You are on page 1of 26

This article was downloaded by: [Purdue University]

On: 17 August 2011, At: 08:59


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954
Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH,
UK

Discourse Processes
Publication details, including instructions for
authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hdsp20

Judgments Concerning the


Valence of Inter-Turn Silence
Across Speakers of American
English, Italian, and Japanese
a b c
Felicia Roberts , Piera Margutti & Shoji Takano
a
Department of Communication, Purdue University,
West Lafayette, IN
b
Department of Language Sciences, Università per
Stanieri di Perugia, Perugia, Italy
c
Department of English, Hokusei Gakuen University,
Sapporo, Japan

Available online: 09 Jun 2011

To cite this article: Felicia Roberts, Piera Margutti & Shoji Takano (2011): Judgments
Concerning the Valence of Inter-Turn Silence Across Speakers of American English,
Italian, and Japanese, Discourse Processes, 48:5, 331-354

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2011.558002

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-


and-conditions

This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes.
Any substantial or systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan,
sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is
expressly forbidden.
The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any
representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to
date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses should be
independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable
for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages
whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection
with or arising out of the use of this material.
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011
Discourse Processes, 48:331–354, 2011
Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 0163-853X print/1532-6950 online
DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2011.558002

Judgments Concerning the Valence of


Inter-Turn Silence Across Speakers of
American English, Italian, and Japanese
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

Felicia Roberts
Department of Communication
Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN

Piera Margutti
Department of Language Sciences
Università per Stanieri di Perugia, Perugia, Italy

Shoji Takano
Department of English
Hokusei Gakuen University, Sapporo, Japan

The fact that people with minimal linguistic skill can manage in unfamiliar or
reduced linguistic environments suggests that there are universal mechanisms of
meaning construction that operate at a level well beyond the particular structure or
semantics of any one language. The authors examine this possibility in the domain
of discourse by focusing on how gaps arising at the juncture between 2 persons’
turns-at-talk (inter-turn silences) are evaluated by speakers of typologically distinct
languages: English, Italian, and Japanese. This cross-linguistic design allows the
testing of both universal and relative aspects in orientation to silence. For this
study, the effects of inter-turn silence are tested using study participants’ ratings
of speakers’ willingness to comply with requests or agree with assessments that
were embedded in conversations. In a 3  2  3 between-groups design, 3 silence
lengths (0 ms, 600 ms, or 1200 ms) were crossed with 2 speech act types (requests

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Felicia Roberts, Department of


Communication, Purdue University, 2170 Beering Hall, West Lafayette, IN 47907, USA. E-mail:
froberts@purdue.edu

331
332 ROBERTS, MARGUTTI, TAKANO

and assessments) in manipulations of telephone conversations that were modeled


on an actual telephone call between friends. Native-speaking study participants, in
their home countries, provided ratings on Likert-type scales. Ratings significantly
decreased within each language group at longer inter-turn silences, indicating a
generalized response to the gaps; however, means were also significantly different
between groups, indicating different expectations for agreement.

Gathering meaning from others’ talk and embodied action is the ongoing busi-
ness of daily life. The fact that people with minimal linguistic skill can manage
even in very unfamiliar or reduced linguistic environments attests to the seamless
integration of social and cognitive skills for human meaning-making (Goodwin,
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

2002; Levinson, 2006). It also suggests that there are mechanisms for meaning
construction that are shared across languages and that operate at a level beyond
the particular structure or semantics of any one linguistic system. In this article,
we examine one such mechanism in the domain of discourse processing by
focusing on how gaps arising at the juncture of turns-at-talk (inter-turn silences)
are evaluated both similarly and differently by speakers of typologically distinct
languages. This approach allows us to engage both sociocultural and cognitive
perspectives, providing a necessary, if as yet preliminary, step toward fuller
studies of the relation between interactional norms and cognitive processing
of speech.
This project extends findings from a series of experiments on inter-turn
silence and prosody in American English (Roberts, Francis, & Morgan, 2006)
by replicating, for native speakers of Italian and Japanese, the design of the
final experiment in the English language project. Although additional prosodic
variables were of interest and were collected in the full replication, those findings
are reported elsewhere (Roberts & Margutti, 2008; Roberts & Takano, 2007). Our
main concern in this analysis is with perceptions of inter-turn silence, as that is a
robustly meaningful cue across the languages studied. Thus, this article draws on
subsets of the original (American English) data and subsequent replication efforts
in Italian and Japanese. This allows us to make cross-linguistic comparisons that
focus on inter-turn silence.
The reason for this focus is that inter-turn silence is a key component in the
construction and display of intersubjective understanding. This is particularly
salient in the context of “conditional relevance” (Schegloff, 1972), those points
in conversation where a first action (e.g. an invitation) establishes the expectation
of some next action or response (e.g., acceptance, rejection). When seamlessly
produced this machinery of mutual understanding goes unnoticed. From an
ethnomethodological standpoint, the accomplishment of intersubjectivity is, thus,
an “operation” (Garfinkel, 1967, p. 30); when there is even a brief gap between
two mutually relevant turns, intersubjectivity is no longer invisible. As demon-
strated in qualitative analyses of talk-in-interaction, participants orient to these
PERCEPTION OF INTER-TURN SILENCE ACROSS LANGUAGES 333

glitches as some form of trouble, as displayed in their pursuit of responses or


reformulations of prior talk (Davidson, 1984; Pomerantz, 1984).
This project is grounded in these descriptive findings and builds on them to
develop an experimental approach that is based on actual interaction. The aim is
to capture a possible overhearer’s perception of others’ conduct with regard to
inter-turn silence (cf., Fox Tree, 2002) and to directly compare these judgments
across language groups. This can help inform us as to the generality (or not) of
overhearers’ responses to disruptions in the forward motion of talk.1 Admittedly,
this is an “external view” of the phenomenon (Heritage, 1995, p. 406), not
an analysis of members’ methods for organizing the “occasions and resources
of understanding” (Schegloff, 1992, p. 1299). Nonetheless, the cross-linguistic
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

approach to the study of inter-turn silence affords a glimpse at one possible entry
point for understanding the seamless interrelation of the social/relative and the
cognitive/universal in language use (Levinson & Enfield, 2006). We begin by
reviewing these two approaches to silence, then provide a rationale for the choice
of the additional languages under study (Italian and Japanese), and finally turn
to the experimental design and study results.

