You are on page 1of 10

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/46094305

Attitudes toward people with disabilities: The perspective of attachment


theory.

Article  in  Rehabilitation Psychology · August 2010


DOI: 10.1037/a0020491 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
27 11,900

3 authors:

Noa Vilchinsky Liora Findler


Bar Ilan University Bar Ilan University
57 PUBLICATIONS   910 CITATIONS    45 PUBLICATIONS   1,291 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Shirli Werner
Hebrew University of Jerusalem
75 PUBLICATIONS   1,968 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Attitudes toward people with disabilities View project

Ethnicity and cardiac rehabilitation View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Shirli Werner on 23 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Rehabilitation Psychology © 2010 American Psychological Association
2010, Vol. 55, No. 3, 298 –306 0090-5550/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0020491

Attitudes Toward People With Disabilities:


The Perspective of Attachment Theory
Noa Vilchinsky and Liora Findler Shirli Werner
Bar-Ilan University Hebrew University of Jerusalem

Objective: Attachment theory was employed as the theoretical framework for the purpose of examining
attitudes toward people with disabilities. Method: A total of 404 Jewish Israeli students without
disabilities completed the Multidimensional Attitudes Scale Toward Persons With Disabilities (MAS)
and the Experiences in Close Relationships Scale (ECR). Results: Reading a scenario about an encounter
with a person with a disability gave rise to more negative emotions than reading a similar scenario about
an encounter with a person without a disability, regardless of participants’ attachment orientations.
However, attachment orientations moderated participants’ positive cognitions and distancing behaviors.
Conclusions: Findings suggest a dynamic process of self-regulation when reacting to a written scenario
about people with disabilities. This process consists of an initial spontaneous negative emotional response
accompanied by compensatory positive cognitions and behavioral tendencies.

Keywords: disability, attachment orientations, attitudes

It is generally agreed upon that a significant number of the cognitive component refers to an individual’s ideas, thoughts,
obstacles encountered by people with disabilities are generated by perceptions, beliefs, opinions, or mental conceptualizations of the
societal attitudes (Antonak & Livneh, 2000). Many studies have referent (person with disability). The affective component is said
sought out the origins of these attitudes. In these studies, gender, to reflect the emotional underpinnings of an attitude (Antonak &
age, socioeconomic status, culture, and prior contact with people Livneh, 1988): that is, the positive or negative feelings toward the
with disabilities were typically investigated (e.g., Antonak & referent. Finally, the behavioral component generally relates to the
Livneh, 1988; Vilchinsky, Werner, & Findler, 2010). Studies have individual’s intent or willingness to behave in a certain manner
also tapped personality characteristics that might account for these toward the referent, or the actual behavioral response (Cook,
attitudes. For example, acceptance of persons with disabilities was 1992). Recently, we (Findler et al., 2007; Vilchinsky et al., 2010)
found to be related to ego-strength, self-esteem, self-awareness, revealed that beyond the classical aspects of attitude, additional
and sense of security, whereas rejection was related to anxiety, distinctive aspects of the affect dimension exist, consisting of
hostility, authoritarianism, ethnocentrism, narcissism, interper- negative affect, interpersonal stress, and calm. Therefore, the cur-
sonal alienation, and rigidity (Chan, Livneh, Pruett, Wang & rent study will examine how men and women’s attachment orien-
Zheng, 2009; Findler, Vilchinsky, & Werner, 2007; Livneh, 1988; tations are related to different dimensions of attitudes toward men
Noonan, Barry, & Davis, 1970; Siller, 1963, 1984). Most studies in and women with disabilities.1
this field have examined the contribution of different traits to the
formation of attitudes toward people with disabilities, yet a more Attachment Theory and Attitudes Toward People
comprehensive theoretical framework of personality and individ- With Disabilities
ual differences is required. The current study applies the theory of
attachment (Bowlby, 1969, 1973, 1988), and especially the con-
An integrative examination of studies in the field of attitudes
nection between the attachment system and reactions to people
toward people with disabilities and different personality traits
who are different, as the framework for examining attitudes toward
seems to lead to a conceptualization of these traits as creating a
people with disabilities.
continuum. One side of this continuum consists of inner resources,
Researchers point to the importance of examining attitudes in a
a sense of security and confidence and therefore positive regard
multidimensional fashion (Antonak & Livneh, 2000). Most re-
toward people with disabilities, whereas the other side consists of
searchers agree that attitudes are constructs with affective, cogni-
negative traits, a lack of ego-strength and anxiety and, as a con-
tive, and behavioral components (Olson & Zanna, 1993). The
sequence, negative attitudes toward people with disabilities (Jan-
drey, 1998; Siller, 1984). This continuum is also the essence of
attachment theory (Bowlby, 1969, 1973, 1988), which appears to
Noa Vilchinsky, Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University; Liora
Findler, School of Social Work, Bar-Ilan University; and Shirli Werner,
1
The Paul Baerwald School of Social Work and Social Welfare, Hebrew The current study is part of a large-scale research project investigating
University of Jerusalem. attitudes toward people with disabilities. For a recently published study
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Noa based on this dataset, please see: Vilchinsky, N., Werner, S., & Findler, L.
Vilchinsky, PhD, Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat- (2010). Gender and attitudes toward people using wheelchairs: A multidi-
Gan, 52900. E-mail: noav18@inter.net.il mensional approach. Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin, 53, 163–174.

