You are on page 1of 14

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

ScienceDirect
Behavior Therapy 46 (2015) 328 – 341
www.elsevier.com/locate/bt

Are 60-Minute Prolonged Exposure Sessions With 20-Minute


Imaginal Exposure to Traumatic Memories Sufficient to
Successfully Treat PTSD? A Randomized Noninferiority
Clinical Trial
Nitsa Nacasch
Tel-Aviv Brull Community Mental Health Center
Jonathan D. Huppert
The Hebrew University of Jerusalem
Yi-Jen Su
National Taiwan University
Yogev Kivity
The Hebrew University of Jerusalem
Yula Dinshtein
Tel-Aviv Brull Community Mental Health Center
Rebecca Yeh
Edna B. Foa
University of Pennsylvania

90-minute sessions that include 40 minutes of IE in treating


The study aims to determine whether 60-minute sessions of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and to explore the
prolonged exposure (PE) that include 20 minutes of relationship of treatment outcome to within- and between-
imaginal exposure (IE) are noninferior to the standard session habituation and change in negative cognitions.
Thirty-nine adult veterans with chronic PTSD were
We want to thank Professor Joseph Zohar for his contribution to randomly assigned to 90-minute (n = 19) or 60-minute
the design and implementation of the study, to Dr. Miki Polliack,
Yonina Zilbermintz, Rachel Gold, and Miri Shalit for their help in
(n = 20) sessions of PE. PTSD symptoms were assessed by
the implementation of the study and to the therapists for their an unaware independent evaluator before and after
rigorous implementation of the treatment procedures. Our thanks treatment and at 6-month follow-up. Self-reports of
are extended to the research assistants at Chaim Sheba Medical
Center, Dana Tzur and Dr. Lea Fostick, and to the research
depression and negative cognitions were assessed before
assistants for their day-to-day help in the study-related activities. and after treatment. Participants in both conditions showed
Last but not least we want to express our gratitude to Josephine significant reductions in PTSD symptoms. Sixty-minute
Curry, Joseph Carpenter, and Rebecca Yeh from the Department of
Psychiatry at the University of Pennsylvania for their diligent work
sessions were found to be noninferior to 90-minute sessions
in preparing the manuscript for submission. in reducing PTSD symptoms, as the upper bound of the
Address correspondence to Edna B. Foa, Ph.D., Department of 95% confidence interval for the difference between condi-
Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania, 3535 Market Street, 6th
Floor, Philadelphia, PA 19104; e-mail: foa@mail.med.upenn.edu.
tions in the PTSD Symptom Scale–Interview (posttreatment:
0005-7894/© 2014 Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies. 6.00; follow-up: 6.77) was below the predefined noninferi-
Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. ority margin (7.00). Participants receiving shorter sessions
sixty vs. ninety minutes of prolonged exposure 329

showed less within- and between-session habituation than are their emotions such as guilt or shame in an
those receiving longer sessions, but no group differences in indirect and empathic manner. Prior studies have
reductions in negative cognitions were found. The current demonstrated the efficacy of imaginal exposure alone
findings indicate that the outcomes of 60-minute sessions of (Bryant, Moulds, Guthrie, Dang, & Nixon, 2003;
PE do not differ from those of 90-minute sessions. In Tarrier et al., 1999) and in vivo exposure alone
addition, change in trauma-related cognitions and between- (Marks, Lovell, Noshirvani, Livanou, & Thrasher,
session habituation are both potential mechanisms of PE. 1998) in treating PTSD. Furthermore, one study
(Bryant et al., 2008) that included in vivo and
imaginal exposure but excluded processing after
Keywords: posttraumatic stress disorder; prolonged exposure; imaginal exposure showed inferior outcomes
habituation; fear activation; emotional processing compared with studies that included both in vivo
exposure and imaginal exposure followed by cogni-
PROLONGED EXPOSURE THERAPY (PE; Foa, Hembree, tive restructuring, suggesting the importance of
& Rothbaum, 2007), a specific exposure therapy conducting processing after imaginal exposure.
program for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), EPT posits two mechanisms of PE: activation
is a highly effective treatment for PTSD across a (emotional engagement) of the trauma memory
variety of traumas (Foa et al., 2005; Powers, through imaginal and in vivo exposure, and presen-
Halpern, Ferenschak, Gillihan, & Foa, 2010). The tation of information during exposure that discon-
PE program usually consists of 8 to 15 weekly or firms the expected harm that leads to reductions in
twice-weekly sessions, each lasting about 90 minutes, negative cognitions. In addition, EPT proposes three
with 45 to 60 minutes of imaginal exposure. The indicators of emotional processing: fear activation,
three main components of PE are in vivo exposure, within-session habituation (i.e., reduction in distress
imaginal exposure, and processing of the imaginal levels from the beginning to end of exposure to a
exposure (McLean & Foa, 2011). The rationale of PE feared memory or stimulus in a treatment session),
is based on emotional processing theory (EPT; Foa, and between-session habituation (i.e., reduction of
Huppert, & Cahill, 2006; Foa & Kozak, 1986). peak distress levels across successive exposure
Briefly, EPT proposes that PTSD reflects the presence sessions). These indicators, along with changes in
of a pathological fear (emotional) structure in which negative cognitions, are hypothesized to be associated
normally safe stimuli representations are associated with successful outcome (cf. Zalta et al., 2013).
with the meaning of danger, and normal responses to Indeed, the rationale for extending imaginal exposure
trauma are associated with the meaning of self- to 60 minutes was derived from the supposition
incompetence. Thus, EPT posits that the erroneous that within-session habituation is associated with
cognitions that “the world is extremely dangerous” reduction in PTSD symptoms. However, although
and “I am extremely weak and incompetent” mediate longer exposures have been shown to promote
the development and maintenance of PTSD by greater within-session habituation than shorter
promoting avoidance that prevents the individual exposures (Chaplin & Levine, 1981; Rabavilas,
from testing and disconfirming these cognitions. Boulougouris, & Stefanis, 1976; van Minnen & Foa,
According to EPT, treatment that aims at ameliorat- 2006), between-session—but not within-session—
ing PTSD symptoms should correct these erroneous habituation has been associated with treatment
associations, either by introducing new information outcome (Jaycox, Foa, & Morral, 1998; van Minnen
or strengthening nonpathological associations and & Hagenaars, 2002). Moreover, in a nonrando-
memories (Foa et al., 2006, see also Brewin, 2006). mized study, van Minnen and Foa (2006) found that
The course by which these changes occur is called 60-minute imaginal exposures did not produce
emotional processing. In PE, emotional processing is superior outcomes to 30-minute imaginal exposures
achieved through the use of in vivo exposure to despite greater within-session habituation in the
avoided trauma-related situations and imaginal longer exposures. Furthermore, although no differ-
exposure to the traumatic event (revisiting of the ences were found in between-session habituation
traumatic memory in imagination and recounting between the longer and shorter exposures, between-
it aloud) followed by postexposure processing, session habituation was related to treatment out-
(i.e., discussion of the revisiting experience), which come. This raises questions about the importance of
entails asking how their experience was while within-session habituation as a reliable indicator of
retelling the trauma, what they learned from the emotional processing proposed by EPT.
retelling or trauma, acknowledging the courage of the In addition to their theoretical implications, van
patient for having recounted the story, emphasizing Minnen and Foa’s (2006) findings are of practical
to the patients that they did not fall apart from importance. At present, there has been widespread
confronting the memory, and exploring how realistic dissemination of PE from academic settings into
330 nacasch et al.

