You are on page 1of 8

37

DEVELOPMENT AND EVALUATION OF A SCALE TO


MEASURE PERCEIVED SELF-EFFICACY IN PEOPLE
WITH ARTHRITIS

KATE LORIG, ROBERT L. CHASTAIN, ELAINE UNG, STANFORD SHOOR,


and HALSTED R. HOLMAN

There is evidence that the psychological attribute ercise, relaxation, and walking) and changes in health
of perceived self-efficacyplays a role in mediating health status (pain, disability, and depression), the expected
outcomes for persons with chronic arthritis who take the associations were weak or were absent (2).
Arthritis Self-Management Course. An instrument to These unexpected findings precipitated an in-
measure perceived self-efficacy was developed through terview with the participants and evaluation of their
consultation with patients and physicians and through experiences in the course. Fifty-four participants were
study of 4 groups of patients. Tests of construct and asked why they found the course helpful or not help-
concurrent validity and of reliability showed that the ful. For half the people interviewed, pain and/or
instrument met appropriate standards. Health outcomes disability had decreased; for the other half, pain and/or
and self-efficacy scores improved during the Arthritis disability had not changed or had increased. The
Self-Management Course, and the improvements were former group attributed their benefits to an increased
correlated. sense of influence over the consequences of arthritis;
the latter group believed that they could do little to
We describe the development and testing of an improve their situation (3).
instrument to measure patients’ perceived self-efficacy The findings from these interviews indicated
(SE) to cope with the consequences of chronic arthri- that a sense of one’s personal ability to affect the
tis. The need for such an instrument arose in the consequences of disease was strong in some subjects
course of studying the effects of the Arthritis Self- and relatively weak in others, and interacted with the
Management Course (ASMC). When evaluated in ran- course to create the health outcomes. This sense of
domized studies, subjects who took the ASMC were ability to effect change (akin to confidence) is similar
found to have less pain and to be more active than to the psychological concept of perceived SE (43). In
controls (1). These results persisted, albeit attenuated, a preliminary study using early instruments to measure
for 20 months after the course, without reinforcement. perceived SE to cope with the consequences of arthri-
However, when the data were examined for antici- tis, we found statistically significant correlations be-
pated associations between changes in behavior (ex- tween perceived SE and health status (ref. 6 and
unpublished observations). We therefore sought to
develop a reliable and valid instrument for measuring
From the Department of Medicine, Stanford University,
Stanford, California. perceived SE.
Supported by NIH Multipurpose Arthritis Center grant Perceived SE, as postulated by Bandura (4), is
20610-05 and by the Arthritis Foundation. one’s belief that one can perform a specific behavior or
Kate Long, DrPH; Robert L. Chastain, MS; Elaine Ung,
BS; Stanford Shoor, MD; Halsted R. Holman, MD. task in the future. It refers to personal judgments of
Address reprint requests to Kate Lorig, DrPH, Senior performance capabilities in a given domain of activity.
Research Associate, Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Although it is related to other psychological concepts,
HRP Building, Room 6 , Stanford, CA 94305.
Submitted for publication May 29, 1987; accepted in revised such as locus of control, learned helplessness, and
form July 8, 1988. self-esteem, it differs in that it is behavior-specific.

Arthritis and Rheumatism, Vol. 32, No. 1 (January 1989)


