You are on page 1of 11

The Full-Term and

Premature Newborn
& & & & & & & & & & & & & &

The Influence of Auditory Experience on the Behavior of Preterm


Newborns
M. Kathleen Philbin, RN, PhD which the infant is exposed. Unless stated otherwise, nursery noise
must be assumed to be a feature of that environment in all studies
conducted in nurseries. The assumption is reasonable because high
This review of the research literature is addressed to clinicians who care for, levels of nursery noise have been documented extensively over the
interact with, and advise parents of preterm newborns. In summary, past thirty years and there are no known reports of quiet hospital
research on the effects of sound on preterm infant behavior and nurseries.1 ± 19 This review of the literature on the effects of auditory
development provide only a little reliable clinical guidance as many of the experience of preterm infants begins, therefore, with a summary of
studies are flawed, some badly. Some studies use a high sound level (i.e., reports of the ambient acoustic environment of hospital nurseries and
loud) stimulus yet fail to report effects of the stimulus on vital signs, a summary of attempts at noise abatement.
movement, and behavioral state. Only a handful of studies report long term Documentation of nursery noise comes from North America,
effects regardless of the length of exposure to the experimental stimulus. A Europe, Scandinavia, India, Asia, and Australia. The authors'
consistent problem is that the ambient sound in the nursery research setting perspectives represent the disciplines of audiology, child life,
is neither described nor considered as a facet of the auditory stimulus yet all education, engineering (biomedical, electrical, mechanical),
reports of ambient nursery sound show high levels without respite periods medicine (neonatology, neurology, otolaryngology, and psychiatry),
quiet. Therefore, unless other documentation is provided, ambient nursery nursing, psychology (perception, child development, clinical,
noise is likely an unreported confounding variable in these studies. The comparative, behavioral), and physiology. In a nutshell, nursery
clinician is cautioned, therefore, to carefully evaluate any finding before room and incubator noises are shown to be generally loud and
implementing an intervention program of sound stimulation with preterm chaotic, lacking in pattern or rhythm.
infants. There are many reports of efforts to reduce nursery noise by The motivations for investigating nursery noise have varied. Long
changing staff behavior but none describes more than marginal success. and his colleagues noticed changes in measurements of cerebral
Given this general failure of noise reduction through behavior change an blood flow accompanying noise bursts.10 A number of investigators
alternative is proposed of achieving quiet by changing the crowded, worried about the contribution of nursery noise to hearing loss.6,19
reverberant nature of the physical space. Recommendations for care that GaÈdeke et al.14 observed disturbances in infant sleep. Philbin et al.20
can be drawn from this literature include measuring and reducing nursery wondered if the noise was associated with preterm infants' difficulties
noise, facilitating parents' efforts to talk and sing to their own babies, and with habituation.
limiting purposefully added sound stimulation to quite specific clinical Figures 1 and 2 display published sound levels for nurseries and
situations. incubators. Some published levels are not included here because
Journal of Perinatology 2000; 20:S76 ± S86. those data appear to be averages of individual measurements. (For
the error involved in taking arithmetic averages of nonlinear
numbers, see Gray.21 ) Recalling that each raise of 6 dBA represents a
perceived doubling of loudness, the nurseries are seen to be
NURSERY NOISE: CHARACTERISTICS AND ABATEMENT
remarkably dissimilar; the most noisy is about 64 times louder than
STRATEGIES
the most quiet. The most quiet nursery is in Lund, Sweden with a
Reports of Nursery Noise
mean level of about 38 dBA in regular working conditions with
To understand the influence of auditory experience on preterm
several incubators running.1 By comparison, a typical home bedroom
newborns it is necessary to account for the acoustic environment to
is about 30 to 40 dBA at night, depending on the type of heating or air
conditioning system. The other A-weighted levels in the figure can be
compared with this lowest level, therefore, and can be understood as a
University of Texas - Houston Medical School, Houston, TX.
multiple of the loudness of the home bedroom at night.
This literature review was supported by grant # 1 RO3 NR03396 - 01 from the National Institute The well-documented contribution of low frequencies to nursery
of Nursing Research and by the University of Texas Ð Houston Departments of Pediatrics and
Otolaryngology. sound is illustrated by the higher C-scale/linear measurement by
Address correspondence and reprint requests to M. Kathleen Philbin, RN, PhD, Department of
Blennow et al.1 for the same room. The 15- to 20-dB differences in
Pediatrics, Otolaryngology Laboratory, Rm. 6.133, 6431 Fannin Street, Houston, TX 77030. levels between the A- and C-weighting scales show large
Journal of Perinatology 2000; 20:S76 ± S86
# 2000 Nature America Inc. All rights reserved. 0743-8346/00 $15
S76 www.nature.com/jp
Influence of Auditory Experience on Preterm Newborns Philbin

Figure 1. Nursery sound levels reported in the literature. Nurseries are shown to be remarkably dissimilar in sound or noise levels. The nursery with the highest
A- weighted sound level ( 75 dBA ) would be perceived as about 64 times more noisy than nursery with the lowest A- weighted level (38 dBA ) . However, as levels
may be even greater inside the incubator (Figure 2 ) measurements of local conditions are needed to inform clinical decisions about housing for the individual
infant.

Figure 2. Incubator sound levels reported in the literature. As perceived loudness doubles every 6 to 10 dB, sound or noise levels inside incubators are shown to
vary considerably. The higher C-weighted /linear levels indicate significant low - frequency components to the noise. Motor design, age, condition, and
cleanliness all determine levels generated by the incubator itself whereas the integrity of seals and contact with the shell determine levels introduced from
outside. An infant's cry can markedly raise interior levels and yet not be heard on the outside.
Journal of Perinatology 2000; 20:S76 ± S86 S77
Philbin Influence of Auditory Experience on Preterm Newborns

