You are on page 1of 7

ARCHIVAL REPORT

Error-Related Brain Activity Predicts Cocaine Use


After Treatment at 3-Month Follow-up
Reshmi Marhe, Ben J.M. van de Wetering, and Ingmar H.A. Franken
Background: Relapse after treatment is one of the most important problems in drug dependency. Several studies suggest that lack of
cognitive control is one of the causes of relapse. In this study, a relative new electrophysiologic index of cognitive control, the error-
related negativity, is investigated to examine its suitability as a predictor of relapse.

Methods: The error-related negativity was measured in 57 cocaine-dependent patients during their first week in detoxification
treatment. Data from 49 participants were used to predict cocaine use at 3-month follow-up. Cocaine use at follow-up was measured by
means of self-reported days of cocaine use in the last month verified by urine screening.
Results: A multiple hierarchical regression model was used to examine the predictive value of the error-related negativity while
controlling for addiction severity and self-reported craving in the week before treatment. The error-related negativity was the only
significant predictor in the model and added 7.4% of explained variance to the control variables, resulting in a total of 33.4% explained
variance in the prediction of days of cocaine use at follow-up.

Conclusions: A reduced error-related negativity measured during the first week of treatment was associated with more days of cocaine
use at 3-month follow-up. Moreover, the error-related negativity was a stronger predictor of recent cocaine use than addiction severity
and craving. These results suggest that underactive error-related brain activity might help to identify patients who are at risk of relapse
as early as in the first week of detoxification treatment.

occurs approximately 25 to 100 msec after an erroneous response is


Key Words: Addiction, cocaine, cognitive control, error made and is thought to reflect the automatic processing of an error
processing, error-related negativity, relapse (20,21). Some theories suggest that the amplitude of the ERN
reflects the ability to monitor ongoing behavior and is used to

O
ne of the major challenges in addiction treatment is to correct or improve subsequent behavior (22,23). Another theory
prevent relapse after detoxification and other treatment. suggests that the relative strength of the ERN alludes to the
Typically, 50% of drug dependent patients drop out of motivational significance of an error for an individual (24–26), which
treatment and consequently relapse into drug use (1–3). Currently, accounts more for individual differences in the strength of the ERN.
there is insufficient knowledge about the factors influencing For example, when errors are more salient to an individual, this
treatment outcome and relapse. Although there are indications elicits a larger ERN compared with someone who experiences an
that drug use severity and self-reported measures such as craving error as less meaningful. In concordance, abnormalities in the ERN
can predict relapse to a certain extent (4–6), new developments in have been found to be associated with several psychiatric disorders,
cognitive neuroscience provide an opportunity to investigate such as anxiety, depression, and substance abuse [for a review, see
additional predictors of relapse that go beyond self-report. Olvet et al. (26)]. Because research has shown that the ERN is a
One of the essential features of substance dependence is the loss stable measure with good psychometric properties, it is suggested
of control over compulsive drug-seeking behavior, which manifests that it represents a trait that might serve as a neurobiological
in two characteristics of substance dependence: not being able to marker of psychopathology (26,27).
stop drug use and relapse after a period of abstinence and the Importantly, it has also been shown that cocaine-dependent
continuation of substance use despite the negative consequences patients have a reduced ERN (28,29). Additionally, a reduced ERN
(7–9). An important index of cognitive control is error processing has also been found in other addictive behaviors, such as
(10,11), which refers to the ability to adequately process adverse smoking (30). It has been suggested that this inadequate
consequences and thereby pursue goal-directed behavior. Neuro- response to errors may underlie one of the hallmark character-
imaging studies have found that activity in the anterior cingulate istics of addiction—that is, the persistence of drug taking despite
cortex (ACC), a consistently observed neural correlate of error adverse consequences. Different results (i.e., increased ERN) have
detection (12,13), is diminished in cocaine-dependent individuals been found in alcohol-dependent individuals (31,32), but this is
during response inhibition and error processing (14–16). arguably related to the comorbid anxiety symptoms in that
Strongly related to ACC activity is the error-related negativity population (32).
(ERN), an electrophysiologic index of error processing (17–19). The More clinically relevant is the plausibility that deficits in the
ERN is a negative deflection in the event-related potential (ERP) that ERN contribute to the maintenance of substance dependence
and relapse to substance use. A few studies that have success-
From the Institute of Psychology (RM, IHAF), Erasmus University fully associated reduced activity in neural correlates of cognitive
Rotterdam; Bouman-GGZ (BJMvdW); and Department of Child and control to relapse support the clinical relevance of neurologic
Adolescent Psychiatry (IHAF), Erasmus MC-University Medical Center, data (33–35). However, no study to date has investigated
Rotterdam, The Netherlands. whether the reduced ERN found in substance-dependent sub-
Address correspondence to Reshmi Marhe, Erasmus University Rotterdam, jects may be a predictor of relapse.
Institute of Psychology, P.O. Box 1738, 3000 DR Rotterdam, The The main goal of this study was to examine whether an
Netherlands; E-mail: marhe@fsw.eur.nl. electrophysiologic index of cognitive control, the ERN, is pre-
Received Aug 14, 2012; revised Dec 21, 2012; accepted Dec 22, 2012. dictive of cocaine use in cocaine-dependent patients 3 months