community practice (Foa et al., 2005; Karlin et al., severity was extended to 6 months, and (e) a measure
2010; McLean & Foa, 2011). One barrier for using of trauma-related cognitions was included.
PE in community clinics is the limitation often The above design allowed us to examine the
imposed on session length by reimbursement policies. following hypotheses: (a) noninferiority: patients
Reducing the length of PE sessions from 90 minutes to who received 60-minute sessions with 20-minute
the “standard” psychotherapy 60-minute sessions imaginal exposure would not have significantly less
would eliminate this critical barrier. Indeed, the reduction in PTSD symptoms, depression, and
impetus to reduce the length of sessions in van Minnen trauma-related cognitions than patients who received
and Foa’s study was the change in reimbursement 90-minute sessions with 40-minute imaginal expo-
regulations in the Netherlands. Moreover, the efficacy sure. Confirmation of noninferiority in this trial was
of PE in the treatment of individuals suffering from established by showing that the difference in mean
combat- and terror-related PTSD has been demon- improvement of outcome measures is smaller than a
strated in several recent studies (e.g., Nacasch et al., prespecified noninferiority margin, as defined by the
2011; Schneier et al., 2012; Tuerk et al., 2011). The reliable change index developed by Jacobson and
number of PTSD cases related to combat and terror is Truax (1991) and determined for PTSD studies by
increasing due to the significant rise in worldwide Devilly and Foa (2001); (b) habituation: patients
combat and terror violence over the last decade. receiving 40-minute imaginal exposure would exhibit
Shortened PE is of great value to address the urgent greater within-session habituation than those receiv-
need for efficacious and time-limited treatment for ing 20-minute imaginal exposure but would not differ
this population. in the degree of between-session habituation; (c) there
Despite the promising results of van Minnen and would be a significant relationship of between-session
Foa (2006), their study contains a number of habituation, but not within-session habituation, and
methodological limitations that prevent strong reduction in PTSD symptom severity; and (d) there
conclusions from their work. First, patients were would be a significant relationship between change in
not randomly assigned to the two conditions. trauma-related cognitions and change in PTSD
Second, outcome variables were measured using symptoms in both conditions.
self-report data. Third, the follow-up period was
Method
only 1 month, which is not long enough to assess
whether participants in the 30-minute exposure participants
condition would be more likely to relapse than To be eligible for the study, patients had to meet
those in the 60-minute exposure condition. Last, a diagnosis of PTSD and to experience moderate
although the relationships of within- and between- to severe symptoms for at least 3 months (PTSD
session habituation to outcome were examined in Symptoms Scale–Interview [PSS-I; Foa, Riggs,
that study, a critical mechanism of PE, reduction in Dancu, & Rothbaum, 1993] score ≥ 20), compa-
negative trauma-related cognitions, was not assessed. rable to previous randomized clinical trial (RCT)
Thus, the effects of length of treatment on cognitions studies for standard PE (e.g., Foa, Dancu, et al.,
could not be examined. Given these limitations, the 1999; Foa et al., 2005). PTSD had to be at least as
efficacy of shortened imaginal exposure still remains severe as other psychiatric diagnoses. If treated with
unestablished and, as the authors stated in their medications, patients had to be on a stable regimen
limitation, needs to be replicated using more rigorous for at least 3 months prior to pretreatment evaluation
methodology. and remain on that dosage throughout treatment.
The goals of the present study were to replicate the Adherence was monitored by the patient’s report
findings of van Minnen and Foa (2006) and to to the psychiatrist. Exclusion criteria were current
further examine the theoretical and practical impli- active substance dependence, current psychotic
cations of reducing the length of imaginal exposure symptoms, bipolar disorder, or severe dissociative
in PE by comparing the efficacy of 90-minute PE symptoms. Patients deemed at high risk for suicidal
sessions that included 40-minute imaginal exposures behavior (i.e., with intent or plan or both) were also
with 60-minute PE sessions that included 20-minute excluded. Severe dissociative symptoms were deter-
imaginal exposure in participants with chronic mined at intake through administration of the
PTSD. To address the limitations of van Minnen Dissociative Experiences Scale (van IJzendoorn &
and Foa’s study, in the present study (a) participants Schuengel, 1996) and an unstructured clinical
were randomly assigned to 90- or 60-minute PE interview by the first author, a psychiatrist (e.g., a
sessions, (b) shorter length of imaginal exposure patient became agitated and stopped answering
(i.e., 20 minutes) was adopted, (c) outcome was questions for a number of minutes, appearing to be
assessed by evaluators unaware of treatment condi- dissociated, multiple times throughout the intake
tion, (d) follow-up assessment of PTSD symptom interview).
sixty vs. ninety minutes of prolonged exposure 331

Fifteen (75%) of the 60-minute group and 14 included 40-minute imaginal exposures, and 20 were
(74%) of the 90-minute group were on medications randomized to 60-minute PE sessions that included
(predominantly selective serotonin reuptake inhib- 20-minute imaginal exposure (for details see the
itors [SSRIs] or serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake CONSORT Chart in Figure 1). A simple randomi-
inhibitors [SNRIs]) during the entire study. The first zation procedure (computer-generated random num-
authors monitored the medication of all patients in bers) was conducted prior to Session 3 by a senior
the study. None of the patients changed their psychiatrist with no clinical involvement in the study.
medication regimen during their participation in Two patients dropped out of the study before
the study. randomization and one patient was withdrawn due
Between March 2005 and March 2010, 84 to severe dissociation. One patient was withdrawn
veterans were referred to the PE clinic of the after randomization but before starting treatment
outpatient psychiatric trauma unit at Sheba Medical because of severe, psychotic depression, and one
Center for participation in the current study. They dropped out during treatment; both were in the
were referred either by psychiatrists of the trauma unit 90-minute condition. The latter patient was included
or by therapists from other mental health clinics. After in intention-to-treat (ITT) analyses.
an initial screening assessment, eligible individuals Table 1 presents the descriptive information of
completed all assessments described below. Of these the study sample. Overall, the mean age of the
individuals, 39 participated in the study. Nineteen of participants was 36.87 years (SD = 13.40). Most
them were randomized to 90-minute PE sessions that participants were male (61.5%), single (46.2%;

FIGURE 1 Consolidated standards of reporting trials diagram of participant flow through the protocol. Note. PE = prolonged exposure;
PTSD = posttraumatic distress disorder; IE = imaginal exposure.* Not in analyses.
332 nacasch et al.