298
ATTACHMENT AND ATTITUDES TOWARD PEOPLE WITH DISABILITIES 299

provide one of the best frameworks for examining individual on the avoidance dimension, however, activation of attachment-
differences and psychological processes related to self-regulation, related mental models during stress may, in fact, lead to suppres-
interpersonal relationships and intergroup relations (e.g., Miku- sion of attachment needs and to demonstrations of high levels of
lincer & Shaver, 2007). self-reliance. Negative models of others (as reflected in an avoid-
In keeping with Bowlby’s (1969, 1973, 1988) theory, attach- ance of closeness) may lead to discomfort and a desire to distance
ment orientation is conceived as a global and stable orientation oneself from the situation so as to minimize the distress associated
toward the self, others, and relationships (Shaver & Hazan, 1993). with the encounter (Mikulincer & Florian, 1998). Both processes
It is believed that the quality of attachment interactions during may result in overall negative attitudes because failure to effec-
infancy produces mental working models which organize cogni- tively regulate negative affect has been found to be one of the
tion, affect, and behavior, and shape self-image as well as social primary influences in the formation of negative attitudes toward
relationships (Mikulincer, Florian, & Tolmacz, 1990). Brennan, persons with disabilities (Siller, 1995).
Clark, and Shaver (1998) have suggested that attachment reflects In contrast to these responses, secure individuals’ positive mod-
two basic orientations: avoidance and anxiety. People low on both els of self and others may provide “inner resources” which, in turn,
dimensions exhibit a secure style and are characterized by a sense help them manage stressors. Greater positivity of models of others,
that the world is a safe place, that one can rely on others, and that or greater security of attachment, should predict positive attitudes
one can therefore securely explore the environment and engage as a result of greater optimism, self-efficacy, and perceptions of
effectively with other people (Mikulincer, Shaver, & Pereg, 2003). control over the situation. Indeed, empirical research shows that
These individuals are comfortable with closeness and interdepen- similar personality characteristics associated with negative atti-
dence, and rely on support-seeking and other constructive means to tudes toward people with disabilities have also been related to
cope with stress. In contrast, insecure individuals have learned insecure attachment (e.g., hostility, anxiety, and rigidity), whereas
through many painful experiences with unavailable or unrespon- personality characteristics which were linked with positive atti-
sive attachment figures that the primary attachment strategy (prox- tudes have also been associated with secure attachment (e.g.,
imity-seeking) often fails to accomplish its emotion-regulation ego-strength, self-esteem, and sense of security) (Jandrey, 1998).
goal, making it necessary to consider alternative secondary strat- Researchers have argued that attachment security improves the
egies: hyperactivation or deactivation of the attachment system. functioning of the caregiving system, which is reflected in a
Hyperactivation of the attachment system is seen in adults with willingness to provide care for others who are in need (Gillath,
anxious attachment who regulate their emotions by signaling or Shaver, & Mikulincer, 2005). In line with this prediction, several
expressing their needs and fears, exaggerating their distress, and studies have found that more securely attached people report more
presenting themselves as extremely vulnerable to pain and injury empathic concern for others (Joireman, Needham, & Cummings,
(Mikulincer & Shaver, 2008). These individuals display a strong 2002), and a stronger inclination to adopt the perspective of a
need for closeness combined with an overwhelming fear of rejec- distressed person (Corcoran & Mallinckrodt, 2000).
tion; they tend to dwell mentally on their emotional state and rely In addition to reacting to a stressful situation, meeting a person
on emotion-focused coping strategies (Mikulincer & Shaver, with a disability may activate, through associative networks, one’s
2007). Adults with avoidant attachment are likely to deactivate own memories, expectations, and attitudes connected to relating to
their attachment systems and rely mainly on themselves to deal someone with a strong need for closeness and dependence (West-
with threats (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2008). These individuals are maas & Cohen-Silver, 2001). Meeting a person with a disability
characterized by a distrust of others’ goodwill and a preference for might also activate working models relevant to rejection in rela-
emotional distance (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). tionships. For example, seeing a person in a wheelchair might
A fundamental tenet of attachment theory is that the experience elicit thoughts about how much rejection he or she might have
of stress or fear is required to activate the attachment system experienced as a consequence of his/her disability (Jones et al.,
(Mikulincer & Shaver, 2008). Bowlby (1969) suggested that the 1984). People might also think about their own likelihood of being
sense of a secure base plays a critical role in regulating fear rejected were they in a similar situation (Westmaas & Cohen-
reactions to other people, especially those who are different from Silver, 2001). With the activation of attachment-relevant thoughts
oneself or relatively unfamiliar. Disability often represents a new and memories, interacting with a person with a disability may
and ambiguous stimulus which might be perceived as a threat produce attachment effects in interpersonal responses.
(Wright, 1983), therefore requiring activation of the attachment In a laboratory study involving behavioral observations, West-
system (Jandrey, 1998). On this basis, we hypothesize that the mass and Cohen-Silver (2001) videotaped people while they in-
anxiety-provoking nature of an interpersonal exchange with a teracted with a confederate whom they thought had recently been
person with a disability is likely to activate attachment-related diagnosed with cancer. As expected, participants high on avoid-
schemas and influence the attitudes which arise toward this person ance were rated as less supportive and less likely to make eye
during the interaction. contact during the interaction than participants who were low on
The way in which attachment orientation is associated with avoidance. Similarly, attachment anxiety was not associated with
coping with stressful events provides a framework for predicting supportiveness and participants high on attachment anxiety re-
specific attitudes toward persons with disabilities. In accordance ported greater discomfort while interacting with the confederate.
with the ideas of Mikulincer and Florian (1998), the negative Finally, employing attachment theory in the study of attitudes
self-models of anxiously attached persons should lead to appraisals toward people with disabilities is based on the theoretical link
of helplessness, uncontrollability, and an inability to cope during between attachment and exploration (Bowlby, 1988). The sense of
stress. This passive coping with stressors should lead to heightened having a secure base allows people to open their schemas to
distress and negative thoughts about the person. For persons high belief-discrepant information (Mikulincer, 1997). This cognitive
300 VILCHINSKY, FINDLER, AND WERNER

