (p. 242)
2007: 227) and thereby a sensitivity to minor of muddy trudging, muscular tiredness and
variations in the surface underfoot, yet at footholds and handholds (the haptic, the
other points the most excruciating bodily pain, proximal). Furthermore, something of the in-
humiliating conditions and self-mutilation due tertwining of the muscular consciousness of
to climatic conditions, malnutrition, madness the walker and the surrounding geomorph-
and disease. In more current and less relatively ology of slopes, peaks and scarps is achieved
harrowing practices of walking, Wylie’s by straining towards a more poetic, expres-
more literary approach accommodates the sive and performative means of expressing
sensuous folds of both pain and pleasure into such haptic knowledges.
the landscape. To some observers this approach might
On his walks to Glastonbury Tor and the stray towards the questionable romanticism
South West Coast Path, Wylie (2002; 2005) of an aesthetic of the sublime, but Wylie
employs a literary style that attempts to en- rejects the simplistic notion of the mutual em-
compass the sensuous depths and folds of bedding of body and landscape and instead
body within the surrounding landscape, in- calls for ‘specific walking corporealities and
fluenced somewhat by Alphonso Lingis’ and sensibilities’ that accommodate affects along
W.G. Sebald’s philosophical travelogues, with sensations (p. 236), making a bid for
simultaneously open to sensuous dispositions that ‘resolute experimentalism’ of methods
yet also to the immediate physicality of pain alluded to earlier, and employing experimental
and muscular strain. At one point he describes writing methods to address both ‘critical and
the rhythm and foot-heavy trudge along a creative registers’ (p. 237) in so doing. Building
pathway as obscuring the assumed goal of the on earlier sections, this type of writing works
walk, how ‘the habitual ambient resonance towards a more affectual phenomenology of
of depths and surfaces distils into knees, hips perception, able to employ a raft of critical, as
and shoulderblades’ (Wylie, 2002: 449). well as creative, techniques in order to reach
Later he describes the vertiginous feeling of towards those corporeal sensibilities and
reaching the peak of the Tor, the assumption haptic knowledges, and which include a range
of the predominance of visibility from a of affects and somatic sensations such as
vantage point over the surrounding fields, pain, weariness, movement, vertigo, bodily
tempered by a visceral series of sensations bearing, assurance, jouissance, rhythm, rest,
attuned to the dramatic depths and folds of trudge-heavy joy or exhausted openness to
the landscape: ‘On the summit this reflexive the landscape that surrounds.
recognition of visibility is sensuous, felt around
the torso’s twists and neck craning to gaze at V Conclusions: writing and the
the sky’ (p. 452). On another trudge within feeling of doing
a different landscape, finding himself liter- To conclude, I now point towards three
ally in the thick of it, he says: ‘Limbs and lungs brief methodological implications for future
working hard in a haptic, step-by-step engage- fieldwork involving haptic geographies. As
ment with nature-matter. Landscape be- we have seen, previous considerations of
coming foothold’ (Wylie, 2005: 239). Partly haptic knowledges in fieldwork have usually
phenomenological, the perception of land- involved an extension of traditional reflexive
scape as a series of paths, transitions, foot- ethnography, or more recently a ‘resolute
holds and restpoints speaks to an embodied experimentalism’ in writing and grappling
784 Progress in Human Geography 33(6)

with sensuous dispositions. But, to return employed phenomenological methods (see,


to the problems in the opening paragraph, for example, Paterson, 2007: 26). The real
that gap between experiencing the feeling problem here is the assumption that there
body and expressing it, this is an appropriate are identifiable sensations ‘out there’ (or,
point to ask how exactly can researchers conversely, felt ‘in here’) reportable in this
attend to haptic geographies, to take account way. As discussed previously, the internal/
of somatic senses such as kinaesthesia, for external spatial differentiation of the body
example, and write about it? Can this be ac- is not always so simple. However, fieldwork
hieved through a combination of traditional can still benefit from the standardization of
and non-traditional methods, involving com- terms. This was partly demonstrated in the
binations of interviewing, participant obser- section ‘Haptic knowledges’, where terms
vation, and sensuous autoethnography for like kinaesthesia, proprioception and the ves-
example? What are the implications for rec- tibular sense were succinctly explained, and
ording technologies such as video, which revisited in various combinations within later
remain multisensory only in so far as they examinations of fieldwork. It contributes to
record video and audio, say? What are ap- the repertoire of terms for the examination
propriate methods for recording somatic and redescription of somatic experience.
