You are on page 1of 12

Original Paper

Brain Behav Evol 2018;91:136–147 Published online: August 10, 2018


DOI: 10.1159/000488889

Human Paleoneurology and the


Evolution of the Parietal Cortex
Emiliano Bruner
Centro Nacional de Investigación sobre la Evolución Humana, Burgos, Spain

Keywords cortex must be interpreted as a derived feature. Spatial mod-


Brain evolution · Hominids · Parietal lobes · Precuneus · els suggest that the dorsal and anterior areas of the precu-
Visuospatial integration neus might be involved in these derived morphological vari-
ations. These areas are crucial for visuospatial integration,
and are sensitive to both genetic and environmental influ-
Abstract ences. This article reviews almost 20 years of my collabora-
Paleoneurology deals with the study of brain anatomy in fos- tions on human parietal lobe evolution, integrating func-
sil species, as inferred from the morphology of their endocra- tional craniology, paleoneurology, and evolutionary neuro-
nial features. When compared with other living and extinct anatomy. © 2018 S. Karger AG, Basel
hominids, Homo sapiens is characterized by larger parietal
bones and, according to the paleoneurological evidence,
also by larger parietal lobes. The dorsal elements of the pos-
terior parietal cortex (superior parietal lobules, precuneus, Paleoneurology and Functional Craniology
and intraparietal sulcus) may be involved in these morpho-
logical changes. This parietal expansion was also associated Morphology is not only a study of material things
with an increase in the corresponding vascular networks, and of the forms of material things, but has its dynamical
aspect, under which we deal with the interpretation,
and possibly with increased heat loads. Only H. sapiens has a in terms of force, of the operations of energy.
specific early ontogenetic stage in which brain form achieves D’Arcy Wentworth Thompson
such globular appearance. In adult modern humans, the pre- (On Growth and Form, 1942)
cuneus displays remarkable variation, being largely respon-
sible for the longitudinal parietal size. The precuneus is also Traditionally, paleoneurology has been defined as the
much more expanded in modern humans than in chimpan- study of fossil endocasts, namely the casts of the cranial
zees. Parietal expansion is not influenced by brain size in fos- cavity in extinct species [Falk, 1987; Holloway et al.,
sil hominids or living primates. Therefore, our larger parietal 2004]. The tight contact between brain and braincase, and

© 2018 S. Karger AG, Basel Emiliano Bruner


Centro Nacional de Investigación sobre la Evolución Humana
Paseo Sierra de Atapuerca 3
E-Mail karger@karger.com
ES–09002 Burgos (Spain)
www.karger.com/bbe
E-Mail emiliano.bruner @ cenieh.es
the fact that the vault bones are largely shaped by the skull exerts important constraints on the brain form at
brain during growth, makes it possible to provide some the three endocranial fossae. The frontal lobes are con-
inferences from skull (endocranial) morphology on brain strained by the orbital structures, the temporal lobes are
anatomy [Bruner, 2017a]. This is particularly relevant constrained by the facial block and by mandibular ele-
when dealing with species for which we only have bone ments, and the cerebellum is constrained by the cranial
remains. Brain tissues do not fossilize, but the endocra- base flexion. Therefore, when handling endocasts, paleo-
nial cavity can supply information on brain size and pro- neurology must carefully consider not only brain anato-
portions, as well as on sulcal patterns, and in the past cen- my but also the general spatial and morphogenetic rela-
tury these have been the most investigated paleoneuro- tionships between skull and brain, so as to determine
logical features. On the endocranial wall, there are also what changes of the brain form are due to brain evolution,
the imprints of some vascular elements, such as the mid- and what are secondary outcomes due to cranial influ-
dle meningeal artery and the venous sinuses. The endo- ences. Both kinds of variations are relevant in evolution-
cranial cavity provides this information by itself, but ary anthropology, but only the former might possibly be
nonetheless paleoneurology (which should be more associated with neural (and ultimately cognitive) varia-
properly termed paleoneurobiology) largely deals with its tions.
positive mold, the endocranial cast. After all, we are pri-
mates, and we are used to relying on our eyes and hands
to interact with our environment. That is why we prefer Parietal Lobes and the Study of Brain Evolution
to have “something to see and to handle,” instead of an
empty cavity. Of course, much information is lost from a Parietal lobes have interested paleoanthropologists for
real brain to its endocranial cast, which is a sort of geo- a long time. In the early days of the discipline, almost one
metrical model able to supply only some scattered infor- century ago, Raymond Dart and Franz Weidenreich al-
mation on cerebral gross cortical features and general ready mentioned morphological differences of the pari-
proportions. Furthermore, the meninges and the cere- etal region when studying australopithecines and Homo
brospinal fluid are interposed between the cerebral cortex erectus, respectively. Ralph Holloway [1981] was proba-
and the internal table of the braincase, separating their bly the first to perform an endocranial shape analysis on
respective surfaces and smoothing the endocranial im- living and fossil hominoids, evidencing a remarkable pa-
prints, most of all in larger skulls [Kobayashi et al., 2014; rietal variability among different species. In humans, dis-
Van Minh and Hamada, 2017]. Yet, despite these limita- tinctive parietal traits have been known since the first half
tions, this is the only direct information we can have on of the 20th century also in neurobiology [Catani et al.,
the brains of extinct taxa and, therefore, deserves atten- 2017], and advances in the study of the parietal cortex
tion. have been stimulating and promising [Mountcastle,
However, not every endocranial variation is due to 1995]. Nonetheless, the posterior parietal cortex was, un-
brain anatomy. Even the word “brain” is a bit misleading til recently, an “uncharted region” [Zilles and Palomero-
and very general when dealing with morphology. Its size Gallagher, 2001], and parietal lobes have never been tak-
and shape depends on neurons but also upon glial cells, en into consideration when dealing with key changes in
cerebrospinal fluid, and blood vessels. Furthermore, its human evolution.
form and geometry are not a real property of the organ There could be at least 5 main reasons for this. First, a
itself, but are largely due to internal blood pressure and large part of the parietal cortex is not superficial but po-
external meningeal tension [Moss and Young, 1960]. Ad- sitioned under the cerebral surface (precuneus and intra-
ditionally, the intimate spatial relationship between skull parietal sulcus). These folds are more difficult to discern
and brain makes influences between bone and cortex re- in terms of dissection and visualization, and they can pass
ciprocal, and the final phenotype is hence shaped by the unnoticed in many observations and surveys. Second,
effects of both soft and hard tissues. In terms of both on- this deep position makes damage to these areas less com-
togeny an phylogeny, skull and brain must co-evolve, fol- patible with survival after impairment. Functional dam-
lowing a principle of structural and functional balance age – important in detecting an association between
[Richtsmeier et al., 2006; Bruner, 2015]. In the dorsal re- structure and function in the past centuries – is less easy
gions (vault), the brain largely molds the bones because to observe in these deep cortical areas when compared
its growth processes are compensated by bone deposition with lesions in other regions (e.g., language or personal-
and modeling. However, in the ventral regions (base), the ity alterations after frontal injury). Third, the parietal cor-

