You are on page 1of 8

Descriptive Analysis of ccupational therapists regularly assess and

the Developmental O treat children with fine motor problems;


these children are often referred due to
handwriting difficulties. Although handwriting is an

Progression of Grip important aspect of fine motor ability, our limited


knowledge of the development of handwriting skill in

Position for Pencil and the nondysfunctional child interferes with effective
evaluation of handwriting deficits. An important

Crayon Control in aspect of handwriting with which occupational thera-


pists are concerned is pencil.grip. The knowledge of

Nondysfunctional age-related changes in pencil grip and arm posture is


fragmented and incomplete. Information on the de-

Children velopment of pencil grip shows that there is a devel-


opmental progression, with changes continuing to
occur until approximately 10.5 years of age (Ziviani,
1983). The evidence identifying the ages at which
Colleen M. Schneck, Anne Henderson developmental levels of pencil grip are achieved,
however, is incomplete, and the studies are not com-
parable due to the different age ranges studied (Ro-
Key Words: child development. gross and senbloom & Horton, 1971; Saida & Myashita, 1979)
fine motor evaluation. hand functions Furthermore, different authors have used different
terms to describe similar or identical grips.
In 1931, Halverson conducted a thorough study
of the development of the motor components of pre-
This study was designed to investigate the develop-
hension leading to handwriting as well as all other
mental progression in pencil and crayon grip. The
subjects were 320 nondysfunctional children aged functional fine motOr skills. He observed the charac-
3 0 to 611 years, with 20 boys and 20 girls at each teristic position of the arm, hand, and fingers of chil-
6-month age interval. On the basis of a review of the dren at successive stages of early development. He
literature, developmental pencil and crayon grtpS reported both a progression from a banging move-
were defined for the study, and the type ofgrips ment of the arm to controlled, discrete finger move-
each child used to peljorm a drawing task and a ments and a progression from a small child's fisted
coloring task were recorded. Many children at each grip of the pencil to the fingertip grip seen in adults.
age level used mature pencil grtps. A developmental The adult fingertip grip described by Halverson was
progression, however, was shown by the percentage termed the dynamic tripod grasp by Wynn-Parry
change 0/ children at each age level who used ma- (1966). Recent studies of the acqUisition of the tripod
ture grips. Forty-eight percent of the youngest group
grip (Rosenbloom & Horton, 1971; Saida & Myashita,
used mature grtPS, compared with 90% of the oldest
children. Two pencil grips-dynamic and lateral 1979) have conlirmed the sequence described by
tripod-appear to be common in older children. Halverson but suggested that the stages of develop-
Differences in the developmental progression ofpen- ment of pencil grip are achieved earlier than he
cil gnp were noted between boys and girls and be- reported.
tween a draWing task and a coloring task. Rosenbloom and Horton (1971) studied the de-
velopment of pencil grip, incluuing the development
of the tripod grasp, in 128 British children between
the ages of 1.6 and 7.0 years. The tripod grasp was
described as a finger posture in which the pencil is
Colleen M. Schneck, SeD, OTR/L, is Senior Occupational
resting on the distal aspect of the middle finger while
Therapist, The Eclucation Center at D. T. Watson, Sewick-
ley, Pennsylvania, and Adjunct Assistant Professor of Occu-
being controlled between the pads of the thumb and
pational Therapy, School of Allied Health Professions, index linger. Pencil grip was seen to develop in three
University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania 15261. stages: (a) a p;llmar grasp with the index finger ex-
At the time of this study, she was a doctoral student at tended along the pencil shaft, (b) a static tripod grasp
Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts. (i.e., the tripod posture of the fingers is acqUired, but
the intrinsic components of movement are not), and
Anne Henderson, PhD, OTR, fAOTA, is Professor of Occupa-
tional Therapy, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts
(c) a dynamic tripod grasp (the three fingers func-
tioning together are able to make small, highly coor-
This article was accepted for publication February 21, 1990
dinated movements).

