You are on page 1of 13

The Journal of Positive Psychology

Dedicated to furthering research and promoting good practice

ISSN: 1743-9760 (Print) 1743-9779 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rpos20

Bringing coherence to positive psychology: Faith in


humanity

Roger G. Tweed, Eric Y. Mah & Lucian Gideon Conway III

To cite this article: Roger G. Tweed, Eric Y. Mah & Lucian Gideon Conway III (2020): Bringing
coherence to positive psychology: Faith in humanity, The Journal of Positive Psychology, DOI:
10.1080/17439760.2020.1725605

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2020.1725605

Published online: 11 Feb 2020.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 13

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rpos20
THE JOURNAL OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY
https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2020.1725605

Bringing coherence to positive psychology: Faith in humanity


Roger G. Tweeda,b, Eric Y. Mahc and Lucian Gideon Conway IIId
a
Department of Psychology, Kwantlen Polytechnic University, Surrey, Canada; bDepartment of Psychology, Douglas College, New Westminster,
Canada; cDepartment of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, Canada; dDepartment of Psychology, University of Montana, Missoula,
MT, USA

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


Currently, positive psychology is experiencing problems with coherence, and the field could Received 6 May 2019
benefit from more organizing concepts linking disparate findings and researchers within the Accepted 24 January 2020
field. This incoherence can be seen in several domains. At a conceptual level, the field has KEYWORDS
produced an abundance of important studies clarifying predictors of well-being, but no consistent Positive psychology; faith in
theory has emerged explaining why these factors predict well-being. In addition, disunity has humanity; FIH; future;
emerged between first wave positive psychologists and second wave positive psychologists, and directions; definition
also between practitioners and researchers. The field could benefit from more unifying constructs
that explain links between constructs and practices within positive psychology. Faith in humanity
(FIH) has potential as a unifying construct. FIH is like a forgotten sibling whose important story is
mentioned rarely and mainly obliquely. In fact, this construct, though seldom mentioned, already
implicitly pervades much of positive psychology, and the field would benefit by explicitly recog-
nizing this fact.

It is highly unlikely that a single construct can account psychology (e.g., Ivtzan, Lomas, Hefferon, & Worth, 2016;
for every phenomenon in a given field. However, there is Wong, 2011). The second wave positive psychologists
also value in bringing together discrepant parts of a field argue that happiness is not the ideal focus, and that
under a larger theoretical umbrella. Currently, positive positive psychologists need to address alternative man-
psychology is experiencing problems with coherence, ifestations of well-being and address individuals in nega-
and as a result, the field could benefit from more orga- tive circumstances. Third, the current distinction
nizing concepts linking disparate findings and research- between positive psychology researchers and practi-
ers within the field. tioners is significant. In the past, a similar distinction
This incoherence can be seen at several levels. First, at between clinical psychologists and research psycholo-
a conceptual level, the field has produced many studies gists contributed to the split between the APA and the
clarifying predictors of well-being (for a review of some, dissident group that formed the APS. These divides
see Veenhoven, 2015), but no consistent theory has within positive psychology could possibly be reduced
emerged explaining why these particular factors predict by further explication of what links these groups.
well-being. The underlying principle creating potency Academic fields that lack coherence can enter disar-
for these factors remains a mystery. The current situation ray, and thus it is important to seek ideas explaining
was foreshadowed by the original formulation of the coherence. Consider the field of trait personality psy-
field. Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi’s (2000) original chology which re-emerged stronger after clarification
influential American Psychologist piece did not define of previously unrecognized themes pervading the disci-
the field with unifying theories generating hypotheses pline. In the 1970s and 80s, the field had many compet-
and explaining prior findings, but instead defined the ing systems of personality such as that of the DSM-III
field by its topic: The study of positive states, traits, and (American Psychiatric Association, 1980), the 20 factors
institutions. Consistent with this focus on topic rather of the CPI (Gough, 1987), the 16 factors of Cattell (Cattell,
than theory, the field has produced lists of predictors of Eber, & Tatsuoka, 1970), and many others, but the links
well-being and lists of positive character strengths, but between these were poorly understood. The five-factor
no central coherent theory making sense of these or model of personality (Digman, 1990) helped redirect the
generating more hypotheses. Second, incoherence has field by providing central organizing principles linking all
emerged between the first and second wave of positive these systems and by replicating in many different

CONTACT Roger G. Tweed roger.tweed@kpu.ca


© 2020 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
2 R. G. TWEED ET AL.

populations and cultures (McCrae, 2002). This framework a good life. Belief that all humanity includes at least some
returned more respectability to the field and generated goodness and something good worth keeping is the creed
new directions for research. Social psychology likewise of FIH. The definition used here will extend that defini-
has been characterized by a wide array of often discon- tion beyond initial contact with others and adds some
nected theories and hypotheses. The field has been details that will matter from a psychological perspective.
revitalized in some ways by the evolutionary perspec- We take FIH to be a (1) readiness to perceive positive
tive, which explains some previously unrelated prior (RtPP) traits, intention, potential, and impact in (a) people
findings and generates many new theoretically driven in general as well as in subgroups and individuals, stran-
hypotheses based on a few simple concepts. gers as well as familiar people (b) in the past, present, and
In the present paper, we argue that faith in humanity future, (c) while remaining aware that the other person
(FIH) similarly links disparate findings and activities may be an outgroup member, may display negative beha-
within positive psychology. In fact, this construct, vior, and may hold opinions unlike those of the self, and (2)
though seldom mentioned, already implicitly pervades a readiness to enact this positive perception (RtEP) by
much of positive psychology, and the field would benefit behaving as if other people have value (i.e., actualizing
by explicitly recognizing this fact. Within positive psy- a better world for and within other people).
chology, FIH might be seen as a forgotten sibling whose In its most compelling forms, FIH manifests in indivi-
story is central to the family, but who is mentioned duals such as Gandhi, who even in the face of violence,
rarely, and then mainly obscurely and obliquely. This bigotry, institutional intransigence, and even threatened
argument need not rely on FIH being the most central assassination, refused to veer from his steadfast belief
construct within positive psychology. A neglected sib- that many of his oppressors and opponents had positive
ling would not be the whole story of a family, but it potential to realize their error and become allies in the
would deserve attention. Likewise, FIH may not be the cause of justice (Preston-Roedder, 2013). Similarly, one
most central theme in positive psychology, but if might think of Martin Luther King, Jr. (Preston-Roedder,
neglected yet pervasive, it may deserve much more 2013), who responded to murderous church bombings
attention. For these reasons, FIH should be particularly with the assertion that even violent segregationists had
attractive to positive psychologists. within them the potential to realize the worth of all
Before describing the justification for these claims, human beings. He did NOT describe segregationists as
however, we will provide a clarification of the concept. ‘good people’ (as someone who believed in basic human
Because any attempt to unify disparate domains is goodness might do), but implored his followers to
bound to be fraught with difficulties, we will address remember that even segregationists may have redeem-
potential concerns as well. ing qualities; he reminded them that some were ‘kind to
neighbors and family, helpful, and good spirited at work’
and King said ‘. . . we must try not to end up with stereo-
The concept: faith in humanity (FIH) types of those we oppose even as they slip all of us into
their stereotypes’ (Coles, 1994, p. 32). Even while being
You must not lose faith in humanity. Humanity is an fully aware of many segregationists’ evils, King still
ocean; if a few drops of the ocean are dirty, the ocean recognized the possibility of redeeming traits. Gandhi
does not become dirty.
and King’s readiness to perceive positives even in those
-Mahatma Gandhi they knew to have engaged in profoundly evil acts
illustrates FIH and provides a starting point for this dis-
We draw here on philosopher Preston-Roedder’s
cussion of positive psychology and FIH. Consistent with
(2013) explication of FIH. Preston-Roedder asserts that
the RtEP aspect of FIH, both Ghandi and King also went
FIH involves a tendency to initially judge people as
beyond perception and acted as if other people have
trustworthy and moral, a tendency to not hastily judge
value. They took steps to better the world for and within
people harshly, and a sensitivity to and focus on ‘the
other people.
good in people’ (2013). Importantly FIH, in Preston
Roedder’s definition, is not blind. The faithful do not
ignore moral failings. People who have FIH, according
Not faith that people are good or can be trusted
to this definition, take steps to protect themselves and
others from the poor behavior by others. Preston- FIH is NOT faith that humans will always be good (King
Roedder also argues that a person expressing FIH also did not describe these people as good), but instead is
will be invested in other people (i.e., act as if they have a recognition that humans will demonstrate humanity.
value), in enabling them to achieve their potential to live They will tend to demonstrate positive traits or
THE JOURNAL OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 3