Table 1
Clinical Characteristics and Raw Scores of Symptoms and Trauma-Related Cognitions by Group
Variable 60-min PE 90-min PE
(n = 20) (n = 19)
n % n %
Male 14 70.0 10 52.6
Employed 12 60.0 9 47.3
Marital status
Single 10 50.0 8 42.1
Married 6 30.0 10 52.6
Divorced 4 20.0 1 5.3
Index trauma type
Single terror attack 8 40.0 7 36.8
Combat 5 25.0 6 31.6
Car accident 4 20.0 3 15.8
Other 3 15.0 3 15.8
Injury in trauma 13 65.0 12 63.2
Comorbidity
Major depressive disorder 5 25.0 4 21.1
Anxiety disorder a 11 55.0 2 10.5

n M SD n M SD Cohen’s d
Age (years) 20 34.40 12.24 19 39.47 14.39 0.38
Time since trauma (years) 20 9.18 13.31 19 6.57 9.94 0.22
PSS-I
Pre 20 32.20 5.96 19 31.58 6.38 0.10
Post 20 13.30 9.52 17 12.24 8.02 0.12
FU 14 13.57 9.26 15 12.20 7.78 0.16
BDI
Pre 15 23.13 8.71 19 20.11 9.60 0.33
Post 18 10.89 9.16 15 10.93 8.98 0.00
PTCI
Pre 13 134.08 36.88 14 132.21 30.29 0.06
Post 16 87.25 40.27 16 91.94 37.26 − 0.12
Note. PE = prolonged exposure; PSS-I = PTSD Symptoms–Interview version; Pre = pretreatment; Post = posttreatment; FU = follow-up;
BDI = Beck Depressive Inventory; PTCI = Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory.
a
Other than posttraumatic disorder.

41.0% married, 17.8% divorced), and employed the short-duration group (60-minute PE session)
(60.0%). The most prevalent index trauma (i.e., the were more likely to have another comorbid anxiety
traumatic event that participants endorsed as the disorder than those in the long-duration group:
most distressing and that was the focus of 90-minute PE session; 55.0% vs. 10.5%, χ 2 (1) =
treatment) was terrorist attack (38.5%), followed 4.50, p = .03. The study protocol was approved by
by combat (28.2%), car accidents (17.9%), and the Sheba Medical Center Institutional Review
sexual (childhood sexual abuse, rape) and nonsexual Board.
assault (15.4%). Psychiatric comorbidity was com-
mon, with 56.4% of the sample having at least one design
comorbid Axis I disorder. The most common All eligible individuals were provided written
comorbid conditions were current major depressive informed consent after receiving a description of
disorder (23.1%) and obsessive–compulsive disorder the study procedures. They were told that some
(23.1%). participants will receive ten to fifteen 60-minute
There were no significant differences between the sessions with 20-minute imaginal exposure to their
two conditions in demographics, index trauma type, most upsetting traumatic memory and other
and clinical characteristics. However, participants in participants will receive ten to fifteen 90-minute
sixty vs. ninety minutes of prolonged exposure 333

sessions with 40-minute imaginal exposure to their BDI is rated on a 4-point scale, with the total score
trauma memory. All participants first received two ranging from 0 (no symptoms) to 63 (very severe); 14
90-minute sessions (for details, see below). to 19 is considered mild, 20 to 28 is moderate, and
above 28 is considered severe. Internal consistency
assessment has been found to be high (α = 0.81–0.86; Beck,
Assessments of current PTSD symptoms were con- Steer, & Garbin, 1988). Scores on the BDI have been
ducted by a master’s degree psychologist unaware of shown to correlate highly with clinician ratings of
treatment assignment. Assessments were conducted depression among female physical and sexual assault
before and after treatment. Outcome assessments survivors (Foa et al., 1993).
included the interview of PTSD severity and self-
report measures described below. At 6-month Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI). The
follow-up, outcome via interview was assessed, but PTCI (Foa, Ehlers, Clark, Tolin, & Orsillo, 1999) is a
no self-report measures were administered. Psychol- 36-item self-report inventory of trauma-related
ogists who conducted assessments were uninvolved thoughts and beliefs that includes three subscales:
in treatment, and patients were asked not to disclose negative cognitions about the self, negative cogni-
their treatment condition to the evaluators. Given tions about the world, and self-blame. Respondents
that all patients received PE, discussion of the content rate the extent to which they agree or disagree with
of treatment would not make the evaluators aware of each statement on a scale of 1 (totally disagree) to 7
their treatment condition. (totally agree). Total scores are calculated as the sum
of all items, with higher scores indicating greater
measures
endorsement of negative cognitions. The total scale
Diagnostic Interview and self, world, and self-blame subscales have high
The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview internal consistency (.97, .97, .88, and .86, respec-
5.0 (MINI). The MINI (Sheehan et al., 1998) is a tively) and good test–retest reliability (between .74
short, structured diagnostic interview that was used and .79 for 1 week in one sample, and between .80
in the study to determine that the primary diagnosis and .85 for 3 weeks in a second sample; see also Zalta
was PTSD and to detect the presence of other Axis I et al., 2013). The PTCI total score correlates strongly
disorders. The interview instrument is designed to with measures of PTSD severity (.79) and depression
cover 17 major Axis I disorders and has good (.75), and state- (.70) and trait anxiety (.75).
correlation with the Structured Clinical Interview for
the DSM-IV (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, Subjective Units of Distress (SUDs). The SUDs
1994). scale (Wolpe & Lazarus, 1966) is commonly used as
a subjective verbal rating of discomfort. During the
Primary Outcome Measure
imaginal exposure, anxiety levels were monitored
The Posttraumatic Symptom Scale-Interview
regularly using the SUDs on a 0–100 scale, with 0
(PSS-I). The PSS-I (Foa et al., 1993) is a semistruc-
indicating no feelings of discomfort at all and 100 the
tured interview that consists of 17 items correspond-
maximum level of discomfort. SUDs were obtained
ing to the DSM-IV PTSD symptoms. Items are rated
from the patients at the beginning of the imaginal
on a scale of 0 (not at all) to 3 (five or more times a
exposure (SUDs start), every 5 minutes during the
week/almost always) for combined frequency and
imaginal exposure, and at the end of the imaginal
severity in the past 2 weeks. Total scores range from 0
exposure (SUDs end).
to 51 with higher scores indicating more severe PTSD
symptoms; 10 to 15 is considered subclinical, 15 to 20 therapists
is mild to moderate, 21 to 25 is moderate, 26 to 30 is Therapy was delivered by eight therapists who were
severe, and above 30 is very severe. Interrater trained and supervised in PE. Therapists were two
reliability for PTSD diagnosis (r = .91) and overall psychiatrists, two master’s degree psychologists,
severity (ICC = .97) have been shown to be excellent and four master’s degree social workers. Therapists
(for psychometric data see also Foa & Tolin, 2000; participated in a 4-day workshop for PE conducted
Powers, Gillihan, Rosenfield, Jerud, & Foa, 2012). by the first and last authors and were supervised in
Secondary Outcome Measures a training case by an expert trainer who has taught
Beck Depression Inventory (BDI). The BDI (Beck, PE and supervised PE cases for over 10 years (first
Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961) is a author) prior to the start of the study. After the
21-item self-report inventory designed to measure workshop, a supervision group was established.
symptoms of depression, and addresses the intensity The group met once weekly throughout the study
of cognitive, affective somatic and performance- and supervision was conducted through viewing
related symptoms with regard to depression. The about 15% of all tapes to ensure adherence to the
334 nacasch et al.

