You are on page 1of 11

doi:10.1111/psyg.

12301 PSYCHOGERIATRICS 2018

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Using virtual reality to distinguish subjects with multiple- but


not single-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment from
normal elderly subjects
Alireza MOHAMMADI ,1 Mahmoud KARGAR2 and Ehsan HESAMI3

1
Neuroscience Research Center, Baqiyatallah Uni- Abstract
versity of Medical Sciences, 2Department of Speech
Therapy, School of Rehabilitation, Tehran Univer- Aim: Spatial disorientation is a hallmark of amnestic mild cognitive impair-
sity of Medical Sciences and 3Department of Speech ment (aMCI) and Alzheimer’s disease. Our aim was to use virtual reality to
Therapy, University of Social Welfare and Rehabili- determine the allocentric and egocentric memory deficits of subjects with
tation Science, Tehran, Iran single-domain aMCI (aMCIsd) and multiple-domain aMCI (aMCImd). For this
Correspondence: Dr Alireza Mohammadi PhD, purpose, we introduced an advanced virtual reality navigation task (VRNT)
Neuroscience Research Center, Baqiyatallah
to distinguish these deficits in mild Alzheimer’s disease (miAD), aMCIsd,
University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran. P.O. Box
19395-6558, Tehran 1413643561, Email: ar. and aMCImd.
mohammadi@bmsu.ac.ir Methods: The VRNT performance of 110 subjects, including 20 with miAD,
Disclosure: The authors have no potential conflicts 30 with pure aMCIsd, 30 with pure aMCImd, and 30 cognitively normal con-
of interest to declare regarding this article. trols was compared. Our newly developed VRNT consists of a virtual neigh-
bourhood (allocentric memory) and virtual maze (egocentric memory).
Received 12 May 2017; revision received 19 July 2017; Verbal and visuospatial memory impairments were also examined with Rey
accepted 26 July 2017. Auditory-Verbal Learning Test and Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test,
respectively.
Results: We found that miAD and aMCImd subjects were impaired in both
allocentric and egocentric memory, but aMCIsd subjects performed simi-
larly to the normal controls on both tasks. The miAD, aMCImd, and aMCIsd
subjects performed worse on finding the target or required more time in the
virtual environment than the aMCImd, aMCIsd, and normal controls, respec-
tively. Our findings indicated the aMCImd and miAD subjects, as well as the
aMCIsd subjects, were more impaired in egocentric orientation than allo-
centric orientation.
Conclusion: We concluded that VRNT can distinguish aMCImd subjects,
but not aMCIsd subjects, from normal elderly subjects. The VRNT, along
Key words: allocentric memory, egocentric memory, with the Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test and Rey-Osterrieth Complex
mild cognitive impairment, spatial disorientation, Figure Test, can be used as a valid diagnostic tool for properly distinguish-
virtual maze, virtual neighbourhood. ing different forms of aMCI.

challenges, it is necessary to identify these AD


INTRODUCTION
Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is the limited deterio- patients as early as possible. As such, the diagnosis
ration of cognitive abilities that is considered as a of this transitional phase as a clinical state between
transitional phase for the onset of Alzheimer’s dis- normal cognitive ageing and dementia can be impor-
ease (AD); it has a prevalence of 5.8–18.5% among tant to preventing recurrence and progression to
the elderly population (50–95 years).1–3 Given the AD.4 MCI is categorized as amnestic (aMCI) or non-
growing number of people with AD and the related amnestic (naMCI) and, based on the affected areas,

© 2018 Japanese Psychogeriatric Society 1


A. Mohammadi et al.

as occurring in a single domain or multiple domains. two main types of spatial navigation. Allocentrism
Memory impairment alone is defined as single- (object- or environment-centred) refers to the ability
domain aMCI (aMCIsd), and memory loss plus other to experience the world from a more impersonal view
cognitive impairments is defined as multiple-domain and more familiar environment; egocentrism (ego- or
aMCI (aMCImd). Similarly, naMCI is categorized as body-centred) refers to the ability to see the world
single domain or multiple domain.5–7 from a personal view, which has a vital role in preser-
Although aMCI patients may have normal general ving a stable moment-to-moment perception.31,32
cognitive functions and participate in activities of daily Studies on the neural basis of spatial navigation have
living, they are usually diagnosed based on objective indicated that the hippocampus and medial regions
memory complaints and subjective memory decline.3,8 of the temporal lobe are involved in allocentric repre-
Initial aMCI symptoms are often impaired memory sentations, whereas the parietal and striatal regions
encoding and retrieval of contextual memory. In par- are important in egocentric processing.33,34
ticular, persons with aMCI have problems remember- Some studies previously reported the spatial navi-
ing the relationship between objects or between an gation deficits of MCI and AD patients 21,35–37; these
object and its context (associative memory).9 The cog- deficits are proportional to the right/left hippocampal
nitive state of some of these patients appears to volume.23,35 Another report stressed the various diffi-
remain constant or even return to normal, but more culties in topographic memory38; it recalled the for-
than half progress to AD within 3–6 years.10–12 merly learned routes, landmarks, positions on the
Working memory (WM), a central executive func- map, and visuospatial attention.29,39–41 Weniger et al.
tion, is a type of short-term memory with a limited designed a virtual reality task to identify egocentric
capacity; it is responsible for processing and manipu- and allocentric memory differences in aMCI and con-
lating information.13,14 Although WM impairments are trol subjects. The task was superior to earlier tasks,
well known in AD,15–18 few studies have evaluated but it did not assess the domains of the aMCI dis-
WM deficits in detail in individuals with aMCIsd and ease (i.e. aMCIsd vs aMCImd) or distinguish them
aMCImd. Patalong-Ogiewa et al. found that patients from AD.35,42 Similarly, reports from Kalová et al.,43
with MCI are impaired not only in WM but also in epi- deIpolyi et al.,21 Cushman et al.,41 Nedelska et al.,23
sodic memory, suggesting that the defect is not lim- Marková et al.,37 and Laczó et al. did not classify the
ited to the hippocampus.13 Additionally, magnetic domains of the disease,44 but they did compare the
resonance imaging (MRI)-based brain volumetric performance of patients with MCI or aMCI with that
studies confirmed grey matter loss in the medial tem- of AD patients. In contrast, Hort et al. demonstrated
poral, parietal, and frontal areas of patients with the spatial navigation differences between AD,
aMCI.19,20 Furthermore, individual performance dur- naMCI, aMCIsd, and aMCImd patients using the Hid-
ing a navigation task was confidently associated with den Goal Task (HGT), which had been previously
the grey matter volume of the dorsolateral prefrontal described by Stepankova.36,45
cortex and hippocampus.21–23 Extensive neuroimag- In this study, we aimed to compare the allocentric
ing studies in adults showed that the dorsolateral and egocentric spatial navigation abilities of miAD,
prefrontal cortex, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, pre- aMCIsd, and aMCImd patients by investigating their
motor cortex, and posterior parietal cortex have criti- impairments. Because conversion to AD is more
cal roles in visuospatial working memory.24–27 common among aMCI patients (naMCI patients may
Therefore, investigating spatial memory deficits in AD develop into non-AD dementias)46 and the similarity
and MCI is important for understanding the patho- in the performance between naMCI patients and
physiology of MCI and predicting dementia.1,28 healthy controls,36,44 we did not investigate naMCI.
Spatial disorientation is one of the early manifesta- Also, based on previous studies that had similar
tions of AD and MCI, and it may become a diagnostic results from real and virtual spatial navigations,36,43,44
marker for these diseases in the near future. This we did not assess navigation in the real environment.
problem occurs when patients are in an unfamiliar Although HGT is a good task for evaluating allo-
place in the early stages of the disease, but it hap- centric and egocentric memory deficits, our
pens even in a familiar environment in advanced advanced virtual reality navigation task (VRNT) offers
stages.29,30 Allocentric and egocentric references are several advantages, including a three-dimensional