contribution of low-frequency sounds to both nursery and incubator sources. Ultimately, there were no noise sources remaining other
environments. than the behaviors of staff themselves. (The contribution of building
Comparing the levels in incubators with those in the nursery, it is acoustics was not considered.) At Boston City Hospital the program to
clear that incubators do not necessarily provide a more benign reduce noise was incorporated into a larger, ongoing infant
auditory environment. Sound inside the incubator may involve development program that emphasized staff education in tailoring
considerable masking and abrupt loud sounds caused by objects care to each infant's tolerance for stimulation.25 In Madison, WI
striking the plastic walls and port doors snapping shut.4,22 sound pressures were measured over the course of a clinical
Additionally, the baby compartment of an incubator may be very intervention project to improve infant developmental outcomes.31
loud when the infant cries because the closed, reverberant container Staff education consisted of a lecture and reading materials and
can function like an amplifier.6 The advantage of Blennow's quiet consultation from a nurse clinician and occupational therapist.
Swedish nursery, therefore, is lost to the infants inside the incubators Although some aspects of the infant's hospital course were improved
because, at 60 to 63 dBA, they are more noisy than the room itself. In the sound pressures were not reduced.
this nursery, therefore, there is good reason for moving infants out of Several groups have attempted to reduce sound pressures by
the incubator earlier rather than later and managing temperature instituting day±night cycling of lights or by limited quiet periods.
stability with clothing and blankets (Blennow G, personal Mann et al.32 for example, report a program to reduce light and noise
communication, 1996). However, in the noisy Vermont nursery for twelve hours each night in their hospital in Nottingham,
measured by Long et al.10 the incubator, although noisy itself, is England. The noise-reduction method was to turn off the radio and
relatively less noisy than the nursery room. ``urge'' staff and visitors ``to make as little noise as possible.'' They
These data suggest that clinicians need to know the parameters of report a 10-dB reduction in sound pressures at night but do not
their own nurseries' acoustic environments and improve conditions report the actual pressures. Strauch et al.26 introduced a quiet hour
in both incubators and rooms accordingly. The acoustic environment once in each 8-hour work shift after a period of planning, staff
of the individual infant depends on the ambient sound levels of the education, and implementation. In their protocol physician rounds,
nursery, on the type and condition of the incubator's motor and seals, the movement of large equipment and phone ringing were excluded
on the presence in the near environment of respiratory equipment, during quiet hour. Staff were urged to whisper at the bedside, to
and on the baby's own behavior. refrain from slamming, banging, and dragging equipment, and to
rearrange nonurgent caregiving to another time. Lighting was also
Abatement of Nursery Noise reduced. Mean sound pressures for the same times of day were
A dozen or more authors have published their efforts to lessen noise reduced from 50 to 78 dBA before the program to 40 to 65 dBA after it
produced by equipment and people in nurseries.23 ± 26 Vidyasagar et was in effect for 2 weeks. This translates to a reduction in perceived
al.27 exemplify the succinct statement, with data, in a classic letter to loudness of a half to a quarter below pre-program levels. Infant sleep
the editor. Gottfried et al. 7,28,29 conducted a lengthy teach-in on the states were also significantly different between the control period and
consequences to social development through a variety of letters, the quiet hour. Light sleep and deep sleep increased from 33.9% to
articles, and book chapters. Lawson et al.,13 writing separately and in 84.5% and crying decreased from 14.3% to 2.4%. Parents and staff
teams, and also Linn et al.,30 likewise described the potential of noisy believed there were other advantages to the quiet hour in terms of
nurseries for bad effects on infants' social development. GaÈdeke et reduced stress and increased opportunities for uninterrupted activities.
al.14 made painstakingly precise measurements of the thresholds for The program did not, however, address the noise levels occurring
noise that disturbed infants' sleep and followed this up with equally during the remaining times of day.
painstaking measurements of the nursery to show that these wake- Several investigators have taken an alternate approach of blocking
up conditions existed in their own facility. Peltzman et al.,12 among sound at the infant's ear. Clinicians should use caution with ear-
the most novel, rigged a bed so that an adult could lay with his head covering devices, however, as they attenuate only some of the
in an incubator for several hours and then tested hearing to show frequencies that affect infants, require a tight seal to the skin all
temporary threshold shifts (i.e., temporary hearing loss). Using the around the ear, and can cause skin breakdown. Baer et al.33 studied
precise methods and calculations of an engineer, Ciesielski et al.17 the effect of ear covers on vital signs of 10 low-birth-weight
dissected incubator noise down to small frequency bands to show newborns before, during, and after a glass bottle was placed on the
exactly where the noise was loudest on the spectrum and then related top of the incubator. There were no effects on heart or respiratory rate
the sounds to specific metabolic and cardiovascular disturbances. changes but there were significantly fewer episodes of oxygen
Programs that combine staff education about noise with desaturation. To maintain the ear-cover seal and protect skin
exhortation to be quiet have had limited or temporary success.10,26,31 integrity, however, data collection was limited to 1 hour and infants
Long et al.10 used a self-correcting approach that involved the staff in were positioned supine. Zahr 34 reports a similarly designed study in
identifying noise sources. Noisy equipment was subsequently repaired, which infants were observed for four 2-hour periods on each of two
moved, or adapted to be more quiet. The nursery was then re- consecutive days. The sound stimulus was unmeasured, ambient
measured, found to be noisy yet, and the staff again identified noise nursery activity. Infants maintained higher and less variable blood
S78 Journal of Perinatology 2000; 20:S76 ± S86
Influence of Auditory Experience on Preterm Newborns Philbin

oxygen saturations when wearing the ear covers, but the use of afterwards. Once in operation the post renovation L min for the NICU
supplemental oxygen is not reported. They also had less frequent was 38 to 45 dBA. With the unit fully occupied, the pressure at 1000
behavioral state changes, longer periods of sleep, and more total Hz at the bedside during a medical emergency was 52 dBA. The
sleep. However, there were no controls for the effects of nursing-care overall effect of changes in the building, therefore, was to
differences as a consequence of the unmasked study conditions. dramatically reduce the amount of loud sound and to greatly
Although hearing protection may have promise for brief periods or increase the amount and degree of quiet.
for specific events, the effects of continuous or prolonged use on In summary, nurseries are reported to be very noisy and attempts
behavioral organization skin integrity, posture, and head shape at making them more quiet by changing staff behavior appear to be
remain unstudied. The effect of long-term masking on speech only marginally successful. Ear coverings to protect individual
discrimination also warrants investigation. infants from the noise are little studied and pose potential problems
In summary, the evidence from both the literature and common for the infants. Until recently, however, the literature has paid little
experience is that behavior change alone is inadequate for abatement attention to the contribution of the physical space to creating
and control of nursery noise. In fact nurseries unlike some other conditions predisposed to high noise levels. Increased space at the
healthcare, business, and residential environments have not been bedside, sound absorbent surface materials, and engineered
designed for quiet. Rather the traditional design priorities of acoustical control may provide successfully in reducing nursery noise
conserving space and infection control have inadvertently determined for the future.
that nurseries would be noisy due to crowding and highly reverberant
surfaces. These conditions provide ongoing disincentives to the
human effort to maintain quiet. EFFECTS OF NURSERY NOISE ON INFANT SLEEP
Traditional design priorities, however, may not satisfy con- Studies of the effects of noise on infant sleep use term infants as
temporary program requirements. New economic and social forces subjects. These studies are reviewed briefly here, however, because
favor priorities for increased space for family privacy and comfort at preterm infants can be expected to show the same or greater
the bedside.35 Further, new information about infant development sensitivity to sound as term infants even at term gestational age.
and stable physiology argues for environments that attract parents to GaÈdeke et al.14 studied the effect of noise similar to that recorded
stay at the bedside provide quiet. Fortunately, new sound-absorbent in childrens' hospital sick rooms on healthy infants 3 to 36 weeks
materials for floors, walls, and ceilings satisfy the traditional old. The noise stimulus included frequencies between 100 and 7000
requirements for infection control. Space requirements for new Hz at sound levels between 50 and 75 dB. The healthy infants who
priorities, however, appear to be at odds with space restrictions served as subjects of the research were able to attain all stages of
favored by traditional priorities. Regardless of the outcome of sleep. However, the results show that most healthy newborns are
individual institution's space allotments, considerable improvement disturbed or wakened in sleep by hospital sounds at these levels. At
in noise levels can be achieved by careful attention to the acoustical lower levels (e.g., 65 dB) about 20% of infants were disturbed after
design of the building.36 12 minutes of exposure. At higher levels (e.g., 70 dB) 1 of 2 to 2 of 3
Gray and Philbin37 studied the relative effects of changing staff of the babies were disturbed or wakened after only 3 minutes of
behavior and the physical space of the nursery using a series of sound exposure. Similar disturbances of sleep by similar sounds are reported
measurements made over 8 years in the same level III nursery. Their by Miller and Byrne,41 Wedenberg,42 and by Steinschneider et al.43 Noise
emphasis was on increasing the periods of quiet, rather than simply levels used in these studies are typical of hospital nurseries reported in
eliminating loud noise events. In 1986 the L min or most quiet the literature. The recommended permissible noise criteria for new
perceptible sound level in a 5-day period was variable, usually at nurseries are based on these demonstrated effects on sleep.44
about 65 dBA and rarely about 55 dBA. Nights were no more quiet
than days, and weekends no different than weekdays. In 1991, after a
10-month period of staff education, internal publicity, and STUDIES COMPARING FULL TERM AND PRETERM
individual demonstrations of the effect of sound on the behavior and INFANT RESPONSES TO SOUND
vital signs of very low birth weight infants, the L min had dropped to a Although it would be helpful to clinicians to understand how preterm
steady level of 56 dBA. Brief but perceptible loud events, described by infant responses to sound differ prior to term gestational age from
L max, were fewer in number but remained high at 80 to 98 dBA. those of term infants, few studies have made the comparison directly.
There were still no differences between day±night and weekend± Most investigators compare the two groups of infants at term
weekday sound levels. In 1993 the nursery was renovated and gestational age and are interested in whether preterm infants show
bacteriostatic carpet and acoustic ceiling materials were installed. more reactivity to a stimulus than term infants do. The results are
Smaller stainless steel sinks replaced the deep steel scrub sinks and mixed and depend on the way in which the question is tested and on
some improvements were made in the air-handling ducts. Bed differences in responsiveness within the subject groups.
locations were not changed. In the empty nursery the mean sound In 1971 Bench and Parker reported differing responses of 10
pressure was 55 dBA at 1000 Hz before renovation and 40 dBA preterm infants (28 to 36 weeks' post conceptional age at birth) and
Journal of Perinatology 2000; 20:S76 ± S86 S79
Philbin Influence of Auditory Experience on Preterm Newborns