treatment. Because all therapists conducted the conducted in a similar fashion: homework review,
same treatment (PE), had been using PE as their imaginal exposure for 20 or 40 minutes followed by
primary modality for treating PTSD for at least a discussion of the imaginal exposure (processing),
5 years, and received weekly supervision conducted and assignment of in vivo exposure homework.
by an expert trainer, no formal adherence ratings Session 3 included rationale and initiation of
were deemed necessary. imaginal exposure in which patients were asked to
close their eyes and recount the traumatic memory
treatments aloud. The trauma narratives were audiotaped, and
PE was implemented according to the treatment the patients were asked to listen to the tapes daily.
manual (Foa et al., 2007). The same therapists During the first three imaginal exposures (Sessions 3–
delivered both types of treatment. Number of 5) patients were guided to repeatedly recount the
sessions of those who completed treatment ranged traumatic memories from the beginning to the end
from 10 to 15. Treatment was determined to be within the allotted time (i.e., 20 or 40 minutes). In
complete when both the supervisor and therapist Sessions 6–14, patients were guided to repeatedly
agreed that the patient’s self-reported symptoms of imagine and recount the most upsetting part of the
PTSD remained low and stable. This rationale was trauma (i.e., “hot spots”) within the allotted time.
chosen on the basis of a prior finding that the The final session consisted of imaginal exposure to
majority of patients who did not reach at least 70% the entire trauma memory once, discussing the
reduction in PTSD symptoms after Session 9 changes that occurred during treatment and the
benefited from additional sessions (Foa et al., treatment component they found most helpful.
2005). The overall mean number of sessions was Patients were asked to listen to a recording of the
13.41 (SD = 2.39). The mean number of sessions for whole session (60 or 90 minutes) once per week and
the short-duration group (M = 13.60, SD = 2.23) to listen to the imaginal exposure recordings (20 or
and long-duration group (M = 13.21, SD = 2.59) 40 minutes) daily as homework. The treatment
were similar, F(1, 37) = 0.25, p = .60. In practice, delivered in the trial followed the protocol used in
patients received either 10(24%), 12(8%), or previous RCTs for PE (e.g., Foa, Dancu, et al., 1999;
15(68%) sessions, and there was no difference Foa et al., 2005).
between groups in the percentage who received 15
sessions, χ 2(1) = .21, p = .65. manipulation check
One hundred and fifty-five sessions (36% of the
Prerandomization Phase: Sessions 1 and 2
total Sessions 3–15) were examined with regard to
This phase comprised two 90-minute sessions.
length of session and the length of the imaginal
Session 1 included presentation of treatment
exposure in 21 randomly selected patients (11 in the
rationale, information gathering about trauma
long-duration group and 10 in the short-duration
history, identification of the most disturbing trauma,
group). In the short-duration group, the mean
and breathing retraining. Session 2 included educa-
length of sessions was 64 minutes and 43 seconds
tion about trauma-related symptoms, rationale and
(SD = 4.70 minutes) and the mean length of
construction of in vivo exposure hierarchy, and in
imaginal exposure was 19 minutes and 27 seconds
vivo homework assignments. In vivo exposure
(SD = 1.64 minutes). In the long-duration group,
involved confrontation of situations that were
the mean length of sessions was 87 minutes and
avoided either because they were trauma reminders
54 seconds (SD = 6.11 minutes) and the mean
or because they were similar to the traumatic event,
length of imaginal exposure was 38 minutes and
and thus evoked fear that the traumatic event will
11 seconds (SD = 3.55 minutes). The mean length
occur again. Patients began to confront low-distress-
of sessions was significantly different between
level situations and proceeded gradually to increas-
groups, F(1, 19) = 89.50, p b .001; the mean length
ingly more distressing ones.
of imaginal exposure was also significantly different
Postrandomization Treatment Phase: Sessions 3–15 between groups, F(1, 19) = 232.60, p b .01.
The treatment was identical in the two conditions
with two differences: the short-duration group statistical analyses
received 60-minute sessions with 20-minute imaginal For Hypothesis 1, our primary analysis, we used tests
exposure (in which they were asked to revisit the for noninferiority of the shorter-duration group
traumatic memory in their imagination and by relative to the longer-duration group (Blackwelder,
recounting the story of the trauma aloud), whereas 2004). This is done by examining whether the
the long-duration group received 90-minute sessions difference between the groups at posttreatment and
with 40-minute imaginal exposure. All other aspects at follow-up is smaller than a predetermined
of the sessions were similar. Sessions 3–14 were clinically reliable difference (i.e., the noninferiority
sixty vs. ninety minutes of prolonged exposure 335