2 © 2018 Japanese Psychogeriatric Society


Multiple- but not single-domain aMCI

(3-D) and fully coloured environment and landmarks 30 cognitively normal controls (NC) with no evidence
that are more realistic looking than those of the com- of cognitive impairment, were recruited from outpa-
puter version of HGT. Moreover, HGT may be con- tient services at the Baqiyatallah Hospital (Tehran,
siderably simpler than our VRNT. As such, it seems Iran). Participants signed a standard informed con-
that our VRNT is much more accurate than HGT in sent approved by the local ethics committee. Sub-
distinguishing the subtypes of MCI. In this regard, jects’ data are presented in Table 1.
new virtual reality tools may be very useful in identify- Patients with miAD were diagnosed based on the
ing spatial navigation and memory deficits. criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Given that aMCIsd is an earlier stage of AD than Mental Disorders, 4th edition. The diagnosis of
aMCImd, we aimed to use virtual reality to detect aMCIsd and aMCImd were performed by an experi-
aMCIsd as a pre-aMCImd phase from cognitively nor- enced neurologist according to Petersen and Negash
mal controls (NC) and to prevent recurrence of the and using the following criteria47: (i) the existence of
disease and development of miAD. For this purpose, any subjective memory complaint; (ii) normal orienta-
we designed the VRNT with two virtual environments tion and general cognitive performance according to
(virtual neighbourhood and virtual maze) to separate Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score
allocentric (virtual neighbourhood) and egocentric (vir- (Persian version)48; (iii) normal score on language
tual maze) spatial navigation. The virtual neighbour- ability using Boston Naming Test; and (iv) no known
hood was designed similarly to a real neighbourhood causes of memory deficit (e.g. use of a medication
and included appropriate navigational landmarks to known to affect memory performance, major medical
help subjects find their way. In contrast, there were or neurological illness).47 Inclusion criteria for NC
no landmarks in the virtual maze, and subjects had to were as follows: (i) no established subjective memory
find the goal by recalling their route from memory. We complaint; (ii) no approved recognizable cognitive
also investigated whether there was a specific com- and memory impairment; (iii) MMSE score ≥27;
parative pattern of spatial navigation deficits among (iv) normal social functioning in the community; and
aMCIsd, aMCImd, and miAD patients. (v) no active neurologic or psychiatric disease.49

Procedure
METHODS All subjects underwent neuropsychological assess-
Participants ment and completed a visuospatial navigation task
A total of 110 subjects, including 20 with miAD, using our recently developed VRNT. It took 12 months
30 with pure aMCIsd, 30 with pure aMCImd, and to administer the tests and evaluate all subjects.

Table 1 Demographic and neuropsychological characteristics of the groups


Characteristics NC (n = 30) miAD (n = 20) aMCImd (n = 30) aMCIsd (n = 30)
Age (years) 69.867  1.432 73.65  2.476* 70.067  1.638*** 70.000  1.681***
MMSE 28.067  1.014 19.3  1.031* 26.167  0.912*,*** 27.033  0.808*,***,*****
Education (years) 13.133  2.725 11.25  2.51** 12.033  2.326 12.967  2.341
Right/left handedness (n) 30/0 20/0 30/0 30/0
Women/men (n) 15/15 13/7 19/11 18/12
Duration of disorder (years) — 4.05  0.825 1.7  0.651*** 2.1  0.712***
RAVLT
Total score 47.133  1.074 18.15  1.814* 25.733  1.436*,*** 32.4  1.773*,***,****
Immediate recall scores 10.167  1.019 1.1  0.911* 2.7  0.749*,*** 4.833  0.874*,***,****
Delayed recall scores 9.667  0.994 0.55  0.759* 2.167  0.746*,*** 4.033  0.889*,***,****
R-OCFT
Immediate recall scores 10.033  0.927 2.55  0.759* 4.467  1.074*,*** 7.033  0.808*,***,****
Delayed recall scores 9.533  0.937 1.2  0.767* 3.333  0.758*,*** 6.233  0.773*,***,****