38 LORIG ET AL

Thus, SE is measured as a specific state, not a gener- Table 1. Steps in the development of the self-efficacy scale
alized trait. For example, one may have high SE for I. Item generation
using medications correctly but low S E for utilizing 1. 23 items generated by a rheumatologist
relaxation to relieve pain. Perceived SE is the belief 2. 20 items refined in patient focus group
11. Initial instrument development study (n = 97)
that one can achieve a behavior or a state of mind; it is 1. Baseline and 4-month values
not an actual measure of accomplishment. 2. Factor analysis
Self-efficacy theory states that 1) perceived SE 3. Function self-efficacy
4. Other symptom self-efficacy
for behaviors that affect health status will predict 5 . Alpha coefficient (measure of internal reliability)
future health status, given that subjects believe that 6 . Tests of construct validity
the outcome of the behavior will be improved health 111. Concurrent validity test (n = 43)
IV. Replication study (n = 144)
status and that they value improved health status, 2) 1. Baseline and 4-month values
SE is not a static trait; it can be altered, and 3) 2. Factor analysis
3. Pain self-efficacy
enhanced SE will be associated with improved health 4. Function self-efficacy
status in the areas affected by those specific behaviors. 5 . Other symptom self-efficacy
As a psychological trait, it is likely that there are many 6 . Alpha coefficient (measure of internal reliability)
V. Confirmatory analysis (n = 144 and n = 97)*
relevant antecedents and components of perceived 1. Factor analysis
SE, such as inherent motor or mental skill, experience 2. Alpha coefficient (measure of internal reliability)
with a particular behavior, general sense of ability or VI. Test-retest reliability study (n = 91)
self-worth, and motivation for accomplishment. Be- * Scales generated on replication sample, then applied to original
cause it is, by definition, behavior-specific, specific development sample.
questions can be designed to describe it (4). Also,
particular learning and practicing activities or tech- disability (measured by the Health Assessment Question-
niques can be devised to facilitate and improve the naire) (9), and depression (measured by the short version of
conduct of the relevant behavior and, thereby, pre- the Beck Depression Scale) (10). All instruments have been
sumably increase an individual’s perceived SE to tested for reliability and, when appropriate, for validity.
Subjects composing the initial group entered the
accomplish the behavior. ASMC study in the fall of 1984. Subjects for the replication
The behavior-specific nature of SE made it group entered the study in the spring of 1985. Subjects in the
necessary to develop an instrument specific for its concurrent validation and reliability group were past ASMC
measurement in people who have arthritis. The stages participants who had not previously completed the efficacy
in the development process are illustrated in Table 1. scale questionnaire. The correlations reported in Tables 5
and 6 are for combined data from experimental and control
They include design of the initial scales from data subjects.
obtained from one population, refinement (replication)
of the scales using a second population, and confirma- RESULTS
tory testing of factors that represent the refined scales Generation of items. Based on the suggestions
using both the original population sample and the
from the 1981 Conference on Outcome Measures (1 l),
replication sample. The results from each stage of
the specific behaviors of importance to persons with
development are described below, and preliminary arthritis were identified as controlling pain and disabil-
tests of hypotheses relating perceived SE to health ity. A rheumatologist composed 23 questions related
status follow. to these behaviors (12). These questions were tested
with 3 focus groups, each of which consisted of 5-10
PATIENTS AND METHODS patients. Each focus group began by answering the
questions. Afterwards, participants discussed the
All tests of this instrument were conducted with
subjects who had been recruited for an ASMC by means of questions and described problems they had had in
public service announcements and referrals from health understanding Or interpreting them. Next, the concept
professionals. After completing an initial application, the of self-efficacy was explained, and participants were
subjects were randomized to a control group (no interm- asked to write down additional items related to control
tion) or an experimental group (participation in the ASMC). of their arthritis symptoms. The focus groups helped
Data were collected by self-administered questionnairesthat
were mailed to the subjects at baseline and again 4 months reshape many of the Original items and added 2o items
later, as previously described (1). We studied such variables that dealt with arthritis symptoms. Thus, there were 43
as pain (measured on a 10-cm visual analog scale) (7,8), items in the original item pool.
SELF-EFFICACY SCALE 39

Table 2. Characteristics of the 3 study samples used to develop and evaluate the perceived
self-efficacy scale
Initial development Replication Reliability
sample (n = 97) sample (n = 144) sample (n = 91)
~ ~

% female 86 80 78
Age (mean ? SD) 64.7 k 12.9 63.7 2 12.9 62.8 ? 13.2
Education (mean ? SD years) 14.1 ? 3.2 14.7 ? 2.8 14.0 ? 2.8
9% with osteoarthritis 56 58 43
% with rheumatoid arthritis 15 22 57
9% with other arthritis 29 20 0