CULTURAL, COGNITIVE, AND CONVERSATION


ANALYTIC CONTRIBUTIONS

Anthropologists and sociolinguists have described cultural differences in the


tolerance for silence in conversation, demonstrating and theorizing how those
differences can affect intercultural interactions (e.g., Basso, 1970; Clancy, 1986;
Lehtonen & Sajavaara, 1984; Nakane, 2006; Nwoye, 1985; Scollon & Scollon,
1979; Tryggvason, 2006; for edited collections about silence in conversation,
see Jaworski, 1997, and Tannen & Saville-Troike, 1985). Viewing orientations
to silence from the perspective of norms that are culturally relative and distinct,
these studies provide grounds for an assumption that members of different
language groups (as a surrogate for cultural groups) will differentially perceive
the valence of inter-turn silence (i.e., the degree of negative or positive attribution
to gaps in conversation).
Conversely, psychologists and psycholinguists have studied silence in terms
of response latency, primarily in the context of factual questions. In this pro-
cessing paradigm, latency is considered a cue to uncertainty or deception (for
an overview, see Andersen, 1999). This cognitive approach, built on the con-
ceptualization of latency to respond as a symptom or signal of processing effort

1 Our aim should not be confused with an analysis of the “meaning” of silence. Silence itself is

featureless (Levinson, 1983), taking on shades of meaning only in the context of social interaction.
334 ROBERTS, MARGUTTI, TAKANO

in finding an answer (Glucksberg & McCloskey, 1981; Smith & Clark, 1993),
assumes that answerers, in the moments of hesitancy, are both doing a search
and monitoring the search process (Nelson, 1993). The outward appearance of
this searching/monitoring process can be characterized as uncertainty (Brennan
& Williams, 1995) possibly construed by listeners as a clue to deception in the
making (Andersen, 1999) or conversely, simply thoughtfulness (Burgoon, Buller,
& Guerrero, 1995). This research suggests, although it is less cross-culturally
robust, that interpretations of inter-silence as “trouble” or thoughtfulness may
be grounded in generalized cognitive processes related to memory, retrieval,
integration of cues, and so on. Most of this cognitively oriented research has
studied silence (and disfluency) in terms of individual rather than collaborative
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

activity, although alternative approaches have been advocated (cf., Clark, 1994;
Schober & Bloom, 2004).
What differentiates this study from both anthropological and psychological
studies is that it entertains the possibility of a cultural difference in orientation
to silence, but examines it at the level of specific collaborative conversational
activities (i.e., requests and assessments). Rather than addressing the orientation
to silence as either culturally specific or cognitively generalized, we address
perception of silence in terms of both and, more important, in terms of par-
ticular discourse environments where specific socially appropriate actions are
expected. This is distinct from research on response latency and filled pauses
in which study participants are evaluating responses to factual questions (e.g.,
Brennan & Williams, 1995) or direct interrogatives concerning beliefs (e.g., Fox
Tree, 2002).
We base our design on the turn-taking model as empirically derived by
Sacks, Schegloff, and Jefferson (1974). From that vantage point, it is clear
that silence in conversation is a great deal more than an absence of speech.
Indeed, Conversation Analysts2 have demonstrated, as briefly noted earlier, that
when inter-turn silence grows subsequent to a speaker’s utterance that sets
up conditional relevance, it is indicative of possible trouble at that point in
the interaction (Davidson, 1984; Pomerantz, 1984). This trouble is empirically
available through speakers’ routine practice of producing “subsequent versions”
(Davidson, 1984, p. 104) of their turn at talk. The generalization that dispreferred
actions can be structured around inter-turn silence (i.e., realized as delays in
launching turns at talk) is borne out in qualitative studies of conversation in
typologically disparate languages such as Japanese (e.g., Mori, 1999; Tanaka,
2005), Finnish (e.g., Sorjonen, 1996), and Italian (e.g., Monzoni, 2007), to
name a few.

2 We capitalize Conversation Analyst to represent those scholars working within a particular

tradition that is rooted in phenomenological sociology and ethnomethodology. Heritage (1984)


explicates these intellectual roots and connections (see also Maynard & Clayman, 1991).
PERCEPTION OF INTER-TURN SILENCE ACROSS LANGUAGES 335

What is not known, however, is how particular courses of action in the context
of particular inter-turn silence lengths are perceived by native speakers of differ-
ent languages. Such comparative research will help us refine our understanding
of the extent to which interactional norms are part of the variable “vocabulary”
of language (Chomsky, 1995) or whether tolerances for specific lengths of
silence are universal and, therefore, have implications for understanding the
social (as well as cognitive) processing of speech. To explore these possibilities,
we proceeded with the design and implementation of this study based on the
following exploratory questions:

1. Do linguistic/cultural groups differ significantly in their judgments of


Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

addressees’ willingness to comply with requests or agree with assessments


as a function of inter-silence?
2. Do groups differ in their judgments of addressees’ willingness to comply
with requests or agree with assessments based on the sequential environ-
ment (i.e., requests vs. assessments) in the context of inter-turn silence?

The second question brings a unique perspective to the study of inter-turn silence.
Because we choose “adjacency pair” as the relevant level of analysis, we move
the study of silence away from descriptions of generalized norms and toward a
comparative investigation based on judgments concerning the same courses of
action (requests and assessments). We are, therefore, better positioned to draw
conclusions about the orientation to inter-turn silence at interactionally relevant
and comparable moments.

LANGUAGE GROUPS: WHY ITALIAN AND JAPANESE?

Stereotypes abound about the interpersonal styles of many linguistic/cultural


groupings. In terms of orientation to inter-turn silence (or propensity for over-
lapping talk) one relatively unexplored explanation for differing conversational
styles is suggested in Schegloff, Ochs, and Thompson (1996). They noted that
structures of projectabilty are likely different across typologically disparate
languages; and, therefore, the opportunities for overlap would be less or more
possible (pp. 28–29). They suggested a possible “confluence of grammar, culture
and turn-taking organization,” which could “raise the possibility : : : of early
entry [into a preceding turn] as a common practice : : : ” (p. 30). We chose,
therefore, to study speakers of typologically distinct languages, which also have
distinct conversational stereotypes attached to them, to provide an initial snapshot
of the degree to which culture/language background is a relevant construct in
orientation to inter-turn silence.
336 ROBERTS, MARGUTTI, TAKANO

Two disparately stereotyped groups, from an Anglo-centric standpoint, are


Italian speakers and Japanese speakers: One is characterized as voluble and
possibly less tolerant of silence (in that speakers are more prone to overlapping
talk), whereas the other is viewed as less voluble and more tolerant of silence.
These distinctly opposed stereotypes, for better or worse, have generated interest
among scholars, although with mixed results.
Italian literature on silence and its functions in conversation is scarce; the
general claim that Italians do not tolerate silence and that they tend to enter
conversation in overlap more frequently than speakers of other languages do
is often mentioned or implied, but rarely demonstrated. A prefatory chapter
in Bazzanella (2002) is promisingly dedicated to silence, but contains few
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

references to empirical studies. The studies of Italian that consider features of the
turn-taking system with respect to silence (Monzoni, 2004, 2007; Shultz, Florio,
& Erickson, 1982) provide contrasting analyses—one that draws on culture as an
explanation (Shultz et al., 1982) and one that draws on participation structure
(Monzoni, 2004, 2007). Nonetheless, these empirical studies do indicate that
there is some orientation to minimizing inter-turn silence among Italians in
multi-party settings, and that this tends to produce overlapping talk.
It is possible to draw quite a different conclusion about Japanese speakers.
Based on anthropological and sociolinguistic research, one could infer that the
Japanese differ from both Americans and Italians in their perceptions of silence.
Sociocultural norms have been described that stress both hierarchy and harmony
in interpersonal relationships (Watanabe, 1993). Thus, there may be potential
social and psychological preferences for agreement among Japanese speakers
along with a more generic structural preference (Pomerantz, 1984) for minimized
inter-turn gaps at sequentially relevant moments.3 Such a confluence could lead
to less tolerance for long gaps. Conversely, a strong underlying social orientation
to agreement might predict more tolerance for long gaps, as there might be a
presumed expectation for agreement; thus, delayed affirmative responses would
not necessarily elicit negative judgments.
In sum, from the few studies of Italian and Japanese that focus on silence,
it is not clear whether norms for orientation to silence are operating at the
level of participation structure or at a broader cultural level or whether, in fact,
there is any stable cultural norm. This study is certainly not the last word on
cultural orientation to silence, but it does provide a stable footing of reasonably

3 Although there is an important distinction in the literature between social and structural

understandings of preference, these are not mutually exclusive conceptions. Indeed, they may be
related under a broader understanding of affiliation or social solidarity. (On the distinctions between,
and relations among, these 2 conceptions of preference, see Heritage, 1984; Lerner, 1996; Levinson,
1983.)
PERCEPTION OF INTER-TURN SILENCE ACROSS LANGUAGES 337

controlled, consequential discourse environments (requests and assessments) on


which to test and build further investigation.