* P < 0.001 compared to the NC group. ** P < 0.05 compared to the NC group. *** P < 0.001 compared to the miAD group. **** P < 0.001 compared to the
aMCImd group. ***** P < 0.003 compared to the aMCImd group. All comparisons made based on ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test. Handedness is based on
the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory. aMCImd, multiple-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment; aMCIsd, single-domain amnestic mild cognitive impair-
ment; miAD, mild Alzheimer’s disease; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; NC, cognitively normal controls; R-OCFT, Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test;
RAVLT, Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test. Values are mean  SD.

© 2018 Japanese Psychogeriatric Society 3


A. Mohammadi et al.

Figure 1 The virtual neighbourhood. (a) First-person view. (b) Overhead view.

Neuropsychological assessment Our advanced VRNT was designed with two virtual
The neuropsychological assessment covered the fol- environments (virtual neighbourhood and virtual
lowing cognitive domains: (i) verbal memory, as maze) to assess allocentric (virtual neighbourhood)
determined by the Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test and egocentric (virtual maze) spatial navigation. The
(RAVLT) (i.e. the sum of five trials, recall after inter- virtual neighbourhood was modelled on the natural
vention, delayed recall after 30 min, and memory rec- environment (i.e. a real neighbourhood) and included
ognition)50; (ii) non-verbal memory, as assessed by pertinent navigational landmarks to help subjects find
the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (R-OCFT) their routes (Fig. 1). In contrast, the virtual maze did
immediate recall; (iii) visuospatial memory, as evalu- not contain any navigational landmarks so individuals
ated by R-OCFT 30-min delayed recall51; and had to find the goal by recalling their route from the
(iv) overall cognitive function, as measured by the memory (Fig. 2). Each of the virtual reality environ-
MMSE.48 ments (virtual neighbourhood and virtual maze) com-
prised a 3-D first-person view and a two-dimensional
overhead view of the environment. First, the two-
Virtual reality environment task dimensional overhead view was shown to the sub-
Test design jects for 60 s, and then the 3-D first-person view was
Computer-based virtual reality environments have presented. Subjects were then instructed to find the
already been described and used as tools for visuo- specified goal (i.e. parking in the virtual neighbour-
spatial memory assessment in previous studies.42,52 hood; the ball in the virtual maze), which had been
These virtual reality environments are colourful, tex- marked on the 2-D overhead view. All subjects had
tured, and 3-D, and they also offer a first-person three trials to familiarize themselves with the task and
view. Actions in the environment can be controlled five trials for their assessment. Reactions and
by individuals using a joystick. response times were recorded.

Figure 2 The virtual maze. (a) First-person view. (b) Overhead view.

4 © 2018 Japanese Psychogeriatric Society


Multiple- but not single-domain aMCI

The 3-D first-person view of the virtual environment Neuropsychological assessments indicated large
were produced using Photoshop CS6 (Adobe, Tehran, between-group effects. On all tests, the miAD group
Tehran, Iran), AutoCAD (Autodesk, Tehran, Tehran, had lower scores than the aMCIsd, aMCImd, and NC
Iran), and 3ds Max (Autodesk) software and run in the subjects (P < 0.001). Similarly, both MCI subtypes
Lumion 5.0 Pro editor (Tehran, Tehran, Iran). The size had lower scores than the NC group on all tests
of overhead view and first-person view images were (P < 0.001), but aMCIsd subjects performed signifi-
1280 × 720 pixels and 1920 × 1080 pixels, respec- cantly better than the aMCImd subjects (P < 0.001).
tively. Both the virtual neighbourhood and virtual maze Patients with miAD and aMCImd performed signifi-
were presented, and the number of correct and incor- cantly worse on the RAVLT (total scores, immediate
rect responses and the completion time of each trial recall, and delayed recall) than the aMCIsd and NC
were recorded. The results were processed to pro- subjects (P < 0.001). Compared with the NC sub-
duce primary data tables for further statistical analysis. jects, the miAD, aMCIsd, and aMCImd subjects were
more impaired according to all of the R-OCFT subt-
Scoring ests (P < 0.001). Additionally, aMCImd subjects
The number of correct responses and response time for scored significantly lower on all subtests of R-OCFT
each subject were calculated for all trials of the virtual than the aMCIsd subjects. These findings indicate
neighbourhood and virtual maze. These were consid- that the classification of the groups was reflected in
ered the dependent variables in the statistical analysis. the results of neuropsychological tests. See Table 1
for more details about the neuropsychological fea-
tures of the groups.
Analysis
One-way ANOVA was used to evaluate the group differ-
ences between normally distributed demographic and Virtual reality tasks: virtual neighbourhood and
neuropsychological variables and the performance of virtual maze
miAD, aMCIsd, aMCImd, and NC on the VRNT (correct
All subjects’ VRNT scores and response times for
responses and response time). Tukey’s post-hoc
the five trials were analyzed using one-way ANOVA
method was applied for multiple comparisons of signif-
test followed by Tukey’s post-hoc test. The results
icant effects between groups. Pearson’s correlation
showed significant differences between groups in
coefficient was used to determine the relationship
the virtual neighbourhood and virtual maze tasks
between neuropsychological parameters and the vir-
(P < 0.001).
tual neighbourhood scores. All analyses were two-
tailed, and the significance level was defined as
P < 0.05. Statistical computations were carried out by Virtual neighbourhood task
the SPSS version 22 (IBM (International Business Comparisons of the scores among the miAD,
Machines Corporation), Tehran, Tehran, Iran.). aMCIsd, aMCImd, and NC subjects across the five
trials showed significant differences on the virtual
neighbourhood task (P < 0.001). Further analysis
RESULTS showed that miAD patients performed significantly
Descriptive statistics and neuropsychological worse than the NC and aMCI subjects on the virtual
scores neighbourhood task (P < 0.001). Similarly, the
Demographic analysis showed significant differences results indicated that the aMCImd subjects were
between groups for age, MMSE score, duration of significantly more impaired than aMCIsd and NC
the disorder (P < 0.001), and years of education subjects (P < 0.001), but they were less impaired
(P < 0.05). The subjects in the miAD, aMCIsd, and than the miAD subjects (P < 0.001). Data indicated
aMCImd groups were predominantly women (65%, that the miAD subjects completed their trials in a
63%, and 60%, respectively), whereas the NC group longer time than the aMCI and NC subjects
had an equal number of men and women. More (P < 0.001), and the aMCImd subjects had a slower
detailed demographic characteristics of the subjects performance than the aMCIsd and NC subjects
are presented in Table 1. (P < 0.001) (Table 2).