0006-3223/$36.00 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2013;73:782–788


http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.12.016 & 2013 Society of Biological Psychiatry
R. Marhe et al. BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2013;73:782–788 783

after the start of detoxification treatment. Specifically, we The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the
hypothesized that a reduced ERN, as measured in cocaine- Erasmus Medical Center, Rotterdam, The Netherlands. All proce-
dependent patients during their first week of detoxification dures were carried out with the adequate understanding and
treatment, would be associated with cocaine use after 3 months. written informed consent of the participants. They received
financial compensation of 20 euros after completion of the
electroencephalography (EEG) measure and 25 euros after com-
Methods and Materials pletion of the 3-month follow-up measure (patients only).

Participants Task
Fifty-seven cocaine-dependent patients were recruited from The Eriksen Flanker task was used to measure error processing
an addiction treatment center (Bouman GGZ) in Rotterdam, The (37), and ERPs were recorded. The stimuli consisted of four letter
Netherlands. Inclusion criteria were 1) age between 18 and 65 strings (HHHHH, SSSSS, HHSHH, SSHSS). Participants were instructed
years, 2) presence of the DSM-IV diagnosis for cocaine depen- to respond to the central letter. On a response box, they had to press
dence (assessed by both a physician and a research psychologist), H with their right index finger when the central letter was an H and S
and 3) the ability to speak, read, and write in Dutch at an eighth- with their left index finger when the central letter was an S. Each
grade literacy level. Exclusion criteria were 1) indications of severe experimental trial started with a fixation cue for 150 msec where
psychopathology (i.e., psychosis, severe mood disorder, as the central (target) letter would appear. The letter string was shown
assessed by a physician), 2) self-reported color blindness or for 52 msec followed by a blank screen for 648 msec. Participants
(uncorrected) defective vision, and 3) pregnant or breastfeeding. had 700 msec from stimulus onset to respond. After the end of the
Of all 57 participants, data from 8 were not included for the response period, a feedback symbol appeared for 500 msec
following reasons: 1) two participants were lost to follow-up; 2) indicating whether the given response was correct (⫹), incorrect
one participant did not understand the flanker task (regarded the (–), or too late (!). An intertrial interval was used of 100 msec.
first letter instead of the central letter as the target); 3) one
participant made too few errors (⬍5) to obtain a reliable ERN Procedure
(36); 4) in four participants, ⬎50% of ERN segments contained Patients entered the treatment center for an inpatient detox-
artifacts. Table 1 shows all demographic variables and substance- ification treatment and were informed about the study on the
use variables for the final sample (n ¼ 49). second day of their treatment. They had 24 hours to decide
Additionally, we also tested 25 healthy control subjects with- whether to participate. Volunteers signed the informed consent
out a history of substance or alcohol dependence. A previous form on the third day of detoxification treatment. First, we
study by our lab showed that cocaine-dependent patients have assessed addiction severity using the Addiction Severity Index
diminished error processing compared with healthy control (38,39) and previous week craving using the Obsessive Compulsive
subjects (28). To confirm these results, we compared the patient Drug Use Scale (40). Second, participants were taken to the EEG
sample of the current study with a control group. Data of two lab of Erasmus University Rotterdam. Upon arrival, participants
control subjects were excluded from analysis because they either were seated in a sound-attenuated room with dimmed lights. The
made too few errors or had too many artifacts in the ERN Eriksen flanker task was explained to them. After a practice phase
segments. Demographics of the final sample of controls (n ¼ 23) consisting of eight-letter strings, participants started the test
are displayed in Table 1. The patient and control group did not phase, which consisted of 400 letter strings, divided in 5 blocks.
significantly differ in age, gender, or education (Table 1). In between blocks, participants could rest as long as needed.