openness should be an asset in encounters with people viewed as reported on at length by the authors (Findler et al., 2007; Vilchin-
different, attenuating negative attitudes toward people with dis- sky et al., 2010). Respondents were asked to react to a social
abilities. scenario vignette which described a casual interaction in a coffee
shop between “Joseph” or “Michelle” and a person who was either
The Current Study using or not using a wheelchair. Male and female respondents were
given questionnaires referring to Joseph or Michelle, respectively.
Attachment theory, which encompasses both the idea of self- In half of the questionnaires, the target person (either using or not
regulation in interpersonal situations as well as the perception of using a wheelchair) was a man, and in the other half a woman.
others who are different, was employed as the theoretical frame- Thus, eight groups were created: gender of observer ⫻ gender of
work in the current study for the purpose of examining attitudes target person ⫻ existence of disability. In the current study, each
toward people with disabilities. We hypothesized that higher levels of these eight groups consisted of between 47 to 55 participants.
of attachment-related anxiety would be associated with elevated The respondents were asked to read the vignette and then relate to
negative affect and interpersonal stress and lower levels of calm, each item, indicating the degree to which they believed the item
especially when reading a scenario about an encounter with a accurately reflected the way the person in the story (Joseph or
person with a disability, as opposed to without a disability. In Michelle) would feel, think, or behave in that situation. Responses
addition, attachment-related avoidance was hypothesized to asso- were marked on a 5-point scale, ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5
ciate with elevated levels of distancing behaviors, especially when (very much). The MAS consisted of five separate factors: Negative
reading a scenario about an encounter with a person with a dis- Emotions (e.g., “Joseph might feel depressed”), Interpersonal
ability. Stress (e.g., “Joseph might feel shy”), Calm (e.g., “Joseph might
feel relaxed”), Positive Cognitions (e.g., “Joseph might think that
Method he seems like an interesting guy”), and Distancing Behaviors (e.g.,
“Joseph might move to another table”). On the basis of these
Participants and procedure. The sample consisted of 404 factors, we computed five scores for each participant by averaging
Jewish Israelis, both men (51.1%) and women (48.9%) without the items on each factor. Higher scores indicated more distancing
disabilities, with a mean age of 24.67 years (SD ⫽ 2.87). The vast behaviors, more positive cognitions, more negative emotions,
majority of participants were college students, with a mean of more interpersonal stress, and more calm. Cronbach’s alphas in the
13.75 years of education (SD ⫽ 1.62). The current study is based current study were .68, .79, .93, .90, and .82 for negative affect,
on a dataset that has been previously used and described (Vilchin- interpersonal stress, calm, positive cognitions, and distancing be-
sky et al., 2010). Briefly, participants were recruited via a conve- haviors, respectively. The internal consistencies of four of the
nience sample. Research assistants approached students on univer- scales were found to be high, except for negative affect, which was
sity and college campuses and asked them to complete the found to be relatively low; however, the mean of inter-item cor-
questionnaires. Both state universities across the country, as well relations for this scale (r ⫽ .33) fell within the acceptable range as
as colleges on the periphery, were approached in an attempt to suggested by Nunnally (1978). Table 1 presents the intercorrela-
reach a more diverse population. The participants who were ap- tions between the MAS factors. It can be seen that the factors only
proached were from various fields of study; however, we excluded moderately associated with each other, with the highest correlation
social work and psychology students whom we suspected to be found between negative emotions and interpersonal stress.
biased because of their educational agenda, which reflects human- Experiences in Close Relationships Scale (ECR; Brennan et
istic values (DePoy & Merrill, 1988; Vilchinsky & Findler, 2004). al., 1998). The ECR was used to assess attachment orientations.
This study was approved by the institutional review board, and The original scale was shortened in order to decrease item repeti-
each participant’s verbal consent was obtained after a short expla- tion and ease the completion of the instrument. The shortened
nation of the procedure involved. version consisted of the 24 items which produced the highest
Instruments. loadings in Brennan et al.’s study. Twelve items reflected attach-
Multidimensional Attitudes Scale toward persons with disabil- ment anxiety (e.g., “I worry about being abandoned”), and 12
ities (MAS). The revised MAS is a 22-item multidimensional reflected attachment avoidance (e.g., “I prefer not to show my
scale (Vilchinsky et al., 2010), based on the original 34-item scale partner how I feel deep down”). Participants rated the extent to
developed by authors (Findler et al., 2007), to measure attitudes which each item was descriptive of their feelings in close relation-
toward people with disabilities. Content and structure validity were ships on a 7-point scale, ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very

Table 1
Intercorrelations Between the MAS Factors
Negative emotions Interpersonal stress Calm Positive cognitions

Negative emotions 1.000


Interpersonal stress .40ⴱⴱⴱ 1.000
Calm ⫺.04 .07 1.000
Positive cognitions .14ⴱⴱ ⫺.04 .25ⴱⴱⴱ 1.000
Distancing behaviors .27ⴱⴱⴱ .33ⴱⴱⴱ .04 ⫺.01
ⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .01. p ⬍ .001.
ATTACHMENT AND ATTITUDES TOWARD PEOPLE WITH DISABILITIES 301

