You are on page 1of 23

Psychology of Consciousness:

Theory, Research, and Practice


© 2018 American Psychological Association 2019, Vol. 6, No. 1, 99 –121
2326-5523/19/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cns0000165

A Survey Exploring Synesthetic Experiences: Exceptional


Experiences, Schizotypy, and Psychological Well-Being
Christine A. Simmonds-Moore Carlos S. Alvarado
University of West Georgia Parapsychology Foundation, New York, New York

Nancy L. Zingrone
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Northcentral University
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

We used an online survey to investigate the relations among synesthesia, schizotypy,


exceptional experiences (ExEs), and well-being. Participants (N ⫽ 1,628 [listwise N ⫽
767]; male ⫽ 619, female ⫽ 1,064) completed a Synesthesia Experience Questionnaire
(SEQ), a general question about synesthetic experiences (Hartmann, 1991), the Anomalous
Experience Subscale (AES) of the Anomalous Experience Inventory (Gallagher, Kumar, &
Pekala, 1994), questions about parapsychological experiences, a multidimensional measure
of schizotypy (Oxford-Liverpool Inventory for Feelings and Experiences Short [O-LIFE
Short]; Mason, Linney, & Claridge, 2005), and the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS;
Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985). Cronbach’s alphas for these measures ranged
from .63 to .90 in our data set. Approximately half (54.4%) of the sample reported 1 or more
synesthetic experiences, although the rate was much lower for synesthesias experienced on
a consistent basis (3.1%). The SEQ was highly internally reliable and correlated positively
with the AES, number of parapsychological experiences, and unusual experiences, and
negatively with introvertive anhedonia. The SEQ was not directly related to the SWLS.
Unusual experiences and synesthesia were the strongest predictors of the AES and para-
psychological experiences in multiple regression models. A cluster analysis of schizotypy
found 4 clusters of schizotypy, including 1 cluster reflecting healthy schizotypy, 1 reflecting
high schizotypy, 1 reflecting low schizotypy, and 1 reflecting negative schizotypy. We
compared clusters in terms of the SWLS, ExEs, and the SEQ and found significant
differences for all variables and higher scores for healthy compared with high schizotypy
on all variables. We discuss the complex relations among synesthesia, schizotypy, and
well-being.

Keywords: synesthesia, positive schizotypy, exceptional experiences, cluster analysis,


satisfaction with life

Synesthesia occurs when a given stimulus Hubbard, 2013). Synesthetic experiences span a
produces an additional sensory or conceptual range of experiences and are comprised of an
response (Marks & Mulvenna, 2013; Simner & inducer, such as a word, the usual sensory re-
sponse, and a concurrent experience in a differ-
ent modality, such as a color or a physical
sensation (Grossenbacher & Lovelace, 2001).
This article was published Online First September 17, For example, in response to the inducing stim-
2018. ulus of spice in a curry, a person may experi-
Christine A. Simmonds-Moore, Department of Psychology, ence a visual color in addition to the sensations
University of West Georgia; Carlos S. Alvarado, Parapsychol-
ogy Foundation, New York, New York; Nancy L. Zingrone,
associated with spicy food. Synesthetic concur-
Department of Psychology, Northcentral University. rents are experienced in a variety of ways: as
The authors would like to acknowledge the Bial Founda- projected out into the external world, experi-
tion for funding this research project. enced in the mind’s eye (Simner & Hubbard,
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed
to Christine A. Simmonds-Moore, Department of Psychology,
2013), and sometimes in more complex ways
University of West Georgia, 1601 Maple Street, Carrollton, (Alvaraz & Robertson, 2013; Eagleman, 2012).
GA 30117. E-mail: csimmond@westga.edu Most research has explored grapheme-color
99
100 SIMMONDS-MOORE, ALVARADO, AND ZINGRONE

synesthesia, which describes the experience of a encing it more frequently or consistently (e.g.,
color or colors (a concurrent) in response to Martino & Marks, 2001). Other theorists (e.g.,
hearing or reading a letter or number (Brang & Deroy & Spence, 2013) have argued that syn-
Ramachandran, 2010). However, there are esthesia should only be defined as such when it
many other types of synesthesia, which vary in occurs automatically and consistently in re-
prevalence (Marks, 2014). For example, another sponse to an inducing stimulus (cf. Marks &
commonly experienced type of synesthesia is Mulvenna, 2013; Simner & Hubbard, 2013).
time-space synesthesia, which describes the ex- Although much research focuses on so-called
perience of time as organized spatially (Mann, strong synesthesia, empirical research supports
Korzenko, Carriere, & Dixon, 2009). In addi- a spectrum of synesthetic experiences (Eagle-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tion, mirror-touch1 synesthesia, in which indi- man, 2012; Marks, 2014). Still other approaches
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

viduals feel they are being touched when they conceptualize synesthesia as a family tree of
see someone else being touched (Banissy, Co- synesthesias with categories and subcategories
hen Kadosh, Maus, Walsh, & Ward, 2009), is of related but different phenomena (Marks,
also relatively common among synesthetes. 2014). Novich, Cheng, and Eagleman’s (2011)
Less common synesthesias include those asso- cluster analytic work lends some support to the
ciated with taste, smell, and pain (Cytowic, idea of different synesthesias.
1995). We adopt a broad approach to synesthetic ex-
A subset of individuals report the experience of periences and contend that either a monist or plu-
synesthesia for as long as they can remember ral approach is potentially valid. We generally
(Simner & Hubbard, 2013). Simner et al. (2006) align with a pluralistic approach—which ac-
found a prevalence rate of lifelong synesthesia to knowledges a general category of synesthesias—
be 4.4% in an unselected sample, but the esti- that can encompass both strong experiences in
mated prevalence rate varies with the sample stud- addition to a continuum of synesthetic experi-
ied and the definition of synesthesia (Marks, ences. Diverse ways of experiencing synesthesia
2014). For individuals who experience lifelong may reflect a family tree of related phenomena,
synesthesia, experiences occur consciously and with different etiologies.
explicitly in response to everyday stimuli (Simner Herein we develop and employ a new scale to
& Hubbard, 2013). Such experiences are often assess synesthesia. The Synesthesia subscale of
labeled as strong or developmental synesthesia, as the Tellegen Absorption Scale (Tellegen & At-
they are typically congenital. These experiences kinson, 1974) is often used to examine synes-
tend to be consistent and automatic in nature. In thesia in unselected samples; however, it taps
addition, most synesthetic experiences are unidi- only weak forms of synesthesia (Terhune, 2009)
rectional in terms of the stimulus and response, and not the full range of synesthesias. We seek
with a few exceptions in which the concurrent to explore how the synesthesias are distributed
experience can also lead to experiences associated in a self-selected sample and examine a number
with the inducer2 (Brang & Ramachandran, of potential correlates of synesthesia, including
2011). However, synesthesia can also be “ac- exceptional experiences, positive schizotypy,
quired” following brain damage and induced dur- and psychological well-being.
ing altered states of consciousness (Grossenbacher
& Lovelace, 2001), including meditation (Walsh,
2005), hypnagogia (Sagiv, Ilbeigi, & Ben-Tal, Exceptional Experiences and Synesthesia
2011), hypnosis (Cardeña, 2005), and the inges-
More specifically, we examine how synes-
tion of psychoactive substances (Luke & Terhune,
thetic experiences and positive schizotypy cor-
2013). Some synesthetic experiences have oc-
relate with several human experiences that oc-
curred after training to produce such experiences
(Marks, 2014; Rothen & Meier, 2014).
Marks (2014) recently described synesthesia 1
Some, however, have challenged the inclusion of mir-
as a “teeming multiplicity,” reflecting the vari- ror-touch synesthesia as a true synesthesia because it does
ety of ways in which it can arise and be expe- not share all of the features of other synesthesias (Marks &
Mulvenna, 2013; Rothen & Meier, 2013).
rienced. Some theorists contend that synesthesia 2
In addition, there is some evidence for unconscious
varies on a continuum, with some people expe- bidirectionality in synesthetic experiences (Knoch, Gianotti,
riencing it in weaker forms and others experi- Mohr, & Brugger, 2005).
SYNESTHETIC EXPERIENCES 101

cur infrequently or deviate from ordinary 2009). Aura vision, for example, might result
experiences. Such anomalous or exceptional ex- from information about another person, which
periences (ExEs), such as out-of-body experi- is experienced as energy, light, or colors (Zin-
ences and extrasensory perception (ESP), can grone et al., 2009). Ramachandran, Miller, Liv-
often challenge the accepted explanations of ingstone, and Brang (2012) found empirical ev-
reality according to the dominant Western par- idence for a systematic transformation of
adigm (Cardeña, Lynn, & Krippner, 2014), and emotions as a colorful aura, which the synes-
are frequently labeled as paranormal (Belz & thete “TK” experienced as projected around an-
Fach, 2012). Rhea White (Brown, 2000) first other person’s face and body. However, Milán
employed the term exceptional experience in et al. (2012) compared the experiences of indi-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

her discussion of exceptional human experi- viduals who claim to be able to read auras and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ences, but more recently, the term has been those with personality-color synesthesia and
employed as an inclusive descriptor for a range concluded that the experiences differ qualita-
of unusual phenomena (Belz & Fach, 2012; tively.
Simmonds-Moore, 2012b), including (see Belz Irwin (1985) has proposed a model of out-of-
& Fach, 2012) (a) anomalies in the experience body experiences (OBEs) that incorporates syn-
of the self (the “self- model”; e.g., internally esthesia, dissociation from the body, and ab-
experienced voices and somatic phenomena that sorbed mental imagery. Synesthetic processes
are felt to derive from an outside/paranormal work to combine the representation of a sepa-
source), (b) anomalies in ways the physical rated self with strong visual imagery, which
world is experienced (the “world-model”; ex- produces a realistic OBE (Irwin, 1985). Re-
ternal and body-based phenomena, including search has supported the proposed relationship
purported psychokinesis and experiences of between synesthesia and OBEs (Terhune,
ghosts), (c) dissociative phenomena (dissocia- 2009), and has identified a potential role for
tion between ways in which one experiences the synesthesia in apparitions and haunting experi-
self and the world models; e.g., mediumistic ences alongside somatic focus and hyperaesthe-
experiences and out-of-body phenomena; sia (Houran, Wiseman, & Thalbourne, 2002).
Cardeña & Alvarado, 2014), and (d) phenom- Accordingly, synesthesia might enable a con-
ena related to perceiving coincidences (coinci- crete experience of somatic and environmen-
dence phenomena; e.g., synchronicities and tally derived stimuli. Moreover, synesthesias
some experiences of ESP). A sizable subset of appear to play a role in representing emotional
the general population reports the full range of information and abstract information as well as
ExEs (e.g., approximately 38% of a randomly auras and precognitive dreams (Simmonds-
selected sample report experiencing ESP; Moore, 2016). In the current study, we attempt
Palmer, 1979). ExEs have been reported across to clarify the relationships among the synesthe-
different cultures and times (Irwin & Watt, sias, different categories of ExEs, and schizo-
2007), are often associated with altered states of typy. We hypothesize positive correlations be-
consciousness (Alvarado, 1998), and can tween a general tendency to experience
emerge spontaneously or as a result of practices synesthesia and ExEs.
intended to alter consciousness (Belz, 2012).
Previous studies have indicated that synes- Schizotypy, ExEs, and Synesthesia
thesia may be implicated in the etiology of
ExEs. Cytowic (1995), for example, observed Few studies have evaluated personality or
that strong synesthetes reported several types of individual difference correlates of synesthesia,
ExEs, including déjà vu, clairvoyance, precog- which may be because of a focus on individual
nition (in dreams), sense of presence, empathic case studies of individuals with strong synes-
healing, and psychokinesis, although these re- thesia rather than on general population trends.
ports do not provide evidence for a causal rela- Schizotypy—a multidimensional construct that
tionship. Other researchers have proposed that reflects personality traits distributed on a con-
synesthesia may actually underpin or play an tinuum in the population—is one such variable
integral role in several types of ExEs (Alvarado, that merits study (Claridge, 1997). Positive
1994; Irwin, 1985; Marwaha & May, 2015; schizotypy is one of four factors comprising the
Williams, 1997; Zingrone, Alvarado, & Agee, schizotypy construct and reflects magical think-
102 SIMMONDS-MOORE, ALVARADO, AND ZINGRONE