METHOD

Overview
We devised a series of telephone conversations that were based on actual tele-
phone calls between college-aged friends. These dialogues were then performed
by American, Italian, and Japanese university students who were native speakers.
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

Digital recordings were made for subsequent sound editing. We inserted inter-
turn silences between requests or assessments and the affirmative responses that
followed them. Undergraduates at study sites in their home countries were re-
cruited as study participants to listen to the dialogues and provide their judgments
(as ordinal ratings) of addressees’ willingness, within the recorded dialogues, to
comply with requests or agree with assessments.
A central challenge in designing this study was to control, as much as
possible, for the vocal qualities of the American, Italian, and Japanese speakers
who performed the dialogues. To fully account for any vocal differences between
the speakers, we would have had to enlist trilingual speakers to perform all of the
dialogues, but even this “matched-guise” approach (Lambert, Hodgson, Gardner,
& Fillenbaum, 1960) has been called into question (Bradac, Cargile, & Halett,
2001). That technique only works under the assumption that the multilingual
speaker is equally skilled in each language or language variety, when in fact the
speaker may present idiosyncratic differences of fluency in the various guises
that could still affect hearer judgments (Bradac et al., 2001, pp. 139–141). Thus,
we proceeded by controlling for production features within each language group,
as detailed later, but we did not control paralinguistic qualities to the extent of
finding one speaker who could perform in all three languages.

Study Participants
Native-speaking American English (n D 70), Italian (n D 72), and Japanese
(n D 80) undergraduate students, all living in their home countries at the time
of data collection, were recruited and compensated according to an approved
institutional review board protocol. The sample populations did not differ reliably
in age (American: M D 19.97, SD D 2.57; Italian: M D 21.96, SD D 2.60;
Japanese: M D 20.14, SD D 1.26), F(1, 217) D 2.35, ns; but they differed in
the representation of the genders. For the American English group, 60% were
women (n D 42); for the Italian group, 74% were women (n D 53); and for
the Japanese group, 84% were women (n D 67), 2 (1, N D 222) D 54.50,
338 ROBERTS, MARGUTTI, TAKANO

p < .01. However, as discussed later, gender did not affect results across or
within language groups.

Materials
To fully replicate the design of the earlier study of American English (Roberts
et al., 2006), the same nine dialogues were used as the foundation for the
Japanese and Italian study materials. The dialogues are based on the transcription
of the opening of an actual telephone conversation between two female American
college-aged friends (Roberts & Robinson, 2004). For this study, one native
speaker for each language group translated the dialogues from English into the
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

target languages. These were then back-translated into English by a second


native speaker from the target language groups. This was to check that the
dialogues were roughly equivalent in the different languages, yet still idiomatic
and comprehensible for their local contexts. Each conversation culminated with
a request or an assessment by the caller; the call recipient responded with an
affirmative one-word response (further details follow).
Of the nine dialogues, six were manipulated for purposes of the study and
three remained as they had been produced by the actors. These three dialogues
(“lures”) were interspersed among the six target stimuli to mask the manipula-
tions. Although the stimuli with long gaps clearly had a different flavor than the
un-manipulated dialogues, study participants had a variety of guesses concerning
the purpose of the study: In response to our verbally posed debriefing question,
“What do you think this study was about?,” study participants mostly said “tone
of voice” or “emotion in conversation,” but some (roughly 1=4 ) were able to
pinpoint that it was about “pauses” in speech. This elicitation of study aims
was done as a precursor to disclosing the actual aims of the study and was not
designed as part of the data collection. Thus, although our conclusion about
study participants’ awareness of study aims is impressionistic, we can say that
it was not generally obvious to participants that the study had targeted gaps in
conversation.

Construction and recording of dialogues. Each conversation, whether


target or lure, included a mundane request or assessment made by the caller.
These were about everyday topics concerning school flyers, going to the gym,
or picking up a new computer. In response to the requests and assessments
surrounding these three themes, the call recipient responded in the affirmative.
The target stimuli controlled for theme, but differed on speech act. For
example, if the theme of the call concerned flyers for a school function, it
ended with the caller either formulating a request in terms of that topic (e.g.,
getting a ride to pick up the flyers) or offering an opinion on the topic at
hand (e.g., reporting that the flyers look good). The call recipient answered in
PERCEPTION OF INTER-TURN SILENCE ACROSS LANGUAGES 339

the affirmative for both sequence types: In English, the token “sure” followed
requests, and “yeah” displayed agreement with the assessments. The roughly
equivalent tokens used for Italian were certo for “sure” and si for “yeah.” For
Japanese, ii yo was used for “sure” and soo da ne for “yeah.” Appendix A
contains examples in English and Appendix B contains the targeted translated
sequences along with notes on similarity of syntactic projectablity.
All of the dialogues (target and lure) were enacted by native-speaking students
in the same age range as the target study population. To control for gender and
voice quality, the target stimuli in each language were performed by the same
two women, who maintained the same caller-call recipient identity. Familiarity
in all of the conversations was maintained by features such as omission of the
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

caller’s name, use of an informal register, and/or truncating the greeting sequence
(Hopper, 1992; Schegloff, 1979). The person portraying the call recipient in
the recording was directed to make responses to requests and assessments
sound agreeable, but not unusually enthusiastic. The student actor was instructed
to maintain a normal register and to respond affirmatively, with no sense of
hesitation (i.e., to sound willing and agreeable in an everyday sort of way).
Clearly, one cannot control for all intervening variables when using human
voices; some voices just sound friendlier or more sincere or more needy and
those qualities can influence judgments. However, to perfectly match the acoustic
characteristics through machine produced language would seriously undercut
face validity and, as mentioned earlier, a matched-guise approach was neither
possible nor completely reliable. Our best approach was to competently direct
actors in their native languages to produce the kind of friendly, familiar demeanor
we were seeking. We did not undertake a full study, either before or after
choosing our actors, of whether their voices were considered “friendly” within
their own cultural contexts. Such a study might yield some reassurance that each
of the voices was, indeed, friendly, whatever that would mean in each culture,
but whether they were equally so across the groups is simply impossible to
calibrate or control. Readers concerned with issues of voice quality are invited
to request digital files of the experimental stimuli by contacting Felicia Roberts.

Agreement token manipulations. To control for possible confounding


from the acoustic qualities of the actors’ slightly different agreement token
pronunciations (during recording) we identified, following procedures detailed
in Roberts et al. (2006), two median agreement tokens (1 “yeah” and 1 “sure”
and their equivalents in the other languages) from all of the agreement tokens
produced by each actor. The ”sure” token and the ”yeah” token, which fell in the
middle in terms of duration, pitch range, and pitch change, were chosen as the
default agreement tokens and edited into the appropriate dialogues. In other
words, within each language condition the agreement tokens were identical
in each dialogue so that only the inter-turn silence between the request or
340 ROBERTS, MARGUTTI, TAKANO

assessment and the response differed. Thus, the acoustic quality of the responses
was fully controlled.