much). The reliability and validity of the scale have been demon- Table 3
strated (Brennan et al., 1998). In the current sample, Cronbach’s Correlations Between Attachment Orientations and the Factors
alpha coefficients were high, both for anxiety (.86) and for avoid- of the Multidimensional Attitudes Scale Toward Persons
ance (.82). Scores were computed for each of the subscales by With Disabilities (MAS)
averaging the responses on the relevant items. MAS factors Anxious attachment Avoidant attachment
ⴱⴱⴱ
Results Negative emotions .25 .20ⴱⴱⴱ
Interpersonal stress .32ⴱⴱⴱ .17ⴱⴱⴱ
Calm .05 ⫺.01
Table 2 presents the means and standard deviations of the Positive cognitions ⫺.05 .15ⴱⴱ
study’s variables (attachment orientations and the five factors of Distancing behaviors .05 .16ⴱⴱⴱ
the MAS). Table 3 presents Pearson correlations between attach- ⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .01. p ⬍ .001.
ment orientations and the five factors of the MAS (i.e., Negative
Emotions, Interpersonal Stress, Calm, Positive Cognitions, and
Distancing Behaviors). Results indicate that, whereas both anxious
and avoidant attachment orientations were positively correlated ever, the step in which this interaction was added was not found to
with negative emotions and interpersonal stress, only avoidant be significant, R2cha ⫽ .02, Fcha(6, 389) ⫽ 1.25, p ⬎ .05. Therefore,
attachment was positively related to positive cognitions and dis- this interaction was not further analyzed.
tancing behaviors. Neither of the two attachment orientations was Significant four-way interactions for both attachment-related
associated with the calm factor. anxiety, R2cha ⫽ .03, Fcha(1, 379) ⫽ 11.63, p ⬍ .001 and attach-
Next, the data were analyzed in a series of four-step, hierarchi- ment-related avoidance, R2cha ⫽ .01, Fcha(1, 379) ⫽ 6.21, p ⬍ .05
cal linear regressions testing the unique and interactive contribu- emerged when predicting positive cognitions. To interpret the
tions of the existence of a disability, observer gender, target person four-way interactions, eight 2-step hierarchical linear regressions
gender, and attachment orientations to the five factors of the MAS. were conducted; half of them targeted anxiety, controlling for
Step 1 of each regression consisted of disability (DIS)—a contrast avoidance, and the other half targeted avoidance, controlling for
code variable comparing meeting a person with a disability (1) to anxiety. These regressions were performed separately for women
control (2); observer gender—a contrast code variable comparing and men who read about encountering either a man or a woman.
women (1) to men (2); target person gender—a contrast code Step 1 of these regressions consisted of target disability and the
variable comparing women (1) to men (2); and the attachment two attachment orientations. Step 2 consisted of the products of the
scores of both anxiety (ANX) and avoidance (AVO). Step 2 predictors’ standardized scores (AVO ⫻ DIS or ANX ⫻ DIS).
consisted of the products of the standardized scores between every Results (Table 6) show a significant interaction between anxiety
two predictors. The three-way interactions were entered in the and disability for women who encountered a virtual woman,
third step and the four-way interaction was added in the fourth R2cha ⫽ .05, Fcha(1, 101) ⫽ 7.27, p ⬍ .01, and for men who
step. Half of the regressions targeted anxiety, controlling for encountered a virtual woman, R2cha ⫽ .04, Fcha(1, 93) ⫽ 4.37, p ⬍
avoidance, and the other half targeted avoidance, controlling for .05. An additional significant interaction was found between
anxiety. For simplicity’s sake, only interactive effects involving avoidance and disability for women who encountered a virtual
attachment and disability were described. Tables 4 and 5 present woman, R2cha ⫽ .04, Fcha(1, 101) ⫽ 5.37, p ⬍ .05.
the standardized regression coefficients (␤s) for each effect at the Aiken and West’s (1991) procedure for examining simple slopes
step in which it was entered into the regression equations. revealed that women low on attachment-related anxiety (1 SD
No significant interactions were found between attachment ori- below the mean) expressed more positive cognitions toward a
entations and disability when predicting negative emotions and virtual woman with a disability than toward a virtual woman
interpersonal stress. Results indicate that attachment- related anx- without a disability, ␤ ⫽ ⫺.75, p ⬍ .001, and this tendency was
iety was associated with higher levels of both negative emotions moderated for women high on attachment-related anxiety (1 SD
and interpersonal stress. Further, attachment-related avoidance above the mean), ␤ ⫽ ⫺.28, p ⬍ .05 (Figure 1). The same pattern
was moderately associated only with interpersonal stress. In addi- was found with regard to the avoidance orientation: women low on
tion, a two-way interaction between attachment-related anxiety attachment-related avoidance (1 SD below the mean) expressed
and target person disability emerged when predicting calm. How- more positive cognitions toward a virtual woman with a disability
than toward a virtual woman without a disability, ␤ ⫽ ⫺.69, p ⬍
.001, and this tendency was moderated for women high on attach-
ment-related avoidance (1 SD above the mean), ␤ ⫽ ⫺.30, p ⬍ .05
Table 2 (Figure 2). The opposite pattern was found for men. Men who
Means and SDs of the Study’s Variables scored high on attachment-related anxiety expressed more positive
Variable M (SD) cognitions toward a virtual woman with a disability than without a
disability, ␤ ⫽ ⫺.40, p ⬍ .01, whereas men who scored low on
Negative emotions 1.74 (.63) attachment-related anxiety expressed no difference in positive
Interpersonal stress 2.63 (.87)
Calm 2.91 (.94)
cognitions toward any virtual woman, with or without a disability,
Positive cognitions 2.85 (.84) ␤ ⫽ ⫺.01, p ⬎ .05 (Figure 3).
Distancing behaviors 2.02 (.73) Finally, as seen in Table 5, higher levels of attachment-related
Anxious attachment 3.34 (1.07) avoidance were related to higher levels of distancing behaviors.
Avoidant attachment 3.01 (.93) Also seen in Table 5 is the significant three-way interaction among
302 VILCHINSKY, FINDLER, AND WERNER

Table 4
Predicting the Factors of Attitudes Toward People With Disabilities From Observer Gender, Target Gender, Disability, and
Attachment-Related Anxiety (N ⫽ 404)
MAS factors
Negative Interpersonal Positive Distancing
emotions stress Calm cognitions behaviors