Initial instrument development study. The 43 which represent the variance of the factors within each
items were transformed into questions. At baseline scale, were 4.83 and 4.76 for FSE and OSE, respec-
and 4 months later, 97 people who were enrolled in an tively. Alpha coefficients, which test for the internal
ASMC answered this self-administered questionnaire. reliability of items within a subscale, were estimated at
The characteristics of this sample are described in 0.93 for FSE and 0.90 for OSE.
Table 2. Using their answers, we reduced the original Self-efficacy theory predicts that present SE
set of 43 items to 25 items that were related to SE by will be related both to present health status and, more
1) removing items that were not related to the total importantly, to future health status. Using baseline
score, and 2) analyzing principal component factors and 4-month data from the above sample, correlations
with varimax (orthogonal) rotation. A factor is a were performed to determine whether the subscales
weighted linear composite of items, and it can be acted as the theory predicts. As illustrated in Table 3,
viewed as a discrete component of a large concept that construct validity was upheld by the finding of signif-
is addressed by a subset of questions that are separa- icant correlations between baseline SE and baseline
ble from those associated with other components. health status, between baseline SE and health status 4
Utilizing the baseline data, an exploratory max- months later, and between 4-month SE and 4-month
imum likelihood factor analysis identified 2 factors health status. FSE was most highly related to function
(1 3). We labeled these as self-efficacy for physical (disability); OSE was most highly related to depres-
function (FSE) and self-efficacy for controlling other sion.
arthritis symptoms (OSE). Item loadings (relative Concurrent validity test. In addition to construct
weights of the individual items to the whole factor validity, the FSE items were tested for concurrent
score) on the FSE ranged from 0.41 to 0.78, and on the validity. A new sample of 43 people with arthritis
OSE, item loadings ranged from 0.44 to 0.82. These completed the FSE scale by mail. A trained observer
loadings are all higher than the 0.40 criterion com-
who was blinded to their responses then visited in their
monly used in questionnaire design (14). Eigenvalues,
homes. Each subject was asked to perform the same
tasks on which they had previously rated their per-
Table 3. Associations between self-efficacy (SE) and health
status (HS) in initial development sample* ceived SE. The observer timed their performance of
the tasks and rated each according to a scale of 0 (no
Pain Disability Depression
difficulty) to 4 (great difficulty). Thus, actual perfor-
Baseline S E with baseline HS mance was compared with perceived ability, or effi-
FSE -0.35 -0.68 -0.45 cacy, to perform. Self-efficacy theory differentiates
OSE -0.45 -0.41 -0.45
Baseline S E with 4-month HS between perceived performance and actual perfor-
FSE -0.44 -0.68 -0.32 mance, but there is usually a moderately high correla-
OSE -0.40 -0.42 -0.39 tion between the two. For this sample, we found a
4-month SE with 4-month HS
FSE -0.48 -0.73 -0.48 correlation of r = 0.61 ( P < 0.01).
OSE -0.55 -0.54
~~
-0.60 Replication study. The same 43-item SE ques-
* Data were combined from a control group (n = 24) and an tionnaire was administered to 144 new subjects who
experimental group (n = 73) who took the Arthritis Self-Manage- had been recruited for an ASMC. Table 2 contains
ment Course (see text for details). Associations were analyzed using demographic information concerning this second
Pearson correlations. All correlations were significantly different
from zero (P < 0.01). FSE = function self-efficacy; OSE = other (“replication”) sample. Using the answers from this
symptom self-efficacy. second sample, the 43 items were reanalyzed in a
40 LORIG ET AL

Table 4. Arthritis Self-Efficacy Scale*


Self-efficacy pain subscale
In the following questions, we'd like to know how your arthritis pain affects you. For each of the following questions, please circle the
number which corresponds to your certainty that you can now perform the following tasks.
1. How certain are you that you can decrease your pain quite a bit?
2. How certain are you that you can continue most of your daily activities'?
3. How certain are you that you can keep arthritis pain from interfering with your sleep?
4. How certain are you that you can make a small-to-moderate reduction in your arthritis pain by using methods other than taking
extra medication?
5 . How certain are you that you can make a &gg reduction in your arthritis pain by using methods other than taking extra
medication'?

