You are on page 1of 20

Second Language Learners’ Attitudes

Toward French Varieties: The Roles of


Learning Experience and Social
Networks
RACHAEL LINDBERG1 AND PAVEL TROFIMOVICH2
1
Concordia University, Department of Education, 1610 Saint-Catherine Street W, Montreal, QC H3H 2S2,
Canada Email: rachael.lindberg@mail.concordia.ca
2
Concordia University, Department of Education, 1610 Saint-Catherine Street W, Montreal, QC H3H 2S2,
Canada Email: Pavel.Trofimovich@concordia.ca

People often believe that some language varieties are more prestigious than others, which can trigger
speech-centered biases and inform social judgments of the speaker. However, it is largely unknown what
types of language experience and exposure might mitigate language biases, especially for second lan-
guage (L2) learners. The goal of this study was to investigate this issue by focusing on L2 French learners’
attitudes toward European and Quebec varieties of French. L2 French learners in Montreal (N = 106)
rated 2 audios recorded by native speakers from France in a listening comprehension task, with 1 of
the 2 speakers introduced as a speaker of Quebec French. The learners described their language learn-
ing experience, filled out a French social network questionnaire, and completed a French proficiency
test. Results revealed some evidence of reverse linguistic stereotyping, with learners preferring to speak
like one speaker significantly more than the other, based on the speaker’s assumed identity, not actual
speech. Four of the 6 speaker ratings were also associated with participants’ oral proficiency scores, social
network density, and positive experiences in Quebec. Findings have implications for the use of speech
models in L2 teaching and for the mitigation of language-centered biases in L2 classrooms.
Keywords: Quebec French; European French; reverse linguistic stereotyping; language attitudes; L2
French

SPEAKERS OF MINORITY OR STIGMATIZED perience, they likely emerge from an internalized