Evolution of the Parietal Lobes Brain Behav Evol 2018;91:136–147 137


DOI: 10.1159/000488889
tex is generally labeled with the elusive term “association ture is probably simply a spatial rearrangement due to the
cortex,” which denotes multiple and integrative func- peculiar position of our face situated under the anterior
tions. The parallel involvement of many distinct func- cranial fossa [Beaudet and Bruner, 2017; Pereira-Pedro et
tions makes experiments and inferences less straightfor- al., 2017a]. Instead, shape analyses revealed that our glob-
ward. Fourth, many parts of the parietal lobes are associ- ular vault is largely due to the size and proportions of the
ated with visuospatial integration which, in general, refers parietal bones [Bruner et al., 2004]. In fact, Homo sapiens
to a set of functions traditionally interpreted as basic spa- has a parietal bone which is larger and more bulging than
tial management, something that was not expected to be in any other human species [Bruner et al., 2011a]. This
involved in “higher” cognitive functions. Fifth, humans feature is not due to the large size of the braincase, and in
and nonhuman primates display many anatomical differ- fact the Neanderthals, with a similar cranial capacity, dis-
ences which often hamper a direct comparative approach. played short and flat parietal bones [Bruner, 2014]. The
Patent inconsistencies between the monkey and human longitudinal size and curvature of the parietal bones do
parietal cortical maps were described since Brodmann’s apparently not influence the position of other endocra-
studies back in 1909 [Zilles and Palomero-Gallagher, nial regions, but have an effect on the general orientation
2001; Zilles and Amunts, 2010]. This lack of correspon- of the skull, thus influencing the general head position
dence, instead of stimulating the study of these areas, may and its functional axis [Bruner et al., 2017a].
have paradoxically demoted interest in these cortical ele- Moving from skull to brain, the boundaries of the
ments. parietal lobes can be tentatively recognized on endocasts
At the end of the 1990s, I began applying shape analy- [Pereira-Pedro and Bruner, 2018]. However, while mor-
sis and multivariate statistics to paleoanthropology and phometrics of the bone are based on fixed and recogniz-
craniology, and it soon became apparent that parietal able craniometric landmarks (sutures), morphometrics
bone shape was characteristic in our own species. Parietal of the lobe must be necessarily performed on anatomical
bone shape differences could have been only a matter of inferences. Sulcal imprints are faint and smooth, and
cranial architecture, but then I found similar results when their localization is based on the experience of the anato-
working with endocasts, brains, or vessels. I was surprised mists. Nonetheless, the localization of cortical references
to find an unexpected paucity of information on many on endocasts relies not only on the recognition of the spe-
cranial, cerebral, and vascular traits associated with pari- cific grooves or bosses on the endocranial surface, but
etal regions, so I began to investigate not only fossils but also on the relative position of other surrounding cortical
also living humans, not only hominoids but also other elements. The use of sample or hemispheric averages can
primates, not only evolutionary aspects but also medical further limit this anatomical uncertainty, providing a
ones, not only the isolated features but also their relation- rough but reasonable estimation of the position of the
ships. This article is hence a review of almost 20 years of central sulcus, postcentral sulcus, parieto-occipital sul-
my personal collaborations on the evolution of parietal cus, intraparietal sulcus, supramarginal gyrus, and angu-
anatomy, a long trail that did not aim to look for answers lar gyrus. According to such estimations, modern hu-
but instead search for proper questions. mans are not only characterized by larger parietal bones
but also by larger parietal lobes (Fig.  1) [Bruner et al.,
2003, 2018a; Bruner, 2004]. Spatially, this difference is ap-
The Fossil Evidence: Parietal Bones, Lobes, parently associated with the dorsal parts of the parietal
and Vessels cortex, possibly the superior parietal lobules or the intra-
parietal sulcus [Bruner, 2010]. A similar situation can be
The modern human skull has always been defined as described when comparing the endocasts of living homi-
“globular” when compared with extinct human species, noids, with modern humans displaying a remarkable ex-
which instead display a flatter braincase [Lieberman et al., pansion of the superior parietal lobule (Fig. 2).
2002]. The frontal bone can partially contribute to this An increase in the size and proportions of the parietal
globular shape because of a more pronounced curvature bone can be explained as an increase in the underlying
of the frontal squama. However, there is no evolutionary brain elements (larger parietal cortex) or, alternatively, as
evidence of macroscopic morphological changes in the a spatial readjustment of the cranial architecture (i.e.,
modern human frontal lobes or in the frontal endocra- flexion and curvature of the skull) with no major changes
nial profile [Bookstein et al., 1999; Semendeferi et al., in brain proportions. However, in the case of an increase
2002; Bruner, 2017b], and therefore such frontal curva- in the parietal lobe, the latter explication does not stand.

138 Brain Behav Evol 2018;91:136–147 Bruner


DOI: 10.1159/000488889
a b c

d e f

Fig. 1. When compared with all the other fossil hominids, Homo eas), and modern humans are further characterized by a general
sapiens’ rounded skull (a), in terms of midsagittal geometry (b), is longitudinal expansion of the same region (SD, sagittal dorsal ar-
largely due to absolute and relative enlargement of the parietal eas). The former variation spatially matches the position of the
bone (c – digital replica of skull and endocast of an australopith, intraparietal sulcus and superior parietal lobules, while the latter
Sts5, and shape deformation associated with modern human cra- corresponds to the position of the superior parietal lobules and
nial form). Some anatomical references of the parietal lobe (cs, precuneus. Neanderthals and modern humans share a similar cra-
central sulcus; pos, parieto-occipital sulcus; ips, intraparietal sul- nial capacity, but the latter shows a general enlargement of the pa-
cus; smg, supramarginal gyrus; ag, angular gyrus) can be tenta- rietal lobes (f – digital replica of skull and endocasts of the Nean-
tively inferred on endocasts (d). When compared with more ar- dertal Saccopastore 1, and shape deformation associated with
chaic human species like Homo erectus (e), Neanderthals display a modern human endocranial anatomy). Images after Bruner [2004,
lateral bulging of the dorsal parietal surface (LD: lateral dorsal ar- 2010, 2014] and Bruner et al. [2004, 2014a, 2015c].