sensations of proprioception, kinaesthesia Second, and somewhat contradictorily,
and vestibular sensations, or their equivalents some of the touchy-feely methods discussed
in non-western cultures? Is writing the most so far are irreducible to a standardized som-
suitable form of dissemination of these atosensory lexicon. As this paper has pro-
somatic experiences? Is there, in other words, gressed, an increasing awareness of the
a future agenda for conceptualizing and manifold conjunctions of sensuous experience,
applying haptic geographies? Here I tender receptivity, interiority and exteriority, and
three implications for fieldwork that address the blurring of the boundaries of the subject
each of the sections of this paper. have complicated this, and I have argued
First, a fairly straightforward ‘reporting for gaining a more detailed appreciation of
back’ of bodily sensations through an intro- the sensuous potentialities within (and of)
spective method remains possible, especially ethnographic research. In other words, we
suited to the earlier psychological termino- have shifted focus from seemingly natural-
logies of Gibson’s haptic system, and cer- istic ‘sensations’ to more complex, enfolded
tainly existing ethnographic techniques sensuous ‘dispositions’. As previously shown,
are being augmented by taking account this is consistent with certain approaches of
of sensory-somatic experience, such that non-representational theory, which con-
ethnographers ‘can examine and expand ceptualizes the body as ‘sensuous, sensitive,
their own practice, so as to consider their own agentive and expressive in relation to the
body and senses more fully as part of their world, knowing and innovating among con-
ethnographic toolkit’, as Bendix (2005: 3) texts and representations that become
posits. Reflecting upon, exploring and writing refigured in practice’, says Crouch (2001: 62,
one’s own sensory experiences into ethno- echoing Csordas, my emphasis). In this case,
graphic accounts is akin to an honest form of the attempt to examine and explore the ‘feel-
Cartesian introspection which identifies and ing of doing’ in embodied fieldwork practice,
clarifies complex combinations of somatic continues Crouch, is ‘a means of grasping
sensations. This method remains the basis the world and making sense of what it feels
for the later ‘descriptive psychology’ of like’. Now, ‘making sense’ involves imposing
Brentano that forms the basis for Husserl’s some order out of the chaos of the body
(1970) phenomenology. While Hetherington in the ‘field’, the physically felt and barely
would characterize this as sociologically articulable sensations that arise through
weak, other social theorists have successfully embodied performances and practices within
Mark Paterson: Haptic geographies 785

the place of fieldwork. It signifies an appar- is testament to this. In part this is achievable
ently prediscursive ‘expressive and sensuous through the power of metaphor, and the
engagement with space’, an engagement English language is full of sensory and even
that is inherently embodied and variously haptic metaphors (eg, ‘ponder’ comes from
aware of the repertoire of senses and haptic Latin ponderare, to weigh). Poetry is full of
knowledges, made as it is by subjects already sensory conjunctions achieved through simile
embedded within a social world; we subjects and metaphor, and allows an alternative
who, as Crouch (2001: 70) puts it, ‘reflexively pathway to the sensory ‘reporting back’ de-
and discursively refigure [our] sensuous/ scribed above. Rhythm, the folding of sen-
expressive and poetic encounters’. More sations, creativity in expression, and the
pertinently, grasping towards that ‘feeling use of sensory similes and metaphors can all
of doing’ within fieldwork requires a more enhance an ‘ethnographer’s toolkit’, make a
supple awareness of the repertoire of haptic sensuous ethnography into a creative one,
knowledges, including sensuous dispositions and make a creative ethnography out of
and the troubling of traditionally imagined sensuous experiences.
spatial relations of interiority and exteriority,
distance and proximity, and sensations per se. Acknowledgements
Third, then, the difficulty of evoking or This article is dedicated in loving memory
representing complex somatic sensations, to my mother Jennifer, whose own ordeal
and their potential irreducibility to a standard with pain and immobility in the last few years
somatosensory lexicon, entails different ways finally ended when she died on 30 August
of linguistically engaging with the haptic 2008. Many thanks go to Ian Cook, who was
experiences involved. Linguistic limitations unreservedly encouraging and supportive
point to the recursive difficulties of tran- of this project throughout its gestation, and
scribing one set of sensations into another also knows about pain. Thanks also to my
language, whether that ‘language’ be meta- colleagues in the Historical and Cultural
phorical (from another discipline like psych- Geography research group at the University
ology or anthropology), or descriptively literal of Exeter, who commented on an early draft
(from a non-English-speaking culture, eg, and provided the necessary time and space
seselelame). However, this is where a poetic for discussion.
sensibility meets a sensuous disposition, for
evoking and describing sensuous dispositions Notes
and haptic knowledges benefits from the 1. ‘The dancer’s body’, programme 1 – ‘A machine that
dances’. BBC Two, transmitted 21 September 2002.