margin [“delta”]). In contrast to traditional analyses, To assess effect sizes of group differences, raw data
a significant effect in noninferiority tests suggests that were used to calculate standard Cohen’s d (Cohen,
outcome in the short-duration group is noninferior to 1988) between groups using pooled variance.
the outcome of the long-duration group. We took Model-based estimates of d were also calculated
two approaches to define delta. First, for each from estimates and standard errors in the mixed-
measure, the deltas were determined using the effects repeated measures models.
reliable change index developed by Jacobson and For Hypothesis 2, between-session and within-
Truax (1991) and determined for PTSD studies by session habituation were examined by recording
Devilly and Foa (2001). This index represents the subjective units of distress during imaginal exposures
difference between two assessments of a given scale, from each chart (cf. Jaycox et al., 1998; Kozak, Foa,
which is expected by chance, and is calculated using & Steketee, 1988; van Minnen & Hagenaars, 2002).
the data of the standard deviation of the scale and We used the first (Session 3) and last (the session
test–retest reliability. Using this formula and data before the final session) imaginal exposure sessions.
from previous studies about our measures (Foa, Between-session habituation was calculated by
Ehlers, et al., 1999; Oliver & Simmons, 1985; subtracting the peak SUDs of the last session from
Powers et al., 2012), we determined that the reliable the peak SUDs of Session 3 (cf. van Minnen & Foa,
change would be 7 points for PSS-I, 4.5 points for 2006). 2 In addition, for both of these imaginal
BDI, and 22 points for PTCI. Our clinical experience exposure sessions, the degree of within-session
suggests that these differences are also clinically habituation was computed by subtracting the end
significant. Second, to provide an “effect size” scale SUDs rating from the peak SUDs rating.
for assessing the size of the difference between the For Hypothesis 3, change in PTSD symptoms and
two groups, we calculated the Cohen’s d (Cohen, change in cognitions were calculated as residualized
1988) effect size between the two groups. While these change scores from pre- to posttreatment in the PSS-I
“standardized effect sizes” have been criticized and PTCI, respectively, and Pearson correlation
(Hoenig & Heisey, 2001; Lenth, 2001) as over- coefficients were used to measure within-person
simplifying the process of sample size determination, association between the two change scores.
they provide a useful scale for comparison of
Results
observed differences. For our purposes, we would
regard effect sizes of less than d = 0.2 (i.e., less than a noninferiority between shorter- and
“small” effect) as supporting the hypothesis of longer-duration groups
noninferiority. We used a one-sided 95% confidence See Table 1 for means, standard deviations, and
level and assumed a t distribution for analyses effect sizes of the outcome measures.
calculating the difference of the means of the two
treatments and adding or subtracting the product of posttraumatic symptoms: pss-i
the pooled standard error by the critical t value For the PSS-I outcomes, tests of noninferiority
needed for rejecting the null hypothesis. A priori suggested that the short-duration group was non-
power analyses for noninferiority studies (Julious, inferior to the long-duration group with respect to the
2004) indicated that in order to achieve a power of PSS-I score at posttreatment, t(35) = 2.10, p = .02,
.80, with a d value of 7 and a standard deviation of 8 95% CI of observed delta [− 3.88, 6.00], and at
on the PSS-I, a sample of 34 would be needed. 1 follow-up, t(27) = 1.85, p = .04, 95% CI [− 4.02,
The data were also examined using multilevel 6.77]; in each case, the upper endpoint of the
linear models, which use all available responses confidence interval for the observed short minus
for each subject, and so enable full ITT analyses long delta is less than our reliable change index,
(i.e., 60-minute PE: n = 20; 90-minute PE: n = 19). indicating noninferiority (see Figure 2). In addition,
The models allowed an unstructured covariance the observed effect sizes for differences between
matrix for the repeated measures within subjects, groups were d = 0.12 for the posttreatment point and
and treated visit as a categorical variable. Models d = 0.16 for follow-up, both in the “small effect”
were fitted for the PSS-I (using the pretreatment, range of Cohen’s d scale.
posttreatment, and follow-up times) and for the According to multilevel analyses on the PSS-I pre-
BDI and PTCI (pre- and posttreatment only). to posttreatment, there was a main effect of time,
F(1, 67.55) = 25.84, p b .001, suggesting a

1 2
Note that the power analysis was calculated only for the main When between-session habituation was defined as the change
outcome measure, the PSS-I. The formula for power for effect sizes in peak SUDs from the third to fourth session, there was no
for noninferiority we used was n = f(α, β) × 2 × σ2/d2 and f(α, significant difference between groups, t(27) = .52, p = .61, and
β) = [Φ−1(α) + Φ−1(β)]2. correlations with outcomes were not significant, rs b .15, ps N .60.
336 nacasch et al.

group: t(37) = 10.88, p b .001; 19.78 unit decrease


for longer-duration group: t(37) = 10.53, p b .001),
with no significant change between posttreatment
and follow-up for either group (p = 0.52 for shorter-
duration group, p = 0.80 for longer-duration
group). Model-based versions of Cohen’s d (based
on least squares means and standard errors) were
0.05, 0.08, and 0.17, for the pretreatment, posttreat-
ment, and follow-up points, respectively. The differ-
ences for the posttreatment and follow-up points are
similar to those found above by direct calculation,
suggesting that missing data had little effect on the
comparisons. As noted above, the effect sizes are all
in the “small” range. In summary, results indicate
that both groups had substantial reduction in
PTSD symptoms, but that there were no apparent
differences between groups at posttreatment or at
follow-up.

depressive symptoms: bdi


For the BDI, we found that tests of noninferiority
approached significance when examining post-
treatment outcomes, t(31) = 1.44, p = .08, 95%
CI [− 5.43, 5.34], suggesting noninferiority. The
observed Cohen’s d between groups was 0.00.
Multilevel analysis of the BDI scores showed a main
effect of time, F(1, 32.5) = 17.68, p b .01, and no
significant Group × Time interaction effect, F(1,
32.4) = 1.99, p = .17. In summary, both groups
demonstrated significant reduction in depression,
with no apparent differences between groups.

change in cognitions: ptci


Noninferiority tests suggested that the short-duration
FIGURE 2 Noninferiority margins and 95% confidence intervals
group was noninferior to the long-duration group
(CI) for difference in outcomes between treatment conditions.
with regard to PTSD-related negative cognitions
Error bars indicate two-sided 95% CIs. The dashed lines at y = Δ
indicate the minimum clinically reliable difference, or the level at posttreatment, t(30) = 2.17, p = .02, 95%
noninferiority margin (Δ = 7 for PSS-I, Δ = 4.5 for BDI, Δ = 22 CI [− 27.97, 18.59], Cohen d = − .12 (see Figure 2).
for PTCI). The vertical axis represents differences in PSS-I, BDI, and Multilevel analysis showed no significant Group ×
PTCI scores between conditions, with negative values indicating Time interaction, F(1, 32) = .28, p = .60, a signifi-
that 60-minute PE sessions resulted in greater improvement in cant effect of time, F(1, 32) = 30.16, p b .0001, and
outcome measures than 90-minute PE sessions. Note. PTSD = no significant group effect F(1, 32) = 0.00, p = .95.
posttraumatic stress disorder; PSS-I = PTSD Symptoms–Interview Model-based Cohen’s d were .05 for pretreatment
version; BDI = Beck Depressive Inventory; PTCI = Posttraumatic and –.06 for posttreatment. In summary, both groups
Cognitions Inventory. demonstrated significant reductions in negative
cognitions, with no apparent differences between
substantial decrease in PSS-I over time in both groups.
groups, but there was no Group × Time interaction,
F(1, 68.22) = .12, p = .73, suggesting that there was within- and between-session habituation
not a significant differential change between the two Table 2 presents data on the within- and between-
groups. Results from post to follow-up did not show session habituation (also see Figure 3). These data
either a significant effect of time, F(1, 69.51) = 0.17, were collected from an outpatient clinic, so some
p = .68, or a Group × Time interaction effect, F(1, cases did not include systematic collection of SUDs
68.97) = 0.17, p = .68. The acute time effect was due data as part of the clinical record and were therefore
to significant decrease from pre- to posttreatment for missing. As expected, patients in the long-duration
each group (18.90 unit decrease for shorter-duration group showed significantly more within-session
sixty vs. ninety minutes of prolonged exposure 337

Table 2
Within- and Between-Session Habituation Based on the SUDs Rating During First and Last Exposure Sessions
Variable 60-min PE 90-min PE t p Cohen’s
(n = 16) (n = 12) d
M SD M SD
Session 3
Peak SUDs 78.63 20.07 84.58 11.96 0.91 .37 0.36
End SUDs 71.44 20.46 58.33 16.83 1.81 .08 0.70
Within-session habituation 7.19 6.32 26.25 20.68 3.09 .010 1.25
Last session
Peak SUDs 45.00 13.23 30.21 18.72 2.41 .02 0.91
End SUDs 29.67 14.20 24.79 19.08 0.76 .45 0.29
Within-session habituation 15.33 15.06 5.42 8.11 2.05 .05 0.82
Between-session habituation 36.44 24.63 54.38 16.52 2.18 .04 0.86
Note. PE = prolonged exposure; SUDs = subjective units of distress.