10 full-term infants in deep sleep and light sleep using a controlled that a small group of preterms function like term infants whereas a
experimental design.45 Both groups were tested at term gestational small group of term infants function like preterms may explain the
age. The stimuli were a series of loud (85 dB), broad-band sound conflicting results of other studies with relatively small sample sizes
pulses each lasting 500 msec and separated by 5 seconds of ambient and more inclusive gestational age cohorts.
room sound (53 dB). The large 30-dB difference between the
background and signal would cause the signal to be perceived as very
loud. Full-term babies were found to respond more reliably than CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS USING SOUND STIMULATION
preterm babies but only in a light sleep state. The groups responded FOR PRETERM INFANTS
similarly in a deep sleep state. The small sample size and difficulty in In general, the studies of clinical interventions using sound
judging responses limit the generalizability of the findings. stimulation for preterm infants lack a substantive theoretical base
Gerber et al.46 and Gerber and Mencher 47 compared the eye and and a history of testing theory with basic science or comparative
limb movements of term and preterm infants in response to wide- animal behavior studies. Many of the studies reviewed in this section
band and narrow-band white-noise stimuli in a controlled are isolated phenomenological reports based on an assumption that
experimental design. Their sample of 11 preterm infants was born nurseries lack a particular kind of auditory stimulation that would
between 26 and 35 weeks post conception and tested at 1 to 18 weeks benefit preterm infants. Linn et al.30 provide an excellent discussion of
after birth. Subjects' ages at testing are reported as, simply ``still questions relevant to adding planned stimulation to the NICU
preterm''.46 The authors found that preterm infants responded to 24% environment. Gardner et al.,51 Ulrich,52 and Harrison53 also provide
of trials whereas term infants responded to only 17%. Both groups reviews of the literature regarding infant stimulation programs.
respond more to wide-band stimuli (judged to be more complex) Philbin and Klaas54 provide a checklist for evaluating research on the
than to narrow-band. The authors do not report the infants' effects of sound on infants.
behavioral state or whether the differences in responses are
statistically significant. Static, Patterned Stimuli: Heartbeat, Breathing Sounds,
In 1979 Field et al.48 studied the heart rate, movement, and and Vestibular Stimulation
behavioral state responses of 18 preterm and 18 full-term infants A number of investigators have combined static, mechanized
across two sound and one tactile stimulus conditions. The term vestibular stimulation with recordings of artificial heartbeats or other
newborns were between 38 and 41 weeks post conception and tested sounds. The rationale for this auditory stimulation was generally
at 3 days after birth. The preterm infants were born at 31 to 36 weeks based on studies of term infants from the 1970s and evidence of
post conception and tested between 36 and 39 weeks. These heartbeat sounds in the uterine environment that has been
differences place the preterm infant group at a younger post supplanted by more accurate methodologies and relevant animal
conceptional age than the term group at time of testing and weaken models. Philbin and Klaas55 provide a review of these studies. The
the comparability of the subject groups. The term infants showed studies reviewed here are grouped along an axis of more protocol
habituation of heart rate to the stimulus during the 10 habituation driven or mechanistic stimulation to more individualized stimulation
trials whereas the preterm infants did not. The number of limb provided by a human caregiver.
movements decreased for both groups over the habituation trials and Most of the studies of protocol-based, mechanistic auditory and
both groups showed a return to greater movement after a strong vestibular stimulation lack information about ambient nursery and
dishabituating stimulus. The term infants tended to show more deep incubator sound and about the acoustical properties of the stimulus.
sleep over the habituation trials whereas the preterm infants showed This is worrisome in that the overall stimulation to the experimental
less. group appears to be very strong in some studies and may continue for
Als et al.49,50 have compared large numbers of term and preterm a long period of time.56 ± 58 Many of the studies use very small samples
infants at term gestational age on a neurobehavioral assessment of and none using both auditory and vestibular stimuli separate the
responses to simple and complex stimuli in auditory and other effects of one from the other. Additionally, the acoustic stimulation
sensory domains. Their studies show the preterm to be more likely provided by the source of vestibular stimulation (usually a moving
than the term infant to have more motor, autonomic, and state waterbed) appears to be unrecognized. Neither is there a control for
changes in response to auditory, visual, handling, and social the halo effect of a special bed even though the outcomes could be
stimuli.38,39 The preterm is also likely to require more examiner influenced by unmasked caregivers. Very few studies follow the
facilitation to complete the test and to have more difficulties in infants' development after discharge, the studies of Thoman et al.,56
attending to stimuli in an alert state. Not all preterms show these Barnard and Bee,59 and Scarr-Salapatek and Williams60 being notable
differences, however, whereas some healthy term infants do show exceptions.
them. Some studies indicate that exposure to patterned, static stimuli
The studies of Als et al. have larger samples than the other studies offers advantages to weight gain and head growth whereas others do
reviewed here and their subjects are grouped into three gestational not. Perhaps the most consistent finding is better state control and
age cohorts at 26 to 32, 33 to 37, and 38 to 41 weeks. Their finding longer periods of sleep for the experimental babies, at least during the
S80 Journal of Perinatology 2000; 20:S76 ± S86
Influence of Auditory Experience on Preterm Newborns Philbin

