You are on page 1of 5

This article was downloaded by: [University of California, San Diego]

On: 02 June 2015, At: 11:22


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Teaching of Psychology
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/htop20

Teaching Self-Regulated Learning Through a "Learning


to Learn" Course
Barbara K. Hofer & Shirley L. Yu
Published online: 20 Nov 2009.

To cite this article: Barbara K. Hofer & Shirley L. Yu (2003) Teaching Self-Regulated Learning Through a "Learning to Learn"
Course, Teaching of Psychology, 30:1, 30-33, DOI: 10.1207/S15328023TOP3001_05

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15328023TOP3001_05

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
METHODS AND TECHNIQUES

Teaching Self-Regulated Learning Through


a “Learning to Learn” Course
Barbara K. Hofer
Middlebury College

Shirley L. Yu
University of Houston
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 11:22 02 June 2015

This study addressed the impact of a semester-long course called ough description of the course design, see Hofer, Yu, &
Learning to Learn, an undergraduate psychology course designed Pintrich, 1998). The attention to general cognitive,
to teach college students to be self-regulated learners. Results of metacognitive, and motivational strategies (e.g., adaptive at-
pretesting and posttesting of 78 students with the Motivated Strat- tributions) is in line with research on the importance of con-
egies for Learning Questionnaire (Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, & sidering both motivation and cognition in models of
McKeachie, 1993) provided support for the intervention. Mean self-regulated learning (Garcia & Pintrich, 1994; Pintrich &
differences and correlational results suggest that students increased De Groot, 1990; Wolters, Yu, & Pintrich, 1996).
in their mastery orientation to learning and their self-efficacy for Previous research on the course (McKeachie, Pintrich, &
learning, increased in their valuing of the course and in cognitive Lin, 1985) did not include attention to motivational factors.
strategy use, and declined in test anxiety over the term. These find- In that study, students enrolled in Learning to Learn, in con-
ings suggest that an intervention that targets a range of cognitive trast to students in comparison groups, decreased in test anx-
and motivational components can have utility for college students iety and increased in their expectancy of success and need for
and that there is value to a stand-alone course in learning to learn cognition, as measured by the Learning and Study Strategies
at the college level. Inventory (Weinstein, 1982). The researchers concluded
that further research was needed to examine the role that
Self-regulated learning is an important aspect of student ac- motivational factors play in a course that targets the fostering
ademic performance and achievement, yet it is seldom an ex- of self-regulated learning. In recent years, knowledge about
plicit goal of classroom instruction at the college level. There students’ motivation and cognition has helped refine both
are various models of self-regulated learning (see Schunk & the conceptual model and the course design. Research about
Zimmerman, 1994b), but all share the assumption that stu- the effects of this intervention on motivation and cognition
dents can actively regulate their cognition, motivation, or be- may be of value to others planning similar courses.
havior and, through these processes, enhance performance
and achieve educational goals (Zimmerman, 1989). However,
Course Description
many college students do not become self-regulated learners,
and psychologists know too little about how self-regulated
learning develops or the effects of formal interventions to in- Learning to Learn is an introductory level course offered
crease it (Schneider & Pressley, 1989; Schunk & Zimmerman, through the Department of Psychology at the University of
1994a). Although there is some evidence that formal attempts Michigan since 1982. The course, which has no prerequisites
to teach students to be self-regulated learners can be successful for enrollment, is targeted to first- and second-year students
(Hattie, Biggs, & Purdie, 1996; Simpson, Hynd, Nist, & Bur- who have experienced difficulty in coursework, who are dis-
rell, 1997), there are still many unresolved issues regarding the appointed in their academic performance, or who simply wish
teaching of cognitive and self-regulatory strategies, particu- to become better learners. Demand usually exceeds enroll-
larly at the postsecondary level. ment, and students enroll from all majors. The course in-
In this study we addressed the impact of a course called volves 4 hr of class time weekly, 2 hr in lecture and 2 hr in a
Learning to Learn, designed to teach college students to be laboratory/discussion format. The instructor presents princi-
self-regulated learners. This undergraduate course integrates ples, concepts, and research findings in the lectures. The lab-
theoretical and conceptual issues related to learning and mo- oratories, led by graduate student instructors, provide the
tivation from cognitive, social, and educational psychology. link between concepts and students’ learning, with demon-
A central component of the course is a weekly laboratory in strations, group work, and activities that enhance application
which students apply course constructs to their learning. Is- and practice in self-regulated learning.
sues we have addressed in the design of this course include The first goal of this course is to teach students concepts of
scope of the course, content, and time frame (for a more thor- cognitive and motivational psychology. We attempt to help