Tbe Amerlcall.!ournal of Occupational Tberapy 893


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930304/ on 03/22/2018 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
This study was replicated by Saida and Myashica palmar grasp, (c) opposition of thumb and finger vari·
(1979) in japan with 154 children between the ages of ations, and (d) pencil held with tip of three radial
2.3 and 6.5 years. Their subjects did not demonstrate fingers. Goodgold found a developmental trend in
the palmar grasp with the index finger extended, as preschool children's acquisition of handwriting
observed by Rosenbloom and Horton (1971), possi- movement skills.
blybecause Rosenbloom and Horton's subjects were A person's sex may also influence grip. Conflict·
younger. Saida and Myashita's (1979) description of ing findings have been reported on the differences
pencil prehension identified the following four between boys and girls in the development of pencil
stages, which involved only supinate postures: (a) a grip. Rosenbloom and Horton (1971), Goodgold
palmar grasp, (b) any variety of transitional grasps (1983), and Schneck (1987) did not find significant
preceding the tripod grasp by the thumb and two ra- sex differences in their studies and therefore grouped
dial digits, (c) a static tripod grasp, and (d) a dynamic all subjects together for the data analysis. Saida and
tripod grasp. Direct comparison of these two studies Myashita (1979), however, did report sex differences
(Rosenbloom & Horton, 1971; Saida & Myashita, in japanese children, but only in the 3-year-old group,
1979) is not possible, because the ages of the subjects in which girls demonstrated more mature grips earlier
and the grip classifications differed It is interesting to than boys. Possibly, sex differences have not been
note, however, that although the japanese children found in the other studies because few or no 3-year·
were older, the median age for dynamic tripod devel- old children were included in the samples. Ziviani
opment was 4.11 years for the japanese children and (1983) also found sex differences, such that girls be·
5.5 years for the British children. tween the ages of 6.8 and 9.9 years were more likely
The first author (Schneck, 1987) observed all of to demonstrate hyperextension of the distal interpha·
the grips reported by Rosenbloom and Horton (1971) langeal joint of the index finger, together with 90° or
and Saida and Myashita (1979) as well as three other more of flexion of the proximal interphalangeal joint,
transitional grips: (a) the cross thumb grasp (mean than were boys of the same age. Sex differences were
age = 4.6 years), (b) the four fingers grasp (mean age also noted in the degree of forearm pronation, with
= 4.8 years), and (c) the lateral tripod grasp (mean young boys more likely than like-aged girls to show a
age = 4.8 years). The age range studied was not, how- greater degree of forearm supination. Ziviani sug-
ever, wide enough to accurately determine the devel- gested that this may be due to boys not applying as
opmental order. much pressure as girls when writing, thus allOWing
Dynamic tripod grasp continues to undergo mod- the forearm to relax in [he more supinated posture.
ification in school-age children Ziviani (1983) stud- Another factor that may influence whether a static
ied finger posture and forearm position in the dy- or dynamic tripod grasp is used is the type of task the
namic tripod grasp in 287 nondysfunctional Australian child is requested to perform. In Saida and Myashita's
children between 7 and 14 years of age. By photo- (1979) study, the children were instructed to trace
graphing grip posture, Ziviani determined that two of rectangles of 13 cm by 15 cm. This size rectangle does
the components of tripod grasp did not change with not require the fine movement use of a dynamic tripod
age: pad-to-pad opposition and the number of fingers grasp, but rather, can be completed with wrist and fore-
positioned on the pencil shaft. Two other compo- arm movement. The grip used may have been related
nents of the dynamic tripod grasp did, however, show to task variables (e.g., the size of the object to be drawn
developmental changes in these older children. In or copied or the implement used for drawing). For
children under 10.5 years of age, proXimal interpha- example, children may use different grips for a smaller
langeal flexion was greater than 90°, whereas after draWing task than for a larger coloring task.
that age it was less than 90°. The degree of forearm The present study was designed to clarify some of
pronation also decreased, from greater than 45° in the findings on the development of pencil and crayon
children under 9.5 years of age to less than 45° after grip in American preschool and school·age children,
9.5 years of age. including the influence of sex and task on that devel-
Goodgold (1983) evaluated the development of opment; to improve existing studies by controlling
arm function as well as grip in pencil use in preschool seating position; and to further extend this informa-
and kindergarten children. A handwriting movement tion by increasing the number of grip categories. The
assessment was devised to assess grip, posture, upper specific purposes of this study were (a) to describe
extremity control in the formation of symbols and in the developmental progreSSion of grip position for
the movement across the paper, and fluency and pencil and crayon control in nondysfunctional chil-
speed in the sequence of movements Pencil grip was dren, (b) to describe and compare the grips used for
divided into four stages with no separation of the dy- pencil and crayon use, and (c) to compare this devel-
namic and static tripod grasps: (a) fisted hand, (b) opment in girls and boys.