Inter-turn silence manipulations. Once the median agreement tokens were


edited into the dialogues, silences were inserted between the first pair part
(the request or assessment) and the agreement token (“sure” or “yeah”). Three
lengths of inter-turn silence were used to test the interaction between silence and
sequence type: 0 ms (no lag time), 600 ms, and 1200 ms. These lengths were cho-
sen to provide a baseline simulation of no gap between the request/assessment
and the affirmative response, and then equal increments leading up to Jefferson’s
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

(1989) proposed limit of “approximately 1 s” as a standard maximum of silence


in conversation. Silence was taken from other dead space in the dialogue (i.e.,
it was not a machine-produced silence) to best maintain the natural acoustic
environment. These natural silences were spliced to the end of phonation on the
request/assessment utterance as visually apparent from the sound wave.

Design
In a 3  2  3 mixed model with repeated measures, silence length (0 ms
gap, 600 ms, and 1200 ms) and sequence type (request and assessment) were
within-group factors; language group (English, Italian, or Japanese) was the
between-groups factor. These independent variables were manipulated in the
context of constructed dialogues that simulated telephone conversations between
two college-aged female friends. The dependent variable was rater perceptions
of an addressee’s willingness to comply with requests or agree with assessments
as elicited from a written question following each dialogue. Judgments were
captured using an ordered scale ranging from 1 (not willing or not in agreement)
to 6 (very willing and very much in agreement). Raters were encouraged to use
the whole scale, and some study participants circled adjacent values (i.e., they
could provide a rating of 2.5 by circling the 2 and the 3 on the scale).
Three different orders of presentation were used. These were counterbalanced
based on the inter-turn silence manipulations of the target stimuli. Admittedly,
this was not a full counterbalancing of sequence types (2 levels) and inter-turn
silence length (3 levels), which would have produced six presentation orders.
Given our recruitment and administration procedure (in classrooms of consenting
instructors; see the Procedure section) and no evidence from an item analysis
that ratings between the six counterbalanced orders were significantly different
for English (see Roberts et al., 2006, Experiment 1), we chose to take this
more economical path. Thus, across the three presentation orders, each inter-
turn silence appeared in each position an equal number of times, but not every
Speech Act  Gap combination was heard in every possible position.
PERCEPTION OF INTER-TURN SILENCE ACROSS LANGUAGES 341

The dialogues intended to mask the purpose of the study (lures) always
appeared in the same place in the audio presentation. Each presentation order
began with a masking dialogue to get study participants oriented to the task.
The other masking dialogues appeared fourth and seventh in the series of nine
conversations.
Each of the three audio presentation orders consisted of nine conversations:
Three ended with the assessment/response pairing (“I think it/they look[s] pretty
good,” followed by “Yeah”), which was separated by one of the three inter-turn
silence lengths (0 ms, 600 ms, or 1200 ms); three conversations ended with
the request/response pairing (“Can you give me a ride over there,” followed
by “Sure”), separated by the three inter-turn silence lengths; and three non-
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

target conversations ended with an assessment or request (also paired with the
affirmative response tokens “sure” and “yeah”), but they were un-manipulated.
The natural inter-turn silence, as originally produced by the actors, was left
intact for the non-target stimuli. Thus, nine stimuli were heard by each study
participant, and each Silence Length  Speech Act type manipulation was
heard once.

Procedure
Study participants were recruited by the study authors from classrooms where
instructors had given permission for the last 10 min of the class to be used
for student participation in “a study about communication.” Students who were
willing remained in the classroom and were given consent forms to review and
sign. Thus, from these classrooms, independent samples of convenience were
drawn. Different classrooms were used for each administration such that no
student ever participated twice.
For each sample group, students were given the same recorded instructions:
that they would be hearing “several telephone conversations among a group of
friends” and that each friend “was just relaxing at home.” The recording also
said that after each conversation there would be a question to answer about
the conversation. Including the consent process and debriefing, the process took
about 15 min. About 6 min were required for the actual administration of the
experimental protocol. Study participants (and the student actors who recorded
the dialogues) were compensated for their time.

RESULTS

The fact that the study participants were homogenous in terms of age (see the
Participants section) and that exploratory analysis revealed that their gender had
no significant effect on their judgments of the silences—F(1, 218) D 0.896,
342 ROBERTS, MARGUTTI, TAKANO

ns—analyses reported here do not include gender or age as covariates.4 In the


remainder of this section, therefore, we report results from an omnibus analysis
of variance (ANOVA), including calculation and interpretation of effect size
for relevant variables. Key interactions and major findings are then explored in
subsections. For ease of presentation, the inter-turn silence length variable is
referred to as “gap,” and the sequence type (request or assessment) is referred
to as “speech act.”
The ANOVA indicated statistically significant main effects for the three
independent variables: gap, F(2, 438) D 529.76, p < .01; language group, F(2,
219) D 51.82, p < .01; and speech act, F(1, 219) D 50.32, p < .01. The
interaction of gap and language group was statistically significant, F(4, 438) D
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

10.57, p < .01; as were the interactions of speech act and language group, F(2,
219) D 44.54, p < .01; and gap and speech act, F(2, 438) D 3.21, p < .01.
There was no three-way interaction (p D .381).
Effect size is one way of estimating the contribution of a particular factor to
an observed effect. Although the ANOVA indicated statistically significant main
and two-way interaction effects for all three independent variables, the effect
sizes, calculated using partial eta-squared (2p ), for Gap  Language (2p D .08)
and Gap  Act (2p D .01), were small. Although interpretation of effect sizes
is made cautiously, subject to several limitations (Cohen, 1988), it does appear
that gap length (2p D .70) and language group (2p D .32) contribute large
percentages (70% and 32%, respectively) of the overall variance. Speech act
accounts for 19% (2p D .19) and Sequence  Language Group is 29% (2p D .29).
Although these values are not additive (i.e., partial eta-squared does not provide
estimates that sum to 100%), they nonetheless provide an indication of the
contribution of each factor (or interaction) as if it were the only variable (Young,
1993). The salient result from the effect size calculations is that inter-turn silence
length (gap) has, by far, the strongest effect on study participants’ ratings of an
addressee’s willingness to comply with requests and agree with assessments.

American, Italian, and Japanese Raters All Judged Longer


Silences More Negatively, but Differed Sufficiently to Suggest
Disparate Cultural Orientations to the Inter-Turn Silences
As Figure 1 illustrates, regardless of language background, all raters judged
speakers to be less willing to comply with requests or agree with assessments the
longer the speaker paused before agreeing: linear trend, F(1, 219) D 794.08, p <
.01. This indicates that, across all three language groups, there is a decreasing
sense of speaker agreement or compliance with increased silence.

4 We tested this using an analysis of covariance model with all levels of silence length and speech

act as within-subjects variables, language group as a between-subject variable, and gender and age
as covariates.
PERCEPTION OF INTER-TURN SILENCE ACROSS LANGUAGES 343
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

FIGURE 1 Effect of inter-turn silence length on judgments of speakers’ agreement and


compliance (error bars represent standard error of the mean).