© 2018 Japanese Psychogeriatric Society 5


A. Mohammadi et al.

Table 2 Performance of groups on the VRNT (virtual neighbourhood, virtual maze)


Virtual reality task NC (n = 30) miAD (n = 20) aMCImd (n = 30) aMCIsd (n = 30)
Virtual neighbourhood
Mean correct response (5 trials) 4.333  0.711 0.35  0.489* 2.1  0.994*,** 4.133  0.507**,****
Mean response time (s) 77.57  17.156 173.49  9.106* 136.433  20.669*,** 83.33  12.793**,****
Virtual maze
Mean correct response (5 trials) 4.033  0.764 0.15  0.366* 1  0.787*,** 3.9  0.712**,****
Mean response time (s) 95.78  17.971 178.02  4.854* 163.713  12.824*,*** 100.513  16.079**,****

* P < 0.001 compared to the NC group. ** P < 0.001 compared to the miAD group. *** P < 0.005 compared to the miAD group. **** P < 0.001 compared to the
aMCImd group. All comparisons made based on ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test. aMCImd, multiple-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment; aMCIsd,
single-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment; miAD, mild Alzheimer’s disease; NC, cognitively normal controls; VRNT, virtual reality navigation task.
Values are mean  SD.

Virtual maze task was found between neuropsychological scores


Comparing the performance of all groups across the (RAVLT total scores, immediate recall, and delayed
five virtual maze trials showed significant differences recall; R-OCFT immediate and delayed recall) and the
in the response scores (mean correct response, mean response time for the virtual neighbourhood task
P < 0.001) and response times (P ≤ 0. 002). Tukey’s (P < 0.01) (Table 3).
post-hoc test revealed that miAD subjects performed
worse than the NC and aMCI groups on the virtual Relationship between neuropsychological
maze task (P < 0.001). The response time analysis variables and virtual maze performance
for all groups showed that miAD subjects required A strong positive relationship was seen between all
more time to complete the trials than the others neuropsychological scores and virtual maze scores
(P < 0.001). Also, there was a significant difference (P < 0.001). However, the relationship between neu-
between aMCIsd and aMCImd subjects (P < 0.001). ropsychological scores and the mean virtual maze
A comparison of aMCI and NC subjects indicated response time showed a strong negative correlation
that there were no significant differences with regard in all groups (P < 0.001) (Table 3).
response scores or response time (P > 0.05)
(Table 2).
DISCUSSION
Relationship between neuropsychological The primary aim of this study was to use virtual reality
variables and virtual neighbourhood to compare the allocentric and egocentric memory of
performance miAD, aMCIsd, aMCImd, and NC subjects. This inves-
Pearson’s correlation coefficient was used to deter- tigation revealed significant differences in the allo-
mine the relationship between neuropsychological centric and egocentric memory impairments among
parameters and the virtual neighbourhood scores. The miAD and aMCI subtypes. The miAD and aMCImd
results showed a strong positive correlation between subjects were impaired based on the results of the vir-
neuropsychological and virtual neighbourhood scores tual neighbourhood (allocentric memory) and virtual
in all groups (P < 0.01). A strong negative correlation maze (egocentric memory) tasks; the aMCIsd and NC

Table 3 Correlation between neuropsychological characteristics and the VRNT scores


Mean response time of Mean response
Test Virtual neighbourhood virtual neighbourhood Virtual maze time of virtual maze
RAVLT
Five trials 0.830† −0.846† 0.784† −0.794†
Immediate recall 0.801† −0.822† 0.753† −0.761†
Delayed recall 0.733† −0.795† 0.725† −0.734†
R-OCFT
Immediate recall 0.885† −0.902† 0.847† −0.849†
Delayed recall 0.886† −0.904† 0.860† −0.864†

Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed). R-OCFT, Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test; RAVLT, Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test; VRNT, virtual
reality navigation task. Values are mean  SD.