habituation in Session 3, t(27) = 3.50, p = .002, and correlated with each other, r(28) = .29, p = .13;
a trend toward less within-session habituation in the r(27) = − .24, p = .22, respectively.
last session, t(27) = 0.76, p = .05, than patients in When both between-session habituation and
the short-duration group. The long-duration group change in PTCI were simultaneously entered into a
had significantly more between-session habituation regression equation to predict reduction in PSS-I
than the short-duration group, t(27) = 2.18, p = .04. symptoms, the overall model approached significance,
F(2, 15) = 2.59, p = .11, and there was no substantial
correlations among process and outcome change in the partial correlations. Both predictors
variables approached significance (PTCI partial r = .28;
As expected, reduction in PSS-I symptoms was between-session habituation partial r = − .45, p =
significantly correlated with reductions in negative .07), suggesting that cognitive change and between-
cognitions as measured by the PTCI, r(25) = .41, session habituation may be separate indicators of
p = .04. In addition, reduction in PSS-I was emotional processing in PE.
significantly related to greater between-session
habituation, r(28) = − .40, p = .04, but not to greater Discussion
early, r(28) = − .17, p = .39, or late, r(27) = .07, The hypothesis that 60-minute PE sessions with
p = .72, within-session habituation. 20-minute imaginal exposures would yield non-
Reduction in PTCI was not correlated with either inferior outcomes to that of 90-minute sessions
between-, r(18) = .01, p = .97, or early or later with 40-minute imaginal exposures was supported;
within-session habituation, r(18) = − .20, p = .44; no group differences were detected on PTSD symp-
r(17) = .12, p = .66, respectively. Furthermore, toms, depression, and trauma-related cognitions, and
within- and between-session habituation were not noninferiority tests were either significant or close to
significant. Our finding, using an RCT design, that
both the long- and short-duration PE result in similar
outcomes is consistent with van Minnen and Foa’s
100.0 (2006) findings from a nonrandomized sample of 92
90.0 patients. Together, these two studies suggest that
duration of the PE sessions and the imaginal exposure
80.0
SUDs 70.0
60.0
60-min PE
90-min PE
can be considerably shortened without sacrificing the
50.0
40.0
benefit patients derive from treatment. In fact, in the
30.0 present study the imaginal exposure was further
20.0 shortened from van Minnen and Foa’s 30- to
20-minute imaginal exposure. By greatly reducing
10.0
0.0
peak first
exposure
end first
exposure
peak last
exposure
end last
exposure
the duration of the imaginal exposure, we were able
Session
to address successfully the PE treatment components
approximately within the 1-hour “standard” treat-
FIGURE 3 Subjective Units of Distress (SUDs) scale scores ment session duration (after a thorough prerandomi-
during first and last imaginal exposures for completers in 60- and zation 2- to 3-hour assessment, which was not
90-minute PE sessions. Note. PE = prolonged exposure. included in time calculations of the treatment). This
338 nacasch et al.

adaptation renders the treatment less burdensome for The present study showed that PE resulted in a
both patients and therapists and more applicable to clinically significant reduction in explicit trauma-
community clinical settings that are required to related negative cognitions that the world is extremely
follow the standard session duration regulated by dangerous and that one is extremely incompetent,
reimbursement policy. The current findings should be with similar reductions in both groups. We also found
replicated in larger, more diverse populations to a significant correlation between change in negative
determine the generalizability of the results. Notably, cognitions and change in PTSD symptom severity.
one should not interpret the current findings to Our results are consistent with prior research and
suggest that imaginal exposure shorter than 20 mi- replicate Foa and Rauch’s (2004) study that found
nutes will also yield a successful outcome. The significant correlations between reduction in negative
efficacy of briefer exposure sessions (e.g., less than cognitions and reduction in PTSD symptoms. In a
20 minutes) is unknown until empirically studied. further study examining the role of cognitions in
As hypothesized, 40-minute imaginal exposures ameliorating PTSD via PE, McLean, Su, and Foa
resulted in greater within-session habituation than (2015) found that reduction in negative cognitions
20-minute exposures. In addition, consistent with mediated symptom reduction in PE. Similarly, Zalta
van Minnen and Foa’s (2006) findings, we detected et al. (2013) found that reduction in trauma-related
a significant relationship between PTSD symptom negative cognitions preceded reduction of PTSD
change and between-session habituation. Contrary symptom severity. Taken together, these findings,
to our hypothesis, however, 40-minute imaginal along with those of other studies, support EPT
exposures resulted in greater between-session hypotheses that erroneous cognitions about the self
habituation than 20-minute exposures. This finding and the world underlie PTSD and that modification of
is somewhat different from that of van Minnen and these cognitions is a potential mechanism of PE (Foa
Foa’s study, in which within-session but not between- & Cahill, 2001). Future studies should consider use of
session habituation was greater in the 60-minute than measures of cognitions that are less biased by demand
in the 30-minute imaginal exposures. This inconsis- characteristics such as implicit measures or reaction
tency may be due to the differences in the duration of time paradigms.
imaginal exposures in the two studies: 60 versus It is important to note that 20 minutes of
40 minutes and 30 versus 20 minutes. In the present imaginal exposure was sufficient for most patients
study the long-duration group showed substantial to recount the traumatic memory at least once, and
between-session habituation (M = 54.38 SUDs), that during the 40-minute imaginal exposure patients
which was significantly greater than that of the recounted their trauma two to three times. The
short-duration group (M = 36.44 SUDs). In contrast, findings that patients who recounted their traumatic
in the van Minnen and Foa study, between-session memory for 20 minutes experience the same reduc-
habituation did not differ between the long- and tion in trauma-related negative cognitions as those
short-duration groups (Ms = 20.2 and 21.1, respec- who recounted it for 40 minutes suggest that
tively). This may suggest that a minimal amount of recounting the traumatic memory once in each
between-session habituation is required for symptom session (and then listening to the audiotapes of their
reduction, and that any gains beyond this minimum recounting as homework) is sufficient for many
are only partially contributing to emotional process- patients to disconfirm their erroneous beliefs that
ing and outcomes. In further support of this engaging with the traumatic memory (rather than
interpretation we found that while both the 20- and avoiding it) would result in them “falling apart”
40-minute imaginal exposure groups had maximum during such engagement. This experience allows the
between-session habituation levels of over 80 SUDs, changing of patients’ negative cognitions about
the 20-minute group included a significant propor- themselves.
tion of patients (7/16) whose level of between-session Interestingly, although the 90-minute PE group
habituation was lower than any of the patients in the received twice as much imaginal exposure time
40-minute group. Despite this difference, the overall compared with the 60-minute PE group, the
relationship of symptom reduction and between- number of sessions to reach criteria for treatment
session habituation was similar in both groups. This completion was the same in both conditions. This
further supports the suggestion that between-session suggests that more imaginal exposure time does not
habituation is related to outcome, but is not a very expedite symptom reduction. Otherwise, participants
strong indicator (Foa et al., 2006). Notably, the in the 90-minute PE group would have completed
variance in outcome accounted for by between- treatment earlier than the 60-minute group. Future
session habituation and outcome was 16%, which studies should examine this issue further by collecting
allows for many other factors to contribute to a measure of session-by-session PTSD symptom
symptom improvement. severity.
sixty vs. ninety minutes of prolonged exposure 339