ing, hallucinations, and altered perceptual expe- cuted chemically, perhaps via serotonergic
riences. Researchers have reported that positive mechanisms (Brogaard, 2013; Luke & Terhune,
schizotypy is associated with ExEs and unusual 2013). Research on the effects of psilocybin on
beliefs (e.g., Irwin, Dagnall, & Drinkwater, the brain supports a serotonergic mechanism for
2013). The other factors that comprise schizo- hyperconnectivity (Petri et al., 2014).
typy include (a) cognitive disorganization, The positive schizotypal nervous system (as
which reflects attentional difficulties and anxi- we refer to it) is similarly characterized by
ety; (b) negative schizotypy (i.e., introvertive hyperconnectivity (McCreery & Claridge,
anhedonia), which refers to a preference for 1996), which enables rapid shifts in states of
solitude and lack of enjoyment from social consciousness (Fleck et al., 2008) and a “loose”
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

sources; and (c) and impulsive nonconformity, cognitive style that has been associated with
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

some of which relate to manic tendencies (e.g., ExEs (Farias, Claridge, & Lalljee, 2005; Gi-
Loughland & Williams, 1997). Synesthetes anotti, Mohr, Pizzagalli, Lehmann, & Brugger,
score higher than controls on both positive and 2001). Simmonds-Moore (2010) has proposed
disorganized schizotypy but comparably with that an interconnected cognitive-perceptual sys-
controls on negative or impulsive schizotypy tem is a defining feature of proneness toward
(Banissy et al., 2012). Synesthesia also corre- ExEs (anomaly proneness), which can occur as
lates with openness to experiences (Banissy et a trait or a state of consciousness.
al., 2013), empathy (Banissy & Ward, 2007), Synesthesia, schizotypy, and ExEs may also
transliminality (Thalbourne, Houran, Alias, & be facilitated by stronger mental imagery (Sim-
Brugger, 2001), boundary thinness (Hartmann, monds-Moore, 2016), which is experienced
1991), and absorption (Rader & Tellegen, more vividly in positive schizotypes (Oertel et
1987). Positive schizotypy shares variance with al., 2009) and synesthetes (Spiller, Jonas, Sim-
several of these variables, including translimi- ner, & Jansari, 2015). Moreover, mental repre-
nality and absorption (Simmonds-Moore, sentations are stronger and more accessible
2010). among synesthetes (Murray, 2010; Terhune,
Banissy et al. (2012) have proposed that syn- Wudarczyk, Kochuparampil, & Cohen Kadosh,
esthesia may be linked to a broader phenotype 2013), and more available to consciousness
that includes ExEs, creativity, mental imagery, among positive schizotypes (Claridge, Clark, &
and mitempfindung (i.e., “referral of a tactile Davis, 1997) and high scorers on transliminality
sensation to a location far away from the stim- (Crawley, French, & Yesson, 2002). These
ulation site,” Burrack, Knoch, & Brugger, 2006, traits may contribute to ExEs via the ability to
p. 151). The relationship between synesthesia mentally represent subtle and abstract aspects of
and schizotypy could either be explained by a human experience that usually fall outside of
“co morbidity between synaesthesia and other conscious awareness.3
cognitive traits that are related to schizotypy” or Our study further explores relationships be-
via “similarities in the underlying mechanisms tween synesthesia and schizotypy. We hypoth-
that give rise to the perceptual reports associ- esize that positive schizotypy will correlate pos-
ated with schizotypy and synaesthesia” (p. itively with a general tendency to experience
1086). Two such mechanisms are (a) cortical synesthesia and that positive schizotypy will
hyperconnectivity, which is an enhanced ten- correlate positively with ExEs. We also explore
dency for cortical neurons to interconnect; and the correlations between measures of schizo-
(b) a tendency to experience very vivid mental typy and both synesthesia and ExEs.
imagery and strong mental representations. In
terms of the former, transliminality and synes- Mental Health and Well-Being
thesia are both underpinned by increased neural
interconnectedness (Thalbourne et al., 2001). Researchers have associated ExEs with psycho-
Synesthesia is associated with both structural pathology (Bradbury, Stirling, Cavill, & Parker,
(i.e., increased connections among cortical ar- 2009; Nettle, 2006) as well as with good mental
eas) and functional hyperconnectivity (Mitchell,
2013; Rouw, 2013), which emerges from re- 3
Examples might include somatic information and usu-
duced neural inhibition and excitation (Banissy ally imperceptible information from the environment such
et al., 2012; Nelson & Rawlings, 2010) exe- as infrasound.
SYNESTHETIC EXPERIENCES 103

health (Goulding, 2004, 2005; Goulding & Individuals that exhibit healthy schizotypy
Ödéhn, 2009), finding meaning in life (Kennedy, also exhibit increased creativity and mating suc-
Kanthamani, & Palmer, 1994), and other adaptive cess (Nettle & Clegg, 2006), which implies that
traits, including creativity (Holt, 2012). Similar these traits are also associated with increased
patterns are evident regarding synesthesia, marked evolutionary fitness (i.e., the tendency for a
by associations with both psychopathology (Bar- given trait to be passed on to the next genera-
on-Cohen et al., 2013; Gimmestad, 2012; Glick- tion). Comparing healthy schizotypy with high
sohn, Alon, Perlmutter, & Purisman, 2001; Marks, schizotypy will help to clarify the relationships
2014) and positive mental health (Cytowic, 1995; among synesthesia, ExEs, and mental health.
Rader & Tellegen, 1987). Like ExEs, synesthesia Given that synesthetes score significantly dif-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

is associated with adaptive benefits, including cre- ferent from a control group in terms of cognitive
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ativity (Brang & Ramachandran, 2011). Marks disorganization (Banissy et al., 2012), synes-
(2014) has noted that synesthesia can interact with thetes that exhibit cognitive disorganization
cognition by enhancing perception or engendering might be more prone to poor mental health. In
perceptual interference. Researchers have associ- our study, we do not explicitly examine mental
ated synesthesia with efficient sensory processing health but assess perceptions of well-being. We
and strong encoding of information that facilitates evaluate the hypothesis that a correlation exists
better memory and other cognitive advantages between synesthetic experiences and psycho-
(Brang & Ramachandran, 2011; Terhune et al., logical well-being and that each type of schizo-
2013). Clearly, the relationships among ExEs, typy will correlate with well-being. We predict
synesthesia, and mental health are multifaceted, that participants that conform to a healthy
potentially complicated, and in need of further schizotypy cluster will score higher than partic-
exploration. Kerns, Karcher, Raghavan, and Be- ipants that conform to a high schizotypy cluster
renbaum (2014) have argued that the shared vari- on measures of synesthesia, ExEs, and psycho-
ance between psychopathology and ExEs might logical well-being. We also aim to clarify the
be better understood in terms of a third variable nature of the relationships among schizotypy,
that contributes to both. We consider that this third the synesthesias, different types of ExEs, and
variable might reflect the mechanisms that con- well-being using correlational analyses and
tribute to positive schizotypal traits characterized multiple regression. Our final aim is to explore
by hyperconnectivity, although the hyperconnec- the prevalence and frequency of different types
tivity associated with synesthesia might arise from of synesthesias.
psychopathology among some people (Marks,
2014). Method
Previous schizotypy research has used cluster
analysis to identify individuals who score high on Participants
measures of positive schizotypy. The first type or
cluster is represented by high scores on positive A total of 1,789 people commenced the survey,
schizotypy and all three of the other factors (i.e., but 161 terminated the study before answering any
high schizotypy), whereas the second type is rep- questions. Twenty-eight participants under 18
resented by high scores on positive schizotypy and years of age were redirected out of the survey by
low scores on the other aspects of schizotypy (i.e., the system. Six hundred nineteen males and 1,064
healthy schizotypy). Both types are associated females completed at least part of the survey. The
with elevated levels of ExEs, but social and emo- listwise valid N for the variables in this study was
tional factors (i.e., negative schizotypy) and the 767. For all hypotheses, data analysis was carried
extent to which cognitions are organized (i.e., out using valid Ns per analysis. In terms of re-
cognitive disorganization) play key roles in deter- ported handedness, 1,353 participants were right-
mining whether ExEs are experienced as positive handed, 125 were left-handed, and 137 partici-
or negative. Accordingly, researchers have sug- pants were ambidextrous. Age data were collected
gested that proneness to ExEs is actually neutral in in terms of the following categories: under 18
terms of mental health (Goulding, 2004, 2005; (n ⫽ 28), 18 to 24 (n ⫽ 307), 25 to 30 (n ⫽ 223),
Goulding & Ödéhn, 2009; Schofield & Claridge, 35 to 40 (n ⫽ 286), 45 to 50 (n ⫽ 261), 55 to 60
2007). (n ⫽ 214), 61 to 65 (n ⫽ 106), 66 to 70 (n ⫽ 57),
104 SIMMONDS-MOORE, ALVARADO, AND ZINGRONE

71 to 75 (n ⫽ 27), 76 to 80 (n ⫽ 10), and over 80 tarot card readers, or astrologers, among others.
(n ⫽ 4). The AES has 29 items4 and was originally for-
mulated from the Mental Experience Inventory
Materials (Kumar & Pekala, cited in Gallagher et al.,
1994). The total number of “yes” responses
Synesthesia Experience Questionnaire. A forms the total score. An example item is “I
56-item synesthesia questionnaire (the Synes- have had memories of a past life.” The instru-
thesia Experience Questionnaire; SEQ) was de- ment possesses good internal consistency
veloped from Barnett et al.’s (2008) synesthesia (KR20 reliability value, Kuder–Richardson cal-
screening questionnaire. The SEQ extends the culation for assessment of internal consistency
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Barnett measure to include 54 main types of of a test or scale of .85; Gallagher et al., 1994).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

synesthesia listed by Murray (2010) in addition In the current study, the Cronbach’s alpha was
to time-space and mirror-touch synesthesia. .89.
Each type of synesthesia was presented as a Traditional parapsychological experiences.
personal statement with the synesthetic concur- Five items asked about waking ESP, dream
rent listed first, followed by the inducing stim- ESP, apparitions, OBEs, and auras. Zingrone et
ulus. For example, “I see colors for days of the al. (2009) used these items in previous survey
week” (or other units of time). Respondents investigations. Items indicate how often partic-
indicated the frequency that they experienced ipants experience these phenomena on a 5-point
each type of synesthesia on a Likert scale in- Likert scale (including the responses never,
cluding the responses never, at least once, oc- rarely, occasionally, frequently, most of the
casionally, often, and consistently (range ⫽ time). An example of an item is
0 –224). Next, four open-ended questions asked
about the state of consciousness associated with Have you ever had an experience in which you felt that
‘you’ were located ‘outside of,’ or ‘away from,’ your
synesthetic experiences, whether experiences physical body; that is, that your consciousness, mind,
occur in response to just thinking about the or center of awareness was in a different place than
inducer, any other circumstances that might af- your physical body?
fect the synesthetic experiences, and any synes- Measures of traditional parapsychological expe-
thetic experiences that were not covered by the riences were based on a total score in addition to
questionnaire (see the Appendix for the full list individual scores for each experience. In this
of SEQ items). sample, Cronbach’s alpha was .81.
General question from Hartmann’s (1991)
The Oxford-Liverpool Inventory of Feel-
Boundary Questionnaire. Hartmann’s (1991)
ings and Experiences-Short (O-LIFE Short;
Boundary Questionnaire includes one item that
Mason et al., 2005). The O-LIFE Short con-
asks a general question about synesthesia. The
sists of four subscales—Unusual Experiences,
question is, “I have often had the experience of
Cognitive Disorganization, Introvertive An-
different senses coming together. For example,
hedonia, and Impulsive Nonconformity—
I have felt that I could smell a color, or see a
which correspond to the four major compo-
sound, or hear an odor.” Respondents indicated
nents of schizotypy. The scale includes 43
the frequency they experienced general synes-
total items (12 items, Unusual Experiences
thesia on a Likert scale ranging from 0 to 4 (0 ⫽ subscale; 11 items, Cognitive Disorganiza-
never, 1 ⫽ at least once, 2 ⫽ occasionally, 3 ⫽ tion subscale; 10 items, Introvertive Anhedo-
often, 4 ⫽ consistently). nia subscale; 10 items, Impulsive Nonconfor-
The Anomalous Experiences Subscale of mity subscale). Mason et al. (2005) reported
the Anomalous Experiences Inventory (Gal- alpha values for subscales that ranged from
lagher, Kumar, & Pekala, 1994). The .63 for Impulsive Nonconformity to .80 for
Anomalous Experience Inventory’s Anomalous Unusual Experiences. The subscales each cor-
Experience Subscale (AES) comprises a range related over .90 with the subscales in the
of traditionally parapsychological items in con- original version of the O-LIFE. In the current
junction with a broader spectrum of phenomena study, the Cronbach’s alpha values for the
such as experiences of altered states of con-
sciousness; mystical experiences; encountering
aliens; and visiting fortune tellers, palm readers, 4
Because of an error, we used 28 of the 29 original items.
SYNESTHETIC EXPERIENCES 105