6 © 2018 Japanese Psychogeriatric Society


Multiple- but not single-domain aMCI

groups performed similarly on both tasks. The aMCIsd classification), and early AD in real or virtual environ-
and aMCImd groups performed significantly better on ments.21,35,37,41,43,44,53 Kalová et al. reported that
finding the target and required less time in the virtual subjects with early-stage AD were severely impaired
environment than the aMCImd and miAD groups, only in allocentric navigation and were able to
respectively. Comparisons of the time spent to find remember several locations, but patients with subjec-
the goal in the virtual maze and the virtual neighbour- tive memory problems showed no impairment.43
hood showed that subjects needed more time to find Kalová et al. used HGT for the first time, and their
the goal in the virtual maze than in the virtual neigh- results were similar in both real and computer ver-
bourhood. The miAD group spent the most time find- sions.43 A case study, Burgess et al. reported a dis-
ing the goal, whereas the NC group spent the least tinct allocentric spatial memory deficit in a patient
time. In addition, the aMCI subtypes had more diffi- with very early AD.53 deIpolyi et al. and Cushman
culty finding their route in the virtual maze than in the et al. reported that patients with mild and/or early-
virtual neighbourhood. In the other words, our findings stage AD or MCI were impaired in recalling previously
indicated that the aMCIsd, aMCImd, and miAD sub- learned routes and landmarks; however, neither study
jects were more impaired in their egocentric than allo- classified the domains of the MCI patients or distin-
centric orientation. The aMCIsd subjects did not guished between egocentric and allocentric
perform as well as the NC subjects, but the difference navigation.21,41
was not significant either in allocentric or egocentric In another study, Laczó et al. demonstrated that
memory; this suggests that aMCIsd subjects cannot hippocampal aMCI subjects were more impaired than
be distinguished from NC subjects via VRNT. How- non-hippocampal aMCI subjects in spatial naviga-
ever, aMCIsd subjects performed significantly better tion, but they did not categorize subjects as having
than the aMCImd and miAD subjects and can be eas- aMCIsd or aMCImd.44 Likewise, Weniger et al. com-
ily distinguished from them. pared the egocentric and allocentric memory deficits
In line with Hort et al.,36 who reported similarities of 29 patients with aMCI (without domain categoriza-
in spatial navigation between aMCImd and miAD tion) with 29 healthy matched controls using a virtual
patients,36 we hypothesized that miAD and aMCImd reality environment (similar to our VRNT) that was
subjects may be more impaired than aMCIsd sub- superior to previously designed tasks.35 They
jects in the virtual neighbourhood and virtual maze. reported allocentric and egocentric spatial navigation
This hypothesis was supported by our results: miAD deficits, in addition to reduced right/left hippocampal
and aMCImd subjects were impaired in both the vir- volumes, in aMCI patients relative to cognitively
tual neighbourhood (allocentric memory) and virtual healthy controls.35 Hort et al. found that patients with
maze (egocentric memory) tasks, but aMCIsd sub- aMCImd and AD were impaired in both allocentric
jects performed similarly to the NC subjects on both and egocentric navigation compared to healthy con-
tasks. The differences between the aMCIsd and trols, whereas aMCIsd patients were impaired only in
aMCImd groups were significant, but those between the allocentric navigation.36 Also, Hort et al. reported
the aMCIsd and NC groups were not. Our findings that aMCIsd group performed 1.5-fold worse than
also indicated that the miAD and aMCImd groups the control group.36
were more impaired than the others with regard to In line with the report by Hort et al., our results
finding their way in the virtual environments; specifi- showed that the miAD and aMCImd subjects were
cally, they could not find landmarks and took a long impaired in both allocentric (virtual neighbourhood)
time to reach the goal (parking in the virtual neigh- and egocentric (virtual maze) navigation, but the
bourhood; finding the ball in the virtual maze). aMCImd subjects performed significantly better than
Although only one previous study has addressed miAD subjects. Unlike that report, we did not find that
allocentric and egocentric memory deficits based on aMCIsd subjects had significantly impaired allo-
distinctions in aMCIsd and aMCImd,36 several stud- centric navigation; in fact, we found that the aMCIsd
ies have examined these deficits among aMCI and group performed similarly to the NC group in allo-
AD subjects. Previous reports considered allocentric centric and egocentric navigation. Our findings indi-
or/and egocentric navigation deficits in patients with cated that the aMCIsd subjects, as well as the
MCI, aMCI (without single- or multiple-domain aMCImd and miAD subjects, were more impaired in

© 2018 Japanese Psychogeriatric Society 7


A. Mohammadi et al.

egocentric than allocentric orientation (Table 2), sug- such as parahippocampal and cingulate cortex, in
gesting that egocentric memory may be a potential patients with AD and MCI.35,57–62 It was reported that
biomarker (along with other neuropsychological performance on a virtual navigation task is confi-
assessments) to identify these patients. These valua- dently associated with the grey matter volume of the
ble results may be thanks to the accuracy and com- hippocampus and that MCI subjects with hippocam-
prehensiveness of our newly developed VRNT. pal memory impairment have many problems in
We also explored the performance of the miAD, either allocentric or egocentric navigation.22,44,63,64
aMCIsd, and aMCImd subjects on the RAVLT and Also, the allocentric navigation performance of aMCI
compared them with those of NC subjects. The and AD patients has been associated with the grey
results showed that miAD, aMCImd, and aMCIsd matter volume of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
subjects performed worse on the RAVLT than the and right hippocampus.21,23 It is believed that the
aMCImd, aMCIsd, and NC subjects, respectively. posterior hippocampus and posterior parietal cortex
Unlike on the VRNT, aMCIsd subjects performed sig- are brain regions that degenerate during the early
nificantly worse than NC subjects on the RAVLT, stages of AD.65–67 Correct route learning and spatial
suggesting that the RAVLT can be used as a subsidi- abilities in MCI and AD patients are positively corre-
ary psychological task to distinguish MCI subtype lated with the volumes of these regions.21 A recent
from normal elderly subjects. functional MRI study showed that the bilateral activity
Auditory-verbal memory is routinely assessed by of the most posterior medial temporal lobe, precu-
RAVLT in MCI and AD patients, and their perfor- neus, postcentral gyrus, and retrosplenial cortex
mance is compared with healthy subjects. It has (noticeable on the right side) increased while sub-
been shown that the RAVLT subtests are reliable jected learned the virtual maze.68 Therefore, analyz-
tools for distinguishing old-old individuals from AD ing spatial disorientation (allocentric and egocentric
patients. Furthermore, performance on the RAVLT navigation deficits), RAVLT scores, and functional
delayed recall can differentiate individuals in the MRI scans of the hippocampus, posterior parietal
prodromal phase of AD and MCI from each other and cortex, precuneus, postcentral gyrus, and retrosple-
from the healthy individuals.54,55 Similarly, Tierney nial cortex can useful in determining a specific pat-
et al. demonstrated that the RAVLT delayed recall tern that identifies aMCIsd, aMCImd, and miAD and
can help predict probable AD with a high degree of preventing aMCIsd from developing into aMCImd
accuracy.56 Our study also revealed that delayed and subsequently miAD.
recall contributes to the early detection of miAD We concluded that miAD and aMCImd subjects
patients and the differentiation of aMCI subtypes demonstrated impaired egocentric, allocentric, visual,
from cognitively normal subjects. Our findings con- and verbal memory in performing the VRNT tasks,
firmed that the RAVLT can help predict probable but their allocentric memory was better than their
aMCI, and we suggest that this test be used in the egocentric memory (Table 4). The current study
early assessment of individuals with subjective mem- documented important differences between the per-
ory complaints. formance of miAD, aMCIsd, aMCImd, and NC sub-
In this study, we analyzed the relationship between jects based on VRNT and RAVLT. These findings
performance on the RAVLT subtests and on the suggest that aMCImd patients, but not aMCIsd
VRNT. Our analysis showed that there is a strong patients, are detectable from normal elderly subjects
positive relationship between the RAVLT total score, by VRNT; this test, along with the RAVLT and R-
immediate recall, and delayed recall and the number OCFT, can be used as a valid neuropsychological
of correct responses in the virtual neighbourhood biomarker to properly distinguish these patients from
and the virtual maze. A strong negative relationship each other. Ultimately, simultaneous use of functional
was observed between the performances on the MRI, VRNT, RAVLT, and other diagnostic techniques
RAVLT subtests and the spent time to find the given may be very useful in obtaining an accurate
goal in the both virtual neighbourhood and the diagnosis.
virtual maze. The main limitation of the present study was the
Several studies have reported a reduction in the low number of aMCIsd and aMCImd subjects and
volume of the hippocampus and some brain regions, the difficulty in finding them. The second limitation of