894 October 1990, Volume 44, Number 10

Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930304/ on 03/22/2018 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms


Method • Radial cross palmar grasp-Pencil positioned
across palm projecting radially, held with fisted
Subjects
hand, forearm fully pronated, full arm move-
The subjects of this study were 320 oondysfuoctional ment (Morrison, 1978).
children, categorized by the following age ranges: 30 • Palmar supinate grasp-Pencil positioned
to 35 years, 3.6 to 3.11 years, 4.0 to 45 years, 4.6 to across palm projecting ulnarly, held with fisted
4.11 years, 5.0 to 55 years, 5.6 to 5.11 years, 6.0 to 65 hand, wrist slightly flexed and supinated away
years, and 6.6 to 6.11 years. Each age group consisted from midposition, full arm movement (Erhardt,
of 20 boys and 20 girls. Children from day-care 1984).
centers, preschools, and elementary schools in the • Digital pronate grasp, only index finger ex-
greater Boston and Pittsburgh areas with no learning, tended-Pencil held in palmar grasp with
physical, or behavioral problems, as reported by their index finger extended along pencil toward tip,
caretaker or classroom teacher, participated in this arm not supported on table, full arm movement
study. Children from school districts who had similar (Morrison, 1978).
entry ages for kindergarten were included. • Brush grasp-Pencil held with fingers, eraser
end of pencil positioned against palm, hand
Procedure pronated with wrist movement present, whole
On the basis of a review of the literature, a develop- arm movement, forearm positioned in air
mental scale of pencil and crayon grips that assessed • Grasp with extended fingers-Pencil held with
the components of grip and arm posture was devel- fingers, wrist straight and pronated with slight
oped. Eleven grips were described in the literature ulnar deviation, forearm moves as a unit.
and were included in the scale. One of these grips • Cross thumb grasp-Fingers fisted loosely into
(interdigital), however, was not observed in any of palm, pencil held against index finger with
the children in preliminary investigations and was thumb crossed over pencil toward index finger,
therefore excluded. The definitions of the 10 grips finger and wrist movement, forearm positioned
used in thiS study, in developmental order, are as fol- on table (Gesell, 1940).
lows (see also Figure 1 for illustrations of these • Static tnpod grasp-Pencil stabilized against
grips) : radial side of third digit by thumb pulp with

\] ~6
~<:f/
~ ~' fv/ (flY
~ \~o/
(a) (b) (c) (d) (e)

(f) (g) (h) (i) (j)

Figure 1. Operational definitions of grip posture in developmental order: (a) radial cross palmar grasp (Morrison, 1978); (b) palmar
supinate grasp (Erhardt, 1984); (c) digital pronate grasp, only index finger extended (Morrison, 1984); (d) brush grasp; (e) grasp with
extended fingers; (f) cross thumb grasp (Gesell, 1940); (g) static tripod grasp (Rosenbloom & Horton, 1971); (h) four fingers grasp; (i)
lateral tripod grasp (Schneck, 1987); (j) dynamic tripod grasp (Rosenbloom & Horton, 1971)

The American./ournal of Occupational Therapy 895

Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930304/ on 03/22/2018 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms


index pulp on top of shaft, thumb stabilized in child in the classroom setting in one 10-min session.
full opposition, wrist slightly extended and The child was positioned appropriately in 3 Tripp
hand moving as a unit, pencil resting in open Trapp Chair 1 with desk height at elbow level. The
web space, forearm resting on table (Rosen- child was requested to perform the t\VO tasks de-
bloom & Honon, 1971). scribed above, drawing for finer movements and col-
• Four fingers grasp-Pencil held with four oring for gross movements, while grip and posture
fingers in opposition, wrist and finger move- were observed and recorded. The child performed
ment, forearm positioned on table. two trials for each of the two tasks, although only the
• Lateral tripod grasp-Pencil stabilized against second trial was used to analyze the data These tasks
radial side of third digit with index pulp on top were presented in a counterbalanced order with the
of shaft of pencil, thu mb addueted and braced Lateral Consistency Test separating the two trials. The
over or under anywhere along lateral borcler of four orders were (a) coloring task, draWing task, Lat-
index finger, wrist slightly extended, fourth eral Consistency Test, coloring task, drawing task; (b)
and fifth digits flexed to stabilize metacarpo- draWing task, coloring task, Lateral Consistency Test,
phalangeal arch and third digit, localized draWing task, coloring task; (c) drawing task, coloring
movement of digits of tripod and wrist move- task, Lateral Consistency Test, coloring task, draWing
ments on tall and horizontal strokes, forearm task; and (d) coloring task, drawing task, Lateral Con-
resting on table (Schneck, 1987). sistency Test, drawing task, coloring task.
• Dynamic tripod grasp-Pencil stabilized Interrater reliability between the researcher (the
against radial side of third digit by thumb pulp first author) and an experienced, trained therapist for
with index pulp on top of shaft of pencil, assessing the children's grips was .90 in a pilot inves-
thumb stabilized in full opposition, wrist tigation of 30 children (Schneck, 1987). Test-retest
slightly extended, fourth and fifth digits flexed reliability was determined on the grips used for
to stabilize metacarpophalangeal arch and third draWing and coloring. The researcher readministered
digit, localized movement of digits of tripod the test after 1 week to 20 of the children; 85% re-
and wrist movements on tall and horizontal ceived the same score on both the draWing and color-
strokes, forearm resting on table (Rosenbloom ing tasks as they had the previous week.
& Honon, 1971).
Results
The type of grip the child used to perform each of two
On the first trial, the children were familiarizing
tasks was recorded during two trials of each task. The
themselves with the procedure. The second trial,
tasks consisted of drawing and coloring tasks. For
therefore, was considered more reliable and was used
each task, the paper was presented at the child's mid-
for data analysis.
line and the pencil or crayon was placed on top of the
paper vertical to the child's body
Drawing
For the drawing task, the child was presented
with a 5 cm by 5 cm piece of paper. The child was The number and percentage of children at each age
then given a sharpened No.2 pencil and instructed to level who demonstrated the various pencil grips in
draw one of several shapes, depending on his or her draWing are presented in Table 1. The first five grips-
age. These shapes were (a) a circle (ages 30 to 311 radial cross palmar; palmar supinate; digital pronate,
years), (b) a cross (ages 4.0 to 45 years), (c) a square only index finger extended; brush grasp; and grasp
(ages 4.6 to 4.11 years), (d) a triangle (ages 5.0 to 5.5 with extended fingers-were classified as primitive
years), and (e) a diamond (ages 5.6 to 6.11 years). For grips, because they were seen infrequently after age 4
the coloring task, the child was presented with a piece years. The cross thumb, static tripod, and four fingers
of paper with two 9 cm diameter circles. On the first grips appeared to be transitional; their use continued
trial, the child performed the drawing task and was into the sixth year of age. The dynamiC tripod is the
given a regular-size crayon and instructed to color the commonly accepted adult grip, but we also classified
bottom circle. On the second trial, the child was pre- the lateral tripod as mature because we observed it in
sented with a new sheet of paper to repeat the draw- many of the oldest children studied.
ing task and was instructed to color the top circle. The data show that large percentages of children
Hand preference was measured by the Lateral at all of the age levels studied used mature pencil
Consistency Test, adapted from Lyle (976). (A de- grips. In all groups, with the exception oftbe children
scription of this test and the results for the subjects in aged 3.6 to 3.11 years, the dynamic tripod was the
the present study are reported in Schneck, 1989).
The researcher (the first author) tested each 'Available from the Equipment Shop, Bedford, MA 01730.