styles and methods involved in experimental 2. ‘Sensory formations’ is a series of edited collections,
or creative writing. There are glimpses of this including Classen’s The book of touch (2005), Howes’
in Stoller, Lingis and Wylie, for example, and Empire of the senses (2005), Bull and Back’s The
one can point to the work of Muecke and auditory culture reader (2003), Korsmeyer’s The taste
so-called fictocriticism (eg, Muecke, 2002) culture reader (2005) and Drobnick’s The smell culture
reader (2006), and will culminate in Edwards and
as a creative engagement with language that Bhaumik’s forthcoming Visual sense (2009).
evokes rather than describes and, like Lingis, 3. Also Howes’ group project ‘Multi-Sensory Market-
encompasses aspects of private experience ing: A Quantitative, Qualitative and Historical
such as memory, loss and shame, attempting Assessment’ (2005–2008), funded by Canada’s
to convey complex politics, histories, moods Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council.
and sensations through more explorative
and expressive language. Good ethnographic References
Aristotle 1984: Sense and sensibilia (De sensu et
work already achieves this, and Geurts’ sensibilibus; translated by J.I. Beare). In Ross, W.D.,
(2002) enfolded analyses of gait, posture, editor, Aristotle: works, volume II, Princeton:
movement and idiom in the Anlo-Ewe people Princeton University Press, 693–713.
786 Progress in Human Geography 33(6)

Aristotle 1986: De anima on the soul (translated by Feld, S. 2005: Places sensed, senses placed. In Howes, D.,
H. Lawson-Tancred). London: Penguin. editor, Empire of the senses: the sensual culture reader,
Bendix, R. 2005: Introduction: ear to ear, nose to nose, Oxford: Berg, 179–91.
skin to skin – the senses in comparative ethnographic Fowler, C.J. 2003: Review of Cameron, O.G., Vis-
perspective. Special Issue, ‘The Anthropology of the ceral sensory neuroscience: interoception. Brain 1266,
Senses’. Etnofoor XVIII1, 3–14. 1505–506.
Boring, E.G., Langfeld, H.S. and Weld, H.P. 1948: Fuller, T. 1732: Gnomologia: adagies and proverbs, wise
Foundations of psychology. New York: Wiley. sentences and witty sayings. London: Barker, and
Bronner, S.J. 1982: The Haptic Experience of Culture. Bettesworth and Hitch.
Anthropos 773, 351–62. Geurts, K.L. 2002: Culture and the senses: embodiment,
Bull, M. and Back, L., editors 2003: The auditory identity, and well-being in an African community.
culture reader. Oxford: Berg. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Butler, R. and Bowlby, S. 1997: Bodies and spaces: — 2005: Consciousness as feeling in the body. In
an exploration of disabled people’s experiences of Howes, D., editor, Empire of the senses: the sensual
public space. Environment and Planning D: Society culture reader, Oxford: Berg, 164–78.
and Space 15, 411–33. Gibson, J.J. 1962: Observations on active touch. Psy-
Buttimer, A. and Seamon, D. 1980: The human chological Review 69, 477–91.
experience of place and space. London: Croom Helm. — 1966: The senses considered as perceptual systems.
Classen, C. 1993: Worlds of sense: exploring the senses in Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
history and across culture. London: Routledge. — 1968: On the difference between perception and pro-
— 1997: Foundations for an anthropology of the senses. prioception. The ‘Purple Perils’ of James J. Gibson.
International Social Science Journal 153, 401–12. Unpublished notes. Retrieved 26 January 2009 from
— 1998: The color of angels: cosmology, gender and the http://huwi.org/gibson/difference.php
aesthetic imagination. London: Routledge. — 1986: The ecological approach to visual perception.
—, editor 2005: The book of touch. Oxford: Berg. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Cloke, P., Cook, I., Crang, P., Goodwin, M., Hetherington, K. 2002: The unsightly: visual impair-
Painter, J. and Philo, C. 2004: Practising human ment, touch and the Parthenon frieze. Theory, Culture
geographies. London: Sage. and Society 195–6, 187–205.
Cole, J. 1995: Pride and a daily marathon. London: MIT — 2003: Spatial textures: place, touch, and praesentia.
Press. Environment and Planning A 3511, 1933–44.
Corbin, A. 1986: The foul and the fragrant: odor and the Howes, D. 2003: Sensual relations: engaging the senses in
French social imagination. Leamington Spa: Berg. culture and social theory. Ann Arbor, MI: University
— 2005: Charting the cultural history of the senses. In of Michigan Press.