language varieties are often aware of the pos- hierarchy of languages and language varieties
sibility of being judged based on their speech and affect both native speakers and second lan-
patterns or accent (Derwing, 2003; Gluszek & guage (L2) learners. The existence of biases can
Dovidio, 2010). Speech-based judgments are be detrimental to different facets of people’s
widespread, appearing in all aspects of life, from personal and professional lives, resulting in social
the workplace to educational settings, and can be disadvantages, because biases promote linguistic
triggered merely by a speech recording or even stereotyping, for example, where speech sam-
speech expectations based on visual cues (McKen- ples from a low-prestige variety trigger negative
zie, 2008; Rubin, 1992). Whether language-based attitudes toward individuals from that speech
attitudes develop from classroom or cultural ex- community (e.g., Dalton–Puffer, Kaltenboeck,
& Smit, 1997; Hume, Lepicq, & Bourhis,
1993).
For L2 speakers or speakers of regional vari-
The Modern Language Journal, 104, 4, (2020) eties, evaluations tainted by language attitudes
DOI: 10.1111/modl.12674 can have far-reaching consequences, impacting
0026-7902/20/822–841 $1.50/0 their education, housing, and employment op-
© National Federation of Modern Language Teachers portunities (e.g., Carlson & McHenry, 2006;
Associations
Davila, Bohara, & Saenz, 1993; Giles, Wilson, &
Rachael Lindberg and Pavel Trofimovich 823
Conway, 1981; Hopper & Williams, 1973). For ex- BACKGROUND
ample, less favorable speech styles may negatively
influence managers’ view of job applicants or Language Attitudes and Their Origins
landlords’ perceptions of potential tenants (Hy-
man, 2002; Munro, 2003). Speech-based biases It is common knowledge that people believe
are also harmful for nonnative-speaking teach- certain languages or language varieties to be more
ers, where discrimination against their accent may prestigious than others, causing them to favor
impact hiring decisions and perceptions of their the perceived superior language or variety (e.g.,
teaching effectiveness (Munro, 2003; Williams, Boulé, 2002; Kircher, 2012, 2014; Laur, 2008;
1976). Clearly, language cues have the power to Zhang & Hu, 2008). Individuals express language
influence social decisions in a variety of situa- preference along the dimensions of status, which
tions. Particularly relevant in educational settings, refers to the degree of utilitarian value language
language-based attitudes can not only influence holds (e.g., how much it will increase employment
how teachers view their students’ abilities and opportunities), and solidarity, which refers to how
character (e.g., Choy & Dodd, 1976; Seligman, much language elicits feelings of attachment to
Tucker, & Lambert, 1972), but can also affect how a community (e.g., how much language is an
L2 speakers view their own speech variety. For in- important aspect of one’s personal identity). Lan-
stance, L2 learners judge speakers with native ac- guage attitudes can also be measured indirectly
cents more positively than L2 speakers from their based on judgments of a speaker’s status (e.g.,
own language background (Chiba, Matsuura, & intelligence, leadership) and solidarity (e.g., like-
Yamamoto, 1995; Hu & Lindemann, 2009; Zhang, ability, sociability) traits: Downgrading a speaker’s
2013). traits after listening to them speak, relative to
Language-based attitudes might have consider- the ratings provided to the same or different
able consequences for the listener as well, such as speakers in another situation (e.g., in another
when a native speaker fails to understand the in- language), would indicate linguistic stereotyp-
tended message of the L2 interlocutor in native– ing. For example, Kircher (2014) used Lambert
nonnative interaction. Even though blame for et al.’s (1960) matched-guise technique to elicit
communication breakdowns is often placed on attitudes: Bilingual speakers made two stimulus
the perceived lack of proficiency of the L2 in- recordings, one in French and one in English,
terlocutor, it is often the native speaker’s nega- which were presented to listeners in a sequence
tive attitudes that hinder a successful exchange, as if the recordings belonged to separate indi-
such that biases restrict the native speaker from viduals. Kircher found evidence consistent with
putting full effort into understanding the L2 in- linguistic stereotyping, where both anglophone
terlocutor (Lindemann, 2002). In fact, listeners’ and francophone listeners judged the speakers
attitudes have been shown to affect their recall presented in the English guise to be better suited
of words (Weener, 1967) and to limit their over- to society than those presented in the French
all listening comprehension (e.g., Rubin, 1992). guise and rated English speakers more posi-
Assuming that attitudes impact language compre- tively than French speakers on all status-related
hension and recall, attitudes might be particularly traits.
detrimental in a classroom setting if, during the Certain languages might be inherently more
lesson, either the instructor or the audio learn- aesthetically pleasing and more linguistically
ing materials used for activities exhibit nonstan- sophisticated, relegating other varieties to the
dard speech. Therefore, what should be of most substandard level. More likely, however, a lan-
concern to language teachers are the educational guage’s value derives from cultural norms, based
consequences of L2 learners having biases against on the status of the social group speaking that
the target language, because these biases could af- variety (Edwards, 1999; Giles et al., 1974). For in-
fect learners’ perception of the target language stance, Giles, Bourhis, and Davies (1979) showed
and, subsequently, their success in the classroom. that adults in Wales who were unfamiliar with
In this study, we addressed this issue within an French did not perceive any variety of French
educational setting by investigating an underex- heard in Quebec (European French, educated
plored question—namely, whether learners hold Canadian French, or working-class Canadian
negative attitudes toward different varieties of the French) to carry more prestige over the other.
target language (such as European versus Que- However, when Welsh learners of French with
bec French) and whether various learner profi- some French experience evaluated these same
ciency and exposure variables could explain their varieties, they attributed the most prestige to
attitudes. European French. In essence, when individuals
824 The Modern Language Journal 104 (2020)
have no linguistic or cultural knowledge of a The Roles of Experience, Exposure, and Proficiency
language, they demonstrate similar attitudes
toward its varieties, but differences emerge as It remains unclear exactly what determines the
they become aware of the culturally charged degree to which one engages in RLS or what influ-
stereotypes through their experience with the ences one’s development of attitudes, but some
language. patterns have emerged. For example, L2 learn-
Language attitudes might also stem from ers tend to have more favorable views toward vari-
people’s own-accent bias, where judgments are eties they are familiar with (Ahn & Kang, 2017;
formed about the speech of other speakers due Chiba et al., 1995; Zhang & Hu, 2008). Listen-
to people’s inherent preference for their own ers with more exposure to an accent also tend
way of speaking (Bestelmeyer, Belin, & Ladd, to perceive that speech more accurately (e.g.,
2015). For example, individuals view speakers McGowan, 2015). Lambert et al. (1960) posited
with accents similar to their own as being more that differences in attitudes between two varieties
understandable, more favorable as teachers would be less pronounced for individuals with
(Gill, 1994), and more trustworthy (Lev–Ari & more experience with both linguistic groups. Sim-
Keysar, 2010) than dissimilarly accented speak- ilarly, results from Giles et al. (1974, 1979) sug-
ers. Children as young as 5 prefer to be friends gested that learners at a higher level of L2 French
with children who share the same accent, even proficiency attributed different levels of prestige
though they understand nonnative-speaking to varieties of French, compared to learners with
children equally well (Kinzler, Dupoux, & Spelke, no French knowledge, who held a comparable
2007). Additionally, language attitudes might view of prestige across varieties. In contrast, Kang
also reflect people’s subjective experience with and Rubin (2009) found that simply more expo-
speech. For instance, Dragojevic and Giles (2016) sure to L2 speech did not mitigate listeners’ judg-
asked participants to rate audios of American ments of the Asian guise in a RLS study. However,
and Punjabi English mixed with white noise, thus listeners who had previous experience teaching
making the processing of speech more difficult English to L2 learners were more likely to rate
for them. Participants provided more positive the Asian guise speech more positively. There-
ratings for the less noisy, more comprehensible fore, perhaps a greater amount of exposure alone
audios, regardless of the variety spoken, which is insufficient to lessen speech-based biases, but
suggests that listeners’ experience of process- meaningful interactions, such as those with stu-
ing difficulty might be associated with their dents or close friends, might produce such an ef-
negative attitudes toward the language and the fect (Kang & Rubin, 2012; Kang, Rubin, & Lin-
speaker. demann, 2015). It may thus be that it is the type
In other cases, language attitudes might be or quality of experience (e.g., dense social net-
constructed from factors extraneous to lan- works, positive interactions) rather than the quan-
guage altogether—an effect known as reverse tity of exposure to the language (e.g., length of
linguistic stereotyping (RLS). In essence, peo- L2 study or of residence in the L2 culture, L2
ple’s language-centered views often stem from proficiency level) that determines the strength of
preconceived ideas, expectations, or stereotypes (negative) attitudes, because “fostering quality so-
of speaker characteristics, such as age (Hay, cial contacts stimulates positive feelings and helps
Warren, & Drager, 2006), gender (Strand, 1999), to replace the negative perception of what is dif-
region of origin (Niedzielski, 1999), or native- ferent” (Cortes–Colomé, Barrieras, & Comellas,
versus-nonnative status (Rubin, 1992)—rather 2016, p. 284).
than from any properties of the speech itself.
Using a matched-guise technique, Rubin (1992) Language Attitudes and L2 Learners
was among the first to show that undergraduate
students evaluating the same recorded speech Regardless of their origins, language attitudes
sample paired with a different image of the can have important consequences, for instance,
speaker (one Caucasian, the other Asian) per- by impacting L2 learners’ performance in the
ceived the speech of the presumed Asian speaker classroom. For example, learner attitudes to-
as being accented and also performed worse on ward different languages or language varieties
a comprehension test based on the lecture (see might hinder their language performance (Hu
also Kang & Rubin, 2009). In this case, percep- & Su, 2015; Rubin, 1992; Ryan & Sebastian,
tions rather than reality appeared to underlie 1980). Using a listening comprehension task, Hu
these listeners’ attitudes and their linguistic and Su (2015) showed that Cantonese L2 En-
performance. glish learners who were told that the speaker
Rachael Lindberg and Pavel Trofimovich 825
was American outperformed those who thought 2015; Rubin, 1992). For instance, as suggested by
that the speaker’s background was Cantonese, Hume et al. (1993), if learners enter a classroom
even though both groups listened to the same with preconceived ideas of the target language
American speaker. L2 learners’ biases can also and the teacher’s accent evokes negative stereo-
lead to distorted perception of speech. Knowing types, learners may view their teacher’s compe-
that Cantonese-accented English is characterized tence negatively and might be less likely to iden-
by word-final stops frequently being unreleased, tify with them and less inclined to learn from
Hu and Lindemann (2009) presented the same them. More research is needed, however, to test
speaker as American and as Cantonese, to see if such possible effects of preconceived biases on
those labels would affect Cantonese L2 English lis- learners’ motivation to learn and their learning
teners’ perception of word-final stops. When lis- outcomes.
teners thought that the speaker was American,
they were more likely to hear a full release, com- THE CURRENT STUDY
pared to when they thought that the speaker was
Cantonese. These listeners likely stigmatized un- In this study, we sought to extend the limited
released stops as imperfect speech, leading them knowledge of how attitudes influence L2 learn-
to idealize American speech to the extent that ers’ evaluations of speech by focusing on learn-
they overlooked unreleased stops produced by ers of French, a previously underexplored pop-
an American speaker, as they were not expecting ulation. Instead of targeting learners’ judgments
them to occur. toward their own group’s L2 speech (e.g., Chiba