Self-efficacy function subscale


We would like Lo know how confident you are in performing certain daily activities. For each of the following questions, please circle
the number which corresponds to your certainty that you can perform the tasks as of now, without assistive devices or help from
another person. Please consider what you routinely can do, not what would require a single extraordinary effort.
AS OF NOW, HOW CERTAIN ARE YOUTHAT YOU CAN:
I . Walk 100 feet on flat ground in 20 seconds?
2. Walk 10 steps downstairs in 7 seconds?
3. Get out of an armless chair quickly, without using your hands for support'!
4. Button and unbutton 3 medium-size buttons in a row in 12 seconds'!
5 . Cut 2 bite-size pieces of meat with a knife and fork in 8 seconds?
6 . Turn an outdoor faucet all the way on and all the way off?
7. Scratch your upper back with both your right and left hands?
8. Get in and out of the passenger side of a car without assistance from another person and without physical aids?
9. Put on a long-sleeve front-opening shirt or blouse (without buttoning) in 8 seconds'!

Self-efficacy other symptoms subscale


In the following questions, we'd like to know how you feel about your ability to control your arthritis. For each of the following
questions, please circle the number which corresponds to the certainty that you can now perform the following activities or tasks.
1 . How certain are you that you can control your fatigue?
2. How certain are you that you can regulate your activity so as to be active without aggravating your arthritis?
3. How certain are you that you can do something to help yourself feel better if you are feeling blue'?
4. As compared with other people with arthritis like yours, how certain are you that you can manage arthritis pain during your
daily activities?
5. How certain are you that you can manage your arthritis symptoms so that you can do the things you enjoy doing?
6. How certain are you that you can deal with the frustration of arthritis?

Each subscale is scored separately, by taking the mean of the subscale items. If one-fourth or less of the data are missing, the score is a mean
of the completed data. If more than one-fourth of the data are missing, no score is calculated. (The authors invite others to use the scale and
would appreciate being informed of study results.)

second factor analysis of baseline data. This time, 3 Confirmatory subscales analysis. The 3 sub-
SE subscales, with a total of 20 items, were identified: scales, as derived from the replication sample, were
an FSE scale of 9 items, an OSE scale of 6 items, and then applied to data from the initial 97-person sample
a pain-management self-efficacy scale (PSE) of 5 items used for development of the instrument. Confirmatory
(Table 4). Item loadings ranged from 0.55 to 0.84 on factor analysis identified item loadings ranging from
the FSE factor, from 0.63 to 0.81 on the OSE factor, 0.59 to 0.75 for the FSE factor, from 0.55 to 0.90 for
and from 0.48 to 0.75 on the PSE factor. Eigenvalues the OSE factor, and from 0.45 to 0.82 for the PSE
were 4.47, 3.61, and 2.11 for FSE, OSE, and PSE, factor (Table 6). Eigenvalues were 4.60 for FSE, 3.61
respectively. Coefficient alpha estimates of internal for OSE, and 2.11 for PSE. Coefficient alpha estimates
reliability were 0.89 for FSE, 0.87 for OSE, and 0.76 of internal reliability were 0.90 for FSE, 0.87 for OSE,
for PSE. The 3 subscales acted as self-efficacy theory and 0.75 for PSE.
predicts, with SE being correlated with present and Test-retest reliability study. Because self-
future health status (Table 5 ) . efficacy is a changeable psychological state, rather
SELF-EFFICACY SCALE 41