Howes, D., editor, Empire of the senses: the sensual —, editor 2005: Empire of the senses: the sensual culture
culture reader, Oxford: Berg, 128–42. reader. Oxford: Berg.
Crang, M. 2003: Qualitative methods: touchy, feely, Hume, L. 2007: Portals: opening doorways to other
look-see? Progress in Human Geography 274, realities through the senses. Oxford: Berg.
494–504. Husserl, E. 1970: The crisis of European sciences and
Crouch, D. 2001: Spatialities and the feeling of doing. transcendental phenomenology (translated by D. Carr).
Social and Cultural Geography 21, 61–75. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
Csordas, T.J. 1993: Somatic modes of attention. Cul- Ingold, T. 2000: The perception of the environment: essays
tural Anthropology 8, 135–56. on livelihood, dwelling and skill. London: Routledge.
Dewsberry, J.D., Harrison, P., Rose, M. and — 2004: Culture on the ground: the world perceived
Wylie, J. 2002: Introduction: enacting geographies. through the feet. Journal of Material Culture 93,
Geoforum 33, 437–40. 315–40.
Downey, G. 2002: Listening to Capoeira: phenomen- Jay, M. 1994: Downcast eyes: the denigration of vision
ology, embodiment, and the materiality of music. in twentieth century french thought. Berkeley, CA:
Ethnomusicology 46, 487–509. University of California Press.
— 2005: Learning Capoeira: lessons in cunning from an Jones, C.A., editor 2007: Sensorium: embodied
Afro-Brazilian art. Oxford: Oxford University Press. experience, technology, and contemporary art. London:
Drobnick, J., editor 2006: The smell culture reader. MIT Press.
Oxford: Berg. Josipovici, G. 1996: Touch: an essay. London: Yale
Edensor, T. 2000: Walking in the British countryside: University Press.
reflexivity, embodiment and ways to escape. Body Koffka, K. 1935: Principles of Gestalt psychology.
and Society 6, 81–106. London: Lund Humphries.
Edwards, E. and Bhaumik, K., editors 2009: Visual Korsmeyer, C., editor 2005: The taste culture reader:
sense: a cultural reader. Oxford: Berg, in press. experiencing food and drink. Oxford: Berg.
Mark Paterson: Haptic geographies 787

Lackner, J.R. and DiZio, P. 2005: Vestibular, proprio- Paterson, M. 2007: The senses of touch: haptics, affects
ceptive, and haptic contributions to spatial orienta- and technologies. Oxford: Berg.
tion. Annual Review of Psychology 56, 115–47. — 2008: Digital craft, digital touch: haptics and design. In
Law, L. 2001: Home cooking: Filipino women and Hawk, B., Rieder, D. and Oviedo, O., editors, Small
geographies of the senses in Hong Kong. Cultural tech: the culture of digital tools, Minneapolis, MN:
Geographies 83, 264–83. University of Minnesota Press.
Leder, D. 1990: The absent body. Chicago: University of — 2009: Remediating touch. Editorial in special issue
Chicago Press. ‘Re-mediating touch’. The Senses and Society 2(3),
— 2005: Visceral perception. In Classen, C., editor, The in press.
book of touch, Oxford: Berg, 335–41. Pocock, D. 1993: The senses in focus. Area 25, 11–16.
Longhurst, R., Ho, E. and Johnston, L. 2008: Using Porteous, J.D. 1985: Smellscape. Progress in Human
‘the body’ as an ‘instrument of research’: kim’chi and Geography 93, 356–78.
pavlova. Area 402, 208–17. — 1986: Intimate Sensing. Area 183, 250–51.
Lorimer, H. 2005: Cultural geography: the busyness Radley, A. 1995: The elusory body and social con-
of being ‘more-than-representational’. Progress in structionist theory. Body and Society 1(2), 3–23.
Human Geography 29, 83–94. Ram, K. 2005: Phantom limbs: South Indian dance and
— 2007: Cultural geography: worldly shapes, differently immigrant reifications of the female body. Journal of
arranged. Progress in Human Geography 31, 89–100. Intercultural Studies 261–2, 121–37.
Lund, K. 2005: Seeing in motion and the touching eye: Reed, E.S. and Jones, R.K. 1979: James Gibson’s
walking over Scotland’s mountains. Etnofoor 181, ecological revolution in psychology. Philosophy of the
27–42. Social Sciences 92, 189–204.
Macpherson, H.M. 2009: Touch in the countryside: Revill, G. 1998: Samuel Coleridge-Taylor’s geography
memory and visualization through the feet. The of disappointment: hybridity, identity and networks
Senses and Society 33, in press. of musical meaning. In Leyshon, A., Matless, D.