intentions or potential or impact even amidst the nega- may take us some way toward this type of simplification
tive elements they display. FIH need not denote blind- of some psychological factors related to positive subjec-
ness to the evil and potential harm brought by others. tive states. In particular, some of the most consistent
Blindness to these aspects would be faith in positivity or psychological predictors of personal well-being share
faith in optimism or pure good will or something other little in common other than an underlying association
than a faith in a realistic understanding of humanity. with FIH: Gratitude toward other people, perceived
Ghandi and King, as victims of hatred, were aware of social support, generalized trust, attachment, and
the potential for evil within humanity and would have generosity.
been aware that some people seem more evil than
others. Gratitude
Gratitude has been studied extensively in positive psy-
chology, and findings suggest gratitude can cause well-
A forgotten theme of positive psychology being (Wood et al., 2016). Gratitude as a construct over-
One could reasonably ask why FIH deserves more atten- laps significantly with the RtPP element of FIH. Gratitude
tion within positive psychology, and part of the answer involves recognizing a benefit that has been provided to
can be found by considering the domains within the the self by an entity outside the self. Gratitude is, in
field. According to an early definitional statement by essence, a positive interpretation of others’ impact,
Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi (2000), the domains of sometimes in spite of mixed evidence. In other words,
positive psychology include the study of (a) ‘positive it is a faith that others have done good things for us. FIH
subjective experience’ [e.g. life satisfaction, happiness], is a broader construct than gratitude in part because FIH
(b) ‘positive individual traits,’ [e.g. character strengths can be forward, present, or backward looking while
and virtues] and (c) ’positive institutions’ (2000, p. 5). gratitude is focused only on the past. Though gratitude
Each of those will be discussed in turn, with more atten- is backward looking, the next construct to be discussed
tion on the first, consistent with the vast amount of focuses on the present and future.
research attention it has received in positive psychology
compared to the others. Second wave positive psychol- Perceived social support
ogy (e.g., Ivtzan et al., 2016, also called Positive Social support has received so much research attention
Psychology 2.0; Wong, 2011) will also be briefly dis- that it has become a truism in psychology that social
cussed as will the behavior of positive psychologists in support promotes well-being, but one possibly puzzling
relation to FIH. We will argue that FIH is an important finding is the repeated result that perceived social sup-
theme within each of these domains, and thereby illus- port has a stronger relation to well-being than does
trates a seldom mentioned, but common link among actual received levels of social support (e.g., McDowell
many positive psychologists and their ideas, one that & Serovich, 2007). This suggests that interpreting people
may have power to show links between findings, as currently providing and being ready in the future to
between researchers, between practitioners, and also provide social support is more important than the extent
may have power to generate new hypotheses. to which those people actually provide support. The
finding becomes less puzzling when one considers per-
ceived social support to be a form of FIH. Unlike grati-
FIH in the original domains of positive tude, which is past focused, perceived social support is
psychology focused more on the positive interpretation of the provi-
sions of other people in providing support to the self
Domain one: predictors of positive subjective states
now and in the future. Once again, a predictor of well-
The first domain, the study of positive states, is in some being is actually a form of FIH. The next construct to be
ways the signature piece of positive psychology in the discussed is not a form of FIH, but attachment theory
sense that it attracts immense interest from academics suggests it is enabled by FIH.
and also journalists and lay people. Numerous studies
within this domain have clarified predictors of positive Healthy social attachments
subjective states, yet no single theory has emerged to Ongoing healthy social attachments also tend to pro-
explain the common elements that underlie those pre- mote positive subjective states, and, consideration of
dictors. Einstein’s relativity theory illustrates the value of adult attachment theory suggests an important role for
simplifying and unifying concepts; his theory clarified FIH in the facilitation of healthy attachments. In particu-
the link between mass and energy, and also the link lar, the adult attachment theory of Bartholomew and
between space and time. At a conceptual level, FIH Horowitz (1991) relies on variance in one’s internalized
4 R. G. TWEED ET AL.