limitations cognitions at posttreatment and follow-up to the


Some caveats of our study should be recognized 40-minute imaginal exposures and 90-minute
while interpreting the current findings. First, the sessions. The current study also showed that longer
sample size was small and the study should be imaginal exposures resulted in greater within- and
replicated with a larger sample. Second, partici- between-session habituation than shorter expo-
pants were randomized prior to Session 3 rather sures. However, because both conditions exhibited
than before the beginning of treatment. Although similar efficacy, it can be reasoned that maximal
the differences in duration of treatment sessions and within- and between-session habituation may not
imaginal exposures started after the randomization, be necessary conditions for successful PE, although
the possible influence of the fact that the first two some level of between-session habituation is related
sessions were equivalent in both groups on the to outcomes (cf. Foa et al., 2006). In contrast,
current findings remains unknown. It is possible negative cognitions and PTSD symptom severity
that if therapists wish to remain within 60 minutes decreased similarly in the long- and short-treat-
for the introductory sessions, it may require three, ment-duration groups and were correlated with
rather than two, sessions prior to starting imaginal reduction of PTSD symptoms severity. These
exposures. Indeed, three introductory sessions were findings support EPT’s supposition that within-
used in an uncontrolled study of PE in the Veterans and between-session habituation are indicators of
Administration in the United States (Eftekhari et al., emotional processing, whereas reduction of nega-
2013). In general, therapists may have to be tive, erroneous cognitions is, as noted above, a
more efficient with time during 60-minute sessions potential mechanism underlying emotional process-
than 90-minute sessions. Third, the psychometric ing and the resultant symptom reduction. Indeed,
properties of the study measures (e.g., interrater change in negative cognitions and between-session
reliability and percentage agreement of the diagnosis) habituation—but not within-session habituation—
and therapist adherence to protocol were not was correlated with treatment outcome and none of
evaluated in the current study. Fourth, homework the three were correlated with one another.
compliance was not measured, and could have
differed between groups. Fifth, the sample was Disclosure Statement for Authors
relatively homogeneous in terms of gender, ethnicity, All authors report no financial relationships with
and traumas. Taken together with van Minnen and commercial interests.
Foa (2006), however, these results may hold across Conflict of Interest Statement
populations and cultures, but more data should be The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest.
collected on this. Sixth, no data on trauma severity
was collected. Perhaps shorter sessions are insuffi-
cient to address the treatment needs of those exposed References
to more severe trauma events. However, the group Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., &
did not differ in the number of participants who Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression.
were injured during the trauma, a common indicator Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004
of trauma severity. In addition, data were not Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric
available to examine the session-by-session change properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five
in between- or within-session habituation and their years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8(1),
relationships with symptom change, and the data 77–100.
on within- and between-session habituation were Blackwelder, W. C. (2004). Current issues in clinical
equivalence trials. Journal of Dental Research, 83,
partial due to therapists not recording these data C113–C115.
systematically. This left these analyses with moderate Brewin, C. R. (2006). Understanding cognitive behaviour
power (approximately .65, below the desired therapy: A retrieval competition account. Behaviour Research
Cohen’s d of .80). The lack of systematic collection and Therapy, 44(6), 765–784.
of SUDs could have made the data less reliable and Bryant, R. A., Moulds, M. L., Guthrie, R. M., Dang, S. T.,
Mastrodomenico, J., Nixon, R. D. V., . . . Creamer, M.
influenced the results. Finally, it should be noted that (2008). A randomized controlled trial of exposure therapy
in this study, as well as in previous studies, a self- and cognitive restructuring for posttraumatic stress disorder.
report inventory was used to measure negative Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 695–703.
cognitions. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012616
Bryant, R. A., Moulds, M. L., Guthrie, R. M., Dang, S. T., &
Conclusions Nixon, R. D. V. (2003). Imaginal exposure alone and
imaginal exposure with cognitive restructuring in treatment
In sum, 20-minute imaginal exposure within of posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and
60-minute sessions yielded noninferior outcomes Clinical Psychology, 71, 706–712. http://dx.doi.org/
in PTSD symptoms and posttraumatic negative 10.1037/0022-006X.71.4.706
340 nacasch et al.