experimental period. However, these studies are weakened by the are important problems with the study that should be considered
absence of important safeguards against bias such as masked before applying the findings to clinical practice. First, the outcome
caregivers and data collectors and the lack of a measurement of measure was apnea as recorded in nurses' notes and data were
nursing intervention. collected retrospectively. Nurses were aware of the infants' group
In 1991 Thoman et al.56 studied infants' responses to an auditory assignment and the study relies on nurses recording every monitor
stimulus patterned to resemble breathing. Infants were exposed alarm warning that the heart rate was less than 100 beats per minute
continuously, for an average of 22 days, to a stuffed toy that emitted or respirations less than 20 per minute. No distinctions were made
breath sounds and to a quiet stuffed toy. The acoustic properties of between spontaneously resolving apneas and those requiring
the stimulus and the ambient room sounds were not reported. The 58 intervention or between apneic events of varying lengths. The authors
subjects were cared for in seven different hospitals and group do not report the effect of the waterbed on the delivery of nursing
assignment was not random. The groups were, however, similar in care, infant positioning and other practices that could affect apneic
gestational age at birth (28 to 29 weeks), age at enrollment in the phenomena. They also do not control for a possible halo effect of the
study (33 weeks), and number of days exposed to the stimulus (22 bed itself, for example with an alternate ``special'' bed. Neither do
to 24 days). they report possible untoward effects of this amount of strong
Outcome measures were the gestational age at discharge, the stimulation, for example on sleep, crying, alert periods, motor
amount of contact with the stuffed toy, and the amount of time in activity, or post-study habituation.
various sleep states (both measured on time lapsed videotape). In 1976 Kramer and Pierpont58 combined an hour of oscillating
Differences in nursing-care practices between the hospitals make the waterbed before each feeding with a tape recording of heartbeat and a
results difficult to interpret but these were well documented through woman's voice for virtually the entire period of housing in an
the videotape recordings. In general the infants exposed to the breath incubator. The small sample of 11 experimental and 9 control
sounds increased their contact with the stuffed toy over the course of 2 infants were between 30 and 34 weeks' gestation at birth, had
weeks whereas infants exposed to the quiet toy did not. There were no uneventful courses, and tolerated enteral feedings by the seventh
significant effects on behavioral state during hospitalization or age at postnatal day. The strong stimulation package included auditory
discharge. stimulation at 74 to 84 dB and 25 to 30 oscillations of the waterbed
Sleep and awake states after discharge were studied by videotape each minute. The experimental group gained significantly more
in five weekly 24-hour sleep recordings. The group with auditory weight and had a greater biparietal diameter but was similar to the
stimulation showed significantly more quiet sleep periods at home control group on the Dubowitz et al.62 or Brazelton63 assessment scales
over all 5 weeks and a greater increase in quiet sleep than the control and in head circumference. The study design does not permit
infants. The groups did not differ in length of quiet or active sleep discriminating between the effects of the waterbed per se, the
periods or duration of the longest sleep period. The finding is oscillations, or either of the two auditory stimulations. There may be
interesting but very difficult to interpret given the sampling problems, a reverse halo effect in that the waterbeds had been used for 4 years
large number of hospital settings, and lack of information about and the caregiving staff regarded the standard bed as less desirable.
room, incubator, and stimulus acoustical properties. Alternative Data collectors were not masked regarding the infants' group
explanations for the effect could be increased sleep due to masking of assignments. Finally, there were no follow-up studies of the infant's
the nursery noise, and qualitatively different nursing and/or parental development or growth or studies of possible adverse effects of the
care between the study groups. long exposure to the strong experimental stimuli.
In an earlier study Korner and her associates 61 studied the effects In 1983 Barnard and Bee59 reported the effects of temporally
of an oscillating waterbed on preterm infants' respiration. The patterned vestibular and auditory stimulation during hospitalization
stimulus was chosen to be strong enough to prohibit habituation and on the development of preterm infants near term and at 2 years. The
infants were stimulated for seven days continuously. Infants included design of the study compares stimuli in a range of contingent
in the study were between 28 and 34 weeks' gestation at birth, not responsiveness to the infant's behavior. The infants were between 30
critically ill, and were breathing room air by the fifth postnatal day. and 33 weeks' gestational age at birth and generally started using the
There were 20 infants in the experimental groups and 11 in the experimental bed 7 days after birth. No measure of illness is reported
control group. Infants were placed on the waterbeds from the third to but the authors suggest that subjects were not critically ill. The
fifth postnatal day and remained there for the duration of the study. infants' economic circumstances were similar, an important
Vital signs, weight loss, and amount of emesis were the same among consideration in terms of the follow-up findings.
all of the groups. The experimental group had significantly less The 28 control infants received standard nursery care whereas the
apnea than the control group. Additionally, the experimental group three groups of experimental infants received heart beat sounds and
apnea decreased over time whereas control group apnea increased side-to-side movement of their beds for at least 15 minutes an hour,
over time. 24 hours a day for at least 7 days. The bed moved 3 in. horizontally
The finding is not surprising given that physical stimulation is the 30 times a minute and the auditory stimulation of heartbeat sounds
usual means of reinstating breathing during apnea. However, there was played at 85 dB about 2 ft from the infant's head with most of
Journal of Perinatology 2000; 20:S76 ± S86 S81
Philbin Influence of Auditory Experience on Preterm Newborns