Table 5 . Associations between self-efficacy (SE) and health ous participants in the ASMC. Characteristics of this
status (HS) in replication sample* third sample are also presented in Table 2, under the
Pain Disability Depression heading of “reliability sample.”
Baseline S E with baseline HS All subjects completed the 3 SE subscales twice
FSE -0.29 -0.76 -0.16 (mean time between completions 9.4 days, range 2-29
OSE -0.27 -0.25 -0.44
PSE -0.29 -0.21 -0.33
days). Utilizing Pearson correlations, item reliabilities
Baseline S E with 4-month HS ranged from 0.71 to 0.85. Subscale reliabilities were
FSE -0.28 -0.71 -0.21 0.85 for FSE, 0.90 for OSE, and 0.87 for PSE.
OSE -0.36 -0.21 -0.41
PSE -0.26 -0.24 -0.31 Tests of hypotheses using the SE scale. The
4-month S E with 4-month HS original reason for designing an arthritis self-efficacy
FSE -0.30 -0.7 I -0.30 instrument was to test 3 hypotheses: first, that there is
OSE -0.47 -0.21 -0.59
PSE -0.39 -0.21 -0.45 an association between perceived SE and both present
and future health status related to arthritis; second,
* Data were combined from a control group (n = 49) and an
experimental group (n = 95) who took the Arthritis Self- that SE can be changed by educational interventions;
Management Course (see text for details). Associations analyzed and third, that growth in SE is associated with im-
using Pearson correlations. All correlations were significantly dif- provement in health status. Our observations (Tables 3
ferent from zero (P < 0.01). FSE = function self-efficacy; OSE =
other symptom self-efficacy; PSE = pain self-efficacy. and 5) are consistent with the first hypothesis.
Outcome data about the experimental and con-
trol groups from the replication sample are given in
than a more permanent personality trait, one would Table 7. Levels of pain and depression declined sig-
expect some variance in the responses over time. To nificantly from baseline values, whereas perceived SE
test the reliability of the subscales, a third (new) for pain and for other symptoms rose significantly
sample of 91 subjects was selected from among previ- from baseline levels for the experimental group but not

Table 6 . Item factor loadings and itemitotal correlations


Two-factor scale Three-factor scale It e mitotal
correlation,
Initial Replication Initial Replication initial
Item no. sample sample sample sample sample
Function 1 0.74* 0.55* 0.75* 0.55* 0.56
Function 2 0.67 0.63 0.69 0.63 0.64
Function 3 0.61 0.66 0.62 0.66 0.69
Function 4 0.75 0.76 0.75 0.76 0.69
Function 5 0.76 0.84 0.75 0.84 0.76
Function 6 0.76 0.69 0.75 0.69 0.63
Function 7 0.59 0.68 0.59 0.68 0.59
Function 8 0.69 0.59 0.69 0.59 0.58
Function 9 0.71 0.72 0.71 0.72 0.65

Other 1 0.571‘ 0.66’F 0.57t 0.63t 0.60


Other 2 0.70 0.78 0.70 0.78 0.64
Other 3 0.54 0.70 0.55 0.7 I 0.61
Other 4 0.89 0.70 0.90 0.74 0.79
Other 5 0.86 0.79 0.87 0.81 0.79
Other 6 0.63 0.76 0.64 0.80 0.55

Pain 1 0.58 0.57 0.598 0.618 0.34


Pain 2 0.79 0.48 0.82 0.48 0.58
Pain 3 0.44 0.56 0.45 0.71 0.56
Pain 4 0.48 0.62 0.49 0.75 033
Pain 5 0.61 0.58 0.62 0.61 0.60
~ ~ ~

* These sections describe the self-efficacy function subscale.


t The following 11 items describe the self-efficacy painiself-efficacyother symptoms subscale (combined).
f These sections describe the self-efficacy other symptoms subscale (separate).
I These sections describe the self-efficacy pain subscale (separate).
42 LORIG ET AL

Table 7. Arthritis Self-Management Course outcomes in the replication sample: baseline values and 4-inonth changes“
Baseline scores Four-month change scores
Experimental Control group Experimental Control group
group (n = 95) (n = 49) group (n = 95) (n = 49) t
-
Health status
Pain (0-10 scale) 5.21 2 2.36 5.61 2 2.0 -1.31 -+ 2.42t -0.33 t 1.75 2.51$
Disability (0-3 scale) 0.72 ? 0.63 0.91 t 0.65 -0.02 -t 0.38 -0.05 t 0.35 -0.68
Depression (0-39 scale) 4.32 2 3.91 4.84 ? 3.72 -0.76 2 2.45s 0.47 2 3.31 2.52$
Self-efficacy
Function (10-100 scale) 73.27 2 20.22 67.94 2 22.45 0.76 2 14.62 0.00 -c 12.81 0.32
Other symptoms ( l & l O O 55.62 2 21.64 49.15 ? 20.56 5.85 i 19.219 2.87 2 13.29 0.98
scale)
Pain (10-100 scale) 52.04 ? 21.14 48.20 ? 17.88 7.10 -t 19.975 2.52 2 15.97 1.41