A volumetric change of a specific cortical region cannot nial vascular network as far as we can see for the menin-
be interpreted as a geometric inflation due to spatial cur- geal and diploic vessels [Bruner et al., 2005; Bruner and
vature and is more likely to be associated with the increase Sherkat, 2008; Rangel de Lázaro et al., 2016]. Vascular
in some cerebral components (neurons, connections, morphology can show important individual variation,
glia, etc.) included within those boundaries. and the mechanisms and factors involved in such diver-
Additional evidence comes from the vascular system: sity are still not known. However, all extinct human spe-
only H. sapiens has a complex and reticulated endocra- cies display simpler vascular networks when compared

Evolution of the Parietal Lobes Brain Behav Evol 2018;91:136–147 139


DOI: 10.1159/000488889
Fig. 2. A principal component analysis on endocranial 3D coordi- uncertainty due to landmark localization (data after Pereira-Pedro
nates from hominoid endocasts using parietal lobe landmarks and Bruner [2018]). Parietal landmarks: ab, angular boss; cs, cen-
shows a PC1 (48% of the total variation) separating humans from tral sulcus (midline estimation); ls, posterior end of the lateral sul-
all apes (above) because of parietal lobe enlargement (middle; red cus; pos, parieto-occipital sulcus (midsagittal estimation); sb, su-
wireframe shows the shape change toward humans). If we con- pramarginal boss. Species: BON, Pan paniscus; CHIM, Pan troglo-
sider only the parietal landmarks (below), PC1 (67%) separates dytes; GIB, Hylobates lar; GOR, Gorilla gorilla; HUM, Homo
again humans from apes because of the enlargement of the dorsal sapiens; ORA, Pongo pygmaeus; SIA, Symphalangus syndactylus.
and anterior regions of the parietal lobe. In this example, endocasts Morphometrics was computed with MorphoJ 1.06d [Klingenberg,
from 1 representative individual of each species (adult males) were 2011].
used, averaging 10 replicas and the 2 hemispheres, so as to limit

with modern humans, with almost no anastomotic con- cause the endocranial vascular system may have a central
nections between the main branches. Apparently, vascu- role in brain thermoregulation [Bruner et al., 2011b]. Nu-
lar complexity does not depend on brain size, and it par- merical simulations showed that the dorsal parietal sur-
ticularly concerns the parietal surface. This is relevant be- face in more platycephalic species is important for endo-

140 Brain Behav Evol 2018;91:136–147 Bruner


DOI: 10.1159/000488889
cranial heat exchange, and the bulging of the parietal brain geometry in adult humans, the most variable fea-
region characteristic of modern humans can hence con- ture regards the precuneus longitudinal extension [Brun-
tribute to increase the heat load of the corresponding er et al., 2014b]. Namely, adult individuals show a notice-
deeper cerebral areas [Bruner et al., 2012, 2014a]. able anatomical diversity because of the length of the pre-
Ontogenetic analysis of the endocranial form suggest- cuneus. Differences mainly deal with the dorsal regions,
ed that modern human brain globularity is achieved ear- and enlargement of the precuneus is associated with in-
ly postnatally [Neubauer et al., 2009] or even prenatally crease in its cortical surface area [Bruner et al., 2015b].
[Ponce de León et al., 2016]. Either way, it is due to a mor- Parietal lobes generally present larger size, more connec-
phogenetic stage which is specific to H. sapiens and absent tions, and larger surface area in males than females, a dif-
in chimps or Neanderthals [Gunz et al., 2010; Neubauer ference associated with distinct visuospatial performance
et al., 2010; Scott et al., 2014]. In sum, head and brain [Koscik et al., 2009]. Nonetheless, the overall precuneus
globularity in our species is largely due to parietal lobes morphology apparently does not depend on brain size,
and bones, and associated with a species-specific growth sex, hemisphere, or geographic ancestry [Bruner et al.,
period. As far as the paleoneurological evidence can show, 2017b]. The individual differences in sulcal variability
such morphological differences are definitely more ap- may also be noticeable in these areas because of a variety
parent than many subtle or individual variations com- of precuneal and subparietal sulci, and precuneus expan-
monly debated in paleoanthropology or evolutionary sion is partially associated with additional folding ele-
neuroanatomy. They should deserve, at least, attention. ments [Bruner et al., 2017b]. Precuneus differences al-
most entirely occur along its longitudinal and vertical size
and extensions, influencing the dorsal midsagittal mor-
The Living Evidence: Parietal Lobe Morphology phology of the brain but not particularly the lateral (para-
sagittal) brain dimensions [Pereira-Pedro and Bruner,
Parietal lobes are located below the parietal bones, al- 2016]. Actually, precuneus lateral development (mainly
though the respective size and positions may vary [Brun- due to the lateral extension of the subparietal sulcus) is
er et al., 2015a]. The parieto-occipital sulcus, separating less variable and does not apparently influence the exter-
the parietal and occipital cortex, is more stable in its cra- nal brain form.
nial location, roughly close to the boundary between the Interestingly, if we compare the midsagittal brain
parietal and occipital bones (lambda). In contrast, the po- morphology in humans and chimpanzees, we also find
sition of the anterior boundary of the lobe (central sulcus that their main difference concerns the extension of the
and postcentral gyrus) is more variable, and it becomes precuneus, which is much larger in our species [Bruner et
relatively closer to the boundary of the frontal and pari- al., 2017c]. This difference seems, again, to be localized in
etal bones (bregma) in brains with larger parietal lobes. the dorsal and anterior part of the precuneus, a region
Although these positions and proportions are influenced that matches the area 7a according to Scheperjians et al.
by many factors and distinct ontogenetic processes, there [2008]. This area includes the anterior portion of the me-
is still a significant correlation between parietal bone and dial surface of the precuneus but also the external dorsal
lobe length even among adult humans. In adults, the form surface of the superior parietal lobules.
of the parietal bone and lobe is integrated with the form In terms of spatial correspondence, the morphological
of the occipital bone and lobe [Gunz and Harvati, 2007; changes associated with the precuneus at intra- and inter-
Bruner et al., 2018b], and bulging of one of the two re- specific levels match the parietal bulging described in the
gions is associated with the flattening of the other. In con- endocranial evolution of modern humans [Bruner et al.,
trast, the size of the parietal lobe is inversely correlated 2014a].
with the size of the frontal and temporal lobes, and not It can be argued that modern human parietal expan-
correlated with the size of the occipital cortex [Allen et al., sion may be due to some intrinsic allometric trend of the
2002]. This may suggest more structural constraints with brain and proportional scaling due to encephalization.
the occipital region and more functional constraints with However, the available evidence largely rejects this pos-
frontal and temporal regions – an entangled morphoge- sibility. In fact, concerning the fossil record, Neander-
netic position, indeed. thals had a brain size comparable with ours but no pari-
A key element of the dorsal parietal cortex is the pre- etal longitudinal expansion [Bruner et al., 2003; Bruner,
cuneus, which displays remarkable morphological vari- 2004; Gunz et al., 2010]. Concerning nonhuman pri-
ability (Fig. 3). When dealing with the whole midsagittal mates, the precuneus is particularly variable within other

Evolution of the Parietal Lobes Brain Behav Evol 2018;91:136–147 141


DOI: 10.1159/000488889
b

Fig. 3. The vertical extension of the precuneus (a) is responsible for the main coronal variation among adult hu-
mans, strictly influencing the height of the dorsal parietal cortex (b). The width of the precuneus (c) is, in contrast,
less variable. The length of the dorsal part of the precuneus is also responsible for the main midsagittal brain shape
differences among adult humans, because of variations in its cortical surface area (d). A similar shape change also
represents the main difference between humans and chimps (e, f). Data after Bruner et al. [2014b], Pereira-Pedro
and Bruner [2016], Bruner et al. [2017b]. Morphometrics was computed with PAST 2.17c [Hammer et al., 2001].