subscales were .79 for Unusual Experiences, were removed from the data set; all calculations
.80 for Cognitive Disorganization, .64 for In- were performed on complete data sets only.
trovertive Anhedonia, and .63 for Impulsive
Nonconformity. Responses to the General Synesthesia Question
The Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS;
Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985). Responses to Hartmann’s (1991) general
The SWLS is a general measure of psycholog- question about synesthesia (using the Likert re-
ical well-being that correlates with indices of sponses never, at least once, occasionally, of-
mental health (Pavot & Diener, 1993). The ten, and consistently) revealed that 3.1% (n ⫽
SWLS is a five-item, 7-point Likert scale (in- 56) of participants reported that they experience
synesthetic phenomena consistently, 8.5% (n ⫽
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

cluding the responses strongly agree, slightly


152) reported the experience often, 19.1% (n ⫽
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

agree, agree, neither agree nor disagree, dis-


agree, slightly disagree, and strongly disagree). 341) reported the experience occasionally,
The scale has good internal reliability and tem- 14.5% (n ⫽ 259) reported at least one synes-
poral reliability scores (Diener et al., 1985). In thetic experience, and 45.6% (n ⫽ 816) reported
addition, the scale demonstrates good conver- never having experienced synesthesia.
gent and discriminant validity (Pavot & Diener,
1993). In the current study, the Cronbach’s al- Frequencies of the Forms of Synesthesias
pha was .90. Measured by the SEQ

Procedure The frequencies and percentages of the various


forms of synesthesia are presented in Table 1.
The study URL (via SurveyMonkey5) was
distributed widely for a 4-week period. The Descriptive Statistics
survey link was sent via e-mail lists and web-
sites to all faculty, staff, and students at the We conducted preliminary analyses to ascer-
University of West Georgia, the mailing lists of tain the internal reliability and factor structure6
the Rhine Research Center the Association for of the new SEQ scale. Cronbach’s alpha was
Research and Enlightenment, and Charles .98, which suggests that a total score provides
Tart’s (i.e., a well-known consciousness and meaningful information. The SEQ correlated
parapsychology researcher) e-mail list. The positively and significantly with the Hartmann
measures were presented in the following order: boundary question (␳ ⫽ .66). Table 2 presents
demographic items, Hartmann’s synesthesia descriptive statistics for the SEQ, the SWLS,
item, the SEQ, the AES, the parapsychological the O-LIFE Short, and both ExE measures.
items, the O-LIFE Short, and the SWLS. The SEQ distribution was negatively skewed,
such that the mean is very low, and there was a
significant proportion of outliers. Stem-and-leaf
Results
plots indicated 32 outliers (scores above 126)
and five very extreme high scorers. We included
Response Patterns
outliers to reflect the true distribution of these
We obtained 372 responses from the Univer- experiences.
sity of West Georgia, 21 responses from the A Kolmogorov–Smirnov normality test
Rhine Research Center (i.e., a well-known cen- found that the distribution was significantly dif-
ter for research on parapsychology), 47 re- ferent from normal (D(1627) ⫽ .1, p ⬍ .0001).
sponses from Atlantic University, five re- The O-LIFE, the SWLS scale, the AES, and the
sponses from the Monroe Institute (i.e., a center
for research on consciousness, spirituality, and 5
The survey may be found at the following URL: https://
parapsychology), and 646 responses labeled as www.surveymonkey.com/s/UWG_Human_Experiences_Survey.
6
“other,” which included a variety of websites, The factor analysis indicated that the scale may consist
blogs, and mailing lists pertaining to interests in of one large factor and seven smaller ones, which aligned
with the main categories of synesthestic concurrents and
ExEs, including Tart’s e-mail list. The data inducers—for example, with colors as a concurrent formed
from participants that failed to complete the one of these factors. The authors are happy to provide the
questionnaire beyond the demographic section details of this analysis to any interested reader.
106 SIMMONDS-MOORE, ALVARADO, AND ZINGRONE

Table 1
Frequency Tables for Types of Synesthesias
Frequency of occurrence
Type of synesthesia Never At least once Occasionally Often Consistently
Written word/color 1,032 (63.9%) 150 (9.3%) 305 (18.9%) 91 (5.6%) 37 (2.3%)
Heard letters/color 1,259 (78.2%) 106 (6.6%) 155 (9.6%) 48 (3.0%) 42 (2.6%)
Read letters/color 1,272 (78.9%) 117 (7.3%) 149 (9.2%) 43 (2.7%) 32 (2.0%)
Heard numbers/color 1,269 (80.6%) 119 (7.6%) 113 (7.2%) 44 (2.8%) 29 (1.8%)
Read numbers/color 1,285 (81.8%) 105 (6.7%) 105 (6.7%) 46 (2.9%) 30 (1.9%)
Sound/color 976 (62.4%) 163 (10.4%) 290 (18.6%) 93 (6.0%) 41 (2.6%)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Heard music/color 774 (49.7%) 171 (11.0%) 356 (22.8%) 174 (11.2%) 83 (5.3%)
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Read music/color 1,351 (86.4%) 78 (5.0%) 96 (6.1%) 20 (1.3%) 19 (1.2%)


Time/color 1,212 (79.8%) 90 (5.9%) 120 (7.9%) 39 (2.6%) 58 (3.8%)
Taste or smell/color 976 (64.5%) 148 (9.8%) 266 (17.6%) 87 (5.8%) 36 (2.4%)
Touch or pain/color 843 (55.8%) 206 (13.6%) 301 (19.9%) 117 (7.7%) 44 (2.9%)
Personality/color 869 (57.5%) 161 (10.6%) 261 (17.3%) 135 (8.9%) 86 (5.7%)
Emotion/color 774 (51.1%) 196 (12.9%) 306 (20.2%) 161 (10.6%) 79 (5.2%)
Temperature/color 971 (66.5%) 147 (10.1%) 217 (14.9%) 89 (6.1%) 36 (2.5%)
Orgasm/color 956 (65.6%) 132 (9.1%) 205 (14.1%) 103 (7.1%) 61 (4.2%)
Vision/sound 813 (55.9%) 194 (13.3%) 252 (17.3%) 145 (10.0%) 50 (3.4%)
Kinetics or movement/sound 913 (63.1%) 163 (11.3%) 219 (15.1%) 111 (7.7%) 42 (2.9%)
Temperature/sound 1,182 (81.5%) 110 (7.6%) 109 (7.5%) 34 (2.3%) 16 (1.1%)
Taste or smell/sound 1,139 (82.4%) 100 (7.2%) 109 (7.9%) 23 (1.7%) 12 (.9%)
Touch/sound 1,038 (75.2%) 133 (9.6%) 142 (10.3%) 49 (3.6%) 18 (1.3%)
Taste/shapes 1,147 (83.5%) 79 (5.8%) 86 (6.3%) 40 (2.9%) 21 (1.5%)
Spoken words/smell 956 (69.7%) 172 (12.5%) 193 (14.1%) 35 (2.6%) 15 (1.1%)
Written words/smell 980 (71.1%) 144 (10.4%) 196 (14.2%) 44 (3.2%) 14 (1.0%)
Emotions/smell 874 (65.8%) 150 (11.3%) 224 (16.9%) 59 (4.4%) 21 (1.6%)
Visual experiences/smell 846 (63.7%) 167 (12.6%) 222 (16.7%) 68 (5.1%) 25 (1.9%)
Temperature/smell 1,014 (76.9%) 113 (8.6%) 123 (9.3%) 45 (3.4%) 24 (1.8%)
Sounds/smell 1,034 (78.5%) 120 (9.1%) 120 (9.1%) 28 (2.1%) 16 (1.2%)
Personalities/smell 1,004 (75.7%) 113 (8.5%) 137 (10.3%) 50 (3.8%) 23 (1.7%)
Letters or numbers/personality 934 (71.7%) 108 (8.3%) 149 (11.4%) 64 (4.9%) 48 (3.7%)
Objects/personality 655 (50.2%) 127 (9.7%) 310 (23.7%) 153 (11.7%) 61 (4.7%)
Letters or numbers/gender 911 (70.2%) 112 (8.6%) 148 (11.4%) 77 (5.9%) 49 (3.8%)
Time/space 908 (70.2%) 99 (7.7%) 102 (7.9%) 75 (5.8%) 110 (8.5%)
Mirror/touch 582 (44.7%) 202 (15.5%) 340 (26.1%) 117 (9.0%) 61 (4.7%)
Emotion/touch 486 (38.1%) 188 (14.7%) 360 (28.2%) 154 (12.1%) 89 (7.0%)
Personality/touch 914 (72.0%) 117 (9.2%) 146 (11.5%) 56 (4.4%) 37 (2.9%)
Sound/touch 871 (68.3%) 146 (11.4%) 169 (13.2%) 60 (4.7%) 30 (2.4%)
Visual experiences/touch 789 (62.3%) 168 (13.3%) 189 (14.9%) 76 (6.0%) 44 (3.5%)
Temperature/touch 912 (71.7%) 118 (9.3%) 143 (11.2%) 64 (5.0%) 35 (2.8%)
Taste/touch 982 (78.8%) 93 (7.5%) 117 (9.4%) 33 (2.6%) 21 (1.7%)
Smell/taste 430 (34.4%) 123 (9.8%) 337 (27.0%) 239 (19.1%) 121 (9.7%)
Emotion/taste 823 (66.2%) 147 (11.8%) 199 (16.0%) 55 (4.4%) 19 (1.5%)
Visual experiences/taste 773 (62.2%) 188 (15.1%) 206 (16.6%) 58 (4.7%) 18 (1.4%)
Touch/taste 962 (76.9%) 114 (9.1%) 129 (10.3%) 36 (2.9%) 10 (.8%)
Words or parts of words/taste 932 (76.4%) 126 (10.3%) 127 (10.4%) 28 (2.3%) 7 (.6%)
Heard music/taste 998 (81.9%) 102 (8.4%) 86 (7.1%) 26 (2.1%) 6 (.5%)
Read music/taste 1,136 (94.0%) 33 (2.7%) 28 (2.3%) 7 (.6%) 4 (.3%)
Sound/taste 949 (78.8%) 127 (10.5%) 104 (8.6%) 18 (1.5%) 6 (.5%)
Temperature-taste 1,008 (83.1%) 101 (8.3%) 85 (7.0%) 10 (.8%) 9 (.7%)
Emotion/temperature 491 (41.1%) 148 (12.4%) 322 (27.0%) 161 (13.5%) 72 (6.0%)
Heard sounds/temperature 812 (68.1%) 135 (11.3%) 166 (13.9%) 54 (4.5%) 25 (2.1%)
Visual experiences/temperature 691 (58.2%) 161 (13.6%) 220 (18.5%) 85 (7.2%) 30 (2.5%)
Smell or taste/temperature 803 (67.7%) 160 (13.5%) 160 (13.5%) 41 (3.5%) 22 (1.9%)
Touch/temperature 589 (49.4%) 139 (11.7%) 252 (21.1%) 134 (11.2%) 78 (6.5%)
Sound/kinetics 638 (56.3%) 135 (11.9%) 224 (19.8%) 95 (8.4%) 42 (3.7%)
Visual experiences/kinetics 632 (56.0%) 49 (13.2%) 214 (19.0%) 87 (7.7%) 47 (4.2%)
SYNESTHETIC EXPERIENCES 107