30 Teaching of Psychology
students understand the mental processes involved in learn- self-report questionnaire that consists of 72 items on student
ing, memory, and problem solving and to help them build motivation and strategy use. Students respond to items on a
conditional knowledge about why and when to use various 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all true of me) to 5
strategies. The second goal is to increase students’ effective- (very true of me) in reference to a particular course. For pur-
ness as learners by developing a repertoire of learning strate- poses of this research, we identified six motivational scales
gies. Given the importance of lifelong learning, we believe and seven cognitive scales as most theoretically relevant to
that good teaching involves teaching students how to learn, the self-regulated learning goals of this course. Alphas ranged
how to remember and think, and how to motivate themselves from .62 to .93 for the motivational scales and from .64 to .80
(Weinstein & Mayer, 1986). for the cognitive scales. We also examined academic perfor-
mance, as measured by final course grade, scored on a 0 to 4.0
Course Materials and Requirements scale (4.0 = A).
Motivational constructs from the MSLQ included Intrin-
sic Goal Orientation, reflecting a concern with learning and
Students purchase two textbooks, a cognitive psychology mastery, and Extrinsic Goal Orientation, focusing on a con-
text and a practical study skills book. Assessment includes cern with grades and getting the highest grades in the class.
two short quizzes, two unit tests, and a final examination. We also included Interest, representing the intrinsic interest
Students also work in small groups to develop and conduct an in the course material; Utility, concerning beliefs about the
empirical research project focused on learning and studying usefulness of the course content; Self-Efficacy, reflecting per-
issues. These projects provide experience in conducting and ceptions of the capability to learn and understand course ma-
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 11:22 02 June 2015

reporting psychological research, help students elaborate and terial; and Test Anxiety, concerning feelings of worry and
apply ideas from the course, generate data that help students anxiety over doing well on tests.
understand more about learning strategies, and give students Cognitive constructs from the MSLQ included Memoriza-
practice in learning to work cooperatively in a situation in tion, use of basic rehearsal and memory strategies; Elabora-
which instructors give explicit attention to skills necessary tion, use of paraphrasing or summarizing strategies;
for effective groupwork. In addition, students keep a journal Organization, use of networking or outlining strategies;
with responses to questions that prompt reflection on read- Planning, goal setting and task analysis; and Monitoring,
ings, lecture, and laboratory, and that require them to inte- such as tracking attention when reading. In addition, we
grate course material with their experiences as learners. An summed elaboration and organization to create a composite
additional journal task is to report on learning and motiva- scale of Deep Processing; similarly, we summed planning and
tion in a “target course,” identified by students as a course in monitoring to create a composite for Metacognition.
which they will deliberately apply new strategies and reflect
on their progress in doing so.
Procedure