Table 1. Demographic and Substance Use Variables of Cocaine Dependent Participants and Control
subjects

Patients Controls
Subject Variable (n ¼ 49) (n ¼ 23) Test Value p Value

Demographic Variables
Age 39.6 (8.4) 39.9 (9.4) t ¼ .18 .86
Male (%) 89 74 w2 ¼ 2.16 .14
Education (%)
Primary 8 0 w2 ¼ 6.27 .18
Junior secondary 59 43
Senior secondary 23 35
Higher education 10 22
Addiction Severity Variables
Total years of cocaine use 12.2 (6.8) NA
Cocaine use in 30 days before treatment entry 17.6 (11.5) NA
Main administration route (%)
Intranasal 27 NA
Smoking 65 NA
Intravenous 8 NA
Craving in the Week Before Treatment Entry
OCDUS Desire and Control (1–5) 3.0 (1.1) NA
Values are mean (SD) unless otherwise indicated (%).
NA, not applicable; OCDUS, Obsessive Compulsive Drug Use Scale.

www.sobp.org/journal
784 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2013;73:782–788 R. Marhe et al.

After the Flanker task, a series of other tasks were adminis- n ¼ 31 reported to have used 11.55 days on average (SD 10.28,
tered but are not reported in this article. The order of the tasks range 1–30).
was not counterbalanced. After completion of all tasks, the
participant was taken back to the treatment center. EEG Recording and Processing
Brain activity was recorded with the EEG using a Biosemi
Treatment ActiveTwo System amplifier (Amsterdam, The Netherlands) from
A typical detoxification treatment in this setting has a 32 scalp sites and one additional scalp site (FCz). Silver chloride
duration of 3 weeks and the specific goal is to reduce withdrawal active (Ag/AgCl) electrodes were placed on the scalp according
symptoms. Treatment interventions in the detoxification phase to the 10-20 International System. Four external electrodes were
include psychoeducation about the detox symptoms and used to measure vertical electro-oculogram (VEOG) and horizon-
individual therapy based on cognitive-behavioral techniques. tal electro-oculogram (HEOG) and were placed above and below
After detoxification treatment, the patients start follow-up the left eye (VEOG) and at the outer canthi of both eyes (HEOG).
treatment in a different department within the same treatment Two external electrodes were used for recording reference
center. This is usually a rehabilitation program with a variable activity. These were placed on the left and right mastoids. All
duration between 1 month and 2 years, depending on the need signals were digitized with a sampling rate of 512 Hz and 24-bit
for treatment. analog-to-digital conversion and were filtered offline. During
offline processing, no more than two bad channels per subject
Outcome Measure were removed from the EEG signal, and new values per channel
Recent cocaine use was measured at follow-up, 3 months where calculated using topographic interpolation. The computed
after study participation. Several procedures were performed average of the mastoids were used as reference. The data were
to ensure compliance with follow-up: 1) we collected as much filtered using a low cutoff of .15 Hz and high cutoff of 30 Hz (24
contact information as possible (e.g., phone numbers, e-mail dB/octave slope). Data were segmented in epochs from 100 msec
addresses) of the participant and other contacts such as preresponse to 600 msec postresponse. HEOG and VEOG artifacts
family, friends, social workers and other professionals involved were corrected using the Gratton and Coles algorithm (41). The
with the patient; 2) participants received a financial compen- mean 100-msec prestimulus period served as baseline. Artifact
sation of 25 euros for completion of the follow-up test; 3) rejection was done automatically. Minimum and maximum
participants were ensured that information on current use allowed amplitude –100 to ⫹100 mV was used. ERPs were
(self-reports and urine screens) was used for research averaged according to response condition (correct and incorrect).
purposes only. The ERN was quantified by mean amplitude measure in the 25- to
Participants were contacted via telephone and/or e-mail to set 100-msec time window. A cluster of electrode sites Fz, FCz, and
up an appointment for the follow-up interview. Follow-up tests Cz were used for the analysis of the ERN. These electrode sites are
were assessed in the treatment center. If a participant was out of typically used in ERN research (28,42–44) because the ERN is
treatment at this point, he or she was asked to return once to the maximal at the frontocentral midline of the scalp (Figure 1).
main treatment center to complete the follow-up test. If the Difference waves were calculated (incorrect minus correct) to
participant was unable to travel to this location, the researcher obtain a single relative measure of error processing (45). All
would perform the assessment at the nearest treatment facility to waveforms are displayed in Figure 1. The magnitude of the ERN is
the participant or arranged taxi transfer to the treatment center negative. Therefore, a negative difference wave amplitude
for the participant. indicates that relative to the correct response, the amplitude
At follow-up, participants were asked to report the number on the incorrect response is larger. Thus, a more negative value
of days they had used cocaine in the past 30 days, which we indicates a larger ERN (i.e., increased error-related brain
labeled “recent cocaine use.” Self-reports were biochemically processing).
verified by means of urine screens. All participants who
reported that they had not used in the past 30 days also had Data Analysis
a negative urine screen at follow-up. Of the final sample, We first conducted an analysis of variance to compare the ERN
n ¼ 18 reported to have used 0 days in the last 30 days and of patients versus controls to confirm that patients in this sample