The effect of language group is also salient in Figure 1. Overall, the Japanese
study participants rated the speakers as more agreeable than either the Italian or
the American participants. Between-groups differences were statistically reliable
in post hoc comparisons (Bonferroni-corrected) across all gap conditions when
comparing the Japanese and American groups (Mdiff D 0.721, p < .01) and
the Japanese and Italian groups (Mdiff D 0.935, p < .01). The only statistically
reliable mean difference between the American and Italian raters was in the
600 ms gap condition (Mdiff D 0.46, p < .01).
The distinctly different ratings between the Japanese and the other groups
suggests that what constitutes “agreeable” in the context of conversational gaps
is calibrated slightly differently for them, and that they may be entering the
scale at a different point. The statistically reliable differences between all three
groups in the 600 ms gap condition suggests that a roughly 1=2 s gap may be
enough to distinguish cultural differences in orientation to inter-turn silence.

Italian Raters Judged the Smaller Inter-Turn Gaps as More


Problematic, Whereas the Japanese Judged the Longer
Inter-Turn Gaps as More Problematic
To further explore the interaction of gap length and language group, we examined
raters’ judgments of the inter-turn silences within their language group and then
compared the groups descriptively. Most notable is the comparison between the
344 ROBERTS, MARGUTTI, TAKANO

Japanese and the Italian raters. As visualized in Figure 1, the magnitude of the
drop in ratings by the Italians from 0 ms to 600 ms is double that of the Japanese,
whereas the drop in ratings for the Japanese is double that of the Italians when
comparing the 600 ms and 1200 ms gaps. This pattern of difference ratings
points to the possibility that the different language groups do not simply differ
in where they enter the scale of “agreeableness,” but may also have different
thresholds at which they find silence problematic.

Americans, Italians, and Japanese Respond Slightly


Differently to the Sequence Types (Requests vs. Assessments)
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

A unique contribution of our approach to the valence of inter-turn silence was


to examine the effects of sequence type (“speech act”) on raters’ judgments. Be-
cause most prior research on gaps in conversation is situated in the environment
of question-asking (factual/trivia questions), we chose to manipulate the stakes
of the face threat by using more naturalistic, collaborative, and consequential
social actions. To further refine our understanding of the significant main effect
of speech act and the interaction of speech act and language group, we explored
differences and similarities within each condition.
Overall, raters judged the request sequences as sounding more agreeable (i.e.,
the ratings were generally higher in this condition; see Table 1), but only the
Japanese rated requests substantially higher than they did assessments (Mdiff D
1.04, p < .01). For all of the groups, this is likely due to the relative strength of
the agreement carried in the different tokens (“sure” vs. “yeah,” as we discuss
further later).

TABLE 1
Mean Ratings for Each Language Group at Each Inter-Turn Silence Length
and for Each Sequence Type

Assessments Requests
Inter-Turn
Silence Length Language n M SD M SD

0 ms Japanese 80 4.08 1.61 5.26 0.70


English 70 3.96 1.05 4.14 0.98
Italian 72 3.92 1.15 4.21 0.96
600 ms Japanese 80 3.56 1.07 4.40 1.20
English 70 2.89 0.99 3.04 0.73
Italian 72 2.63 0.83 2.38 0.99
1200 ms Japanese 80 2.10 0.93 3.23 1.22
English 70 2.10 0.75 2.17 0.85
Italian 72 2.01 0.79 1.82 0.90
PERCEPTION OF INTER-TURN SILENCE ACROSS LANGUAGES 345

Although the comparison of raters’ judgments of the speech act types may
be obscured by the semantics of the different response tokens, the result for
the request condition between the three language groups is clear: Participants’
ratings are significantly different, F(2, 221) D 102.731, p < .01—a finding that
was reliable in all post hoc pairwise comparisons. This may reflect different
underlying cultural orientations to compliance with requests in general, but this
is an initial speculation and remains to be systematically explored in further
studies.
In sum, what seems to be driving the interaction effect of sequence type
and language group is the fact that (a) the Japanese participants rated requests
and assessments quite differently; and (b) between the three language groups,
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

primarily requests were rated significantly differently from one group to the next.

DISCUSSION

This study was designed to examine the effect of inter-turn silence and sequence
type (speech act) on native speakers’ judgments of an addressee’s willingness
to comply with requests or agree with assessments. We tested for these effects
across identical “friendly telephone conversations,” which were idiomatically
translated from American English for Italian and Japanese study participants.
The limitations of this exploratory experimental approach are acknowledged and
addressed, but we begin with the strengths of our design, a discussion of the main
findings, and their relevance for building research that could help synthesize the
contributions of cognitive and social approaches to discourse studies.
The basis for the experimental design was the empirically derived turn-taking
model as described in Sacks et al. (1974). Empirical research based on that
model indicates both a preference for progressivity in talk (Stivers & Robinson,
2006) and that gaps arising in the context of conditional relevance (utterances
designed for, and expecting, responses of some sort) will prove problematic for
speakers (Davidson, 1984; Pomerantz, 1984). What the prior research could not
help us to tease apart is the extent to which this perception of “trouble” would be
truly universal (all speakers responding the same to the same silence lengths) or
relative (cultural groups displaying different norms), or whether there was some
intersection of these two dimensions. Our aim, therefore, was to maximally
control for a variety of acoustic, thematic, and speech act factors so that we
could explore potential relations among language/cultural group, sequence type,
and inter-turn silence.
Among the American, Italian, and Japanese undergraduate students in our
sample, length of the inter-turn silent gap matters. Across language groups, the
longer the inter-turn silence length, the lower the ratings of willingness to comply
346 ROBERTS, MARGUTTI, TAKANO

with requests or agree with assessments. This supports emerging evidence of a


widely shared social orientation to silence as problematic (Stivers et al., 2009).
A socially oriented theory might posit that a preference for progressivity in
conversation is what underlies this “distaste” for the growing silence. A more
individualistic (cognitive) theory would claim that elapsed time reflects searching
and monitoring activity as participants spin out attributions in their minds. We
underscore the argument presented elsewhere (e.g., Clark, 1996; Levinson, 2006)
that these are complementary dimensions of analysis and should not be either/or
explanations. These levels must be integrated in future research as discourse
scholars become more familiar with and find ways to explore and test the notion
of a socially embedded interaction engine (Levinson, 2006). This study provides
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

an initial step in that direction.