the acoustic energy below 500 Hz. Together and separately, these are advance notice, infants had a higher proportion of heart rate in the
strong stimuli relative to the threshold of responding in preterm normal range ( <140 beats per minute [bpm] ) during
infants at this age. One group of 26 infants received this treatment stimulation, and greater alertness after stimulation. Touch without
regardless of their behavior. A second group of 23 infants received the advance auditory stimulation produced higher than normal heart
stimulation each time they maintained motor inactivity for 90 rates ( >140 bpm). The authors conclude that ``tactile stimulation
seconds and could reactivated it by nonmovement any number of alone may be too arousing for these infants. . .''.68 This finding
times within an hour. A third group of 10 infants could activate the supports consistent behavioral observations regarding better state,
stimulation only once in each hour. There was no attempt to motor, and autonomic control when caregivers speak softly to a
coordinate the stimulation with sleep cycles. preterm infant before touching or handling.
The immediate effect of the rocking bed was to reduce overall Further contrast with the studies of mechanized stimulation is
activity and shorten the periods of activity in the experimental provided by a highly individualized program with follow-up after
groups. These shorter cycles of activity were still present in the self- discharge described by Scarr-Salapatek and Williams.60 This
activating group at the end of the treatment period. When compared controlled study of 60 infants found significantly greater weight gain
on the Brazelton scales one or more of the experimental groups during hospitalization and significantly better developmental
showed a wider range of behavioral states at 34 weeks and better outcomes during hospitalization and at 12 months for the
orientation to auditory and visual stimuli at discharge.63 All three experimental infants. The individualized intervention in the hospital
treatment groups exceeded the control group on the Mental was holding, rocking, talking to, and playing with the infants at
Development Index of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development at 24 feedings and at designated play times. The stimulation was provided
months but the findings are confounded by more organized by practical nurses skilled in baby care. The control infants received
environments as measured on the HOME scale.64,65 The study shows only the handling associated with nursery routines. There was no
that preterm infants at 30 to 33 weeks' gestational age can be shaped attempt to influence parent participation, which limited for both
to respond contingently but does not address the appropriateness of groups. After discharge the experimental mothers, all very poor and
directing this capacity toward extended vestibular and auditory mostly young and single, were visited weekly by an experienced infant
stimulation. development specialist/social worker who provided simple, age-
By contrast with these studies of static, mechanical stimulation, appropriate toys and coached the mother in a wide range of skills
Korner and Thoman66 studied some of the short-term effects of related to child development. The effects of the experimental care
common methods of comforting used by parents in the Western during hospitalization argues well for nursing assignments that
World. They standardized various holding positions, seating positions, allow time for holding and playing and for hospital programs that
and the vestibular stimulation involved in pushing a stroller to and facilitate parental care.
fro. These standard stimuli were presented in random order following In conclusion, then, many of the studies of mechanistic
spontaneous crying. Subjects were all newborn term infants. stimulation lack expected safeguards for validity and reliability and
Comparisons of the time to stop crying following each type of use combinations of strong stimuli. Their findings are mixed and do
stimulation showed that vestibular-proprioceptive stimulation was not include possible, untoward short- and long-term effects of the
the most effective intervention followed by interventions that provided stimulation. The studies of protocols or mechanical devices that
contact. The least effective were those that provided no contact. There provide some opportunity for the infant to influence the stimulation
were no other differences between the groups of infants that could also tend to include more rigorous methodology. These studies find
account for these effects. limited physiological or developmental advantages of the stimula-
In another study that combined human responsiveness to the tion.59,68 By contrast, a well-done study of individualized (non±
individual infant with a stimulation protocol White-Traut and her protocol-driven) stimulation provided by sensitive, skilled caregivers
colleagues report a multimodal intervention for preterms provided by shows both developmental and physiologic advantage during the
a skilled caregiver. The protocol consisted of 10 minutes a day of hospital period. In addition, follow-up guidance for parents during
gentle massage, 5 minutes of rocking, and concurrent talking and the first year of life shows significant developmental advantages at 12
singing stimulation.67 The 17 experimental infants received this months.60
treatment daily for a minimum of ten days. The control infants
received standard nursing care. There was no control for possible Music
halo influences of the ``special treatment'' status of the experimental Despite many commentary articles attributing positive effects of
infants on nursing care. The results of this carefully done study music on hospitalized preterm and full-term infants there are few
showed no significant differences between the groups in weight gain studies of the phenomena and most of these lack common
or length of hospital stay. safeguards for validity and reliability.69 ± 72 A review of this literature in
In a controlled follow-up study, the auditory stimulation (gentle the Journal of Music Therapy raises concerns about the methodology
talking by the caregiver) was separated from the other stimuli such for presenting the sound stimulus and stresses the need to determine
that the caregiver spoke to the infant before touching.68 With this appropriate levels and frequencies of the music stimulus as well as
S82 Journal of Perinatology 2000; 20:S76 ± S86
Influence of Auditory Experience on Preterm Newborns Philbin

appropriate gestational ages for exposure.73 The article also an earlier date by the experimental group. This finding is frequently
comments on the potential for improving music therapy research by cited in other music studies as a positive effect of music on growth
including scientific content from medicine, nursing, audiology, and although no statistical analysis of the data was presented and only
psychology. group means (without variability, range, or measures of
The following studies represent the range of research available. significance) were given to show comparability of groups at birth.
Details of the studies are presented to provide a basis for the critique This study had a large sample of 50 subjects in each of three groups
of this literature, with the more solidly designed studies presented drawn from three different hospitals. No information is given
first. regarding the assignment of infants from each hospital to study
Two studies exemplify attention to the science and details of groups, leaving the possibility that each group was drawn exclusively
behavioral research. A small sample observational study by Burke et from a different hospital. If this was the case, the findings could be
al. in 1995 contrasted the effects of music on the recovery from due to nutritional management between hospitals rather than to the
endotrachial suctioning of four preterm infants with bronchopul- experimental condition.
monary dysplasia.74 The experimental conditions included an aural Although the range of gestational ages at birth was very large (26
music stimulus, a combined aural-vibro-tactile music stimulus, and to 33 weeks), no information is given regarding the acute or chronic
ambient room sound. Observations were done in a closed isolation conditions of the subjects and all are reported to have had ``. . .no
room to limit the intrusion of nursery noise. Each of the 18 trials physiological problems at birth''.77 The method of random
lasted 15 minutes following suctioning, were counterbalanced, and assignment to groups is not given and it appears that investigators,
spread over 8 to 21 days. Subjects ranged in age from 34 weeks' staff, and parents knew the group assignments. A follow-up study
gestational age to 3 months corrected age at entry into the study. using the same subjects tested infant growth and development at
Results showed an effect of aural music on lowering heart rate and of discharge (with the Rosenblith scales) and 9 months (with the
both types of music on improving oxygen saturation. The aural- Bayley scales) post discharge.64,78,79 No significant group differences
vibro-tactile music resulted in more alert behavioral states and both were found on any measure.
types of music resulted in increased sleep and high arousal. This Another frequently cited study by Collins and Kuck reports
multidisciplinary team of investigators uses caution in making improved oxygen saturation and less active behavioral states in
recommendations and reminds the reader of the need to preterm infants as a consequence of exposure to music.80 This study is
individualize care for each infant and to put a specific intervention in based on an explicit assumption ``that infants in a NICU are subject
the larger context of a treatment program. to the same stressors that negatively affect the physiologic parameters
Another carefully done, interdisciplinary study by Lieb et al.75 of anxious adult ICU patients''.81 The assumption is that auditory
used a small sample and phase-lag design to test the effect of a perception, and physiologic and behavioral responses are also similar
multimodal intervention. The study presented preterm infants with between adults and preterms, an assumption not substantiated by
simultaneous visual, auditory, vestibular, and tactile stimuli while known physiology or brain/behavior development of the preterm
they were feeding. The auditory portion of the stimulation package newborn. The study used a small sample of 17 infants with a wide
consisted of the nurse talking or singing to the infant and range of gestational ages at birth and time of testing. The gestational
modulating the stimulation in interaction with the infant's age at testing was 25 to 39 weeks' gestational age, yet all subjects
behavior. The investigators, parents, and staff were all aware of the were intubated. However, despite the very different kinds of
group assignment. No effects of the intervention were shown in physiologic problems and behavioral repertoires known among
Brazelton Scales at discharge or in weight gain at discharge, intubated infants at 25 weeks' gestational age and those at 39 weeks'
although the intervention infants are reported to have received fewer gestational age, no information is given regarding the medical
calories per kilogram of body weight.63 The intervention infants, histories of the subjects.
however, had higher 6-month mental and motor scores on the All infants were initially observed in an agitated, fussy state that
Bayley Scales of Infant Development.64 Possible sources of the served as the baseline control. After 10 minutes the music was played
developmental advantage include the experimental condition itself loudly at 80 dB for the next 10 minutes. Without alternate
as well as the long-term influence of the clinicians' behavioral presentation of experimental and control conditions or a separate
example on parents' social interactions with their infants after control group receiving no music, it is predictable that most of these
discharge. intubated infants would be less active due to fatigue after 20 minutes
Another group of studies of the effects of music has problems in of fussing. The positive results are based on paired t-tests of
subject selection, group assignment, data collection and analysis, and unmasked behavioral ratings by the investigator and on a difference
interpretation of results. For example, a frequently cited study by in mean oxygen saturations of 89% and 92%. This difference in
Chapman76 was designed to test the effect of multiple, short periods of oxygen saturations is arguably not physiologically relevant in any
tape-recorded music or speech on the pattern and amount of limb case and particularly lacks meaning given the disparate physiologic
movement. However, the only significant effect reported is an conditions of the subjects and the lack of information about
unplanned outcome, namely the attainment of an arbitrary weight at respiratory management during the study period.
Journal of Perinatology 2000; 20:S76 ± S86 S83
Philbin Influence of Auditory Experience on Preterm Newborns