* Values are the mean k SD. t values calculated by Student’s 2-tailed, 2-sample f-test comparing change scores of experimental and control
groups. The 2-tailed, paired t-test was used to compare baseline with 4-month change scores for experimental and control groups.
i P < 0.01 versus baseline.
$ P < 0.05, experimental versus control change scores.
0 P < 0.05 versus baseline.

for the control group. The change scores for pain and Experience with the Arthritis Self-Management
depression were significantly different between the 2 Course prompted our formal effort to create an instru-
groups, but those for S E were not. The correlations of ment with which to measure an individual’s perceived
change scores for FSE, OSE, and PSE with disability, self-efficacy to cope with and/or ameliorate the conse-
depression, and pain were, respectively, -0.32, quences of chronic arthritis. We used standard meth-
-0.29, and -0.41 in the experimental group and ods for designing and testing this questionnaire, based
-0.20, 0.07, and 0.08 in the control group. Thus, the on specific issues that are important to people who
health outcome improvement values were similar to have arthritis. The initial instrument was derived from
those found in prior studies (1,2), SE increased during one sample, refined with another sample, and then the
the ASMC, although not to a statistically significant refined instrument was retested with the original sam-
level, and the improvements in health outcomes and ple. This process yielded an instrument with 3 sub-
SE were correlated in the experimental group. These scales and a total of 20 questions, which met reason-
observations are consistent with the second and third able standards for construct and concurrent validity
hypotheses. and test-retest reliability.
This consistency serves as evidence of con- When used to evaluate the 2 groups of the
struct validity of the instrument. However, the data replication sample (those who took the ASMC and
were obtained from the sample used to develop the those who did not), the instrument revealed that there
instrument, and participants from courses that did not was significant growth in self-efficacy among subjects
emphasize enhancement of SE were included. There- in the ASMC group, and a high association between
fore, inferences concerning the validity of the hypoth- the increase in perceived SE and the decline in pain.
eses must remain tentative until independent samples Thus, the instrument performed well during its devel-
are tested and appropriate interventions are used. opment and in a preliminary test, discriminating pa-
tients who received educational intervention from
patients in the control group.
DISCUSSION The design and performance of these scales
It is widely recognized that many of the at- raise a number of issues. First, do the scales actually
tributes of patients and their circumstances (e.g., measure perceived self-efficacy for behavior, or do
biological, psychological, sociological characteristics) they measure outcome, or some combination of the
influence the outcome of illness, especially chronic two? The interrelationships between perceived SE for
illness. However, the mechanisms and relative contri- behavior and expectations of outcomes from the be-
butions of each attribute are poorly understood. One havior are a highly disputed issue in psychology. The
obstacle to understanding has been the absence of controversy can be illustrated in this case by the
instruments for measuring many of the attributes. question “How certain are you that you can decrease
SELF-EFFICACY SCALE 43