Despite the negative judgments of longer silences across language groups,
results also indicate that the American, Italian, and Japanese raters differed in
their tolerance for the different gaps. By tolerance, we simply mean that ratings
differ between language groups in a way that reflects slightly different underlying
expectations for agreement (among peers, in this study) and for the rapidity with
which agreement is offered. The Japanese average ratings were consistently
highest on ratings of willingness and agreement, the Italians tended to give the
lowest ratings, and the American judgments were generally in between (although
tending closer to the Italian group’s ratings).
An alternative explanation for the different ratings among groups would be
based on an assumption that the different linguistic structures of the languages
differentially stall a projectable possible completion point (Schegloff et al.,
1996). Briefly, projectability refers to the hearer’s ability to anticipate that
a particular type of utterance is unfolding. Because the target stimuli in all
of the languages were similar on this dimension (see Appendix B), we were
confident that responses to the inter-turn silences were not based on differential
syntactic processing of the utterance underway. We suggest, therefore, that
projectability being equal in these dialogues it is the inter-turn silence itself
that is driving the disparity in judgments, not hesitation due to completion of
syntactic processing.
Thus, any group distinction in orientation to silence is not about differential
need for syntactic processing based on formal features of the language, but about
cultural expectations for the speed with which agreement is offered. If there is
a baseline sense that agreement is expected (e.g., in the Japanese context), it
makes sense that ratings of willingness and agreement among the Japanese are
less attenuated, as we found, from 0 ms to 600 ms. Conversely, if there is less
expectation of agreement, then as soon as some silence is heard, judgments are
affected and any additional increments, although still negatively valenced, are
perhaps less marked (e.g., in the Italian context, as we found). This pattern merits
further investigation using additional, and incremental, lengths of silence. In this
PERCEPTION OF INTER-TURN SILENCE ACROSS LANGUAGES 347

way, it might be possible to get a sense of a dose response and to more finely
tune the overall finding of culturally differentiated response patterns within a
universally obvious sense of decreasing agreement.
The fact that addressees’ responses to requests were rated as reflecting greater
willingness than responses to assessments is best explained by the fact that
their response to the requests (“sure”) is a stronger form of agreement than
the response used for the assessments (“yeah”; on the full character of weak
agreements, see Davidson, 1984, and Pomerantz, 1984). Thus, although the
response tokens were prosodically similar, they were semantically distinct. We
cannot conclusively state that it was the sequential environment itself (the
conditional relevance set up by the request or the assessment) that was driving
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

the perceptions. It could well be that the weaker form of agreement, which is
broader in scope, is also more ambiguous and, therefore, did not elicit equally
strong positive responses.
It is also possible that the Japanese study participants’ significantly higher
ratings of the “sure” condition (ii yo in Japanese) is simply reflecting an even
stronger distinction between the tokens ii yo and so da ne than for the English
“sure” and “yeah” or Italian certo and si. It would be possible to retest the
findings about sequence type using parallel terms for English and Italian (e.g.,
“yeah” and “si” for both requests and assessments), but it would be harder to
match them prosodically. For Japanese, other tokens such as “eh” might be
appropriate for gauging response to sequence type without risk of confounding
from the semantics of the agreement tokens. This is clearly an area for further
exploration within each language.
Whether the sequence types were tapping a semantic difference (based on
response token) or a pragmatic difference (based on assessment vs. request
condition) or even a difference in face threat, it is important to note that the
requests not only received consistently higher ratings across all groups and gap
lengths, but they also were more distinctive between groups. Because requests
can be considered face-threatening acts (i.e., in Brown & Levinson’s, 1987,
terms, a threat to negative face or the desire for autonomy), study participants
may be reflecting cultural attitudes about not infringing on others’ autonomy.
As a more obvious threat, this may have been a more salient concern than that
represented in the threat posed by an assessment (i.e., agreement with another’s
opinion).
In sum, the somewhat murky results for speech act open up the possibility
for further clarification of the relative weight of the threat posed by the utterance
versus the semantics of the response. Because all responses were affirmative,
we were able to isolate the effect of the inter-turn silence, which was clearly
the most powerful factor coloring study participants’ judgments; however, the
question does remain open as to the role of the response token itself in shading
response to the sequence types.
348 ROBERTS, MARGUTTI, TAKANO

Given our finding that ratings are more similar between all language groups
at 1200 ms than at 600 ms, there is reason to consider Jefferson’s (1989)
proposal of “approximately 1 s” (she recognized a span from 0.9 s–1.2 s) as a
possible maximum for silence in conversation. However, because our silences
were machine timed (and Jefferson was using an analogue procedure, measuring
silence in relation to the surrounding talk), it may be that in absolute terms we
overshot the mark. In as much as the Italian and American ratings were similar,
and the Japanese ratings remained significantly different from both of those
groups, we do not yet see strong support for the proposal.
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

Challenges for Cross-Linguistic Study of Discourse


There are numerous weaknesses in any experimental study of human social in-
teraction. One cannot control for all of the natural, in-the-moment inventiveness
and complexity of real conversation, particularly in a cross-linguistic study. One
can raise questions about the representativeness of the study population (un-
dergraduate students), voice comparability of the recorded speakers, recognize
possible differences in cultural preference/aversion to the types of requests and
assessments being made, and consider whether true translation of base dialogues
from English to the other languages is even possible. All of these caveats are
implicit in our discussion of findings and as limits to their generalizability.
Indeed, one could argue that the robust result concerning different cultural
responses to silence could simply be an artifact of the method; although the
dialogues were parallel in terms of semantic content, the quality of the voices
of the different speakers in the different languages may have had some effect.
Unfortunately, there is no body of acoustics literature comparing issues of “tone”
(e.g., friendliness of voice) in these languages, nor sufficient research on what
such acoustic correlates might be for each language. Thus, researchers must
privilege their native speaker auditory perception over acoustic measurements
until there are, if ever, reliable cross-cultural measures of the paralinguistic
correlates of speaker affect.
As noted in the Method section overview, only a matched-guise approach
using trilingual actors might have addressed such concerns about paralinguistic
comparability, but even that technique is not perfect (Bradac et al., 2001).
Multilingual speakers may not have full command over their affective orientation
to the languages they speak and may, therefore, have differences of fluency or
vocal quality in the various guises they are asked to perform, which then can
affect hearer judgments.
The most relevant and potentially feasible comparison to make, given the aims
of this study, would be to assess speaking rates because a silence might sound
long or short depending on the speed of the surrounding talk. However, speaking
rates are calculated on different units of analysis for the languages in this study:
PERCEPTION OF INTER-TURN SILENCE ACROSS LANGUAGES 349

Japanese is a mora-timed language, English is stress-timed language, and Italian


is generally considered a syllable-timed language; but such classifications are
not uncontroversial (see Fletcher, 2010).5
From our vantage point as scholars of language and social interaction, the
major weakness of this study is the reliance on contrived conversations and
assessment of external perceptions (listener judgments), rather than internally
motivated analyses of natural interaction. We recognize that our approach is not
a substitute for the internal proof procedures of qualitative analysis (Heritage,
1995) and that we tread dangerously by treating “sequence type” (request and
assessment) as an abstract category (i.e., a speech act), rather than as a particular
and real course of action (see Schegloff, 1995, pp. xxviii–xxix).
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

However, within the confines of the experimental method, we have worked


to ground the design in findings from studies of talk-in-interaction and based
the materials on actual telephone calls. Our aim was to investigate a measurable
and manipulable aspect of the operation of the turn-taking machinery (gaps)
within specific courses of action; we were not attempting to provide an account
or analysis of the course of action itself. We forced, in fact, a particular hearing
of the silences—one in which agreement was at stake rather than, for example,
friendliness or thoughtfulness or comfort or comprehension. We make no claim
that disagreement is the only interpretation that study participants might have
conjured from the silence.

CONCLUSION

There is a striking overall similarity of judgments in response to inter-turn silence


across American, Italian, and Japanese raters, yet there are also compelling fine-
grained differences between the groups. This lends support to furthering the
investigation of the complex interrelation among cognition, social practices, and
discourse processes (see, inter alia, Gee, 1992; Lave & Wegner, 1991; Levinson
& Enfield, 2006; Ochs, 1996; Rogoff, 1990; for discussions of usage-based
approaches to cross-linguistic studies of language universals, see Sidnell, 2007,
and Tomasello, 2003).