Difference wave Controls


Difference wave Patients
-5 Correct Controls
Correct Patients
Amplitude (µV)

Incorrect Controls
Incorrect Patients

0 100

25 ms - 100 ms
-5.28 µV 0.00 µV 5.28 µV

Time (ms)

Figure 1. Scalp topography (left) and response-locked correct, incorrect, and difference waves at scalp site FCz (right) of cocaine-dependent patients and
control subjects. Responses occurred at 0 msec. mV, mean amplitude.

www.sobp.org/journal
R. Marhe et al. BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2013;73:782–788 785

Table 2. Correlations Between Outcome Measure “Recent Cocaine Use” and All Predictor Variables

Cocaine Use in Administration Administration Craving in the Error-


Total Years of 30 Days Before Route Intranasal Route Intranasal Week before Related
Cocaine Use Treatment vs. Smoking vs. Intravenous Treatment Negativity

Recent Cocaine Use .28a .34a .23 .25 .30a .29a


Total Years of Cocaine Use .17 .30a .13 .09 .26
Cocaine Use in 30 Days Before Treatment .12 .32a .59b .07
Administration Route Intranasal vs. Smoking .41b .09 .17
Administration Route Intranasal vs. Intravenous .40b .01
Craving in the Week Before Treatment .27
a
p ⬍ .05.
b
p ⬍ .01.

had a reduced ERN. Second, we examined the direct associations Third, and most important, are the results of the hierarchical
(using Pearson’s correlation) between recent cocaine use and all regression to predict recent cocaine use (Table 3). In Step 1 of the
predictor variables to show the effect size for each measure model, the addiction severity variables and craving in the past
independently. Third, a hierarchical regression analyses was week before treatment entry were entered. Together these
conducted to predict recent cocaine use. In Step 1, addiction variables explained 26% of the variance in the number of
severity variables (total years of cocaine use, cocaine use in days of cocaine use in the past month (F5,43 ¼ 3.03, p ⬍ .05).
30 days before treatment entry, and cocaine administration Individually, only administration route intranasal versus
route) and craving in the week before treatment were entered smoking was significant, indicating that smoking was associated
as control variables. Because administration route was a catego- with more days of cocaine use at follow-up. In Step 2, the
rical variable with three categories, two dummy variables were ERN was entered. This model, including addiction severity
created, both coded as 0 versus 1: intranasal versus smoking and variables, craving and the ERN explained an additional 7.4%
intranasal versus intravenous. In Step 2, the ERN was entered into (medium to small effect) (48) of the variance in the number of
the equation to examine its unique contribution to the model. To days of cocaine use in the past month (Fchange,1,42 ¼ 4.62,
check robustness of the individual predictors, bootstrapping was p ⬍ .05). Thus, the second model explained a total of 33.4% of
used with 2000 bootstrapped samples and a 95% confidence the variance (F6,42 ¼ 3.51, p ⬍ .01). The added predictive value of
interval (CI) (46,47). Finally, predictors were checked for multi- the ERN (R2change ¼ .074) was a medium to small effect (48), and
collinearity by means of tolerance statistics. the ERN was the only significant individual predictor in this
model. The results showed that a reduced ERN (measured during
Results the first week of treatment) was associated with more days of
recent cocaine use (measured at 3-month follow-up). Figure 2
First, we confirmed that the cocaine-dependent patients in displays the direct association between the ERN and recent
the current sample have a reduced ERN compared with non- cocaine use. Finally, collinearity statistics indicated that the
dependent control subjects (F1,70 ¼ 10.03, p ⬍ .01; Figure 1), predictors were not associated with each other (all
which is similar to the results of Franken and colleagues (28). tolerance $.50).
Results on the behavioral measures of the flanker task are Although it was not our main interest, we also examined
reported in Supplement 1. associations between behavioral measures of error processing
Second, recent cocaine use was directly associated with total and recent cocaine use. Pearson’s correlations showed that
years of cocaine use, cocaine use in the 30 days before treatment, behavioral measures were not significantly associated with recent
craving in the week before treatment, and the ERN (Table 2). cocaine use (all ps ⬎ .15).