your pain quite a bit?” In this case, the behavior is ing, and reinterpretation of physiological symptoms)
represented by the word “decrease,” and the outcome over a short term to educate a group of patients with
is represented by a pain level. It would be difficult to significant rheumatoid arthritis, and we have observed
distinguish absolutely a person’s perceived SE to even greater gains in perceived SE and larger correla-
decrease pain (by whatever means) from some expec- tions with health outcomes (6). It is thus the task of
tation about ultimate levels of pain, and although the further study to identify the operational components of
distinction is clearly important in psychological the- efficacy in this setting, their relative importance to a
ory, we do not believe it is central to our purposes. favorable outcome, ways in which they can be altered,
Our goal is to reduce the patient’s level of pain and possibly, the relationship between perceptions of
in order to improve the patient’s level of well-being efficacy for behavior and expectations for outcome.
and activity potential. We therefore seek to identify The foregoing discussion bears on a second
those elements of health education that contribute to issue concerning the interpretation of scale results.
this goal. At the outset of these studies, we anticipated Could the strength of the correlations between per-
a straightforward sequential relationship, progressing ceived SE and health outcomes be the result of a
from intervention, through behavior change (e.g., spe- similarity between efficacy and outcome expectations?
cific exercise, use of relaxation, and general activity), That is, are we measuring very similar things and,
to reduced pain and depression, accompanied by in- hence, finding them to be correlated? Again, until we
creasing physical capability. The associations actually can identify the behaviors about which the individuals
observed, however, did not sustain a strong sequential perceive themselves to be efficacious, we cannot disen-
relationship. Rather, the health outcomes correlated tangle their independent effects. Yet, without conscious
much more strongly with a perceived ability “to do attention to this concept in the ASMC, perceived SE
things” (perceived self-efficacy) that would yield the was enhanced and outcomes were improved, and
desired outcomes. Although the patients’ perceived changes in SE were correlated with changes in health
ability “to do things” grew during the course, the status. Perhaps SE grew because of the course’s
“things” themselves were apparently unspecified in general emphasis on self-management or because of
the course or were not the principal behaviors that similarities between some of the course content and
were taught. So, to return to the issue of the inter- efficacy-enhancing techniques. Whatever the ultimate
relationship between a perceived ability to decrease explanation of the results and the correlations, it is
pain and the expected outcome of a lower level of clear that desired events occurred and that under-
pain, the interrelationships can be pursued meaning- standing and strengthening those events is the appro-
fully only when the “things” that can be done to priate focus for future studies. We do not know
diminish pain are known. We are currently attempting whether the results will fit neatly into present concepts
to identify those “things.” of SE or will require others.
What are the “things” that the patients might An interesting related issue concerns the rea-
do to decrease their pain or depression level? The sons SE and outcomes improved for pain and depres-
principal behaviors that were taught in the ASMC sion but not for specific measures of physical function.
probably play a role, but the role appears to be small The data do not indicate an answer, but we surmise
even as an aggregate, given the weak associations with that physical capability was probably relatively fixed
outcomes and the low degree of outcome variance that over the time of the study. The ASMC primarily
they explain. Other possible behaviors are seeking or influenced the ways people perceived their physical
using social support in spite of handicap; reducing fear disease and used their existing physical capabilities;
about an activity causing physical harm; learning that is, the course improved patients’ adjustments to
useful techniques from discussions with others in the their situations. The absence of change in both the
class (e.g., cognitive distraction, acupressure, massage perceived SE for physical function and the measured
and/or counter-stimulation); developing more realistic physical function suggests that the individuals knew
expectations concerning the prognosis; and develop- their physical limits and that their outcome expecta-
ing a greater capacity to tolerate discomfort. Pres- tions in this domain influenced their SE perceptions.
ently, these ideas are only speculative. However, in a A third issue is whether perceived SE is a
small preliminary study, we have used conventional mediator of the outcomes, and if it is a mediator, what
efficacy-enhancing methods (e.g., individual goals. is the magnitude of its effect. At best, imperfect
specific instruction and practice, contracting, model- answers can be inferred. Perceived SE correlated with
44 LORIG ET AL