Chaplin, E. W., & Levine, B. A. (1981). The effects of total Jacobson, N. S., & Truax, P. (1991). Clinical significance: A
exposure duration and interrupted versus continuous statistical approach to defining meaningful change in
exposure in flooding therapy. Behavior Therapy, 12, psychotherapy research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
360–368. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(81)80124-4 Psychology, 59, 12–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral 0022-006X.59.1.12
sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Jaycox, L. H., Foa, E. B., & Morral, A. R. (1998). Influence of
Devilly, G. J., & Foa, E. B. (2001). The investigation of emotional engagement and habituation on exposure therapy
exposure and cognitive therapy: Comment on Tarrier et al. for PTSD. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
(1999). Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 66, 185.
114–116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.69.1.114 Julious, S. A. (2004). Sample sizes for clinical trials with normal
Eftekhari, A., Ruzek, J. I., Crowley, J. J., Rosen, C. S., data. Statistics in Medicine, 23, 1921–1986. http://dx.doi.org/
Greenbaum, M. A., & Karlin, B. E. (2013). Effectiveness 10.1002/sim.1783
of national implementation of prolonged exposure therapy Karlin, B. E., Ruzek, J. I., Chard, K. M., Eftekhari, A., Monson,
in veterans affairs care. JAMA Psychiatry, 70, 949–955. C. M., Hembree, E. A., . . . Foa, E. B. (2010). Dissemination of
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.36 evidence‐based psychological treatments for posttraumatic
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. stress disorder in the Veterans Health Administration. Journal
(1994). Structured clinical interview for Axis I DSM-IV of Traumatic Stress, 23, 663–673. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
disorders. New York, NY: Biometrics Research. jts.20588
Foa, E. B., & Cahill, S. P. (2001). Psychological therapies: Kozak, M. J., Foa, E. B., & Steketee, G. (1988). Process and
Emotional processing. In S. N. J. B. P. B. (Ed.), International outcome of exposure treatment with obsessive-compulsives:
encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences Psychophysiological indicators of emotional processing.
(pp. 12363–12369). Oxford, UK: Elsevier. Behavor Therapy, 19, 157–169. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
Foa, E. B., Dancu, C. V., Hembree, E. A., Jaycox, L. H., S0005-7894(88)80039-X
Meadows, E. A., & Street, G. P. (1999). A comparison of Lenth, R. V. (2001). Some practical guidelines for effective sample
exposure therapy, stress inoculation training, and their size determination. The American Statistician, 55, 187–193.
combination for reducing posttraumatic stress disorder in http://dx.doi.org/10.1198/000313001317098149
female assault victims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Marks, I., Lovell, K., Noshirvani, H., Livanou, M., & Thrasher, S.
Psychology, 67, 194–200. (1998). Treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder by
Foa, E. B., Ehlers, A., Clark, D. M., Tolin, D. F., & Orsillo, S. exposure and/or cognitive restructuring: A controlled study.
M. (1999). The Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI): Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 317–325. http://dx.doi.org/
Development and validation. Psychological Assessment, 11, 10.1001/archpsyc.55.4.317
303–314. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.11.3.303 McLean, C. P., & Foa, E. B. (2011). Prolonged exposure
Foa, E. B., Hembree, E. A., Cahill, S. P., Rauch, S. A., Riggs, therapy for post-traumatic stress disorder: A review of
D.S., Feeny, N. C., & Yadin, E. (2005). Randomized trial of evidence and dissemination. [Review]. Expert Review of
prolonged exposure for posttraumatic stress disorder with Neurotherapeutics, 11, 1151–1163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1586/
and without cognitive restructuring: Outcome at academic ern.11.94
and community clinics. Journal of Consulting and Clinical McLean, C. P., Su, Y. J., & Foa, E. B. (2015). Mechanisms of
Psychology, 73, 953–964. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ symptom reduction in a combined treatment for comorbid
0022-006X.73.5.953 posttraumatic stress disorder and alcohol dependence.
Foa, E. B., Hembree, E. A., & Rothbaum, B. O. (2007). Manuscript in preparation.
Prolonged exposure therapy for PTSD: Emotional processing Nacasch, N., Foa, E. B., Huppert, J. D., Tzur, D., Fostick, L.,
of traumatic experiences: Therapist guide. New York, NY: Dinstein, Y., ... Zohar, J. (2011). Prolonged exposure
Oxford University Press. therapy for combat- and terror-related posttraumatic stress
Foa, E. B., Huppert, J. D., & Cahill, S. P. (2006). Emotional disorder: A randomized control comparison with treatment
processing theory: An update. In B. O. Rothbaum (Ed.), as usual. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 72, 1174–1180.
Pathological anxiety: Emotional processing in etiology and http://dx.doi.org/10.4088/JCP.09m05682blu
treatment (pp. 3–24). New York, NY: Guildford Press. Oliver, J. M., & Simmons, M. E. (1985). Affective disorders
Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: and depression as measured by the Diagnostic Interview
Exposure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin, Schedule and the Beck Depression Inventory in an unselected
99, 20–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.99.1.20 adult population. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41, 469–477.
Foa, E. B., & Rauch, S. A. (2004). Cognitive changes during http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(198507)41:4b469::
prolonged exposure versus prolonged exposure plus cognitive AID-JCLP2270410405N3.0.CO;2-R
restructuring in female assault survivors with posttraumatic Powers, M. B., Gillihan, S. J., Rosenfield, D., Jerud, A. B., &
stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Foa, E. B. (2012). Reliability and validity of the PDS and PSS-I
72, 879–884. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.72.5.879 among participants with PTSD and alcohol dependence.
Foa, E. B., Riggs, D. S., Dancu, C. V., & Rothbaum, B. O. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 26, 617–623. http://dx.doi.org/
(1993). Reliability and validity of a brief instrument for 10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.02.013
assessing post‐traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Powers, M. B., Halpern, J. M., Ferenschak, M. P., Gillihan, S.J.,
Stress, 6, 459–473. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jts.2490060405 & Foa, E. B. (2010). A meta-analytic review of prolonged
Foa, E. B., & Tolin, D. F. (2000). Comparison of the PTSD exposure for posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical Psychology
Symptom Scale–Interview version and the clinician- Review, 30, 635–641. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
administered PTSD scale. Journal of Traumatic Stress, j.cpr.2010.04.007
13, 181–191. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A: Rabavilas, A. D., Boulougouris, J. C., & Stefanis, C. (1976).
1007781909213 Duration of flooding sessions in the treatment of
Hoenig, J. M., & Heisey, D. M. (2001). The abuse of power. obsessive–compulsive patients. Behaviour Research and
The American Statistician, 55, 19–24. http://dx.doi.org/ Therapy, 14, 349–355. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-
10.1198/000313001300339897 7967(76)90022-X
sixty vs. ninety minutes of prolonged exposure 341

Schneier, F. R., Neria, Y., Pavlicova, M., Hembree, E., Suh, E.J., populations: Meta-analytic validation of the Dissociative
Amsel, L., & Marshall, R. D. (2012). Combined prolonged Experiences Scale (DES). Clinical Psychology Review, 16,
exposure therapy and paroxetine for PTSD related to the 365–382. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0272-7358(96)00006-2
World Trade Center attack: A randomized controlled trial. van Minnen, A., & Foa, E. B. (2006). The effect of imaginal
American Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 80–88. http://dx.doi.org/ exposure length on outcome of treatment for PTSD. Journal
10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020321 of Trauma Stress, 19, 427–438. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., jts.20146
Janavs, J., Weiller, E., . . . Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The Mini- van Minnen, A., & Hagenaars, M. (2002). Fear activation and
International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): The habituation patterns as early process predictors of response
development and validation of a structured diagnostic to prolonged exposure treatment in PTSD. Journal of
psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 15, 359–367.
Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 22–33. Wolpe, J., & Lazarus, A. A. (1966). Behavior therapy techniques: A
Tarrier, N., Pilgrim, H., Sommerfield, C., Faragher, B., guide to the treatment of neuroses. New York, NY: Pergamon
Reynolds, M., Graham, E., & Barrowclough, C. (1999). Press.
A randomized trial of cognitive therapy and imaginal Zalta, A. K., Gillihan, S. J., Fisher, A. J., Mintz, J., Cahill, S. P.,
exposure in the treatment of chronic posttraumatic stress Yehuda, R., & Foa, E. B. (2013). Change in trauma-related
disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, cognitions drives symptom reduction in prolonged exposure
67, 13–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.67.1.13 for PTSD. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
Tuerk, P. W., Yoder, M., Grubaugh, A., Myrick, H., Hamner, 82, 171–175. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034735
M., & Acierno, R. (2011). Prolonged exposure therapy
for combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder: An
examination of treatment effectiveness for veterans of R E C E I V E D : April 16, 2014
the wars in Afghanistan and Iraq. Journal of Anxiety A C C E P T E D : December 6, 2014
Disorders, 25, 397–403. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ Available online 13 December 2014
j.janxdis.2010.11.002
van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Schuengel, C. (1996). The
measurement of dissociation in normal and clinical

You might also like