perception of self and others. People who perceive the (Peterson & Seligman, 2004), which was designed to clas-
self negatively and others negatively are fearful in rela- sify virtues recognized across cultures. Here we describe
tionships. People who perceive the self positively but some of the central traits and their relevance to FIH.
others negatively are dismissive. People who perceive
the self negatively and others positively are preoccu- Humility
pied. In contrast, secure attachment is built on positive Humility is important enough to get a 2017 issue of the
cognitive models of self and others that enable the self Journal of Positive Psychology devoted to one form of
to be comfortable with both intimacy and autonomy. humility. Tangney’s (2000) influential discussion of humi-
Admittedly, attachment theory’s model of others is spe- lity suggests that philosophical and psychological discus-
cific to the domain of interpersonal relationships (and sions do not tend to portray humility as low self-regard,
often more specifically romantic relationships), so it is but instead focus on aspects such as low self-focus and
narrower than FIH. Nonetheless, according to this adult willingness to recognize, appreciate, and accept contribu-
attachment theory, a positive model of others in perso- tions from others. This willingness to attend to others and
nal relationships enables the healthy attachments that recognize the value in others’ contributions would be
are associated with positive subjective states. Once difficult if not impossible for people who lacked the FIH
again, one of the disparate set of predictors of well- readiness to perceive positive in others.
being is strongly connected to and may even require
FIH. This theoretical orientation also concurs with work Kindness
by Canevello and Crocker (2010) suggesting that readi- The VIA (Peterson & Seligman, 2004) also includes kind-
ness to perceive positive in one’s partner may create ness. The discussion of generosity above already clarifies
improved interactions that self-perpetuate and contri- the role of FIH in facilitating generosity. Kindness like-
bute to ongoing relational strength (Canevello & wise will be easier if one perceives others as deserving
Crocker, 2010). and if one perceives others as having value. In contrast,
the opposite orientation, which would involve dehuma-
Generosity nization, clears the psychological path for harsh treat-
One notable finding of positive psychology is the ment and even violence toward others (Haslam &
value of generosity in promoting subjective well- Loughnan, 2016).
being, an effect that generalizes well across cultures
and contexts (Aknin, Broesch, Hamlin, & Van De Other strengths
Vondervoort, 2015). Generosity will be much easier if The VIA structure (Peterson & Seligman, 2004) also
one has a high view of others (i.e., FIH). One could includes other dimensions related to FIH such as open-
conceivably engage in prosocial behaviors for the self- mindedness (also called judgment), which involves
ish purpose of gaining positive affective states, but a willingness to see value in others’ perspectives (similar
that type of selfish effort probably undermines promo- to the philosophical virtue of charity: being inclined
tion of positive affective states (Martin, 2008). toward positive interpretations of others’ ideas).
The second dimension of FIH, the idea that other Likewise, Peterson and Seligman’s discussion of appre-
people have value (Preston-Roedder, 2013) provides ciation of beauty and excellence involves a strong social
further justification for generosity. This focus echoes element. According to their description, people with this
positive psychology pioneer Chris Peterson’s (Park & strength appreciate beauty and excellence not only at
Seligman, 2013, 2008) contention that a core idea in a material level but also in the lives and acts of other
positive psychology is the belief that other people people. Patience, a traditional virtue, does not absolutely
matter (OPM; see also Hwang on “other-esteem,” require FIH, but it is plausible that patience will be much
1995). easier if one believes that in spite of momentary slow
progress, positive potential and value exist in those
testing your patience. Peterson and Seligman’s VIA fra-
Domain two: positive traits
mework of strengths (2004) also lists leadership as
The study of positive traits – often referred to as character a character strength; leadership efforts would lack justi-
strengths or virtues – is quite distinct from and has fication unless one presumes that people have the
received less attention than have positive states, yet potential and proclivity to rise up in response to leader-
once again, it seems that FIH underlies many of the con- ship and move in a positive direction. Similarly, team-
structs central to this domain of positive psychology. work is considered a character strength, but teamwork
Within positive psychology, character strengths are most efforts would not be justified unless one has a tendency
often organized according to the VIA Strengths framework to perceive positive potential in one’s teammates.
THE JOURNAL OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 5

FIH itself (Dodge, Price, Bachorowski, & Newman, 1990). That find-
At a broad level, Preston-Roedder (2013) argued that ing is consistent with other research indicating that an
people whom societies recognize as exemplars of virtue absence of generalized trust (an element of FIH) may be
tend to manifest a strong sense of FIH (e.g., Ghandi, associated with a more violent society (Elgar & Aitken,
Martin Luther King). Because FIH is so prevalent among 2010). Similarly, Beck (2019), the founder of cognitive
people manifesting virtue, and for other reasons, therapy, argued that dyadic anger and aggression are
Preston-Roedder argued that FIH itself deserves to be associated with cognitive biases leading each party to
designated as a separate virtue. notice mainly negative behavior, overinterpret minor
Not all positive traits are manifestations of FIH (e.g., negative behavior, perceive neutral behavior negatively,
self-control, zest, humor), but nonetheless, the theme of and draw globalized negative judgments of a complete
FIH underlies a number of the central constructs. Thus, person. He argued that these same processes are also
both the domains of positive states and positive traits a source of group conflict and even war and genocide.
have a recurring theme of FIH. For these types of issues, allophilia, positive attitude
towards members of an outgroup (Pittinsky, Rosenthal,
& Montoya, 2011) may be particularly relevant. In an
Domain three: positive institutions
interesting application, Staub (2019) has worked to
Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi’s (2000) definition of reduce societal discord by means of entertainment pro-
positive psychology also included the study of institu- grams promoting more realistic perceptions of ‘enemy’
tions facilitating positive states and positive traits. groups. The realization that people tend to harshly mis-
Institutions receive less attention in positive psychology characterize their enemies is not new (e.g., Orwell, 1946/
research journals than do positive states and traits. 2001), so it is not a feature unique to modern society. FIH
Nonetheless, some research (Gheilen, van Woerkom, & may provide a helpful corrective while possibly avoiding
Meyers, 2017) indicates that organizations implement- the opposite problem of unbelievable simplistic positive
ing strengths interventions, which would indicate lea- portrayals of others.
dership readiness to perceive positive potential in their
workers, succeed in promoting well-being and work
FIH in second wave positive psychology
engagement among their employees. Also, even the
desire to create a positive institution (i.e., one that pro- These previous paragraphs have illustrated that though
motes positive states and positive traits) relies on per- FIH is seldom mentioned in positive psychology, it
ceptions of positive potential in the people that will be emerges as a strong theme within all three of the
influenced. For example, government leaders who seek major domains of positive psychology. There is, how-
to promote psychological well-being through purpose- ever, a growing area within positive psychology that
ful policy choices (Tweed, Mah, Dobrin, Van Poele, & has been referred to as positive psychology 2.0 (Wong,
Conway, 2017), must presume that people can be influ- 2011) or second wave positive psychology (Ivtzan et al.,
enced in positive ways, that is, believe that positive 2016). Advocates of this second wave have complained
change is possible. that positive psychology is so focused on happiness and
FIH may deserve a large role in the discussions of positive emotion that it can be irrelevant to people who
positive psychology’s application to public policy and live amidst trauma and the inevitable accompanying
other large societal issues (Tweed et al., 2017). For exam- frequent negative emotions (e.g., Wong, 2011). These
ple, societies with more equal incomes tend to have scholars argue for explicitly and consistently including
lower rates of homicide, and one element of FIH (gen- consideration of negative experiences and negative
eralized trust) may serve as a mediator facilitating this emotions within the purview of positive psychology
effect (Elgar & Aitken, 2010). Similarly, historical data research and practice.
suggest that periods with more equal incomes coincide One could reasonably ask why this second wave
with higher reports of happiness, and again an element deserves the title of ‘positive psychology’ if it consis-
of FIH (generalized trust) may mediate this effect as well tently includes discussion of the negative. Again, FIH
(Oishi, Kesebir, & Diener, 2011). Helliwell (2003) argued emerges as a central, if not defining element
for the same mediator between social capital and for second wave positive psychology. If we ask how
happiness. positive psychology 2.0 approaches the study of nega-
FIH may also have relevance to societal violence and tive emotion and trauma, the answer is that this
discord. At the individual level, an opposite of FIH which approach emphasizes that even amidst trauma and
has been called ‘hostile attribution bias,’ has long been negative emotions, humans can grow, become stronger,
known to be associated with interpersonal violence show resilience, and find meaning (Ivtzan et al., 2016).
6 R. G. TWEED ET AL.