Several studies use a nearly identical method of nonrandom meager data analysis. Many attribute benefits that are not
subject assignment termed matching.82 ± 84 Although subjects were supported by the data and some promote commercial products.
non-randomly selected to achieve similar group averages for days Possible adverse effects of the experimental stimulation are not
of life at testing, birth weight, and sex, there is no statistical investigated.
analysis of initial group differences on these parameters despite a
range of 1 to 67 days of life at testing in one study and a range of
2 to 34 days in another.83 In one study, for example, the group RECOMMENDATIONS FOR CLINICAL PRACTICE
exposed to music had 3 of the 10 subjects on assisted ventilation at Measure Noise Levels in Nurseries and Incubators
the time of testing whereas the control group had 7 of the 10 on Given the known, adverse effects of nursery noise on the behavior and
assisted ventilation. Not surprisingly, the less ill experimental group physiology of newborns, and the abundant documentation of high
(exposed to music) are reported to have better oxygen saturation levels of nursery noise around the globe, hospitals are arguably
levels than the more ill control group that heard a tape recording obliged to measure the sound levels of nurseries and incubators.
of the mother's voice. Several publications describe methods of measurement so that
Another small sample study of preterm infants investigates hospital personnel can accomplish the task independently.86,87 In
whether they will suck on a pacifier longer if the sucking is addition, professional acoustical engineers can perform these
accompanied by music.85 The theoretical basis for the study assumes measurements.
a causal relationship between longer durations of non-nutritive,
pacifier sucking and less fatigue during nutritive sucking. Such a Reduce Nursery Noise
relationship, however, is not documented in the literature. The aim of The acoustic conditions of hospital nurseries are unlike any normal
the study is to demonstrate that music accompanying pacifier infant environment and provide the background against which all
sucking increases the duration of non-nutritive sucking and, other sound is discriminated Ð or not. Nursery noise also causes
therefore, decreases fatigue and improves nutritive bottle-feeding in a physiologic stress for infants interferes with sleep and can contribute
group of undefined ``poor'' feeders. The pacifier-music apparatus is to adults' work-related errors.88,89 The priority for improving early
in development as a commercial product. The 12 experimental auditory experience for preterm infants therefore, is to make nurseries
infants were born between 24 and 33 weeks' gestational age at birth more quiet.36,44
and tested between 32 and 41 weeks. The infants' time in hospital
ranges from 1.3 to 9 weeks at time of testing. Despite the wide age Provide Individualized, Parental Care for Hospitalized
range, however, the subjects are not stratified. No information is Infants
given about the infants' acuity of illness, appropriateness of Various aspects of parental care are shown to be associated with
gestational age for weight at birth, hospital course, or chronic illness. developmental advantages for preterm infants if they are
No information is presented regarding the investigators' reliability in individualized to the particular infant by a sensitive caregiver. These
identifying behavioral state or behavioral signs of stress, observations aspects of parental care include holding and rocking to comfort,
crucial to the design. talking and singing during feeding, speaking before handling, and
To test non-nutritive sucking a pressure sensitive pacifier is held time together in social interaction and physical closeness.60,66,68,75
into the infant's mouth by an investigator who is aware of the Therefore, clinicians can be reasonably assured that it is beneficial
auditory condition being tested. The most minimal sucking pressure for family members to hold a preterm infant in a relaxed atmosphere
will activate the music. There is no control for the obligatory nature and to speak or sing to the infant as they are naturally inclined to do,
of the preterm infant's suck response and the infant is unable to rather than as they are directed to do.
expel the pacifier. Despite this, however, the study is described as an
investigation of contingent learning. The experimental and control Limit Added, Auditory and Vestibular Stimulation to
conditions are alternated but the experimental music condition is 2.5 Specific Situations
times longer than the control condition. All investigators and There appears to be little or no basis for exposing hospitalized sick
assistants are aware of the condition while data is recorded. No data and preterm infants to tape recordings regardless of their content. Not
regarding behavioral state or physiologic indicators of stress are only are the purported benefits not established, the possible negative
offered yet all infants are reported as showing no signs of stress. There short-term and long-term consequences are largely unexplored.
is no statistical analysis of the data and results are shown only as a Additionally persuading parents and staff to expose hospitalized
graph of means without error bars. The infants are judged to be infants to tape recordings tends to place a nonresponsive machine
selectively sucking to hear the music. The study reports improved between the caring person and the infant as illustrated by
feeding in the experimental infants. Bouteloup.71 The single, demonstrated exception may by a temporary
In summary, many but not all, of the studies of the effects of benefit of assisting agitated, term-age infants hospitalized with
music have problems in subject selection and group assignment, chronic respiratory disease and poor state regulation to calm
sample size, potential for investigator bias in data collection, and themselves following a stressful procedure.74
S84 Journal of Perinatology 2000; 20:S76 ± S86
Influence of Auditory Experience on Preterm Newborns Philbin