species too, but there are no consistent differences in size et al., 2010] which, implicitly, should mean a larger pari-
and proportions between different species, including be- etal cortex.
tween species with very distinct brain size [Pereira-Pedro In general, as expected after a visual inspection, hu-
et al., 2017b]. Therefore, according to the converging ev- mans and nonhuman primates display further differenc-
idence we have on extinct and extant species, we can con- es in their whole parietal organization. The pattern of
clude that the derived parietal proportions in H. sapiens connections is distinct, notably at the inferior regions
are not due to general allometric effects associated with a [Catani et al., 2017]. Although fossils have provided
large brain. It is worth noting that, when using apes as an scanty evidence of gross morphological differences asso-
allometric reference, humans display an expected parie- ciated with the inferior parietal lobules, these areas are
to-occipital volume for their brain size [Semendeferi and known to be specialized in humans as well [Bzdok et al.,
Damasio, 2000] but smaller occipital volume [De Sousa 2016]. The intraparietal sulcus is also far more complex

142 Brain Behav Evol 2018;91:136–147 Bruner


DOI: 10.1159/000488889
in humans than in other primates, in terms of size and [Land, 2014] and conscious self-centered memory recall
organization [Grefkes and Fink, 2005; Choi et al., 2006], [Fletcher et al., 1995]. Visual imagery is the basis for sim-
and its evolutionary changes in our species are possibly ulation and mental experiments. The body, in this sense,
related to tool use [Kastner et al., 2017]. The homology is used as a measure of physical space, but also in terms
between humans and nonhuman primates for their pari- of chronological and social relationships (i.e., generating
etal cortex is not clear, although the complexity of the a chronological space and a social space according to an
intraparietal sulcus in H. sapiens may have displaced and egocentric perspective) [Hills et al., 2015; Maister et al.,
repositioned internal and external folding regions, gener- 2015; Peer et al., 2015]. The intraparietal sulcus is also
ating a lack of correspondence between human and non- specialized in visuospatial capacities, particularly in eye-
human cortical topology [Zlatkina and Petrides, 2014]. It hand coordination [Grefkes and Fink, 2005; Tunik et al.,
must be taken into account that most inferences are based 2007; Martin et al., 2011; Verhagen et al., 2012]. These
on comparisons between humans and macaques, two lin- visuospatial functions are relevant for all primates, but
eages that are not closely related in terms of phylogeny, they are far more crucial for our species, which is charac-
and that underwent separation and independent evolu- terized by complex tools and technology, visual symbols,
tion for some 20 million years. Therefore, most results and extensive social structure. Interestingly, potential
deal with only one genus of primates (Macaca) and in early modern humans, dated at around 100–200,000
particular with one that is not related to human evolution. years ago, did not display larger parietal bones and lobes,
Many areas seem evolutionarily conserved in humans suggesting that modern human phylogenetic origins may
and macaques [Orban, 2016], but for those elements that have preceded the evolution of a modern human brain
are evolutionarily derived – in humans as well as in ma- form [Bruner and Pearson, 2013; Bruner et al., 2018a]. An
caques – we still miss a comprehensive comparative sce- inclusive survey suggested that, since the phylogenetic
nario. Both modern humans and Neanderthals display a origin of our species, these changes may have followed a
lateral bulging and widening of the dorsal parietal surface gradual pattern [Neubauer et al., 2018]. Paleoanthropolo-
[Bruner et al., 2003], which could be tentatively interpret- gists find fossils with bulging parietal bones and lobes
ed as a cortical increase and outfold of the intraparietal roughly since the same dates (≈50–100,000 years ago) ar-
cortex [Pereira-Pedro and Bruner, 2016]. chaeologists find distinctive modern cultural traits, like
Comparative data on the superior parietal lobule are complex technology, larger social groups, and visual cul-
still lacking. According to the parcellation proposed by ture (paintings and ornamentation, for example). The ex-
Scheperjans et al. [2008], what we call superior lobule in act timing and dynamics of these changes are not known,
humans is largely the outer extension of the anterior and but, on an evolutionary and geological time scale, the two
posterior areas of the precuneus. This dorsal external sur- changes are almost matching. We should consider wheth-
face has not been sufficiently investigated yet. At least er or not this is due to chance.
three areas have been identified in the human dorsal re- The case of Neanderthals merits attention, because pa-
gion, but comparative data for nonhuman primates are leoneurology, skeletal biology, ecology, and archaeology
lacking. These outer cortical folds are apparently involved are supplying converging evidence on some aspects of
in our derived parietal morphology, and future surveys their visuospatial behavior [see Bruner and Lozano, 2014,
should be specifically dedicated to investigate this issue. 2015 for a detailed discussion]. As we mentioned, al-
though they had a similar or even larger brain volume
than modern humans, they did not have large parietal
Visuospatial Integration and Body Cognition lobes. At the same time, there is no evidence of a robust
and complex visual culture, their social groups and ter-
The precuneus is involved in a functional integration ritories were smaller, and there is not even any evidence
of body and vision, bridging somatosensory and occipital of projectile technology (fine spears, spear throwers, or
signals [Cavanna and Trimble, 2006; Margulies et al., arrows). Also, according to the traces found on their front
2009; Zhang and Li, 2012; Freton et al., 2014]. Its anterior teeth, they largely relied on the mouth to handle their
area deals mostly with body cognition, the posterior area tools, more than any modern human population. This
with visual cognition, and the intermediate area inte- distinctive (and hazardous) degree of use of the mouth as
grates both signals. This process, generally named visuo- a “third hand” may suggest that hand management may
spatial integration, not only deals with body-environ- have been less efficient in dealing with their material cul-
ment physical coordination, but also with visual imaging ture [Bruner et al., 2016]. Hypotheses in cognitive archae-