896 October 1.990, Volume 44, Number 10


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930304/ on 03/22/2018 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
Table 1
Number and Percentage of Children Using Pencil Grips in the Drawing Task (N = 320)
Age in Years
Grip 30-35 36-311 40-4.5 4.6-4.11 50-5.5 56-511 6.0-6.5 66-6.11
Primitive grips
Radial cross palmar 1 (2.5%) 1 (25%)
Palmar supinate 6 (150%) 5 (12.5%) 1 (25%) 1 (2 5%)
Digital pronate,
only index
finger extended 2 (50%) 2 (50%) 2 (50%)
Brush 2 (5.0%) 1 (25%)
Grasp with
extended fingers 3 (7.5%) 3 (7.5%) 3 (7.5%) 2 (50%)
Transitional grips
Cross thumb 3 (75%) 2 (5.0%) 1 (25%) 3 (75%)
Static tripod 2 (5.0%) 9 (225%) 8 (20.0%) 3 (75%) 1 (25%) 4 (10.0%) 2 (5.0%) 1 (2.5%)
Four fingers 5 (12.5%) 4 (10.0%) 4 (10.0%) 9 (225%) 2 (5.0%) 5 (125%) 2 (5.0%) 1 (2.5%)
Mature grips
Lateral tripod 3 (75%) 4 (10.0%) 4 (10.0%) 7 (17.5%) 10 (25.0%) 10 (250%) 11 (275%) 9 (225%)
Dynamic tripod 16 (40.0%) 9 (22.5%) 16 (400%) 19 (475%) 21 (525%) 21 (52.5%) 25 (625%) 29 (72.5%)
Note. Each age group consisted of 20 boys and 20 girls.

most common grip demonstrated. Nevertheless, a de- ious grips in coloring. A comparison of the data shows
velopmental progression can be noted, both through that the use of pencil grips is influenced by the task
the decrease in the use of the primitive and the tran- for which the writing implement is used (see Tables 1
sitional grips and in the increase in the use of the two and 3). Many children used less mature grips when
mature grips. Use of the dynamic and lateral tripod coloring spaces than when drawing. The most com-
grips increased from a combined percentage of 475% mon grip used for coloring was the static tripod grasp,
in the youngest children to 95% in the oldest whereas for drawing, it was the dynamic tripod grasp,
children. which facilitates fine finger movements. For coloring
Table 2 shows the difference in grips among the spaces, the static tripod grasp was used to stabilize the
girls and boys. Although the general developmental crayon while moving the wrist, forearm, and shoulder.
trends are similar, more girls showed mature grips We noted that more boys (77) than girls (69)
than did boys. Only 9 of the total sample of girls llsed used the static tripod grasp, and more girls (35) than
one of the primitive grips, whereas 26 boys, including boys (27) used the lateral tripod grasp for the coloring
more than half of the boys in the youngest age group, task.
used one of those five primitive grips. Within the
youngest age group, only 5 boys, compared with 14 Discussion
girls, used one of the mature grips. This difference The dynamic tripod grasp is considered by most
between boys and girls was not demonstrated in the teachers and therapists to be ideal It was the most
older children. Beyond the age of 4.6 years, boys common grip used in the present study at all age
showed mature grips as frequently as did girls. More levels. About one fourth of the children in each of the
boys than girls used the dynamic tripod in the older four highest age levels, however, used the lateral tri-
groups. pod grasp. Furthermore, Bergmann (1990) found that
12% of a sample of 485 adults used pencil grips other
Coloring than the dynamic tripod. The most common alternate
During the administration of the coloring task, we grip was the lateral tripod grasp, which was used by
noted that often the children would first color the 9.3% of the adults observed. In the present study, the
edge of the circle using a more mature grip and then ratio of lateral tripod grasp to dynamic tripod grasp
color in the center of the circle using a less mature began to decrease at the two oldest age levels. We
grip. This was more noticeable in younger children; would therefore expect the use of the lateral tripod
older children would simply slow down to color the grasp to further decrease in older children, eventually
edge and then continue with the same grip for the reaching the adult level.
center of the circle. We chose to analyze the grip used Levine, Brooks, and Shonkoff (1980) noted that
to color the center of the circle. children with learning disabilities used a lateral tri-
Table 3 presents the number and percentage of pod grasp as a maladaptive grasp, which they believed
children at each age level who demonstrated the var- would lead to significant problems as written volume

The American .Journal of Occupational Therapy 897

Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930304/ on 03/22/2018 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms


Table 2
Number and Percentage of Girls and Boys Using Pencil Grips in the Drawing Task
Age in Years
Grip 30-35 3.6-311 4.0-4.5 46-411 5.0-5.5 5.6-511 60-65 66-6.11
GIRLS (n = 160)
Primitive grips
Radial cross palmar 1 (50%)
Palmar supinate 1 (5.0%) 1 (50%) 1 (50%) 1 (5.0%)
Digital pronate,
only index
finger extended
Brush
Grasp with
extended fingers 2 (100%) 1 (5.0%) 1 (50%)
Transitional grips
Cross thumb 1 (5.0%) 2 (10.0%)
Static tripod 3(15.0%) 2 (100%) 1 (5.0%) 4 (200%) 1 (5.0%)
Four fingers 4 (20.0%) 3 (150%) 3 (J 50%) 5 (250%) 2 (10.0%) 1 (5.0%) 2 (10.0%) 1 (50%)
Maw re grips
Lateral tripod 3 (150%) 2 (100%) 3 (J 50%) 3 (15.0%) 5 (250%) 6 (300%) 7 (35.0%) 5 (25.0%)
Dynamic tripod 11 (55.0%) 8 (40.0%) 10 (500%) 10 (500%) 10 (500%) 9 (45.0%) 10 (500%) 14 (70.0%)
Boys (n = 160)
Primitive grips
Radial cross palmar 1 (5.0%)
Palmar supinate 5 (25.0%) 4 (200%)
Digital pronate,
only index
finger extended 2 (100%) 2 (100%) 2 (10.0%)
Brush 2 (100%) 1 (50%)
Grasp with
extended fingers 3 (15.0%) 1 (50%) 2 (100%) 1 (5.0%)
Transitional grips
Cross thumb 2 (100%) 2 (10.0%) 1 (5.0%) 1 (5.0%)
Static tripod 2 (10.0%) 6(30.0%) 6 (300%) 2 (100%) 1 (50%) 1 (50%) 1 (5.0%)
Four fingers 1 (5.0%) 1 (5.0%) 1 (5.0%) 4 (200%) 4 (20.0%)
Mature grips
Lateral tripod 2 (100%) 1 (5.0%) 4 (20.0%) 5 (25.0%) 4 (200%) 4 (200%) 4 (20.0%)
Dynamic tripod 5 (25.0%) 1 (5.0%) 6 (30.0%) 9 (45.0%) 11 (55.0%) 12 (60.0%) 15 (750%) 15 (750%)
Note For both girls and boys, each age group consisted of 20 children.

and time constraints increased in later school grades. In the present study, some of the children in the
Bergmann's (1990) study, as well as the present study, age group of 3.6 to 3.11 years (the youngest age group
indicate that, until further studies are conducted, the tested was 3.0 to 3.5 years) had achieved tripod
lateral tripod grasp should be considered an accept­ grasps. The static tripod grasp was first noted at 2.7
able alternative to the dynamic tripod grasp. It should years of age (the youngest age group tested was 1.6
also be noted that two studies (Jaffe, 1987; Ziviani & years) for the British children (Rosenbloom & Hor­
Elkins, 1986) have failed to show that pencil grip is ton, 1971) and 25 years of age (the youngest age
related to skill in handwriting. group tested was 2.3 years) for the Japanese children
Ziviani and Elkins (1986) studied children 7 to 14 (Saida & Myashita, 1979). The earlie'st achievers of the
years of age and found four major variations within the dynamic tripod grasp were 4.0 years old for the British
dynamic tripod grasp. They concluded that the em­ children (Rosenbloom & Honon, 1971), 2.11 years
phasis placed on "correct" pencil grip may be unwar­ old for the Japanese children (Saida & Myashita,
ranted. Jaffe (1987), in a study of nondysfunctional 1979), and 3.0 years old for the American children in
adUlts, supported Ziviani's (1983) conjecture that the present study. Saida and Myashita attributed early
there is little difference in handwriting with pencil development of the dynamic tripod grasp in Japanese
grip as a group criterion. Comparisons of nondys­ children to the use of chopsticks, whose use requires
functional adults with a dynamic tripod grasp and fine, dexterous movements. Although American chil­
versus those with nontripod grasps showed that there dren have no such tool that can be identified with the
were no differences in legibility, fatigue, or strength early development of fine, dexterous movements,
in a handwriting task (Jaffe, 1987) they did develop the dynamic tripod grasp at about

898 October ]990, Volume 44, Number ]0


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930304/ on 03/22/2018 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
Table 3

Number and Percentage of Children Using Pencil Grips to Color the Center of the Circle (N = 320)