Purpose of This Study Students completed the MSLQ during the first and last
class period. This instrument also provided students with a
In this study we examined whether students in the Learning motivational and cognitive strategy profile relevant to this
to Learn course showed changes in motivation and cognition. course, which we discussed immediately after they completed
Based on the goals and design of the course, and on suggestions the instrument on the first day of class.
of earlier researchers that motivation may be an important fac-
tor in this course, we were interested in two main questions: (a)
What changes occur in motivation and cognition from the be- Results
ginning of the course to the end? and (b)What is the relation
between motivation and cognition over time? The first research question concerned changes in motiva-
tion and cognition from the beginning of the course (Time 1)
to the end (Time 2). With an alpha level set at .003 based on a
Method Bonferroni adjustment to control for Type I error, paired t tests
showed significant increases in three motivational variables
Participants (see Table 1 for statistical values): intrinsic goal orientation,
utility, and self-efficacy. Test anxiety decreased significantly.
We conducted this study during one term of the course, In regard to cognitive variables, there were significant in-
with 78 students enrolled in three laboratory sections; 70 stu- creases in reported use of memorization, elaboration, organiza-
dents voluntarily completed both the precourse and tion, deep processing, planning, and metacognition.
postcourse measures. Of the 78 students, 55% were women, The second research question concerned the relations
and 73% were first- or second-year students. among motivational and cognitive variables (see Table 2).
Most notably at Time 1, intrinsic goal orientation was posi-
tively correlated with deep processing, r(75) = .26, p < .05;
Measures planning, r(75) = .34, p < .01; monitoring, r(75) = .37, p <
.01; and metacognition, r(75) = .42, p < .05. Extrinsic goal
The Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire orientation was also correlated with planning, r(75) = .24, p
(MSLQ; Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, & McKeachie, 1993) is a < .05; monitoring, r(75) = .24, p < .01; and metacognition,

Vol. 30 No. 1, 2003 31


Table 1. Summary Statistics for Motivation and Cognition Over Time

Pretest Posttest Change

Variable M SD M SD t p

Motivation
Intrinsic goal 3.80 0.79 4.03 0.83 –3.20 .002
Extrinsic goal 3.24 0.93 3.21 1.11 0.75 ns
Interest 3.81 0.73 3.67 0.85 1.70 ns
Utility 4.34 0.72 3.96 1.11 3.15 .002
Self-efficacy 4.02 0.78 4.43 0.65 –4.55 .001
Test anxiety 2.86 1.15 2.48 0.99 3.46 .001
Cognition
Memorization 3.77 0.79 4.15 0.78 –4.23 .001
Elaboration 3.28 0.70 3.67 0.79 –4.75 .001
Organization 3.62 0.63 3.96 0.70 –3.89 .001
Deep processing 3.46 0.54 3.82 0.67 –5.11 .001
Planning 3.46 0.63 3.84 0.72 –3.96 .001
Monitoring 3.47 0.84 3.62 0.92 –1.21 ns
Metacognition 3.47 0.63 3.73 0.76 –2.61 .01
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 11:22 02 June 2015

Note. N = 70. Degrees of freedom range from 70 to 78.

Table 2. Zero-Order Correlations Between Motivation and Cognition at Time 1 and Time 2

Motivation Time 1 Motivation Time 2

Intrinsic Extrinsic Self- Test Intrinsic Extrinsic Self- Test


Variable Goal Goal Interest Utility Efficacy Anxiety Goal Goal Interest Utility Efficacy Anxiety

Cognition (Time 1)
Memorization –.07 .27* .27* .21 .22 .17 –.21 .30** –.05 –.01 .08 –.01
Elaboration .22 .11 .11 .07 .11 –.23* .19 –.07 .17 .02 .12 –.16
Organization .20 .17 .40*** .12 .42*** –.01 .12 .03 .05 .09 .43*** –.06
Deep
processing .26* .17 .30** .12 .32** –.17 .19 –.03 .13 .07 .33** –.14
Planning .34** .24* .21 –.03 .15 –.14 .16 .08 .01 –.06 .30** –.12
Monitoring .37** .24* .13 .04 .11 –.25* .14 .16 –.01 –.22 –.03 –.13
Metacognition .42*** .28* .19 .01 .15 –.24* .17 .15 –.01 –.17 .14 –.15
Cognition (Time 2)
Memorization .12 .32** .41*** .14 .21 .17 .14 .05 .38*** .33** .23 .08
Elaboration .22 .08 .48*** .28* .04 .00 .37*** –.34** .51*** .40*** .24* –.09
Organization .26* .20 .51*** .27* .01 .18 .38*** –.19 .57*** .37** .35** –.01
Deep
processing .27* .15 .54*** .31** .03 .10 .42*** –.30* .59** .43*** .32** –.05
Planning .31** .20 .44*** .24* –.14 .14 .33** –.14 .57** .32** .19 .11
Monitoring –.25* .19 .27* .37** –.02 .12 .34** –.22 .54** .44*** .24* .05
Metacognition .30* .21 .37** .34** –.08 .14 .37** –.20 .60** .42*** .23* .08
Final grade .15 .25* .10 .07 –.12 .22 .21 –.03 .17 .05 .25* –.02