Evolution of the Parietal Lobes Brain Behav Evol 2018;91:136–147 143


DOI: 10.1159/000488889
ology cannot be tested to the same extent we can do in early and abandoned term quadrate lobule due to its
neontological fields, but all this evidence together points, square shape, it does not even have its own name, being
independently, to possible limits in visuospatial resources defined in relation to something else (precuneus means
and body cognition. This is not necessarily a verdict for anterior to the cuneus). Its broad designation includes
extinction, and it may even suggest that their large brains some inferior areas which intermingle with the posterior
may have had some distinct specializations that we did cingulate and retrosplenial cortex. All this block, that
not evolve [Pearce et al., 2013]. Nonetheless, the com- bridges the parietal lobe with the cingulate cortex, is high-
parison with Neanderthals further supports the hypoth- ly interconnected, but formed by distinct parts with dis-
esis of derived – and probably enhanced – visuospatial tinct functions, and should be referred to as to posterior
cognition in modern humans, associated with body- medial cortex [Bzdok et al., 2015]. The inferior areas are
environment management and body-tool functional spe- part of the default mode network and are crucial to func-
cialization. tions that are integrated but distinct from those of the
In the last 2 million years, our culture was not simply dorsal areas [Fransson and Marrelec, 2008; Margulies et
“object assisted” but instead “object dependent” [Plum- al., 2009; Utevsky et al., 2014; Yang et al., 2014; Barks et
mer, 2004]. Our technology is actually part of our cogni- al., 2015]. Therefore, probably the term precuneus should
tive processes, and there are several theories in current be strictly limited to the dorsal areas above the subparietal
neuroscience interpreting cognition as the result of inte- sulcus. I wonder whether the term quadrate lobule should
gration between brain, body, and tools [Malafouris, 2010; also be rescued and used to indicate this region. It also
Iriki and Taoka, 2012; Byrge et al., 2014]. In this sense, remains to be established whether (or to what extent) the
our parietal cortex and visuospatial functions can play a superior parietal lobule is part of the same element. Of
major role in the management of such interactions be- course, the features described in this article should be in-
tween the nervous system, the body interface, and tech- tegrated within a more comprehensive scenario. For ex-
nological resources [Bruner and Iriki, 2016]. Interesting- ample, we currently recognize the crucial importance of
ly, a comprehensive analysis on neural circuits in pri- the frontoparietal system, these two regions being deeply
mates evidenced major human changes in the genetic interconnected by functional and connective networks
expression associated with the striatum, an element of the [Jung and Haier, 2007; Caminiti et al., 2015].
basal ganglia deeply involved in body management [Sou- The noticeable degree of morphological variation of
sa et al., 2017]. the parietal lobes, and in particular of the precuneus, has
Of course, the nature behind this association between been evidenced in distinct samples and through distinct
form and function remains unclear. The parietal cortex is methods. It is clear that there are differences between hu-
sensitive to genetic programming [Chen et al., 2012] but mans and other primates, and probably the same region
also to environmental influences, and its components are displays a pronounced enlargement in H. sapiens com-
highly susceptible to training [Quallo et al., 2009]. At pared with fossil human species. There are multiple and
present, we ignore to what extent parietal differences be- independent sources of evidence suggesting that the deep
tween and within species are due to genes, culture, or to folds of the parietal cortex have undergone some special-
different kinds of feedback between both factors. Culture ization in modern humans. A mandatory future step is,
can influence phenotypic expression, but also genetic se- therefore, to study the histological changes associated
lection, through direct effects (autocatalysis) or indirect with these macroanatomical differences.
channeling of phenotypic plasticity [the so-called Bald- Geometrical models point to the dorsal and anterior
win effect; see for example Crispo, 2007]. Thus, although areas as a main source of variation. Nonetheless, also the
parietal differences begin now to be acknowledged in evo- posterior (parieto-occipital) region may show human
lutionary neuroanatomy, their causes, factors and mech- specialization for body-tool coordination and allocentric
anisms still remain, at present, largely unexplained. imaging [Hutchison et al., 2015; Sulpizio et al., 2016]. En-
hanced morphological models can help to localize more
specific areas involved in these volumetric variations in
Perspectives human fossils as well as in living primates.
The dorsal parietal cortex is largely involved in visuo-
The parietal cortex has been largely neglected in evo- spatial integration, and future surveys should investigate
lutionary neuroanatomy (a case of “parietal neglect”!). possible functional or cognitive factors associated with its
The precuneus is a large cortical element but, despite an morphological diversity. Although differences in the pa-

144 Brain Behav Evol 2018;91:136–147 Bruner


DOI: 10.1159/000488889
rietal cortex are associated with visuospatial performance Taking into account the many disputes and disagree-
[Koscik et al., 2009], at present, there is no evidence cor- ments on subtle anatomical or statistical issues in paleo-
relating the overall precuneus morphology with tradi- anthropology and evolutionary neuroanatomy, it is cu­
tional psychometric scores [Bruner et al., 2015b]. In this rious how the large parietal differences have passed un-
sense, and taking into account the cortical diversity of the noticed, and they are not even mentioned in many
precuneus, future neurofunctional analyses should con- scientific reviews. Nonetheless, at this time, neglecting
sider more specific cortical areas and probably more tar- such evidence seems to be unreasonable. Once the differ-
geted behavioral tasks. ence has been noticed, it should be investigated properly.
Together with this “to-do” list, there is also a “not-to- And, if visuospatial functions have a major role in extend-
do” list, which particularly refers to things that should be ing our “prosthetic” capacity in terms of space, time, tech-
avoided when handling the fossil record. First, hypothe- nology, body cognition, and social perception, we should
ses should be tested through samples and not through once more consider that brain evolution is basically a
specimens. Many paleoneurological traits display large matter of anatomy, while cognitive evolution must deal
intragroup variation and scarce intergroup differences. with additional components (body and culture) that go
Therefore, individual fossils can hardly supply consistent beyond the neuronal content of a braincase.
support to promote or demote complex evolutionary
scenarios. Second, quantitative analyses are necessary to
provide a proper comparative framework. Descriptive Acknowledgments
studies should be restricted to preliminary and introduc-
I would like to thank Ashley Morhardt and Georg Striedter for
tory surveys only. Third, speculations should be used giving me the opportunity to participate in the 29th Karger Work-
with discretion and common sense, and quickly substi- shop “From Fossils to Function: Integrative and Taxonomically
tuted by specific hypotheses that can be (at least partially) Inclusive Approaches to Vertebrate Evolutionary Neuroscience.”
tested. Such hypotheses should be proposed and corrobo- Many and diverse studies on parietal evolution have been possible
thanks to the collaboration with many people, including Giorgio
rated through multiple and independent lines of evi-
Manzi, Ralph Holloway, Jim Rilling, Todd Preuss, Naomichi Ogi-
dence, and not following specific or punctual results. hara, Manuel Martín-Loeches, Roberto Colom, Heidi Jacobs, Marina
Fourth, endocasts are valuable sources of information, Lozano, Philipp Gunz, Simon Neubauer, Atsushi Iriki, Duilio Garo-
but their general shape is the result of distinct factors (cra- foli, Amélie Beaudet, Barbara Saracino, Fred Coolidge, Roberto Mac-
nial and cerebral) and distinct elements (cortical areas). chiarelli, and Xiujie Wu. I am particularly grateful to all the students
who have supported and developed my laboratory over the years,
Therefore, overall shape analyses should be limited to
namely Sofia Pereira-Pedro, Gizéh Rangel de Lázaro, José Manuel de
preliminary geometrical surveys. More detailed morpho- la Cuétara, Annapaola Fedato, Alannah Pearson, María Silva Gago,
metrics, based on anatomical boundaries, should be used Hideki Amano, and Stana Eisová. These research lines were primar-
to test specific hypotheses in evolutionary neuroanatomy ily funded by the Proyecto Atapuerca (Spanish Government), coor-
[Bruner and Ogihara, 2018]. Fifth, more information is dinated by José María Bermudez de Castro, as well as by the Italian
Institute of Anthropology and the Wenner-Gren Foundation.
needed about modern human variation. Many paleoneu-
rological traits are not even known, in terms of variability
and function, in our species. Therefore, proper knowl- Disclosure Statement
edge of large living samples is mandatory before we make
inferences on a few fragmented bony remains. The author declares no conflict of interest.