Table 2 deviations from normality, particularly when


Descriptive Statistics for the SEQ, SWLS, O-LIFE the sample size is large (Howell, 1989, p. 299).
Short, and ExE Measures In terms of demographic data, females scored
Measure (n) M SD Skewness Kurtosis higher on the SEQ (M ⫽ 41.45, SD ⫽ 38.45)
than did males (M ⫽ 28.65, SD ⫽ 34.39),
SEQ (903) 37.75 38.34 1.54 2.88
t(877) ⫽ ⫺4.84, p ⬍ .0001, and their responses
SWLS (1,123) 23.63 7.19 ⫺.64 ⫺.33
AES (1,032) 10.23 6.27 .25 ⫺.91 were more variable. In addition, participants
Parapsychological who reported they were ambidextrous scored
Experiences (1,115) 5.46 4.09 .74 .07 higher on synesthesia (M ⫽ 62.78, SD ⫽ 50.22)
Unusual Experiences compared with right-handed (M ⫽ 34.78, SD ⫽
⫺.91
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(1,095) 5.45 3.17 .11 34.74) or left-handed (M ⫽ 38.97, SD ⫽ 39.52)


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Cognitive Disorganization
(1,101) 4.27 3.09 .39 ⫺.85
participants, F(2, 892) ⫽ 20.34, p ⬍ .0001.
Impulsive Nonconformity There were no significant differences between
(1,100) 2.85 2.10 .67 ⫺.12 the age group categories on the SEQ.
Introvertive Anhedonia
(1,090) 3.20 2.25 .51 ⫺.47 Correlational Analyses
Note. SEQ ⫽ Synesthesia Experience Questionnaire;
SWLS ⫽ Satisfaction with Life Scale; O-LIFE Short ⫽ A correlation matrix was computed between
Schizotypy measures; ExE ⫽ Exceptional experiences; the O-LIFE Short, the SWLS scale, the AES,
AES ⫽ Anomalous Experiences Scale; Parapsychological and the traditional parapsychological items and
experiences ⫽ number of parapsychological experiences.
is presented in Table 3.
A correction for multiple analysis reset the
alpha level to .00625. In terms of ExEs, total
parapsychological experiences were also tested scores on the SEQ correlated positively and
for normality and were all statistically non- significantly with the AES and the total number
normal. Data should ideally be analyzed using of parapsychological items, thus supporting our
nonparametric statistics; however, we opted to hypotheses concerning synesthesia and ExEs.
use parametric statistics for most of the analy- The AES correlated positively and significantly
ses, as there are no parametric alternatives for with traditional parapsychological experiences.
some of the key analyses (factor analysis, re- Moreover, total SEQ scores correlated posi-
gression, cluster analysis). In addition, paramet- tively and significantly with unusual experi-
ric statistics are robust in the face of minor ences, as predicted. For the nondirectional hy-

Table 3
Pearson’s Correlation Matrix Between the SEQ, ExE Variables, O-LIFE Short Subscales, and SWLS
Unusual Introvertive Cognitive Impulsive
Variable (n) AEI Parapsychological experiences anhedonia disorganization nonconformity SWLS SEQ
ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱ
AES (1,032) 1 .82 .63 ⫺.09 ⫺.03 .06 ⫺.08 .51ⴱⴱⴱ
Parapsychological
(1,115) 1 .57ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.06ⴱ ⫺.01 .05 ⫺.03 .56ⴱⴱⴱ
Unusual experiences
(1,095) 1 ⫺.11ⴱⴱ .36ⴱⴱ .38ⴱⴱ ⫺.18ⴱⴱⴱ .43ⴱⴱⴱ
Introvertive anhedonia
(1,090) 1 .39ⴱⴱ .23ⴱⴱ ⫺.30ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.09ⴱⴱⴱ
Cognitive disorganization
(1,101) 1 .57ⴱⴱ ⫺.33ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.01
Impulsive nonconformity
(1,100) 1 ⫺.31ⴱⴱⴱ .08ⴱ
SWLS (1,123) 1 .03
SEQ (903) 1
Note. Valid N ⫽ 767. SEQ ⫽ Synesthesia Experience Questionnaire; ExE ⫽ Exceptional experiences measures; O-LIFE
Short Subscales ⫽ Schizotypy measures; SWLS ⫽ Satisfaction with Life Scale; AES ⫽ Anomalous Experiences Scale;
Parapsychological ⫽ number of parapsychological experiences.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .00625.
108 SIMMONDS-MOORE, ALVARADO, AND ZINGRONE

potheses concerning the other subscales of the perience of auras, after Zingrone et al., 2009;
O-LIFE Short, the only correlation that survived ␳ ⫽ .42, p ⬍ .0001).
the correction for multiple analysis was a neg-
ative correlation with introvertive anhedonia. Consistent Versus Inconsistent Synesthesia
Unusual experiences correlated positively and
significantly with the AES and with the tradi- As a crude assessment of differences between
tional parapsychological experiences. Intro- consistent and inconsistent synesthesia, re-
vertive anhedonia correlated negatively with the sponses to the Hartmann synesthesia item were
AES but not traditional parapsychological ex- recoded to compare consistent experiencers
periences. In contrast to our hypothesis, there with individuals who experienced some synes-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

was no significant relationship between scoring thesia, by combining once, occasionally, and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

on the SEQ and SWLS. Following the correc- often responders together. Mann–Whitney U
tion for multiple analysis, neither measure of tests were run to compare consistent with in-
ExEs correlated with the SWLS. All of the consistent synesthesia on SWLS and the two
schizotypy subscales correlated negatively and measures of ExEs. The Mann–Whitney U test
significantly with the SWLS. was statistically significant for scoring on the
As the responses to the individual questions parapsychological items (z ⫽ ⫺3.668, p ⱕ
about parapsychological experiences were or- .0001). Specifically, consistent synesthetes had
dinal, a Spearman’s rho (␳) correlation matrix an average higher rank of 305.95, whereas in-
was computed between responding to the five consistent synesthetes had an average rank of
individual parapsychological items and the 245.65. The Mann–Whitney U test was also
SEQ to examine how synesthesia relates to statistically significant for scoring on the AES
different types of parapsychological experi- (z ⫽ ⫺3.378, p ⱕ .0001). Specifically, consis-
ence. The Spearman’s correlations are pre- tent synesthetes had an average higher rank of
sented in Table 4. 282.95, whereas inconsistent synesthetes had an
A correction for multiple analysis reset the average rank of 229.75. Finally, the Mann–
alpha level to .0007. In line with our hypothe- Whitney U test was also statistically significant
ses, the SEQ correlated positively and signifi- for scoring on the SWLS (z ⫽ ⫺2.322, p ⱕ
cantly with all traditional parapsychological .020). Specifically, consistent synesthetes had a
items. In addition, all parapsychological items higher average rank of 292, whereas inconsis-
intercorrelated positively and significantly. We tent synesthetes had an average rank of 253.82.
also computed a post hoc Spearman’s correla- Finally, we calculated separate correlations for
tion to explore the relationship between a par- consistent and inconsistent synesthetes on the Un-
ticular type of synesthesia and a corresponding usual Experiences subscale of the O-LIFE Short
ExE (personality-color synesthesia and the ex- and the SEQ. The correlation for consistent syn-

Table 4
Spearman’s Correlation Matrix for SEQ and Five Traditional
Parapsychological Experiences
Parapsychological
experience (N) Dream ESP Waking ESP Apparition OBE Aura SEQ
a a a a
Dream ESP (1,135) 1 .59 .42 .39 .34 .41a
Waking ESP (1,138) 1 .51a .42a .41a .43a
Apparitions (1,137) 1 .49a .49a .42a
OBE (1,132) 1 .48a .35a
Aura (1,133) 1 .39a
SEQ (903) 1
Note. SEQ ⫽ Synesthesia Experience Questionnaire; Dream ESP ⫽ Extrasensory experi-
ences in dreams; Waking ESP ⫽ Extrasensory experiences while awake; Apparitions ⫽
Perception of a figure; OBE ⫽ Out of body experiences; Aura ⫽ Perception of a light
surrounding a person.
a
p ⬍ .007 level (following a correction for multiple analysis).
SYNESTHETIC EXPERIENCES 109

esthetes was .30 (p ⬍ .0001), and the correlation Cluster Analysis of Schizotypy Variables
for inconsistent synesthetes was .37 (p ⬍ .0001).
Psychological well-being was further ex-
Exploring Predictors of ExEs Using plored in terms of schizotypy clusters. Follow-
Multiple Regression ing Goulding and Ödéhn (2009), two comple-
mentary cluster analyses were undertaken on
We employed multiple regression analysis (us- standardized forms of these data. First, we per-
ing the “enter” method) to test whether schizotypy formed a hierarchical cluster analysis using the
subscales, the SEQ, and the SWLS significantly Ward method and defining similarities between
participant profiles by squared Euclidean dis-
predicted the scores on the AES. We found that
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tance. Inspection of the dendrogram plot sug-


the six predictors explained 52% of the variance
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

gested three to four clusters. Following this, we


(R2 ⫽ .52), F(6, 771) ⫽ 144.24, p ⬍ .0001. All performed a second K-means cluster analysis,
variables were significant predictors of scoring on prespecifying a four-cluster solution, given that
the AES: unusual experiences (␤ ⫽ .62), t(771) ⫽ previous cluster analyses have found a four-
20.37 p ⬍ .0001; SEQ (␤ ⫽ .234), t(771) ⫽ 8.24, cluster solution (Goulding, 2005; Loughland &
p ⬍ .0001; cognitive disorganization (␤ ⫽ ⫺.19), Williams, 1997). The four subscales of the
t(771) ⫽ ⫺5.75, p ⬍ .0001; impulsive noncon- O-LIFE were saved as standardized variables
formity (␤ ⫽ ⫺.094), t(771) ⫽ ⫺3.05, p ⬍ 002; for the purposes of the cluster analysis. The
SWLS (␤ ⫽ ⫺.083), t(771) ⫽ ⫺3.08, p ⬍ .002; cluster analyses were undertaken without the
and introvertive anhedonia (␤ ⫽ ⫺.067), t(771) ⫽ inclusion of impulsive nonconformity, follow-
⫺2.45 p ⬍ .002. A stepwise regression analysis ing the precedent set by Goulding (Goulding,
indicated that the model explained 41% of the 2004, 2005; Goulding & Ödéhn, 2009; Lough-
variance, with unusual experiences as the sole land & Williams, 1997). Loughland and Wil-
predictor, which increased to 47% with the addi- liams (1997) have argued that impulsive non-
tion of cognitive disorganization, and 51% with conformity might reflect manic rather than
the addition of synesthesia, 52% with the addition schizotypal tendencies and should not be in-
of impulsive nonconformity, and only a .5% in- cluded in cluster analysis conducted on schizo-
crease with the addition of the SWLS. typy variables. Table 5 provides a summary of
A second multiple regression analysis exam- the cluster analysis.
ined how the schizotypy subscales, synesthesia, Cluster 1 reflects above-average scoring on
and psychological well-being statistically pre- cognitive disorganization, above-average scor-
dicted the scores on traditional parapsychological ing on unusual experiences, and above-average
experiences. Six predictors explained 47.4% of scoring on introvertive anhedonia (we label this
the variance (R2 ⫽ .47), F(6, 803) ⫽ 122.38, p ⬍ cluster high schizotypy). Cluster 2 reflects
above-average scoring on unusual experiences,
.0001. The following variables were significant
with below-average scoring on cognitive disor-
predictors of the traditional Parapsychological Ex-
ganization and introvertive anhedonia (we label
periences scale: unusual experiences (␤ ⫽ .49), this cluster healthy schizotypy). Cluster 3 re-
t(803) ⫽ 15.77, p ⬍ .0001; SEQ (␤ ⫽ .35),
t(803) ⫽ 12.24, p ⬍ .0001; cognitive disorgani-
zation (␤ ⫽ ⫺.13), t(803) ⫽ ⫺3.95, p ⬍ .0001;
and impulsive nonconformity (␤ ⫽ ⫺.094), Table 5
Cluster Analysis for Z Scores of O-LIFE Short
t(803) ⫽ ⫺2.96, p ⬍ .003. Satisfaction with life
Subscales (Four-Cluster Solution)
and introvertive anhedonia did not significantly
predict traditional parapsychological experiences. Cluster number
A stepwise regression analysis indicated that the O-LIFE Short subscale 1 2 3 4
model explained 32% of the variance, with un-
Unusual experiences .87 .66 ⫺.90 ⫺.73
usual experiences as the sole predictor, which Cognitive disorganization 1.20 ⫺.32 ⫺.73 .01
increased to 44% with the addition of synesthesia, Introvertive anhedonia .78 ⫺.60 ⫺.78 .90
47% with the addition of cognitive disorganiza- Note. O-LIFE Short ⫽ Schizotypy scales; Cluster 1 ⫽
tion, and only a .5% increase with the addition of High schizotypy; Cluster 2 ⫽ Healthy schizotypy; Cluster
impulsive nonconformity. 3 ⫽ Low schizotypy; Cluster 4 ⫽ Negative schizotypy.
110 SIMMONDS-MOORE, ALVARADO, AND ZINGRONE