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

r(75) = .28, p < .05, as well as memorization strategies, r(75) course showed a significant correlation with all end-of-term
= .27, p < .05; but not with deep processing. Both interest cognitive strategy variables, and utility with all but memori-
and self-efficacy were correlated with organization, r(75) = zation. Final grade in the course was positively correlated
.40, p < .001, and r(75) = .42, p < .001; and deep processing with only one pretest motivational variable, extrinsic motiva-
r(75) = .30, p < .01, r(75) = .32, p < .01. Finally, test anxi- tion, r(73) = .25, p < .01.
ety showed a significant negative correlation with elabora- We also examined correlations between motivational and
tion, r(75) = –.23, p < .05; monitoring, r(75) = –.25, p < cognitive variables within Time 2. At the course’s conclu-
.05; and metacognition, r(75) = –.24, p < .05. sion, intrinsic goal orientation was positively correlated with
Correlations of Time 1 motivation variables with Time 2 all cognitive strategy variables except memorization, whereas
cognitive variables indicated that intrinsic goal orientation at interest and utility were correlated with reported use of all six
the start of the course was positively correlated with the same cognitive strategies and self-efficacy with all except memori-
four cognitive variables at the end of the course as at the be- zation and planning. Extrinsic orientation correlated with
ginning, as well as with organization, r(70) = .26, p < . 05, only elaboration and deep processing, and test anxiety
whereas extrinsic goal orientation at the outset of the semes- showed no significant correlations. The only end-of-term
ter was correlated only with the use of memorization strate- motivational variable significantly correlated with final grade
gies by course conclusion, r(70) = .32, p < .01. Interest in the was self-efficacy, r(73) = .25, p < .05.

32 Teaching of Psychology
Discussion self-regulatory strategies. In D. H. Schunk & B. J. Zimmerman
(Eds.), Self-regulation of learning and performance: Issues and educa-
tional applications (pp. 127–153). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
The mean differences and correlational results suggest Associates, Inc.
that students in the Learning to Learn course increased in Hattie, J., Biggs, J., & Purdie, N. (1996). Effects of learning skills in-
mastery orientation and self-efficacy for learning, increased terventions on student learning: A meta-analysis. Review of Edu-
in their valuing of the course, and declined in test anxiety. cational Research, 66, 99–136.
Students also increased their cognitive strategy use in one Hofer, B. K., & Pintrich, P. R. (1997). The development of
term. Moreover, at the end of the term, motivational beliefs epistemological theories: Beliefs about knowledge and knowing and
and strategy use were positively correlated for most of the their relation to learning. Review of Educational Research, 67, 88–140.
constructs of interest in the expected direction and in line Hofer, B. K., Yu, S. L, & Pintrich, P. R. (1998). Teaching college stu-
dents to be self-regulated learners. In D. H. Schunk & B. J.
with previous research in this area (Pintrich & Schrauben, Zimmerman (Eds.), Self-regulated learning: From teaching to
1992). Although we cannot make strong inferential claims self-reflective practice (pp. 57–85). New York: Guilford.
without a control group, this one-term course intervention McKeachie, W. J., Pintrich, P. R., & Lin, Y.-G. (1985). Teaching
appeared to have positive effects in developing motivation learning strategies. Educational Psychologist, 20, 153–160.
and strategy skills for self-regulated learning. Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review
These findings both parallel and supplement earlier re- of Educational Research, 66, 543–578.
search on the course (McKeachie et al., 1985). The needed re- Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and
search on motivational components such as intrinsic self-regulated learning components of classroom academic perfor-
Downloaded by [University of California, San Diego] at 11:22 02 June 2015