References
Allen JS, Damasio H, Grabowski TJ (2002): Nor- Beaudet A, Bruner E (2017): A frontal lobe surface Bruner E (2004): Geometric morphometrics and
mal neuroanatomical variation in the human analysis in three archaic African human fos- paleoneurology: brain shape evolution in the
brain: an MRI-volumetric study. Am J Phys sils: OH 9, Buia, and Bodo. Comptes Rendus genus Homo. J Hum Evol 47:279–303.
Anthropol 118:341–358. Palevol 16:499–507. Bruner E (2010): Morphological differences in the
Barks SK, Parr LA, Rilling JL (2015): The default Bookstein F, Schafer K, Prossinger H, Seidler H, parietal lobes within the human genus: a neu-
mode network in chimpanzees (Pan troglo- Fieder M, Stringer C, Weber GW, Arsuaga JL, rofunctional perspective. Curr Anthropol
dytes) is similar to that of humans. Cereb Cor- Slice DE, Rohlf FJ, Recheis W, Mariam AJ, 51:S77–S88.
tex 25:538–544. Marcus LF (1999): Comparing frontal cranial
profiles in archaic and modern Homo by mor-
phometric analysis. Anat Rec 257:217–224.

Evolution of the Parietal Lobes Brain Behav Evol 2018;91:136–147 145


DOI: 10.1159/000488889
Bruner E (2014): Functional craniology, human ingeal vessels: variation and evolution in Cavanna AE, Trimble MR (2006): The precuneus:
evolution, and anatomical constraints in the Homo erectus, Neanderthals, and modern hu- a review of its functional anatomy and behav-
Neanderthal braincase; in Akazawa T, Ogi- mans. Eur J Morphol 42:217–224. ioural correlates. Brain 129:564–583.
hara N, Tanabe HC, Terashima H (eds): Dy- Bruner E, Manzi G, Arsuaga JL (2003): Encepha- Chen CH, Gutierrez ED, Thompson W, Panizzon
namics of Learning in Neanderthals and lization and allometric trajectories in the ge- MS, Jernigan TL, Eyler LT, Fennema-Notes-
Modern Humans. Tokyo, Springer, vol 2, pp nus Homo: evidence from the Neandertal and tine C, Jak AJ, Neale MC, Franz CE, Lyons MJ,
121–129. modern lineages. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA Grant MD, Fischl B, Seidman LJ, Tsuang MT,
Bruner E (2015): Functional craniology and brain 100:15335–15340. Kremen WS, Dale AM (2012): Hierarchical
evolution; in Bruner E (ed): Human Paleo- Bruner E, Ogihara N (2018): Surfin’ endocasts: genetic organization of human cortical sur-
neurology. Basel, Springer, pp 57–94. the good and the bad on brain form. Palaentol face area. Science 335:1634–1636.
Bruner E (2017a): The fossil evidence of human Electr 21.1.1A:1–10. Choi H-J, Zilles K, Mohlberg H, Schleicher A,
brain evolution; in Kaas J (ed): Evolution of Bruner E, Pearson O (2013): Neurocranial evolu- Fink GR, Armstrong E, Amunts K (2006): Cy-
Nervous Systems, ed 2. Oxford, Elsevier, vol tion in modern humans: the case of Jebel toarchitectonic identification and probabilis-
4, pp 63–92. Irhoud 1. Anthropol Sci 121:31–41. tic mapping of two distinct areas within the
Bruner E (2017b): Language, paleoneurology, and Bruner E, Pereira-Pedro AS, Bastir M (2017a): anterior ventral bank of the human intrapari-
the fronto-parietal system. Front Hum Neu- Patterns of morphological integration be- etal sulcus. J Comp Neurol 495:53–69.
rosci 11:349. tween parietal and temporal areas in the hu- Crispo E (2007): The Baldwin effect and genetic
Bruner E, Amano H, de la Cuétara JM, Ogihara N man skull. J Morphol 278:1312–1320. assimilation: revisiting two mechanisms of
(2015a): The brain and the braincase: a spatial Bruner E, Pereira-Pedro AS, Chen X, Rilling JK evolutionary change mediated by phenotypic
analysis on the midsagittal profile in adult hu- (2017b): Precuneus proportions and cortical plasticity. Evolution 61:2469–2479.
mans. J Anat 227:268–276. folding: a morphometric evaluation on a ra- De Sousa AA, Sherwood CC, Mohlberg H,
Bruner E, Amano H, Pereira-Pedro AS, Ogihara cially diverse human sample. Ann Anat 211: Amunts K, Schleicher A, MacLeod CE, Hof
N (2018a): The evolution of the parietal lobes 120–128. PR, Frahm H, Zilles K (2010): Hominoid vi-
in the genus Homo; in Bruner E, Ogihara N, Bruner E, Preuss T, Chen X, Rilling J (2017c): Evi- sual brain structure volumes and the position
Tanabe H (eds): Digital Endocasts. Tokyo, dence for expansion of the precuneus in human of the lunate sulcus. J Hum Evol 58:281–292.
Springer, pp 219–258. evolution. Brain Struct Funct 222:1053–1060. Falk D (1987): Hominid paleoneurology. Annu
Bruner E, de la Cuétara JM, Holloway RL (2011a): Bruner E, Rangel de Lázaro G, de la Cuétara JM, Rev Anthropol 16:13–30.
A bivariate approach to the variation of the Martín-Loeches M, Colóm R, Jacobs HIL Fletcher PC, Frith CD, Baker SC, Shallice T,