flects below-average scoring on all variables Our hypotheses concerning healthy schizotypy
(we label this cluster low schizotypy). The final were therefore supported.
cluster (Cluster 4) reflects high scoring on in-
trovertive anhedonia, average scoring on cogni- Discussion
tive disorganization, and low scoring on un-
usual experiences (we label this cluster negative We documented a high rate of synesthesia,
schizotypy). depending on how we defined synesthesia.
We compared quality of life, synesthesia, and When synesthesia was judged present when it
ExEs among these cluster groupings. Descrip- occurs “at least once,” 54.3% of participants
tive statistics are presented in Table 6. Partici- reported synesthesia. Because the question re-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

pants in the low schizotypy cluster had the fers to a general description of synesthetic ten-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

highest scores on the SWLS, followed by par- dencies, it may be subject to interpretation.
ticipants in the healthy schizotypy cluster, then However, because it is possible to have a “one
the negative schizotypy cluster, with the lowest off” experience, our finding implies that a com-
score on SWLS for the high schizotypy cluster. plete account of synesthesias should not be re-
For synesthesia, the highest scoring group was stricted to a definition based entirely on a con-
the healthy schizotypy cluster, followed by the sistent response to an inducing stimulus.
high schizotypy cluster, then low schizotypy, Relatedly, Simner (2012) has challenged “con-
and, finally, negative schizotypy. For the AES sistency” as the defining characteristic of syn-
and parapsychological items, the highest score esthesia. Indeed, the occasional synesthetic ex-
was for the healthy schizotypy cluster, followed perience could imply that synesthesia (or
by the high schizotypy group, then the low synesthesia-like experiences) might occur in
schizotypy group, and, finally, the negative certain contexts for some people, consistent
schizotypy group. ANOVAs for differences be- with the idea of state (contextually bound) and
tween the four schizotypy cluster groups found trait anomalous experience proneness, which
that all main effects were statistically signifi- could extend to synesthetic experiences.
cant. There was a significant difference between When we defined general synesthesia in
these groups in terms of satisfaction with life, terms of consistency, 3.1% of the participants
F(3, 1038) p ⬍ .0001, anomalous experiences, reported synesthetic experiences, a statistic
F(3, 830) ⫽ 38.751, p ⬍ .0001, parapsycholog- somewhat lower than the 4.4% reported in a
ical experiences, F(3, 1025) ⫽ 82.43, p ⬍ sample of undergraduate students (Simner et al.,
.0001, and synesthesia, F(3, 965) ⫽ 113.86, p ⬍ 2006). Our finding contributes to the existing
.0001. The hypotheses that schizotypy clusters discussion of congenital synesthesia, as the re-
would differ for each dependent variable were ported frequencies of synesthesias differ across
supported. Tukey’s tests found significant dif- surveys (Marks, 2014). The presence of “at least
ferences between participants in the high com- once” or “occasionally” options in our survey
pared with healthy cluster groups for parapsy- could have encouraged more affirmative en-
chological experiences (p ⱕ .003), anomalous dorsements compared with presentation of only
experiences (p ⬍ .0001), synesthesia (p ⱕ a categorical choice. In addition, the imprecise
.037), and satisfaction with life (p ⬍ .0001). wording of the general question (e.g., specific

Table 6
Descriptive Statistics for Scoring on ExE, Satisfaction With Life, and Synesthesia
Between Clusters of Schizotypy
Cluster of schizotypy AES Parapsychological SWLS SEQ
High schizotypy 12.12 (5.67) 6.35 (3.74) 20.39 (7.67) 43.91 (40.08)
Healthy schizotypy 13.87 (5.66 7.63 (4.10) 24.20 (6.73) 53.92 (42.55)
Low schizotypy 7.80 (5.56) 3.93 (3.46) 26.75 (5.52) 27.90 (30.45)
Negative schizotypy 5.82 (4.48) 3.21 (3.10) 22.22 (7.46) 19.43 (24.30)
Note. ExE ⫽ Exceptional experiences measures; AES ⫽ Anomalous Experiences Scale;
Parapsychological ⫽ number of parapsychological experiences SWLS ⫽ Satisfaction with
Life Scale; SEQ ⫽ Synesthesia Experience Questionnaire.
SYNESTHETIC EXPERIENCES 111

forms of synesthesias were not specified) may similar to previous findings (e.g., personality-
also have encouraged more positive responses. color; Murray, 2010), whereas rates of other
The fact that more than half of our sample experiences differ from previous estimates (cf.
reported synesthesia confirms that synesthetic Simner & Hubbard, 2013). For example, Brang,
experiences (when broadly defined) are not un- Teuscher, Ramachandran, and Coulson (2010)
common. estimated the time-space synesthesia prevalence
The SEQ explored the varieties and frequen- to be 2.2%, which is lower than the level found
cies of synesthesias. As with the responses to in our study, whereas other research indicates a
the general Hartmann question, the SEQ was higher estimate of around 12% (cf. Sagiv &
not normally distributed and exhibited a nega- Ward, 2006). Grapheme- and phoneme-color
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tive skew. Synesthetic experiences appear to synesthesias, which are generally understood to
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

reflect a tendency or set of traits that are dis- be the most common types, were all consis-
tributed on a continuum of frequency of occur- tently reported at around 2% in our sample,
rence. However, the N for participants respond- which corresponds with previous studies (Brang
ing to the items was much lower than the other & Ramachandran, 2011).
items in the questionnaire battery, indicating Some of our findings may reflect ways that
that many respondents may have skipped these our sample differs from a general population
questions, which could have falsely inflated the sample. For example, participants with interests
levels of synesthetic experiences reported in ExEs may experience different rates and
among the overall sample. However, the graded types of synesthesia compared with individuals
nature of synesthetic tendencies identified in with no or little special interest in ExEs, which
this study implies that synesthesias occur on a likely characterizes the general population.
continuum rather than bimodally, which sug- Moreover, some senses are more likely to be
gests a monist or pluralist, rather than a dualist, experienced together; for example, the co-
interpretation of synesthesia. occurrence of taste and smell and temperature
The question concerning the exact nature of and touch may not reflect genuine examples of
the relationships between inconsistent and con- synesthesia, therefore raising questions about
sistent synesthesia remains open. Our post hoc the meaning of frequencies reported regarding
analyses suggest that consistent and inconsis- such types of synesthesia.
tent synesthetes differed in their responses re- If synesthetic experiences or types are de-
garding the main variables in this study, which fined in terms of their phenomenology, as in our
lends support to the notion of a continuum of study, they may well have different etiologies.
similar traits (which may yet have different In addition, different types of synesthesia have
explanations) that vary in their level of expres- different prevalence rates. A parsimonious ap-
sion. proach to understanding the synesthesias is that
they reflect a family tree of phenomena that
Multiple Types of Synesthesia occur on a negatively skewed continuum; as
such, we echo Marks’s (2014) description of
The SEQ was internally reliable and corre- synesthesia as a “teeming multiplicity” rather
lated with the Hartmann item, thereby providing than a unitary phenomenon.
support for the concurrent validity of the latter
measure. Responses to the SEQ support the Synesthesia and Positive Schizotypy
existence of multiple types of synesthesias (af-
ter Marks, 2014). The most commonly reported As predicted, we found a relationship be-
types of synesthesia (i.e., percentage of people tween unusual experiences (a measure of pos-
that experience a type of synesthesia on a con- itive schizotypy) and the SEQ, which corrob-
sistent basis) were taste-smell synesthesia orates Banissy et al.’s (2012) findings and
(9.7%), time-space synesthesia (8.5%), touch- might be explained by the similarities be-
emotion synesthesia (7%), temperature-touch tween attributes of the positive schizotypal
synesthesia (6.5%), personality-color synesthe- nervous system and individuals more likely to
sia (5.7%), music-color synesthesia (5.3%), and experience the world synesthetically (i.e., a
mirror-touch synesthesia (4.7%). Some of these higher SEQ score). Both nervous systems ex-
(congenital) synesthetic experience rates are hibit increased cortical hyperconnectivity
112 SIMMONDS-MOORE, ALVARADO, AND ZINGRONE

(McCreery & Claridge, 1996; Mitchell, 2013; reported ExEs. Our data indicate a marginally
Rouw, 2013), which may facilitate the psy- stronger role for cognitive disorganization in
chological tendencies associated with both explaining the variance for AES than for tradi-
variables. However, contrary to Banissy et al. tional parapsychological experiences. Impulsive
(2012), we did not find an association be- nonconformity, introvertive anhedonia, and sat-
tween synesthesia and cognitive disorganiza- isfaction with life all significantly and indepen-
tion. Instead, we found a negative relationship dently predicted scoring on the AES. Neverthe-
with introvertive anhedonia. These differ- less, their individual contributions to the overall
ences in findings may be because of method- model were small. Impulsive nonconformity
ological differences across studies: We exam- also significantly and independently predicted
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ined synesthesia in a self-selected sample, scoring on parapsychological experiences, but,


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

whereas Banissy et al. contrasted levels of again, its individual contribution was small. Ad-
schizotypy between strong synesthetes and ditionally, and in contrast to the first model,
matched control participants. Differences introvertive anhedonia did not significantly pre-
across studies may also be explained by self- dict parapsychological experiences. These pat-
selection biases, as noted previously. We terns imply that schizotypy might play different
found the relationship between synesthesia roles in different types of ExEs, which warrants
and unusual experiences to be moderate, at attention in future studies.
best; thus, other factors are implicated in the The strongest predictor of ExEs in both mod-
etiology and/or reports of synesthetic experi- els was the unusual experiences measure of
ences. positive schizotypy, which supports the view
that positive schizotypy— but not so much other
Predictors of ExEs types of schizotypy—is associated with greater
proneness to anomalous experiences (anomaly
Schizotypy. Consistent with predictions, proneness). This finding is in line with previous
positive schizotypy (unusual experiences) cor- research and may reflect increased brain con-
related positively with broadly defined ExEs nectivity, tendencies to experience shifts in
(i.e., the AES) and with traditional parapsycho- states of consciousness, a “loose” cognitive
logical experiences. The moderate correlations style, and stronger, more “available” mental
(around .4) suggest that the etiology or concom- imagery characteristic of the positive schizo-
itants of ExEs include additional factors. A gen- typal nervous system (cf. Simmonds-Moore,
eral tendency to endorse different types of 2010).
anomalous experiences could account for the The negative relationships between cognitive
positive relations we found among variables. disorganization and both measures of ExEs in
However, responses to the other schizotypy the regression models could imply that a disor-
subscales do not exhibit similar relationships ganized system is less conducive to ExEs. How-
with synesthesia or ExEs, so the positive asso- ever, given the correlational nature of these
ciations observed are unlikely to be explained relationships, it is not possible to ascertain their
by a general tendency to endorse unusual expe- causal direction. In addition, cognitive disorga-
riences indiscriminately. nization did not correlate with either measure of
The most meaningful and strongest predictor ExEs, although cognitive disorganization may
of ExEs in both multiple regression analyses moderate ExEs in terms of mental health, such
was the Unusual Experiences subscale of the that positive appraisals of ExEs are associated
O-LIFE Short, such that higher scoring on un- with better cognitive organization (Schofield &
usual experiences was associated with reporting Claridge, 2007).
more ExEs. In addition, Unusual Experiences Cognitive disorganization explained more of
contributed most of the variance in both models the variance for AES than traditional parapsy-
of ExEs. The other subscales of the O-LIFE chological experience in our regression models,
Short (Cognitive Disorganization, Impulsive which could be related to our selected sample,
Nonconformity, and Introvertive Anhedonia) as described previously. Although it is possible
were each associated with a tendency for report- that self-selected participants possessed a more
ing fewer ExEs. In both regression models, cog- organized cognitive system for understanding
nitive disorganization independently predicted their ExEs, we found no differences in descrip-
SYNESTHETIC EXPERIENCES 113