5 Because Italian and English are, theoretically, comparable on syllable units, we calculated

speech rates, post hoc, for the English and Italian speakers in our stimuli. They were remarkably
similar: 319 syllables per minute and 336 syllables per minute, respectively (using a measure of
speaking rate, not articulation rate.) Although we cannot compare this to a Japanese rate of speech
measure, we can say that the same semantic content was produced by that speaker in roughly
the same amount of time as the Italian rate of speech (7.35 s and 6.22 s, respectively.) Ultimately,
however, this analysis is vacuous because the Americans only used 4.8 s to produce the same stretch
of talk, yet their rate of speech is the same as the Italians. Thus, from various vantage points, speech
rate is a problematic comparison.
350 ROBERTS, MARGUTTI, TAKANO

Clearly, something generalized about human perceptual processing generates


an observable-reportable phenomenon of silence as indicative of trouble in
conversation. In turn, the valence of that silence (as good, bad, long, or short) is,
as has been argued, culturally conditioned. This type of Pan-human, ontological
basis for social cues has been strongly argued by researchers of non-vocal
communication, as in (albeit controversial) proposals on the universality of
several facial displays of emotion (Ekman, Sorenson, & Friesen, 1969), the
deployment of which is nonetheless governed by cultural display rules (e.g.,
Ekman & Friesen, 1975; Saarni, 1993).
Tolerance for silence is, of course, a dynamic factor that will significantly
vary depending on the types of communicative activities underway and the
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

identities being enacted in and through the deployment of silence. Although


we assume that norms are flexible and deployable-avoidable on an “as needed
basis” as resources for accomplishing and enacting identity and solidarity, we
also maintain that there is room for considering the more universal or generalized
cognitive underpinnings of these practices. This study allows us to move beyond
descriptive comparisons between languages to a position where the “adaptations
and inflections” of generic features of talk-in-interaction (Sidnell, 2007, p. 230)
can be specifically examined. This involves breaking down the artificial boundary
between social and cognitive. Although it may be somewhat tidier to study them
separately, that bifurcation is difficult to maintain as we move toward more
comprehensive understandings of human communication in terms of discourse
processes and processing.

REFERENCES

Andersen, P. A. (1999). Nonverbal communication: Forms and functions. Mountain View, CA:
Mayfield.
Basso, K. (1970). To give up on words: Silence in Western Apache culture. Southwestern Journal
of Anthropology, 26, 213–230.
Bazzanella, C. (2002). Le voci del silenzio [Voices of silence]. In C. Bazzanella (Ed.), Sul dialogo.
Contesti e forme di interazione verbale [On dialogue. Contexts and structures of verbal interaction]
(pp. 35–44). Milano, Italy: Edizioni Angelo Guerini e Associati SpA.
Bradac, J. J., Cargile, A. C., & Halett, J. (2001). Language attitudes: Retrospect, conspect, and
prospect. In H. Giles & P. Robinson (Eds.), The new handbook of language and social psychology
(pp. 137–155). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Brennan, S. E., & Williams, M. (1995). The feeling of another’s knowing: Prosody and filled pauses
as cues to listeners about the metacognitive states of speakers. Journal of Memory & Language,
34, 383–398.
Brown, P., & Levinson, S. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Burgoon, J. K., Buller, D. B., & Guerrero, L. K. (1995). Interpersonal deception: IX. Effects of
social skill and nonverbal communication on deception success and detection accuracy. Journal
of Language and Social Psychology, 14, 289–311.
PERCEPTION OF INTER-TURN SILENCE ACROSS LANGUAGES 351

Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. Boston, MA: MIT Press.


Clancy, P. M. (1986). The acquisition of communicative style in Japanese. In B. B. Schieffelin
& E. E. Ochs (Eds.), Language socialization across cultures (pp. 213–250). Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Clark, H. (1994). Managing problems in speaking. Speech Communication, 15, 243–250.
Clark, H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Davidson, J. (1984). Subsequent versions of invitations, offers, requests, and proposals dealing
with potential or actual rejection. In J. M. Atkinson & J. Heritage (Eds.), Structures of social
action: Studies in conversation analysis (pp. 102–128). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Press.
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the face: A field guide to recognizing emotions
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

from facial clues. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.


Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of
emotions. Science, 164, 86–88.
Fletcher, J. (2010). The prosody of speech: Timing and rhythm. In W. J. Hardcastle, J. Laver, &
F. E. Gibbon (Eds.), The handbook of phonetic sciences (pp. 523–602). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Fox Tree, J. E. (2002). Interpreting pauses and ums at turn exchanges. Discourse Processes, 34,
37–55.
Garfinkel, H. (1967). Studies in rthnomethodology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Gee, J. P. (1992). The social mind. London, UK: Bergin & Garvey.
Glucksberg, S., & McCloskey, M. (1981). Decisions about ignorance: Knowing that you don’t know.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 7, 311–325.
Goodwin, C. (2002). Conversational frameworks for the accomplishment of meaning in aphasia.
In C. Goodwin (Ed.), Conversation and brain damage (pp. 90–116). New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Heritage, J. (1984). Garfinkel and ethnomethodology. Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.
Heritage, J. (1995). Conversation analysis: Methodological aspects. In U. M. Quasthoff (Ed.), Aspects
of oral communication (pp. 391–418). Berlin, Germany: de Gruyter.
Hopper, R. (1992). Telephone conversation. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.
Jaworski, A. (Ed.). (1997). Silence: Interdisciplinary perspectives. Berlin, Germany: de Gruyter.
Jefferson, G. (1989). Preliminary notes on a possible metric which provides for a “standard maxi-
mum” silence of approximately one second in conversation. In D. Roger & P. Bull (Eds.), Con-
versation: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 166–196). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
Lambert, W., Hodgson, R., Gardner, R., & Fillenbaum, S. (1960). Evaluational reactions to spoken
languages. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60, 44–51.
Lave, J., & Wegner, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press.
Lehtonen, J., & Sajavaara, K. (1984). The silent Finn. In D. Tannen & M. Saville-Troike (Eds.),
Perspectives on silence (pp. 193–201). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Lerner, G. (1996). Finding “face” in the preference structures of talk-in-interaction. Social Psychol-
ogy Quarterly, 59, 303–321.
Levinson, S. C. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Levinson, S. C. (2006). On the human “interaction engine.” In S. C. Levinson & N. J. Enfield (Eds.),
Roots of human sociality (pp. 39–69). Oxford, UK: Berg.
Levinson, S. C., & Enfield, N. J. (Eds.). (2006). Roots of human sociality. Oxford, UK: Berg.
Maynard, D. W., & Clayman, S. E. (1991). The diversity of ethnomethodology. Annual Review of
Sociology, 17, 385–418.
Monzoni, C. M. (2004). Do Italians “prefer” disagreeing? Some interactional features of dispu-
tational talk in Italian multi-party family interaction. In K. Aijmer (Ed.), Understanding and
352 ROBERTS, MARGUTTI, TAKANO

misunderstanding in dialogue. Selected papers from the 8th IADA conference, Göteborg 2001
(pp. 119–130). Tübingen, Germany: Max Niemeyer Verlag.
Monzoni, C. M. (2007). Disputational talk in Italian multi-person family interaction. Unpublished
manuscript, University of York, UK.
Mori, J. (1999). Negotiating agreement and disagreement in Japanese: Connective expressions and
turn-construction. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.
Nakane, I. (2006). Silence and politeness in intercultural communication in university seminars.
Journal of Pragmatics, 38, 1811–1835.
Nelson, T. (1993). Metacognition: Core readings. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Nwoye, G. (1985). Eloquent silence among the Igbo of Nigeria. In D. Tannen & M. Saville-Troike
(Eds.), Perspectives on silence (pp. 185–191). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Ochs, E. (1996). Linguistic resources for socializing humanity. In J. J. Gumperz & S. C. Levinson
(Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity (pp. 407–437). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