As discussed previously, RLS appears to trigger et al., 1995; Zhang, 2013), we examined their per-
distorted perceptions for native speakers evaluat- ceptions of different French varieties (European
ing various speech patterns belonging to regional French, Quebec French) spoken by native speak-
dialects and native-speaker varieties (Hay et al., ers as part of language teaching materials. We ex-
2006; Niedzielski, 1999) and occurs among L2 plored learners’ attitudes toward these varieties
learners when they are led to believe that they are in two ways: through speech ratings within the
exposed to L2 speech (Hu & Su, 2015). Yet what framework of RLS (on the assumption that biases
remains to be investigated is whether L2 learn- might emerge based on a speaker’s social attribu-
ers engage in RLS, thus revealing preconceived tion alone) and through learners’ qualitative reac-
attitudinal biases, when they listen to native- tions to the speaker. With respect to RLS research,
speaker varieties of the language they are learn- our study is novel as it extends prior RLS work
ing. It appears that L2 learners do express explicit (which to date has generally targeted listener atti-
preferences for specific language varieties, which tudes toward presumed nonnative speakers of En-
closely correspond to native speakers’ preferences glish) by eliciting attitudes toward speakers pre-
for standard or prestige varieties over ‘substan- sumed to speak two different regional varieties of
dard’ ones (e.g., Brown, Giles, & Thakerar, 1985; French. The RLS framework (presentation of a
Kircher, 2012). For example, L2 English learn- European French speaker as a speaker of the Que-
ers have been shown to rate British English sys- bec French variety) was essential in that it allowed
tematically more favorably than American English us to capture L2 learners’ preconceived attitudes
(Jarvella et al., 2001), and both more favorably about Quebec French based on the presumed (re-
than Australian English (Zhang & Hu, 2008), gional) identity of the speaker, rather than the ac-
revealing a prestige hierarchy. Similarly, when tual variation in the speaker’s speech.
speakers of Standard European French, Quebec The study was conducted in Montreal, Que-
French, Ontarian French, and English-accented bec, which is historically characterized by nega-
French were presented as possible teachers to tive attitudes toward speakers of Quebec French
English Canadian students, the students evalu- by various groups of language users, includ-
ated the linguistic and professional competence ing L2 learners studying French outside Que-
of the teacher more favorably when she was speak- bec (Hume et al., 1993), English-speaking resi-
ing the European French variety and responded dents and young immigrants in Quebec (Kircher,
more positively for wanting her as their instruc- 2012), and even by Quebec French speakers
tor (Hume et al., 1993). Because teachers often themselves (D’Anglejan & Tucker, 1973; Kircher,
provide the main source of input for learners 2012; Lambert et al., 1960). Although negative
and act as their speech model, less favorable atti- attitudes toward Quebec French appear to be
tudes toward certain language varieties may lead less pronounced in recent reports (Evans, 2002;
to concerning implications, especially if learn- Piechowiak, 2009), European French is still seen
ers’ comprehension is impacted (e.g., Hu & Su, as a preferred variety, especially by newcomers to
826 The Modern Language Journal 104 (2020)
Quebec (Kircher, 2012), likely because it remains have had the same amount of French study, we
the variety most commonly taught to L2 learn- obtained study-specific listening comprehension
ers worldwide (Bourhis, 1997; Kircher, 2012). and speaking measures for all participants (see
Whether university students currently studying the next section) so that these measures could
French in Quebec share similar (largely negative) be used in subsequent analyses. When asked what
language attitudes toward the local French variety variety of French they spoke, 38.2% of partic-
remains largely unknown. ipants considered their own French variety to
In addition, because experience and exposure be closer to Quebec French, whereas 61.8% of
variables seem to be key factors underlying lan- participants reported speaking a variety closer
guage attitudes (e.g., Kang & Rubin, 2009; Lam- to European French. The participants’ length of
bert et al., 1960), we also explored the role of Quebec residence varied between 2 weeks and
learners’ experience in relation to their attitudes 28 years (M = 3.14 years, SD = 5.15). They were
toward the Quebec variety of French. Language students at English-medium universities or were
attitudes are culturally formed, yet the question taking French classes at a community center.
remains as to the amount and type of experience
or the degree of language proficiency required
for learners to overcome any stereotypical biases Materials
about the target language. The study addressed
The materials included a listening task (audio
the following two research questions:
recordings accompanied by speaker and listener
versions of maps and rating scales), a language
RQ1. Do L2 French learners demonstrate any background questionnaire, a social network sur-
biases toward Quebec French through vey, a French comprehension test, and an oral
their ratings of speakers within the proficiency rubric.
framework of RLS or through their
Audio Recordings. Two 21-year-old female na-
qualitative responses (i.e., choice of
tive French speakers from Metz, France each
adjectives to describe speakers, stated
made a short audio recording. Female speakers
reasons for preferring one speaker over
were chosen because women represent the ma-
another)?
jority of teachers in Canada (Statistics Canada,
RQ2. How are learners’ French listening com-
2017). The audio content was prompted by maps
prehension and speaking performance
with marked routes (see Online Supporting In-
as well as exposure to and experience
formation A), which the speakers used to give di-
with Quebec French related to their at-
rections. Inspired by McKenzie’s (2008) map task,
titudes toward this French variety?
the maps were used to elicit natural but some-
what controlled speech. Each speaker made her
METHOD recording using a different map, resulting in two
Participants audios, one per map and speaker (see Online
Supporting Information A for transcripts), mean-
The participants were 106 adult residents of ing that each speaker narrated a unique set of
Montreal (65 females; Mage = 27.43 years, SD map directions. The two maps contained the same
= 6.96, range = 18–67), who were learning L2 images so that the lexical content remained con-
French at the time of the study. Participants repre- sistent, but the pictures were scrambled to allow
sented 30 different native languages, the majority the directions to differ.
of which were English (24 participants), Persian The two target recordings were drawn from a
(21), and Spanish (14). Their educational back- set of 14 audios by the same speakers using differ-
ground ranged from the BA level (38) to the MA ent speech rates (faster vs. slower), subject pro-
(48) and PhD (10) levels, while 10 participants nouns (tu vs. vous), speaking styles (longer vs.
provided no information about their status. As shorter pauses between directions), and with dif-
learners at any French level were invited to partici- ferent assignment of speakers to map versions.
pate, participants’ amount of French study varied The target recordings were chosen through pi-
as well, where 46 participants (45.1%) had been lot testing, during which they (along with 12
studying French for less than 1 year, 36 partici- additional audios) were evaluated by 10 native
pants (35.3%) had been studying the language French speakers (6 speakers of Quebec French,
for 1–5 years, and 20 (19.6%) had more than 4 speakers of European French) using 100-mm
5 years of French study. However, because pro- scales, where higher ratings indicated more posi-
ficiency level can vary, even among those who tive perceptions. The two target recordings, which
Rachael Lindberg and Pavel Trofimovich 827
were comparable in speech rate (112.12 and (desire to have the speaker as a teacher, desire to
115.7 words per minute) and length (35 and 46 speak like the speaker). All negative ratings were
seconds), received similar ratings for the four labeled on the left, and positive ratings were la-
rated dimensions: naturalness (MSpeaker1 = 94, beled on the right. The rated dimensions were
MSpeaker2 = 82), accentedness (MSpeaker1 = 99, selected from previous work on attitudes (Hume
MSpeaker2 = 96), comprehensibility (MSpeaker1 = 96, et al., 1993; Kircher, 2012, 2014; Lambert et al.,
MSpeaker2 = 93), and French variety (MSpeaker1 = 91, 1960). The final scale, asking participants to place
MSpeaker2 = 94), where a rating of 100 meant 100% the speaker’s French variety on a continuum be-
certainty that the speaker was from France. Ac- tween definitely Quebec French and definitely
cording to paired-samples t-tests, there were no European French served as a critical measure to
significant differences between the ratings for the determine whether participants believed that the
two recordings (ts < 1.54, ps > .16); additionally, speaker was truly speaking the French variety that
due to the counterbalanced presentation (see the was announced to them prior to playing the au-
next section), it was not always the same record- dio. Comment boxes were provided below each
ing presented as a Quebec French speaker, so scale for participants to explain the reason for
each audio had an equal chance to elicit poten- their rating.
tially harsher or more lenient reactions from par- The final question asked participants to circle
ticipants. Therefore, it could be assumed that any words from a word bank, consisting of 13
any difference in participants’ reactions to the positive and 12 negative descriptors, to describe
two recordings would be driven by their belief the speaker’s speech. These adjectives were either
about the speaker’s linguistic identity, not by the chosen based on words used by participants to
speaker’s actual speech output. describe European and Quebec French in this
study’s pilot project (pure, international, nonstan-
Listener Maps and Audio Rating Scales. The lis- dard, standard, proper, the original, educated, sophis-
tener maps were identical to the speaker maps, ticated, unclear, normal), or were selected from
but without the marked route. These were given Kircher’s (2012) word bank used to describe
to participants so they could attempt to follow the Quebec French (bizarre, accented, unattractive,
audio directions. Participants who self-identified unsophisticated, uneducated, difficult, incorrect, an-
as beginner-level learners (approximately 37) noying, unpleasant, not classy, easy, elegant, musical,
were given maps with labeled images. Having par- clear, smooth). After listening to both speakers,
ticipants complete the maps promoted active lis- participants circled their preferred speaker for
tening and simulated a realistic listening activity the activity and (optionally) explained their
appropriate for their level, which also increased choice.
the likelihood that the study’s results might clar-
Background Questionnaire. This questionnaire
ify how attitudes can arise in classrooms. The
elicited details about participants’ age, gender,
37 participants who self-identified as beginner-
country of origin, language background, years of
level learners obtained lower speaking scores (M
French study, length of stay in Quebec, and their
= 19.97, SD = 1.99, range = 16.00–23.50) and
attitudes toward Quebec French. There were also
lower listening comprehension scores (M = 8.24,
eleven 100-mm scales targeting participants’ ex-
SD = 3.29, range = 0–14.00) based on study-
posure to and familiarity with European and Que-
specific measures (as described in a later sec-
bec French, including in their previous French
tion), compared to the remaining 69 participants’
classes, their familiarity with Quebec French, and
speaking (M = 27.20, SD = 2.89, range = 23.00–
the perception of their own French variety on
34.50) and listening comprehension scores (M
a continuum between European and Quebec
= 12.69, SD = 3.02, range = 6.00–19.00). This
French.
implied that our decision to assist beginner-level
learners with map-task vocabulary was justifiable Social Network Survey. Adapted from a so-
based on their self-reports. cial network instrument by Doucerain et al.
To accompany the maps, participants received (2015), this survey collected information about
two sets of rating scales (one per audio), both the French speakers from each participant’s social
consisting of the same 100-millimeter continuous network (see Online Supporting Information B).
scales (a line with labeled endpoints). The scales It allowed participants to record the French speak-
were used to rate the speaker’s speech (for accent- ers (native or nonnative) that they interacted with
edness, comprehensibility), and also included rat- and whether those speakers knew one another.
ings pertaining to the dimensions of status (intel- Each person’s variety of French was noted, as
ligence, competence as a teacher) and solidarity well as participant’s relationship to them, amount
828 The Modern Language Journal 104 (2020)
TABLE 1
Data Collection Procedure