References 25. Cole JG, Begish - Duddy A, Judas ML, Jorgensen KM. Changing the NICU
1. Blennow G, Svenningsen NW, Almquist B. Noise levels in infant incubators environment: the Boston city hospital model. Neonatal Network 1990;9:15 ± 23.
( adverse effects? ) . Pediatrics 1974;53:29 ± 31. 26. Strauch C, Brandt S, Edwards - Beckett J. Implementation of a quiet hour:
2. Falk SA, Woods NF. Hospital noise level and potential health hazards. N Engl effect on noise levels and infant sleep states. Neonat Network 1993;12:31 ± 5.
J Med 1973;289:774 ± 81. 27. Vidyasagar D, Joseph ME, Hamilton LR. Letter to the editor: noise levels in the
3. Bess FH, Peek BF, Chapman JJ. Further observations on noise levels in infant neonatal intensive care unit. J Pediatr 1976;88:115.
incubators. Pediatrics 1979;63:100 ± 6. 28. Gottfried AW, Wallace - Land P, Sherman - Brown S, King J, Coen C,
4. Seleny FL, Streczyn M. Noise characteristics in the baby compartment of Hodgeman JE. Physical and social environment of newborn infants in special
incubators. Amer J Dis Child 1969;117:445 ± 50. care units. Science 1981;214 ( 4521,6 Nov ) :673 ± 5.
5. Stennert E, Schulte FJ, Vollrath M, Brunner E, Frauenrath C. The etiology of 29. Gottfried AW, Hodgeman JE, Brown KW. How intensive is newborn intensive
neurosensory hearing defects in preterm infants. Arch Otolaryngol care? An environmental analysis. Pediatrics 1984;74:292 ± 4.
1978;221:171 ± 82. 30. Linn PL, Horowitz FD, Fox HA. Stimulation in the NICU: is more necessarily
6. Anagnostakis D, Petmezakis J, Messaritakis J, Matsaniotis N. Noise pollution better? Clin Perinatol 1985;12:407 ± 22.
in neonatal units: a potential health hazard. Acta Paediatr Scand 31. Becker PT, Grunewald PC, Moorman J, Stuhr S. Outcomes of developmentally
1980;69:771 ± 3. supportive nursing care for very low birth weight infants. Nurs Res
7. Gottfried AW. Environment of newborn infants in special care units. In: AW 1991;40:150 ± 5.
Gottfried, JL Gaiter, editors. Infant Stress Under Intensive Care. Baltimore: 32. Mann P, Haddow R, Stokes L, Goodley S, Rutter N. Effect of night and day on
University Park Press; 1985. p.23 ± 54. preterm infants in a newborn nursery: randomized trial. Br Med J
8. Hursey FX, Powers WE. Noise levels in ICUs [ letter ] . Crit Care Med 1986;293:1265 ± 7.
1978;6:193. 33. Baer JE, Hall JW, Walsh W, Ashmead D. Infant physiologic response to noise
9. Hursey FX, Powers WE. Noise pollution [ letter ] . Pediatrics 1980;65:188 ± 9. attenuation in the intensive care nursery. Poster presentation at the American
10. Long JG, Lucey JF, Philip AGS. Noise and hypoxemia in the intensive care Academy of Audiology Convention ( Vanderbilt University ) 1994, April.
nursery. Pediatrics 1980;65:143 ± 5. 34. Zahr LK. Premature infant responses to noise reduction by earmuffs: effects
11. Bernard PA, Pechere JC. Does incubator noise increase risks of aminoglyco- on behavioral and physiological measures. J Perinatol 1995;15:448 ± 55.
side ototoxicity? Audiology 1984;23:309 ± 20. 35. White RW. Recommended standards for newborn ICU design. J Perinatol
12. Peltzman P, Kitterman JA, Ostwald PF, Manchester D, Heath L. Effects of 1999;19 part 2:1 ± 12.
incubator noise on hearing. J Aud Res 1970;10:335 ± 9. 36. Evans J, Philbin MK. Facility and operations planning for quiet hospital
13. Lawson K, Daum C, Turkewitz G. Environmental characteristics of a neonatal nurseries. J Perinatol 2000; Supplemental Issue.
intensive - care unit. Child Dev 1977;48:1633 ± 9. 37. Gray L, Philbin MK. Levels of quiet in an intensive care nursery. J Acoust Soc
14. GaÈdeke R, Doring B, Keller F, Vogel A. The noise level in a children's Am 1991;90:2321.
hospital and the wake - up threshold in infants. Acta Paediatr Scand 38. Als H, Duffy FH, McAnulty G. Behavioral differences between preterm and full
1969;58:164 ± 70. term newborns as measured with the APIB system score: I. Infant Behav Dev
15. Falk SA, Farmer JC. Incubator noise and possible deafness. Arch Otolaryngol 1988a;11:305 ± 18.
1973;97:385 ± 7. 39. Als H, Duffy FH, McAnulty GB. The APIB, an assessment of functional
16. League R, Parker J, Robertson M, Valentine V, Powell J. Acoustical competence in preterm and full term newborns regardless of gestational age
environments in incubators and infant oxygen tents. Prev Med 1972;1: at birth: II. Infant Behav Dev 1988b;11:319 ± 31.
231 ± 9. 40. Duffy FH, Als H, McAnulty G. Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence for
17. Ciesielski S, Kopka J, Kidawa B. Incubator noise and vibration Ð possible gestational age effects in healthy preterm and fullterm infants studied two
iatrogenic influence on the neonate. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol weeks after expected due date. Child Dev 1990;61:1271 ± 86.
1980;1:309 ± 16. 41. Miller CL, Byrne JM. Psychophysiologic and behavioral response to auditory
18. Keefe MR. Comparison of neonatal nighttime sleep ± wake patterns in nursery stimuli in the newborn. Infant Behav Dev 1983;6:369 ± 89.
versus rooming - in environments. Nurs Res 1987;36:140 ± 4. 42. Wedenberg E. Auditory tests on new - born infants. Acta Otolaryngol
19. Abramovich SJ, Gregory S, Slemick M, Stewart A. Hearing loss in very 1956;46:446 ± 61.
birthweight infants treated with neonatal intensive care. Arch Dis Child 43. Steinschneider A, Lipton EL, Richmond JB. Auditory sensitivity in the infant:
1979;54:421 ± 6. effect of intensity on cardiac and motor responsivity. Child Dev 1966;37:
20. Philbin MK, Ballweg D, Gray L. The effect of an intensive care unit 233 ± 52.
environment on the development of habituation in healthy avian neonates. 44. Philbin MK, Robertson A, Hall JW III. Recommended permissible noise
Dev Psychobiol 1994;27:11 ± 21. criteria for occupied, newly constructed or renovated hospital nurseries. J
21. Gray L. Properties of sound. J Perinatol 2000;20:S5 ± S10. Perinatol 1999;19 Part 1:559 ± 63.
22. Benini F, Magnavita V, Lago P, Arslan E, Pisan P. Evaluation of noise in the 45. Bench J, Parker A. Hyper - responsivity to sounds in the short - gestation baby.
neonatal intensive care unit. Am J Perinatol 1996;13:37 ± 41. Dev Med Child Neurol 1971;13:15 ± 9.
23. Elander G, Hellstrom G. Reduction of noise levels in intensive care units 46. Gerber SE, Lima CG, Copriviza KL. Auditory arousal in preterm infants. Scand
for infants: evaluation of an intervention program. Heart Lung 1995;24: Audiol Suppl 1982;17:88 ± 93.
376 ± 9. 47. Gerber SE, Mencher GT. Arousal responses of neonates to wide band and
24. MichaeÈlsson M, Risenfeld T, SagreÂn A. High noise levels in infant incubators narrow band noise. American Speech ± Language ± Hearing Association
can be reduced. Acta Paediatr 1992;81:843 ± 4. convention; 1979.