parietal curvature in the genus Homo. Anat (2014b): Midsagittal brain variation and MRI Frackowiak RSJ, Dolan RJ (1995): The mind’s
Rec 294:1548–1556. shape analysis of the precuneus in adult indi- eye – precuneus activation in memory-related
Bruner E, de la Cuétara JM, Masters M, Amano viduals. J Anat 224:367–376. imagery. NeuroImage 2:195–200.
H, Ogihara N (2014a): Functional craniology Bruner E, Román FJ, de la Cuétara JM, Martín- Fransson P, Marrelec G (2008): The precuneus/
and brain evolution: from paleontology to Loeches M, Colóm R (2015b): Cortical surface posterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role
biomedicine. Front Neuroanat 8:19. area and cortical thickness in the precuneus of in the default mode network: evidence from a
Bruner E, de La Cuétara JM, Musso F (2012): adult humans. Neuroscience 286:345–352. partial correlation network analysis. Neuro-
Quantifying patterns of endocranial heat dis- Bruner E, Saracino B, Ricci F, Tafuri M, Passarello Image 42:1178–1184.
tribution: brain geometry and thermoregula- P, Manzi G (2004): Midsagittal cranial shape Freton M, Lemogne C, Bergouignan L, Delaveau
tion. Am J Hum Biol 24:753–762. variation in the genus Homo by geometric P, Lehéricy S, Fossati P (2014): The eye of the
Bruner E, Esteve-Altava B, Rasskin-Gutman D morphometrics. Coll Antropol 28:99–112. self: precuneus volume and visual perspective
(2018b): Networking brains: modeling spatial Bruner E, Sherkat S (2008): The middle menin- during autobiographical memory retrieval.
relationships of the cerebral cortex; in Bruner geal artery: from clinics to fossils. Childs Nerv Brain Struct Funct 219:959–968.
E, Ogihara N, Tanabe H (eds): Digital Endo- Syst 24:1289–1298. Grefkes C, Fink GR (2005): The functional orga-
casts. Tokyo, Springer, pp 191–204. Byrge L, Sporns O, Smith LB (2014): Develop- nization of the intraparietal sulcus in humans
Bruner E, Grimaud-Hervé D, Wu X, de la Cué- mental process emerges from extended brain- and monkeys. J Anat 207:3–17.
tara JM, Holloway R (2015c): A paleoneuro- body-behavior networks. Trends Cogn Sci 18: Gunz P, Harvati K (2007): The Neanderthal “chi-
logical survey of Homo erectus endocranial 395–403. gnon”: variation, integration, and homology.
metrics. Quat Int 368:80–87. Bzdok D, Hartwigsen G, Reid A, Laird AR, Fox J Hum Evol 52:262–274.
Bruner E, Iriki A (2016): Extending mind, visuo- PT, Eickhoff SB (2016): Left inferior parietal Gunz P, Neubauer S, Maureille B, Hublin J-J
spatial integration, and the evolution of the lobe engagement in social cognition and lan- (2010): Brain development after birth differs
parietal lobes in the human genus. Quat Int guage. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 68:319–334. between Neanderthals and modern humans.
405:98–110. Bzdok D, Heeger A, Langner R, Laird AR, Fox PT, Curr Biol 20:R921–R922.
Bruner E, Lozano M (2014): Extended mind and Palomero-Gallagher N, Vogt BA, Zilles K, Hammer Ø, Harper D, Ryan P (2001): PAST: pa-
visuo-spatial integration: three hands for the Eickhoff SB (2015): Subspecialization in the leontological statistics software package for
Neandertal lineage. J Anthropol Sci 92: 273– human posterior medial cortex. NeuroImage education and data analysis. Palaeontol Electr
280. 106:55–71. 4:9.
Bruner E, Lozano M (2015): Three hands: one Caminiti R, Innocenti GM, Battaglia-Mayer A Hills TT, Todd PM, Lazer D, Redish AD, Couzin
year later. J Anthropol Sci 93:191–195. (2015): Organization and evolution of parie- ID (2015): Exploration versus exploitation in
Bruner E, Lozano M, Lorenzo C (2016): Visuo- to-frontal processing streams in macaque space, mind, and society. Trends Cogn Sci 19:
spatial integration and human evolution: the monkeys and humans. Neurosci Biobehav 46–54.
fossil evidence. J Anthropol Sci 94:81–97. Rev 56:73–96. Holloway RL (1981): Exploring the dorsal surface
Bruner E, Mantini S, Musso F, de la Cuétara JM, Catani M, Robertsson N, Beyh A, Huynh V, de of hominoid brain endocasts by stereoplotter
Ripani M, Sherkat S (2011b): The evolution of Santiago Requejo F, Howells H, Barrett RLC, and discriminant analysis. Philos Trans R Soc
the meningeal vascular system in the human Aiello M, Cavaliere C, Dyrby TB, Krug K, Pti- Lond B 292:155–166.
genus: from brain shape to thermoregulation. to M, D’Arceuil H, Forkel SJ, Dell’Acqua F Holloway RL, Broadfield DC, Yuan MS (2004):
Am J Hum Biol 23:35–43. (2017): Short parietal lobe connections of the The Human Fossil Record, vol III: Brain En-
Bruner E, Mantini S, Perna A, Maffei C, Manzi G human and monkey brain. Cortex 97: 339– docasts: The Paleoneurological Evidence.
(2005): Fractal dimension of the middle men- 357. Hoboken, Wiley-Liss.