Data collection task Description Timing

Consent Participants were introduced to the study and affirmed consent 3 minutes
Practice The researcher administered a practice exercise with sample 3 minutes
map and scale
Speaker The first speaker was introduced as a French teacher from 1 minute
introduction Quebec or from France, depending on counterbalanced order
assignment
Map Task 1 Participants followed the speaker’s directions by drawing a line 1 minute
on their map accordingly
Rating scales Participants used scales to rate the speaker and optionally left 7 minutes
comments regarding their ratings
Map Task 2 Steps 3–5 were repeated with the second speaker, who was 9 minutes
introduced as a French teacher from France or from Quebec,
depending on counterbalanced order assignment
Questionnaires Participants filled out the background questionnaire and social 10 minutes
network survey
Listening Participants completed the TEFAQ listening comprehension test 20 minutes
comprehension (used to derive a listening comprehension measure)
Speaking activity Participants recorded responses to two open-ended questions in 5 minutes
French with a partner (used to derive a measure of L2
speaking)
Debrief Participants were informed of the true purpose of the study and 3 minutes
were told that both speakers were from France

Note. TEFAQ = Test for Evaluating French for Access to Quebec.

of interaction with them in French, and level Procedure


of closeness (intimacy) to them (where 1 rep-
resented someone they did not know very well, To disguise the true purpose of the experi-
and 5 represented someone who they shared a ment, the project was presented to participants as
close relationship with). The intimacy rating is a study investigating the effectiveness and quality
an important measure because attitudes are re- of French listening comprehension activities.
lated to both the frequency of social interac- The procedure (summarized in Table 1) either
tions and to the quality (intimacy levels) of ex- took place during regular classroom instruction
changes (Cortes–Colomé et al., 2016; Kang & Ru- (with classes of 6–15 students) or during sched-
bin, 2009). uled times outside class hours (in groups of 4–8
people), with participants completing the testing
Measures of L2 French Listening Comprehension procedure individually. After signing the consent
and Speaking. To obtain a study-specific measure form, a practice exercise consisting of a sample
of participants’ L2 listening, a listening compre- scale and a simple listener map with different
hension test was administered based on the stan- items was administered by the researcher in a
dardized Test for Evaluating French for Access live (i.e., not prerecorded) presentation. For
to Quebec (TEFAQ; Chambre de commerce et classes of beginner-level learners (37), the vocab-
d’industrie Paris Ile-de-France, 2018), recognized ulary used in the activity was introduced prior to
by Quebec’s Ministry for Immigration, Diversity, starting the task.
and Inclusion and available publicly (https:// The main research activities involved partici-
www.lefrancaisdesaffaires.fr/tests-diplomes/test- pants listening to two different recordings fea-
evaluation-francais-tef/tef-quebec-tefaq). To de- turing separate map tasks, each recorded by a
rive a study-specific measure of participants’ L2 different speaker (see the previous section). Us-
speaking (see the next section), digital recorders ing a modified matched-guise procedure (Lam-
were also provided to pairs of participants, where bert et al., 1960), the speaker narrating the task
they recorded themselves answering two open- was introduced to participants under a different
ended questions related to their future plans and guise—as a French teacher from either France
opinions about Montreal. (European guise) or Quebec (Quebec guise). Put
Rachael Lindberg and Pavel Trofimovich 829
differently, although both audios were recorded graduate?” Learners who had only recently started
by European French speakers, one of the two learning French during the few weeks prior
was introduced as a speaker of European French (n = 9), all of whom were among the 37 partic-
(true identity) while the other was presented as ipants who self-identified as beginner-level learn-
a speaker of Quebec French (false identity), to ers, were asked to simply present themselves and
see if the false attribution of linguistic identity their interests. At the end of the session, partici-
alone would affect participants’ ratings, just as the pants were debriefed about the study’s goals and
false Asian identity did for participants in previous were informed that both speakers used for the
RLS research (Kang & Rubin, 2009; Rubin, 1992). map tasks were in fact from France.
Participants followed the speaker’s directions by
drawing a line on their map. Beginner-level learn- DATA ANALYSIS
ers were only asked to connect the images in the
order they heard them in the recording. The same Audio Ratings
audio was then played again after which partici-
pants filled out the rating scales for that audio. The ratings were measured (in millimeters)
This procedure was repeated with the second map from the left side of the scale to the cross marked
and second speaker (see Table 1). It was not feasi- by participants. The key measures were the six
ble to have a distractor activity before the second speaker ratings: accentedness, comprehensibility,
map task because it would have extended the test- intelligence, competence as a teacher, partici-
ing procedure beyond the limited classroom time pants’ desire to have the speaker as a teacher, and
we were given. their desire to speak like her. Rating reliability
There were four counterbalanced lists in which (Cronbach’s alpha) across all participants’ assess-
the two map tasks were arranged, and each par- ments of these six dimensions in each guise ex-
ticipant experienced one list only. Across the four ceeded .91 per dimension.
lists, each map task appeared twice as the first and Background Questionnaire
twice as the second in a sequence of two tasks;
and each task was presented under the European The questionnaire ratings were coded the same
guise twice and under the Quebec guise twice as way as the audio ratings. To reduce the num-
well. Participant assignment to each list was ran- ber of (potentially associated) variables, an ex-
dom, but we ensured that approximately an equal ploratory principal component analysis (PCA)
number of participants (out of 106) completed with Oblimin rotation was conducted to deter-
the study using each list. In the end, 22 partici- mine whether the 11 rated background variables
pants completed the testing using List 1, 29 partic- showed any underlying patterns. The Kaiser–
ipants experienced the tasks using List 2, 26 par- Meyer–Olkin value (.643) exceeded the required
ticipants performed the tasks using List 3, and 29 .60 for sampling adequacy, and Bartlett’s test of
participants completed the tasks through List 4. sphericity, χ 2 (55) = 226.02, p < .001, indicated
Therefore, all participants experienced both tasks that correlations between variables were suffi-
(i.e., heard each of the two speakers) but in differ- ciently large for PCA (Hutcheson & Sofroniou,
ent task orders and in different guises, such that 1999). An initial analysis revealed four underly-
the Quebec guise was not always associated with ing factors with eigenvalues over Kaiser’s criterion
the same task (speaker) and did not always appear of 1, accounting for 64.75% of the variance. After
in the same order. the first four components in the scree plot, there
The background questionnaire and social net- was a clear discontinuity, revealing four underly-
work survey were introduced after participants ing factors. Table 2 shows the factor loadings for
completed the map tasks. Participants filled out these four factors.
the social network table, then drew lines to con- Factor 1, labeled Classroom exposure and familiar-
nect the people that knew one another within ity with Quebec French, encompassed participants’
their social network. Next, the TEFAQ listen- familiarity with Quebec French, what they be-
ing comprehension test was administered to the lieved their own French variety to be, and their ex-
entire group, and participants marked their re- posure to each variety in their French classes (with
sponses in a test booklet. Finally, audio recorders greater exposure to European French associated
were distributed, and each pair of participants with less exposure to and familiarity with Quebec
recorded their response to the following two ques- French). Factor 2, with questions targeting how
tions: “If you were to describe Montreal to some- much participants found Quebec French to be
one from your hometown, what would you tell an integral part of their identity and their opin-
them?” and “What are your plans for after you ion of the overall usefulness of Quebec French
830 The Modern Language Journal 104 (2020)
TABLE 2
Factor Loadings From Principal Component Analysis of Background Questionnaire

Component

Questionnaire item 1 2 3 4

Exposure to European French in class −.77


Own French variety −.77
Exposure to Quebec French in class .76
Familiarity with Quebec French .60
Quebec French as part of identity .85
Usefulness of Quebec French .84
Feel welcome in Quebec .87
Experience with Quebec French speakers .83
Perceived importance of French in Quebec .78
Quebec French media exposure .64
Speaking with Quebec French speakers .54
Eigenvalue 2.85 1.70 1.35 1.23
Variance explained (%) 25.86 15.42 12.28 11.18

compared to European French, was labeled Per- specific measure of French listening compre-
sonal relevance of Quebec French. Factor 3, labeled hension. To obtain a measure of participants’
Positive experience in Quebec, captured how welcome L2 speaking, their audio recordings were evalu-
the participants felt in Quebec and the extent to ated by two native French judges (one European
which they experienced positive interactions with French speaker, one Quebec French speaker)
Quebec French speakers. Factor 4, labeled Quebec who both had 10 years of experience teaching
French use, dealt with participants’ amount of ex- French and were employed as evaluators for the
posure to Quebec French through media or na- TEFAQ exam (with 1 and 6 years of experience,
tive speakers, and with how important they per- respectively). To stay consistent with the grading
ceived the use of French to be in Quebec. Four of the TEFAQ, the standardized evaluation crite-
factor scores (one per component) were derived ria for the Common European Framework of Ref-
for further analyses using the Anderson–Rubin erence for Languages (CEFR) were used to rate
method for obtaining noncorrelated scores fol- participants’ range in linguistic forms and vocabu-
lowing PCA. lary, their grammatical accuracy, speech flow, and
coherence. The levels consisted of A0, A1, A2, B1,
B2, C1, and C2 (where A0 corresponds to very lim-
Social Network Survey
ited knowledge of French, and C2 implies native-
Following Doucerain et al. (2015), four mea- like performance). For statistical analysis, each
surements were calculated per participant: L2 net- level was converted to a numerical value (where
work size (total number of French speakers in C2 was worth 10 points, as the highest level at-
one’s social network), L2 intimacy (average close- tainable, and A0 was worth 4 points). Therefore,
ness rating across all people listed), L2 inclusive- a score corresponding to a level was assigned to
ness (number of nonisolated people divided by each speaking component (range, accuracy, flu-
the L2 network size), and L2 density (number of ency, coherence), for a total score of 40 possible
links between the people divided by the number points. The two raters demonstrated high consis-
of possible links). Because we focused on attitudes tency in their ratings (.90), so their overall scores
toward Quebec French, further analyses only in- for each participant were averaged to derive a
cluded the scores for Quebec French speakers in single measure. The two tests thus yielded two
participant networks. separate measures: a L2 listening comprehension
score, which was relevant to the target listening
task, and a L2 speaking score, which provided a
Measures of L2 French Speaking and Listening
study-specific estimate of participants’ speaking
Comprehension
ability (given that our participants came from vari-
The TEFAQ comprehension test was graded ous language programs and course levels). These
out of 20 points, with the score used as a study- two measures were explored separately for their
Rachael Lindberg and Pavel Trofimovich 831
FIGURE 1
Histogram of Participants’ Ratings of Speakers’ Regional Identity

50 50

40 40

Number of parcipants
Number of parcipants

30 30

20 20

10 10

0 0
0 20 40 60 80 100 0 20 40 60 80 100
European guise rang Quebec guise rang
Note. 0 = Quebec French, 100 = European French.

potential relationships with participants’ audio plain) the variation in participants’ tendency to
ratings. go along with the manipulation and to explore its
possible impact on speaker ratings, participants’
rating of the Quebec guise (reflecting their de-
RESULTS
gree of sensitivity to the manipulation) was in-
Preliminary Analysis cluded as a variable in subsequent quantitative
analyses.
Before proceeding with the main analysis, we
first checked the extent to which participants
L2 Learners’ Attitudes Toward Quebec French
believed that the Quebec guise indeed involved
a Quebec French speaker. This check focused Speech Ratings. To answer the first RQ, which
on the scale asking participants to identify the asked whether L2 learners demonstrate any at-
speaker’s French variety (0 = Quebec French, titudinal biases toward Quebec French, we first
100 = European French). The speaker in the Eu- compared participants’ ratings of the speakers in
ropean guise was recognized consistently as Eu- the two guises for the six target dimensions: ac-
ropean French, as illustrated in the negatively centedness, comprehensibility, intelligence, com-
skewed histogram in Figure 1 (left panel). In con- petence as a teacher, desire to have the speaker
trast, the speaker in the Quebec guise elicited as a teacher, and desire to sound like her. If par-
a wide range of ratings, sometimes identified as ticipants held any attitudinal biases toward Que-
Quebec French and sometimes (correctly) as Eu- bec French, they should rate the two speakers dif-
ropean French (Figure 1, right panel). Thus, ferently depending on their presented linguistic
there was a range of sensitivity to the RLS ma- identity as a speaker of either European or Que-
nipulation among participants, such that some bec French. As summarized in Table 3, planned
participants believed the false presentation of the pairwise comparisons yielded only one significant
speaker’s identity while others rejected the false difference—namely, participants were less willing
identity by labeling the speaker correctly as Euro- to sound like the speaker in the Quebec guise
pean French. We assumed that those who believed (M = 69.39) than the speaker in the European
our false presentation of the speaker’s identity guise (M = 76.20), with a small effect size (Plon-
would be those whose preconceived ideas about sky & Oswald, 2014). This result should be inter-
Quebec French might be activated. We expected preted cautiously given that a conservative Bonfer-
these participants to reveal attitudinal biases to- roni correction (α = .008) would have rendered
ward Quebec French through more negative rat- the obtained difference nonsignificant.
ings, compared to the ratings for the speaker in Although participants generally showed a pref-
the European guise (e.g., Kang & Rubin, 2009). erence to sound like the speaker in the Euro-
Therefore, to account for (and ultimately ex- pean guise, this tendency was amplified by their
832 The Modern Language Journal 104 (2020)
TABLE 3
Summary Statistics for Rated Dimensions and Between-Guise Comparisons

European guise Quebec guise Paired-samples t-test

Rated dimension M (SD) M (SD) t df p d

Accentedness 52.73 (31.69) 52.54 (26.88) 0.07 104 .948 0.00


Comprehensibility 61.34 (28.68) 61.64 (26.78) −0.09 103 .926 0.01
Desire to speak like 76.20 (24.73) 69.39 (26.26) 2.18 103 .032 0.26
Good teacher 72.53 (22.12) 70.77 (20.21) 0.76 102 .448 0.08
Their teacher 66.63 (22.88) 64.63 (22.47) 0.76 104 .449 0.09
Intelligence 66.64 (20.56) 66.66 (19.44) −0.01 95 .989 0.00

Note. Degrees of freedom (df) vary because of several missing responses per rated dimension.