Admittedly, this perspective on difficult situations is not exemplars of FIH may serve to increase FIH. Admittedly,
completely new (e.g., Rutter, 1987), so our intent is not low FIH may be difficult to change because a low view of
to defend second wave positive psychology as distinct particular groups may become central to one’s meaning
from other domains, but instead to show that even this system, as suggested by both recent research (Rovenpor,
offshoot of positive psychology has FIH as a core ele- O’Brien, Roblain, Guissme, & Chekroun, 2019) and classic
ment. Similarly, some resilience research suggests that theory (Kelly, 1955), and, at times, low FIH may seem
FIH may have value for people amidst distress (e.g., sensible and self-protective for relations with particular
Tingey, McGuire, Stebbins, & Erickson, 2017). We also individuals. However, the testing of potential FIH inter-
recommend reading work by Yeager (2017) for discus- ventions may produce a fruitful research program. Also,
sion of the benefits of an FIH-related construct amidst research could examine the extent to which broader soci-
social difficulty and even bullying. etal change enables or inhibits FIH.

FIH in positive psychologists Measurement of FIH

At a much more practical level, interacting with positive If FIH inhabits many constructs in positive psychology, there
psychologists provides strong evidence that many posi- could be value in developing a reliable and valid measure of
tive psychology researchers and practitioners are driven FIH. The process, however, may be difficult as discussed in
by a strong FIH regarding the potential for positive concern #3 below.
change in others. FIH becomes very obvious at positive
psychology conferences when one realizes that every Possible concerns regarding the usefulness of
session on making schools more positive, or making FIH
organizations more positive, or making people happier,
or treating pathology with positive psychology interven- Thus far, we have argued that FIH has the potential to
tions presumes that the targets of the intervention have enhance coherence in the field of positive psychology by
potential for positive change. We know of no direct highlighting an implicit theme that underlies the work of
research on the nature of positive psychologists, but many positive psychologists. In sum, what has previously
our informal encounters make it clear that positive psy- been a disparate set of predictors of well-being,
chologists tend to embody a strong FIH. One notable strengths of character, traits of positive organizations,
example of FIH involves positive psychologists interven- and a new movement within positive psychology starts
ing to build positive, rich, and good lives for prisoners to seem more unified when one considers that a number
(Huyn, Hall, Hurst, & Bikos, 2015). This orientation would of these predictors, strengths, traits, and the movement
not be possible if these positive psychologists were fall squarely under the FIH conceptual umbrella. Each
unable to see positive potential even within people domain of positive psychology seems to have FIH as
that others might see as unworthy of societal care (see a central theme. However, this argument is not without
also Fortune, Ward, & Mann, 2015 for work with sex potential objections that it would be remiss of us to
offenders). Thus, FIH can be central to not only the ignore. Below, we deal with several of these.
study but also the practice of positive psychologists.
Potential concern 1: is FIH the same as generalized
trust?
Research directions
One possible reaction to what we have said so far is that
Development of FIH interventions
we are simply re-naming existing constructs. One possi-
Perhaps interventions to build FIH could even have value. bility involves generalized trust. Generalized trust is
Prior research in positive psychology has shown that a belief that people will tend to be helpful, fair, and
observing exemplars of virtue and excellence can inspire trustworthy. Generalized trust has shown stronger rela-
individuals to pursue virtue and excellence themselves tions at the country level with well-being (β= .62) than
(Algoe & Haidt, 2009). Witnessing ‘second-hand’ excel- has GDP (β= .36, Helliwell, Layard, & Sachs, 2012).
lence can lead to increased motivation, elevation, grati- FIH (which involves RtPP and other valuing) overlaps
tude, admiration, generosity, and courtesy among others, to some degree with prior conceptualizations of general-
and exemplars can be celebrities, workplace leaders, and ized trust. In fact, generalized trust has origins in sociolo-
even fictional characters (Algoe & Haidt, 2009; Bandura, gist Rosenberg’s (1956) ‘faith in people’ questionnaire;
2019; Galliani & Vianello, 2012; Niemiec, 2012; Staub, a modified form of Rosenberg’s (1956) ‘faith in people
2019). Thus, exposing individuals to past or current scale’ was reworked into the measure of trust for the 1972
THE JOURNAL OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 7