8 © 2018 Japanese Psychogeriatric Society


Multiple- but not single-domain aMCI

Table 4 Spatial disorientation and visual/verbal memory deficits in the aMCIsd, aMCImd, and miAD patients
Patients Allocentric Egocentric Verbal memory Visual memory
†,‡,§ †,‡,§ †,‡,§
miAD Impaired Impaired Impaired Impaired†,‡,§
Lowest performance among all Lowest performance Worst performance among Worst performance among
subjects among all subjects subjects subjects
Getting lost and confused in Getting lost (too often) and Lower than other groups Lower than other groups
navigation confused in navigation
Better than egocentric navigation Worse than allocentric
navigation
aMCImd Impaired†,‡,¶ Impaired†,‡,¶ Impaired†,‡,¶ Impaired†,‡,¶
Better performance than miAD, Better performance than Better performance than Better performance than
but significantly worse than NC miAD, but worse than miAD, but worse than miAD, but worse than
and aMCIsd subjects aMCIsd and NC aMCIsd and NC aMCIsd and NC
subjects subjects subjects
Getting lost and confused in Worse than allocentric
navigation, but less so than navigation
miAD subjects
More confused than in
allocentric navigation
aMCIsd Almost not impaired¶,††,‡‡ Almost not impaired¶,††,‡‡ Almost not impaired†,¶,†† Almost not impaired†,¶,††
Performed similar to NC Performed similar to NC Significantly worse than Significantly worse than
NC, but significantly NC, but significantly
better than miAD and better than miAD and
aMCImd aMCImd
Slightly worse than NC, but not Slightly worse than NC,
significant but not significant
Significantly better than miAD Significantly better than
and aMCImd miAD and aMCImd
Slight confusion in navigation Lower than allocentric
navigation

Significantly worse than NC subjects (P < 0.001). ‡ Significantly worse than aMCIsd subjects (P < 0.001). § Significantly worse than aMCImd subjects
(P < 0.001). ¶ Significantly worse than miAD subjects (P < 0.001). †† Significantly better than aMCImd subjects (P < 0.001). ‡‡ Slightly worse than NC, but not sig-
nificant. aMCImd, multiple-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment; aMCIsd, single-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment; miAD, mild Alzheimer’s
disease; NC, cognitively normal controls.

this study was the unwillingness of some subjects to 4 Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG,
Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization
simultaneously perform the VRNT and complete the
and outcome. Arch Neurol 1999; 56: 303–308.
RAVLT and R-OCFT with MRI during the second 5 Espinosa A, Alegret M, Valero S et al. A longitudinal follow-up
phase of this project. of 550 mild cognitive impairment patients: evidence for large
conversion to dementia rates and detection of major risk fac-
tors involved. J Alzheimers Dis 2013; 34: 769–780.
6 Grundman M, Petersen RC, Ferris SH et al. Mild cognitive
impairment can be distinguished from Alzheimer disease
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS and normal aging for clinical trials. Arch Neurol 2004; 61:
This research was supported by a grant from the 59–66.
Neuroscience Research Center, Baqiyatallah Univer- 7 Petersen RC, Parisi JE, Dickson DW et al. Neuropathologic fea-
tures of amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 2006;
sity of Medical Sciences (grant no. BMSU/950896). 63: 665–672.
We thank the subjects who participated in this study. 8 Ganguli M, Dodge HH, Shen C, DeKosky ST. Mild cognitive
impairment, amnestic type: an epidemiologic study. Neurology
2004; 63: 115–121.
9 Troyer AK, Murphy KJ, Anderson ND, Hayman-Abello BA,
REFERENCES Craik FI, Moscovitch M. Item and associative memory in
1 Collie A, Maruff P. The neuropsychology of preclinical Alzhei- amnestic mild cognitive impairment: performance on standar-
mer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurosci Biobe- dized memory tests. Neuropsychology 2008; 22: 10.
hav Rev 2000; 24: 365–374. 10 Fisk JD, Rockwood K. Outcomes of incident mild cognitive
2 Barker A, Jones R, Jennison C. A prevalence study of age- impairment in relation to case definition. J Neurol Neurosurg
associated memory impairment. Br J Psychiatry 1995; 167: Psychiatry 2005; 76: 1175–1177.
642–648. 11 Fisk JD, Merry HR, Rockwood K. Variations in case definition
3 Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A et al. Current concepts in mild affect prevalence but not outcomes of mild cognitive impair-
cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 2001; 58: 1985–1992. ment. Neurology 2003; 61: 1179–1184.