Table 3. Results of Hierarchical Regression Analysis with Bootstrap Procedure

Standardized Unstandardized Bootstrapped


Coefficients Coefficients 95% Confidence
Predictor Variable R2 (¼ b) (¼ B) Interval

Step 1 .26a
Total years of cocaine use .17 .25 .29 to .78
Cocaine use in 30 days before .10 .08 .28 to .39
treatment entry
Administration route
Intranasal vs. smoking .28a 5.75a 1.01 to 10.56a
Intranasal vs. intravenous .24 8.47 3.68 to 17.02
Craving in the week before .19 1.64 1.68 to 5.61
treatment
Step 2 .33b
Error-related negativity .30a .77a .11 to 1.50a
a
p ⬍ .05.
b
p ⬍ .01.

www.sobp.org/journal
786 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2013;73:782–788 R. Marhe et al.

35 prediction studies have merely looked at direct associations


Recent Cocaine Use (days)

30 between the independent variable of interest and the outcome


measure. For example, several studies have investigated the
25 predictive utility of craving, a phenomenon that plays a central
20 role in influential theories of addiction (53,54). Some studies did
15 report direct associations between craving and subsequent
r = 0.29 cocaine use (6,55), but other studies found that self-reported
10
craving at the start of treatment was not predictive of cocaine
5 use at follow-up (56,57). This suggests that other factors might
0 influence relapse and drug use. In line with this idea are
-15.00 -10.00 -5.00 0.00 5.00 10.00 substance relapse studies showing that (neuro)cognitive mea-
ERN (µV) sures are better predictors of subsequent relapse than self-
reported craving, such as attentional bias for drug cues (58,59)
Figure 2. Direct association between the error-related negativity (ERN) and increased brain activity after exposure to cocaine stimuli in
and recent cocaine use at 3-month follow-up. mV, mean amplitude. areas such as the posterior cingulate cortex (60). Correspond-
ingly, our data have provided robust evidence that a neurophy-
Discussion siological measure of cognitive control, the ERN, is a stronger
predictor of cocaine use at follow-up than addiction severity and
The current study is one of the few to link basic neurocog- craving.
nitive control processes directly with clinical outcome. It is the The present results yield new insight in the clinical relevance
first to examine error-related brain activity as a predictor of of cognitive control in cocaine-dependence treatment and have
cocaine use after treatment. In line with our hypothesis, the important implications for clinical practice. There is growing
results showed that a reduced ERN in cocaine-dependent interest in the use of the ERN as a screening tool for treatment
patients in treatment is associated with later cocaine use. The outcome or diagnostic instrument in several psychiatric disorders
ERN seems a robust predictor of cocaine use after treatment, showing abnormalities in the ERN (26,61). The advantage of using
even when controlling for other predictors such as addiction EEG to assess neurophysiologic components of cognitive control
severity and craving. This finding suggests that cocaine- is that it is noninvasive, less expensive and more accessible than
dependent individuals with strongly diminished error-related other neuroimaging techniques, such as functional magnetic
brain activity are more at risk of relapse. Moreover, it shows that resonance imaging. With respect to cocaine-dependence treat-
error-related brain processes measured in the first week of ment, it might be helpful to use EEG to assess the ERN as a
detoxification treatment are associated with 3-month later routine screening tool at the start of detoxification. The relative
cocaine use, which suggests that neurocognitive control mea- strength of the ERN amplitude can provide more insight in a
sures such as the ERN can provide useful information on relapse patient’s responsiveness to treatment. Additionally, treatment
risk as early as in the first week of treatment. programs could be adjusted to the individual patient to improve
Several studies, including the current study, have shown that outcomes and ultimately prevent relapse.
error processing is diminished in drug-dependent individuals, as Limitations of the study should be noted. First, we only
indicated by a reduced ERN (28,29). These studies, together with measured days of cocaine use in the past month of the 3-month
other neuroimaging research showing diminished error-related follow-up period. Second, we did not examine the influence of
ACC activity in drug dependent individuals (14–16), support the comorbid psychiatric disorders on cocaine use outcome. Future
theory that reduced cognitive control is one of the main research should address these limitations by measuring cocaine
characteristics of drug dependency (8). Poor performance mon- use during the whole 3-month period to examine time to relapse
itoring influences performance on a higher level of self-control and using a larger sample size to test more predictors in a multiple
such as impulse control and decision making (49). Hence, it regression model, such as comorbidity.
might be that deficits in the ERN and ACC functioning, reflecting Overall, our findings have provided evidence for the role that
poor error monitoring, represent the fundamental process neurophysiological measures of cognitive control can play in
underlying dysfunctional decision making that is frequently identifying cocaine-dependent individuals who are at risk of
observed in drug-dependent patients (50). Most pertinent to relapse as early as in their first week of detoxification treatment.
this study is that individual variations in these neurophysiologic Identification of these cognitive processes and neural correlates
correlates of cognitive control have been proposed to account as possible predictors of drug relapse, on top of other well-
for differences in relapse risk (9,34,35). Our results indeed established predictors, have important implications for the
suggest that variations in the ERN amplitude are associated with clinical practice. Ultimately, electrophysiologic screening tools
cocaine use at follow-up. More specifically, patients with reduced may be implemented in treatment programs to identify patients
ERN are more at risk of subsequent relapse than patients with who are more susceptible to relapse.
larger ERN.
The identification of multiple predictors of treatment outcome This study was supported by a grant of The Netherlands
and relapse have been a priority in drug addiction research, Organization for Health Research and Development (ZonMw Grant
and it has been discussed that models of substance-use No. 31160203 to IHAF). We thank Radha Jagroep, Yavuz Kilic, and
relapse should be multifactorial (51,52). For example, Donovan Alexandra de Raaij for their assistance with data collection.
(51) proposed that theoretically relevant variables should always The authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential
be implemented in prediction models, and Poling and colleagues conflicts of interest.
(52) argued that in prediction research, baseline drug-use severity
variables should be taken into account because addiction Supplementary material cited in this article is available
populations generally vary in severity. Nonetheless, many online.