Step 1 ␤ ␤ ␤ ␤ ␤
Observer gender .04 ⫺.13ⴱⴱ ⫺.04 ⫺.01 .08
Target gender .08 ⫺.01 .12ⴱ .17ⴱⴱⴱ .10ⴱ
Disability ⫺.36ⴱⴱⴱ .13ⴱⴱ .07 ⫺.30ⴱⴱⴱ .19ⴱⴱⴱ
Attachment-related avoidance .07 .11ⴱ ⫺.01 .13ⴱ .16ⴱⴱ
Attachment-related anxiety .18ⴱⴱⴱ .29ⴱⴱⴱ .06 ⫺.12ⴱ .03
Step 2
Anxiety ⫻ Disability ⫺.06 .07 .12ⴱ ⫺.005 ⫺.08
Step 3
Anxiety ⫻ Disability ⫻ Observer Gender .06 .07 .07 ⫺.05 ⫺.03
Anxiety ⫻ Disability ⫻ Target Gender ⫺.06 ⫺.05 .09 ⫺.05 ⫺.14ⴱⴱ
Step 4
Anxiety ⫻ Disability ⫻ Observer Gender ⫻ Target Gender .02 .02 .05 .16ⴱⴱ .01

Note. MAS ⫽ Multidimensional Attitudes Scale Toward Persons With Disabilities. Disability (yes ⫽ 1, no ⫽ 2); Observer gender (female ⫽ 1, male ⫽ 2). Target
gender (female ⫽ 1, male ⫽ 2).

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.

attachment-related anxiety, disability, and target gender which was pressed less distancing behaviors toward a virtual man with a
found to explain this factor, R2cha ⫽ .02, Fcha(4, 385) ⫽ 2.36, p ⫽ disability than a virtual man without a disability, ␤ ⫽ .35, p ⬍
.05. To interpret the three-way interactions, two 2-step hierarchical .001. This pattern was not detected in participants high on attach-
linear regressions were performed separately for reading about ment-related anxiety (1 SD above the mean), ␤ ⫽ ⫺.07, p ⬎ .05.
encounters with a man or a woman. Step 1 consisted of the
existence of disability and attachment-related anxiety, with attach- Discussion
ment-related avoidance controlled. Step 2 consisted of the product
of the predictors’ standardized scores. The ANX ⫻ DIS interaction The current study examined attitudes toward persons with dis-
was significant only when reading about encounters with a man, abilities within the framework of attachment theory, by applying
R2cha ⫽ .05, Fcha(1, 193) ⫽ 10.00, p ⬍ .01, but not a woman, the multidimensional approach of attitude research. Using this
R2cha ⫽ .005, Fcha(1, 200) ⫽ 1.14, p ⬎ .05. Aiken and West’s approach, it became possible to reveal the intricate relationship
(1991) procedure, presented in Figure 4, revealed that participants between attachment orientations and the various dimensions of
low on attachment-related anxiety (1 SD below the mean) ex- attitudes toward persons with disabilities.

Table 5
Predicting the Factors of Attitudes Toward People With Disabilities From Observer Gender, Target Gender, Disability, and
Attachment-Related Avoidance (N ⫽ 404)
MAS factors
Negative Interpersonal Positive Distancing
emotions stress Calm cognitions behaviors

Step 1 ␤ ␤ ␤ ␤ ␤
Observer Gender .04 ⫺.13ⴱⴱ ⫺.04 ⫺.01 .08
Target Gender .08 ⫺.01 .12ⴱ .17ⴱⴱⴱ .10ⴱ
Disability ⫺.36ⴱⴱⴱ .13ⴱⴱ .07 ⫺.30ⴱⴱⴱ .19ⴱⴱⴱ
Attachment-related Anxiety .18ⴱⴱⴱ .29ⴱⴱⴱ .06 ⫺.12ⴱ .03
Attachment-related Avoidance .07 .11ⴱ ⫺.01 .13ⴱ .16ⴱⴱ
Step 2
Avoidance ⫻ Disability .02 ⫺.01 .10 .07 .03
Step 3
Avoidance ⫻ Disability ⫻ Observer Gender ⫺.002 ⫺.05 ⫺.08 .03 .07
Avoidance ⫻ Disability ⫻ Target Gender .03 ⫺.05 .04 ⫺.05 ⫺.08
Step 4
Avoidance ⫻ Disability ⫻ Observer Gender ⫻ Target Gender .008 ⫺.002 .02 .12ⴱ ⫺.03

Note. MAS ⫽ Multidimensional Attitudes Scale Toward Persons With Disabilities. Disability (yes ⫽ 1, no ⫽ 2); Observer gender (female ⫽ 1, male ⫽ 2); Target
gender (female ⫽ 1, male ⫽ 2).

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
ATTACHMENT AND ATTITUDES TOWARD PEOPLE WITH DISABILITIES 303

Table 6
Predicting Positive Cognitions From Target Disability and Attachment-Related Anxiety and Avoidance (N ⫽ 96)
Observer, Man Observer, Woman
Target, Man ␤ Target, Woman ␤ Target, Man ␤ Target, Woman ␤

Step 1
Target disability (DIS) ⫺.17 ⫺.20ⴱ ⫺.36ⴱⴱ ⫺.50ⴱⴱⴱ
Attachment-related avoidance (AVO) .05 .26ⴱ .10 .02
Attachment-related anxiety (ANX) ⫺.19 ⫺.13 ⫺.06 ⫺.09
Step 2
DIS ⫻ ANX .07 ⫺.21ⴱ ⫺.17 .23ⴱⴱ
DIS ⫻ AVO .19 ⫺.12 .00 .20ⴱ

Note. Target disability (disability ⫽ 1, control ⫽ 2).



p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.