© 2018 Japanese Psychogeriatric Society 9


A. Mohammadi et al.

12 Gauthier S, Reisberg B, Zaudig M et al. Mild cognitive impair- 33 Burgess N, Maguire EA, Spiers HJ, O’Keefe J. A temporoparie-
ment. Lancet 2006; 367: 1262–1270. tal and prefrontal network for retrieving the spatial context of
13 Patalong-Ogiewa MB, Siuda JS, Opala GM. Working and epi- lifelike events. Neuroimage 2001; 14: 439–453.
sodic memory in the MCI group. J Neurol Sci 2009; 283: 291. 34 Etchamendy N, Bohbot VD. Spontaneous navigational strate-
14 Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol 2013; gies and performance in the virtual town. Hippocampus 2007;
64: 135. 17: 595.
15 Bherer L, Belleville S, Hudon C. Executive function deficits in 35 Weniger G, Ruhleder M, Lange C, Wolf S, Irle E. Egocentric and
normal aging, Alzheimer’s disease, and frontotemporal demen- allocentric memory as assessed by virtual reality in individuals
tia. Psychol Neuropsychiatr Vieil 2004; 2: 181–189. with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia
16 Gagnon LG, Belleville S. Working memory in mild cognitive 2011; 49: 518–527.
impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: contribution of forgetting 36 Hort J, Laczó J, Vyhnálek M, Bojar M, Bureš J, Vlc ek K. Spatial
and predictive value of complex span tasks. Neuropsychology navigation deficit in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Proc
2011; 25: 226–236. Natl Acad Sci U S A 2007; 104: 4042–4047.
17 Kalpouzos G, Eustache F, de la Sayette V, Viader F, 37 Marková H, Laczó J, Andel R, Hort J, Vlc ek K. Perspective tak-
Chételat G, Desgranges B. Working memory and FDG–PET dis- ing abilities in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzhei-
sociate early and late onset Alzheimer disease patients. J Neu- mer’s disease. Behav Brain Res 2015; 281: 229–238.
rol 2005; 252: 548–558. 38 Bird CM, Chan D, Hartley T, Pijnenburg YA, Rossor MN,
18 Toepper M, Beblo T, Thomas C, Driessen M. Early detection of Burgess N. Topographical short-term memory differentiates
Alzheimer’s disease: a new working memory paradigm. Int J Alzheimer’s disease from frontotemporal lobar degeneration.
Geriatr Psychiatry 2008; 23: 272–278. Hippocampus 2010; 20: 1154–1169.
19 Karas G, Scheltens P, Rombouts S et al. Global and local gray 39 Tetewsky SJ, Duffy CJ. Visual loss and getting lost in Alzhei-
matter loss in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s dis- mer’s disease. Neurology 1999; 52: 958.
ease. Neuroimage 2004; 23: 708–716. 40 Cherrier M, Mendez M, Perryman K. Route learning perfor-
20 Barbeau E, Ranjeva J, Didic M et al. Profile of memory impair- mance in Alzheimer disease patients. Cogn Behav Neurol 2001;
ment and gray matter loss in amnestic mild cognitive impair- 14: 159–168.
ment. Neuropsychologia 2008; 46: 1009–1019. 41 Cushman LA, Stein K, Duffy CJ. Detecting navigational deficits
21 deIpolyi AR, Rankin KP, Mucke L, Miller BL, Gorno- in cognitive aging and Alzheimer disease using virtual reality.
Tempini ML. Spatial cognition and the human navigation net- Neurology 2008; 71: 888–895.
work in AD and MCI. Neurology 2007; 69: 986–997. 42 Weniger G, Irle E. Posterior parahippocampal gyrus lesions in
22 Bohbot VD, Lerch J, Thorndycraft B, Iaria G, Zijdenbos AP. the human impair egocentric learning in a virtual environment.
Gray matter differences correlate with spontaneous strategies Eur J Neurosci 2006; 24: 2406–2414.
in a human virtual navigation task. J Neurosci 2007; 27: 43 Kalová E, Vlc ek K, Jarolímová E, Bureš J. Allothetic orientation
10078–10083. and sequential ordering of places is impaired in early stages of
23 Nedelska Z, Andel R, Laczó J et al. Spatial navigation impair- Alzheimer’s disease: corresponding results in real space tests
ment is proportional to right hippocampal volume. Proc Natl and computer tests. Behav Brain Res 2005; 159: 175–186.
Acad Sci U S A 2012; 109: 2590–2594. 44 Laczó J, Vlc ek K, Vyhnálek M et al. Spatial navigation testing
24 Jonides J, Smith EE, Koeppe RA, Awh E, Minoshima S, discriminates two types of amnestic mild cognitive impairment.
Mintun MA. Spatial working memory in humans as revealed by Behav Brain Res 2009; 202: 252–259.
PET. Nature 1993; 363: 623–625. 45 Stepankova K, Pastalkova E, Kalova E, Kalina M, Bures J. A
25 Smith EE, Jonides J. Neuroimaging analyses of human working battery of tests for quantitative examination of idiothetic and
memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1998; 95: 12061–12068. allothetic place navigation modes in humans. Behav Brain Res
26 Duncan J, Owen AM. Common regions of the human frontal 2003; 147: 95–105.
lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci 46 Alexopoulos P, Grimmer T, Perneczky R, Domes G, Kurz A.
2000; 23: 475–483. Progression to dementia in clinical subtypes of mild cogni-
27 Owen AM. The role of the lateral frontal cortex in mnemonic tive impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006; 22:
processing: the contribution of functional neuroimaging. Exp 27–34.
Brain Res 2000; 133: 33–43. 47 Petersen RC, Negash S. Mild cognitive impairment: an over-
28 Wenger MJ, Negash S, Petersen RC, Petersen L. Modeling and view. CNS Spectr 2008; 13: 45–53.
estimating recall processing capacity: sensitivity and diagnostic 48 Ansari NN, Naghdi S, Hasson S, Valizadeh L, Jalaie S. Valida-
utility in application to mild cognitive impairment. J Math Psy- tion of a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for the Persian
chol 2010; 54: 73–89. population: a pilot study. Appl Neuropsychol 2010; 17:
29 Monacelli AM, Cushman LA, Kavcic V, Duffy CJ. Spatial disori- 190–195.
entation in Alzheimer’s disease: the remembrance of things 49 Sargolini F, Fyhn M, Hafting T et al. Conjunctive representation
passed. Neurology 2003; 61: 1491–1497. of position, direction, and velocity in entorhinal cortex. Science
30 Klein DA, Steinberg M, Galik E et al. Wandering behaviour in 2006; 312: 758–762.
community-residing persons with dementia. Int J Geriatr Psy- 50 Jafari Z, Steffen Moritz P, Zandi T, Aliakbari Kamrani A,
chiatry 1999; 14: 272–279. Malyeri S. Psychometric properties of Persian version of the
31 O’Keefe J, Nadel L. The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) among the elderly.
Behav Brain Sci 1979; 2: 487–494. Iran J Psychiatry Clin Psychol 2010; 16: 56–64.
32 Barry C, Lever C, Hayman R et al. The boundary vector cell 51 Meyers JE, Meyers KR. Rey Complex Figure Test under four
model of place cell firing and spatial memory. Rev Neurosci different administration procedures. Clin Neuropsychol 1995; 9:
2006; 17: 71–97. 63–67.