FIGURE 2
Relationship Between Participants’ Sensitivity to the Guise Manipulation and Desire to Emulate Guise

100
Desire to speak like Quebec guise

80

60

40

20

0 20 40 60 80 100
Quebec guise rang
Note. The regression line shows the best linear fit to the data.

sensitivity to the guise manipulation (illustrated tential attitudinal reactions that might underlie
in Figure 2 through regression). As shown by a their ratings. Regarding the question after each
significant β coefficient of .22, where a linear re- audio asking participants to choose descriptors
lationship proved to provide the best fit, t = 2.31, characterizing each speaker’s speech, they tended
p = .02, 95% CI = [.03, .40], those who tended to to choose positive and negative adjectives at simi-
believe the Quebec guise manipulation (low rat- lar rates in response to the Quebec French guise
ings on the x-axis) were less likely to prefer the (81% positive, 19% negative) and the European
presumed Quebec French speaker as a desirable French guise (86.2% positive, 13.8% negative).
speech model (by an average of −22 points on The descriptors most often chosen for Quebec
a 100-point scale) than those who were not sus- French were normal (63 out of 106 participants),
ceptible to the manipulation (high ratings on the clear (57), smooth (56), standard (49), and ac-
x-axis). cented (38), and these rates were similar to those
shown for the European French guise (see On-
Qualitative Responses. We next examined par-
line Supporting Information C for full frequency
ticipants’ qualitative responses to capture any po-
Rachael Lindberg and Pavel Trofimovich 833
TABLE 4
Sample Reasons for Participants’ Preferred Speaker

Preference for European French guise Preference for Quebec French guise

“[She] speak as native France French. She didn’t “I’m more used to her pronunciation and the rest
use the accent, her voice was pure and of the people at Quebec pronounces like her.”
international.” “If you are living in Quebec, you should get in use
“Preferred that accent and more universal to this accent.”
(standard).” “Since I live in Quebec, I would choose this
“I felt that it’s more useful to learn standard teacher at the moment.”
French as it is better understood worldwide.” “Since I personally need to get more used to a
“Because French originated in France and I prefer Quebecois accent.”
French teachers.” “Easier to understand, more accented speaking.”
“More sophisticated.” “Because she has the accent Quebecois and I want
“More elegant and was easy to understand and to learn like that.”
clear, which is required.” “I liked the more accented voice.”
“More used to France French accent, so find it
easier to follow.”

data). However, participants appeared to have pants who justified their preference for the Que-
attributed certain positive descriptors more fre- bec French guise, most reasons were also related
quently to the European French guise than to to the speaker’s speech being easier to understand
the Quebec French guise: international (26 vs. 8), (52% of the reasons given), more pleasant to lis-
proper (50 vs. 36), the original (24 vs. 10), sophisti- ten to (20%), or more familiar (8%)—but also
cated (27 vs. 11), elegant (33 vs. 20), and pure (26 because they preferred to learn Quebec French
vs. 17). In addition, when given the same word (20%). As seen in Table 4, at least some of these
bank on the background questionnaire and di- reasons seem to have been based on the assump-
rectly asked to circle those describing Quebec tion that the two speakers had different linguistic
French, twice as many participants overall chose backgrounds, revealing attitudes toward the two
accented (76 out of 106 participants), making it French varieties. Taken together, the results from
the most frequent descriptor, followed by difficult speech ratings and questionnaire responses sug-
(47), unclear (41), nonstandard (33), bizarre (27), gest that participants did not generally differ in
and normal (27). Negative descriptors were cho- their evaluations of the two speech guises (shown
sen over three times more frequently to describe through ratings) but that they nevertheless held
Quebec French on the questionnaire (32.9% pos- some bias against Quebec French (shown through
itive, 67.1% negative) than after listening to the their qualitative responses).
speaker (81% positive, 19% negative).
At the end of the session, participants selected
Proficiency, Exposure, and Experience and Learners’
the speaker they preferred for the activity, allow-
Attitudes Toward Quebec French
ing them to express their overall impressions af-
ter being able to compare both speakers. Based To answer the second RQ, which investigated
on 33 participants who explained their prefer- possible associations between L2 learners’ atti-
ence for the European French guise, their pref- tudes toward Quebec French and their profi-
erence was largely based on the speaker’s speech ciency and exposure profiles, the next analysis in-
being clearer and easier to understand (46.5% volved Pearson correlations (two-tailed) between
of the reasons), even though they did not distin- participants’ ratings of the speaker in the Quebec
guish the two speakers through comprehensibil- guise and their proficiency, exposure, and expe-
ity ratings. In addition, their choice was explained rience variables specific to Quebec French (see
through reference to the speaker’s French be- Online Supporting Information D for a parallel
ing more standard (14%), more aesthetically set of analyses targeting participants’ attitudes to-
pleasing, purer and more sophisticated (11.6%), ward European French; although these analyses
more pleasant to listen to (9.3%), more famil- fall outside this study’s scope, they might be of in-
iar (9.3%), or because they simply preferred the terest to some readers). As shown in Table 5, four
accent from France (9.3%). Of the 25 partici- of the six ratings of the speaker in the Quebec
834 The Modern Language Journal 104 (2020)
TABLE 5
Correlations Between Background Variables and Speaker Ratings in the Quebec French Guise

Speak Good Their


Background variable Accent Comprehensibility like teacher teacher Intelligent

1. Length of French study .01 .25* −.14 .01 −.02 −.08


2. Length of Quebec .04 .23* −.05 .05 .01 −.10
residence
3. Social network size −.03 .21* −.15 .06 .06 −.05
4. Social network intimacy −.06 .10 −.26** −.12 −.14 .03
5. Social network .04 .17 −.24* −.06 −.10 −.11
inclusiveness
6. Social network density .06 .11 −.25** −.12 −.19 −.11
7. Listening .11 .26** −.09 .03 −.10 −.12
comprehension score
8. Speaking score .14 .35** −.15 .06 −.04 −.12
9. Classroom exposure −.13 −.01 −.02 −.05 −.03 .08
and familiarity with
Quebec French
10. Relevance of Quebec .02 −.12 .11 −.07 .01 .16
French
11. Positive experience in .20 .27** .27** .28** .21: .10
Quebec
12. Use of Quebec French .05 .26** −.11 .07 .11 −.06
13. Quebec guise .21* .19 .23* .20* .12 .08
sensitivity

*p < .05. **p < .01 (two-tailed).

French guise (comprehensibility, desire to speak in the Quebec French guise (0 = Quebec French,
like the speaker, her competence as a teacher, and 100 = European French) were entered in Step 1
desire to have her as a teacher) were associated in both models, to account for participants’ sensi-
with several background and exposure variables. tivity to the guise manipulation, and the relevant
The PCA-derived background variable of having background and exposure variables were entered
had positive experiences in Quebec (i.e., feeling in a stepwise procedure in Step 2. For comprehen-
welcome in Quebec, having positive experiences sibility, the predictors entered in the model were
with Quebec French speakers) was the only vari- Variables 1–3, 7, 8, 11, and 12 (as shown in Ta-
able positively linked to the two teacher ratings, ble 5). For desire to speak like the speaker, the
with weak associations (Plonsky & Oswald, 2014). predictors entered in the model were Variables 4–
Those participants who had reported more pos- 6 and 11 (as listed in Table 5). The predictors cho-
itive experiences in Quebec, compared to those sen for each model only included those that had
with fewer positive experiences, provided higher the potential to influence the criterion variable in
ratings for the speaker in the Quebec guise, eval- each model, based on their nontrivial associations
uating her as a more competent teacher and ex- (r > .20) with the measures of comprehensibility
pressing a stronger desire to have her as their and desire to speak like the speaker. The sample
teacher. size of 106 was deemed sufficiently large to detect
To determine the relative weight of multiple a medium-size effect with up to 10 predictors en-
background and exposure variables associated tered into the model (Field, 2009).
with the ratings of comprehensibility and desire The regression model for comprehensibility
to speak like the speaker (see Table 5), we carried (summarized in Table 6) yielded a two-factor solu-
out two regression analyses separately for these tion, accounting for 19% of shared variance. First,
two ratings. In each analysis, the speech rating participants’ sensitivity to the Quebec guise was
was the criterion variable, and the background associated with the rating of how comprehensi-
and exposure variables in Table 5 that were signif- ble they found the speaker (7% of shared vari-
icantly correlated with the relevant speech rating ance), F(1,90) = 6.94, p = .01, such that those
served as predictors. The ratings of speaker origin who were more likely to go along with the guise
Rachael Lindberg and Pavel Trofimovich 835
TABLE 6
Results of Multiple Regression Analyses Using Background and Exposure/Experience Variables as Predictors
of Speaker Ratings in the Quebec French Guise