tive statistics between our sample and norma- ogy of ExEs. Enhanced conscious access to
tive values for cognitive disorganization (see unusual experiences is consistent with previous
Mason et al., 2005). It is also possible that speculations regarding how synesthesia contrib-
traditional parapsychological experiences are utes to ExEs (Irwin, 1985; Zingrone et al.,
more aligned with an organized system com- 2009), and is also supported by a phenomeno-
pared with other types of ExEs. Future research logical analysis of the experiences of a synes-
could explore how cognitive disorganization thete who also experiences ExEs (Simmonds-
mediates different types of ExEs. Moore, 2016).
Synesthesia. Consistent with our predic- Schizotypy, synesthesia, and ExEs. Although
tions, the SEQ correlated positively and signif- it is not possible to discern causal relationships
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

icantly with the AES and traditional parapsy- among variables, the shared variance between un-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

chological experiences. The SEQ was the usual experiences, synesthesia, and ExEs may be
second strongest contributor to the overall vari- associated with a common neural substrate shared
ance for ExEs in both regression models, after by synesthesia and positive schizotypy, such as
unusual experiences. The SEQ played a stron- hyperconnectivity and the enhanced vividness and
ger role in the regression model for parapsycho- accessibility of mental imagery. Moreover, synes-
logical experiences than it did for AES, which thesia and ExEs can both emerge in altered states
suggests that traits associated with synesthesia of consciousness. In addition, participants who
are important in relation to a variety of ExEs, score high on measures of positive schizotypy
particularly traditional parapsychological expe- shift rapidly among different states of conscious-
riences. Future research could explore the rela- ness (Fleck et al., 2008) and may therefore be
tive contribution of synesthetic experiences to more likely to report both synesthetic experiences
different types of ExEs. and ExEs, related to neural characteristics that
The relationships between synesthesia and enable both. For other individuals, synesthetic ex-
ExEs could be explained by a tendency to en- periences and ExEs might occur contextually via
dorse a wide range of unusual experiences the hyperconnectivity that emerges as a product of
among synesthetes. Alternatively, participants an altered state of consciousness. Therefore, cer-
who score higher on positive schizotypy (un- tain states of consciousness or contexts may stim-
usual experiences) might be more likely to re- ulate a synesthetic potential, which might only
spond positively to both synesthesia and ExE become manifest in the presence of certain altered
items. When we examined how a particular states of consciousness. Context-based experi-
synesthetic experience was associated with a ences suggest an interaction between a hypercon-
related ExE (personality-color synesthesia and nected neural system (characteristic of individuals
aura experiences), the correlation was around high in positive schizotypy) and the influence of
.4, a moderate effect size that implies that (a) serotonergic agents in the etiology of some syn-
the type of synesthesia, albeit related to a type esthetic experiences (Brogaard, 2013; Luke &
of ExE, is only one of several potential vari- Terhune, 2013). Such agents may, in turn, pro-
ables that contributes to the overall experience; mote neural hyperconnectivity that enables synes-
and (b) there may be qualitative differences thetic experiences and ExEs. Still, brain imaging
between the experiences of individuals who studies are necessary to document more defini-
claim to be able to read auras and individuals tively the role of hyperconnectivity.
with personality-color synesthesia (after Milán Responses to the open-ended questions re-
et al., 2012). Certainly, more empirical work is vealed that the synesthesias occur in multiple
needed to determine the contributions of synes- states of awareness—ranging from full wake-
thesia to a variety of ExEs. fulness in strong synesthetes to drug-induced,
Synesthesia is related to a heterogeneous set sleep-related, and meditative states of con-
of experiences, encompassing anomalous per- sciousness in individuals that do not experience
ceptions, experiences, and cognitions, which synesthesia consistently. Post hoc analysis cor-
may all contribute to ExEs (Kerns et al., 2014). relating unusual experiences with the SEQ for
Brang and Ramachandran (2011) note that syn- consistent and inconsistent synesthetes found a
esthesia might be related to an enhancement of correlation of similar strength in both groups,
“sensory and inter-sensory processing” (p. 4), which suggests that positive schizotypy may
which we suggest might contribute to the etiol- contribute to synesthesia similarly for both
114 SIMMONDS-MOORE, ALVARADO, AND ZINGRONE

ways (i.e., consistent vs. inconsistent) of being schizotypy clusters also differed statistically on
synesthetic. Future research might investigate SWLS, such that healthy schizotypy was asso-
different etiologies of synesthetic experiences ciated with greater psychological well-being.
and ExEs among high scorers on positive This finding implies that higher levels of syn-
schizotypy using qualitative methodologies. esthesia and unusual experiences (without cog-
The cluster analysis informs how schizotypy nitive disorganization and introvertive anhedo-
and synesthesia might work together in terms of nia) are associated with good mental health to
ExEs. As in Goulding (2005), our cluster analysis the extent that mental health is associated with
identified four clusters, including two with higher well-being.
scores on the unusual experiences subscale of the Our findings align with previous research that
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

O-LIFE Short— high schizotypy and healthy has tied both healthy positive schizotypy (Nettle,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

schizotypy. Members of the high schizotypy and 2006) and synesthesia (Brang & Ramachandran,
healthy schizotypy clusters both scored higher 2011) to the adaptive traits of creativity, although
than members of the other two clusters on ExEs. creativity has a complicated relationship with
However, participants in the healthy schizotypy well-being and mental health (Holt, 2012). In ad-
cluster also scored significantly higher than did dition, previous work has found evidence for in-
individuals in the high schizotypy cluster on both creased evolutionary fitness among those with
measures of ExEs. Accordingly, unusual experi- traits associated with positive schizotypy, perhaps
ences (as a measure of positive schizotypy) ap- via the association with creativity (Nettle, 2006).
pears to reflect a trait that is generally associated Accordingly, positive schizotypy may also be se-
with an increased tendency for both ExEs and lected, in part, because of the variety of other
synesthesia. When the trait co-occurs with cogni- benefits associated with synesthesia, including en-
tive disorganization, reports of synesthetic experi- hancements of sensory processing, mental repre-
ences and the level of reported ExEs decrease. sentation, and memory (Brang & Ramachandran,
These findings suggest that the otherwise direct 2011). Given the association between psycholog-
relationship between positive schizotypy and both ical well-being and mental health, our results may
synesthesia and ExEs is attenuated by the traits help to explain some of the discrepancies in the
associated with cognitive disorganization, which literature regarding synesthesia and mental health.
include attentional difficulties. In terms of synes- First, there may be healthy and less healthy forms
thesia, Rich and Mattingley (2013) have noted of synesthesia given that healthy schizotypy and
that a stimulus must be “attended to or con- high schizotypy are both associated with synes-
sciously perceived to trigger a concurrent experi- thetic experiences. In addition, higher scores on
ence” (p. 265). The disruption of attentional SWLS for consistent, compared with inconsistent,
mechanisms in a cognitive-perceptual system,
synesthetes suggest that congenital synesthesia is
which might otherwise be prone to synesthesia
potentially associated with better psychological
(e.g., a system that exhibits greater hyperconnec-
well-being than induced or acquired synesthesias.
tivity), may thus attenuate synesthetic experiences
These findings warrant further exploration in stud-
and ExEs. Cognitive disorganization in synesthe-
ies comparing the diverse ways synesthesias are
sia bears study among preselected participants that
experienced.
represent different schizotypy clusters to better
Psychological well-being and ExEs. Psycho-
understand how synesthetic experiences vary
across cluster membership. logical well-being did not correlate with tradi-
tional parapsychological experiences. Notably,
psychological well-being was excluded from
Psychological Well-Being
the final regression model for traditional para-
Synesthesia. Synesthesia did not correlate psychological experiences. In contrast, psycho-
positively with well-being, although schizotypy logical well-being added a small amount of
clusters differed significantly in terms of both variance to the overall model for AES. Our
well-being and synesthesia. In addition, the research implies that well-being is not related to
cluster group with the highest level of synesthe- traditional parapsychological experiences,
sia was also the group with the second highest which lends support to the idea that ExEs are
score on SWLS (healthy schizotypy cluster). In neutral in terms of well-being and perhaps men-
line with predictions, the healthy and high tal health (Simmonds-Moore, 2012a).
SYNESTHETIC EXPERIENCES 115

Methodological Limitations Multiple analysis and non-normality. We


acknowledge the possibility of Type I errors,
Online sampling. As noted previously, the despite corrections for multiple analysis, and
possibility exists that self-selection biases pro- our results should be interpreted with caution,
duced a sample that overrepresented individuals as some of the effect sizes were small. More-
with active interests in ExEs. Descriptive statis- over, the non-normality of the variables could
tics for the AES were slightly higher in our have reduced the reliability of findings, and
study (M ⫽ 10.23) than in a prior survey based missing SEQ data could have compromised fre-
on a general population sample (M ⫽ 7.5; Sim- quency estimates. Future studies should seek to
monds-Moore, 2009), and therefore may have replicate and validate our findings with a more
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

overrepresented the frequencies of synesthetic representative sample.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

experiences. Our findings thus require replica-


tion in more representative random samples. Future Research
Methodological limitations with the SEQ.
Some participants might have scored high on Our correlational design does not permit the
the SEQ because they experienced one or only determination of causal relationships. Kerns et al.
a few synesthesias consistently, whereas other (2014) articulated the need for longitudinal studies
participants could have obtained the same score, to further understand “peculiarity” and to ascertain
or an even higher score, by reporting more causality. To address this need, synesthetes’ and
synesthesias that occurred less often. Accord- positive schizotypes’ reports of ExEs could be
ingly, a person might be a strong or congenital assessed over a period of time using experience or
synesthete yet score relatively low on the SEQ. descriptive sampling methods. Longitudinal stud-
Future development of the scale could weight ies, beginning in childhood, would prove invalu-
scores based on the consistent experience of able in specifying the causal direction of relation-
synesthesia. Still, the observation of different ships. It would also be worthwhile to study the
types of responders to questions about synes- experiences of people who score high on synes-
thesia is revealing with regard to the multifac- thesia but low on positive schizotypy, and high on
eted nature of synesthesia. positive schizotypy but low on synesthesia, using
SEQ responses may reflect how frequently a qualitative methods. Differences between healthy
participant encounters a given inducer. Some and high scorers on schizotypy measures using
inducers are rarer than others—for example, such methods could also be investigated. Sim-
reading words or letters is an everyday experi- monds-Moore (2016) identified an interesting re-
ence, whereas experiencing emotions associated lationship between synesthesia and mental health.
with numbers is less common. Although differ- Although synesthesia and ExEs are an integral and
ences in base rates of inducers might add noise accepted part of the experiencer’s life, when they
to the interpretation of our questionnaire find- occurred at an earlier time in life, they were ap-
ings, the relationships revealed in our research praised negatively. Similar studies might explore
are, nevertheless, noteworthy with regard to how synesthesia and ExEs impact everyday expe-
synesthetic experiences and how they relate to riences in terms of mental health, psychopathol-
ExEs and the traits associated with positive ogy, positive functioning, and mental imagery.
schizotypy. Another issue is whether partici- The fact that some synesthesias were one-off
pants truly experience the world synesthetically or occasional warrants further attention. Con-
or whether they report that they do so in the textual synesthesia might be more systemati-
absence of synesthesia. Presenting questions re- cally studied by interviewing individuals who
garding synesthesias in the same survey that report synesthesias that occur less commonly
also enquires about ExEs might inflate the cor- and by examining individuals who report syn-
relations obtained, as the measures are admin- esthesias that are drug and/or meditation re-
istered in the same test context. Future studies lated, for example, versus more consistent and
could examine the role of such context effects automatic (congenital). Studies of training syn-
(Council, 1993) by assessing the reliability of esthesia (Marks, 2014) could be extended to
the correlations when the measures are pre- evaluate whether healthy positive schizotypes
sented in independent test contexts. are more likely to respond to training to incul-
116 SIMMONDS-MOORE, ALVARADO, AND ZINGRONE

cate synesthetic experiences compared with study. Paper presented at the 37th Annual Conven-
their less psychologically healthy counterparts. tion of the Parapsychological Association, Amster-
Researchers could also incorporate perfor- dam, the Netherlands.
mance-based tests of genuineness of synesthe- Alvarado, C. (1998). ESP and altered states of con-
sia to explore these relationships and examine sciousness: An overview of conceptual and re-
search trends. Journal of Parapsychology, 62, 27–
ExEs among strong synesthetes. Preselecting
63.
people who regularly experience different types Alvaraz, B. D., & Robertson, L. C. (2013). Synes-
of ExEs and determining the associated inci- thesia and binding. In J. Simner & E. Hubbard
dence and types of synesthesia could also be (Eds.), Oxford handbook of synesthesia (pp. 317–
revealing. For example, individuals who expe- 333). UK: Oxford University Press.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