10 © 2018 Japanese Psychogeriatric Society


Multiple- but not single-domain aMCI

52 Weniger G, Irle E. Allocentric memory impaired and ego- 60 Fox NC, Schott JM. Imaging cerebral atrophy: normal ageing to
centric memory intact as assessed by virtual reality in Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet 2004; 363: 392–394.
recent-onset schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2008; 101: 61 Gerardin E, Chételat G, Chupin M et al. Multidimensional classi-
201–209. fication of hippocampal shape features discriminates Alzhei-
53 Burgess N, Trinkler I, King J, Kennedy A, Cipolotti L. Impaired mer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment from normal
allocentric spatial memory underlying topographical disorienta- aging. Neuroimage 2009; 47: 1476–1486.
tion. Rev Neurosci 2006; 17: 239. 62 Risacher SL, Saykin AJ, Wes JD et al. Baseline MRI predictors
54 Incalzi RA, Capparella O, Gemma A, Marra C, Carbonin P. of conversion from MCI to probable AD in the ADNI cohort.
Effects of aging and of Alzheimer’s disease on verbal memory. Curr Alzheimer Res 2009; 6: 347–361.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1995; 17: 580–589. 63 Pigott S, Milner B. Memory for different aspects of complex vis-
55 Estévez-González A, Kulisevsky J, Boltes A, Otermín P, García- ual scenes after unilateral temporal- or frontal-lobe resection.
Sánchez C. Rey verbal learning test is a useful tool for differen- Neuropsychologia 1993; 31: 1–15.
tial diagnosis in the preclinical phase of Alzheimer’s disease: 64 Wolbers T, Wiener JM, Mallot HA, Büchel C. Differential recruit-
comparison with mild cognitive impairment and normal aging. ment of the hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex, and the
Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2003; 18: 1021–1028. human motion complex during path integration in humans.
56 Tierney M, Szalai J, Snow W et al. Prediction of probable Alz- J Neurosci 2007; 27: 9408–9416.
heimer’s disease in memory-impaired patients a prospective 65 Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-
longitudinal study. Neurology 1996; 46: 661–665. related changes. Acta Neuropathol 1991; 82: 239–259.
57 Chetelat G, Desgranges B, De La Sayette V, Viader F, 66 Delacourte A, David J, Sergeant N et al. The biochemical path-
Eustache F, Baron JC. Mapping gray matter loss with voxel- way of neurofibrillary degeneration in aging and Alzheimer’s
based morphometry in mild cognitive impairment. Neuroreport disease. Neurology 1999; 52: 1158.
2002; 13: 1939–1943. 67 Whitwell JL, Przybelski SA, Weigand SD et al. 3D maps from
58 De Leon M, DeSanti S, Zinkowski R et al. Longitudinal CSF and multiple MRI illustrate changing atrophy patterns as subjects
MRI biomarkers improve the diagnosis of mild cognitive impair- progress from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s dis-
ment. Neurobiol Aging 2006; 27: 394–401. ease. Brain 2007; 130: 1777–1786.
59 Dickerson BC, Goncharova I, Sullivan M et al. MRI-derived 68 Weniger G, Siemerkus J, Schmidt-Samoa C et al. The human
entorhinal and hippocampal atrophy in incipient and very parahippocampal cortex subserves egocentric spatial learning
mild Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 2001; 22: during navigation in a virtual maze. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2010;
747–754. 93: 46–55.

© 2018 Japanese Psychogeriatric Society 11

You might also like