Predictors R R2 β 95% CI t p

Comprehensibility
Sensitivity to guise manipulation .27 .07 0.26 [0.07, 0.46] 2.63 .010
Speaking score .44 .12 2.13 [0.95, 3.31] 3.58 .001
Desire to speak like the speaker
Sensitivity to guise manipulation .23 .05 0.22 [0.03, 0.41] 2.31 .023
Social network density .36 .08 −25.64 [−43.46, −7.81] −2.86 .005
Positive experience in Quebec .47 .09 8.01 [3.22, 12.79] 3.32 .001

manipulation (i.e., labeling the speaker as Que- network density (8% of variance explained) and
bec French) tended to provide lower compre- positive experience in Quebec (8% of variance ex-
hensibility ratings. Speaking score was the only plained) predicting participants’ desire to sound
other significant predictor accounting for addi- like the presumed Quebec French speaker.
tional variance (12%) in rated comprehensibil-
ity, F(2,90) = 10.34, p < .001. Thus, regardless of
their sensitivity to the guise manipulation, partic- DISCUSSION
ipants with higher L2 speaking scores, compared
L2 Learners’ Attitudes Toward Target Language
to those with lower scores, tended to assign higher
Varieties
ratings to the speaker in the Quebec guise.
The regression model targeting participants’ The first RQ asked whether L2 French learn-
desire to sound like the speaker (see Table 6) ers demonstrate any attitudinal bias toward Que-
yielded a three-factor solution, with sensitivity to bec French, either through speaker ratings in a
guise manipulation, social network density, and RLS procedure or through their qualitative re-
positive experience in Quebec jointly accounting sponses. With respect to speaker ratings, any dif-
for 22% of shared variance in participants’ de- ference in the ratings given to two speakers of
sire to sound like the speaker (R2 = .22). Partici- European French (one of whom was falsely pre-
pants’ sensitivity to the guise manipulation again sented as a Quebec French speaker) would be
predicted the rating (5%), F(1,95) = 5.35, p = consistent with RLS, due to learners’ judgments
.023. The density of participants’ Quebec French being influenced by the label of the speaker’s
social networks accounted for additional variance identity. Despite our expectations, there was only
(8%), F(2,95) = 6.96, p = .002, with a negative as- weak support for possible bias. Of the six target
sociation. The PCA-derived background variable measures, only the rating of learners’ desire to
of having had positive experiences in Quebec was sound like the speaker (a solidarity trait) was asso-
the final predictor (9%), F(3,95) = 8.83, p < .001, ciated with a between-speaker difference, where
with a positive association. Thus, regardless of learners expressed a marginally stronger prefer-
their sensitivity to the guise manipulation, partici- ence to speak like the speaker in the European
pants with sparser Quebec French social networks French guise than in the Quebec French guise.
and those with more positive experiences in Que- Thus, at least for this measure, learners seemed to
bec tended to express a stronger desire to sound have engaged in RLS, with knowledge of Quebec-
like the speaker in the Quebec guise compared to specific stereotypes contributing to their ratings.
those with denser social networks and fewer posi- Social bias linked to a particular social group
tive experiences in Quebec. Finally, we conducted often affects not only speaker preference but
two post hoc regression analyses (suggested by an also listening comprehension (Hu & Su, 2015;
anonymous reviewer) where participants’ ratings Kang & Rubin, 2009; Rubin, 1992). To the ex-
of speaker origin in the European French guise tent that comprehensibility (a measure of ease of
was entered as a variable in Step 1. Results re- understanding) captures some aspects of speech
mained unchanged, with participants’ speaking comprehension (see Kang, Thomson, & Moran,
scores being the only significant predictor of the 2019), there was no quantitative evidence in this
presumed Quebec French speaker’s comprehen- study of preconceived ideas influencing learn-
sibility (9% of variance explained) and with social ers’ comprehension of the speakers, as both were
836 The Modern Language Journal 104 (2020)
rated equally comprehensible. Similarly, there cult, yet these expectations did not seem to lead
were no between-speaker differences for mea- them to assign harsher ratings to the presumed
sures of speaker status. For example, the ratings of Quebec French speaker, as both guises were rated
speaker intelligence were comparable across both as equally accented and equally easy to under-
guises because the majority gave a neutral rating stand. Of course, it is possible that our guise ma-
and responded that they could not accurately rate nipulation did not produce the intended effect,
either speaker based solely on a voice recording, such that learners did not believe that one speaker
while also suggesting that a speech variety is inde- was speaking Quebec French, which might have
pendent of a speaker’s intelligence. precluded them from expressing their attitudes.
Based on prior results by Hume et al. (1993), In addition, even though participants were as-
where undergraduate L2 French learners in On- sured that their responses would remain anony-
tario rated the European French teacher highest mous, any learners holding negative bias may have
on status traits and professional competence, we been unwilling to express their attitudes (Munro
also expected that the European French speaker et al., 2006). Nevertheless, there were many learn-
would be evaluated more positively regarding her ers who went along with the manipulation, down-
teaching competence and would be rated more fa- grading the presumed Quebec French speaker
vorably as a potential teacher, compared to the re- (see Figures 1 and 2), such that those who be-
actions to the presumed Quebec French speaker. lieved the false presentation of the speaker’s iden-
However, there was no difference in learners’ tity were the ones who expressed more negative
preference for having either speaker as a teacher, attitudes toward wanting to speak like her.
and one was not believed to be a better teacher A more likely explanation for the lack of strong
than the other. Instead, learners generally showed RLS effects in this study is that we targeted two
awareness of the importance of being exposed to native varieties, where ratings could depend on
different varieties, with 86 out of 106 responding listeners’ familiarity and experience with each
that having a combination of both French vari- variety. In contrast, in most prior RLS studies,
eties spoken would be ideal. listeners were led to believe that they were ex-
Whereas speech ratings (collected through a posed to L2 speakers (e.g., Hu & Su, 2015; Ru-
RLS procedure) yielded weak evidence for con- bin, 1992), which triggered their biases against
sistent bias effects, learners’ qualitative responses accented speech. Indeed, when it comes to ac-
suggested that many believed European French to cented speech, listeners can easily distinguish
carry more prestige than Quebec French. Outside native speakers from L2 speakers (e.g., Munro,
the rating task, learners often expressed explicit Derwing, & Burgess, 2010), but they may know
preference for European French or generally less about which speech patterns differentiate
associated it with positive descriptors, choosing two native varieties (e.g., Clark & Schleef, 2010)
negative adjectives for Quebec French over three and, more specifically, have little understanding
times more frequently on the posttask question- of what phonetic features characterize Quebec
naire than during the rating task. L2 learners French (Bergeron, 2019). It remains for future re-
referred to European French as being more search to establish whether and to what degree
standard, universal, familiar, and sophisticated, L2 learners can distinguish different varieties of
implying that they consider it the standard variety. French and to examine whether biases are am-
Similarly, they most frequently chose accented to plified or mitigated when learners evaluate actual
describe Quebec French (76 out of 106 partici- (rather than presumed) speakers of a particular
pants), which is a biased expectation because ac- variety.
centedness was assessed similarly in the two guises.
These attitudes toward Quebec French, expressed Proficiency, Social Networks, and Exposure
outside the rating task, indicated a prestige hi-
erarchy consistent with that held by participants If listeners’ attitudes depend on their familiar-
of Kircher’s (2012) study, who classified Quebec ity and experience with each speech variety, then
French as bizarre, anglicized, and accented. it is important to understand how listener back-
The conclusion that emerges from both sets of ground and experiential profiles contribute to
findings (rating task, qualitative responses) is that their ratings. Our second RQ therefore aimed to
learners held negative attitudes toward Quebec explore what background and experiential vari-
French but that these attitudes did not generally ables were associated with learners’ ratings of
influence their ratings in a RLS procedure. For the Quebec French guise, which represents a
example, the top two descriptors chosen by learn- novel contribution. Learners’ experience in in-
ers for Quebec French were accented and diffi- teractions involving Quebec French speakers was
Rachael Lindberg and Pavel Trofimovich 837
the composite variable associated with four of instead related more to the type and quality of
the six rated dimensions, such that having more the experience, as suggested by Kang & Rubin
positive experiences was linked to greater speaker (2009). The current results reflected this idea,
comprehensibility, stronger preference to sound such that L2 learners who had experienced more
like the speaker, greater perceived teacher com- positive interactions with Quebec French speak-
petence, and stronger desire for learners to have ers and felt more welcome in Quebec were more
the speaker as their teacher (see Table 5). Multi- likely to wish to sound like the presumed Que-
ple regressions further clarified that learners’ per- bec French speaker, rate her as comprehensible,
ception of the presumed Quebec French speaker perceive her as a good teacher, and want her as
as being more difficult to understand and as a their French teacher. In fact, positive experience
less desirable speech model could be explained in Quebec emerged as one of two factors uniquely
through background variables, where lower oral predicting L2 learners’ desire to sound like the
proficiency (for comprehensibility) and fewer speaker in the Quebec French guise.
positive experiences in Quebec and greater so- Besides positive experience in Quebec, learn-
cial network density (for desire to sound like ers’ social network density involving Quebec