As hypothesized, attachment-related anxiety and to a lesser Gillath, & Nitzberg, 2005). It seems that the negative emotional
degree avoidance were associated with higher levels of negative reaction toward people with disabilities is so innate and spontane-
emotions and interpersonal stress toward any target person, regard- ous that even a strong sense of psychological security is not
less of disability. This finding is in line with attachment theory’s enough to attenuate it. Our findings are in line with the assertion
prediction that people high on both attachment orientations find that people without a disability will at first experience anxiety,
social situations emotionally stressful (Mikulincer & Shaver, unease and rejecting feelings toward people with disabilities, and
2007). The higher levels of emotions expressed by attachment- only additional contacts with them may enable them to surmount
related anxious participants corroborate previous findings indicat- these negative emotions (Rimmerman, Hozmi, & Duvdevany,
ing that these individuals tend toward hyperactivation of the at- 2000).
tachment system and therefore excessively express their distress, Furthermore, Oatley and Jenkins (1996) claimed that emotions
whereas people high on attachment-related avoidance tend to arise in conjunction with appraisals and are expressed through
deactivate their attachment system, and as a consequence repress changes in thought, action tendencies, behaviors and subjective
and conceal their distress (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2008). Also, as feelings. We therefore suggest a dynamic process of reaction to
hypothesized, attachment-related avoidance was associated with people with disabilities consisting of an initial spontaneous nega-
higher levels of distancing behaviors toward any target person tive emotional response accompanied by compensatory positive
regardless of disability. The tendency of people high on attach- cognitions and positive behavioral tendencies. Our findings imply
ment-related avoidance to refrain from social contact (Mikulincer that this compensation is a more difficult transaction for the
& Florian, 1998) was again supported. insecurely attached to enact.
Contrary to our hypothesis, attachment orientations did not Whereas attachment orientations were not found to moderate the
moderate the negative emotions or interpersonal stress toward a negative emotions dimension of attitudes, their impact was salient
virtual person with a disability. Reading about an encounter with with regard to the cognitive and behavioral dimensions. We found
a person with a disability gave rise to more negative emotions than that (except for anxiously attached men who encountered a woman
reading about an encounter with a person without a disability with a disability) participants low on anxiety and avoidance tended
regardless of participants’ attachment orientations. This finding is to express more positive attitudes toward virtual people with
surprising in light of the large number of studies in the field of disabilities than toward virtual people without disabilities, and the
attachment and interpersonal relations demonstrating the benevo- existence of insecure attachment moderated this tendency. In terms
lent impact of attachment security on reactions to others (Miku- of cognitions and behaviors, it seems the benevolent effect of
lincer et al, 2001; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2001; Mikulincer, Shaver, secure attachment was realized to a greater degree.
positive cognitions

positive cognitions

Disability Control Disability Control


Target person Target person
High on attachment -related anxiety low on attachment -related anxiety High on attachment -related avoidance low on attachment -related avoidance

Figure 1. Predicting women’s positive cognitions toward a woman with Figure 2. Predicting women’s positive cognitions toward a woman with
and without a disability as a function of attachment-related anxiety. and without a disability as a function of attachment-related avoidance.
304 VILCHINSKY, FINDLER, AND WERNER

disabilities, may explain why participants high on attachment-


positive cognitions

related anxiety, whose negative self-models lead to excessive


appraisals of helplessness and uncontrollability (Mikulincer &
Florian, 1998), are more threatened by reading about a man with a
disability and therefore do not show the same tendency to ap-
proach him as their securely attached counterparts do.
Contrary to our expectations, anxiously attached men expressed
Disability Control more positive cognitions toward a virtual woman with a disability
than toward a virtual woman without a disability. We propose that
Target person
anxiously attached men may feel less threatened in terms of
High on attachment -related anxiety low on attachment -related anxiety
potential rejection by a woman with a disability and therefore
present more positive appraisals of meeting with her, virtually,
Figure 3. Predicting men’s positive cognitions toward a woman with and
than they do with a virtual woman without disabilities. This
without a disability as a function of attachment-related anxiety.
putative explanation is in line with the assertion that men and
women with disabilities are taken to be asexual, or seen as a “third
gender” (Sakellariou, 2006; Shakespeare, 1999, p. 60).
Whereas the emotional response of the securely attached was as In sum, people without disabilities express negative feelings
negative as that of the insecurely attached, the securely attached toward virtual people with disabilities regardless of their attach-
seemed better able to bounce back from the initial negative re- ment orientations. However, people also present a compensatory
sponse, going on to display a stronger tendency to approach the pattern of exhibiting more positive cognitions and less distancing
virtual person with the disability. This process is in line with the behaviors toward virtual people with disabilities. This shift in
predictions of attachment theory regarding emotion regulation. response is stronger among the less anxiously attached partici-
According to Mikulincer and Shaver (2007), when a secure person pants, especially in instances of encountering a virtual man with a
encounters external stimuli that provoke undesirable emotions, disability, and when anxiously attached women encounter a virtual
he/she is more likely to place the negative event in perspective, woman with a disability.
turning the threat into a challenge, and adjusting his/her plans
accordingly. Limitations and Future Studies
The difficulty in overcoming the initial negative emotional
response and to think more positively of the person with the The current findings must be examined in light of several
disability was detected mostly among anxiously attached women limitations. First, all of the participants were college students, thus
who read about an encounter with a woman with a disability. We creating a somewhat homogenous sample, reflected in the rela-
suggest that this finding can be understood in light of self– other tively small differences found in the mean scores. For this reason,
distinctiveness (Mikulincer & Florian, 2001). Batson (1991) the current sample is not representative of other parts of the
claimed that the arousal of empathic responses demands self– other population in terms of age, stage of life, and education. Future
distinctiveness. In accordance with the theories of Mikulincer et al. studies should examine people of various ages and various back-
(2001), one can delineate the following sequence of mediating ground characteristics, and should also parse differences in atti-
events: upon reading a scenario which describes seeing a woman tudes toward different disability groups.
with a disability, insecurely attached women would increase their With regard to the instrument, the study relies on findings
focus on the other’s suffering as well as on self– other merging obtained from a self-report. Therefore, these findings cannot nec-
which, in turn, would result in the activation of self-related neg- essarily be taken to reflect real-life encounters with people with
ative cognitions and emotions. This cognitive–affective state disabilities. An important contribution would be to examine actual
would be manifested in overwhelming personal distress as well as
in the inhibition of empathic reactions.
Of interest, in the current study, this inhibition of positive
cognitions toward women with disabilities was not channeled into
Distancing behaviors