Press.
Pomerantz, A. (1984). Agreeing and disagreeing with assessments: Some features of preferred/
dispreferred turn shapes. In J. M. Atkinson & J. Heritage (Eds.), Structures of social action:
Studies in conversation analysis (pp. 57–101). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Roberts, F., Francis, A. L., & Morgan, M. (2006). The interaction of inter-turn silence with prosodic
cues in listener perceptions of “trouble” in conversation. Speech Communication, 48, 1079–1093.
Roberts, F., & Margutti, P. (2008, March). The interaction of inter-turn silence, sequence type, and
prosody in American and Italian perceptions of trouble in interaction. Paper presented at the 30th
annual convention of the American Association of Applied Linguistics, Washington, DC.
Roberts, F., & Robinson, J. D. (2004). Interobserver agreement on first-stage conversation analytic
transcription. Human Communication Research, 30, 376–410.
Roberts, F., & Takano, S. (2007, May). The interaction of inter-turn silence and prosody in American
and Japanese perceptions of trouble in interaction. Paper presented at the 57th annual convention
of the International Communication Association, San Francisco, CA.
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New York,
NY: Oxford University Press.
Saarni, C. (1993). Socialization of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of
emotions (pp. 435–446). New York, NY: Guilford.
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of
turn-taking for conversation. Language, 50, 696–735.
Schegloff, E. A. (1972). Sequencing in conversational openings, In J. J. Gumperz & D. H. Hymes
(Eds.), Directions in sociolinguistics (pp. 346–380). New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
Schegloff, E. A. (1979). Identification and recognition in telephone conversation openings, In G.
Psathas (Ed.), Everyday language: Studies in ethnomethodology (pp. 23–78). New York, NY:
Irvington.
Schegloff, E. A. (1992). Repair after next turn: The last structurally provided defense of intersub-
jectivity in conversation. American Journal of Sociology, 97, 1295–1345.
Schegloff, E. A. (1995). Introduction. In G. Jefferson (Ed.), Lectures on conversation (Vol. 1, pp. ix–
lxii). Cambridge, UK: Blackwell.
Schegloff, E. A., Ochs, E., & Thompson, S. A. (1996). Introduction. In E. Ochs, E. A. Schegloff,
& S. A. Thompson (Eds.), Interaction and grammar (pp. 1–51). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Schober, M. F., & Bloom, J. E. (2004). Discourse cues that respondents have misunderstood survey
questions. Discourse Processes, 38, 287–308.
Scollon, R., & Scollon, S. B. K. (1979). Linguistic convergence: An ethnography of speaking at
Fort Chipewyan, Alberta. New York, NY: Academic.
Shultz, J. J., Florio, S., & Erickson, F. (1982). Where’s the floor? Aspects of the cultural organization
of social relationships in communication at home and in school. In P. Gilmore & A. Glatthorn
PERCEPTION OF INTER-TURN SILENCE ACROSS LANGUAGES 353

(Eds.), Children in and out of school. Language and ethnography Series #2 (pp. 88–123).
Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.
Sidnell, J. (2007). Comparative studies in conversation analysis. Annual Review of Anthropology,
36, 229–244.
Smith, V. L., & Clark, H. H. (1993). On the course of answering questions. Journal of Memory &
Language, 32, 25–38.
Sorjonen, M. (1996). On repeats and responses in Finnish conversations. In E. Ochs, E. A. Schegloff,
& S. A. Thompson (Eds.), Interaction and grammar (pp. 277–327). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Stivers, T., Enfielf, N. J., Brown, P., Englert, C., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T., et al. (2009). Universals
and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 106, 10587–10592.
Stivers, T., & Robinson, J. D. (2006). A preference for progressivity in interaction. Language in
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

Society, 35, 367–392.


Tanaka, H. (2005). Grammar and the “timing” of social action: Word order and preference organi-
zation in Japanese. Language in Society, 34, 389–430.
Tannen, D., & Saville-Troike, M. (1985). Perspectives on silence, Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Tomasello, M. (2003). Introduction: Some surprises for psychologists. In M. Tomasello (Ed.), The
new psychology of language: Cognitive and functional approaches to language structure (pp. 1–
14). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Tryggvason, M. (2006). Communicative behavior in family conversation: Comparison of amount
of talk in Finnish, Swedish Finnish, and Swedish families. Journal of Pragmatics, 38, 1795–
1810.
Watanabe, S. (1993). Cultural differences in framing: American and Japanese group discussions. In
D. Tannen (Ed.), Framing in discourse (pp. 176–209). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Young, M. A. (1993). Supplementing tests of statistical significance: Variation accounted for. Journal
of Speech & Hearing Research, 36, 644–656.

APPENDIX A
Examples of a Request and an Assessment in English

1. (Telephone rings)
A: Hello?
B: Rachel?
A: Yeah,
B: Hey it’s me.
A: Hey:: How’s it goin.
B: Good. I: just called the copy shop,
A: uh huh
B: An:d the flyers are ready. Can you give me a ride over there?
A: Sure.

Question: How willing is Rachel to give her friend a ride?

Scale: 1 2 3 4 5 6
354 ROBERTS, MARGUTTI, TAKANO

2. (Telephone rings)
A: Hello?
B: Rachel?
A: Yeah,
B: Hey it’s me.
A: Hey.:: How’s it goin.
B: Good. I just saw the flyers. They look pretty good.
A: Yeah.

Question: How much does Rachel agree with her friend about the flyers?
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:59 17 August 2011

Scale: 1 2 3 4 5 6

APPENDIX B
Examples of English, Italian, and Japanese Glosses of Targeted
Request and Assessment Formulations

1. Request
“I just called the copy shop and the flyers are ready : : : ”
English: Can you give me a ride over there?
Italian: Ti va di darmi un passaggio in macchina?
Japanese: Kuruma ni nosete tte kurenai?
2. Assessment
“I just saw the flyers : : : ”
English: They look pretty good.
Italian: Mi sembrano belli.
Japanese: Yoku dekiteru yo ne.

In all three languages, the listener could likely guess that a request is coming
prior to the end of the turn. In English, “Can you give me : : : ” projects a
yes/no request of some sort early in the turn, as do the formulations “ti va di”
(glossable as, “Is it okay/convenient for you”) in Italian and after “nosete” in
Japanese (glossable as get/give a ride). Likewise, for the assessments, in Italian
after “mi sembrano : : : ” (glossable as “Seems to me”), it could be clear that
an assessment is being formulated; and in Japanese, perhaps right after “Yoku”
(“Well”), but certainly after “dekiteru” (“being done”).
From this, we conclude that projectability, from both grammatical and prag-
matic standpoints, is equivalent in the three languages.

You might also like