www.sobp.org/journal
R. Marhe et al. BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2013;73:782–788 787

1. Gossop M, Green L, Phillips G, Bradley B (1987): What happens to 27. Hajcak G (2012): What we’ve learned from our mistakes: Insights from
opiate addicts immediately after treatment: A prospective follow up error-related brain activity. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 21:101–106.
study. Br Med J 294:1377–1380. 28. Franken IHA, van Strien JW, Franzek EJ, Van de Wetering BJ (2007):
2. Franken IHA, Hendriks VM (1999): Predicting outcome of inpatient Error-processing deficits in patients with cocaine dependence. Biol
detoxification of substance abusers. Psychiatr Serv 50:813–817. Psychol 75:45–51.
3. Hättenschwiler J, Rüesch P, Hell D (2000): Effectiveness of inpatient 29. Sokhadze E, Stewart C, Hollifield M, Tasman A (2008): Event-related
drug detoxification: Links between process and outcome variables. potential study of executive dysfunctions in a speeded reaction task
Eur Addic Res 6:123–131. in cocaine addiction. J Neurother 12:185–204.
4. Carroll KM, Power M-ED, Bryant K, Rounsaville BJ (1993): One-year 30. Luijten M, Van Meel CS, Franken IHA (2011): Diminished error
follow-up status of treatment-seeking cocaine abusers: Psychopathol- processing in smokers during smoking cue exposure. Pharmacol
ogy and dependence severity as predictors of outcome. J Nerv Ment Biochem Behav 97:514–520.
Dis 181:71–79. 31. Padilla ML, Colrain IM, Sullivan EV, Mayer BZ, Turlington SR, Hoffman
5. Reiber C, Ramirez A, Parent D, Rawson RA (2002): Predicting treat- LR, et al. (2011): Electrophysiological evidence of enhanced perfor-
ment success at multiple time points in diverse patient populations of mance monitoring in recently abstinent alcoholic men. Psychophar-
cocaine-dependent individuals. Drug Alcohol Depend 68:35–48. macology (Berl) 213:81–91.
6. Weiss RD, Griffin ML, Mazurick C, Berkman B, Gastfriend DR, Frank A, 32. Schellekens AF, De Bruijn ER, Van Lankveld CA, Hulstijn W, Buitelaar
et al. (2003): The relationship between cocaine craving, psychosocial JK, De Jong CA, et al. (2010): Alcohol dependence and anxiety
treatment, and subsequent use. Am J Psychiatry 160:1320–1325. increase error-related brain activity. Addiction 105:1928–1934.
7. American Psychiatric Association. (1994): Diagnostic and Statistical 33. Bauer LO (1997): Frontal P300 decrements, childhood conduct
Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. Washington, DC: American disorder, family history, and the prediction of relapse among
Psychiatric Press. abstinent cocaine abusers. Drug Alcohol Depend 44:1–10.
8. Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND (2002): Drug addiction and its underlying 34. Paulus MP, Tapert SF, Schuckit MA (2005): Neural activation patterns
neurobiological basis: Neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of of methamphetamine-dependent subjects during decision making
the frontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry 159:1642–1652. predict relapse. Arch Gen Psychiatry 62:761–768.
9. Garavan H, Hester R (2007): The role of cognitive control in cocaine 35. Brewer JA, Worhunsky PD, Carroll KM, Rounsaville BJ, Potenza MN
dependence. Neuropsychol Rev 17:337–345. (2008): Pretreatment brain activation during Stroop task is associated
10. Gehring WJ, Goss B, Coles MGH, Meyer DE, Donchin E (1993): A neural with outcomes in cocaine-dependent patients. Biol Psychiatry 64:
system for error detection and compensation. Psychol Sci 4:385–390. 998–1004.
11. Botvinick MM, Carter CS, Braver TS, Barch DM, Cohen JD (2001): 36. Olvet DM, Hajcak G (2009): The stability of error-related brain activity
Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev 108:624–652. with increasing trials. Psychophysiology 46:957–961.
12. Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M, Crone EA, Nieuwenhuis S (2004): The 37. Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW (1974): Effects of noise letters upon the
role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science 306: identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept Psychophys
443–447. 16:143–149.
13. Carter CS, Van Veen V (2007): Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict 38. McLellan AT, Luborski L, Woody GE, O’Brien CP (1980): An improved
detection: An update of theory and data. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci diagnostic evaluation instrument for substance abuse patients: The