rience auras might be more likely to report Banissy, M. J., Cassell, J. E., Fitzpatrick, S., Ward, J.,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

personality-color or emotion-color synesthesia Walsh, V. X., & Muggleton, N. G. (2012). Increased


than people who experience other synesthesias. positive and disorganised schizotypy in synaesthetes
In addition, participants that report experiencing who experience colour from letters and tones. Cor-
ESP might be more likely to report mirror-touch tex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous
synesthesia or emotion-color or other concur- System and Behavior, 48, 1085–1087. http://dx.doi
rent responses to an emotional inducer. Another .org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.06.009
approach would be to survey synesthetes about Banissy, M. J., Cohen Kadosh, R., Maus, G. W.,
ExEs in a more representative sample that Walsh, V., & Ward, J. (2009). Prevalence, charac-
teristics and a neurocognitive model of mirror-
would enable replication of our findings and
touch synaesthesia. Experimental Brain Research,
provide additional insights into the shared vari- 198, 261–271. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-
ances among variables considered herein. 009-1810-9
Banissy, M., Holle, H., Cassell, J., Annett, L., Tsa-
Conclusions kanikos, E., Walsh, V., . . . Ward, J. (2013).
Personality traits in people with synaesthesia: Do
We found support for a not-uncommon ten- synaesthetes have an atypical personality profile?
dency to experience synesthetic phenomena, and Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 828–
we replicated findings that some experiences of 831. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.12.018
synesthesia are more common than others. We Banissy, M. J., & Ward, J. (2007). Mirror-touch
also determined that synesthesia shares variance synesthesia is linked with empathy. Nature Neu-
with positive schizotypy (unusual experiences) roscience, 10, 815– 816. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/
and with ExEs, when defined broadly, and tradi- nn1926
Barnett, K. J., Finucane, C., Asher, J. E., Bargary, G.,
tional parapsychological experiences. Regression
Corvin, A. P., Newell, F. N., & Mitchell, K. J.
analyses supported the potential contributing roles (2008). Familial patterns and the origins of indi-
of both unusual experiences and synesthesia in the vidual differences in synaesthesia. Cognition, 106,
etiology of ExEs. 871– 893. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition
Synesthesia and traditional parapsychological .2007.05.003
experiences were unrelated to psychological well- Baron-Cohen, S., Johnson, D., Asher, J., Wheel-
being, although we found a small relationship wright, S., Fisher, S. E., Gregersen, P. K., & Al-
between anomalous experiences (defined more lison, C. (2013). Is synaesthesia more common in
widely) and poorer psychological well-being. autism? Molecular Autism, 4, 40. http://dx.doi.org/
Cluster analyses implied that healthy schizotypy is 10.1186/2040-2392-4-40
more aligned with synesthesia than high schizo- Belz, M. (2012). Clinical psychology for people with
typy. Our research supports the hypothesis that the exceptional experiences in practice. In C. Sim-
traits associated with positive schizotypy (when monds-Moore (Ed.), Exceptional experience and
not accompanied by cognitive disorganization) re- health: Essays on mind, body, and human potential
(pp. 223–241). Jefferson, NC: McFarland.
flect a healthy anomaly prone personality, which
Belz, M., & Fach, W. (2012). Theoretical reflections
can encompass the experience of synesthesia. on counseling and therapy for individuals report-
ing EE (exceptional experiences). In W. H.
References Kramer, E. Bauer, & G. H. Hövelmann (Eds.),
Perspectives of clinical parapsychology (pp. 168–
Alvarado, C. S. (1994, August). Synesthesia and 189). Bunnik, the Netherlands: Stichting Het Johan
claims of psychic experiences: An exploratory Borgman Fonds.
SYNESTHETIC EXPERIENCES 117

Bradbury, D. A., Stirling, J., Cavill, J., & Parker, A. ence, 2, 31–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-
(2009). Psychosis-like experiences in the general 8721.ep10770636
population: An exploratory factor analysis. Per- Crawley, S. E., French, C. C., & Yesson, S. A.
sonality and Individual Differences, 46, 729–734. (2002). Evidence for transliminality from a sub-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.01.035 liminal card-guessing task. Perception, 31, 887–
Brang, D., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2010). Visual 892.
field heterogeneity, laterality, and eidetic imagery Cytowic, R. E. (1995). Synesthesia: Phenomenology
in synesthesia. Neurocase, 16, 169–174. http://dx and neuropsychology: A review of current knowl-
.doi.org/10.1080/13554790903339645 edge. PSYCHE, 2(10). Retrieved from https://
Brang, D., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2011). Survival philpapers.org/rec/CYTSPA-2
of the synesthesia gene: Why do people hear colors Deroy, O., & Spence, C. (2013). Why we are not all
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and taste words? PLoS Biology, 9(11), e1001205. synesthetes (not even weakly so). Psychonomic
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001205 Bulletin & Review, 20, 643– 664. http://dx.doi.org/


Brang, D., Teuscher, U., Ramachandran, V. S., & 10.3758/s13423-013-0387-2
Coulson, S. (2010). Temporal sequences, synes- Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin,
thetic mappings, and cultural biases: The geogra- S. (1985). The Satisfaction With Life Scale. Jour-
phy of time. Consciousness and Cognition: An nal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75. http://
International Journal, 19, 311–320. http://dx.doi dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13
.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.01.003 Eagleman, D. M. (2012). Synaesthesia in its protean
Brogaard, B. (2013). Serotonergic hyperactivity as a guises. British Journal of Psychology, 103, 16–19.
potential factor in developmental, acquired and http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02020.x
drug-induced synesthesia. Frontiers in Human Farias, M., Claridge, G., & Lalljee, M. (2005). Per-
Neuroscience, 7, 657. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/ sonality and cognitive predictors of new age prac-
fnhum.2013.00657 tices and beliefs. Personality and Individual Dif-
Brown, S. (2000). The exceptional human experience ferences, 39, 979–989. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
process: A preliminary model with exploratory map. .paid.2005.04.003
International Journal of Parapsychology, 11, 69–111. Fleck, J. I., Green, D. L., Stevenson, J. L., Payne, L.,
Burrack, A., Knoch, D., & Brugger, P. (2006). Mi- Bowden, E. M., Jung-Beeman, M., & Kounios, J.
tempfindung in synaesthetes: Co-incidence or (2008). The transliminal brain at rest: Baseline
meaningful association? Cortex: A Journal De- EEG, unusual experiences, and access to uncon-
voted to the Study of the Nervous System and scious mental activity. Cortex: A Journal Devoted
Behavior, 42, 151–154. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior,
S0010-9452(08)70339-3 44, 1353–1363. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex
Cardeña, E. (2005). The phenomenology of deep .2007.08.024
hypnosis: Quiescent and physically active. Inter- Gallagher, C., Kumar, V., & Pekala, R. (1994). The
national Journal of Clinical and Experimental anomalous experiences inventory: Reliability and
Hypnosis, 53, 37–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ validity. Journal of Parapsychology, 58, 402– 428.
00207140490914234 Gianotti, L. R. R., Mohr, C., Pizzagalli, D., Lehmann,
Cardeña, E., & Alvarado, C. S. (2014). Anomalous D., & Brugger, P. (2001). Associative processing
self and identity experiences. In E. Cardeña, S. J. and paranormal belief. Psychiatry and Clinical
Lynn, & S. Krippner (Eds.), Varieties of anoma- Neurosciences, 55, 595– 603. http://dx.doi.org/10
lous experiences: Examining the scientific evi- .1046/j.1440-1819.2001.00911.x
dence (2nd ed., pp. 175–212). Washington, DC: Gimmestad, K. D. (2012). Assessment of transient
American Psychological Association. http://dx.doi negative affect in synesthesia. Dissertation Ab-
.org/10.1037/14258-007 stracts International: Section B: The Sciences and
Cardeña, E., Lynn, S. J., & Krippner, S. (Eds.). Engineering, 72(11B), 7046.
(2014). Varieties of Anomalous Experience: Ex- Glicksohn, J., Alon, A., Perlmutter, A., & Purisman,
amining the scientific evidence (2nd ed.). Wash- R. (2001). Symbolic and syncretic cognition
ington, DC: American Psychological Association. among schizophrenics and visual artists. Creativity
Claridge, G. (Ed.). (1997). Schizotypy: Implications Research Journal, 13, 133–143. http://dx.doi.org/
for illness and health. New York, NY: Oxford 10.1207/S15326934CRJ1302_1
University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/med: Goulding, A. (2004). Schizotypy models in relation
psych/9780198523536.001.0001 to subjective health and paranormal beliefs and
Claridge, G., Clark, K., & Davis, C. (1997). Night- experiences. Personality and Individual Differ-
mares, dreams and schizotypy. British Journal of ences, 37, 157–167. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
Clinical Psychology, 36, 377–386. .paid.2003.08.008
Council, J. R. (1993). Context effects in personality Goulding, A. (2005). Healthy schizotypy in a popu-
research. Current Directions in Psychological Sci- lation of paranormal believers and experients. Per-
118 SIMMONDS-MOORE, ALVARADO, AND ZINGRONE

sonality and Individual Differences, 38, 1069– Luke, D. P., & Terhune, D. B. (2013). The induction
1083. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.07.006 of synaesthesia with chemical agents: A systematic
Goulding, A., & Ödéhn, N. (2009). Schizotypy and review. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 753. http://dx
mental health in the general population: A pilot .doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00753
study. Personality and Mental Health, 3, 193–202. Mann, H., Korzenko, J., Carriere, J. S. A., & Dixon,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pmh.86 M. J. (2009). Time-space synaesthesia—A cogni-
Grossenbacher, P. G., & Lovelace, C. T. (2001). tive advantage? Consciousness and Cognition: An
Mechanisms of synesthesia: Cognitive and physi- International Journal, 18, 619– 627. http://dx.doi
ological constraints. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, .org/10.1016/j.concog.2009.06.005
5, 36– 41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613 Marks, L. E. (2014). Synesthesia: A teeming multi-
(00)01571-0 plicity. In E. Cardeña, S. J. Lynn, & S. Krippner
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Hartmann, E. (1991). Boundaries in the mind: A new (Eds.), Varieties of anomalous experience: Exam-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