motivation, interest, value, and self-efficacy, as suggested in mance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 33–40.
McKeachie et al., was conducted in this study with the use of Pintrich, P. R., & Schrauben, B. (1992). Students’ motivational be-
liefs and their cognitive engagement in classroom tasks. In D. H.
the MSLQ. These findings add further support for a model that Schunk & J. L. Meece (Eds.), Student perceptions in the classroom:
incorporates motivational components into self-regulated Causes and consequences (pp. 149–183). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
learning. Furthermore, the increase in self-efficacy and the Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
correlation between self-efficacy and cognition suggested by Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A. F., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W. J.
this study support the importance of self-efficacy in cognitive (1993). Reliability and predictive validity of the Motivated Strat-
engagement and its possible mediational role in performance egies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ). Educational and Psy-
(Pajares, 1996; Pintrich & De Groot, 1990). chological Measurement, 53, 801–813.
Further work is needed to assess the degree to which stu- Schneider, W., & Pressley, M. (1989). Memory development between
dents are able to effectively transfer these motivational 2 and 20. New York: Springer-Verlag.
changes and cognitive strategies to other courses. These Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (1994a). Self-regulation in edu-
cation: Retrospect and prospect. In D. H. Schunk & B. J.
changes suggest that students are developing a more Zimmerman (Eds.), Self-regulation of learning and performance: Is-
self-reflective approach to their learning, but more research is sues and educational applications (pp. 305–314). Hillsdale, NJ: Law-
needed to assess the degree to which they sustain this change rence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
over time. Changes in the course, such as the inclusion of more Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (1994b). Self-regulation of learn-
domain-specific strategies, may also be necessary to better sup- ing and performance: Issues and educational applications. Hillsdale,
port both transfer and sustained change. Additional research NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
is also needed in the area of epistemological beliefs (see Hofer Simpson, M., Hynd, C., Nist, S., & Burrell, K. (1997). College aca-
& Pintrich, 1997) to examine how students’ beliefs about demic assistance programs and practices. Educational Psychology
knowledge may constrain the use of deeper processing strate- Review, 9, 39–87.
gies. Perhaps it is not only that college students do not know Weinstein, C. E. (1982). LASSI user’s manual for those administering
the Learning and Study Strategies Inventory. Clearwater, FL: H & H.
how and when to use strategies, but that they have beliefs Weinstein, C. E., & Mayer, R. (1986). The teaching of learning
about knowledge that limit their use of strategies. For example, strategies. In M. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching
thinking that knowledge is a collection of facts may restrict (3rd ed., pp. 315–327). New York: Macmillan.
students to the use of memorization as a study strategy. If stu- Wolters, C. A., Yu, S. L., & Pintrich, P. R. (1996). The relation between
dents hold such beliefs, then effective interventions might goal orientation and students’ motivational beliefs and self-regulated
need to target these beliefs. learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 8, 211–238.
Overall, our findings suggest that an intervention that ad- Zimmerman, B. J. (1989). A social cognitive view of self-regulated
dresses a range of cognitive and motivational components academic learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 329–339.
can have utility for college students. Results suggest that mo-
tivational components are an important part of students’
Notes
self-regulation and can help to facilitate students’ use of vari-
ous cognitive and self-regulatory strategies. These findings
also indicate that there is value to a stand-alone course in 1. An earlier version of this article was presented at the annual meeting
learning to learn at the college level, as college instructors of the American Psychological Association, Boston, August 1999.
2. This course was developed by Wilbert J. McKeachie, to whom we
may be unlikely to teach general learning and self-regulatory
are indebted for providing us the opportunity to teach the course
strategies in their discipline-specific courses. and the encouragement to conduct research on its effects. We
also appreciate the continued collaboration of Paul Pintrich in re-
fining this model of self-regulated learning and the assistance of
References
Yi-Guang Lin with early analyses of the data.
3. Send correspondence to Barbara K. Hofer, Psychology Depart-
Garcia, T., & Pintrich, P. R. (1994). Regulating motivation and cog- ment, Bicentennial Hall, Middlebury College, Middlebury, VT
nition in the classroom: The role of self-schemas and 05753; e-mail: bhofer@middlebury.edu.
Vol. 30 No. 1, 2003 33

You might also like