women participants’ distancing behaviors. The effect of attach-


ment on distancing behaviors was observed only in regard to
reading about encounters with a man. Our findings showed that
anxiously attached participants did not demonstrate the same in-
terest as the securely attached in approaching a virtual man with a
disability. A possible explanation can be drawn from the field of
social support. Belle (1987) found that men tend to seek and
receive less social support than women. Seeking social support is
conceived by many men as signifying weakness and dependence, Disability Control
and as inversely related to trait masculinity (Butler, Giordano, & Target person
Neren, 1985; Winstead, Derlega, & Wong, 1984). Subsequently, High on attachment -related anxiety low on attachment -related anxiety
when a man incurs a severe disabling injury, his identity is often
influenced by a perceived loss of his masculine traits (Marini, Figure 4. Predicting participants’ distancing behaviors toward a man
2001). These views, shared both by people with and without with and without a disability as a function of attachment-related anxiety.
ATTACHMENT AND ATTITUDES TOWARD PEOPLE WITH DISABILITIES 305

behavior, instead of reported behavior, by means of an observa- Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruism question: Towards a social-psycholog-
tional study. ical answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
In accordance with Antonak and Livneh’s (2000) recommenda- Belle, D. (1987). Gender differences in the social moderators of stress. In
tion for the use of indirect measures, the authors of the MAS R. C. Barnett, L. Biener, & G. K. Baruch (Eds.), Gender and stress (pp.
(Findler et al, 2007) purposely employed and rephrased Fichten 257–277). New York: Free Press.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
and Amsel’s (1988) social scenario vignette in order to enable
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol 2. Separation. New York:
respondents to project their own emotions, thoughts, and behaviors Basic Books.
onto the given situation. However, it is possible that perceptions as Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base. London: Routledge.
to how others might react toward people with disabilities differ Brennan, K. A., Clark, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement
from one’s own perceptions. Finally, the study focuses on wheel- of adult attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S.
chair users; therefore, any attempt to generalize from these results Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46 –76).
to people with other disabilities, or to people with disabilities as a New York: Guilford Press.
group, should be done cautiously. Finally, although we found Butler, T., Giordano, S., & Neren, S. (1985). Gender and sex-role attributes
several significant interactions, one should note that the interaction as predictors of utilization of natural support systems during personal
terms accounted for a relatively small amount of incremental stress events. Sex Roles, 13, 515–524.
variance (ranging from 1% to 5%); therefore, the interpretation of Chan, F., Livneh, H., Pruett, S., Wang, C. C., & Zheng, L. X. (2009).
Societal attitudes toward disability: Concepts, measurements, and inter-
these interactions should be exercised with caution.
ventions. In F. Chan, E. Da Silva- Cardoso, & J. A. Chronister (Eds.),
Future studies are advised to focus on different ways of chang-
Understanding psychosocial adjustment to chronic illness and disability
ing the attitudes of individuals who harbor different attachment (pp. 333–370). New York: Springer.
orientations, as well as looking for possible moderators that may Cook, D. (1992). Psychological impact of disability. In R. M. Parker &
influence their attitudes toward people with disabilities. Future E. M. Szymanski (Ed.), Rehabilitation counseling basics and beyond
studies are also advised to trace the sequence of reactions toward (pp. 249 –272). Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
people with disabilities. Such studies might further validate our Corcoran, K., & Mallinckrodt, B. (2000). Adult attachment, self-efficacy,
suggestion that the activation of attachment schemas contributes to perspective taking and conflict resolution. Journal of Counseling &
attitudes only in terms of thoughts and behavioral intentions, while Development, 78, 473– 483.
even the existence of a secure attachment does not override the DePoy, E., & Merrill, S. (1988). Value acquisition in an occupational
initial negative emotional reactions people have toward persons therapy curriculum. Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 8, 259 –
with disabilities. Additional experimental research examining ac- 274.
Fichten, C. S., & Amsel, R. (1988). Thoughts concerning interaction
tual interactions between real people is required to verify this
between college students who have a physical disability and their non-
suggestion. It would also be useful to look at people who have
disabled peers. Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin, 32, 22– 40.
experience dealing with others with disabilities (e.g., people who Findler, L., Vilchinsky, N., & Werner, S. (2007). The Multidimensional
have a sibling with a disability) to test the limit of the negative Attitudes Scale toward persons with disabilities (MAS): Construction
emotions effect. and validation. Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin, 50, 166 –176.
Gillath, O., Shaver, P. R., & Mikulincer, M. (2005). An attachment-
Practical Implications theoretical approach to compassion and altruism. In P. Gilbert (Ed),
Compassion: Conceptualizations, research and use in psychotherapy
(pp. 121–147). New York: Routledge.
Understanding the attachment-disability issue not only adds
Jandrey, B. L. (1998). Attitudes toward persons with a physical disability:
theoretical insight but also bears practical implications. Informa-
The contributing factor of attachment style in a law enforcement popu-
tion about the public’s tendency to accept or reject persons on the lation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The Fielding Institute, Santa
basis of their disabilities is relevant to planning and evaluating Barbara, CA.
interventions designed to increase social integration. Based on the Joireman, J. A., Needham, T., & Cummings, A. (2002). Relationship
current findings, resources should be provided to change the atti- between dimensions of attachment and empathy. North American Jour-
tudes of people with anxious-attachment orientation. In addition, nal of Psychology, 4, 63– 80.
interventions for changing attitudes should target each attitude Jones, E. E., Farina, A., Hastorf, A. H., Markus, H., Miller, D. T., & Scott,
dimension separately, with a particular emphasis on the affect R. A. (1984). Social stigma: The psychology of marked relationships.
dimension. Our findings suggest that the affect factor is so innate New York: Freeman.
that even those personality constructs which emerge at the earliest Livneh, H. (1988). A dimensional perspective on the origin of negative
developmental stages—i.e., security and ego-strengths— do not attitudes toward persons with disabilities. In H. E. Yuker (Ed.), Attitudes
toward people with disabilities (pp. 35– 46). New York: Springer.
moderate it.
Marini, I. (2001). Cross-cultural counseling issues of males who sustain a
disability. Journal of Applied Rehabilitation Counseling, 32, 36 – 44.
References Mikulincer, M. (1997). Adult attachment style and information processing:
Individual differences in curiosity and cognitive closure. Journal of
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1217–1230.
interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (1998). The relationship between adult
Antonak, R. F., & Livneh, H. (1988). The measurement of attitudes toward attachment styles and emotional and cognitive reactions to stressful
people with disabilities: Methods, psychometrics and scales. Springfield, events. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and
IL: Charles C. Thomas. close relationships (pp. 143–165). New York: Guilford Press.
Antonak, R. F., & Livneh, H. (2000). Measurement of attitudes toward Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (2001). Attachment style and affect regula-
persons with disabilities. Disability and Rehabilitation, 22, 211–224. tion: Implications for coping with stress and mental health. In G. J. O.
306 VILCHINSKY, FINDLER, AND WERNER