psychology of personality. New York, NY: Basic- ining the scientific evidence (2nd ed., pp. 79–108).
Books. Washington, DC: American Psychological Associ-
Holt, N. (2012). The muse in the machine: Creativity, ation. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14258-004
anomalous experiences and mental health. In C. Sim- Marks, L. E., & Mulvenna, C. (2013). Synesthesia, at
monds-Moore (Ed.), Exceptional experience and and near its borders. Frontiers of Psychology, 4,
health: Essays on mind, body and human potential 651. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00651
(pp. 131–170). Jefferson, NC: McFarland. Martino, G., & Marks, L. E. (2001). Synesthesia:
Houran, J., Wiseman, R., & Thalbourne, M. A. Strong and weak. Current Directions in Psycho-
(2002). Perceptual-personality characteristics as- logical Science, 10, 61– 65. http://dx.doi.org/10
sociated with naturalistic haunt experiences. Euro- .1111/1467-8721.00116
pean Journal of Parapsychology, 17, 17– 44. Marwaha, S. B., & May, E. (2015). Rethinking ex-
Howell, D. C. (1989). Fundamental statistics for the trasensory perception: Toward a multiphasic
behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: model of precognition. SAGE Open, 5, 1–17.
PWS–Kent.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2158244015576056
Irwin, H. J. (1985). Parapsychological phenomena
Mason, O., Linney, Y., & Claridge, G. (2005). Short
and the absorption domain. Journal of the Ameri-
scales for measuring schizotypy. Schizophrenia
can Society for Psychical Research, 79, 1–11.
Research, 78, 293–296. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
Irwin, H. J., Dagnall, N., & Drinkwater, K. (2013).
j.schres.2005.06.020
Parapsychological experience as anomalous expe-
McCreery, C., & Claridge, G. (1996). A study of
rience plus paranormal attribution: A questionnaire
hallucination in normal subjects–II. Electrophysi-
based on a new approach to measurement. Journal
of Parapsychology, 77, 39–53. ological data. Personality and Individual Differ-
Irwin, H. J., & Watt, C. A. (2007). An introduction to ences, 21, 749–758. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
parapsychology (5th ed.). Jefferson, NC: McFarland. 0191-8869(96)00116-X
Kennedy, J. E., Kanthamani, H., & Palmer, J. (1994). Milán, E. G., Iborra, O., Hochel, M., Rodríguez Arta-
Psychic and spiritual experiences, health, well be- cho, M. A., Delgado-Pastor, L. C., Salazar, E., &
ing, and meaning in life. Journal of Parapsychol- González-Hernández, A. (2012). Auras in mysti-
ogy, 58, 353–383. cism and synaesthesia: A comparison. Conscious-
Kerns, J. G., Karcher, N., Raghavan, C., & Beren- ness and Cognition: An International Journal, 21,
baum, H. (2014). Anomalous experiences, pecu- 258–268. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011
liarity and psychopathology. In E. Cardeña, S. J. .11.010
Lynn, & S. Krippner (Eds.), Varieties of anoma- Mitchell, K. J. (2013). Synesthesia and cortical connec-
lous experience: Examining the scientific evidence tivity. In J. Simner & E. Hubbard (Eds), The Ox-
(2nd ed., pp. 57–76). Washington, DC: American ford handbook of synesthesia (pp. 530–557). New
Psychological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10 York, NY: Oxford University Press. http://dx.doi
.1037/14258-003 .org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199603329.013.0027
Knoch, D., Gianotti, L. R., Mohr, C., & Brugger, P. Murray, A. L. (2010). Can the existence of highly
(2005). Synesthesia: When colors count. Cognitive accessible concrete representations explain savant
Brain Research, 25, 372–374. http://dx.doi.org/10 skills? Some insights from synaesthesia. Medical
.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.05.005 Hypotheses, 74, 1006–1012. http://dx.doi.org/10
Loughland, C. M., & Williams, L. M. (1997). A .1016/j.mehy.2010.01.014
cluster analytic study of schizotypal trait dimen- Nelson, B., & Rawlings, D. (2010). Relating schizo-
sions. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, typy and personality to the phenomenology of cre-
877– 883. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869 ativity. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36, 388–399.
(97)00086-X http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbn098
SYNESTHETIC EXPERIENCES 119

Nettle, D. (2006). Schizotypy and mental health Sagiv, N., Ilbeigi, A., & Ben-Tal, O. (2011). Reflec-
amongst poets, visual artists, and mathematicians. tions on synesthesia, perception, and cognition.
Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 876– 890. Intellectica, 55, 81–94.
Nettle, D., & Clegg, H. (2006). Schizotypy, creativity Sagiv, N., & Ward, J. (2006). Crossmodal interac-
and mating success in humans. Proceedings of the tions: Lessons from synesthesia. Progress in Brain
Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, Research, 155, 259–271.
273, 611– 615. Schofield, K., & Claridge, G. (2007). Paranormal
Novich, S., Cheng, S., & Eagleman, D. M. (2011). Is experiences and mental health: Schizotypy as an
synaesthesia one condition or many? A large-scale underlying factor. Personality and Individual Dif-
analysis reveals subgroups. Journal of Neuropsy- ferences, 43, 1908–1916. http://dx.doi.org/10
chology, 5, 353–371. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j .1016/j.paid.2007.06.014
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

.1748-6653.2011.02015.x Simmonds-Moore, C. A. (2009). Sleep patterns, per-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Oertel, V., Rotarska-Jagiela, A., van de Ven, V., sonality, and subjective anomalous experiences.
Haenschel, C., Grube, M., Stangier, U., . . . Linden, Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 29, 71–
D. E. (2009). Mental imagery vividness as a trait 86. http://dx.doi.org/10.2190/IC.29.1.e
marker across the schizophrenia spectrum. Psychi- Simmonds-Moore, C. A. (2010). Personality vari-
atry Research, 167, 1–11. http://dx.doi.org/10 ables in spontaneous psi research: Contextualising
.1016/j.psychres.2007.12.008 the boundary construct in its relationship to spon-
Palmer, J. (1979). A community mail survey of psy- taneous psi phenomena. In C. Roe, W. Kramer, &
chic experiences. Journal of the American Society L. Coly (Eds.), Proceedings of an International
for Psychical Research, 73, 221–251. Conference Utrecht II: Charting the future of
Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (1993). Review of the Sat- parapsychology (pp. 151–215). New York, NY:
isfaction with Life Scale. Psychological Assess- Parapsychology Foundation.
ment, 5, 164–172. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040- Simmonds-Moore, C. A. (2012a). Exploring ways of
3590.5.2.164 manipulating anomalous experiences for mental
health and transcendence. In C. A. Simmonds-
Petri, G., Expert, P., Turkheimer, F., Carhart-Harris, R.,
Moore (Ed.), Exceptional experience and health:
Nutt, D., Hellyer, P. J., & Vaccarino, F. (2014).
Essays on mind, body, and human potential (pp.
Homological scaffolds of brain functional networks.
171–195). Jefferson, NC: McFarland Press.
Journal of the Royal Society, Interface, 11,
Simmonds-Moore, C. A. (2012b). What is excep-
20140873. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2014.0873
tional psychology? Journal of Parapsychology, 76,
Rader, C. M., & Tellegen, A. (1987). An investiga-
54–57.
tion of synesthesia. Journal of Personality and Simmonds-Moore, C. (2016). An interpretative phe-
Social Psychology, 52, 981–987. http://dx.doi.org/ nomenological analysis exploring synesthesia as
10.1037/0022-3514.52.5.981 an exceptional experience: Insights for conscious-
Ramachandran, V. S., Miller, L., Livingstone, M. S., ness and cognition. Qualitative Research in Psy-
& Brang, D. (2012). Colored halos around faces chology, 13, 303–327. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
and emotion-evoked colors: A new form of synes- 14780887.2016.1205693
thesia. Neurocase, 18, 352–358. http://dx.doi.org/ Simner, J. (2012). Defining synaesthesia. British
10.1080/13554794.2011.608366 Journal of Psychology, 103, 1–15. http://dx.doi
Rich, A. N., & Mattingley, J. B. (2013). The role of .org/10.1348/000712610X528305
attention in synesthesia. In J. Simner & E. Hubbard Simner, J., & Hubbard, E. (Eds.). (2013). Oxford hand-
(Eds.), The Oxford handbook of synesthesia (pp. book of synesthesia. UK: Oxford University Press.
265–282). New York, NY: Oxford University http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199603329
Press. .001.0001
Rothen, N., & Meier, B. (2013). Why vicarious ex- Simner, J., Mulvenna, C., Sagiv, N., Tsakanikos, E.,
perience is not an instance of synesthesia. Fron- Witherby, S. A., Fraser, C., . . . Ward, J. (2006).
tiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 128. http://dx.doi Synaesthesia: The prevalence of atypical cross-
.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00128 modal experiences. Perception, 35, 1024–1033.
Rothen, N., & Meier, B. (2014). Acquiring synaes- http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p5469
thesia: Insights from training studies. Frontiers in Spiller, M. J., Jonas, C. N., Simner, J., & Jansari, A.
Human Neuroscience, 8, 109. http://dx.doi.org/10 (2015). Beyond visual imagery: How modality-
.3389/fnhum.2014.00109 specific is enhanced mental imagery in synesthe-
Rouw, R. (2013). Synesthesia, hyperconnectivity, sia? Consciousness and Cognition, 31, 73– 85.
and diffusion tensor imaging. In J. Simner & E. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.10.010
Hubbard (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of synesthe- Tellegen, A., & Atkinson, G. (1974). Openness to
sia (pp. 501–518). New York, NY: Oxford Uni- absorbing and self-altering experiences (“absorp-
versity Press. tion”), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility.
120 SIMMONDS-MOORE, ALVARADO, AND ZINGRONE

Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83, 268–277. synesthesia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Dis-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0036681 ease, 189, 190–192. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/
Terhune, D. B. (2009). The incidence and determinants 00005053-200103000-00009
of visual phenomenology during out-of-body experi- Walsh, R. (2005). Can synesthesia be cultivated?
ences. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Journal of Consciousness Studies, 12, 1–13.
Nervous System and Behavior, 45, 236–242. http:// Williams, C. (1997). The role of imagination in the
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2007.06.007 construction of anomalous experience. Unpub-
Terhune, D. B., Wudarczyk, O. A., Kochuparampil,
lished PhD Thesis. Edinburgh University.
P., & Cohen Kadosh, R. (2013). Enhanced dimen-
sion-specific visual working memory in grapheme- Zingrone, N., Alvarado, C., & Agee, N. (2009). Psy-
color synesthesia. Cognition, 129, 123–137. http:// chological correlates of aura vision: Psychic expe-
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.06.009 riences, dissociation, absorption, and synaesthesia-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Thalbourne, M. A., Houran, J., Alias, A. G., & Brug- like experiences. Australian Journal of Clinical &
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ger, P. (2001). Transliminality, brain function, and Experimental Hypnosis, 37, 131–168.

Appendix

Items From the Synesthesia Experience Questionnaire


1. I see colors when I hear spoken words 23. I experience smells when I hear spoken words
2. I see colors when I read written words 24. I experience smells when I read written words
3. I see colors when I hear individual letters 25. I experience smells when I experience emo-
of the alphabet tions
4. I see colors when I read individual letters 26. I experience smells when I have visual expe-
of the alphabet riences
5. I see colors when I hear individual numbers 27. I experience smells when I experience tem-
6. I see colors when I read individual numbers peratures
7. I see colors when I hear sounds 28. I experience smells when I hear sounds
8. I see colors when I hear music 29. I experience smells for personalities
9. I see colors when I read music 30. I experience personalities for letters or num-
10. I see colors for days of the week (or other bers
units of time) 31. I experience personalities for objects
11. I see colors when I experience tastes or smells 32. I experience gender for letters or numbers
12. I see colors when I experience touch or pain 33. I experience numbers, days of the week, or
13. I see colors for personalities months of the year in a spatial arrangement
14. I see colors when I experience emotions (e.g., a circle, ellipse, oval)
15. I see colors when I experience temperatures 34. I experience touch when I see someone else
16. I see colors when I experience an orgasm being touched
17. I hear sounds when I have visual experiences 35. I experience touch or pain when I experience
18. I hear sounds when I experience kinetics emotions
(movement) 36. I experience touch for personalities
19. I hear sounds when I experience temperatures 37. I experience touch when I hear sounds
20. I hear sounds when I experience taste or 38. I experience touch when I have visual expe-
smells riences
21. I hear sounds when I experience touch 39. I experience touch when I experience temper-
22. I experience shapes when I taste food ature

(Appendix continues)
SYNESTHETIC EXPERIENCES 121

40. I experience touch when I experience smell or Experiences where people experience sen-
taste sory responses in addition to the normal sense
41. I experience taste when I experience smell are known as “synesthesia.” Please use the
42. I experience taste when I experience emotions space below to tell us more details about your
43. I experience taste when I have a visual expe- “synesthetic” experiences (if you have them).
rience We are particularly interested in:
44. I experience taste when I experience touch
45. I experience taste when I experience words 1. The state of consciousness associated
(or parts of words) with your experience—i.e., did it, or
46. I experience taste when I hear music does it occur when you are wide awake,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

47. I experience taste when I read music in a meditative state, in another altered
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

48. I experience taste when I hear sounds state (e.g., a drug-related state) or when
49. I experience taste when I experience temper- you are falling asleep or just waking up?
ature 2. Do your experiences (also) occur when
50. I experience temperature when I experience you just think about the inducer (the per-
emotions ceptual experience which leads to your
51. I experience temperature when I hear sounds
synesthetic concurrent experience)
52. I experience temperature when I have visual
3. Are there any other circumstances which
experiences
53. I experience temperature when I experience affect your “synesthetic” experiences?
smells or tastes 4. Please also use this space to describe a
54. I experience temperature when I experience “synesthetic” experience that is not cov-
touch ered by this questionnaire
55. I experience kinetics (movement) when I hear
a sound Received February 24, 2016
56. I experience kinetics (movement) when I Revision received April 30, 2018
have a visual experience Accepted July 20, 2018 䡲

You might also like