Age in Years
Grip 30-3.5 36-311 40-45 46-411 50-5.5 56-5,11 60-6.5 6.6-6,11
Primitive grips
Radial cross
palmar 1 (25%) 2 (50%) 1 (25%)
Palmar supinate 3 (7.5%) 3 (7.5%) 2 (50%)
Digital pronate,
only index
linger
extended 4 (10,0%) 8 (200%) 2 (5,0%) 4 (100%) 1 (25%) 1 (2 5%) 1 (25%)

Brush 1 (25%) 1 (2.5%)

Grasp with
extended
lingers 2 (50%) 4 (100%) 2 (50%) 1 (2.5%) 2 (50%)
Transitional grips
Cross thumb 1 (2,5%) 1 (2,5%) 2 (50%) 2 (50%) 2 (50%) 1 (25%)
Static tripod 16 (400%) 14 (350%) 21 (52,5%) 17 (42,5%) 17 (425%) 16 (40.0%) 20 (50.0%) 25 (625%)
Four lingers 8 (20,0%) 4 (100%) 4 (10,0%) 10 (25.0%) 2 (5,0%) 8 (20,0%) 2 (5,0%) 2 (5,0%)
Mature grips
Lateral tripod 4 (10.0%) 4 (10,0%) 6 (15,0%) 4 (100%) 13 (32,5%) 9 (22.5%) 11 (27.5%) 11 (27.5%)
Dynamic tripod 1 (25%) 3 (75%) 3 (75%) 3 (75%) 5 (12.5%) 2 (5.0%)
Note, Each age group consisted of 20 boys and 20 girls,

the same age as the Japanese children, Future studies, grasp to perform a precision task, as was evidenced in
therefore, must include even younger children in this study? It appears that more girls than boys use a
order to fully outline the development of pencil grip power grip in both drawing and coloring tasks. Possi­
in American children. bly, girls need this power configuration more than
The data show that there is a high degree of vari­ boys,
ability both in the age at which children achieve ma­ Elliott and Connolly (1984) found that digital
ture pencil grip and in the variety of immature and manipulation leads to economy, variety, and conve­
transitional grips they select. Although all of the grips nience, because it is advantageous to minimize in­
studied can be observed in the grips of young chil­ volvement of the upper extremity when moving a
dren, it is clear that not all children use every grip, A grasped object. This provides evidence that the dy­
developmental progression from immature to transi­ namic tripod grasp, which involves digital manipula­
tional to mature grips does not seem a reasonable tion, would be the most efficient for handwriting and
expectation, but longitudinal studies beginning at age that less mature grips may lead to handwriting diffi­
1 or 2 years will be needed to accurately determine culties. The belief of the superiority of the tripod
this sequence, grasp, however, is based on tradition and has not,
The findings of the present study do not indicate until recently, been studied through systematic
the reasons children used different grips, but we can research,
pose an explanation through the differentiation of The present study has several implications for the
power and precision grips, Napier (1956) suggested evaluation of children with fine motor problems,
that the intended nature of the activity influenced the Children 3 years of age can be expected to use pencil
posture of the hand. He therefore classified grips into grips ranging from primitive to mature, By the age of
two basic types, distinct in the anatomical and func­ 4 V2 years, children can be expected to use transitional
tional sense, which he termed power and precision or mature grips, Children 6\1;> years of age and older
grips, In a power grip, the palm and combined fingers typically use either the lateral tripod or the dynamic
secure the object, while all the movement of the tripod grasp,
grasped object comes from the proximal joints. Tn a Boys and girls seem to have different develop­
precision grip, the object is held pinched between the mental progressions for both drawing and coloring.
flexor aspect of the fingers and opposed thumb and is This supports Saicla and Myashita's (1979) findings of
manipulated with the fingers, In a lateral tripod grasp, differences by sex in 3-year-old children. We must
the adducted thumb is unavailable to contribute to the therefore keep sex differences in mind when evaluat­
vertical elongation of the strokes usually made by the ing children with fine motor problems.
fingers, which classifies it as a power grip. Why do Different measures should also be considered
children use a power grip such as the lateral tripod when one evaluates children's grips in a coloring task