outcomes both before and after the course, and as REFERENCES


outcomes improved during the course, perceived SE
1 . Lorig K, Lubeck D, Kraines RG, Seleznick M, Holman
grew. Thus, the data reveal associations that are
HR: Outcomes of self-help education for patients with
compatible with SE causing outcomes, outcomes arthritis. Arthritis Rheum 28:68&685, 1985
causing SE, an interaction both ways, or a third factor 2. Lorig K, Seleznick M, Lubeck D, Ung E, Chastain RL.
causing both. Clarification of the relationship depends Holman HR: The beneficial outcomes of the arthritis
upon identifying clearly behaviors which are affected self-management course are not adequately explained by
by perceived SE and which are distinct from the behavior change. Arthritis Rheum 32:91-95, 1989
outcome variables. The results of the aforementioned 3. Lenker SL, Lorig K , Gallagher D: Reasons for the lack
preliminary study using deliberate SE-enhancement of association between changes in health behavior and
techniques are compatible with a causal sequence improved health status: an explanatory study. Patient
from efficacy to outcome. If perceived SE, a synonym Educ Counsel 6:69-72, 1984
for which might be “confidence,” were the mediator, 4. Bandura A: Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of
it could achieve both its magnitude and its effects as a behavior change. Psychol Rev 84: 191-215, 1977
result of a combination of experience and new, specific 5. Bandura A: Social Foundations of Thought and Action.
learning activities. Judging from our prior study ( 2 ) , if Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice-Hall, 1986
6. O’Leary A, Shoor S , Lorig K , Holman HR: A cognitive-
the magnitude of the SE effect were causal with regard
behavioral treatment for rheumatoid arthritis. Health
to pain, it would explain about 30% of the outcome
Psychol (in press)
variance in pain scores. 7. Dixon JS, Bird HA: Reproducibility along a 10-cm
With regard to the performance of the instru- vertical visual analogue scale. Ann Rheum Dis 40237-89,
ment, it is highly improbable that the 3 categories of 1981
either efficacy or health outcomes are independent of 8. Downie WW, Leatham PA, Rhind VM, Wright V,
each other. One can readily imagine the ways in which Branco JA, Anderson JA: Studies with pain rating
physical function, pain, and depression interact. This scales. Ann Rheum Dis 37:378-381, 1978
interaction would help explain our observations of 9. Fries JF, Spitz P, Kraines RG, Holman HR: Measure-
significant correlations between each efficacy category ment of patient outcome in arthritis. Arthritis Rheum 23:
and all outcome variables. In this setting, the choice 137-145, 1980
between 2 or 3 instrument subscales was largely 10. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M , Mock J. Erbaugh J:
arbitrary. Because pain is such a major concern of An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psy-
arthritis patients, and because of the superior perfor- chiatry 4561-571, 1961
mance of the “other” category in relation to depres- 11. Decker JL: Summary of the Conference on Outcome
sion, we prefer the instrument with 3 subscales. Measures in Rheumatological Clinical Trials, Hamilton,
Testing the performance of the instrument with Ontario, Canada, December 7-8, 1981. J Rheumatol
other groups of patients in other settings is clearly 9:802-806, 1982
essential, as is comparing the instrument results with 12. Shoor SM, Holman HR: Development of an instrument
to explore psychological mediators of outcome in
those that measure related psychological states, such
chronic arthritis patients. Trans Assoc Am Physicians
as general coping skills (15), learned helplessness (16), 97~32.5-331, 1985
locus of control (17), and sense of coherence (18). 13. Lawly DN, Maxwell AE: Factor Analysis as a Statistical
However, the results thus far encourage the view that Method. New York, Macmillan, 1971
an arthritis-specific instrument that reveals important 14. Nunnally JC: Psychometric Theory. Second edition.
psychological determinants of health outcomes for New York, McGraw-Hill, 1978
persons with chronic arthritis can be constructed. Use 15. Lazarus RS, Folkman S: Stress Appraisal and Coping.
of such an instrument might help to disentangle and New York, Springer, 1984
illuminate the multiple factors that influence those 16. Seligman M: Helplessness: On Depression, Develop-
outcomes. ment, and Death. San Francisco, WH Freeman, 1975
17. Wallston BS, Wallston KA, Kaplan GD, Maides SA:
Development and validation of the health locus of con-
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS trol (HLC) scale. J Consult Clin Psychol 44:58&585,
The authors acknowledge the extensive, expert sta- I976
tistical assistance and contributions of Byron W. Brown, Jr., 18. Antonovsky A: Health Stress and Coping. San Fran-
PhD and Daniel A. Bloch, PhD. cisco, Jossey-Bass, 1980

You might also like