General Social Survey (Miller & Mitamura, 2003). This Potential concern 2: are other constructs more
measure consists of three questions: ‘Generally speaking, central in positive psychology?
would you say that most people can be trusted or that you
Critics might also suggest that other, more prominent
can’t be too careful in dealing with people?’, ‘Would you say
constructs may better enhance coherence in positive
that most of the time people try to be helpful, or that they
psychology. Optimism, for example, has been consid-
are mostly just looking out for themselves?’, and ‘Do you
ered to be the cornerstone of positive thinking (Scheier
think most people would try to take advantage of you if
& Carver, 1993) and is implicated in many concepts
they got a chance or would they try to be fair?’ (Smith,
central to positive psychology: happiness, perseverance,
Hout, & Marsden, 2013). The notion is called ‘generalized’
achievement, health, resilience, coping, and hope
trust because the questions do not ask about any specific
among others (Carver, Scheier, & Segerstrom, 2010;
person, or time, or place. This construct of generalized
Peterson, 2000). In fact, one might argue that FIH as
trust has thus been defined as a ‘generalized expectancy
defined in this paper is simply a form of optimism,
held by an individual that the word, promise, oral or
namely optimism about humans.
written statement of another individual or group can be
Optimism, however, leaves out much of positive psy-
relied upon’ (Rotter, 1980, p. 1) or similarly a belief that
chology. Definitions of optimism are predominantly self-
others will act in our interests (Newton, 2001).
focused and future-focused; optimism is ‘the extent to
Though generalized trust clearly overlaps with FIH,
which people hold generalized favorable expectancies
there are several reasons why conceptualizations and
for their future’ (Carver et al., 2010, p. 1); optimism is ‘a
measurements of generalized trust do not fully capture
mood or attitude associated with an expectation . . .
FIH. These trust items make no mention of a broader
which the evaluator regards as socially desirable, to his
perception that people have positive potential or other
[or her] advantage, or for his [or her] pleasure’ (Tiger,
positive traits, but instead, focus on this idea of whether
1979, p. 18 as cited in Peterson, 2000). Thus, even opti-
others will be helpful or harmful toward the self.1
mism about other people tends to be framed in terms of
Indeed, the measures of generalized trust ignore posi-
the benefits to the self. Because of these characteristics
tive traits beyond trustworthiness. Generalized trust also
and because optimism references only the future rather
ignores readiness to hold positive perceptions of past
than current or past, we argue that it lacks the potential
behaviors by others. Furthermore, one can have general-
value of FIH. Perhaps these aspects explain why opti-
ized trust (i.e., believe that people tend to be trust-
mism’s long and otherwise fruitful research program has
worthy, helpful, and benign) without perceiving good
thus far failed to serve as a unifying concept in positive
within people. According to the social contract theory
psychology.
of Thomas Hobbes (1651/1998), selfish individuals will
Well-being and virtue, two pillars of positive psychol-
cooperate not out of goodness, but because it preserves
ogy, might also be seen as alternative candidates.
the social order that prevents society from descending
Between them, well-being and virtue encompass char-
into a chaotic state that would inevitably lead to perso-
acter strengths, values, happiness, health, flourishing,
nal ruin. For example, I can believe that my neighbors
optimal functioning, self-acceptance, personal growth,
are trustworthy and helpful, not because they are
purpose, autonomy, life satisfaction, and many more of
morally virtuous, but because I know they are bound
the most important measures of the good life (Wong,
by the force of law and pushed by social pressures to
2011). However, well-being and virtue (and the other
cooperate. Also, generalized trust lacks some of the
pillars of positive psychology) stand as somewhat dis-
nuances of FIH, as defined here. Humans are complex
tinct subfields within positive psychology. Virtue, well-
and can act morally but can also be selfish and destruc-
being, meaning, and resilience all employ different fra-
tive. Generalized trust does not capture this complexity.
meworks with different instruments and different ter-
Also, generalized trust is typically operationalized as
minologies to explain the good life. We argue that FIH
a belief about people in general, but FIH is broader
can tie together these crucial pillars of positive psychol-
because it relates also to attitudes toward specific indi-
ogy. FIH also includes the recognition and development
viduals. Given these problems, it is clear that the con-
of well-being and character strengths in others.
struct of FIH is not fully captured by generalized trust.
Other potential competitors include prosociality, ethi-
FIH may often manifest in trust of others, but FIH
cal-mindedness, empathy, and eudaimonia. Each of these
includes a broader time perspective, individuals, as well
alternate terms has some justification for deserving to be
as generalized populations, and a number of character-
a central theme within the science of positive psychology.
istics beyond trustworthiness.
8 R. G. TWEED ET AL.

However, FIH does add something beyond these terms. developing an adequate and brief measure of FIH will be
First of all, eudaimonia is problematic as an organizing particularly difficult because this construct is large, and
theme because many positive psychologists have focused in fact might be better conceived of as a meta-construct.
on happiness, so a focus on eudaimonia, rather than Each sub-element may have a cumulative effect, but the
uniting, might highlight existing divisions within the presence of one sub-element may not make another
field. Eudaimonia more clearly inhabits one particular much more likely. Furthermore, an adequate measure
stream within the discipline of positive psychology than of FIH will need to assess multiple time perspectives
does FIH. Ethical-mindedness has the same problem. including expectations of the future (e.g., generalized
Some researchers perceive their focus within positive trust), perceptions of the present (e.g., perceived social
psychology to be virtue, so again, a focus on ethical- support) and interpretation of the past (e.g., gratitude). It
mindedness would be boosting one area of positive psy- will also include various levels of specificity such as
chology rather than linking disparate areas within positive beliefs about people or groups in general and also
psychology. Prosociality is an interesting suggestion beliefs about individuals. An adequate measure will
because, if we focus on the level of behavior, one could also assess various domains including beliefs about
argue that prosociality does emerge as a central construct others within romantic relations, within family relations,
within positive psychology. The one difference is that within friendship relations, within stranger relations,
prosociality has already received much attention within within authority relations (e.g., bosses and underlings),
positive psychology (e.g., Aknin et al., 2015), but has thus and toward strangers. It will also assess FIH toward
far not served to unify it conceptually; as a result, prosoci- members of ingroups and outgroups (e.g., personal ene-
ality may have less potential as a construct that provides mies, adherents to opposing political or activist move-
new realizations and new perspectives regarding links ments, and groups perceived as usurping one’s
within positive psychology. Likewise, empathy may resources). Also, an adequate measure should assess
seem central to positive psychology, but empathy may both beliefs and behavioral indicators that the beliefs
at times increase bias against outgroups (Bloom, 2016), so are impacting behavior (e.g., acts of generosity, trust in
may actually counteract FIH. Additionally, these and other games) because proclaimed beliefs are sometimes
potential competitors can arguably be reduced to or inconsistent with implicit or effective beliefs and
explained by FIH, while the reverse does not seem to be because the definition of FIH includes not only percep-
true. As such, they may not suffice as central themes tions but also the idea that these perceptions must
within positive psychology. manifest in behavior. A measure of this many disparate
subcomponents may be difficult to create. That is why
the term ‘meta-construct’ rather than ‘construct’ might
Potential concern 3: what about the lack of
be a better descriptor of FIH. We may never have a brief
adequate measures?
measure of this construct because of its great breadth.
Within psychology, one often important step in studying Nonetheless, composite measures that include multiple
a topic is the creation of an adequate measure, yet no other measures could potentially be created.
single adequate measure of FIH currently exists. Perhaps Some have recently argued that the field of psychology is
the closest to an actual measure of FIH is the measure of undergoing a ‘theory crisis’ (e.g., Oberauer & Lewandowsky,
generalized trust, but we have argued that, while gen- 2019), in which weak theoretical-empirical links result in
eralized trust overlaps with FIH’s RtPP manifestation, it poor replicability. Others (e.g., Kruglanski, 2001) have argued
also contains many differences. Thus, we agree that the for the importance of broad theorizing for addressing sev-
current lack of a good measurement of FIH is a problem. eral negative consequences in the field, including field frag-
We hope that our arguments for the value of FIH blaze mentation and a lack of engagement with general cultural
a trail for the creation and validation of measures and dialogues. The theoretical coverage of FIH may have the
a novel experimental literature exploring the nomologi- potential to both address some of the fragmentation in
cal network of the construct. If FIH connects disparate positive psychology and to provide a new framework
areas in positive psychology, as we argue, such efforts for 1) synthesizing disparate findings from different subdis-
would ideally involve large-scale, multi-lab collabora- ciplines of positive psychology and 2) deriving theory-driven
tions involving researchers from all three core subfields hypotheses that can guide new research (e.g., that new
of positive psychology. Beyond the benefits of such measures of FIH will relate in predictable ways to other
collaborations for theoretical coherence in the field, uni- positive psychology concepts). Thus, while empirical work
ted research fronts would likely serve to increase the is necessary, there is also value in an attempt to outline
replicability of findings in positive psychology (c.f., the a broad theory that can explain and guide positive
Open Science Collaboration, 2015). On the other hand, psychology.
THE JOURNAL OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 9