7:367–379. Addiction Severity Index. J Nerv Ment Dis 168:26–33.
14. Kaufman JN, Ross TJ, Stein EA, Garavan H (2003): Cingulate hypoac- 39. Hendriks VM, Kaplan CD, Van Limbeek J, Geerlings P (1989): The
tivity in cocaine users during a GO-NOGO task as revealed by event- Addiction Severity Index: Reliability and validity in a Dutch addict
related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 23: population. J Subst Abuse Treat 6:133–141.
7839–7843. 40. Franken IHA, Hendriks VM, Van den Brink W (2002): Initial validation of
15. Hester R, Garavan H (2004): Executive dysfunction in cocaine addic- two opiate craving questionnaires: The Obsessive Compulsive Drug
tion. Evidence for discordant frontal, cingulate, and cerebellar activity. Use Scale and the Desires for Drug Questionnaire. Addict Behav 27:
J Neurosci 24:11017–11022. 675–685.
16. Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND (2011): Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex 41. Gratton G, Coles MG, Donchin E (1983): A new method for off-line
in addiction. Neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat Rev removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 55:
Neurosci 12:652–669. 468–484.
17. Mathalon DH, Whitfield SL, Ford JM (2003): Anatomy of an error: ERP 42. Ridderinkhof KR, De Vlugt Y, Bramlage A, Spaan M, Elton M, Snel J,
and fMRI. Biol Psychol 64:119–141. et al. (2002): Alcohol consumption impairs detection of performance
18. Miltner WHR, Lemke U, Weiss T, Holroyd C, Scheffers MK, Coles MGH errors in mediofrontal cortex. Science 298:2209–2211.
(2003): Implementation of error-processing in the human anterior 43. Hajcak G, Moser JS, Yeung N, Simons RF (2005): On the ERN and the
cingulate cortex: A source analysis of the magnetic equivalent of the significance of errors. Psychophysiology 42:151–160.
error-related negativity. Biol Psychol 64:157–166. 44. Hajcak G, Foti D (2008): Errors are aversive: Defensive motivation and
19. Van Veen V, Carter CS (2002): The anterior cingulate as a conflict the error-related negativity: Research report. Psychol Sci 19:103–108.
monitor: fMRI and ERP studies. Physiol Behav 77:477–482. 45. Weinberg A, Klein DN, Hajcak G (2012): Increased error-related brain
20. Gehring WJ, Coles MG, Meyer DE, Donchin E (1995): A brain potential activity distinguishes generalized anxiety disorder with and without
manifestation of error-related processing. Electroencephalogr Clin comorbid major depressive disorder. J Abnorm Psychol 121:885–896.
Neurophysiol S44:261–272. 46. Efron B, Tibshirani RJ (1993): An Introduction to the Bootstrap. New
21. Bernstein PS, Scheffers MK, Coles MGH (1995): “Where did I go York: Chapman & Hall.
wrong?” A psychophysiological analysis of error detection. J Exp 47. Mooney CZ, Duval RD (1993): Bootstrapping: A Nonparametric
Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21:1312–1322. Approach to Statistical Inference. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
22. Holroyd CB, Coles MGH (2002): The neural basis of human error 48. Cohen J (1988): Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences.
processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
negativity. Psychol Rev 109:679–709. 49. Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW III, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS
23. Yeung N, Botvinick MM, Cohen JD (2004): The neural basis of error (2004): Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in
detection: Conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. control. Science 303:1023–1026.
Psychol Rev 111:931–959. 50. Bechara A, Noël X, Crone EA (2006): Loss of willpower: Abnormal
24. Gehring WJ, Goss B, Coles MGH, Meyer DE, Donchin E (1993): A neural neural mechanisms of impulse control and decision making in
system for error detection and compensation. Psychol Sci 4:385–390. addiction. In: Wiers RW, Stacy AW, editor. Handbook of Implicit
25. Hajcak G, Moser JS, Yeung N, Simons RF (2005): On the ERN and the Cognition and Addiction. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
significance of errors. Psychophysiology 42:151–160. 51. Donovan DM (1996): Assessment issues and domains in the predic-
26. Olvet DM, Hajcak G (2008): The error-related negativity (ERN) and tion of relapse. Addiction 91(suppl):S29–S36.
psychopathology: Toward an endophenotype. Clin Psychol Rev 28: 52. Poling J, Kosten TR, Sofuoglu M (2007): Treatment outcome pre-
1343–1354. dictors for cocaine dependence. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 33:191–206.