the speaker) predicted negative ratings. These French speakers was also predictive of their desire
relationships held even after learners’ sensitivity to sound like the speaker in the Quebec French
to the guise manipulation had been accounted guise, such that denser social networks were as-
for. sociated with lower ratings. L2 speakers with so-
Considering that “attitudes specifically associ- cial network density scores in the top quartile pro-
ated with the group or the language are quite vided a rating of 62 (on a 100-point scale) for
probably dormant until the student is confronted wanting to speak like the Quebec guise, while
with learning the language” (Gardner, 1985, those with density scores in the bottom quar-
p. 8), we expected that lower level learners (e.g., tile assigned a rating of 77. Because denser net-
presumably those who had started learning the works allow for greater exposure to and famil-
language recently) would display more neutral iarity with Quebec French, this linguistic expe-
attitudes toward the Quebec French guise com- rience may have enabled learners to develop a
pared to higher level learners (e.g., those with more accurate perception of how Quebec French
a lengthier experience with language learning). differs from European French (e.g., Baker &
This expectation was also consistent with Lam- Smith, 2010; Bergeron, 2019), resulting in learn-
bert’s (1967) hypothesis that L2 learners’ atti- ers’ preference to not consider Quebec French
tudes can be influenced by language learning, as a desirable speech model. Consistent with this
as they begin to identify with the language and idea, learners with network density greater than 0
the speech community during this process. How- rated their familiarity with Quebec French higher
ever, there were no major associations between (52) than learners with low network density (35).
learners’ proficiency measures and their ratings. It is also possible that Quebec French speak-
In fact, as shown in Table 5, listening compre- ers themselves—known to hold negative views of
hension and speaking scores were linked to one their own variety (e.g., Kircher, 2012)—also ex-
of the six measures—speaker comprehensibility posed learners within dense social networks to
(with small-to-medium associations). Learners in negative attitudes toward Quebec French (e.g.,
the top quartile of the proficiency range (B1 and Baker, 2008), leading learners to solidify their
above) rated the speaker’s comprehensibility at stereotypes.
74 (on a 100-point scale) while those in the bot- Taken together, these findings begin to sup-
tom quartile (A1) rated her comprehensibility port the idea that developing quality contacts
at 46. More advanced L2 proficiency likely en- within a language community can have a measur-
abled learners to assess speaker comprehensibil- able social impact, which can determine how atti-
ity more accurately, in a manner that was not tudes are adopted (Cortes–Colomé et al., 2016).
colored by attitudes. Alternatively, learners’ own In some cases, quality contacts can foster posi-
ease with language (presumed at higher L2 lev- tive feelings, leading to positive attitudinal con-
els) helped them to assess the speaker as more sequences. However, other quality contacts—such
comprehensible. as those involving dense social networks within
Because these results generally do not align the target language variety—may result in some
with previous findings indicating more pro- attitudes becoming potentially entrenched, for
nounced attitudes for learners who gain greater example, through more intensive exposure ex-
amounts of experience with and linguistic knowl- periences. These findings also crucially support
edge of the target language (Giles et al., 1974, the idea that it is often the quality, not the quan-
1979), perhaps the development of attitudes is tity, of linguistic experiences that matters for L2
838 The Modern Language Journal 104 (2020)
development (e.g., Moyer, 2011), including the munity, it could be that their integrative motiva-
development and maintenance of attitudes. tion toward learning French played a role in how
they viewed Quebec French speakers. This was ev-
LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE WORK ident in our participants’ desire to hear different
French varieties in their language input. Conduct-
Although these findings are suggestive of the ing similar research in a less linguistically diverse
complex interplay of learners’ language attitudes, city, or with learners who have no intention of im-
their L2 proficiency, and their social experiences migrating to the local community, may reveal a
with the target language variety, these findings different attitudinal profile.
must be revisited in future research. For exam- Similarly, our lack of strong attitudinal differ-
ple, researchers could examine in more detail ences in response to two French varieties may be
the root causes of any negative feelings that attributed to issues of methodology. Unlike prior
learners adopt toward Quebec French during research, where images have often been used to
their language instruction and even prior to their establish a speaker’s identity (e.g., Rubin, 1992),
exposure to it, to see if language teachers could our procedure involved only providing partic-
address these issues in class. Perhaps learners ipants with a verbal description of a speaker’s
encounter difficulties and frustrations when com- origin, because one cannot unambiguously differ-
municating with Quebec French speakers, and entiate a French speaker from France from a Que-
such occurrences of communication breakdowns bec French speaker based on physical features
could be lessened through more practice and in- alone. Thus, a verbal label of a speaker’s origin
teraction with the local variety in a safe classroom may not have been sufficiently powerful to acti-
environment. Therefore, one possible implica- vate any potential attitudinal biases. Last but not
tion for L2 French instructors could be to expose least, because we tested most of this study’s partic-
French learners to diverse pedagogical models, ipants during their regular classroom instruction,
which would also familiarize learners with the we could not include many experimental features
linguistic reality of la Francophonie and potentially typical of the standard matched-guise procedure
contribute to learners having balanced attitudes (e.g., presentation of multiple files with distractor
toward different French varieties, speakers, and audios). Therefore, our findings must be revisited
cultures. Alternatively, perhaps learners are sen- in research employing stricter experimental con-
sitive to Quebec speakers’ own negative attitudes trols among other more focused methodological
toward their home variety (e.g., Kircher, 2012; tools, such as including an obligatory comment
Lambert et al., 1960), which would call for a section and conducting stimulated recall inter-
change in attitudes in Quebec French speakers views to assess participants’ sensitivity to the guise
themselves, before they can project positive feel- manipulation and its impact on their ratings.
ings about their language to others. Similarly, this
study has begun to identify L2 learners’ existing CONCLUSION
attitudes toward French varieties and to uncover
their possible experiential origins. However, what In summary, our findings shed light on the
remains to be investigated is how these attitudes attitudes that L2 French learners in Montreal
can affect learners’ L2 motivation and their learn- hold toward Quebec and European French. L2
ing outcomes. This work would be important to learners generally held some negative attitudes
establishing the role, if any, that attitudes toward toward Quebec French. However, apart from
language varieties play in L2 development. rating the presumed Quebec French speaker
The results of this study are also inextricably as the less preferred model to follow, they did
linked to the research context. For instance, it not seem to attribute their negative attitudes in
is possible that our participants’ attitudinal re- rating native French speakers while completing
actions are less pronounced than those docu- a realistic listening-comprehension activity. The
mented in prior work (Hu & Su, 2015, Rubin, most important finding was that feeling welcome
1992) due to the specific characteristics of our in Quebec and having positive interactions with
participants and our research setting. Perhaps Quebec French speakers, along with higher L2
Montreal’s culturally diverse environment lends speaking scores and greater social engagement
itself to a more linguistically aware population of with Quebec French speakers within dense social
learners who may not hold as strong language bi- networks, was the strongest predictor of learners’
ases as those in a more monolingual setting. Ad- attitudes toward what was presumed to be Quebec
ditionally, because many of our participants were French speech, revealing a strong experiential
immigrants or international students who arrived component to language attitudes. We call for
in Montreal with intentions of settling in the com- more research, particularly in multilingual and
Rachael Lindberg and Pavel Trofimovich 839
multicultural contexts, targeting the role of social parer au TEF. Accessed 16 October 2020
and experiential variables in the development at https://www.lefrancaisdesaffaires.fr/tests-
and maintenance of L2 learners’ attitudes toward diplomes/se-preparer/tutoriels-tef
native varieties of the target language, to gain a Chiba, R., Matsuura, H., & Yamamoto, A. (1995).
Japanese attitudes toward English accents. World
better understanding of how language attitudes
Englishes, 14, 77–86.
shape L2 learning.
Choy, S., & Dodd, D. (1976). Standard-English speaking
and non-standard Hawaiian-English-speaking chil-
dren: Comprehension of both dialects and teach-
Open Research Badges ers’ evaluations. Journal of Educational Psychology,
68, 184–193.
Clark, L., & Schleef, E. (2010). The acquisition of so-
ciolinguistic evaluations from Polish-born adoles-
This article has earned an Open Materials badge. cents learning English: Evidence from perception.
Open Materials are available at https://www.iris- Language Awareness, 19, 299–322.
database.org. Cortes–Colomé, M., Barrieras, M., & Comellas, P.
(2016). Changes in immigrant individuals’ lan-
guage attitudes through contact with Catalan: The
mirror effect. Language Awareness, 25, 272–289.
REFERENCES Dalton–Puffer, C., Kaltenboeck, G., & Smit, U. (1997).
Learner attitudes and L2 pronunciation in Aus-
Ahn, S. Y., & Kang, H. S. (2017). South Korean university tria. World Englishes, 16, 115–128.
students’ perceptions of different English varieties D’Anglejan, A., & Tucker, R. (1973). Sociolinguistics
and their contribution to the learning of English correlates of speech style in Quebec. In R. Shuy &
as a foreign language. Journal of Multilingual and R. Fasold (Eds.), Language attitudes, current trends