Potential concern 4: is too much FIH a bad thing? change in some contexts but hinder it in others. The
impact of FIH on societal change is an empirical question
Individuals with too much trust are vulnerable to betrayal
and thus could be explored further.
and manipulation (e.g., McNulty & Fincham, 2012). Thus,
practitioners may be hesitant to promote FIH, and aca-
demics may be unwilling to value FIH. Potential concern 6: what about the lacunae?
In response to this concern, both ourselves and
Preston-Roedder (2013) distinguish FIH from blind Arguably, some domains within positive psychology are
trust – those high in FIH focus on the positives and work not encompassed by FIH. For example, self-compassion
to build these but are aware of and will often take steps to and mindfulness (with the exception of loving-kindness
protect themselves from the flaws in those around them. mindfulness), self-esteem, and time perspective are not
We have purposely selected the term ‘FIH’ as opposed to explicitly encompassed by FIH. Two considerations
‘trust in humanity’ not only to be consistent with Preston- deserve attention here. Firstly, if FIH is extended to
Roedder’s prior discussion but also because the word include attitudes toward the self as human, more of
‘trust’ carries a stronger connotation of making oneself these constructs are encompassed. Secondly, even if
vulnerable; it could be ethically irresponsible to give FIH is not expanded in this way, a concept need not
a society-wide recommendation that people should be explain everything to have explanatory power or to
more trusting, thereby making themselves more vulner- increase coherence within a field. Consider that evolu-
able to the whims of others (Schneier, 2012). That recom- tionary psychology does not explain all of social psychol-
mendation to trust could be particularly problematic if ogy, and Einstein’s general theory of relativity does not
those people encouraged to be more trusting live in explain all findings of physics, and neither the Big 5
unstable environments. FIH is not the opposite of cyni- (McCrae, 2002), nor McAdams and Pals (2006) larger
cism (belief that people are bad) and is closer to being the framework explain all of personality, but each still has
opposite of borderline-type splitting, which is the percep- cohesive and explanatory power. We suggest that FIH
tion of each person as bad or good and not a mix of both can bring together many (but not all) disparate topics of
(Gould, Prentice, & Ainslie, 1996). FIH, as defined by positive psychology, while increasing theoretical parsi-
Preston-Roedder (2013), and as adapted for positive psy- mony in the field.
chology by us, recognizes that people are flawed, so
caution in relations is warranted.
Potential concern 7: what about resistance to the
terminology and related view of human nature?
Potential concern 5: does FIH promote political
The term ‘faith in humanity’ may create resistance within
complacency?
some groups. Some may presume the term implies
Saguy (2018) argued that positive attitudes towards something religious and may resist for that reason.
powerful others hinder motivation for social change, Others may believe faith should only be placed in
and one could reasonably wonder whether FIH will simi- a deity. Others may question the view of human nature
larly promote political complacency. Perceiving positives implied by the term. We realized the term can create
in one’s opposition may, in fact, drain energy from misunderstandings, and we struggled to find a brief
efforts to fight for a cause and may cause one to neglect name that engenders fewer misunderstandings, but in
a need for social change. We accept this possible limita- the end decided to take the term as received (Preston-
tion and recognize that further research on that issue Roedder, 2013) and resolved instead to carefully explain
may have merit. Motivation for political change may the meaning and provide qualifications. Nonetheless, in
sometimes rely on anger and in those cases may reach cases of resistance, alternative terms might help clarify
its peak when thinking is simplified and FIH is limited. the concept. The term ‘enacted belief in humanity’ may
Nonetheless, we also suggest that once motivation and clarify the concept for groups who want to avoid the
action emerge, uncompromising negative characteriza- sound of a religious concept. ‘Enacted belief in imago
tion of one’s opponent, though common (Tetlock, 2003; dei’ may create more openness for those with
Van Boven, Ehret, & Sherman, 2018), might often achieve a traditional western orientation, and possibly ‘faith in
less than collaboration with and recalibration of percep- human yang’ may work for a Chinese context. Creative
tions of opposing groups (Kahane, 2017; Sherman, 2011; adaptation may have merit.
Staub, 2019). There may not always need to be Also, admittedly, the term does imply a somewhat
a dichotomy between promoting societal change and positive view of human nature. The current manuscript
promoting FIH. Perhaps FIH will speed positive social is not focused on the accuracy of that view of human
10 R. G. TWEED ET AL.