Journal of Perinatology 2000; 20:S76 ± S86 S85


Philbin Influence of Auditory Experience on Preterm Newborns

48. Field TM, Dempsey JR, Hatch J, Ting G, Clifton RK. Cardiac and behavioral 68. White - Traut RC, Nelson MN, Silvestri JM, Cunningham N, Patel M. Response
responses to repeated tactile and auditory stimulation by preterm and term of infants to unimodal and multimodal sensory intervention. Pediatr Nurs
neonates. Dev Psychol 1979;15:406 ± 16. 1997;23:169 ± 75,193.
49. Als H, Lester BM, Tronick E, Brazelton TB. Manual for assessment of preterm 69. Lind J, Hardgrove C. Lullaby bonding. Keeping abreast. J Nurturing
infants' behavior ( APIB ) . In: Fitzgerald HE, Lester BM, Yogman MW, editors. 1978;July ± September:184 ± 90.
Theory and Research in Behavioral Pediatrics, Vol.1. New York: Plenum; 70. Anderson J. Sensory intervention with the preterm infant in the neonatal
1982a. p.64 ± 133. intensive care unit. Am J Occup Ther 1986;40:19 ± 26.
50. Als H, Lester BM, Tronick E, Brazelton TB. Towards a research instrument for 71. Bouteloup P. Des musiciens en neonatologie. Soins GyneÂcol ObsteÂt PueÂric
the assessment of preterm infants. In: Fitzgerald HE, Lester BM, Yogman MW, PeÂdiatr 1994;159 ± 160:20 ± 5.
editors. Theory and Research in Behavioral Pediatrics, Vol.1. New York: 72. Hicks F. The role of music therapy in the care of the newborn. Nurs Times
Plenum; 1982b. p.35 ± 63. 1995;91:31 ± 3.
51. Gardner JM, Karmel BZ, Dowd JM. Relationship of infant psychobiological 73. Cassidy JW, Ditty KM. Presentation of aural stimuli to newborns and
development to infant intervention programs. Special issue: infant intervention premature infants: an audiological perspective. J Music Ther 1998;35:70 ± 87.
programs: truths and untruths. J Child Contemp Soc 1985;17:93 ± 108. 74. Burke M, Walsh J, Oehler J, Gingras J. Music therapy following suctioning.
52. Ulrich B. The effects of stimulation programs on the development of high Neonat Network 1995;14:41 ± 9.
risk infants: a review of the research. Adapt Phys Act Q 1984;1:68 ± 80. 75. Lieb SA, Benfield G, Guidubaldi J. Effects of early intervention and
53. Harrison L. Effects of early supplemental stimulation programs for premature stimulation on the preterm infant. Pediatrics 1980;66:80 ± 90.
infants: review of the literature. Matern Child Nurs J 1986;14:69 ± 90. 76. Chapman JS. Influence of varied stimuli on development of motor patterns in
54. Philbin MK, Klass P. Evaluating studies of the behavioral effects of sound on the premature infant. The National Foundation: Birth Defects: The original
newborns. J Perinatol 2000; Supplemental Issue. article series. 1979;15:61 ± 80.
55. Philbin MK, Klass P. Hearing and behavioral responses to sound in full term 77. Chapman JS. Influence of varied stimuli on development of motor patterns in
newborns. J Perinatol 2000;20:S66 ± S74. the premature infant. The National Foundation: Birth Defects: The original
56. Thoman EB, Ingersol EW, Acebo C. Premature infants seek rhythmic article series. 1979;15:83.
stimulation, and the experience facilitates neurobehavioral development. Dev 78. Molloy GB. The relationship between maternal and musical auditory
Behav Pediatr 1991;12:11 ± 8. stimulation and the developmental behavior of premature infants. The
57. Burns KA. 1980. Developmental Progress in Premature Infants as a Function National Foundation: Birth defects: Original article series. 1979;15:81 ± 98.
of Oscillating Waterbed and Suditory Stimulation. Ann Arbor: University 79. Rosenblith JF. Modified Graham behavior test for neonates; test ± retest
Microfilms International; 1980. PhD Dissertation. reliability, normative data, and hypotheses for future work. Biol Neonate
58. Kramer LI, Pierpont ME. Rocking waterbeds and auditory stimuli to enhance 1961;3:174 ± 92.
growth of preterm infants. J Pediatr 1976;88:297 ± 9. 80. Collins SK, Kuck K. Music therapy in the neonatal intensive care unit. Neonat
59. Barnard KE, Bee HL. The impact of temporally patterned stimulation on the Network 1991;9:23 ± 6.
development of preterm infants. Child Dev 1983;54:1156 ± 67. 81. Collins SK, Kuck K. Music therapy in the neonatal intensive care unit.
60. Scarr - Salapatek S, Williams ML. The effects of early stimulation on low - Neonatal Network 1991;9. p.24.
birth - weight infants. Child Dev 1973;44:94 ± 101. 82. Caine J. The effects of music on the selected stress behaviors, weight, caloric
61. Korner AF, Kraemer HC, Haffner ME, Cosper LM. Effects of waterbed flotation and formula intake, and length of hospital stay of premature and low birth
on premature infants: a pilot study. Pediatrics 1975;56:361 ± 7. weight neonates in a newborn intensive care unit. J Music Ther
62. Dubowitz LMS, Dubowitz V, Palmer P, Verghote M. A new approach to the 1991;28:181 ± 92.
neurological assessment of the preterm and full - term infant. Brain Dev 83. Standley J, Moore R. Therapeutic effects of music and mother's voice on
1980;2:3 ± 14. premature infants. Pediatr Nurs 1995;21:509 ± 12,574.
63. Brazelton TB. The neonatal behavioral assessment scale. 2nd ed. Clinics in 84. Standley JM. The effect of music and multimodal stimulation on responses of
Developmental Medicine. No. 88. Spastics International Medical Publications, premature infants in neonatal intensive care. Pediatr Nurs 1998;24:532 ± 8.
with JB Lippincott, PA; 1984:125 pages. 85. Standley JM. The effect of contingent music to increase non - nutritive
64. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development: Birth to Two Years. New York: sucking of premature infants. Pediatr Nurs 2000. [ In press ] .
Psychological Corp.; 1969. 86. Robertson A, Kohn J, Vos P, Cooper - Peel C. Establishing a noise
65. Caldwell BM, Bradley RH. Manual for the home observation for measurement measurement protocol for neonatal intensive care units. J Perinatol
of the environment. Unpublished manuscript. Little Rock: University of 1998;18:126 ± 30.
Arkansas; 1978. 87. Gray L, Philbin MK. Measuring sound in hospital nurseries. J Perinatol 2000;
66. Korner AF, Thoman EB. The relative efficacy of contact and vestibular - Supplemental Issue.
proprioceptive stimulation in soothing neonates. Child Dev 1972;43: 88. Thomas K, Martin P. NICU sound environment and the potential problems
443 ± 53. for caregivers. J Perinatol 2000;20:S91 ± S96.
67. White - Traut RC, Tubeszewski KA. Multimodal stimulation of the premature 89. Morris BH, Philbin MK, Bose C. Physiological effects of sound on the
infant. J Pediatr Nurs 1986;1:90 ± 5. newborn. J Perinatol 2000;20:S53 ± S58.

S86 Journal of Perinatology 2000; 20:S76 ± S86

You might also like