www.sobp.org/journal
788 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2013;73:782–788 R. Marhe et al.

53. Robinson TE, Berridge KC (1993): The neural basis of drug craving: 58. Marissen MAE, Franken IHA, Waters AJ, Blanken P, Van den Brink W,
An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Rev 18: Hendriks VM (2006): Attentional bias predicts heroin relapse following
247–291. treatment. Addiction 101:1306–1312.
54. Tiffany ST (1990): A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use 59. Marhe R, Waters AJ, Van de Wetering BJM, Franken IHA (2012): Implicit
behavior: Role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychol Rev and explicit drug-related cognitions during detoxification treatment
97:147–168. are associated with drug relapse: An ecological momentary assessment
55. Preston KL, Vahabzadeh M, Schmittner J, Lin J, Gorelick DA, Epstein study [published online ahead of print Dec 10]. J Consult Clin Psychol.
DH (2009): Cocaine craving and use during daily life. Psychopharma- 60. Kosten TR, Scanley BE, Tucker KA, Oliveto A, Prince C, Sinha R, et al.
cology 207:291–301. (2006): Cue-induced brain activity changes and relapse in cocaine
56. Weiss RD, Griffin ML, Hufford C (1995): Craving in hospitalized cocaine dependent patients. Neuropsychopharmacology 31:644–650.
abusers as a predictor of outcome. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 21:289–301. 61. Hoffmann S, Falkenstein M (2012): Predictive information processing
57. Bordnick PS, Schmitz JM (1998): Cocaine craving: An evaluation across in the brain: Errors and response monitoring. Int J Psychophysiol 83:
treatment phases. J Subst Abuse 10:9–17. 208–212.

www.sobp.org/journal

You might also like