nature, and much of the argument contained herein human tendencies, at a meta-theoretical level, we
about the pervasiveness of FIH within positive psychology argue that an acknowledgment of this pervasiveness
could be accurate even if that view of human nature is could serve to highlight coherence in positive
mistaken. Nonetheless, positive psychologists, as argued psychology.
above, have often implicitly presumed a somewhat posi-
tive view of human nature. Furthermore, a number of
researchers, many of them not explicitly positive psychol- Conclusion
ogists, deserve credit for clarifying the extent to which We have argued that faith in humanity (FIH) functions as
positive perceptions of others can be justified. A number an implicit but seldom mentioned theme within positive
of authors (e.g., Crocker, Canevello, & Brown, 2017; psychology. We have also clarified that FIH is distinct
Fowers, 2015; Hare, 2017; Keltner, Kogan, Piff, & Saturn, from trust and distinct from the belief that all people are
2014; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2010; Tomasello, 2016, 2019) good. The importance of this theme is evident in all the
provide important support for this anticipation of positive domains of positive psychology reviewed here: each of
traits or positive potential among others. As Keltner and the original domains (positive states, positive traits, posi-
others have suggested, prosociality seems to be ‘(a) intui- tive institutions), second wave positive psychology, and
tive, (b) widespread, and deeply engrained in human the behavior of positive psychologists. Some disciplines
behavioral tendencies’ (Keltner et al., 2014). have been revitalized by recognition of a theme linking
Furthermore, a number of these same scholars, such as the findings of various practitioners and generating
Fowers and Tomasello have also provided helpful theories a new set of theoretically driven hypotheses (e.g., the
about evolutionary selective pressures favoring kindness Big Five in personality psychology, evolutionary theory
and other prosocial tendencies. in social psychology, and relativity in physics). Positive
If that somewhat positive view of human nature is psychology as a discipline is far from any impending
correct, FIH may provide a helpful corrective to common doom, but some problems exist. Within positive psychol-
views of human nature and to views of the readiness ogy, there exists a disparate set of findings regarding
with which many people will demonstrate positive traits. causes of well-being, but little discussion of what some
The hypothesized need for a corrective is consistent with of these causes share in common. Related criticisms
the Helliwell and Wang (2011) wallet drop analysis. could be made for research on strengths, positive
People were asked what would happen if lost wallets institutions, second wave positive psychology, and the
containing $200 were found by strangers. The respon- behavior of positive psychologists in their coaching and
dents evinced a relatively negative view of human nat- therapeutic interventions. Our discussion suggests that
ure by estimating that 25% of the wallets would be FIH is central to all the domains of positive psychology
returned. In fact, when wallets were purposely left to and thereby provides a linking theme, and provides
be found by a stranger, those wallets were returned 80% a conceptual rather than topical tie, and it may generate
of the time. This misestimate suggests that, in at least new hypotheses.
some contexts, FIH may provide a corrective that
increases the accuracy of views of human nature.
Admittedly, however, humans are complex and vary, so Note
a truly nuanced discussion of the positives and negatives
1. There were some who argued that trust involves seeing
of human nature would require much space and would
others as basically good (e.g., to trust someone is to
exceed the scope of this manuscript. expect that they have ‘goodwill’ and ‘benign intentions’
The FIH perspective adds to some prior work on Miller & Mitamura, 2003) but these definitions are less
human nature by not merely recognizing that positive representative of the construct used by most scholars,
traits are widespread in humans, but by recognizing who tend to focus on people in general and the expec-
tations of how they would treat the self.
the extent to which recognition of this fact inhabits
many constructs associated with well-being and also
permeates a number of aspects of positive psychology.
Disclosure statement
The current paper thereby extends beyond arguments
about human nature to a focus on how humans are No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
perceived. These topics overlap but are distinct. Yet
even if that research on human nature was not accu-
rate, the rest of this manuscript illustrates the way in Funding
which FIH, nonetheless, pervades much of positive psy- This work was supported by a Kwantlen Polytechnic University
chology. Going beyond the empirical work on positive [Chancellor’s Chair award].
THE JOURNAL OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 11

References Hare, B. (2017). Survival of the friendliest: Homo sapiens


evolved via selection for prosociality. Annual Review of
Aknin, L. B., Broesch, T., Hamlin, J. K., & Van De Psychology, 68(1), 155–186.
Vondervoort, J. W. (2015). Prosocial behavior leads to happi- Haslam, N., & Loughnan, S. (2016). How dehumanization pro-
ness in a small-scale rural society. Journal of Experimental motes harm. In A. G. Miller (Ed.), The social psychology of
Psychology: General, 144(4), 788–795. good and evil (2nd ed., pp. 140–158). New York: Guilford.
Algoe, S. B., & Haidt, J. (2009). Witnessing excellence in action: Helliwell, J., Layard, R., & Sachs, J. (2012). World happiness
The ‘other-praising’ emotions of elevation, gratitude, and report. New York: Sustainable Development Solutions
admiration. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4(2), 105–127. Network.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statis- Helliwell, J. F. (2003). How’s life? Combining individual and
tical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: national variables to explain subjective well-being.
Author. Economic Modelling, 20(2), 331–360.
Bandura, A. (2019). Applying theory for human betterment. Helliwell, J. F., & Wang, S. (2011). Trust and well-being.
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(1), 12–15. International Journal of Wellbeing, 1(1), 42–78.
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles Hobbes, T. (1998). Leviathan. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
among young adults: A test of a four-category model. (Original work published 1651).
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(2), 226–244. Huyn, K. H., Hall, B., Hurst, M. A., & Bikos, L. H. (2015). Evaluation of
Beck, A. T. (2019). A 60-year evolution of cognitive theory and the positive re-entry in corrections program. International
therapy. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(1), 16–20. Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 59
Bloom, P. (2016). Against empathy. New York, NY: HarperCollins. (9), 1006–1023.
Canevello, A., & Crocker, J. (2010). Creating good relationships: Hwang, P. O. (1995). Other-esteem: A creative response to
Responsiveness, relationship quality, and interpersonal a society obsessed with promoting the self. San Diego, CA:
goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(1), Black Forrest Press.
78–106. Ivtzan, I., Lomas, T., Hefferon, K., & Worth, P. (2016). Second
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2010). wave positive psychology: Embracing the dark side of life.
Optimism. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(7), 879–889. New York: Routledge.
Cattell, R. B., Eber, H. W., & Tatsuoka, M. M. (1970). Handbook for Kahane, A. (2017). Collaborating with the enemy. Oakland, CA:
the Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire (16PF). Berrett-Koehler.
Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing. Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. Oxford,
Coles, R. (1994). The call of service. New York: Houghton Mifflin England: W. W. Norton.
Harcourt. Keltner, D., Kogan, A., Piff, P. K., & Saturn, S. R. (2014). The
Crocker, J., Canevello, A., & Brown, A. A. (2017). Social motiva- Sociocultural Appraisals, Values, and Emotions (SAVE) frame-
tion: Costs and benefits of selfishness and otherishness. work of prosociality: Core processes from gene to meme.
Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 299–325. Annual Review of Psychology, 65(1), 425–460.
Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the Kruglanski, A. W. (2001). That “vision thing”: The state of theory
five-factor model. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 417–440. in social and personality psychology at the edge of the new
Dodge, K. A., Price, J. M., Bachorowski, J.-A., & Newman, J. P. millennium. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80
(1990). Hostile attributional biases in severely aggressive (6), 871–875.
adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99(4), 385–392. Martin, M. W. (2008). Paradoxes of happiness. Journal of
Elgar, F. J., & Aitken, N. (2010). Income inequality, trust and Happiness Studies, 9(2), 171–184.
homicide in 33 countries. European Journal of Public Health, McAdams, D. P., & Pals, J. L. (2006). A new big five. American
21(2), 241–246. Psychologist, 61(3), 204–217.
Fortune, C.-A., Ward, T., & Mann, R. (2015). Good lives & the McCrae, R. R. (2002). NEO-PI-R data from 36 cultures: Further
rehabilitation of sex offenders: A positive treatment intercultural comparisons. In R. R. McCrae & J. Alik (Eds.), The
approach. In S. Joseph (Ed.), Positive psychology in practice five-factor model of personality across cultures (pp. 105–125).
(2nd ed., pp. 635–657). Hoboken: NY.John Wiley & Sons. New York, NY: Kluwer.
Fowers, B. J. (2015). The evolution of ethics: Human sociality and McDowell, T. L., & Serovich, J. M. (2007). The effect of perceived
the emergence of ethical mindedness. New York, NY: Palgrave and actual social support on the mental health of
Macmillan. HIV-positive persons. AIDS Care, 19(10), 1223–1229.
Galliani, E. M., & Vianello, M. (2012). The emotion of admiration McNulty, J. K., & Fincham, F. D. (2012). Beyond positive
improves employees’ goal orientations and contextual per- psychology. American Psychologist, 67(2), 101–110.
formance. International Journal of Applied Psychology, 2(4), Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.). (2010). Prosocial motives,
43–52. emotions, and behavior: The better angels of our nature (1st
Gheilen, T. S., van Woerkom, M., & Meyers, M. C. (2017). ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Promoting positive outcomes through strengths Miller, A. S., & Mitamura, T. (2003). Are surveys on trust
interventions. The Journal of Positive Psychology. Advance trustworthy? Social Psychology Quarterly, 66(1), 62–70.
online publication. doi:10.1080/17439760.2017.1365164 Newton, K. (2001). Trust, social capital, civil society, and democracy.
Gough, H. G. (1987). California psychological inventory administra- International Political Science Review, 22(2), 201–214.
tor’s guide. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press Inc. Niemiec, R. M. (2012). Cinematic elevation and cinematic
Gould, J. R., Prentice, N. M., & Ainslie, R. C. (1996). The splitting admiration: Can watching movies positively impact you?
index. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66(2), 414–430. Amplifier, Spring/Summer, (2012, 10–11.
12 R. G. TWEED ET AL.