146 Brain Behav Evol 2018;91:136–147 Bruner


DOI: 10.1159/000488889
Hutchison RM, Culham JC, Flanagan JR, Everling Neubauer S, Gunz P, Hublin J-J (2009): The pat- Scheperjans F, Hermann K, Eickhoff SB, Amunts
S, Gallivan JP (2015): Functional subdivisions tern of endocranial ontogenetic shape chang- K, Schleicher A, Zilles K (2008): Observer-in-
of medial parieto-occipital cortex in humans es in humans. J Anat 215:240–255. dependent cytoarchitectonic mapping of the
and nonhuman primates using resting-state Neubauer S, Gunz P, Hublin J-J (2010): Endocra- human superior parietal cortex. Cereb Cortex
fMRI. NeuroImage 116:10–29. nial shape changes during growth in chim- 18:846–867.
Iriki A, Taoka M (2012): Triadic (ecological, neu- panzees and humans: a morphometric analy- Scott N, Neubauer S, Hublin JJ, Gunz P (2014): A
ral, cognitive) niche construction: a scenario sis of unique and shared aspects. J Hum Evol shared pattern of postnatal endocranial devel-
of human brain evolution extrapolating tool 59:555–566. opment in extant hominoids. Evol Biol 41:
use and language from the control of reaching Neubauer S, Hublin J-J, Gunz P (2018): The evo- 572–594.
actions. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci lution of modern human brain shape. Sci Adv Semendeferi K, Damasio H (2000): The brain and
367:10–23. 4:eaao5961. its main anatomical subdivisions in living
Jung RE, Haier RJ (2007): The Parieto-Frontal In- Orban GA (2016): Functional definitions of pari- hominoids using magnetic resonance imag-
tegration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: con- etal areas in human and non-human pri- ing. J Hum Evol 38:317–332.
verging neuroimaging evidence. Behav Brain mates. Proc Biol Sci 283:20160118. Semendeferi K, Lu A, Schenker N, Damasio H
Sci 30:135–154. Pearce E, Stringer C, Dunbar RIM (2013): New (2002): Humans and great apes share a large
Kastner S, Chen Q, Jeong SK, Mruczek REB insights into differences in brain organization frontal cortex, Nat Neurosci 5:272–276.
(2017): A brief comparative review of primate between Neanderthals and anatomically Sousa AMM, Zhu Y, Raghanti MA, Kitchen RR,
posterior parietal cortex: a novel hypothesis modern humans. Proc Biol Sci 280:1758. Onorati M, Tebbenkamp ATN, Stutz B, Mey-
on the human toolmaker. Neuropsychologia Peer M, Salomon R, Goldberg I, Blanke O, Arzy S er KA, Li M, Kawasawa YI, Liu F, Perez RG,
105:123–134. (2015): Brain system for mental orientation in Mele M, Carvalho T, Skarica M, Gulden FO,
Klingenberg CP (2011): MorphoJ: an integrated space, time, and person. Proc Natl Acad Sci Pletikos M, Shibata A, Stephenson AR, Edler
software package for geometric morphomet- USA 112:11072–11077. MK, Ely JJ, Elsworth JD, Horvath TL, Hof
rics. Mol Ecol Resour 11:353–357. Pereira-Pedro AS, Bruner E (2016): Sulcal pat- PR, Hyde TM, Kleinman JE, Weinberger DR,
Kobayashi Y, Matsui T, Haizuka Y, Ogihara N, tern, extension, and morphology of the precu- Reimers M, Lifton RP, Mane SM, Noonan JP,
Hirai N, Matsumura G (2014): Cerebral sulci neus in adult humans. Ann Anat 208:85–93. State MW, Lein ES, Knowles JA, Marques-
and gyri observed on macaque endocasts: Pereira-Pedro AS, Bruner E (2018): Landmarking Bonet T, Sherwood CC, Gerstein MB, Sestan
in Akazawa T, Ogihara N, Tanabe HC, Tera­ endocasts; in Bruner E, Ogihara N, Tanabe H N (2017): Molecular and cellular reorganiza-
shima H (eds): Dynamics of Learning in (eds): Digital Endocasts. Tokyo, Springer, pp tion of neural circuits in the human lineage.
Neanderthals and Modern Humans. Tokyo, 127–142. Science 358:1027–1032.
Springer, vol 2, pp 131–137. Pereira-Pedro AS, Masters M, Bruner E (2017a): Sulpizio V, Committeri G, Lambrey S, Berthoz A,
Koscik T, O’Leary D, Moser DJ, Andreasen NC, Shape analysis of spatial relationships be- Galati G (2016): Role of the human retro­
Nopoulos P (2009): Sex differences in parietal tween orbito-ocular and endocranial struc- splenial cortex/parieto-occipital sulcus in per-
lobe morphology: relationship to mental rota- tures in modern humans and fossil hominids. spective priming. NeuroImage 125:108–119.
tion performance. Brain Cogn 69:451–459. J Anat 231:947–960. Tunik E, Rice NJ, Hamilton A, Grafton ST (2007):
Land MF (2014): Do we have an internal model of Pereira-Pedro AS, Rilling JL, Chen X, Preuss TM, Beyond grasping: representation of action in
the outside world? Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Bruner E (2017b): Midsagittal brain variation human anterior intraparietal sulcus. Neuro-
Biol Sci 369:20130045–20130045. among non-human primates: insights into Image 36:T77–T86.
Lieberman DE, McBratney BM, Krovitz G (2002): evolutionary expansion of the human precu- Utevsky AV, Smith DV, Huettel SA (2014): Pre-
The evolution and development of cranial neus. Brain Behav Evol 90:255–263. cuneus is a functional core of the default-
form in Homo sapiens. Proc Natl Acad Sci Plummer T (2004): Flaked stones and old bones: mode network. J Neurosci 34:932–940.
USA 99:1134–1139. biological and cultural evolution at the dawn Van Minh N, Hamada Y (2017): Age-related
Maister L, Slater M, Sanchez-Vives MV, Tsakiris of technology. Yrb Phys Anthropol 47: 118– changes of sulcal imprints on the endocrani-
M (2015): Changing bodies changes minds: 164. um in the Japanese macaque (Macaca fusca-
owning another body affects social cognition. Ponce de León MS, Bienvenu T, Akazawa T, Zol- ta). Am J Phys Anthropol 163:285–294.
Trends Cogn Sci 19:6–12. likofer CPE (2016): Brain development is sim- Verhagen L, Dijkerman HC, Medendorp WP,
Malafouris L (2010): The brain-artefact interface ilar in Neanderthals and modern humans. Toni I (2012): Cortical dynamics of senso­
(BAI): a challenge for archaeology and cultur- Curr Biol 26:R665–R666. rimotor integration during grasp planning. J
al neuroscience. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 5: Quallo MM, Price CJ, Ueno K, Asamizuya T, Neurosci 32:4508–4519.
264–273. Cheng K, Lemon RN, Iriki A (2009): Gray and Yang Z, Chang C, Xu T, Jiang L, Handwerker DA,
Margulies DS, Vincent JL, Kelly C, Lohmann G, white matter changes associated with tool-use Castellanos FX, Milham MP, Bandettini PA,
Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, Villringer A, Castella- learning in macaque monkeys. Proc Natl Zuo XN (2014): Connectivity trajectory
nos FX, Milham MP, Petrides M (2009): Pre- Acad Sci USA 106:18379–18384. across lifespan differentiates the precuneus
cuneus shares intrinsic functional architec- Rangel de Lázaro G, de la Cuétara JM, Píšová H, from the default network. NeuroImage 89:
ture in humans and monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Lorenzo C, Bruner E (2016): Diploic vessels 45–56.
Sci USA 106:20069–20074. and computed tomography: segmentation Zhang S, Li CR (2012): Functional connectivity
Martin K, Jacobs S, Frey SH (2011): Handedness- and comparison in modern humans and fossil mapping of the human precuneus by resting
dependent and -independent cerebral asym- hominids. Am J Phys Anthropol 159: 313– state fMRI. NeuroImage 59:3548–3562.
metries in the anterior intraparietal sulcus 324. Zilles K, Amunts K (2010): Centenary of Brod-
and ventral premotor cortex during grasp Richtsmeier JT, Aldridge K, de Leon VB, Panchal mann’s map – conception and fate. Nat Rev
planning. NeuroImage 57:502–512. J, Kane AA, Marsh JL, Yan P, Cole TM (2006): Neurosci 11:139–145.
Moss ML, Young RW (1960): A functional ap- Phenotypic integration of neurocranium and Zilles K, Palomero-Gallagher N (2001): Cyto-,
proach to craniology. Am J Phys Anthropol brain. J Exp Zool 306B:360–378. myelo-, and receptor architectonics of the hu-
18:281–292. man parietal cortex. NeuroImage 14:S8–S20.
Mountcastle VB (1995): The parietal system and Zlatkina V, Petrides M (2014): Morphological
some higher brain functions. Cereb Cortex 5: patterns of the intraparietal sulcus and the an-
377–390. terior intermediate parietal sulcus of Jensen in
the human brain. Proc Biol Sci 281:20141493.

Evolution of the Parietal Lobes Brain Behav Evol 2018;91:136–147 147


DOI: 10.1159/000488889

You might also like