Fletcher, M. S. Clark (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psychology: toward individuals with disabilities among students tutoring children
Interpersonal processes (pp. 537–557). Oxford, United Kingdom: with developmental disabilities. Journal of Intellectual and Develop-
Blackwell Publishers. mental Disability, 25, 13–18.
Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., & Tolmacz, R. (1990). Attachment styles and Sakellariou, D. (2006). If not the disability, then what? Barriers to reclaim-
fear of personal death: A case study of affect regulation. Journal of ing sexuality following spinal cord injury. Sexuality and Disability, 24,
Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 273–280. 101–111.
Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., Halevy, V., Avihou, N., Avidan, S., & Eshkoli, Shakespeare, T. (1999). The sexual politics of disabled masculinity. Sex-
N. (2001). Attachment theory and reactions to others’ needs: Evidence uality and Disability, 17, 53– 64.
that activation of the sense of attachment security promotes empathic Shaver, P. R., & Hazan, C. (1993). Adult romantic attachment: Theory and
responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1205– evidence. In D. Perlman & W. Jones (Eds.), Advances in personal
1224. relationships (Vol. 4, pp. 29 –70). Greenwich, CT: JAI.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2001). Attachment theory and intergroup Siller, J. (1963). Reactions to physical disability. Rehabilitation Counsel-
bias: Evidence that priming the secure base schema attenuates negative ing Bulletin, 7, 12–16.
reactions to out-groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Siller, J. (1984). The role of personality in attitudes toward those with
81, 97–115. physical disabilities. In C. J. Golden (Ed.), Current topics in rehabili-
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Struc- tation psychology (pp. 201–227). New York: Grune & Stratton.
ture, dynamics and change. New York: Springer. Siller, J. (1995). Reactions to physical disability by the disabled and the
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2008). Adult attachment and affect nondisabled. In J. Siller & K. R. Thomas (Eds.), Essays and research on
regulation. In J. Cassidi & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: disability (pp. 3–10). Athens, GA: Elliot & Fitzpatrick. (Original work
Theory, research and clinical applications (pp. 503–531). New York: published 1959)
Springer. Vilchinsky, N., & Findler, L. (2004). Attitudes toward Israel’s Equal
Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., Gillath, O., & Nitzberg, R. E. (2005). Rights for People with Disabilities Law: A multi-perspective approach.
Attachment, caregiving, and altruism: Augmentation of attachment se- Rehabilitation Psychology, 49, 309 –316.
curity increases compassion and helping. Journal of Personality and Vilchinsky, N., Werner, S., & Findler, L. (2010). Gender and attitudes
Social Psychology, 85, 817– 839. toward people using wheelchairs: A multidimensional approach. Reha-
Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., & Pereg, D. (2003). Attachment theory and bilitation Counseling Bulletin, 53, 163–174.
affect regulation: The dynamics, development, and cognitive conse- Westmaas, J. L., & Cohen-Silver, R. (2001). The role of attachment in
quences of attachment-related strategies. Motivation and Emotion, 27, response to victims of life crises. Journal of Personality and Social
77–102. Psychology, 80, 425– 438.
Noonan, J. R., Barry, J. R., & Davis, H. C. (1970). Personality determi- Winstead, B. A., Derlega, V. J., & Wong, P. T. P. (1984). Effect of sex-role
nants in attitudes toward visible disability. Journal of Personality, 38, orientation on behavioral self-disclosure. Journal of Research in Per-
1–15. sonality, 18, 541–553.
Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory (2nd ed.). New York: Wright, B. A. (1983). Physical disability: A psychosocial approach. New
McGraw-Hill. York: Harper & Row.
Oatley, K., & Jenkins, J. M. (1996). Understanding emotions. Cambridge,
MA: Blackwell.
Olson, J. M., & Zanna, M. P. (1993). Attitudes and attitude change. Annual Received February 4, 2010
Review of Psychology, 44, 117–154. Revision received May 31, 2010
Rimmerman, A., Hozmi, B., & Duvdevany, I. (2000). Contact and attitudes Accepted June 1, 2010 䡲

View publication stats

You might also like