Multicultural Development, 38, 712–725. and prospects (pp. 1–27). Washington, DC: George-
Baker, W. (2008). Social, experiential, and psychologi- town University Press.
cal factors affecting L2 dialect acquisition. In M. Davila, A., Bohara, A., & Saenz, R. (1993). Accent penal-
Bowles, R. Foote, S. Perpiñán, & R. Bhatt (Eds.), ties and the earnings of Mexican Americans. Social
Selected Proceedings of the 2007 Second Language Re- Science Quarterly, 74, 902–916.
search Forum (pp. 187–198). Somerville, MA: Cas- Derwing, T. (2003). What do ESL students say about
cadilla Proceedings Project. their accents? The Canadian Modern Language Re-
Baker, W., & Smith, L. C. (2010). The impact of L2 view, 59, 547–567.
dialect on learning French vowels: Native En- Doucerain, M., Varnaamkhaasti, R., Segalowitz, N., & Ry-
glish speakers learning Québécois and European der, A. (2015). Second language social networks
French. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 66, and communication-related acculturative stress:
711–738. The role of interconnectedness. Frontiers in Psychol-
Bergeron, A. (2019). L2 French learners’ sociolinguis- ogy, 6, 1–12.
tic awareness of Quebec French (Unpublished doc- Dragojevic, M., & Giles, H. (2016). I don’t like you
toral dissertation). Montreal, Canada: Concordia because you’re hard to understand: The role
University. of processing fluency in the language attitudes
Bestelmeyer, P. E. G., Belin, P., & Ladd, D. R. (2015). process. Human Communication Research, 42, 396–
A neural marker for social bias toward in- group 420.
accents. Cerebral Cortex, 25, 3953–3961. Edwards, J. (1999). Refining our understanding of lan-
Boulé, J. J. (2002). Attitudes of young Quebecers towards En- guage attitudes. Journal of Language and Social Psy-
glish and French (Unpublished MA thesis). Mon- chology, 18, 101–110.
treal, Canada: Concordia University. Evans, B. E. (2002). Attitudes of Montreal students to-
Bourhis, R. Y. (1997). Language policies and language wards varieties of French. In D. Long & D. R.
attitudes: Le monde de la francophonie. In N. Preston (Eds.), Handbook of perceptual dialectology
Coupland & A. Jaworski (Eds.), Sociolinguistics: A (pp. 71–93). Philadelphia/Amsterdam: John Ben-
reader and coursebook (pp. 306–322). New York: St. jamins.
Martin’s Press. Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS (3th ed.).
Brown, B., Giles, H., & Thakerar, J. (1985). Speaker eval- Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
uations as a function of speech rate, accent and Gardner, R. (1985). Social psychology and second language
context. Language and Communication, 5, 207–220. learning. London: Edward Arnold.
Carlson, H. K., & McHenry, M. A. (2006). Effect of ac- Giles, H., Bourhis, R., & Davies, A. (1979). Prestige
cent and dialect on employability. Journal of Em- speech styles: The imposed norms and inherent
ployment Counseling, 43, 70–83. value hypotheses. In W. McCormack & S. Wurm
Chambre de commerce et d’industrie Paris Ile- (Eds.), Language and society: Anthropological issues
de-France. (2018). Les tutoriels pour se pré- (pp. 589–596). New York: Mouton.
840 The Modern Language Journal 104 (2020)
Giles, H., Bourhis, R., Lewis, A., & Trudgill, P. (1974). national teaching assistants. TESOL Quarterly, 49,
The imposed norm hypothesis: A validation. Quar- 681–706.
terly Journal of Speech, 60, 405–410. Kang, O., Thomson, R., & Moran, M. (2019). The ef-
Giles, H., Wilson, P., & Conway, A. (1981). Accent and fects of international accents and shared first lan-
lexical diversity as determinants of impression for- guage on listening comprehension tests. TESOL
mation and employment selection. Language Sci- Quarterly, 53, 56–81.
ences, 3, 92–103. Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E. S. (2007). The
Gill, M. (1994). Accent and stereotypes: Their effect on native language of social cognition. PNAS: Proceed-
perceptions of teachers and lecture comprehen- ings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
sion. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 22, States of America, 104, 12577–12580.
348–361. Kircher, R. (2012). How pluricentric is the French
Gluszek, A., & Dovidio, J. (2010). Speaking with a non- language? An investigation of attitudes towards
native accent: Perceptions of bias, communication Quebec French compared to European French.
difficulties, and belonging in the United States. Journal of French Language Studies, 22, 345–
Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 29, 224– 370.
234. Kircher, R. (2014). Thirty years after Bill 101: A contem-
Hay, J., Warren, P., & Drager, K. (2006). Factors in- porary perspective on attitudes towards English
fluencing speech perception in the context of a and French in Montreal. The Canadian Journal of
merger-in-progress. Journal of Phonetics, 34, 458– Applied Linguistics, 17, 20–50.
484. Lambert, W. E. (1967). A social psychology of bilingual-
Hopper, R., & Williams, F. (1973). Speech characteris- ism. Journal of Social Issues, 23, 91–109.
tics and employability. Speech Monographs, 46, 296– Lambert, W. E., Hodgson, R. C., Gardner, R. C., & Fillen-
302. baum, S. (1960). Evaluational reactions to spoken
Hu, G., & Lindemann, S. (2009). Stereotypes of Can- languages. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy-
tonese English, apparent native/non-native status, chology, 60, 44–51.
and their effect on non-native English speakers’ Laur, E. (2008). Contribution à l’étude des perceptions
perception. Journal of Multilingual and Multicul- linguistiques. La méthodologie des faux-couples
tural Development, 30, 253–269. revisitée. [Contribution to the study of lin-
Hu, G., & Su, J. (2015). The effect of native/non-native guistic perceptions. Methodology of the
information on non-native listeners’ comprehen- matched guise technique revisited]. Gou-
sion. Language Awareness, 24, 273–281. vernement du Québec. Accessed 16 October
Hume, E., Lepicq, D., & Bourhis, R. (1993). Attitudes 2020 at https://www.oqlf.gouv.qc.ca/ressources/
des étudiants Canadiens anglais face aux accents sociolinguistique/2008/notemetholaur.pdf
des professeurs de français en Ontario. [English Lev–Ari, S., & Keysar, B. (2010). Why don’t we believe
Canadian students’ attitudes towards the accents non-native speakers? The influence of accent on
of French professors in Ontario]. The Canadian credibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
Modern Language Review, 49, 209–235. 46, 1093–1096.
Hutcheson, G. D., & Sofroniou, N. (1999). The multivari- Lindemann, S. (2002). Listening with an attitude: A
ate social scientist: Introductory statistics using general- model of native-speaker comprehension of non-
ized linear models. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE. native speakers in the United States. Language in
Hyman, H. K. (2002). Foreign accented adult ESL learn- Society, 31, 419–441.
ers: Perceptions of their accent changes and employabil- McGowan, K. B. (2015). Social expectation improves
ity qualifications (Doctoral dissertation). Accessed speech perception in noise. Language and Speech,
5 October 2020 at ProQuest Dissertations & The- 58, 502–521.
ses (Accession number 200210856). McKenzie, R. (2008). Social factors and non-native at-
Jarvella, R. J., Bang, E., Jakobsen, A. L., & Mees, I. M. titudes towards varieties of spoken English: A
(2001). Of mouths and men: Nonnative listeners’ Japanese case study. International Journal of Applied
identification and evaluation of varieties of En- Linguistics, 18, 63–88.
glish. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 11, Moyer, A. (2011). An investigation of experience in L2
37–56. phonology: Does quality matter more than quan-
Kang, O., & Rubin, D. L. (2009). Reverse linguistic tity? The Canadian Modern Language Review, 67,
stereotyping: Measuring the effect of listener ex- 191–216.
pectations on speech evaluation. Journal of Lan- Munro, M. J. (2003). A primer on accent discrimination
guage and Social Psychology, 28, 441–456. in the Canadian context. TESL Canada Journal, 20,
Kang, O., & Rubin, D. L. (2012). Intergroup contact 38–51.
exercises as a tool for mitigating undergradu- Munro, M. J., Derwing, T. M., & Burgess, C. S.
ates’ attitudes toward nonnative English-speaking (2010). Detection of nonnative speaker status
teaching assistants. Journal on Excellence in College from content-masked speech. Speech Communica-
Teaching, 23, 159–166. tion, 52, 626–637.
Kang, O., Rubin, D., & Lindemann, S. (2015). Mitigat- Munro, M. J., Derwing, T. M., & Sato, K. (2006). Salient
ing U.S. undergraduates’ attitudes toward inter- accents, covert attitudes: Consciousness-raising for
Rachael Lindberg and Pavel Trofimovich 841
pre-service second language teachers. Prospect, 21, at https://www.statcan.gc.ca/eng/dai/smr08/
67–79. 2014/smr08_190_2014#a4
Niedzielski, N. (1999). The effect of social information Strand, E. (1999). Uncovering the role of gender stereo-
on the perception of sociolinguistic variables. Jour- types in speech perception. Journal of Language and
nal of Language and Social Psychology, 18, 62–85. Social Psychology, 18, 86–100.
Piechowiak, A. (2009). What is “good” quality oral Weener, P. D. (1967). The influence of dialect differences on
French? Language attitudes towards “differently” ac- the immediate recall of verbal messages (Unpublished
cented French in Quebec (Unpublished doctoral dis- doctoral dissertation). Ann Arbor, MI: University
sertation). Montreal, Canada: McGill University. of Michigan. Accessed 5 October 2020 at the Edu-
Plonsky, L., & Oswald, F. L. (2014). How big is “big”? cation Resources Information Center (ERIC Num-
Interpreting effect sizes in L2 research. Language ber ED017901).
Learning, 64, 878–912. Williams, F. (1976). Explorations of the linguistic attitudes of
Rubin, D. L. (1992). Nonlanguage factors affecting un- teachers. New York: Newbury House.
dergraduates’ judgments of nonnative English- Zhang, Q. (2013). The attitudes of Hong Kong stu-
speaking teaching assistants. Research in Higher Ed- dents towards Hong Kong English and Mandarin-
ucation, 33, 511–531. accented English. English Today, 29, 9–16.
Ryan, E. B., & Sebastian, R. J. (1980). The effects of Zhang, W., & Hu, G. (2008). Second language learn-
speech style and social class background on social ers’ attitudes towards English varieties. Language
judgments of speakers. British Journal of Social and Awareness, 17, 342–347.
Clinical Psychology, 19, 229–233.
Seligman, C., Tucker, G. R., & Lambert, W. E. (1972).
The effects of speech style and other attributes on SUPPORTING INFORMATION
teachers’ attitudes towards pupils. Language in So-
ciety, 1, 131–142. Additional supporting information may be found
Statistics Canada. (2017, September 15). Back to online in the Supporting Information section at
school… by the numbers. Accessed 2 November 2018 the end of the article.

You might also like