The American.!oumal a/Occupational Therapy 899

Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930304/ on 03/22/2018 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms


that appears to require a more powerful grip. It is Elliott, J. M., & Connolly, K. J. (1984) A classification
important to evaluate differences in coloring the edge of manipulative hand movements. Developmental Medicine
and Cbild Neurology, 26, 283-296.
(precision) versus the center (power) of a picture, Erhardt, R. P. (1984). Erhardt Developmental Prehen­
because younger children tend to use different grips sion Assessment. Laurel, MD: RAMSCO.
for each of these tasks. As children develop more re­ Gesell, A. (1940). Thefirstfive years of life. New York
fined skills, however, they tend to use the same grip Harper & Row.
for both the edge and the center of a picture. Goodgold, S. A. (1983). Handwriting movement qual­
ity in prekindergarten and kindergarten children. Archives
of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 64,471-475.
Conclusion Halverson, H. M. (1931). An experimental study of
Descriptions of pencil grip in children aged 3.0 to prehension in infants by means of systematic cinema rec­
ords. Genetic Psychology Monograph, 10, 107-284.
6.11 years showed a developmental progression, as
Jaffe, L. (1987). Influences of gnp on legibility, speed,
has been shown in previous studies (Halverson, 1931; and fatigue in adult handwriting. Unpublished manu­
Rosenbloom & Horton, 1971; Saida & Myashita, script.
1979). Two pencil grips-the dynamic tripod and the Levine, M., Brooks, R., & Shonkoff, R. P. (1980). A
lateral tripod grasps-appear to be used commonly as pediatric approach to learning disorders. New York: Wiley.
Lyle, J. (1976). Development of lateral consistency and
mature grips. It is interesting to note that the lateral
its relation to reading and reversals. Perceptual and Motor
tripod grasp was used by apprOXimately one fourth of Skills, 43, 695-698.
the children who demonstrated mature grips. Differ­ Morrison, A. (1978). Occupational therapy for writing
ences in the developmental progression of pencil difficulties in spina bifida children with myelomeningocele
grip were noted between boys and girls. The findings and hydrocephalus. British journal of Occupational Ther­
apy, 41, 394-397
from the present study also suggest that a different
Napier, J. R. (1956). The prehensile movements of the
developmental progression exists for a draWing task human hand. journal of Bone and joint Surgery, 38B,
than for a coloring task and that these tasks should, 902-913.
therefore, be evaluated separately..... Rosenbloom, L., & Horton, M. E. (1971). The matura­
tion of fine prehension in young children. Developmental
Medicine and Child Neurology, 13, 3-8.
Acknowledgments
Saida, Y., & Myashita, M. (1979). Development of fine
We thank Professors Sharon Cermak and Jane Coryell of motor skill in children: Manipulation of a pencil in young
Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, for their advice children aged 2-6 years old. journal ofHuman Movement
and assistance. We also thank the children who participated Studies, 5, 104-113.
in this study. Schneck, C. M. (1987). Developmental change in the
This study was supported in part through funding re­ use of writing tools in normal 3.6 to 6 a year old children.
ceived from the Dudley Allen Sargent Research Fund, Sar­ Unpublished manuscript.
gent College of Allied Health Professions, Boston Univer­ Schneck, C. M. (1989). Developmental cbangesin the
sity, Boston, Massachusetts, and from the Maternal and use of writing tools in normal 3. a to 6. 11 year old children
Child Health Traineeship administered through Boston and first-grade children with handwriting problems. Un­
University. published doctoral dissertation, Boston University, Boston,
This study was completed in partial fulfillment of the MA
first author's doctor of science degree in therapeutic studies Wynn-Parry, C. B. (1966). Rehabilitation of the hand
at Boston University. (2nd ed.). London: Butterworth.
Ziviani, J. (1983). Qualitative changes in dynamic tri­
pod grip between 7 and 14 years of age. Developmental
References Medicine and Child Neurology, 25, 778-782.
Bergmann, K. (1990). Incidence of atypical pencil Ziviani,]., & Elkins, J. (1986). Effect of pencil grip on
grasps among nondysfunctional adults. American journal handwriting speed and legibility. Educational Review, 38,
of Occupational Therapy, 44, 736-740. 247-257.

900 October 1990, Volume 44, Number 10

Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajot/930304/ on 03/22/2018 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms

You might also like