Mauss, M. 1992: Techniques of the body. In Crary, J. and Revill, G., editors, The place of music, London:
and Kwinter, S., editors, Zone 6:incorporations, New Routledge, 197–221.
York: Zone. — 2005: Vernacular culture and the place of folk music.
McCormack, D.P. 2002: A paper with an interest in Social and Cultural Geography 6, 693–706.
rhythm. Geoforum 33, 469–85. Rodaway, P. 1994: Sensuous geographies: body, sense
Merleau-Ponty, M. 1992 [1945]: The phenomenology and place. London: Routledge.
of perception (translated by C. Smith). London: Roe, E.J. 2006: Things becoming food and the em-
Routledge. bodied, material practices of an organic food con-
— 2000: The visible and the invisible (seventh edition; sumer. Sociologia Ruralis 462, 104–21.
translated by A. Lingis; edited by C. Lefort). Rowles, G.D. 1978: Reflections on experiential field-
Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press. work. In Ley, D. and Samuels, M., editors, Humanistic
Mountcastle, V.B. 2005: The sensory hand: neural geography: prospects and problemsi, New York:
mechanisms of somatic sensation. Cambridge MA: Wiley, 173–93.
Harvard University Press. — 1980: Toward a geography of growing old. In Buttimer,
Muecke, S. 2002: The fall: fictocritical writing. Parallax A. and Seamon, D., editors, The human experience of
8(4), 108–12. space and place, London: Croom Helm, 55–72.
Obrador-Pons, P. 2007: A haptic geography of the Scarry, E. 1985: The body in pain: the making and un-
beach: naked bodies, vision and touch. Social and making of the world. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cultural Geography 81, 123–41. Seremetakis, N. 1994: The senses still: perception and
Paterson, M. 2005a: Affecting touch: towards a felt memory as material culture in modernity. Oxford:
phenomenology of therapeutic touch. In Davidson, J., Westview Press.
Bondi, L. and Smith, M., editors, Emotional geo- Serres, M. and Latour, B. 1995: Conversations on
graphies, Aldershot: Ashgate, 161–76. science, culture and time (translated by R. Lapilus).
— 2005b: Consumption and everyday life. London: Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.
Routledge. Sherrington, C. 1947: The integrative action of the ner-
— 2006a: Feel the presence: technologies of touch and vous system. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
distance. Environment and Planning D: Society and Smith, S. 1994: Soundscape. Area 26, 232–40.
Space 245, 691–708. Smith, S. 2000: Performing the soundworld. Environ-
Paterson, M. 2006b: ‘Seeing with the hands’: blindness, ment and Planning D: Society and Space 18, 615–37.
touch and the Enlightenment spatial imaginary. Stoller, P. 1989: The taste of ethnographic things: the
British Journal of Visual Impairment 242, 52–60. senses in anthropology. Philadelphia, PA: Temple
— 2006c: Seeing with the hands, touching with the University Press.
eyes: vision, touch and the Enlightenment spatial — 1997: Sensuous scholarship. Philadelphia, PA:
imaginary. The Senses and Society 12, 224–42. University of Pennsylvania Press.
788 Progress in Human Geography 33(6)

Stoller, P. 2004: Sensuous ethnography, African Tuan, Y.-F. 1974: Topophilia: a study of environmental
persuasions, and social knowledge. Qualitative perception, attitudes, and values. Englewood Cliffs,
Inquiry 106, 817–35. NJ: Prentice Hall.
Straus, E.W. 1966: Phenomenological psychology: the — 1977: Space and place: the perspective of experience.
selected papers of Erwin W. Straus (translated by London: Edward Arnold.
Erling Eng). London: Tavistock. van den Berg, J.H. 1952: The human body and the
Thrift, N. 1997: The still point: resistance, embodiment significance of human movement: A phenomenological
and dance. In Pile, S. and Keith, M., editors, Geo- study. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research
graphies of resistance, London: Routledge, 134–152. 132, 159–83.
— 2000: Afterword. Environment and Planning D: Wylie, J. 2002: An essay on ascending Glastonbury
Society and Space 18, 213–255. Tor. Geoforum 33, 441–54.
— 2004: Movement-space: the changing domain of — 2005: A single day’s walking: narrating self and
thinking resulting from the development of new landscape on the South West Coast Path. Transac-
kinds of spatial awareness. Economy and Society 334, tions of the Institute of British Geographers NS 30,
582–604. 234–47.
Thrift, N. 2007: Non-representational theory: space, Yusoff, K. 2007: Antarctic exposure: archives of the
politics, affect. London: Routledge. feeling body. Cultural Geographies 14, 211–33.

You might also like