Oberauer, K., & Lewandowsky, S. (2019). Addressing the theory crisis Sherman, S. (2011). Changing the world: The science of trans-
in psychology. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26, 1596–1618. formative action. In R. Biswas-Diener (Ed.), Positive psychol-
Oishi, S., Kesebir, S., & Diener, E. (2011). Income inequality and ogy as social change (pp. 329–345). London: Springer.
happiness. Psychological Science, 22, 1095–1100. Smith, T. W., Hout, M., & Marsden, P. V. (2013). General social
Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproduci- survey, 1972–2012 [Cumulative file]. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-
bility of psychological science. Science, 349 (6251). university Consortium for Political and Social Research.
doi:10.1126/science.aac4716 Staub, E. (2019). Promoting healing and reconciliation in
Orwell, G. (2001). The prevention of literature. In P. Davison Rwanda. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(1),
(Ed.), The complete works of George Orwell: I belong to the left: 60–64.
1945 (Rev. ed., Vol. 17, pp. 369–381). London: Secker & Tangney, J. P. (2000). Humility: Theoretical perspectives,
Warburg. (Original work published 1946). empirical findings and directions for future research.
Park, N., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2013). Christopher M. Peterson Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 19(1), 70–82.
(1950–2012). American psychologist, 68(5), 403. Tetlock, P. E. (2003). Thinking the unthinkable: Sacred values
Peterson, C. (2000). The future of optimism. American and taboo cognitions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(7),
Psychologist, 55(1), 44–55. 320–324.
Peterson, C. (2008, June 17). Other people matter: Two exam- Tiger, L. (1979). Optimism: The biology of hope. New York: Simon
ples (web log comment). Retrieved from https://www.psy & Schuster.
chologytoday.com/ca/blog/the-good-life/200806/other- Tingey, J. L., McGuire, A. P., Stebbins, O. L., & Erickson, T. M.
people-matter-two-examples (2017). Moral elevation and compassionate goals. Journal of
Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Character strengths and Positive Psychology. Advance online publication.
virtues: A handbook and classification. New York, NY: Oxford doi:10.1080/17439760.2017.1402077
University Press. Tomasello, M. (2016). A natural history of human morality.
Pittinsky, T. L., Rosenthal, S. A., & Montoya, R. M. (2011). Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.
Measuring positive attitudes toward outgroups: Tomasello, M. (2019). Becoming human: A theory of ontogeny.
Development and validation of the Allophilia Scale. In Cambridge, Massachusetts: The Belknap Press of Harvard
L. R. Tropp & R. K. Mallett Eds., Moving beyond prejudice University Press.
reduction: Pathways to positive intergroup relations (41–60). Tweed, R. G., Mah, E., Dobrin, M., Van Poele, R., & Conway, L. G.,
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. III. (2017). How can positive psychology influence public
doi:10.1037/12319-002 policy and practice? In C. Proctor (Ed.), Positive psychology
Preston-Roedder, R. (2013). Faith in humanity. Philosophy and interventions in practice (pp. 257–271). Cham, Switzerland:
Phenomenological Research, 87(3), 664–687. Springer.
Rosenberg, M. (1956). Misanthropy and political ideology. Van Boven, L., Ehret, P. J., & Sherman, D. K. (2018). Psychological
American Sociological Review, 21(6), 690–695. barriers to bipartisan public support for climate policy.
Rotter, J. B. (1980). Interpersonal trust, trustworthiness, and Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(4), 492–507.
gullibility. American Psychologist, 35(1), 1–7. doi:10.1177/1745691617748966
Rovenpor, D. R., O’Brien, T. C., Roblain, A., Guissme, L. D., & Veenhoven, R. (2015). Informed pursuit of happiness: What we
Chekroun, P. (2019). Intergroup conflict self-perpetuates via should know, do know and can get to know? Journal of
meaning. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 116(1), Happiness Studies, 16(4), 1035–1071.
119–140. Wong, P. T. (2011). Positive psychology 2.0: Towards a balanced
Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. interactive model of the good life. Canadian Psychology, 52
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57(3), 316–331. (2), 69–81.
Saguy, T. (2018). Downside of intergroup harmony? When Wood, A. M., Emmons, R. A., Algoe, S. B., Froh, J. J.,
reconciliation might backfire and what to do. Policy Lambert, N. M., & Watkins, P. (2016). A dark side of gratitude?
Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5(1), 75–81. Distinguishing between beneficial gratitude and its harmful
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1993). On the power of positive impostors for the positive clinical psychology of gratitude
thinking: The benefits of being optimistic. Current Directions and well-being. In A. M. Wood & J. Johnson (Eds.), The Wiley
in Psychological Science, 2(1), 26–30. handbook of positive clinical psychology (pp. 137–151).
Schneier, B. (2012). Liars and outliers: Enabling the trust that Oxford, UK: Wiley.
society needs to thrive. Indianapolis, IN: Wiley. Yeager, D. S. (2017). Dealing with social difficulty during ado-
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psy- lescence: The role of implicit theories of personality. Child
chology: An introduction. American Psychologist, 55(1), 5–14. Development Perspectives, 11(3), 196–201.

You might also like